MODULE 5: ALTRUISM IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY The scientific interest in Altruism dates back in 1960s. Over the years psychologists, sociobiologists even economist have delved into defining and measuring this concept. Review on theory and research on altruism by Piliavin and Charng (1990) and Feigin, Owens and Goodyear-Smith (2014) noted that writers from different disciplines define altruism differently. Altruism: General definition ➢ Selfless concern for the well-being of others. ➢ A prosocial behavior - any positive human behaviors which sustain societies, including cooperation, charity and other forms of helping. ➢ Introduced by Philosopher Comte (1875) Other Definitions: • • • • Auguste Comte - devotion to the interests of others as an action-guiding principle/helping in which the costs or risks to the helper are more obvious than any potential gains/no gain to the helper. Sociobiologist Wilson - self-destructive behavior performed for the benefit of others. Liebrand – in social dilemma, altruists are defined as individuals who give more weight to others than to their own. Margolis - actor could have done better for himself had he chosen to ignore the effects of his choice on others. Prosocial Behavior: ➢ a voluntary behavior that is carried out to benefit another person. ➢ excludes beneficial actions that are not performed voluntarily or are not performed with the intention of helping another. While the definition of altruism seem to lead more on a positive note, there are differing ways on defining the concept, especially among scholars who seem uncomfortable to simply agree to its one-side definition. A closer analysis on the aspect of helping has yielded two-sides to the altruistic coin. Two Factors of Helping: The emphasis on studying altruism, according to Krebs (1987), involves two factors: Intention and amount of benefit or cost to the actor. Two Types/Forms of Helping based on motives (by Comte): Egoistic helping and altruistic helping. 1. Egoistic helping - where the person helps and wants something in return (based on egoism) because the ultimate goal of the helper is to increase his/her own welfare. • Pseudo-altruistic approach/selfishly motivated helping - there is an intentional and voluntary act performed to benefit another person as the primary motivation yet with the conscious or unconscious expectation of reward. • Normative altruism - includes commonplace acts of helpfulness governed by social rewards and punishments. 2. Altruistic Helping - where the person expects nothing in return (based on altruism) because the ultimate goal is to increase another’s welfare. • Altruistic approach in helping - there is a voluntary act to help in order to benefit the other without a conscious expectation of reward. • Autonomous altruism - where acts of helpfulness is not governed by social rewards or punishments. Bar-Tal (1985-1986) noted that the motivational aspect of altruism is that it: a) must benefit another person b) must be performed voluntarily c) must be performed intentionally d) the benefit must be the goal itself e) must be performed without expecting any extremal reward THEORIES on WHY DO PEOPLE HELP Various theorist has suggested on models as to why people help. The concept of altruism includes approaches even outside of psychology. A. EARLY THEORIES Evolutionary Theories - influenced theoretical and empirical approaches to explaining human altruism. This theory also sprang the Sociobiological approach to altruism. The early notion of human altruism as possessing underlying selfish motivation. This concept of altruism is linked to Charles Darwin’s theory of evolution of man and natural selection. The paradox of natural selection and altruism was introduced with the 1 notions of group and kin selection, inclusive fitness and reciprocal altruism. a. GENETIC EXPLANATIONS: Wilson employs the principles of evolutionary theory. One principle of sociobiology is that any social behaviors that enhance reproductive success will continue to be passed on from one generation to the next. • Evolutionary theory - altruism is ultimately selfish because it serves only to increase one’s genetic fitness that is, reciprocal altruism is favored in natural selection. This early understanding of altruism was believed to have made an impact on some early psychological theories concerned with parental care and selfsacrifice and influenced psychological theoretical arguments that human altruism is ultimately selfishly motivated. • Among the animal species, the care and protection toward same kind against predators is an altruistic act to ensure that the genecarrying species will thrive across its lifespan and pass on its genes to the next. And so this act (of protecting the specie) is learnt eventually by its offspring and passes it on to their future off springs. This notion also puts a genetic explanation to altruism. However, Wilson’s