See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/51631084 Explaining the Justice-Performance Relationship: Trust as Exchange Deepener or Trust as Uncertainty Reducer? Article in Journal of Applied Psychology · September 2011 DOI: 10.1037/a0025208 · Source: PubMed CITATIONS READS 466 5,165 5 authors, including: Jason A Colquitt Ronald F Piccolo University of Georgia University of Central Florida 47 PUBLICATIONS 18,954 CITATIONS 55 PUBLICATIONS 12,191 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Bruce Louis Rich University of Florida 10 PUBLICATIONS 8,470 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: ob Engagement: Antecedents and Effects on Job Performance View project Goal setting chapter View project All content following this page was uploaded by Ronald F Piccolo on 23 May 2014. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. SEE PROFILE Journal of Applied Psychology 2012, Vol. 97, No. 1, 1–15 © 2011 American Psychological Association 0021-9010/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0025208 Explaining the Justice–Performance Relationship: Trust as Exchange Deepener or Trust as Uncertainty Reducer? Jason A. Colquitt Jeffery A. LePine University of Georgia Arizona State University Ronald F. Piccolo Cindy P. Zapata Rollins College Georgia Institute of Technology Bruce L. Rich California State University San Marcos Past research has revealed significant relationships between organizational justice dimensions and job performance, and trust is thought to be one mediator of those relationships. However, trust has been positioned in justice theorizing in 2 different ways, either as an indicator of the depth of an exchange relationship or as a variable that reflects levels of work-related uncertainty. Moreover, trust scholars distinguish between multiple forms of trust, including affect- and cognition-based trust, and it remains unclear which form is most relevant to justice effects. To explore these issues, we built and tested a more comprehensive model of trust mediation in which procedural, interpersonal, and distributive justice predicted affect- and cognition-based trust, with those trust forms predicting both exchange- and uncertainty-based mechanisms. The results of a field study in a hospital system revealed that the trust variables did indeed mediate the relationships between the organizational justice dimensions and job performance, with affect-based trust driving exchange-based mediation and cognition-based trust driving uncertainty-based mediation. Keywords: justice, trust, social exchange, uncertainty, performance 1975). Still other studies have focused on interpersonal justice, which reflects the perceived fairness of the interpersonal treatment received as procedures are enacted (Bies & Moag, 1986; Greenberg, 1993). What explains the significant connection between justice and job performance? One potential mediator that has been examined in past research is trust, defined as confident, positive expectations about the words, actions, and decisions of another in situations entailing risk (Lewicki & Bunker, 1995; McAllister, 1995). The notion that trust serves as a mediator of justice effects is partially responsible for an increased integration of the justice and trust literatures in recent years. A number of studies have included both justice and trust in their investigations (e.g., Ambrose & Schminke, 2003; Aryee, Budhwar, & Chen, 2002; Begley, Lee, & Hui, 2006; Cropanzano, Prehar, & Chen, 2002; Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Frazier, Johnson, Gavin, Gooty, & Snow, 2010; Kernan & Hanges, 2002; Konovsky & Pugh, 1994; Pillai, Schriesheim, & Williams, 1999; Stinglhamber, De Cremer, & Mercken, 2006). Moreover, meta-analytic reviews have revealed moderately positive correlations among justice and trust concepts (Cohen-Charash & Spector, 2001; Colquitt, Conlon, Wesson, Porter, & Ng, 2001; Dirks & Ferrin, 2002). Despite that empirical integration, there remain many questions about the justice–trust connection (Colquitt & Mueller, 2008; Lewicki, Wiethoff, & Tomlinson, 2005), two of which are relevant to understanding the justice–job performance relationship. First, Research in the justice literature has linked fair treatment to a number of beneficial employee attitudes and behaviors (for a historical review, see Colquitt, Greenberg, & Zapata-Phelan, 2005). For example, research has linked justice perceptions to all three facets of job performance, including task performance, citizenship behavior, and counterproductive behavior (e.g., Masterson, Lewis, Goldman, & Taylor, 2000; Moorman, Blakely, & Niehoff, 1998; Rupp & Cropanzano, 2002; Tekleab, Takeuchi, & Taylor, 2005; Wayne, Shore, Bommer, & Tetrick, 2002). Some of those studies have focused on distributive justice, which reflects the perceived fairness of decision outcomes (Adams, 1965; Homans, 1961; Leventhal, 1976). Other studies have focused on procedural justice, which reflects the perceived fairness of decision-making processes (Leventhal, 1980; Thibaut & Walker, This article was published Online First September 12, 2011. Jason A. Colquitt, Department of Management, University of Georgia; Jeffery A. LePine, Department of Management, Arizona State University; Ronald F. Piccolo, Department of Management, Rollins College; Cindy P. Zapata, Department of Organizational Behavior, Georgia Institute of Technology; Bruce L. Rich, Department of Management, California State University San Marcos. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jason A. Colquitt, University of Georgia, Department of Management, Terry College of Business, 412 Brooks Hall, Athens, GA 30602-6256. E-mail: colq@uga.edu 1 2 COLQUITT ET AL. the functional relevance of trust has varied in much of the theorizing utilized in justice research. Social exchange theory argues that trust is vital to the development and deepening of social exchange relationships (i.e., relationships where unspecified favors are exchanged over an indefinite time horizon) because it reduces uncertainty about a partner’s reciprocation while fostering a sense of obligation (Blau, 1964). When trust is present, exchange partners are more likely to discharge their obligations, in part because they believe that such efforts will be reciprocated down the line. Trust functions in a similar manner in fairness heuristic theory, which argues that trust guides decisions about whether to cooperate with authorities when there is uncertainty about potential exploitation (Lind, 2001; van den Bos, 2001a; van den Bos, Lind, & Wilke, 2001). However, the functional relevance of trust is decidedly different in uncertainty management theory—a direct successor to fairness heuristic theory (Lind & van den Bos, 2002; van den Bos & Lind, 2002). That theory argues that high levels of justice can mitigate or reduce the effects of virtually any form of uncertainty, including (but not limited to) concerns about trust and reciprocation. From this perspective, the sense that an authority is fair and trusted may engender a reciprocation of obligations, but it may also instill a sense of comfort that mitigates uncertainty in a much more general sense. Second, justice scholars have neglected to incorporate some of the distinctions drawn by trust scholars between different types of trust. As Lewicki et al. (2005) noted, “No assessments of the justice–trust link in the justice literature provide a fine-grained understanding or analysis of the rich trust literature. None explore the emerging research suggesting multiple forms of trust (Lewicki & Bunker, 1995, 1996; McAllister, 1995)” (p. 254). McAllister (1995) distinguished between affect-based trust, which reflects a confidence rooted in emotional investments, expressions of genuine care and concern, and an understanding of reciprocated sentiments, and cognition-based trust, which reflects a confidence rooted in someone’s track record and reputation for dependability, reliability, and professionalism. Lewicki and Bunker (1995, 1996) drew a similar distinction in their discussion of identificationbased trust and knowledge-based trust. Because justice scholars have treated trust as a unidimensional construct in their studies, it remains unclear which form of trust is related to justice and which form of trust mediates relationships with job performance. With these two questions in mind, we conducted this study to gain a more fine-grained understanding of the role of trust in the justice–job performance relationship. In particular, we built and tested a comprehensive model of trust mediation in which procedural, interpersonal, and distributive justice predicted both affectand cognition-based trust, with those trust forms fulfilling two different mediating functions.1 One function was that of “exchange deepener,” where trust fosters a deeper sense of obligation within an exchange relationship. We operationalized that function using normative commitment, which reflects a sense of obligation to one’s employer (Meyer & Allen, 1997). The other function was that of “uncertainty reducer,” where trust serves to mitigate a general sense of work uncertainty. We operationalized that function using perceived uncertainty, which reflects the general sense that an employee lacks the information or situational understanding needed to accurately predict the future (van den Bos & Lind, 2002). Our model of trust mediation is shown in Figure 1. The sections to follow describe trust concepts in more detail before offering hypotheses about the specific linkages in our model. Justice and Affect- vs. Cognition-Based Trust As noted above, an increasing number of studies have integrated justice and trust concepts, often in an attempt to explain relationships between justice and various performance behaviors (e.g., Ambrose & Schminke, 2003; Aryee et al., 2002; Begley et al., 2006; Cropanzano et al., 2002; Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Frazier et al., 2010; Kernan & Hanges, 2002; Konovsky & Pugh, 1994; Pillai et al., 1999; Stinglhamber et al., 2006). Given that trust scholars have criticized some of this work for its casual or simplistic approach to conceptualizing trust (Lewicki et al., 2005), some more extensive definitional discussion is warranted. There are at least three approaches to defining and conceptualizing trust in the extant literature: (a) trust as unidimensional confident, positive expectations; (b) trust as multidimensional confident, positive expectations; and (c) trust as the willingness to be vulnerable (see Rousseau, Sitkin, Burt, & Camerer, 1998, for more discussion of such definitional distinctions). Trust as unidimensional confident, positive expectations is a perspective found in many of the seminal trust measures in the organizational sciences. Roberts and O’Reilly (1974) operationalized trust by asking employees how they would expect their supervisor to act in various situations, such as when they needed to discuss work problems or when the supervisor needed to make a decision that was contrary to employee interests. Gabarro and Athos (1976) assessed trust by asking employees whether they expected their supervisors to have good motives and intentions, to be open and upfront, and to act in an honest and predictable manner. Cook and Wall (1980) measured trust by asking employees whether their supervisor was efficient, made sensible decisions, and made concerted attempts to protect employee interests in an honest manner. Variations of these measures have been utilized by a number of justice scholars (e.g., Aryee