Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Transportation Research Part F journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/trf Public education messages aimed at smartphone use among young drivers: A mixed methods exploration of their effectiveness Cassandra S. Gauld a,d,⇑, Ioni M. Lewis a,d, Katherine M. White b,d, Judy J. Fleiter b,c, Barry Watson a,d a Centre for Accident Research and Road Safety, Queensland University of Technology, Victoria Park Rd, Kelvin Grove, Queensland 4059, Australia School of Psychology and Counselling, Queensland University of Technology, Victoria Park Rd, Kelvin Grove, Queensland 4059, Australia Global Road Safety Partnership, International Fédération of Red Cross & Red Crescent Sociétés, Route de Pré-Bois 1, CH-1214, Vernier, Switzerland d Institute of Health and Biomedical Innovation, Queensland University of Technology, 60 Musk Ave, Kelvin Grove, Queensland 4059, Australia b c a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 30 June 2018 Received in revised form 20 September 2018 Accepted 25 October 2018 Available online 9 November 2018 Keywords: Concept testing Smartphone Social interactive technology Public education messages Young drivers Step approach to Message Design and Testing (SatMDT) a b s t r a c t The main aim of this study was to concept test nine public education messages; with three different messages targeting each of three salient underlying beliefs in accordance with the Step Approach to Message Design and Testing (SatMDT) framework. The underlying beliefs were: (1) believing you are a good driver would encourage a young driver to monitor/read and respond to social interactive technology while driving; (2) slow-moving traffic would encourage a young driver to monitor/read and respond to social interactive technology while driving; and, (3) friends and peers would approve of a young driver monitoring/reading and responding to communications on their smartphone. Consistent with the SatMDT, the testing aimed to establish which three messages (each targeting a different underlying belief) young drivers reported as being the most effective. A mixed methods approach was utilised to provide an in-depth examination of individuals’ thoughts and feelings about the messages, with such responses assessed via an individual self-report survey and focus group discussions/interviews. Participants (N = 33; 19F, 14 M) were aged 17–25 years, had a current driver’s licence, owned a smartphone, and resided in the Australian state of Queensland. Means for each of the survey items were compared across message concepts to determine which ones were rated highest. Focus group discussion/interview responses underwent a data-led thematic analysis. The results of the quantitative and qualitative analyses were integrated to identify three messages that were deemed the most effective, one for each of the three underlying beliefs. Each of these three messages elicited positive emotion and modelled positive behaviour. This research highlights the importance of concept testing message content with the target audience. The results support current research that suggests road safety messages modelling positive behaviour and eliciting positive emotions may be especially persuasive for young drivers. Ó 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. ⇑ Corresponding author at: Centre for Accident Research and Road Safety, Queensland University of Technology, Victoria Park Rd, Kelvin Grove, Queensland 4059, Australia. E-mail address: c1.gauld@qut.edu.au (C.S. Gauld). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2018.10.027 1369-8478/Ó 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 312 C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 1. Introduction 1.1. Young drivers Worldwide, road trauma is the leading cause of death among young people aged 15–29 years (WHO, 2015). In Australia, young drivers aged 17 to 25 years constitute just 12.4% of the population (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2015) yet are represented in over 20% of road crash fatalities (Department of Infrastructure and Regional Development, 2014). Smartphones are most popular among 18 to 29 year olds (Rainie, 2012) and young drivers are more likely than other age groups to access the additional capabilities and computer functions, such as Facebook and email while driving (AAMI, 2012, 2015). Young drivers, therefore, are at increased risk of road trauma from smartphone use. 1.2. Monitoring/reading and responding to social interactive technology Social interactive technology refers to smartphone functions that allow the user to communicate with other people via, for example, social networking sites (e.g., Facebook, Twitter), emails, and also texting and calling. Recent research has investigated discrete behaviours associated with mobile phone use, such as reading and responding to communications, as these behaviours have different rates of prevalence, and have been associated with different underlying motivations and risk perceptions (e.g., Atchley, Atwood, & Boulton, 2011; Shi, Xiao, & Atchley, 2016; Waddell & Wiener, 2014). Young drivers, for example, perceive that replying to text messages is riskier than reading text messages (Shi et al., 2016). This perception may be encouraging young drivers to read communications more often than respond to them (Gauld et al., 2016); however, recent research has shown that simply hearing a notification on one’s phone can significantly disrupt performance on an attention-demanding task (Stothart, Mitchum, & Yehnert, 2015). It is possible, therefore, that reading a communication while driving may not be as safe as young drivers perceive it to be. Although these aforementioned studies were limited to calling and texting behaviours, the differences in motivation, prevalence, and risk perception may also apply to other social interactive technologies. 1.3. Public education messages The main aim of public education messages in road safety is to encourage safer road user behaviours (Elliott, 1993; Lewis, Watson, & White, 2009; Watson, 1996). Given the substantial difficulties police face with the enforcement of mobile phone use while driving (e.g., Goodwin, O’Brien, & Foss, 2012; Jessop, 2008; McCartt, Hellinga, Strouse, & Farmer, 2010), the development of effective theory-based public education messages, as part of a comprehensive strategy, is critical. 1.3.1. The step approach to message design and testing (SatMDT) The Step approach to Message Design and Testing ([SatMDT], Lewis et al., 2009; Lewis, Watson, & White, 2016) framework was devised to guide the development and evaluation of road safety public education messages. The framework is based on social psychological theories of decision making and attitude-behaviour relations and behaviour change including the Theory of Planned Behaviour ([TPB] Ajzen, 1991), the Extended Parallel Processing Model ([EPPM] Witte, 1992), the Elaboration Likelihood Model ([ELM] Petty & Cacioppo, 1986), and Social Learning Theory (Bandura, 1969). The four main steps of the framework are: (1) identification of pre-existing individual characteristics; (2) development of message-related characteristics; (3) individual responses; and (4) evaluation of message outcomes (Lewis et al., 2009; Lewis, Watson, et al., 2016) (see Fig. 1). The current study applied Step 3 of the framework. The aim of this step is to assess individuals’ responses (both emotional and cognitive) to message content in order to determine whether the messages are operating as intended by the message developers (i.e., concept test the messages). Formal manipulation checks of message content have not always been conducted in past research; rather, researchers have relied on a priori assumptions regarding a message’s content and the assumption that the desired effect will be achieved (Dillard, Plotnick, Godbold, Freimuth, & Edgar, 1996; Eveland and McLeod 1999; LaTour & Rotfeld, 1997; Lewis et al., 2009; Phillips, Ulleberg, & Vaa, 2011; Plant, Reza, & Irwin, 2011). This lack of checking may be problematic because, for example, if the emotions are not checked, the degree of message persuasiveness may be attributed to an assumed emotion rather than the actual emotion/s that is elicited (Lewis et al., 2009). A key feature of this step is to conduct manipulation checks of the anticipated emotional response to the message. In accordance with the methods and materials designed to apply the SatMDT (Lewis, White, Watson, & Elliott, 2017), the concept testing phase comprises a mixed methods approach to ensure an in depth and comprehensive examination of participants’ thoughts and feelings about the content of each message. Immediately after being presented with each message concept, and prior to the focus group discussion taking place, participants are invited to complete a short survey regarding their immediate response to the concept. Survey questions include, for example, the degree of perceived persuasiveness of the concept for themselves and others, and the emotional response to the concept. The subsequent focus group discussion will then allow the participants to deliberate on various aspects of the messages in more detail. An important part of concept testing involves refinement of message content in response to the participants’ feedback (Lewis, Watson, et al., 2016). 