explanation of altruism as simply a part of the species heritage has stirred a lot of controversies because people consider altruistic acts to be the most noble of human actions. In spite of the controversy, he did not totally dismiss his view. Wilson described many human behaviors that involved risks, danger or even death of the person who offered help. He cited insect social behavior like the honeybee workers were stinging an intruder will lead to its very own death, for its sting will remain embedded in the intruder, is a self-sacrifice act to protect the queen and the hive. According to him, it can also be so to individuals of this type characteristically sacrificing themselves in defense of their social group. • But critics have noted that self-sacrificing insects do not really contribute to the species heritage as they are sterile, while members who can reproduce are not self-sacrificing, rather are competitive. Potential healthy genecarriers always seek to perpetuate themselves and not sacrifice so that others may reproduce. b. KIN SELECTION: The understanding of altruistic acts are greatly expressed and shared with blood relatives to preserve, again, one’s genes even when there are risks is a helpful act. This principle of kin selection states that one will exhibit preferences for helping blood relatives because this will increase odds that your genes will be transmitted to subsequent generation. • Yet, it seems logical to consider that in the Kin Selection Process or the Survival of the Fittest Gene, we asume that we first help our own kind. It further notes that animals sharing high percentage of genes by common descent will be altruistic towards each other. The nearer the kin, the greater will be the readiness to sacrifice. This particular concept of altruism assumes that being helpful to another, especially if they are a kin/blood relative is selfsacrificing to guarantee a perpetuation of genetic heritage and thus more likely to help those who are related to us by blood. • However, this arguement of altruism to protect the gene survival does not explain the countless incidences of stranger helping. c. RECIPROCAL HELPING. Robert Trivers(1971) argued that there is a kind of helping strangers that can lead to natural selection which he termed as reciprocal helping. This principle involves mutual helping, where people are likely to help strangers if it us understood that the recipient is expected to return the favor at some time in the future. • Trivers believe that altruistic acts expand far beyond simply protecting genes for reproductive purposes and helping only one’s kins and blood relatives. While species survival and kins are important, so are other other species and individuals becoming good and beneficial for one’s survival – hence a reciprocal helping can happen. Helping a total stranger or a friend and neighbor can also be observed. However, according to Trivers, for reciprocal helping to evolve, the benefit to the recipient must be high and the cost of the helper must be relatively low. • Trivers also believe that reciprocal helping exists in three (3) conditions: a) Social group living, so that individuals have ample opportunity to give and receive help, b) Mutual dependence, in which species survival depends on cooperation and 2 c) The lack of rigid dominance hierarchies, so that reciprocal helping will enhance each (animal’s) power. The belief held by this reciprocal helping can be concretely expressed with the quote “I do you a favor, you do a favor back to me”; “I scratch your back, you scratch mine” as Trivers explained in a YouTube interview by The Dissenter (2019). However, returning back the favor does not happen in the same time the help was given, rather there is a presumed timeline of returning back the favor. It can be assumed then that when someone had helped you, you remember that helped in your lifetime, and when an opportunity comes for this person (who have helped you) need help, then you help back in one way or the other. Trivers also further explained that when, in some case, this person who have helped you passed away, you somehow find other means to return the helping favor – perhaps to his family or to others. B. NORMATIVE THEORY OF ALTRUISM Although prosocial behavior may have genetic basis, social mechanisms would develop to help enforce evolutionary adaptive helping strategies. The prosocial norms are expectations to behave selflessly in bestowing benefits upon others. According to this theory, there are three basic influences on altruism: 1. the intensity of moral (personal) obligation, 2. cognitive structure of norms and values and 3. relevance or appropriateness of feelings of moral obligation. To understand this theory is to take note of the set of influences for altruism that begin as external factors and become internalized as social norms where rules of conduct, that is established and maintained within societies, are