LANGUAGE AND VARIATION Mie Hiramoto 1 1.0 INTRODUCTION. Language variation, often discussed in the field of sociolinguistics, is an important subject which studies linguistic differences caused by social or regional factors. 2 Most people are aware of the fact that systematic differences exist between different varieties of the same language; for example, British English is different from American English. Within American English, there are different varieties such as Southern English, Boston English, African American English, or even newscaster English. The systematic differences existing within languages are often referred to as internal variation—the property of languages that enable speakers to express the same meaning in different ways. This chapter focuses on how languages vary internally and what contributes to language variation between social groups, places, and situations. Variation exists in all languages; people are sensitive to how others use the same language differently and we often witness these sensitivities. For example, in Hawai‘i, many people speak Pidgin (an informal term referring to Hawai‘i Creole), yet even among speakers of Pidgin, there are often differences. One may say there is a difference between ‘city’ and ‘country’ Pidgin, or that titas and mokes speak ‘heavier’ Pidgin. There are even social situations where a Hawai‘i Creole speaker may switch to English. In this chapter, we will look at linguistic variation in the following contexts: speech community, regional and geographic variation, and social variation. 2.0 SPEECH COMMUNITIES. A speech community is the term used to describe a group of people who speak the same dialect. Membership in a speech community is usually affected by factors like geographic area, economic class, age, or gender. In a broad sense, the state of Hawai‘i may be considered a speech community consisting of Hawai‘i Creole speakers. However, even within this community, not all the people speak Hawai‘i Creole to the same extent—Honolulu may form a speech community of the ‘town’ variety of Hawai‘i Creole while Wai‘anae may be a separate speech community of ‘country’ Hawai‘i Creole speakers. Very rarely do we find a speech community where there is no variation. That is because a true ‘untainted’ dialect requires an almost impossible level of communicative isolation, whether that isolation is geographic, socioeconomic, or ethnic. Communicative isolation is only possible when a group of speakers forms a coherent speech community isolated from speakers outside the community. While this degree of isolation might have been possible a long time ago, it is becoming quite rare in an increasingly globalized world. Social and geographic mobility, mass communication, and internet access make it make it very difficult to find a situation of communicative isolation. Many people believe that languages can be ‘pure’ or that some people speak a standard variety of a language. Furthermore, many people view these languages and varieties as superior to others. However, there is no such thing outside of speakers’ ideologies. Ni‘ihau—the island with an almost exclusively Native Hawaiian population—provides a relevant example. Ni‘ihau is the only Hawaiian island where Hawaiian is the primary language; however, the 1 Deptment of English Language & Literature, National University of Singapore I thank Tsz-him Tsui for the figures of dialect continuum from his lecture notes and Kent Sakoda for the exercise from Da Pidgin Coup meeting. I would also like to thank Yukiko Ogawa and Yuichi Endo of SUNRISE Inc., International Branch who kindly supplied me with the artwork used in this paper. My thanks are also due to Jake Terrell, the editor of this volume, as well as Katie Drager, John Kupchik and Amy Schafer, the program coordinator for Ling 102. Last but not least, my sincere appreciation goes to Ben George for his endless support and encouragement for my work. 2 81 Language Variation residents are not isolated from the outside world. It was not until 1915 when the owners of the island, the Robinson family of the Gay & Robinson Sugar Plantation, closed the island to most outsiders. According to the Honolulu Star-Bulletin (1997), although Ni‘ihau is generally restricted to the owners’ family, some US Navy personnel, government officials and invited guests have access to the island. Even wealthy tourists are able to fly to the island for a limited supervised tour for snorkeling and picnicking ($250 per person) or hunting ($1,500 person); both tours operate six days a week. The residents also have electricity, television and helicopter transportation to Kaua‘i. As such, Ni‘ihau cannot be said to exist in communicative isolation and any claims about a “pure” language or dialect being spoken on the island would be very suspect. Instead, the variety spoken by a speech community will most likely be influenced by interactions of many different factors. 3.0 STYLE AND REGISTER. The notion of appropriateness is something most of us have internalized, and thus we probably do not give the idea much thought. The ‘inappropriateness’ of an utterance may be due to culture gaps or insufficient understanding about the nuances of a social situation. Among local Japanese speakers in Hawai‘i, especially older speakers, a commonly term for a bathroom is benjo, literally ‘place for excrement’. Even though Japanese speakers in Japan would understand the word, it is considered vulgar and has become almost obsolete. Local Japanese people visiting Japan may elicit shock or surprise at their use of the term benjo, despite the fact that both the speaker and listener understand the meaning. The more commonly used word for ‘bathroom’ in Japan is the English loanword toire ‘toilet’, ironically, a term that American English speakers stopped using for the same reason as the Japanese benjo. A Japanese visitor asking a local Japanese for toire would probably meet with the same embarrassing situation. In both cases the two expressions, benjo and toire represent distinct speech styles. Speech styles may be considered a ‘social’ dialect, because they are usually described in terms of formality depending on factors like topic, setting, and interlocutor (participant in a discourse), rather than any geographic factors. Benjo in Hawai‘i belongs to the speech style of local Japanese, while toire is of the style used by Japanese speakers in Japan. Without paying much attention, people selectively speak their best language to fit certain contexts. The local Japanese people are unlikely to use terms like benjo when speaking to their