313 C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 STEP 4 Pre-existing individual characteristics Message-related characteristics Individual responses Message outcomes Identify Focus & Content Emotional & Cognitive + + Extent & nature of involvement in/with behaviour Strategies for avoiding behaviour (response efficacy) Focus of Message Challenge perceived benefits and/or Highlight perceived disadvantages Key content *Emotional appeal type (e.g., fear-based, humourbased) Emotional responses (anticipated emotion elicited?) + Cognitive responses (e.g., perceptions of response efficacy, involvement) *Modelling of behaviour *Strategies Acceptance & Rejection Intentions to adopt message and/or denial, defensive avoidance reactions Persuasive effects measured over time Salient beliefs Methodology Step 1 -Pilot work Gender/ age Elicit Methodology Step 4 - Quantitative-based assessment of persuasive effects STEP 3 Methodology Step 3- Concept testing & message checks STEP 2 Methodology Step 2- Message exposure STEP 1 Fig. 1. The SatMDT (Lewis et al., 2009; Lewis, Watson, et al., 2016). The messages to undergo concept testing in the current study were previously developed in accordance with Step 2 of the SatMDT. Specifically, when designing the messages, consideration was given to various factors that have been shown to enhance message effectiveness. The inclusion of contextual features in message scenarios that are relevant to young drivers, such as using a phone while stopped at traffic lights, have been shown to enhance message effectiveness (Lewis et al., 2012). Both positive (i.e., the driver does not use their smartphone) and negative (i.e., the driver does use their smartphone) modelling of behaviour have the potential to encourage enactment of the desired behaviour (Bandura, 1969; Lewis, Watson, White, & Tay, 2007). While threat appeals that elicit fear have traditionally been the most common form of road safety public education message in Australia, research suggests that young people, particularly young males, may respond better to messages that elicit positive emotions (Lewis, Watson, & White, 2008; Lewis, Watson, White, et al., 2007). The inclusion of strategies that are likely to elicit a high level of response efficacy (i.e., the belief of the target audience that strategies recommended in the message to avoid engaging in the target behaviour will be successful in avoiding a negative consequence [Witte, 1992]) has been directly related to message effectiveness (e.g., Lewis, Watson, & White, 2010, 2013; Lewis, Watson, White, et al., 2007; Tay & Watson, 2002; Witte, 1992). 1.4. The current study The current mixed methods study applied Step 3 of the SatMDT (Lewis et al., 2009; Lewis, Watson, et al., 2016) and investigated which of nine public education messages aimed at monitoring/reading and responding to social interactive technology on smartphones were reported as being the most persuasive by members of the target audience; namely, young drivers aged 17–25 years. Specifically, the messages each targeted one of three salient beliefs identified previously (Authors, 2016). These beliefs were: (1) believing you are a good driver would encourage a young driver monitor/read or respond to social interactive technology on their smartphone; (2) slow-moving traffic would encourage a young driver monitor/read or respond to social interactive technology on their smartphone; and, (3) friends and peers would approve of a young driver monitoring/reading or responding to communications on their smartphone. The current study, therefore, aimed to identify the three most effective message concepts, each targeting a different underlying belief. 2. Method 2.1. Participants Participants (N = 33; 19F, 14 M) were aged 17–25 years (M = 19 years, SD = 1.99), had a current driver’s licence (provisional: n = 23; open: n = 9; international: n = 1), owned a smartphone, and resided in the Australian state of Queensland. Most participants (n = 29) were first year psychology students who took part for partial course credit. The remaining participants (n = 4) were recruited via university email lists, university webpages, and the researchers’ family and friends. Participants who were not eligible for course credit were offered a coffee voucher to thank them for their time. 314 C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 2.2. Materials 2.2.1. Public education messages As previously outlined in the introduction, the messages presented in this study had been developed in accordance with Step 2 of the SatMDT framework and included key message-related features that have been shown to improve message effectiveness (see Lewis, Watson, et al., 2016). Table 1 outlines the key features and brief descriptions of each public education message. Nine messages had been developed each containing different combinations of these key features. 2.2.2. Brief survey The brief survey was adapted from previous studies which had applied the SatMDT (e.g., Lewis, Ho, & Lennon, 2016; see also Lewis et al. 2017) and examined participants’ perceptions of each message’s effectiveness as well as their emotional and cognitive responses to the messages. All responses were measured on 5-point Likert scales (except for two general questions about positive and negative emotional responses that were measured on 7-point Likert scales). Participants were firstly asked about their reaction to the message content in regards to 13 discrete emotions (e.g., ‘Please indicate on the scale provided, the extent that the ad made you feel (e.g., fearful, amused; 1 [strongly disagree] to 5 [strongly agree]’). Five items then assessed how effective they perceived the message to be (e.g., ‘How persuasive do you think the message was?’ ‘How believable do you think the message was?’ ‘How relevant is the message to you personally?’). The survey also asked about the strategies included in the message (e.g., ‘How likely are you to adopt the strategies recommended in the message?’). 2.2.3. Focus group/interview question schedule The semi-structured interview schedule, developed by Lewis, Watson, et al. (2016), was adapted for use in the current study. Questions included participants’ initial reaction to the message, whether it challenged their beliefs, what strategies the message provided to help young drivers to stop using social interactive technology, and how realistic and believable it was perceived to be. The interview schedule also asked participants if they believed the message could be easily adapted to the other behaviour (e.g., if the message targeted monitoring/reading, could it be easily adapted to responding?). At the end of each focus group (or interview), and consistent with the study’s aim to identify the three most persuasive messages, participants were asked to identify the top three messages that they perceived to be the most effective, including which message was their favourite. 2.3. Procedure There were 10 focus group discussions in total, each comprising 2 to 4 participants, and seven individual interviews (see Table 2 for a summary of participant groups). Consent was obtained verbally. Each of the nine messages was presented to young drivers as a written outline. The participants were told that the messages were in the early stages of development; however, it was intended that they would be developed into video format in the future (e.g., an audio-visual message intended for YouTube or television). The first of the nine messages was presented to each participant to read silently to themselves. Participants then answered the brief survey questions individually so there was no influence from other group members on their immediate responses (Lewis, Watson, et al., 2016). The focus group discussion or interview then took place to ensure the messages’ emotional and cognitive content was being conveyed to the participants as intended. The discussion was allowed to flow naturally; however, if some of the questions on the interview schedule remained unanswered near the end of the discussion, participants were asked these questions directly. This process was then repeated for each of the remaining messages. The messages were presented to the participants in the same order, which had been randomly predetermined. The interviews and focus group discussions were audio recorded and transcribed verbatim by the first author to ensure maximum familiarity with the text. 2.4. Data analysis In the initial analysis, the quantitative and qualitative data were analysed separately. For the quantitative component, means for each of the survey items were compared across message concepts to determine which ones were rated highest on each of the emotion and effectiveness items. As the sample size was small for a quantitative analysis, these results were analysed descriptively only. For the qualitative component, responses underwent a data-led thematic analysis (Howitt & Cramer, 2014) to determine which public education messages were deemed the most effective and why. Specifically, the analysis involved coding the data and then developing overarching themes from the coding. The other authors reviewed the findings and regularly provided feedback. The findings for each message were compared with and contrasted to the findings across the other messages. In the final stage of the analysis, and consistent with best practise in mixed methods research (see Creswell, Klassen, Plano Clark, & Smith, 2011), the qualitative and quantitative data were integrated to determine which of the messages were deemed the most effective. Please note that, in response to the ongoing feedback obtained in this study, messages that were consistently deemed ineffective were removed during the course of the focus group discussions and not included in the subsequent analysis (i.e., Message 1 ‘Red Arrows’ and Message 3 ‘Roll Back’). Table 1 Brief descriptions and key features of each public education message. Brief description Underlying belief challenged Modelling of behaviour Anticipated valence of predominant affect Strategies Contextual features 1.