used to guide the correct behavior of all individuals. Moral (Personal) Obligations are influenced by shared group expectations about appropriate behavior and social rewards, varying from individual to individual. People help because they perceive it as the appropriate response either due to previous experience or observation from others. Personal norms also play and important influential role both cognitively and affectively. For example, people may possess expectations of behavior based on personal standards (cognitive) or experience emotions (affective), such as pity or guilt when witnessing another needing help. According to Schwartz (1973), personal norms are based on personally held values. There is a self-based model that specifies how a motivation to help – in the form of a specific personal norm – is activated in a situation. It infers that individual whose central values are relevant to moral choices are most likely to offer assistance to others and non-helpful people often reduces their own feelings of moral obligation with various psychological defenses such as denial of responsibility (Schwartz, 1973; Schwartz and Howard, 1980). According to Schwartz (1973), personal norms are based on personally held values. There is a self-based model that specifies how a motivation to help – in the form of a specific personal norm – is activated in a situation. It infers that individual whose central values are relevant to moral choices are most likely to offer assistance to others and non-helpful people often reduces their own feelings of moral obligation with various psychological defenses such as denial of responsibility (Schwartz, 1973; Schwartz and Howard, 1980). THREE SOCIAL NORMS AS GUIDELINES FOR PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR 1. RECIPROCITY is based on maintaining fairness in social relationships. This meant helping those who have helped us. But it also leads to some discomfort when people who have received help are unable or cannot give back in return. This norm of reciprocity includes the demands that people should help those who have helped them and must not also injure them. Central to this social norm is that ir encourages a variety of prosocial behaviors and discourages antisocial ones. It also leads to the idea that reciprocal debts encourage cooperation on tasks that are too large for one person to handle. The concept of reciprocity suggested that people weigh the personal costs and rewards associated with altruism and always assess the possibility that they might be helped later by those they help. 2. SOCIAL RESPONSIBILITY dictates that people should help due to a greater awareness of what is right. It assumes that we are to help others in need and dependent on us. Say, it is the responsibility of parents to protect their children or teachers have the obligation (responsibility) to mentor students or that police enforcers have the social responsibility to implement peace and order in the community. This social responsibility norm requires help-givers or have otherregarding sentiments to render assistance 3 regardless of who the recipient is without an expectation of being rewarded. 3. SOCIAL JUSTICE aligns to the “just world hypothesis” proposed by Michael Lerner (1980) that indicates a shared belief that the world is fair and people get what they deserve and vice versa. The principle behind this is that we help those who have helped us and not those who have denied us help. In other words, it stipulates that people should help only when they believe others deserves their assistance. Underlying the social justice norm can elicit either sympathy or condemnation of help-seekers. It can cause people to help those who are perceived to have been unjustly harmed, but it can also cause people to judge victims as “deserving” their suffering. C. SOCIAL LEARNING PERSPECTIVE OF ALTRUISM The social learning perspective argues that our moral responses to help others are acquired through laws of learning. Internalisation of values as facilitated by observational learning. Parental models seen as the strongest and most prolonged influences on the internalisation process. MODELING AND REINFORCEMENT One of the major psychological approach to altruism is based on the idea that we learn altruistic behavior in some ways as behaviors by modeling the desired behaviors and reinforcing them when they occur. When understanding altruism in this perspective, it leads us to believe that When people behave altruistically, they are seen as models (being publicized) and are offered a variety of benefits (rewards – reinforcement). Both social reinforcement and modeling increases the altruistic behavior. Example: Angel Locsin is often publicized for her philanthropic activities such as raising funds to provide donations for fellow Filipinos who were victims of calamities and the most recent was raising funds in order to donate personal protective equipment and