et al., 2002; Begley et al., 2006; Cropanzano et al., 2002; Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Kernan & Hanges, 2002; Konovsky & Pugh, 1994), making the unidimensional confident, positive expectations perspective the default framework in studies of the justice–trust connection. Trust as multidimensional confident, positive expectations is a perspective that gained prominence in the mid-1990s. Drawing in part on earlier theorizing by Lewis and Weigert (1985), McAllister (1995) suggested that conceptualizations of trust were actually 1 Our hypothesized model casts the justice dimensions as antecedents of affect- and cognition-based trust. Although this causal ordering matches the conventional wisdom in the literature, it does—at first glance—seem inconsistent with Tyler and Lind’s work on the group-value and relational models (Lind & Tyler, 1988; Tyler & Lind, 1992). Those models include a concept—sometimes labeled “trust” (Tyler, 1989), sometimes labeled “trustworthiness” (Tyler, 1994), and sometimes labeled “trust in benevolence” (Lind, Tyler, & Huo, 1997)—that is viewed as an antecedent of fairness perceptions. However, a close inspection of the definition and operationalization of that construct shows that it has little in common with the trust variables in our study, despite its labeling. Instead, the construct tends to assess efforts to be fair (Lind et al., 1997; Tyler, 1989, 1994) and the provision of voice (Lind et al., 1997; Tyler, 1994). Such operationalizations serve as indicators of procedural justice rather than trust. JUSTICE, TRUST, AND PERFORMANCE 3 Figure 1. Affect- and cognition-based trust as mediators of justice–performance effects. tapping two distinct concepts, with affect-based trust reflecting expectations of reciprocal care and concern and cognition-based trust reflecting expectations of reliability and dependability. Drawing on earlier theorizing by Shapiro, Sheppard, and Cheraskin (1992), Lewicki and Bunker (1995, 1996) drew a similar distinction, with identification-based trust reflecting expectations of a reciprocal understanding of wants and knowledge-based trust reflecting expectations of predictability. Given that there remain no published measures of Lewicki and Bunker’s trust types, McAllister’s trust scales have become the most common operationalization of the multidimensional version of trust. These scales have been used in two studies integrating justice and trust. Unfortunately, one study collapsed across affect- and cognition-based trust (Ambrose & Schminke, 2003) and the other omitted affect-based trust altogether (Stinglhamber et al., 2006). Trust as the willingness to be vulnerable is a perspective rooted in Mayer, Davis, and Schoorman’s (1995) integrative model of trust. Mayer et al. defined trust as the willingness of a trustor to be vulnerable to a trustee based on positive expectations about the trustee’s actions. Those positive expectations, which were used to capture trust in the perspectives described above, were instead used to capture three components of “trustworthiness”: (a) benevolence, which captures expectations about caring and supportive motives; (b) ability, which captures expectations about competence and skills; and (c) integrity, which captures expectations about a consistent adherence to sound principles. Although the Mayer et al. and McAllister (1995) perspectives differ in the way they define the trust concept itself, it is important to acknowledge that benevolence has much in common with affect-based trust, whereas ability and integrity have much in common with cognition-based trust. Our study utilizes McAllister’s conceptualization because the confident, positive expectations version of trust has been used more frequently by justice scholars (see Colquitt and Mueller, 2008, for a conceptual discussion of justice and Mayer et al.’s trust conceptualization). However, all of our affect-based trust predictions could also be made for benevolence, and all of our cognition-based trust predictions could also be made for ability and integrity. Predictions for Justice and Trust Although the various justice dimensions are fostered by the adherence to distinct sets of justice rules, there are conceptual reasons to expect all of the dimensions to be positively related to both affect- and cognition-based trust, albeit for different reasons. Procedural justice is fostered when authorities provide employees with input into key decisions and when authorities utilize procedures that are consistent, accurate, unbiased, correctable, representative of group concerns, and ethical (Leventhal, 1980; Thibaut & Walker, 1975). Many of those procedural rules are likely to be especially relevant to cognition-based trust, as an emphasis on consistency and accuracy should create the kind of predictable and dependable track record that underlies that trust form. Procedurally just decision making also instills a sense of legitimacy to organizational authorities (Tyler & Lind, 1992), which should help promote a sense of professionalism and regard. With respect to affect-based trust, an emphasis on allowing employee input and learning about group concerns should foster the sort of mutual investment and sharing of information that underlies that trust form. Procedurally just decision making also fulfills key psychological needs on the part of employees, injecting a sense of belonging into organizational relationships (Cropanzano, Byrne, Bobocel, & Rupp, 2001). Interpersonal justice is fostered when authorities treat employees with dignity and respect and refrain from improper remarks or comments during the enactment of procedures (Bies & Moag, 1986; Greenberg, 1993). These interpersonal rules are likely to be particularly relevant to affect-based trust, as it would be difficult 4 COLQUITT ET AL. for a sense of reciprocal care and concern to develop in a context where communications were rude, disrespectful, or inappropriate. Indeed, such actions are associated with a number of negative emotions, including anger and hostility (Barclay, Skarlicki, & Pugh, 2005; Rupp & Spencer, 2006). The relevance of interpersonal justice rules is perhaps less immediately apparent to cognition-based trust. However, it is important to note that many organizations have climates that emphasize (or at least espouse to emphasize) social relations, supportiveness, morale, and warmth (Carr, Schmidt, Ford, & DeShon, 2003). From this perspective, violations of interpersonal justice rules would be indicative of a lack of professionalism— one component of cognition-based trust. Distributive justice is fostered when authorities utilize appropriate allocation norms when doling out key outcomes (Leventhal, 1976). In most business contexts, the appropriate norm is equity, where outcomes are allocated in accordance with relevant inputs (Adams, 1965; Leventhal, 1976). Distributive justice is likely to be relevant to affect-based trust because equity has important affective consequences. Adams suggested that inequitable allocations will trigger a sense of equity distress, with anger resulting from underpayment inequity and guilt resulting from overpayment inequity. Those negative emotions are associated with action tendencies such as retaliation or withdrawal (Lazarus & CohenCharash, 2001), both of which could undermine the development of reciprocal sentiments and investments. Distributive justice should also be relevant to cognition-based trust. Making decisions about performance evaluations, salary increases, bonuses, job assignments, and informal spot rewards represents a key job duty for many organizational authorities. As such, failing to make such decisions in an equitable fashion could lead to doubts about an authority’s competence, dedication, reliability, and professionalism. Ambrose and Schminke’s (2003) field study of employees in several different industries provides some empirical support for the predictions laid out above, as procedural and interpersonal justice had significant unique effects on an index combining affect- and cognition-based trust (though distributive justice lacked any significant effect). In addition, Stinglhamber et al.’s (2006) field study of telecommunications employees revealed a significant linkage between interpersonal justice and cognition-based trust (though the effects of procedural justice were not modeled and data were not gathered on distributive justice or affect-based trust). Aside from these two studies, much of the research linking justice and trust concepts has utilized the unidimensional confident, positive expectations perspective and has included only one or two justice dimensions (Begley et al., 2006; Folger & Konovsky, 1989; Konovsky & Pugh, 1994; Pillai et al., 1999). One exception to that latter observation is Aryee et al.’s (2002) field study of employees of an Indian coal company, which showed that procedural, interpersonal, and distributive justice all had significant unique effects on a unidimensional form of trust. We therefore hypothesized Hypothesis 1: Procedural justice is positively related to affect- and cognition-based trust. Hypothesis 2: Interpersonal justice is positively related to affect- and cognition-based trust. Hypothesis 3: Distributive justice is positively related to affect- and cognition-based trust. Trust as Exchange Deepener or Trust as Uncertainty Reducer? Even if the relationships between justice and affect- and cognition-based trust can be used to explain effects on job performance, a key question remains: What exactly is it about trust that explains those effects? As noted above, the mediating role of trust could be explained by two distinct functional mechanisms: trust as an exchange deepener and trust as an uncertainty reducer. Trust as an Exchange Deepener In his discussion of exchange relationships, Blau (1964) described the unique character of social exchanges: Social exchange, whether it is in this ceremonial form or not, involves favors that create diffuse future obligations, not precisely specified ones, and the nature of the return cannot be bargained about but must be left to the discretion of the one who makes it . . . . Since there is no way to assure an appropriate return for a favor, social exchange requires trusting others to discharge their obligations. (pp. 93–94) Blau’s (1964) description of social exchanges gained more visibility in the justice literature when Organ (1990) used it to describe why procedural justice might be associated with increased levels of citizenship behavior. Organ suggested that fair treatment would be viewed as a favor on the part of a supervisor that deserved reciprocation by the employee. That citizenship, in turn, would constitute a favor on the part of the employee that deserved reciprocation by the supervisor. The reciprocation-inducing effects of fair treatment can be described from two perspectives. First, high levels of the various justice dimensions may be the exception rather than the rule in some organizations, giving fairness a discretionary feel that warrants reciprocation. Alternatively, injustice may short-circuit the natural or default levels of reciprocation in many work units. Konovsky and Pugh (1994) operationalized Organ’s (1990) assertions in one of the first studies that integrated justice and trust. They noted that trust provides the basis for social exchange relationships and serves as the key element in the emergence and deepening of such relationships. Trust is particularly vital during periods where one party’s contributions outweigh the