‘Red Arrows’ A young driver is continually checking their smartphone while making driver errors (e.g., swerving into the next lane, indicates in the wrong direction). Each time the young driver makes driving errors, three big red arrows (which are overlaid on the visual) point at the car and flash. When the young driver brakes suddenly at a stop light, they look up and cringe when they see the other drivers looking over angrily. Voice over: So you think you can check yours smartphone and others won’t know? Don’t kid yourself – it’s like driving with big red arrows pointing at you. Tagline: Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Even good drivers become bad drivers when they are distracted. Believing you are a good driver makes it easier Negative Negative Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Alone; moving traffic 2. ‘Help your Friends’ Three friends (one young driver, and 2 friends) are driving in a suburban area and chatting about the party they went to last night. The driver picks up their phone to check a notification to which the driver tries to respond. The friends tell the driver that they feel afraid when the driver uses their phone and one friend offers to respond for the driver, to which the driver agrees. Tagline: Look out for your mates. Don’t let them drive distracted. Friends/peers would approve Negative Positive Passenger using young driver’s phone for them. Two passengers; suburb; moving freely 3. ‘Rollback’ A young driver is stuck in a traffic jam caused by roadworks. The driver checks their smartphone and the car slowly rolls backwards into the car behind. The driver is embarrassed when they realise what has happened. Voice over: You might think it is OK to check your smartphone in a traffic jam - but it’s not. Tagline: Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Stay alert in all traffic situations. Slow-moving traffic makes it easier Negative Negative Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Alone; slow moving traffic 4. ‘Good driver’ A young driver waves goodbye to their Dad as they leave for university. The Dad waves back and shouts out ‘drive safely’ to which the young driver replies ‘Yeah, yeah, dad, I am a good driver you know’. As the young driver leaves a notification ‘ding’ is heard on their smartphone. The driver reaches over to check it but hesitates as they remember that they told their Dad they were a good driver. A dog runs in front of the car and the car stops safely. Voiceover: Be the good driver you say you are. Tagline: Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Good drivers don’t check their smartphone while behind the wheel. Believing you are a good driver makes it easier Positive Positive Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Alone; suburb 5. ‘Dating’ Two male friends are driving to the beach. The driver reads a communication from ‘Sarah’ who is checking where the party is next weekend. The driver starts responding while the car is in motion and swerves onto the other side of the road. The passenger looks nervous and reminds the driver that he may need to take Sarah to the party on his skateboard if he is no longer able to drive a car. Voiceover: Listen to your friends when they say it can wait. Don’t drive distracted. Tagline Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. No regrets. #YOLO Friends/peers would approve Positive & Negative Positive & Negative Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. One passenger; freely moving traffic 6. ‘Traffic Lights’ A young driver pulls up at traffic lights, first in the queue. As they are checking their smartphone they fail to notice when the traffic lights change to green. Several car horns are heard and frustrated drivers are yelling at the young driver who then floors the car and, in the commotion, loses their smartphone out the window. A police officer picks up the smartphone from the middle of the road and the young driver looks worried as the police officer approaches. Slow-moving traffic makes it easier Negative Negative Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Alone; stopped at traffic lights 315 (continued on next page) C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 Message name 316 Table 1 (continued) Message name Brief description Underlying belief challenged Modelling of behaviour Anticipated valence of predominant affect Strategies Contextual features Voiceover: Think it’s OK to check your smartphone at the traffic lights? It’s not. You are still driving even when the light is red. Tagline: Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. You haven’t stopped driving just because you have stopped at traffic lights. Two animated (cartoon-style) cars are being driven by young drivers. The driver of Car 1 is intermittently using their smartphone while driving and making driving errors (e.g., swerving into the next lane, almost hitting a pedestrian when they step onto a crossing). The two cars are having a ‘conversation’ (via thought bubbles) about how bad the driver of Car 1 becomes when they are using their smartphone and how this behaviour has resulted in the cars getting damaged. Voiceover: Thinking you’re a good driver does not mean you can safely check your smartphone. Tagline: Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Even good drivers become bad drivers when they are distracted. Believing you are a good driver makes it easier Negative Negative Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Alone; moving traffic 8. ‘Animated Smartphone’ A young driver is in a slow-moving traffic jam when a ‘ding’ is heard. They reach over to the back seat to retrieve their smartphone and check the notification. An animated smartphone is shown crawling away from the driver’s hand and hiding under the driver’s jacket which is also on the back seat. When a pedestrian suddenly runs out in front of the car, it stops in plenty of time. A tired smartphone peeps out from under the jacket and appears relieved. Voiceover: Think it’s OK to answer your smartphone in a traffic jam? It’s not. There are unexpected dangers in slow-moving traffic. Tagline: Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Stay alert, and away from your smartphone, in all traffic situations. Slow-moving traffic makes it easier Positive Positive Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Alone; slow moving traffic 9. ‘Voice your Opinion’ A young driver is alone in their car in a suburban area on their way to a friend’s place. The driver ignores two notifications. When they arrive at their friend’s place two of the friends ask if the driver received their communications to which the driver replies that they were driving. The two friends who had sent messages can’t believe the driver didn’t respond. The three other friends support the driver by describing various negative consequences of such action (e.g., crashing, demerit points). The driver checks their phone and is surprised at how trivial the message actually is (i.e., picking up pizza on the way over). Voiceover: Keep it real. If you don’t like your friends < checking/answering > their smartphone while driving, tell them so. Tagline: Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Voice your opinion – let your friends know what you really think. Friends/peers would approve Positive Positive Pull over. Put it in the boot. Put it on silent. Alone; suburb; moving freely Note: the anticipated valence of the predominant affect was categorised by the researchers. C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 7. ‘Animated Cars’ 317 C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 Table 2 Summary of participant groups. Group number Group type Number of participants Participant age and gender 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 Interview Focus group Focus group Focus group Focus group Interview Focus group Focus group Focus group Interview Focus group Interview Interview Focus group Interview Interview Focus group 1 2 2 3 3 1 3 3 2 1 2 1 1 2 1 1 4 19M 20F, 17F 22F, 18F 18F, 18F, 17F 22F, 19F, 18F 18F 18F, 18F, 18F 18M, 21F, 18F 17M, 17F 19M 18F, 18F 20M 19M 20M, 25M 20M 19M 18M, 19M, 19M, 25M discussion discussion discussion discussion discussion discussion discussion discussion discussion discussion Note: In the final column of this table, ‘M/F’ denotes gender and the number denotes age. For examples, ‘19M’ means a 19-year-old male. 3. Results and discussion Results for each of the remaining seven message concepts (i.e., messages 2, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, and 9) are found in Tables 3–5. Specifically, the emotional response to each of the message concepts, and the means and standard deviations for each item are presented in Table 3. Encouragingly, this emotion check found that for five of the seven message concepts (i.e., messages 4, 6, 7, 8, and 9), the valence of the predominant affect was as anticipated (and as outlined in Table 1). As previously stated, it is important to check the individuals’ emotional responses; otherwise, the degree of message persuasiveness may be attributed to an anticipated emotion rather than the actual emotion elicited (Lewis et al., 2009). The perceived effectiveness of each message concept and the means and standard deviations for each item are presented in Table 4. The perceived effectiveness is an important construct as it has been shown to be a causal antecedent of actual effectiveness (Dillard, Shen, & Vail, 2007). Results of the thematic analysis and supporting quotes for each of the remaining seven message concepts that were tested on all the participants are presented in Table 5. Included in this table is the number of times each message was reported to be in a participant’s top three messages, including how many times it was cited as the favourite. The type of social interactive technology represented in the messages was checked with participants to ensure the desired range of technology was being depicted. As anticipated, the ‘communications’ and ‘notifications’ referred to in the messages were commonly believed to represent any of a number of forms of social interactive technology. Participants believed, however, that they were most likely to represent a text message, a phone call, a Facebook message or comment notification, or an email. These forms of social interactive technology are also the most commonly reported forms accessed while driving (Gauld, Lewis, White, Fleiter, & Watson, 2017). Overall, participants consistently deemed the strategy ‘put it in the boot1’, that appeared in the tagline of all messages (except Message 2), as not practical. Specifically, participants did not believe that young drivers would be likely to use that strategy because their phone would not be available should an emergency arise. As such, at the conclusion of this study, it was replaced with the strategy ‘put it right out of sight’. 3.1. Message 2 (‘Help your Friends’) Overall, this message concept was perceived as effective. Despite anticipating that that the valence of the predominant emotion when reading this message would be positive, participants reported feeling the most anxious than both any other emotion and any other message. For example, as reported in the focus groups: ‘It made me feel uneasy ‘cos it is so relatable that this is something that could happen to us, so if we don’t see the pedestrian while we are texting, we could hit the pedestrian’ (F21). This message scored the highest on the three survey measures of relevance, likelihood to adopt strategies, and likelihood to reduce/stop smartphone use while driving. With regard to relevance, the focus group discussions also reported that this message was relevant in relation to the context of driving with friends and to the scenario itself. For example: 1 ‘Boot’ is the Australian term for ‘trunk’ of a car. 318 Message concept Negative* Positive* Sad Fearful Anxious Annoyed Relaxed Competent Happy Proud Excited Amused Flattered Agitated Relieved 2. ‘Help your friends’ 4. ‘Good driver’ 5. ‘Dating’ 3.94 (1.56) 3.39 (1.30) 3.52 (1.28) 4.09 (1.44) 4.09 (1.26) 3.81 (1.09) 3.94 (1.41) 3.97 (1.56) 4.73 (1.31) 4.00 (1.44) 3.48 (1.42) 3.84 (1.35) 4.34 (1.31) 4.30 (1.43) 2.30 (0.98) 2.24 (0.75) 2.19 (0.90) 2.34 (1.10) 2.55 (1.00) 2.03 (0.92) 2.39 (1.09) 3.03 (1.10) 2.42 (0.83) 2.76 (1.12) 2.82 (1.13) 3.27 (1.04) 2.70 (1.16) 2.30 (1.05) 3.30 (0.98) 2.78 (0.98) 3.06 (1.09) 3.21 (0.99) 3.24 (1.03) 3.09 (1.10) 2.55 (1.06) 2.91 (1.18) 2.25 (0.95) 2.88 (1.22) 3.42 (1.12) 3.15 (1.15) 2.76 (1.20) 3.12 (1.36) 2.38 (1.04) 2.73 (1.07) 2.21 (0.93) 1.82 (0.77) 2.06 (0.97) 2.33 (1.14) 2.64 (1.22) 2.42 (0.87) 2.91 (0.98) 2.42 (0.94) 2.00 (0.79) 2.18 (0.88) 2.21 (0.99) 2.45 (1.09) 2.73 (1.07) 3.18 (0.98) 2.58 (1.20) 2.09 (0.95) 2.39 (1.14) 2.67 (1.22) 2.76 (1.20) 2.36 (1.03) 3.12 (1.02) 2.48 (1.06) 1.76 (0.75) 2.06 (0.86) 2.36 (1.03) 3.21 (1.17) 2.03 (0.81) 2.42 (0.94) 2.39 (1.09) 2.06 (1.03) 2.12 (0.93) 2.27 (1.18) 2.45 (1.18) 2.45 (1.00) 2.61 (1.03) 3.18 (1.24) 2.73 (1.33) 3.15 (1.12) 3.18 (1.10) 2.94 (1.32) 1.94 (0.86) 2.21 (0.93) 2.24 (1.06) 2.09 (0.93) 2.24 (0.94) 2.00 (0.83) 2.33 (1.14) 2.48 (1.12) 2.33 (0.96) 2.82 (1.04) 3.21 (0.89) 2.82 (1.13) 2.52 (1.00) 2.79 (1.22) 2.76 (1.25) 3.79 (0.99) 2.79 (1.19) 2.21 (0.86) 2.64 (1.17) 3.12 (1.02) 3.00 (1.06) 6. ‘Traffic lights’ 7. ‘Animated cars’ 8. ‘Animated smartphone’ 9. ‘Voice your opinion’ Note. Messages 1 and 3 received relatively weak feedback and were removed prior to the end of the study. * These items were measured on a 7-point Likert scale (1) strongly disagree, (7) strongly agree. All other items were measured on a 5-point scale, with (5) indicating a more positive response. C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 Table 3 Means and Standard Deviations for the Emotion Check (for Messages 2, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, & 9) from Survey Data. 319 C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 Table 4 Means and Standard Deviations for the Effectiveness Check (for Messages 2, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, & 9) from Survey Data. Message concept Persuasiveness Believability Relevance Likelihood of adopting strategies Useful strategies Likelihood of reducing/stopping SP use while driving 2. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 3.18 3.39 3.03 3.12 2.88 3.12 3.55 3.75 3.45 3.33 2.69 2.82 2.85 3.76 3.48 3.27 2.85 2.91 2.67 2.91 3.36 3.70 3.36 3.15 2.97 2.85 3.03 3.42 3.21 3.36 2.97 3.03 3.06 2.94 3.39 3.28 3.27 2.97 3.06 3.03 3.09 3.27 ‘Help your friends’ ‘Good driver’ ‘Dating’ ‘Traffic lights’ ‘Animated cars’ ‘Animated smartphone’ ‘Voice your opinion’ (0.73) (0.61) (0.88) (0.89) (0.93) (0.78) (0.97) (0.57) (0.67) (0.82) (1.06) (0.81) (0.83) (0.94) (0.87) (0.91) (0.87) (0.84) (0.92) (0.84) (0.96) (0.85) (0.74) (0.83) (0.95) (0.91) (0.85) (0.87) (0.89) (0.60) (0.85) (0.95) (0.75) (0.66) (0.75) (0.92) (0.84) (0.92) (0.97) (0.85) (0.95) (0.88) Note. All items were measured on a 5-point scale, with (5) indicating a more positive response. ‘SP’ means smartphone. ‘I could relate to this one, um I think it is something a lot of young people could relate to as well. It is quite persuasive’ (M19) Relevance was also discussed in the focus groups in relation to passengers checking and responding to the driver’s smartphone for them (i.e., the strategy presented in this message). This finding supports the survey finding regarding this message being the most likely to reduce/stop smartphone use while driving, possibly because the passenger can do this for them. For example: ‘I thought it was really relatable, if you have friends in your car and you get a message they can respond for you if you are the driver’ (F20). In addition, the survey finding that this message would be the most likely to stop the driver using their smartphone while driving. This could, in part, be due to the driver’s feelings of responsibility for their passengers and the idea that that driver’s smartphone is everyone’s responsibility, as discussed in the focus groups. For example: ‘It was kind of like saying that you’ve got other lives in your car so you are not only responsible for your own’ (M17). The focus group discussions, however, suggested that the passenger’s fake response on the driver’s smartphone (i.e., pretending to text the driver’s workplace saying that they would rather go to the beach) as described in the message would have been quite distracting for the driver. In addition, many participants reported that they were already using this strategy and, therefore, the ability of the message to change young drivers’ behaviour may be limited. 3.2. Message 4 (‘Good Driver’) This message received positive feedback. It was reported as being in the top three messages for 16 participants and the favourite for five. As anticipated, this message concept elicited predominantly positive emotions. In comparison to the other message concepts, participants reported feeling the highest level of relaxation, competence, happiness, and relief. For example, as reported in the focus group discussions: ‘‘I like how it is not depressing again. . .good that it shows that he stops in time and all good because you know what would happen otherwise (F18) Focus group discussions reported that it was refreshing to have a positive message with a young driver depicted in a responsible manner (i.e., the young driver modelled positive behaviour by not checking their smartphone), thereby promoting a message of prevention (Lewis, Watson, White, et al., 2007). For example: ‘I liked the way it was focused in the effects of positive behaviour rather than negative behaviour’ (F21). In the survey, this message scored in the top three messages for all the effectiveness checks (e.g., persuasiveness, believability, relevance). These results were supported in the focus group discussions, for example: ‘Parental factors for me are high driving forces. I just imagine how devastated my parents would be if I got into a significant accident ‘cos I was being an idiot’ (F17); and, ‘. . .it is persuasive and I think the message has strong content because we are talking about being responsible’ (M18) This combination of positive affect and high levels of perceived effectiveness supports current literature that suggests it is worthwhile broadening the scope of the emotional appeal of messages to include positive appeals (Lewis, Watson, White, et al., 2007). Previous literature also shows that young drivers believe their parents would disapprove of them using their smartphone while driving (e.g., Gauld, Lewis, & White, 2014). This finding, combined with participants in the current study reporting a sense of accountability towards their parents (i.e., behaving like a good driver not just saying they were a good driver) and wanting their parents to be proud of their actions, appears to have enhanced the effectiveness of this message. 