make-shift sleeping and rest facilities for front liners due to the rise of positive Covid19 patients in Metro Manila. Angel Locsin now being the altruistic model (modelling) and being publicized for her helping activities (can be a form of reinforcement). We might also assume that this particular altruistic act of Angel Locsin might have been learnt from her childhood (from family members as models) and being praised for sharing help when was a child (as reinforcement) One drawback, however of the social learning perspective of altruism, can undermine altruistic motives under some circumstances. If a reward is expected in advance, the help-giver may be perceived as motivated by the reward (egoistic altruism) rather than their own altruistic motives (altruistic helping) and may be thought of us someone who will only help when external rewards are at hand. WHEN DO PEOPLE HELP The decision to engaging in altruistic acts can be complex. Social psychologist have not only investigated how people develop the helping or prosocial behavior but also the time in which individuals actually engages in such behavior. D. SITUATIONAL PERSPECTIVES OF ALTRUISM 1. BYSTANDER INTERVENTION MODEL also known as Decision Model of Bystander Intervention proposes that the decision to help others depend on five (5) factors: a) Noticing something unusual and identifying a negative change in someone’s (victim/helped) circumstances. b) Involving a decisionmaking process and recognizing that help is needed. c) Determining the extent to which they have the personal responsibility (or not) to help the other. d) Making a decision on what kind of help is to be offered. When one assumes the responsibility to help, one makes appropriate form of assistance to render. e) Making the final decision to give help. This happens especially when the unusual circumstances are interpreted as an emergency, assumes responsibility and is able to decide the best way to provide the help and eventually implements the course of prosocial action. This five-factor Bystander Intervention Model, proposed by Bibb Latané and John Darley (1969, 1970) empowers bystander action or may lead to bystander apathy (inaction). Bystander intervention is a form of 4 prosocial behavior that occurs when onlookers act to provide direct aid or protection to victims, defend victims, confront or distract aggresors, alert authorities or call police or emergency medical professinals on behalf of others (Burn, 2017). Bystanders may intervene alone or with a group of people to mitigate harm to victims and the act can be herois and lifesaving. 2. AUDIENCE INHIBITION EFFECT - that can be a concommitant action to the bystander effect denoting that on factor 1 of the bystander intervention, an ambiguity of the circumstances is occuring. It meant that when others are present, people not only are less likely to define a potentially dangerous situation as an emergency but that they also respond more slowly to the possible emergency. This audience inihibition effect is likely to occur when other people are acting calmly or dismissive of the situation. The inhibition effect was seen as a resultant of how people perceive and interpret the circumstances in two ways: information dependence and outcome dependence. When one is not clear about defining the situation, s/he is most likely dependent on others to define the situation (informationdependence), wherein s/he interprets the situation out of the reactions of others. However, the decision to do something about the unusual circumstances is also being processed by the person, where he puts importance on how others would evaluate him on whether or not s/he decides to help (outcome dependence). Say for example when witnessing a man in a busy street yelling out for help, you look at the man and at the same time look around and the people around this man yelling for help. You start observing what others do, who have also heard the man yelling for help. You see other people stopping and starring, others still moving along, others moving away from the man. All these observations are your information dependence. Then you start to think about the situation, whether you decide to help this man or not. But this decision will not be very easy for you. At the back of your mind, you have a lot going on – what is it that this man needs? What help should be given? And many others things on your mind that will also include what others moght think about you if you do something for this man – that is your outcome dependence. 3. DIFFUSION OF RESPONSIBILITY - collaterally connected still with the bystander effect indicating that the knowledge of the presence of others who might help inhibits intervention in an emergency. Latané, et.al.