other’s, as trust provides some assurance that obligations will balance out over the long term. Konovsky and Pugh suggested that trust would predict citizenship because an employee would be more willing to go the extra mile when the leader could be trusted to balance the scales down the road. Subsequent studies have continued to use trust as a means of operationalizing social exchange effects (Aryee et al., 2002; Cropanzano et al., 2002; Pillai et al., 1999; Stinglhamber et al., 2006), with trust emerging as a definitional component of such exchanges (Cropanzano & Mitchell, 2005; Shore, Tetrick, Lynch, & Barksdale, 2006). In their review of the literature, Dirks and Ferrin (2002) referred to such studies as the relationship-based perspective in the trust literature, given that the work focuses on how followers come to understand the nature of their relationship with their manager. JUSTICE, TRUST, AND PERFORMANCE Despite the relevance of trust to Blau’s (1964) theorizing, any study that shows trust to be a mediator of justice–performance effects can only assume that such mediation represents a social exchange mechanism. Barring an explicit operationalization of the exchange-deepening function that trust is purported to foster, that assumption remains unsubstantiated. The key question therefore becomes, which construct could best serve as an operationalization of that exchange dynamic? Blau’s description suggests that the obligation concept lies at the core of social exchange. Blau (1964) described social exchange reciprocations as “given and repaid under obligation” (p. 89), noting that it “is a necessary condition of exchange that individuals, in the interest of continuing to receive needed services, discharge their obligations for having received them in the past” (p. 92). One construct that reflects such obligation is normative commitment, which Meyer and Herscovitch (2001) described as “the mind-set of obligation” (p. 317). Justice scholars have suggested that normative commitment could serve as a construct-valid representation of the exchange dynamic (Colquitt, Greenberg, & Scott, 2005). We are not aware of any research linking trust and normative commitment, as Meyer and colleagues have typically focused on socialization experiences or organizational investments as antecedents of that commitment form (Meyer & Allen, 1997; Meyer, Stanley, Herscovitch, & Topolnytsky, 2002). However, Blau’s (1964) theorizing includes a number of ideas that support significant relationships between affect- and cognition-based trust and the sense of obligation represented by normative commitment. With respect to affect-based trust, Blau described how the steady expansion of mutual investments in a relationship can engender the sort of trust that encourages obligation. With respect to cognitionbased trust, an authority that provides services such as advice, aid, or assistance signals that he or she is dependable and reliable, which makes the discharging of one’s obligations more customary. Indirect support for these assertions can be taken from Colquitt, Scott, and LePine’s (2007) suggestion that an authority’s benevolence and ability can serve as exchange currencies that foster a motivation to reciprocate. In a meta-analytic test, Colquitt et al. showed that benevolence and ability predicted social exchange when operationalized as affective commitment, noting that too few studies focused on obligation-based forms of commitment to include them in their review. We therefore hypothesized Hypothesis 4: Affect- and cognition-based trust are positively related to normative commitment. Meyer and Herscovitch’s (2001) model of commitment can be used to ground predictions regarding normative commitment and job performance (see also Meyer & Allen, 1991). Meyer and Herscovitch suggested that the “mind-set of obligation” binds one to courses of action that benefit the entity to which one is committed. In particular, they argued that the sense of obligation would increase efforts to fulfill the focal behaviors of one’s work role. The sense of obligation can also increase discretionary behaviors, especially when those behaviors benefit the target of the obligation. Empirically speaking, studies of normative commitment have lagged behind studies of affective commitment. Meta-analyses of the few studies that have been conducted have illustrated that normative commitment has a moderately strong positive relationship with citizenship aspects of job performance and weaker 5 relationships with task performance aspects (Meyer et al., 2002). On the basis of Meyer and Herscovitch’s (2001) theorizing, we predicted Hypothesis 5: Normative commitment is positively related to job performance. Trust as an Uncertainty Reducer Whereas trust is somewhat of a behind-the-scenes player in Blau’s (1964) discussion of social exchange theory, it is clearly front and center in discussions of fairness heuristic theory (Lind, 2001; van den Bos, 2001a; van den Bos et al., 2001). Fairness heuristic theory’s propositions are rooted in the “fundamental social dilemma” faced by all employees working for authority figures in organizations (Lind, 2001). This dilemma acknowledges that an employee’s outcomes are ultimately maximized by cooperating with authorities and fully participating in organizational activities. However, choosing to do so also opens the door to potential exploitation, as authorities may abuse their power by asking employees to perform assignments that will never be rewarded. This fundamental social dilemma therefore creates a sense of uncertainty that pervades working life, particularly early on in one’s tenure and during times of organizational change (Lind, 2001). Fairness heuristic theory suggests that employees should, ideally, cope with this uncertainty by gauging their trust in authorities (Lind, 2001; van den Bos, 2001a; van den Bos et al., 2001). That theoretical premise is reminiscent of Luhmann’s (1979) conceptualization of trust as a reducer of complexity. Luhmann noted that individuals face an ever increasing level of complexity when navigating the social world, with complexity defined as an overload of possible events and threats to stability. Trust reduces that complexity by minimizing certain dangers (e.g., exploitation, breach) while highlighting possibilities for action that would otherwise have been impractical. Also along the lines of Luhmann’s theorizing, fairness heuristic theory acknowledges that trust must be learned. That learning process is difficult, however, because the building blocks of trust may be ambiguous or unobservable. In response to that difficulty, fairness heuristic theory argues that employees evaluate the fairness of the authority’s actions, using justice as a heuristic for guiding decisions about trust and cooperation. In practice, those justice evaluations may be grounded in procedural, distributive, or interpersonal justice, depending on which form of justice is first encountered in an organizational relationship and which is most easily interpreted (Lind, 2001; van den Bos, 2001a; van den Bos et al., 2001). Regardless, justice is used as a means of gauging trust, with trust used to help cope with the uncertainty regarding authority actions. Uncertainty management theory, cast as the successor to fairness heuristic theory, broadened the importance of justice, noting that it could be used to help manage uncertainty in a much more general sense, whether rooted in authority dynamics, organizational events, or a pervading sense of anxiety or lack of control (Lind & van den Bos, 2002; van den Bos & Lind, 2002). In fact, previous research has demonstrated that fairness can help manage a wide range of uncertainty forms, including a general sense of being uncertain or not in control (van den Bos, 2001b) or even a sense of being uncertain about one’s own mortality or disruptions in one’s own personal life (van den COLQUITT ET AL. 6 Bos & Miedema, 2000). The connection between justice and general uncertainty is purported to exist despite any real association between the uncertainty experienced and the treatment received. According to Lind and van den Bos (2002), the connection between fairness and uncertainty “is so fundamental . . . that it occurs whether there is a logical link between the fair treatment and the source of the anxiety or not” (p. 193). Thus, in addition to authority uncertainty, justice and subsequent trust can help individuals manage more general work uncertainty. The theorizing described above does not draw a distinction between affect- and cognition-based trust, leaving open the question of whether one form is more relevant to uncertainty than the other. Are both trust forms uniquely relevant to an employee’s sense that his or her work life is predictable and foreseeable? A sense that an authority’s behavior is predictable lies at the core of cognition-based trust, as such predictability is fostered by a track record of dependability and reliability (Lewicki & Bunker, 1995, 1996; McAllister, 1995). When employees view authorities as dedicated and competent, they can feel more confident that key organizational decisions and events will be handled in an effective manner. The relevance of affect-based trust to uncertainty may be less apparent than the relevance of cognition-based trust. Still, the sense that an authority has made emotional investments of reciprocal care and concern into a relationship should signal that employee needs will be protected under conditions of risk, whatever these may prove to be (Lewicki & Bunker, 1995, 1996; McAllister, 1995). When employees sense that authorities legitimately care for them, that sense may provide comfort that mitigates even unrelated sources of anxiety or confusion. We therefore predicted Hypothesis 6: Affect- and cognition-based trust are negatively related to uncertainty. between justice dimensions and job performance. The exchange deepener mechanism travels through normative commitment, suggesting that justice fosters trust, which is important because trust instills a sense of obligation in exchange relationships (Blau, 1964). The uncertainty reducer mechanism travels through uncertainty perceptions, suggesting that justice fosters trust, which is important because trust reduces the sense of uncertainty that employees can feel in their organizational lives. Taken together, those linkages result in the following hypotheses: Hypothesis 8: The relationship between justice and job performance is mediated by trust (both affect and cognition based) and normative commitment. Hypothesis 9: The relationship between justice and job performance is mediated by trust (both affect and cognition based) and uncertainty. Method Sample Participants were 195 employees of a hospital network located in the southeastern United States. The sample was predominantly female (86%). One hundred participants were Caucasian, 79 were African American, six were Hispanic, and three were Asian American, and the remainder indicated multiple ethnicities. Organizational tenure averaged 6.93 years. The sample was diverse in terms of pay grade (13) and functional background (nursing, medical technology, physical plant, customer service, records, laboratory testing, and information systems). Procedure Finally, there are a number of conceptual reasons to expect perceptions of uncertainty to predict job performance. First, employees who experience uncertainty should have less cognitive energy available to dedicate to job performance. As described by trust scholars, the presence of uncertainty