320 Table 5 Key Themes and Supporting Quotes for the Qualitative Analysis (for Messages 2, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, & 9) of Focus Group Data. Themes Supporting quotes No of times in top 3 2. ‘Help your Friends’ 1. The scene (i.e., driving with friends) is very common for young drivers ‘It made me feel uneasy ‘cos it is so relatable that this is something that could happen to us, so if we don’t see the pedestrian while we are texting, we could hit the pedestrian’ (F21) ‘I could relate to this one, um I think it is something a lot of young people could relate to as well. It is quite persuasive’ (M19) ‘I thought it was really relatable, if you have friends in your car and you get a message they can respond for you if you are the driver’ (F20) ‘Well, I wouldn’t like hand them my smartphone and say like ‘go for gold’ but if I was driving and like a text message came up I could say ‘oh would you just check that?’ (F18) ‘It was kind of like saying that you’ve got other lives in your car so you are not only responsible for your own’ (M17) ‘I thought it was like the message about how it is everyone’s responsibility not just the driver was good’ (F18) 12 (3 favourite) 2. Young drivers’ willingness to let their passengers (friends) use their smartphone 3. The importance of the driver being accountable to their friends 4. The smartphone is everyone’s responsibility when in the car 4. ‘Good Driver’ 5. The importance of a sensible response from the passenger using the smartphone ‘Yeah and he’s also like not focused on driving, he is focused on the friend, so it is almost a distraction’ (M20) ‘I felt a little bit angry because even though the friends say they want to help the driver to text back they make a joke and make the driver distracted’ (M18). 1. The scene is very believable ‘I know my parents always say to me when I am going out ‘drive safely’ and all the time you just shrug it off; this might make you actually stop and think about it’ (F17) ‘I was proud of him in the end too; like ‘well done’; at the start I was a bit annoyed and then at the end when he put it away, saved the dog, that was good (F20) ‘It was different to any of the ads I’ve seen before in road safety’ (F18) ‘I liked the way it was focused on the effects of positive behaviour rather than negative behaviour’ (F21) ‘‘I like how it is not depressing again. . .good that it shows that he stops in time and all good because you know what would happen otherwise (F18) ‘..the thought of hitting a dog, absolutely I could not handle that’ (F18) ‘Scene 7 is a good focus on the dog. I think the dog is good as so many people like dogs’ (M19) ‘Parental factors for me are high driving forces, I just imagine how devastated my parents would be if I got into a significant accident cos I was being an idiot’ (F17) ‘..it is persuasive and I think the message has strong content because we are talking about being responsible’ (M18) ‘this one would be remembering the ideal of your parents and how they think you should be driving (M19) 16 (5 favourite) ‘I think it is good that the passenger incorporated humour into his message, um, because it was a lot more effective’ (F18) ‘. . .when he said ‘on your skateboard’ that really made me laugh’ (M19) ‘I mean the ad should be teaching you to say something like not mocking them and as soon as you’re mocking them you are even more distracted and something happens and then you are like. . .when the friend was one of the main causes of the problem’ (F18) ‘. . .and also here where he is looking embarrassed but he is also looking at his friend which means he isn’t looking at the road. . .’ (M19) 6 (2 favourite) 2. Refreshing to have a positive message 3. The possibility of hitting the dog represented a sufficient consequence 4. The importance of wanting your dad to be proud and of being accountable to what you say to your dad are motivating 5. ‘Dating’ 1. Good use of humour 2. Message was confused as the driver is also distracted by the passenger mocking them C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 Message concept 6. ‘Traffic Lights’ ‘It sort of feels a bit half-hearted. . .where I know personally with my friends I’d be like ‘don’t do that – you’re being an idiot’(F17) ‘..that the mate spoke up. . .it took him a while to speak up which is kind of like. . .hmmm’ ‘I just felt like it half hit the mark’ (F22) 1. Beginning of the scenario is common and relatable ‘It had a really good start like stopped at the traffic lights. . .and then it turns’ (F18) ‘I think it is relatable because lots of people get the urge to check their smartphone at traffic lights (F20) ‘. . .the smartphone flying out the window was a bit of a stretch. . .’ (M19) ‘the point when he loses the smartphone that kind of lost it cos that tongue in cheek and it is supposed to be a serious message . . .’ (F17) ‘the situation is relatable but slides out of control when they lost their smartphone out the window’ (F17) ‘I think this one is bringing out the embarrassment when there is a lot of cars behind you’ (F18) ‘Yeah, because that screeching is so embarrassing, yeah if I do that I feel like everyone knows you were distracted by something, there is no-one else in the car. . .you were checking your smartphone. . .and when that happens to me it’s like ‘I’m not going to check my smartphone’ and that lasts for a week, a couple of weeks and seeing it in the ad form probably would help because any driver who has been driving on their own for more than 3 months has probably experienced this’ (M19) ‘If drivers yell at me when I am doing something I get so embarrassed . . .if someone beeps their horn at me I get stressed and embarrassed and I try not to do it again. . .so I think a lot of drivers relate to that embarrassment’ (F18) 11 (2 favourite) ‘I thought it was visually stimulating’ (F18) ‘More people would be like ‘oh, what’s this?’ and want to look at it’ (M17) ‘I quite like the ad animation because it was different from what we have seen before’ (F18) ‘I think it was good, like as in entertaining to watch’ (F18) ‘Cartoons are probably not an age-appropriate was to communicate to 20 year olds’ (M19) ‘I wasn’t a fan of giving the cars personality or whatever, I just thought that was a bit childish’ (F21) ‘I think it is a lot less persuasive because it is a cartoon’ (F18) ‘I feel it wouldn’t be taken seriously’ (M18) ‘It puts the focus on the cars – like wow our cars have feelings, we should not crash because of our cars. It doesn’t put focus on the responsibility of the driver as much’ (F20) ‘I think it tried to but I don’t think it is very effective’ (F21) ‘This one showed a bit of humour which was interesting’ (M20) ‘I found it comedic, so I felt quite amused when I was reading it (F22) 9 (2 favourite) ‘Personifying the smartphone was a smart idea’ (M19) ‘I liked the idea of the animated mixture in this one’(F21) ‘I liked that one. I thought it was smart’ (F20) ‘Yeah, people would actually enjoy watching it’ (F18) 11 (3favourite) 2. The second half of the scenario is not believable (from the point where the smartphone flies out the window) 3. The driver’s embarrassment/humiliation is a strong consequence 7. ‘Animated Cars’ 1. Animation is eye catching 2. Animation is not age-appropriate (it is too young) 3. It is not an effective message 4. It is amusing 8. ‘Animated Smartphone’ 1. The smartphone animation made the message memorable, amusing, and entertaining C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 3. The passenger’s protest about the driver using their smartphone was too weak (i.e., the message was not strong enough). (continued on next page) 321 322 Table 5 (continued) Message concept Themes 2. The message was clear 3. The scene was relatable 4. The smartphone running away may have been more distracting for the driver 9. ‘Voice your Opinion’ 1. The message and scene are believable, realistic, and relatable 2. Showing both perspectives (i.e., that some friends check their smartphone and some friends don’t) is a strength 3. Depicting more than one consequence is a strength 4. The triviality of the actual communication (i.e., buying pizza) emphasises the point that using your smartphone while driving is not worth the risk No of times