(1981) found roboust evidence of the effect of perceived group size on helping. Studies showed that in the bystander effect, when an individual believes that there are other bystanders who can offer help, pressure to rescue the victim is reduced. The belief that the presence of other people in an emergency situation makes one less personally responsible to offer help concretely explains the diffusion of responsibility. E. EMOTIONAL PERSPECTIVE TO ALTTRUISM: Extending the work on Bibb Latané and John Darley and attempting to explain more on why people in a group of bystanders often do not interpret and event as an emergency and why help sometimes is not clearly defined, Jane Piliavin and her collegues (1981) and other social psychologists focused on Factors 3 (personal responsibility), 4(deciding what to do) and 5 (implementing action). 1. AROUSAL COST-REWARD MODEL According to this model, witnessing an emergency is emotionally arousing and is generally experienced as an uncomfortable tension which bystander seek to decrease (Gaetner & Dovidio, 1977). Further, this model proposed that witnessing the distress of another or their suffering creates unpleasant empathic arousal in the observer and thus the observer is motivated to help (Hoffman, 1981). In another way, reducing the uncomfortable tension of witnessing a distress can also be done by ignoring the danger signs and simply fleeing away from the event. Whatever the bystander chooses will be a result of their analysis of the costs and rewards for helping and for not helping. What could be the costs for helping? This could involve time, energy, personal resources, financial costs, health, or even life. It can also lead to self-blame or experience of loss self-esteem or even social 5 disapproval or embarrassment for ignoring or not rendering assistance to the sufferer. According to Piliavin, if the costs of helping are low and the cost of not helping are high, then the bystander is most likely to show altruistic action. On the other hand, if both (helping and not helping) costs are low, to show prosocial behavior depends on the perceived social norm in the situation. However, if the costs of helping and not helping are both high – this can lead the bystander in a very challenging situation. In this case, two (2) possible courses of action can happen: a) One is for the bystander to intervene indirectly – either by calling for police, an ambulance or other professional helping source. b) Another is for the bystander to refine the situation in a way that results in them not helping – either that they have assessed that there is no emergency at all in the situation or that there are other people who can help instead or that the victim deserves to suffer, hence, help is not provided. 2. AROUSAL-REDUCTION and NEGATIVE STATE RELIEF MODEL almost similar to arousal cost-reward model. This model argues that altruism is motivated by reduction in aversive arousal or tension. When an observer witnesses another’s suffering this cause negative feelings and helping then minimizes the negative feelings. Such occurs when one experiences guilt in response to another’s suffering – this can be a powerful motivator for altruistic acts. Feelings of guilt endangers self-esteem and helping behaviors enables self-image reparation (Feigin, et.al. 2014) 3. EMPATHY involves feeling of sympathy and a desire to relieve another’s suffering is dependent on the observer’s developmental stage. Children’s development of empathy is a highlight to their emotional growth and they eventually become aware of others and understand environmental cues to experience and express empathy. 4. MOODS AND HELPING. Research also demonstrates that people’s willingness to help is affected by the mood they happen to be in when assistance is needed. In general, positive moods have a prosocial effect. People are more likely to help others when they in a good mood (Cunningham, 1979; Isen & Levin, 1972; Wilson, 1981). The correlation between good mood and altruistic tendencies has several implications: ☺ when one is in a positive mood, s/he is more likely to perceive other people as nice, honest or decent and thus deserving of help (Forgas & Bower, 1987; Isen, 1987) ☺ another possibility is that happy people help others in order to prolong their good mood (Williamson & Clark, 1982) ☺ happy people are less likely to be self-absorbed in their own thoughts and hence are more attentive to other’s needs (McMillen, Sander and Solomon, 1977) ☺ good mood increases the likelihood that people think about the rewarding nature of social activities, whereby helpfulness expected to provide in return positive stimuli. But how about negative moods? Isen and collegues (1973) found that when people believed they have failed, they were more likely to help another person as well. A possible link to this is the rewarding properties of helping. Because helping others often increases good mood, people in bad mood might help in order to escape a bad mood. Robert Cialdini and Douglas Kenrick (1976) proposed that when people are in a bad mood and yet decides to help another is based on a simple