necessitates off-task allocations of time and attention. Mayer and Gavin (2005) noted that uncertainty can cause employees to restrict their behaviors, eschew potentially risky actions, and focus their attention on contingency plans rather than job success. Similarly, McAllister (1995) suggested that uncertainty creates a need for monitoring and defense behaviors as individuals attempt to protect their interests. In other words, uncertainty and worry should result in decreased focus of attention, which can detract from job performance. In addition, the literature on stress and strain provides arguments for a relationship between uncertainty and job performance. Models in that literature often frame uncertainty as a stressor that can place significant strain on an employee, eventually harming job performance (Sonnentag & Frese, 2003). We therefore predicted Hypothesis 7: Uncertainty is negatively related to job performance. Summary In sum, our model of trust mediation, shown in Figure 1, provides two different mechanisms whereby trust can explain the relationships Data were collected as part of a training evaluation conducted for the human resource management department of the hospital network. We used a longitudinal design with two data collections separated by 3-month intervals. The first data collection included the measures of justice, affect- and cognition-based trust, normative commitment, and uncertainty. Participants received $10 to complete the survey and return it in a self-addressed stamped envelope. The second data collection gathered information on job performance from archival data taken from company records. Missing data on one or more variables resulted in a final usable sample size of 156 employees. Measures The survey items used to measure the justice, trust, normative commitment, and uncertainty variables are shown in Table 1. Justice. The justice dimensions were measured with Colquitt’s (2001) scales, with all items using response anchors of 1 ⫽ To a Very Small Extent to 5 ⫽ To a Very Large Extent. The procedural justice scale asked participants to consider the procedures their supervisor uses to make decisions about evaluations, promotions, and rewards. Its coefficient alpha was .90. The interpersonal justice scale asked participants to consider how they were treated by their supervisor during the implementation of procedures. Its coefficient alpha was .95. Finally, the distributive justice scale asked participants to consider the outcomes they received JUSTICE, TRUST, AND PERFORMANCE Table 1 Survey Items and Factor Loadings Survey item Procedural justice 1. Are you able to express your views during those procedures? 2. Can you influence the decisions arrived at by those procedures? 3. Are those procedures applied consistently? 4. Are those procedures free of bias? 5. Are those procedures based on accurate information? 6. Are you able to appeal the decisions arrived at by those procedures? 7. Do those procedures uphold ethical and moral standards? Interpersonal justice 1. Has he/she treated you in a polite manner? 2. Has he/she treated you with dignity? 3. Has he/she treated you with respect? 4. Has he/she refrained from improper remarks or comments? Distributive justice 1. Do those outcomes reflect the effort that you have put into your work? 2. Are those outcomes appropriate for the work you have completed? 3. Do those outcomes reflect what you have contributed to your work? 4. Are those outcomes justified, given your performance? Affect-based trust 1. My supervisor and I freely share our ideas and feelings. 2. I can talk freely to my supervisor about difficulties I am having at work. 3. We would both feel a sense of loss if one of us was transferred. 4. My supervisor responds caringly when I share my problems. 5. My supervisor and I have invested a lot in our working relationship. Cognition-based trust 1. My supervisor approaches his/her job with dedication. 2. I see no reason to doubt my supervisor’s competence for the job. 3. I can rely on my supervisor not to make my job more difficult. 4. Most people trust and respect my supervisor as a coworker. 5. My peers consider my supervisor to be trustworthy. 6. If people knew more about my supervisor, they would be more concerned and monitor his/her performance more closely. (R) Normative commitment 1. I do not feel any obligation to remain with my current employer. (R) 2. Even if it were to my advantage, I do not feel it would be right to leave my organization now. 3. I would feel guilty if I left my organization now. 4. This organization deserves my loyalty. 5. I would not leave my organization right now because I have a sense of obligation to the people in it. 6. I owe a great deal to my organization. Uncertainty 1. There is a lot of uncertainty at work right now. 2. Many things seem unsettled at work currently. 3. If I think about work, I feel a lot of uncertainty. 4. I cannot predict how things will go at work. Note. n ⫽ 156. (R) ⫽ reverse-worded items. ⴱ p ⬍ .05. Loading .75ⴱ .73ⴱ .84ⴱ .80ⴱ .84ⴱ .74ⴱ .86ⴱ .96ⴱ .98ⴱ .98ⴱ .88ⴱ .92ⴱ .93ⴱ .98ⴱ .97ⴱ .93ⴱ .93ⴱ .75ⴱ .93ⴱ .88ⴱ .82ⴱ .93ⴱ .86ⴱ .91ⴱ .92ⴱ .39ⴱ .20ⴱ .57ⴱ .65ⴱ .70ⴱ .88ⴱ .75ⴱ .90ⴱ .95ⴱ .91ⴱ .76ⴱ 7 from their supervisor, including their evaluations, promotions, and rewards. The coefficient alpha for distributive justice was .96. One concern when utilizing self-report measures of the justice dimensions is whether such subjective appraisals truly capture differences in the objective qualities of treatment. For example, are procedural justice perceptions truly rooted in objective differences in the amount of voice, the accuracy of information, the availability of appeals, and so forth? Three different types of data suggest that self-report appraisals do pick up true variance in objective treatment. First, laboratory studies in the justice literature routinely yield strong relationships between objective manipulations of justice concepts and self-report manipulation checks (e.g., Brockner et al., 2007; De Cremer & Tyler, 2007; De Cremer, Van Knippenberg, Van Knippenberg, Mullenders, & Stinglhamber, 2005; Rupp & Spencer, 2006; Siegel, Post, Brockner, Fishman, & Garden, 2005). Second, laboratory versus field breakdowns in metaanalyses have shown that self-report justice measures yield outcome relationships that are similar to relationships with manipulated justice (Cohen-Charash & Spector, 2001). Finally, research on justice climate illustrates that multiple employees within work units hold similar appraisals of the way their supervisor or organization treats the unit (Dietz, Robinson, Folger, Baron, & Schulz, 2003; Johnson, Korsgaard, & Sapienza, 2002; Liao & Rupp, 2005; Naumann & Bennett, 2000, 2002; Sinclair, 2003). Trust. Affect- and cognition-based trust were measured with McAllister’s (1995) scales, with all items using response anchors of 1 ⫽ Strongly Disagree to 5 ⫽ Strongly Agree. The affect-based trust items were designed to capture the emotional investment, mutual concern, and deep sentiments that characterize that form of trust. These items had a coefficient alpha of .93. The cognitionbased trust items were designed to capture the sense of professionalism and dedication that characterize that form of trust. These items had a coefficient alpha of .88. Normative commitment. Normative commitment was measured with Meyer and Allen’s (1997) scale, which is a revised version of the original Allen and Meyer (1990) measure. All items used response anchors of 1 ⫽ Strongly Disagree to 5 ⫽ Strongly Agree. The items reflected a sense of obligation toward one’s employer and the sense that a debt is owed to the organization. The scale had a coefficient alpha of .75. Uncertainty. A measure created for this study was used to assess general perceptions of work uncertainty, in accordance with uncertainty management theory (Lind & van den Bos, 2002; van den Bos & Lind, 2002). The items incorporate the elements of uncertainty described by van den Bos and Lind (2002), Milliken (1987), and Sitkin and Pablo (1992), including the inability to predict or control future events, the existence of environmental volatility, and a sense of environmental complexity or heterogeneity. Note that the items were not referenced to any particular target (e.g., the supervisor), because the theory clearly states that its focal construct is very broad and general. The items used a scale of 1 ⫽ Strongly Disagree to 5 ⫽ Strongly Agree and had a coefficient alpha of .91. Job performance. Archival data were used to measure job performance. The organization’s contact person provided the overall job performance ratings for the participants during the year in which they completed the survey, once all surveys had been completed. These ratings were kept in the organization’s Peoplesoft software system and had a scale of 1 ⫽ Unacceptable, 2 ⫽ COLQUITT ET AL. 8 Needs Improvement, 3 ⫽ Effective, and 4 ⫽ Outstanding. On average, 100 days had elapsed between the participant’s completion of the justice scales and his or her supervisor’s completion of the performance rating in Peoplesoft. This supports some degree of temporal precedence for the justice–performance relationships that underlie our model. measurement repetition, no coefficient alpha was available for job performance. In a recent study of organizational justice and customer service attitudes, Ambrose, Hess, and Ganesan (2007) suggested that a .90 alpha be utilized for single-item measures that are factually based and a .70 alpha be utilized for single-item measures that represent subjective beliefs. We utilized a coefficient alpha of .80 for job performance because it was a compromise between those two levels and represented a more conservative choice than the .70 value. It should be noted, however, that the choice of alpha had little impact on our results. We allowed the exogenous justice dimensions to covary to represent unmeasured common causes, such as a higher order organizational justice factor. We also allowed the disturbance terms for affect- and cognition-based trust to covary, again to represent unmeasured common causes, such as a higher order trust factor. We also included direct effects from the justice dimensions to job performance, because such paths are needed to test our mediation predictions (MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002). The resulting model provided a good fit to the data: 2(9) ⫽ 20.44, p ⬍ .05, CFI ⫽ .99, SRMR ⫽ .05, RMSEA ⫽ .09. The unstandardized path coefficients from the default LISREL output are shown in Figure 2. Hypotheses 1–3 predicted that procedural, interpersonal, and distributive justice would be significantly related to affect- and cognition-based trust, respectively. As shown in Figure 2, all three hypotheses were supported. Procedural justice was a significant predictor of affect-based trust (b ⫽ .33) and cognition-based trust (b ⫽ .34). Interpersonal justice was also a significant predictor of affect-based trust (b ⫽ .26) and cognition-based trust (b ⫽ .32). Similarly, relationships with distributive justice were significant for affect-based trust (b ⫽ .29) and for cognition-based trust (b ⫽ .22). Taken together, these results suggest that the justice dimensions are relevant to both trust forms. Hypothesis 4 predicted that affect- and cognition-based trust would be positively related to normative commitment. This hypothesis was partially supported, as affect-based trust was a significant predictor of normative commitment (b ⫽ .51) but cognition-based trust was not. Hypothesis 5 predicted that normative