in top 3 ‘This is the best message so far out of all of them because it is believable, there is some sort of relatability to it, how we have been in a situation, it accurately portrays how friends would act to each other cos in the end it also shows the driver not backing down’ (M19) ‘I liked it as well because it had everything in it – some humour, some reality conversation, some good advice of being yourself and listen to your friends’ (M18) ‘Very relatable, very realistic’ (M19) ‘This one was brilliant’ (M19) ‘they’ve got both sides of the argument and then obviously the better side wins which is good’ (F18) ‘there is a good comparison between the people who know it’s wrong and the others and you can tell people who think that it is right don’t have an appreciation of what could happen’ (F20) ‘I liked that there was a group of friends saying well this happened to a friend of mine and this happened to a friend of mine and wouldn’t want to fix anyone else’s car, so I thought that was good’ (F21) ‘It’s good that they say the things that could happen because of it and then they are probably more likely to think, maybe a better idea that you didn’t check it’ (F18) ‘It’s a good point like showing, oh, you only wanted me to get pizza on the way over because it is showing that you could have risked all of those things that the friends talked about just because they wanted you to buy pizza’ (F18) ‘yeah it’s nice because it focused on how trivial messages usually are’ (M19) 21 (15 favourite) C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 5. The message is targeting a younger audience Supporting quotes ‘It definitely gave me a new perspective to think about. Showing a situation in a different light is more likely to make me stop and think about my actions’ (F17) ‘It was entertaining’ (F18) ‘It is clear, it is not complicated or anything’ (F18) ‘This is a really good message’ (M20) ‘What I am getting out of this is that you’re choosing to do it – it doesn’t want to be used – I like that message. . .makes it entertaining’ (F18) ‘I liked the context of the traffic jam, that is a very relevant situation’ (M20) ‘I think a lot of people rummage around in the back seat for something so I think this will get the attention. . ... . .’(M19) ‘The fact is the smartphone was distracting her as compared to if she just found her smartphone. . .just a bit hypocritical to me. . .’ (F18) ‘Yeah, I think that is a lot more dangerous when people are turning around to the back to find their smartphone’ (F18) ‘I thought this one wasn’t very relatable in terms of age bracket. . ... . .’(M20) ‘I don’t think it is particularly age appropriate, a bit condescending....’ (M19) C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 323 3.3. Message 5 (‘Dating’) This message, along with Message 8 (‘Animated Smartphone’), amused participants the most; which was supported in the focus group discussions: ‘I think it is good that the passenger incorporated humour into this message, um, because it was a lot more effective’ (F18) On the survey items, however, it scored relatively low on all the effectiveness checks. Many participants indicated that the message was not clear because the driver was also distracted by the passenger mocking them. An example from the focus group discussions: ‘I mean the ad should be teaching you to say something like not mocking them and as soon as you’re mocking them you are even more distracted and something happens and then you are like. . .when the friend was one of the main causes of the problem’ (F18) Participants believed the passenger’s response to the driver’s phone use (i.e., ‘hey mate, can’t it wait?’) did not set a strong enough example for young drivers. For example: ‘I just felt like it half hit the mark’ (F22). 3.4. Message 6 (‘Traffic Lights’) As anticipated, this message elicited predominantly negative emotions. Specifically, this message concept, along with Message 7 (‘Animated Cars’), elicited the highest level of negative emotions, in particular, the emotions of annoyance and agitation. Dillard et al. (1996) found that anger inhibited the effectiveness of messages. While annoyance and agitation (i.e., the two highest scoring emotions for this message) are discretely different emotions to anger, they may have contributed to this message’s relatively low score on all the survey effectiveness checks. In the focus group discussions, it was reported that the first half of this message was believable and the driver’s embarrassment was a strong consequence, for example: ‘It had a really good start like stopped at the traffic lights. . .and then it turns’ (F18); and ‘I think this one is bringing out the embarrassment when there is a lot of cars behind you’ (F18) Most participants, however, indicated that it became unrealistic when the smartphone flew out the window and, as a result, the message may not be taken seriously: ‘. . .the smartphone flying out the window was a bit of a stretch. . .’ (M19) 3.5. Message 7 (‘Animated Cars’) As anticipated, this message, along with Message 6 (‘Traffic Lights’) elicited the highest level of negative emotions, in general, with participants reporting feeling the most sad and fearful. On the effectiveness checks, it scored relatively low on all the effectiveness checks. While it was reported that the animation would be eye catching, these survey results corresponded with the focus group discussions. For example: ‘I feel it wouldn’t be taken seriously’ (M18); and ‘I think it is a lot less persuasive because it is a cartoon’ (F18) Specifically, while the novelty of the animation was seen as eye-catching by many participants, they believed the message would be more effective for a younger demographic than the target audience, for example: ‘I wasn’t a fan of giving the cars personality or whatever, I just thought that was a bit childish’ (F20) 3.6. Message 8 (‘Animated Smartphone’) As anticipated, the predominant valence of emotion reported when reading this message was positive. Specifically, this message, along with Message 5 (‘Dating’) amused participants the most. For example: ‘Yeah, people would actually enjoy watching it’ (F18) While it received average scores on the effectiveness checks. The qualitative analysis suggested that there was a clear message regarding not using a smartphone while driving: 324 C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 ‘What I am getting out of this is that you are choosing to do it – it doesn’t want to be used- I like that message. . .makes it entertaining’ (F18) Some participants believed that having the smartphone run away may have caused the driver to be more distracted. For example: ‘Yeah, I think it is a lot more dangerous when people are turning around to the back to find their smartphone’ (F18) Many participants, however, reported that they could relate to this scene and would often reach for their smartphone in a traffic jam to alleviate their boredom: ‘I think a lot of people rummage around in the back seat for something so I think this will get attention’ (M19) Previous studies have shown that messages containing the element of surprise and novelty enhanced message effectiveness (Dillard et al., 1996; Morley, 1987; Morley & Walker, 1987). Indeed, many participants expressed surprise at the novelty of an animated smartphone and believed this message would be memorable, amusing, and entertaining. For example: ‘It definitely gave me a new perspective to think about. Showing a situation in a different light is more likely to make me stop and think about my actions’ (F17). 3.7. Message 9 (‘Voice your Opinion’) This message received positive feedback in both the focus group discussions and the survey. It was listed in the top three messages for 21 participants and the favourite for 15. As anticipated, the predominant valence of emotion reported was positive and participants reported feeling the most proud, excited, and flattered when reading this message and it was scored the highest for persuasiveness, believability, influence on self, influence on others, and as containing useful strategies to help reduce monitoring/reading social interactive technology while driving. Focus group discussions supported the survey findings as participants reported believing the scene was realistic and the potential consequences were clearly depicted. For example: ‘This message is the best so far out of all of them because it is believable, there is some sort of relatability to it, how we have been in a situation, it accurately portrays how friends would act to each other cos in the end it shows the driver not backing down’ (M19); and ‘there is a good comparison between the people who know it’s wrong and the others and you can tell people who think that it is right don’t have an appreciation of what could happen’ (F20) Some of the language, however, was not deemed to be realistic and so was amended during the study. In the original message outline, for example, the friend who had sent the driver a message said ‘You’re kidding right? Can’t believe you don’t check your phone!’ was amended to ‘Are you serious? I always check mine when I am driving, that’s why ya have two hands!’ Overall, the message was reported to be strong, enhanced by the triviality of the communication sent to the driver which is revealed at the end of the message (i.e., to buy pizza on your way over): ‘It’s a good point like showing, oh, you only wanted me to get pizza on the way over because it is showing that you could have risked all of those things that the friends talked about just because they wanted you to buy pizza’ (F18) As reported for Message 4 (‘Good Driver’), and in line with current literature, participants liked that this message modelled positive behaviour and depicted the young driver as responsible, thereby focusing on prevention (Hoekstra & Wegman, 2011; Lewis, Watson, White, et al., 2007). The underlying belief challenged in this message (i.e., that friends and peers would approve of a young driver using their smartphone while driving) is also consistent with previous literature that demonstrated the strong influence of friends’ and peers’ opinions on whether a young driver uses their smartphone while driving (e.g., Buckley, Chapman, & Sheehan, 2014; Gershon, Zhu, Klauer, Dingus, & Simons-Morton, 2017; Trivedi, Haynie, Bible, Liu, & Simons-Morton, 2017). In particular, these studies have found that young drivers whose friends and peers were more likely to use their smartphone while driving were themselves more likely to use their smartphone while driving. Buckley et al. (2014), suggested that interventions targeting this belief should take into account the strong desire for young drivers to please and fit in with their friends and peers. It is encouraging, therefore, that this message was deemed to be the most effective in the integrated analysis, given that the predominant opinion of the young driver’s friends depicted in this message was not to use one’s smartphone while driving. 4. General discussion The current study investigated which of nine public education messages aimed at monitoring/reading and responding to social interactive technology on smartphones were reported as being the most persuasive by members of the target audience; namely, young drivers aged 17–25 years. Concept testing is a vital step in message development as it ensures that the messages are actually operating as intended, as opposed to the assumption that they are operating as intended based on the researcher’s expectations. C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 325 Based on this integrated data analysis, Message 4 (‘Good Driver’), Message 8 (‘Animated Smartphone’), and Message 9 (‘Voice your Opinion’) were deemed the most effective messages for each of the three underlying belief categories2. Specifically, ‘Good Driver’ targeted ‘believing you are a good driver makes it easier’; ‘Animated Smartphone’ targeted ‘slow moving traffic makes it easier’; and ‘Voice Your Opinion’ targeted ‘friends and peers would approve’ of a young driver monitoring/reading or responding to communications on their smartphone. Of note, these three messages all elicited positive emotion and modelled positive behaviour, thereby supporting past research suggesting that young drivers respond better to messages that are positive in nature (e.g., Hoekstra & Wegman, 2011; Lewis, Watson, & Tay, 2007; Lewis, Watson, White, et al., 2007). Participants also believed that, while these three messages targeted monitoring/reading behaviour, they could be readily adapted to target responding behaviour with very few changes to the message content, thereby avoiding the possible influence that different message content may have on their persuasive impact. A major strength of this study lies in its mixed-methods approach and its ability to elicit feedback regarding all aspects of the message content directly from members of the target audience as both individual feedback (i.e., the survey) and as a group discussion. This feedback included the effectiveness of the message manipulations (e.g., of the underlying beliefs and the emotional responses), the realistic nature of the language used, and the practicality of the suggested strategies. In accordance with Step 3 of the SatMDT (Lewis et al., 2016), concept testing may involve refinement of the message concepts. Indeed, the current study identified examples of unrealistic language (particularly in Message 9) which were later amended. In addition, the strategy ‘put it in the boot’ was consistently deemed impractical and was later replaced with ‘put it right out of sight’. These formal checks of message content have not always been conducted in past research (Dillard et al., 1996; Eveland and McLeod, 1999; LaTour & Rotfeld, 1997; Lewis et al., 2009; Phillips et al., 2011; Plant et al., 2011) and, as such, concept testing should be considered an important step in the development of future road safety messages. There are some limitations of the current study that should also be acknowledged. As for other studies assessing the impact of persuasive messages (Elliott & Armitage, 2009; Glendon & Cernecca, 2003; Lewis, Watson, White, et al., 2007), this study utilised a within-subjects design to assess the effects of the messages. It is possible, however, that the effectiveness of a message may have been affected by exposure to the previous message (Tormala & Petty, 2007). While all messages were shown in the same sequence, this sequence was determined randomly. Although it is acknowledged that it is not possible to separate the effect of the message from the effect of the order, by holding the ordering of the advertisements constant across all group sessions, any effects would have been consistent across sessions. Almost all of the participants in this study were university students. University students may be more educated than the general population, particularly regarding the dangers of smartphone use while driving. They may also feel more accountable to their parents, given it is reasonable to assume that a larger proportion of students live with their parents compared to young people who are in the workforce and living independently. This accountability may have influenced the effectiveness of Message 4 (‘Good Driver’) in particular. In this message, the young driver wanted their father to know that they were not using their smartphone while driving, thereby being the good driver they said they were. The study sample, therefore, may limit the generalisability of the findings. In conclusion, the current study adds to the extant literature by highlighting the importance of rigorously concept testing new message content with the target audience. Specifically, this study tested nine road safety public education messages aimed at young drivers monitoring/reading and responding to social interactive technology on their smartphones. The integrated findings from the quantitative and qualitative components of this study supported current research suggesting that road safety messages that model positive behaviour and elicit positive emotions may be especially persuasive for young drivers. References AAMI. (2012). Young driver index 2012. Retrieved from www.aami.com.au/sites/default/files/fm/news/Young%20Driver%20Index%22012_12_14.pdf. AAMI. (2015). AAMI targets social stigma to tackle distracted driving. Retrieved from https://www.aami.com.au/media-centre/aami-targets-social-stigmatackle-distracted-driving.html. Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 179–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978 (91)90020-T. Atchley, P., Atwood, S., & Boulton, A. (2011). The choice to text and drive in younger drivers: Behavior may shape attitude. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 43(1), 134–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2010.08.003. Authors (2016). Young drivers’ engagement with social interactive technology on their smartphones: Critical beliefs to target in public education messages. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 96, 208–218. Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2015). Australian demographic statistics, December, 2014. Retrieved from http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/mf/ 3101.0. Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Buckley, L., Chapman, R., & Sheehan, M. (2014). Young driver distraction: State of the evidence and directions for behavior change programs. Journal of Adolescent Health, 54(5), S16–S21. Creswell, J., Klassen, A., Plano Clark, V., & Smith, K. (2011). Best practices for mixed methods research in the health sciences. August 2011. National Institutes of Health.: Retrieved from https://obssr.od.nih.gov/training/mixed-methods-research/. Department of Infrastructure and Regional Development. (2014). Road trauma Australia 2014 Statistical Summary. Retrieved from http://bitre.gov.au/ publications/ongoing/files/Road_trauma_Australia_2014_statistical_summary_N_ISSN.pdf. Dillard, J. P., Plotnick, C. A., Godbold, L. C., Freimuth, V. S., & Edgar, T. (1996). The multiple affective outcomes of aids psas: Fear appeals do more than scare people. Communication Research, 23(1), 44–72. https://doi.org/10.1177/009365096023001002. 2 Please note that is not the intention of this study that all three messages would be run as part of a single campaign against smartphone use while driving. 326 C.S. Gauld et al. / Transportation Research Part F 60 (2019) 311–326 Dillard, J. P., Shen, L., & Vail, R. G. (2007). Does perceived message effectiveness cause persuasion or vice versa? 17 consistent answers. Human Communication Research, 33(4), 467–488. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2007.00308.x. Elliott, B. (1993). Road safety mass media campaigns: A meta analysis (9780642512529;0642512523;). Retrieved from Canberra: Elliott, M., & Armitage, C. (2009). Promoting drivers’ compliance with speed limits: Testing an intervention based on the theory of planned behaviour. British Journal of Psychology, 100(1), 111–132. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712608X318626. Eveland, W. P., Jr., & McLeod, D. M. (1999). The effect of social desirability on perceived media impact: Implications for third-person perceptions. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 11(4), 315–333. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijpor/11.4.315. Gauld, C., Lewis, I., & White, K. M. (2014). Concealing their communication: Exploring psychosocial predictors of young drivers’ intentions and engagement in concealed texting. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 62, 285–293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2013.10.016. Gauld, C., Lewis, I., White, K., & Watson, B. (2016). Key Beliefs Influencing Young Drivers’ Engagement with Social Interactive Technology on Their Smartphones: A Qualitative Study. Traffic Injury Prevention, 17(2), 128. Gauld, C., Lewis, I., White, K. M., Fleiter, J., & Watson, B. (2017). Smartphone use while driving: What factors predict young drivers’ intentions to initiate, read, and respond to social interactive technology? Computers in Human Behavior, 76, 174–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.07.023. Glendon, A. I., & Cernecca, L. (2003). Young drivers’ responses to anti-speeding and anti-drink-driving messages. Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour, 6(3), 197–216. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1369-8478(03)00026-3. Goodwin, A. H., O’Brien, N. P., & Foss, R. D. (2012). Effect of North Carolina’s restriction on teenage driver cell phone use two years after implementation. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 48, 363–367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2012.02.006. Gershon, P., Zhu, C., Klauer, S., Dingus, T., & Simons-Morton, B. (2017). Teen’s distracted driving behaviour: Prevalence and predictors. Journal of Safety Research, 63, 157. Hoekstra, T., & Wegman, F. (2011). Improving the effectiveness of road safety campaigns: Current and new practices. IATSS Research, 34(2), 80–86. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.iatssr.2011.01.003. Howitt, D., & Cramer, D. (2014). Introduction to research methods in psychology (vol. 4) New York: Pearson. Jessop, G. (2008). Who’s on the line? Policing and enforcing laws relating to mobile phone use while driving. International Journal of Law, Crime and Justice, 36 (3), 135–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlcj.2008.03.001. LaTour, M. S., & Rotfeld, H. J. (1997). There are threats and (maybe) fear-caused arousal: Theory and confusions of appeals to fear and fear arousal itself. Journal of Advertising, 26(3), 45–59. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.1997.10673528. Lewis, I., Ho, B., & Lennon, A. (2016). Designing and evaluating a persuasive child restraint television commercial. Traffic Injury Prevention, 17(3), 271–277. https://doi.org/10.1080/15389588.2015.1072626. Lewis, I., Watson, B., & Tay, R. (2007). Examining the effectiveness of physical threats in road safety advertising: The role of the third-person effect, gender, and age. Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour, 10(1), 48–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2006.05.001. Lewis, I., Watson, B., & White, K. M. (2008). An examination of message-relevant affect in road safety messages: Should road safety advertisements aim to make us feel good or bad? Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour, 11(6), 403–417. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2008.03.003. Lewis, I., Watson, B., & White, K. M. (2009). What do we really know about designing and evaluating road safety advertising? : Current knowledge and future challenges. Paper presented at the 2009 Australasian Road Safety Research Policing and Education Conference: Smarter, Safer Directions, Sydney. Lewis, I., Watson, B., & White, K. M. (2010). Response efficacy: The key to minimizing rejection and maximizing acceptance of emotion-based anti-speeding messages. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 42(2), 459–467. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2009.09.008. Lewis, I., Watson, B., & White, K. M. (2013). Extending the explanatory utility of the EPPM beyond fear-based persuasion. Health Communication, 28(1), 84–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2013.743430. Lewis, I., Watson, B., & White, K. M. (2016). The Step approach to Message Design and Testing (SatMDT): A conceptual framework to guide the development and evaluation of persuasive health messages. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 97, 309–314. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2015.07.019. Lewis, I., White, K. M., Watson, B., & Elliott, B. (2017) Insights into evaluating road safety advertising based upon the Step Approach to Message Design and Testing (SatMDT). In 2017 Australasian Road Safety Conference, 10-12 October 2017, Perth, W.A. Lewis, I., Watson, B., White, K. M., Elliott, B., Thompson, J., & Cockfield, S. (2012). How males and females define speeding and how they’d feel getting caught for it: Some implications for anti-speeding message development. Paper presented at the Australasian Road Safety Research, Policing and Education Conference, Wellington, New Zealand. Lewis, I., Watson, B., White, K. M., & Tay, R. (2007). Promoting public health messages: Should we move beyond fear-evoking appeals in road safety? Qualitative Health Research, 17(1), 61–74. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732306296395. McCartt, A. T., Hellinga, L. A., Strouse, L. M., & Farmer, C. M. (2010). Long-term effects of handheld cell phone laws on driver handheld cell phone use. Traffic Injury Prevention, 11(2), 133–141. https://doi.org/10.1080/15389580903515427. Morley, D. D. (1987). Subjective message constructs: A theory of persuasion. Communication Monographs, 54(2), 183–203. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 03637758709390225. Morley, D. D., & Walker, K. B. (1987). The role of importance, novelty, and plausibility in producing belief change. Communication Monographs, 54(4), 436–442. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637758709390243. Petty, R., & Cacioppo, J. (1986). The Elaboration Likelihood Model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.). Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123–205). New York: Academic. Phillips, R. O., Ulleberg, P., & Vaa, T. (2011). Meta-analysis of the effect of road safety campaigns on accidents. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 43(3), 1204–1218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2011.01.002. Plant, B., Reza, F., & Irwin, J. (2011). A systematic review of how anti-speeding advertisements are evaluated. Journal of the Australasian College of Road Safety, 22(4), 18–33. Rainie, L. (2012). Samrtphone ownership update: September 2012. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/2012/09/11/smartphone-ownershipupdate-september-2012/. Shi, J., Xiao, Y., & Atchley, P. (2016). Analysis of factors affecting drivers’ choice to engage with a mobile phone while driving in Beijing. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 37, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2015.12.003. Stothart, C., Mitchum, A., & Yehnert (2015). The attentional cost of receiving a cell phone notification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance.. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000100. Tay, R., & Watson, B. (2002). Changing Drivers’ Intentions and behaviours using fear-based driver fatigue advertisements. Health Marketing Quarterly, 19(4), 55–68. https://doi.org/10.1300/J026v19n04_05. Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2007). Contextual contrast and perceived knowledge: Exploring the implications for persuasion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(1), 17–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2005.11.007. Trivedi, N., Haynie, D., Bible, J., Liu, D., & Simons-Morton, B. (2017). Cell phone use while driving: Prospective association with emerging adult use. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 106, 450–455. Waddell, L. P., & Wiener, K. K. K. (2014). What’s driving illegal mobile phone use? Psychosocial influences on drivers’ intentions to use hand-held mobile phones. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour, 22, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2013.10.008. Watson, B. (1996). Enhancing driver management in Queensland. Retrieved from Fortitude Valley, Qld. WHO. (2015). Global state report on road safety. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/violence_injury_prevention/road_safety_status/2015/en/. Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals – The extended parallel process model. Communication Monographs, 59(4), 329–349. https://doi.org/ 10.1080/03637759209376276.