self-serving question – will helping make me feel better? WHOM DO WE HELP A. PERCEIVED SIMILAR OTHERS - Because similarity influences interpersonal attraction, it is conducive to liking and liking is conducive to helping (Myers, 2010). Miller and colleagues (2001) noted that we are more empathic and helpful toward those similar us. Perceiving a needy person as similar to us increases our willingness to lend assistance. In a social experiment done in London and actor posed himself as somebody who needed help. He was simply dressed in rugged overalls and a pair of jeans laid himself on the ground pressing his abdomen and asking for people passing by for help. For twenty minutes, 6 people simply passed by him. Others not even having to take a look at him. Then, in an hour left to change his clothes. This time wearing a suit and tie with polished leather shoes. Still in the same busy area, he posted as someone who seemed to have passed out and fallen on the ground and in no time, within six minutes, a young lady dressed in office attire and a gentleman also in his suit and coat, called him “sir” and asked if he was okay and what has just happened to him. This experiment seem to clearly show that when one identifies along with another as needing assistance, it is more likely that help is offered. Lisa DeBruine (2002) quotes “no face is more familiar than one’s own.” Burger and colleagues (2004) also noted that even just sharing a birthday, a first name leads people to respond more to a request for help. This can probably explain why, when one is in another country and see a fellow needing help and is seemingly looking like him (say Asian-looking or Hispanic-looking), he is more likely to give that person some assistance. B. GENDER CONSIDERATIONS IN HELPING - Alice Eagly and Maureen Crowley (1986) reported that when faced with potentially dangerous situations in which strangers need help, men are more likely to help. In safer situations, women are slightly more likely to help. George and colleagues presented that among women, they respond with greater empathy and spare more time to help a friend, that is, women are more likely than men to provide emotional support to others (Shumaker and Hill, 1991). These findings show that their gender differences interact depending on the situation. Myers (2010) in his book Social Psychology also noted a report during the tragic sinking of the Titanic. When it sank, it was found that 70% of the females and 20% of the males on board survived. Thanks to gender norms, most of those who were helped were women than men. This shows that male helpers were perfectly in accord with the male gender role, which nurtures helping as a heroic and chivalrous act that is generally directed toward the benefit of female victims. However, this finding seem to clearly present men as offering help in an egoistic manner. accidents, calamities, health crisis such as the coronavirus. The reason to why people help “deserving” others is true to the principle of social justice. One of the unfortunate consequences of believing in a just world is that people tend to make defensive attributions when explaining plight of victims and indicates that people are prone to blame the sufferer for their distresses and misfortunes. Does TRUE ALTRUISM exist? The emotional perspective of altruism, according to social psychologists, assume an egoistic motive underlying prosocial behavior. EMPATHY-ALTRUISM HYPOTHESIS - Daniel Batson (1987, 1991), Hoffman (1981) and others (Karylowski, 1984; Kreb, 1975) argued that sometimes people’s intention to help are truly altruistic. Batson, mentioned by Feigin, et al (2014) separates the dominant egoistic motivation for helping. In this hypothesis, Batson characterizes six (6) empirically testable psychological process: 1.a) perception of another’s need is a function of several factors including a perceptible discrepancy between the current and potential state of wellbeing. The perception of the other’s need leads to empathic emotion. 1.b) adoption of the other’s perspective and a perspective-taking set 1.c) attachment that contributes to the experience of empathy 1.d) arousal of empathy due to empathy where the level of attachment affects the likelihood of perspective-taking and the strength of attachment that affects the magnitude of empathic emotions 1.e) the experience of personal empathy is characterized by sympathy and compassion and evokes altruistic motivation 1.f) relative benefit analysis performed by the observer with the intention of determining the most effective behavior possible to address the needs of the other. C. HELPING DESERVING OTHERS - This is dependent on inferences made about the cause of people’s distress. Following the principle of attribution theory, social psychologist posits that we are more likely to help someone if we attribute the cause of their problems to external or uncontrollable circumstance. Ex. Car 7