commitment would be positively related to job performance. This hypothesis was supported, as normative commitment was a significant predictor of job performance (b ⫽ .38). Hypothesis 6 predicted that affect- and cognition-based trust would be negatively related to uncertainty. This hypothesis was partially sup- Confirmatory Factor Analysis We conducted a confirmatory factor analysis on our survey measures to verify their factor structure and to provide some evidence of construct validity. We conducted this analysis using LISREL 8.52 (Jöreskog, & Sörbom, 1996). Our default sevenfactor model provided a good fit to the data: 2(573) ⫽ 847.37, p ⬍ .001, comparative fit index (CFI) ⫽ .98, standardized root mean residual (SRMR) ⫽ .07, root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) ⫽ .05. All factor loadings were statistically significant and are shown in Table 1. Given the typically high correlations between the justice dimensions and between affectand cognition-based trust, we compared our default measurement model with models that collapsed across those dimensions. A model combining procedural, interpersonal, and distributive justice into a single justice factor provided a significantly worse fit to the data: 2(584) ⫽ 1,687.01, p ⬍ .001, ⌬2(11) ⫽ 839.64, p ⬍ .001. Similarly, a model combining affect- and cognition-based trust into a single trust factor provided a significantly worse fit to the data: 2(579) ⫽ 945.14, p ⬍ .001, ⌬2(6) ⫽ 97.77, p ⬍ .001. Our hypotheses were therefore tested following the structure in Table 1. Results Descriptive Statistics The means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations among the study variables are shown in Table 2. Coefficient alphas are reproduced on the diagonal. Tests of Hypotheses Given our sample size, we tested the model in Figure 1 using a “partially latent” approach in which scale scores were used as single indicators of the latent variables with error variances set to (1 ⫺ alpha) * variance (Kline, 2005). Given the absence of Table 2 Descriptive Statistics and Zero-Order Correlations Variable 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. Procedural justice Interpersonal justice Distributive justice Affect-based trust Cognition-based trust Normative commitment Uncertainty Job performance M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 3.32 4.17 3.24 3.45 3.59 3.06 3.02 3.31 0.91 0.98 1.19 1.09 1.00 0.76 1.06 0.48 (.90) .55ⴱ .50ⴱ .55ⴱ .55ⴱ .22ⴱ ⫺.28ⴱ .15ⴱ (.95) .37ⴱ .65ⴱ .67ⴱ .22ⴱ ⫺.38ⴱ .25ⴱ (.96) .49ⴱ .43ⴱ .33ⴱ ⫺.43ⴱ .27ⴱ (.93) .77ⴱ .31ⴱ ⫺.47ⴱ .28ⴱ (.88) .23ⴱ ⫺.55ⴱ .24ⴱ (.75) ⫺.19ⴱ .23ⴱ (.90) ⫺.17ⴱ (.80) Note. n ⫽ 156. Values along diagonal are coefficient alphas. SD ⫽ standard deviation. p ⬍ .05, two-tailed. ⴱ JUSTICE, TRUST, AND PERFORMANCE Figure 2. 9 Unstandardized path coefficients from structural equation modeling results. ported, as cognition-based trust was a significant predictor of uncertainty (b ⫽ ⫺.66) but affect-based trust was not. Hypothesis 7 predicted that uncertainty would be negatively related to job performance. This hypothesis was supported, as uncertainty was a significant predictor of job performance (b ⫽ ⫺.32). Hypothesis 8 predicted that the relationship between justice and job performance would be mediated by trust and normative commitment. Hypothesis 9 made the same prediction, substituting uncertainty in place of normative commitment. We tested mediation using the “product of coefficients” approach described by MacKinnon et al. (2002), in which mediation is demonstrated by a statistically significant product of independent variable 3 mediator and mediator 3 outcome relationships (i.e., a statistically significant indirect effect). There are a number of variants of the product of coefficients approach, with the Sobel (1982) test being the best known. We specifically utilized LISREL’s effect decomposition statistics to derive the indirect and total effects of the justice dimensions on job performance. One benefit of using LISREL’s statistics is that they can calculate the statistical significance of indirect effects with three stages (i.e., where justice leads to trust, trust leads to normative commitment and uncertainty and normative commitment and uncertainty lead to performance) and indirect effects that involve multiple mediators (in the case of normative commitment and uncertainty). The traditional “causal steps” approach described by Baron and Kenny (1986) does not provide a direct estimate of the indirect effect, nor does it test the effect’s statistical significance (MacKinnon et al., 2002). It is also not designed for models with three stages or with multiple mediators. Finally, the Baron and Kenny approach contains unnecessary steps that cause it to have lower statistical power than most product of coefficients approaches (MacKinnon et al., 2002). LISREL’s effect decomposition statistics are summarized in Table 3. As shown in the top panel, all three justice dimensions had significant indirect effects on job performance, with values of .11, .09, and .08 for procedural, interpersonal, and distributive justice, respectively. Mediation was only partial for procedural ⴱ p ⬍ .05. justice, as evidenced by the fact that it maintained a statistically significant direct effect on job performance when the mediators were also modeled (b ⫽ .25). For purposes of clarity, note that the indirect and direct effects sum to the total effects, with the total effects indicating what would occur if job performance was regressed onto the three justice dimensions. To tease apart the dual components of the trust mediation, we attempted to isolate the exchange- and uncertainty-based mediation mechanisms. In particular, we tested a model in which the uncertainty 3 job performance path was constrained to be 0, forcing the justice dimensions to exert their indirect effects only through normative commitment. This model provided a significantly worse fit to the data than the structure in Figure 2: 2(10) ⫽ 39.44, p ⬍ .001, ⌬2(1) ⫽ 19.00, p ⬍ .001. As shown in the middle panel of Table 3, the magnitude of the indirect effects was reduced by around 60%, with interpersonal justice no longer having a significant indirect effect on job performance. This pattern suggests that a considerable portion of trust’s mediating impact is due to effects on uncertainty. We also tested a model in which the normative commitment 3 job performance path was constrained to be 0, forcing the justice dimensions to exert their indirect effects only through uncertainty. This model also provided a significantly worse fit to the data than the structure in Figure 2: 2(10) ⫽ 49.47, p ⬍ .001, ⌬2(1) ⫽ 29.03, p ⬍ .001. As shown in the bottom panel of Table 3, the magnitude of the indirect effects was reduced by around 40%, though all three justice dimensions retained their significant indirect effects. This pattern suggests that a substantial portion of trust’s mediating impact is due to effects on normative commitment. Discussion One potential explanation for the continued popularity of justice as a topic of study is the relationship between the justice dimensions and job performance. However, the theoretical and practical value in such effects depends, to some extent, on understanding COLQUITT ET AL. 10 Table 3 Effect Decomposition Results for Mediation Tests Effects on job performance Justice dimension Indirect effect Direct effect Total effect Exchange and uncertainty-based mediation (the structure in Figure 2) Procedural justice Interpersonal justice Distributive justice .11ⴱ .09ⴱ .08ⴱ .25ⴱ .09ⴱ .11ⴱ .36ⴱ .18ⴱ .19ⴱ Exchange-based mediation only (uncertainty 3 performance constrained to 0) Procedural justice Interpersonal justice Distributive justice .05ⴱ .03ⴱ .04ⴱ .27ⴱ .15ⴱ .22ⴱ .32ⴱ .18ⴱ .26ⴱ Uncertainty-based mediation only (normative commitment 3 performance constrained to 0) Procedural justice Interpersonal justice Distributive justice .07ⴱ .07ⴱ .05ⴱ .27ⴱ .11ⴱ .21ⴱ .34ⴱ .18ⴱ .26ⴱ Note. n ⫽ 156. ⴱ p ⬍ .05. exactly why they exist. Scholars have suggested that trust can act as a mediator of justice-performance effects (e.g., Aryee et al., 2002; Konovsky & Pugh, 1994; Pillai et al., 1999), and social exchange theory has been offered as a theoretical explanation for trust mediation (Blau, 1964). However, the social exchange relevance of trust has only been assumed in such studies, and those studies fail to account for the other potential reason for trust’s importance: uncertainty reduction (Lind & van den Bos, 2002; van den Bos & Lind, 2002). With those issues in mind, our study tested a model that included both exchange-based and uncertainty-based mechanisms for trust mediation. In so doing, we also answered Lewicki et al.’s (2005) call for more research linking justice to multiple trust types by operationalizing both affect- and cognitionbased trust (McAllister, 1995). Our results showed that procedural, interpersonal, and distributive justice were each significantly related to affect- and cognitionbased trust. We reasoned that the consistency and accuracy aspects of procedural justice could instill the sort of legitimacy needed to foster cognition-based trust, whereas the voice and representativeness aspects could foster the sort of mutual communication and investment that underlies affect-based trust. In the case of interpersonal justice, we reasoned, dignified and proper communication could allow reciprocal care and concern to develop, encouraging affect-based trust, while signaling an adherence to professional norms and climates, encouraging cognition-based trust. Finally, distributively just outcomes prevent the anger and guilt that could undermine affect-based trust while highlighting the sort of effective role fulfillment that underlies cognition-based trust in organizational authorities. These results show that justice is relevant to trust for both “hot” and “cold” reasons. It can foster a sense of mutual emotional investment while also signaling that authorities are reliable and dependable. Aside from the specific linkages with affect- and cognitionbased trust, what stands out most from our results is that the exchange-deepening and uncertainty-reducing mechanisms both had significant mediating roles in the justice–performance relationship. When combined into the structure in Figure 2, they resulted in significant indirect effects for all three justice dimensions. Indeed, interpersonal and distributive justice failed to exert significant direct effects when the exchange and uncertainty-based trust effects were controlled. That the procedural justice direct effect remained significant represents an interesting avenue for future research. It may be that procedural effects are also transmitted through mechanisms from other theoretical perspectives. For example, Moorman and Byrne (2005) suggested that justice could impact performance behaviors though social-identity-based mechanisms, such as group identification or group pride. Regardless, our results showed that the exchange- and uncertainty-based mechanisms were both needed to fully capture the mediating effects of trust. Removing either mechanism from the structure in Figure 2 resulted in a significantly worse fitting model and less of a trust-based indirect effect of justice on performance. Looking more closely at our results for trust as an exchange deepener, it is important to note that affect-based trust had a significant relationship with normative commitment but cognitionbased trust did not. Drawing on Blau’s (1964) theorizing, we had reasoned that affect-based trust would reflect the sort of mutual investment that instills a mind-set of obligation among exchange partners, with cognition-based trust signaling an abundance of the instrumental services that Blau described. We suspect that the more significant effects of affect-based trust can be explained by its more reciprocal nature. McAllister’s conceptualization (1995) emphasizes a mutual sharing of ideas, a mutual sharing of workrelated difficulties, and a mutual sense of caring and investment. That sense of reciprocation is echoed in Blau’s discussion of the social exchange dynamic, which emphasizes a mutual sense of obligation, owing, debt, and loyalty. It is also worth noting that both McAllister and Lewicki and Bunker (1995, 1996) viewed affect-based trust as signaling a more mature and established relationship. Indeed, they described affect-based trust as building JUSTICE, TRUST, AND PERFORMANCE on a foundation of cognition-based trust in much the same way that Blau described social exchanges as supplanting economic exchanges. Regardless, these results suggest that much of the socialexchange-based mediation observed in past research may be due primarily to the affect-based aspect of trust. In contrast, cognition-based trust had a significant relationship with uncertainty but affect-based trust did not. We had suggested that the dependability and reliability inherent in cognition-based trust would give employees a sense of confidence when thinking about future circumstances, with affect-based trust signaling that employee needs would be protected as a show of care and concern. It may be that a dependable track record is simply more salient in uncertain situations than a sense of emotional investment. In indirect support of that premise, past research has suggested that trustee ability is a stronger driver of risk-taking decisions than is trustee benevolence (Colquitt et al., 2007). Alternatively, it may be that the effects of the two trust forms would differ if the uncertainty was made more specific. Lind and van den Bos described a number of different forms of uncertainty in their writings on uncertainty management theory (Lind & van den Bos, 2002; van den Bos & Lind, 2002). Consistent with their explications, our measure of uncertainty perceptions was intentionally global, reflecting a general sense that work events were unpredictable. However, one might expect cognition-based trust to matter more during times of organizational crisis, whereas affect-based trust might matter more during political struggles or times of relationship conflict. Taken together, these results offer three important theoretical contributions to the justice literature. First, they reinforce Lewicki et al.’s (2005) suggestion that justice scholars should adopt a more nuanced view of trust in their theorizing. It may be that the past reliance on the unidimensional confident, positive expectations perspective has obscured some potentially important distinctions that can explain exactly why justice impacts performance behaviors. We would therefore suggest that scholars continue to assess both affect- and cognition-based trust (or, alternatively, the benevolence-, ability-, and integrity-based forms of trustworthiness described by Mayer et al., 1995) in order to further explore those nuances (see also Colquitt & Mueller, 2008). Second, our results show that social exchange theory should not be the only theoretical lens used to explain the effects of justice on performance behaviors. A combination of exchange-based mechanisms and mechanisms rooted in uncertainty management theory was needed to more fully explain why trust served as a mediator of justice effects. Supplementing social-exchange-based mechanisms with more uncertainty-based mechanisms could complement recent research that examines justice from a workplace stress lens (e.g., Greenberg, 2006). Given that work uncertainty is framed as a stressor in many stress models, justice could be viewed as having a buffering role that is similar to the functioning of supervisor or coworker support in the stress literature (Sonnentag & Frese, 2003). Third, our results illustrate the value in integrating multiple theories when examining justice phenomena. The mechanisms in different theories may often be complementary to one another by emphasizing different aspects of the psychological experience of justice—aspects that may uniquely explain subsequent reactions. That observation is particularly relevant to the literatures on fairness heuristic theory and uncertainty management theory. We 11 noted earlier that uncertainty management theory was cast as a successor to fairness heuristic theory. The discussions of uncertainty management theory ascribe no special importance to trust (Lind & van den Bos, 2002; van den Bos & Lind, 2002), and tests of the theory do not operationalize any trust-related constructs (van den Bos, 2001b; van den Bos & Miedema, 2000). Moreover, the introduction of uncertainty management theory seems to have had a dampening effect on studies that apply fairness heuristic theory to ground predictions or that use its mechanisms to inspire the choice of independent or mediating variables. Our results suggest that fairness heuristic theory remains a valuable conceptual lens because it simultaneously emphasizes the importance of trust and of uncertainty. As a result, there may be value in viewing fairness heuristic theory as a complement to uncertainty management theory, rather than as an ancestor that has lost its usefulness. Moreover, there may be value in moving beyond the global uncertainty that is described and tested in uncertainty management theory, to see if a more nuanced operationalization would shed new insights. For example, there could be value in articulating a taxonomy of work and nonwork-related uncertainty, to see if those facets contribute beyond a global form, or to see if justice is uniquely relevant to some facets more than others. Limitations This study has some limitations that should be noted. One limitation is the use of self-report scales for the justice, trust, normative commitment, and uncertainty variables. The reliance on self-reports raises the possibility that the relationships among those variables are inflated by common method variance (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003). However, it should be noted that the relationships with job performance were not same source, given the use of archival data. Common method variance could have contributed to some of the high correlations between our predictors, especially affect- and cognition-based trust. Such significant multicollinearity raises questions about the robustness of the results for those predictors in Figure 2. Fortunately, a dominance analysis supported the relative importance of those predictors for normative commitment and uncertainty (Azen & Budescu, 2003; Lebreton, Ployhart, & Ladd, 2004).2 Another limitation concerns the correlational nature of our data. The term mediation conveys a causal quality to the examined relationships that cannot be inferred from correlational data (Stone-Romero & Rosopa, 2004). Although our job performance data were collected several months after our self-report variables, we would have needed to collect experimental or cross-lagged data to test the causal nature of the relationships among justice, trust, and performance. In the absence of such data, the only means of judging the validity of our causal order is to compare the structure in Figure 2 with structures with alternative orderings of the justice, trust, and mediator linkages. We investigated five such alternatives. Four provided a worse fit to the data, three of those to a statistically significant degree (full details of this analysis are available from the first author upon request). The fifth alternative provided an equivalent fit and cast normative commitment and uncertainty as antecedents of the trust dimensions and the trust 2 Those analyses are available from the first author upon request. 12 COLQUITT ET AL. dimensions as antecedents of the justice dimensions. However, we are not aware of any theories that would support that structure, unlike our a priori structure in Figure 2. Other limitations concern our use of normative commitment to operationalize the exchange-deepening dynamic in our study. We chose to use normative commitment because it captures the notion of obligation, which is central to Blau’s (1964) description of social exchange relationships. Other common operationalizations of social exchange, such as Eisenberger, Armeli, Rexwinkle, Lynch, and Rhoades’s (2001) perceived support (e.g., Masterson et al., 2000; Moorman et al., 1998; Tekleab et al., 2005; Wayne et al., 2002), Graen and Uhl-Bien’s (1995) leader–member exchange (e.g., Masterson et al., 2000; Wayne et al., 2002), and Meyer and Allen’s (1991, 1997) affective commitment (e.g., Shore & Tetrick, 1991; Shore & Wayne, 1993) do not reference a sense of obligation in any way. However, four of the six items in Meyer and Allen’s (1997) normative commitment scale bound the obligation to the desire to remain, which is only one aspect of reciprocation. A better operationalization of the exchange-deepening dynamic might therefore have been Eisenberger et al.’s felt obligation construct, which represents a more all-encompassing sense of reciprocation. Alternatively, we could have utilized Morrison and Phelps’s (1999) felt responsibility scale, or we could have conceptualized obligation as a broadening of role definitions, along the lines of McAllister, Kamdar, Morrison, and Turban (2007). Our normative commitment measure was also referenced to the organization, whereas our justice and trust measures were referenced to the supervisor. Although we believe that employees can reciprocate for beneficial supervisory treatment with attitudes and behaviors that support the organization— because the supervisor is an agent of the organization—a supervisor-targeted measure of obligation would have yielded a cleaner test. We therefore suspect that the path coefficients involving normative commitment would have been stronger if supervisory normative commitment (or felt obligation toward one’s supervisor) had served as our operationalization of the exchange dynamic. Practical Implications Despite those limitations, our results offer a number of practical implications. First and foremost, the justice effects observed in our data reinforce the practical value in sensitizing managers to justice concerns. Fortunately, a stream of research has shown that managers can be successfully trained to adhere to justice rules (for a review, see Skarlicki & Latham, 2005). Such training could become an aspect of the managerial and executive development programs that are already so common in today’s organizations. We would further suggest pairing such programs with annual survey assessments of justice and trust variables. Many organizations already conduct annual attitude surveys, but often those surveys focus more on general job attitudes than on specific justice or trust variables. Such survey efforts would allow individuals to identify the departments or work units in which justice or trust is particularly low, making justice training a more immediate priority. Our results for affect-based trust illustrate the value in initiatives that increase the frequency and transparency of communications with employees, as the frequent sharing of ideas and information is a key component of that trust form (McAllister, 1995). Finally, our results for cognition-based trust reinforce the importance of staff- ing and training strategies, all of which can be leveraged to improve the competence and reliability of organizational employees (Arthur, Bennett, Edens, & Bell, 2003; Rynes, 1991). Fostering affect- and cognition-based trust in these manners should help employees cope with uncertain periods in their organizational lives while they maintain their focus on their work obligations. References Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 267–299). New York, NY: Academic Press. Allen, N. J., & Meyer, J. P. (1990). The measurement and antecedents of affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the organization. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 63, 1–18. Ambrose, M. L., Hess, R. L., & Ganesan, S. (2007). The relationship between justice and attitudes: An examination of justice effects on event and system-related attitudes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 103, 21–36. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2007.01.001 Ambrose, M. L., & Schminke, M. (2003). Organization structure as a moderator of the relationship between procedural justice, interactional justice, perceived organizational support, and supervisory support. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 295–305. doi:10.1037/00219010.88.2.295 Arthur, W., Bennett, W., Edens, P. S., & Bell, S. T. (2003). Effectiveness of training in organizations: A meta-analysis of design and evaluation features. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 234 –245. doi:10.1037/ 0021-9010.88.2.234 Aryee, S., Budhwar, P. S., & Chen, Z. X. (2002). Trust as a mediator of the relationship between organizational justice and work outcomes: Test of a social exchange model. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 23, 267– 285. doi:10.1002/job.138 Azen, R., & Budescu, D. V. (2003). The dominance analysis approach for comparing predictors in multiple regression. Psychological Methods, 8, 129 –148. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.8.2.129 Barclay, L. J., Skarlicki, D. P., & Pugh, S. D. (2005). Exploring the role of emotions in injustice perceptions and retaliation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 629 – 643. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.4.629 Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173 Begley, T. M., Lee, C., & Hui, C. (2006). Organizational leadership as a moderator of the relationship between justice perceptions and workrelated reactions. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27, 705–721. doi:10.1002/job.388 Bies, R. J., & Moag, J. F. (1986). Interactional justice: Communication criteria of fairness. In R. J. Lewicki, B. H. Sheppard, & M. H. Bazerman (Eds.), Research on negotiations in organizations (Vol. 1, pp. 43–55). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. New York, NY: Wiley. Brockner, J., Fishman, A. Y., Reb, J., Goldman, B., Spiegel, S., & Garden, C. (2007). Procedural fairness, outcome favorability, and judgments of an authority’s responsibility. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1657– 1671. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.6.1657 Carr, J. Z., Schmidt, A. M., Ford, J. K., & DeShon, R. P. (2003). Climate perceptions matter: A meta-analytic path analysis relating molar climate, cognitive and affective states, and individual level work outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 605– 619. doi:10.1037/00219010.88.4.605 Cohen-Charash, Y., & Spector, P. E. (2001). The role of justice in organizations: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86, 278 –321. doi:10.1006/obhd.2001.2958 JUSTICE, TRUST, AND PERFORMANCE Colquitt, J. A. (2001). On the dimensionality of organizational justice: A construct validation of a measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 386 – 400. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.386 Colquitt, J. A., Conlon, D. E., Wesson, M. J., Porter, C. O. L. H., & Ng, K. Y. (2001). Justice at the millennium: A meta-analytic review of 25 years of organizational justice research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 425– 445. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.425 Colquitt, J. A., Greenberg, J., & Scott, B. A. (2005). Organizational justice: Where do we stand? In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), The handbook of organizational justice (pp. 589 – 619). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Colquitt, J. A., Greenberg, J., & Zapata-Phelan, C. P. (2005). What is organizational justice? A historical overview. In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), The handbook of organizational justice (pp. 3–56). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Colquitt, J. A., & Mueller, J. B. (2008). Justice, trustworthiness, and trust: A narrative review of their connections. In S. W. Gilliland, D. D. Steiner, & D. P. Skarlicki (Eds.), Justice, morality, and social responsibility (pp. 101–123). Charlotte, NC: Information Age. Colquitt, J. A., Scott, B. A., & LePine, J. A. (2007). Trust, trustworthiness, and trust propensity: A meta-analytic test of their unique relationships with risk taking and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 909 –927. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.4.909 Cook, J., & Wall, T. (1980). New work attitude measures of trust, organizational commitment and personal need non-fulfillment. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 53, 39 –52. Cropanzano, R., Byrne, Z. S., Bobocel, D. R., & Rupp, D. E. (2001). Moral virtues, fairness heuristics, social entities, and other denizens of organizational justice. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58, 164 –209. doi: 10.1006/jvbe.2001.1791 Cropanzano, R., & Mitchell, M. S. (2005). Social exchange theory: An interdisciplinary review. Journal of Management, 31, 874 –900. doi: 10.1177/0149206305279602 Cropanzano, R., Prehar, C. A., & Chen, P. Y. (2002). Using social exchange theory to distinguish procedural from interactional justice. Group & Organization Management, 27, 324 –351. doi:10.1177/ 1059601102027003002 De Cremer, D., & Tyler, T. R. (2007). The effects of trust in authority and procedural fairness on cooperation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 639 – 649. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.3.639 De Cremer, D., Van Knippenberg, B., Van Knippenberg, D., Mullenders, D., & Stinglhamber, F. (2005). Rewarding leadership and fair procedures as determinants of self-esteem. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 3–12. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.3 Dietz, J., Robinson, S. L., Folger, R., Baron, R. A., & Schulz, M. (2003). The impact of community violence and an organization’s procedural justice climate on workplace aggression. Academy of Management Journal, 46, 317–326. doi:10.2307/30040625 Dirks, K. T., & Ferrin, D. L. (2002). Trust in leadership: Meta-analytic findings and implications for research and practice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 611– 628. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.4.611 Eisenberger, R., Armeli, S., Rexwinkle, B., Lynch, P. D., & Rhoades, L. (2001). Reciprocation of perceived organizational support. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 42–51. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.1.42 Folger, R., & Konovsky, M. A. (1989). Effects of procedural and distributive justice on reactions to pay raise decisions. Academy of Management Journal, 32, 115–130. doi:10.2307/256422 Frazier, M. L., Johnson, P. D., Gavin, M., Gooty, J., & Snow, D. B. (2010). Organizational justice, trustworthiness, and trust: A multifoci examination. Group & Organization Management, 35, 39 –76. doi:10.1177/ 1059601109354801 Gabarro, J. J., & Athos, J. (1976). Interpersonal relations and communications. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. (1995). Relationship-based approach to 13 leadership: Development of leader–member exchange (LMX) theory of leadership over 25 years: Applying a multi-level multi-domain perspective. Leadership Quarterly, 6, 219 –247. doi:10.1016/10489843(95)90036-5 Greenberg, J. (1993). The social side of fairness: Interpersonal and informational classes of organizational justice. In R. Cropanzano (Ed.), Justice in the workplace: Approaching fairness in human resource management (pp. 79 –103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Greenberg, J. (2006). Losing sleep over organizational injustice: Attenuating insomniac reactions to underpayment inequity with supervisory training in interactional justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 58 – 69. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.1.58 Homans, G. C. (1961). Social behaviour: Its elementary forms. London, England: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Johnson, J. J., Korsgaard, M. A., & Sapienza, H. J. (2002). Perceived fairness, decision control, and commitment in international joint venture management teams. Strategic Management Journal, 23, 1141–1160. doi:10.1002/smj.277 Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1996). LISREL 8: User’s reference guide. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software. Kernan, M. C., & Hanges, P. J. (2002). Survivor reactions to reorganization: Antecedents and consequences of procedural, interpersonal, and informational justice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 916 –928. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.5.916 Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Konovsky, M. A., & Pugh, S. D. (1994). Citizenship behavior and social exchange. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 656 – 669. doi:10.2307/ 256704 Lazarus, R. S., & Cohen-Charash, Y. (2001). Discrete emotions in organizational life. In R. Payne & C. Cooper (Eds.), Emotions at work (pp. 45– 81). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Lebreton, J. M., Ployhart, R. E., & Ladd, R. T. (2004). A Monte Carlo comparison of relative importance methodologies. Organizational Research Methods, 7, 258 –282. doi:10.1177/1094428104266017 Leventhal, G. S. (1976). The distribution of rewards and resources in groups and organizations. In L. Berkowitz & W. Walster (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 9, pp. 91–131). New York, NY: Academic Press. Leventhal, G. S. (1980). What should be done with equity theory? New approaches to the study of fairness in social relationships. In K. Gergen, M. Greenberg, & R. Willis (Eds.), Social exchange: Advances in theory and research (pp. 27–55). New York, NY: Plenum. Lewicki, R. J., & Bunker, B. B. (1995). Trust in relationships: A model of development and decline. In B. B. Banker & J. Z. Rubin (Eds.), Conflict, cooperation, and justice (pp. 133–173). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Lewicki, R. J., & Bunker, B. B. (1996). Developing and maintaining trust in working relationships. In R. M. Kramer & T. M. Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organizations: Frontiers of theory and research (pp. 114 –139). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Lewicki, R. J., Wiethoff, C., & Tomlinson, E. C. (2005). What is the role of trust in organizational justice? In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice (pp. 247–270). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Lewis, J. D., & Weigert, A. (1985). Trust as a social reality. Social Forces, 63, 967–985. doi:10.2307/2578601 Liao, H., & Rupp, D. E. (2005). The impact of justice climate and justice orientation on work outcomes: A cross-level multifoci framework. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 242–256. doi:10.1037/00219010.90.2.242 Lind, E. A. (2001). Fairness heuristic theory: Justice judgments as pivotal cognitions in organizational relations. In J. Greenberg & R. Cropanzano (Eds.), Advances in organizational justice (pp. 56 – 88). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. 14 COLQUITT ET AL. Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The social psychology of procedural justice. New York, NY: Plenum Press. Lind, E. A., Tyler, T. R., & Huo, Y. J. (1997). Procedural context and culture: Variation in the antecedents of procedural justice judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 767–780. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.4.767 Lind, E. A., & van den Bos, K. (2002). When fairness works: Towards a general theory of uncertainty management. In B. M. Staw & R. M. Kramer (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 24, pp. 181– 223). Boston, MA: Elsevier. Luhmann, N. (1979). Trust and power. New York, NY: Wiley. MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.83 Masterson, S. S., Lewis, K., Goldman, B. M., & Taylor, M. S. (2000). Integrating justice and social exchange: The differing effects of fair procedures and treatment on work relationships. Academy of Management Journal, 43, 738 –748. doi:10.2307/1556364 Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of organizational trust. Academy of Management Review, 20, 709 –734. Mayer, R. C., & Gavin, M. B. (2005). Trust in management and performance: Who minds the shop while the employees watch the boss? Academy of Management Journal, 48, 874 – 888. doi:10.5465/ AMJ.2005.18803928 McAllister, D. J. (1995). Affect- and cognition-based trust as foundations for interpersonal cooperation in organizations. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 24 –59. doi:10.2307/256727 McAllister, D. J., Kamdar, D., Morrison, E. W., & Turban, D. B. (2007). Disentangling role perceptions: How perceived role breadth, discretion, instrumentality, and efficacy relate to helping and taking charge. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1200 –1211. doi:10.1037/00219010.92.5.1200 Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. (1991). A three-component conceptualization of organizational commitment. Human Resource Management Review, 1, 61– 89. doi:10.1016/1053-4822(91)90011-Z Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. (1997). Commitment in the workplace: Theory, research, and application. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Meyer, J. P., & Herscovitch, L. (2001). Commitment in the workplace: Toward a general model. Human Resource Management Review, 11, 299 –326. doi:10.1016/S1053-4822(00)00053-X Meyer, J. P., Stanley, D. J., Herscovitch, L., & Topolnytsky, L. (2002). Affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the organization: A meta-analysis of antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 61, 20 –52. doi:10.1006/jvbe.2001.1842 Milliken, F. J. (1987). Three types of perceived uncertainty about the environment: State, effect, and response uncertainty. Academy of Management Review, 12, 133–143. Moorman, R. H., Blakely, G. L., & Niehoff, B. P. (1998). Does perceived organizational support mediate the relationship between procedural justice and organizational citizenship behavior? Academy of Management Journal, 41, 351–357. doi:10.2307/256913 Moorman, R. H., & Byrne, Z. S. (2005). How does organizational justice affect organizational citizenship behavior? In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), The handbook of organizational justice (pp. 355–380). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Morrison, E. W., & Phelps, C. C. (1999). Taking charge at work: Extrarole efforts to initiate workplace change. Academy of Management Journal, 42, 403– 419. doi:10.2307/257011 Naumann, S. E., & Bennett, N. (2000). A case for procedural justice climate: Development and test of a multilevel model. Academy of Management Journal, 43, 881– 889. doi:10.2307/1556416 Naumann, S. E., & Bennett, N. (2002). The effects of procedural justice climate on work group performance. Small Group Research, 33, 361– 377. doi:10.1177/10496402033003004 Organ, D. W. (1990). The motivational basis of organizational citizenship behavior. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior (Vol. 12, pp. 43–72). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Pillai, R., Schriesheim, C. A., & Williams, E. S. (1999). Fairness perceptions and trust as mediators for transformational and transactional leadership: A two-sample study. Journal of Management, 25, 897–933. doi:10.1177/014920639902500606 Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method bias in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 879 –903. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879 Roberts, K. H., & O’Reilly, C. A. (1974). Measuring organizational communication. Journal of Applied Psychology, 59, 321–326. doi:10.1037/ h0036660 Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. (1998). Not so different after all: A cross-discipline view of trust. Academy of Management Review, 23, 393– 404. doi:10.5465/AMR.1998.926617 Rupp, D. E., & Cropanzano, R. (2002). The mediating effects of social exchange relationships in predicting workplace outcomes from multifoci organizational justice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 89, 925–946. doi:10.1016/S0749-5978(02)00036-5 Rupp, D. E., & Spencer, S. (2006). When customers lash out: The effects of customer interactional justice on emotional labor and the mediating role of discrete emotions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 971–978. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.971 Rynes, S. L. (1991). Recruitment, job choice, and post-hire consequences: A call for new research directions. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 399 – 444). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Shapiro, D. L., Sheppard, B. H., & Cheraskin, L. (1992). Business on a handshake. Negotiation Journal, 8, 365–377. doi:10.1111/j.15719979.1992.tb00679.x Shore, L. M., & Tetrick, L. E. (1991). A construct validity study of the survey of perceived organizational support. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 637– 643. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.76.5.637 Shore, L. M., Tetrick, L. E., Lynch, P., & Barksdale, K. (2006). Social and economic exchange: Construct development and validation. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 837– 867. doi:10.1111/j.00219029.2006.00046.x Shore, L. M., & Wayne, S. J. (1993). Commitment and employee behavior: Comparison of affective commitment and continuance commitment with perceived organizational support. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 774 –780. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.78.5.774 Siegel, P. A., Post, C., Brockner, J., Fishman, A. Y., & Garden, C. (2005). The moderating influence of procedural fairness on the relationship between work-life conflict and organizational commitment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 13–24. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.13 Sinclair, A. L. (2003). The effects of justice and cooperation on team effectiveness. Small Group Research, 34, 74 –100. doi:10.1177/ 1046496402239578 Sitkin, S. B., & Pablo, A. L. (1992). Reconceptualizing the determinants of risk behavior. Academy of Management Review, 17, 9 –38. Skarlicki, D. P., & Latham, G. P. (2005). How can training be used to foster organizational justice? In J. Greenberg & J. A. Colquitt (Eds.), Handbook of organizational justice (pp. 499 –522). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. In S. Leinhardt (Ed.), Sociological methodology 1982 (pp. 290 –312). Washington, DC: American Sociological Association. Sonnentag, S., & Frese, M. (2003). Stress in organizations. In W. C. Borman, D. R. Ilgen, & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: JUSTICE, TRUST, AND PERFORMANCE Vol. 12. Industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 453– 491). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Stinglhamber, F., De Cremer, D., & Mercken, L. (2006). Perceived support as a mediator of the relationship between justice and trust. Group & Organization Management, 31, 442– 468. doi:10.1177/1059601106286782 Stone-Romero, E. F., & Rosopa, P. J. (2004). Inference problems with hierarchical multiple regression-based tests of mediating effects. In J. Martocchio (Ed.), Research in personnel and human resources management (Vol. 23, pp. 249 –290). Oxford, England: Elsevier. Tekleab, A. G., Takeuchi, R., & Taylor, M. S. (2005). Extending the chain of relationships among organizational justice, social exchange, and employee reactions: The role of contract violations. Academy of Management Journal, 48, 146 –157. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2005.15993162 Thibaut, J., & Walker, L. (1975). Procedural justice: A psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Tyler, T. R. (1989). The psychology of procedural justice: A test of the group-value model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 830 – 838. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.830 Tyler, T. R. (1994). Psychological models of the justice motive: Antecedents of distributive and procedural justice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 850 – 863. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.5.850 Tyler, T. R., & Lind, E. A. (1992). A relational model of authority in groups. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 25, pp. 115–191). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. van den Bos, K. (2001a). Fairness heuristic theory: Assessing the information to which people are reacting has a pivotal role in understanding organizational justice. In S. Gilliland, D. Steiner, & D. Skarlicki (Eds.), View publication stats 15 Theoretical and cultural perspectives on organizational justice (pp. 63– 84). Greenwich, CT: Information Age. van den Bos, K. (2001b). Uncertainty management: The influence of uncertainty salience on reactions to perceived procedural fairness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 931–941. doi:10.1037/ 0022-3514.80.6.931 van den Bos, K., & Lind, E. A. (2002). Uncertainty management by means of fairness judgments. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 1– 60). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. van den Bos, K., Lind, E. A., & Wilke, H. A. M. (2001). The psychology of procedural and distributive justice viewed from the perspective of fairness heuristic theory. In R. Cropanzano (Ed.), Justice in the workplace: From theory to practice (Vol. 2, pp. 49 – 66). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. van den Bos, K., & Miedema, J. (2000). Toward understanding why fairness matters: The influence of mortality salience on reactions to procedural justice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 355–366. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.3.355 Wayne, S. J., Shore, L. M., Bommer, W. H., & Tetrick, L. E. (2002). The role of fair treatment and rewards in perceptions of organizational support and leader–member exchange. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 590 –598. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.3.590 Received February 1, 2008 Revision received June 20, 2011 Accepted June 27, 2011 䡲