non-local friends, and general local people will probably avoid speaking in Hawai‘i Creole toward outsiders. If you were given a job as a television newscaster, you would certainly not speak on air in the same way as you do to your family members or friends. At the same time, you will also almost automatically adjust your speech style when you are talking to your pets to train them to behave the way you want them to. These switches from one speech style to another are called style shifting. Style shifting occurs between different degrees of speech formality such as ‘formal’ vs. ‘informal’, ‘casual’ vs. ‘careful’, etc. The different degrees of speech formality are known as registers and inappropriateness can also be caused by selecting the wrong speech register. For example, it is normally considered inappropriate to speak to your pets in a formal speech register. Some languages, including Korean and Japanese, have complex ways to mark different degrees of formality according to speakers’ age and social status; these registers are often referred to as honorifics. For this reason, a language like Japanese requires information regarding the social status of the interlocutors. The following sentences demonstrate how hierarchical differences between speakers are reflected in their choices of verbs, all of which have the meaning ‘give’ in English. (1) Talking about someone in a higher social status: Otsuka-sensei-ni ocha-o sashiageru. Otsuka-teacher-Dative tea-Accusative give (honorific)-non.past ‘I’ll give tea to Professor Otsuka.’ (2) Talking about someone in an equal or in-group social status: 82 Language Variation Tomodachi/Okâ-san-ni ocha-o Friend/mother-Dative tea-Accusative ‘I’ll give tea to my friend/my mother.’ ageru. give (neutral)-non.past (3) Talking about someone in a lower social status or non-humans Otôto/Ueki-ni mizu-o yaru. Younger brother/plant-Dative water-Accusative give (non-honorifics)-non.past ‘I’ll give water to my younger brother/plants.’ As seen in the sentences above, a word meaning ‘give’ has many forms in Japanese according to the relevant relationships among interlocutors. 4.0 STANDARD AND NON-STANDARD DIALECTS. People are generally sensitive about their language use and sometimes judge one variety to be “correct” and others “bad” or “lazy” (recall our discussion in A brief look at grammar). In Hawai‘i, we hear crude criticisms against Hawai‘i Creole English by people who believe it to be broken English. This misunderstanding is rooted in social stereotypes and biased judgments and is not based on linguistic facts. Generally, linguistics does not propose that people’s use of any particular variety of language or dialect is a reflection of their intelligence. No one language or dialect is more systematic, more logical, or more correct than any other language or dialect because all languages have rule-governed grammar that functions as a means of effective communication. The notion of a “standard” language or dialect may be useful when describing a variety that is used in an official capacity. Usually, the standard variety is thought to be used by politicians, public media and in the school system or other high domains. Because of the domains for which it is deemed appropriate, people tend to think the standard variety is prestigious. Indeed, socially speaking, the standard variety does represent prestige and power; the variety that comes to be viewed as the standard is that which is spoken by people in power. However, the prestige of any speech variety depends on the prestige of the speakers and their contextual roles. Newscasters’ speech style (e.g. CNN English) is based on the concept of a prescriptive standard such as Standard American English, and is often considered prestigious. Newscasters of many different regional and ethnic backgrounds, e.g. a Hawaiian man from Maui or an African American woman from New Orleans, are all trained to speak in this register. On the other hand, when our hypothetical Hawaiian newscaster goes surfing at Kīhei, the prestigious language there is probably not CNN English but Hawai‘i Creole. These two different types of prestige are referred to as overt prestige, the officially recognized register, and covert prestige, the locally recognized register. Overt prestige is associated with a specific variety within a broad speech community, often spoken by people of a higher socioeconomic class or education. On the other hand, covert prestige is usually a non-standard variety spoken in a certain speech community. This prestige defines how people should speak in order to be considered a member of a specific, typically smaller, speech community. At a local surf spot, the Hawaiian newscaster may want to refrain from using a variety with overt prestige (the English he uses on the air), as his desire to belong to the local surfer community can be an overriding factor in his use of the language with local covert prestige (Hawai‘i Creole English). Determining the precise roles that ‘prestige’ plays in situational contexts is not always clear-cut, as important factors in these situations, such as socioeconomic class, age, or gender, vary greatly and sometimes overlap. A speech community tends to encompass a large number of members of a specific language- or dialect-speaking community. For example, ‘the local Japanese speech community in Hawai‘i’ refers to local people throughout the Hawaiian Islands who speak Japanese. The concept of a speech community may not be fitting to analyze language use among a small group of speakers. Sociolinguists Penny Eckert and Sally McConnell-Ginet (1992:464) suggest the idea of a community of practice—“an aggregate of people who come together around mutual engagement in an endeavor.” Unlike a speech 83 Language Variation community, The benefits of a CofP approach are that it recognizes that different individuals and groups of individuals within a community have different wants and goals 5.0 REGIONAL AND GEOGRAPHICAL VARIATION. Terms like ‘the Chinese language’ or ‘the Arabic language’ are often used without a thought as to what they imply. These terms, of course, are not useless, but their meanings are often rather different from the ways in which they are generally used. Both the Chinese and the Arabic languages are spoken over wide geographic areas with many different varieties of each language appearing throughout. The website Ethnologue: Languages of the world (Gordon 2005), for example, lists at least 13 varieties of Chinese that are currently spoken—Gan, Hakka, Huizhou, Jinyu, Mandarin, Min Bei, Min Dong, Min Nan, Ming Zhong, Pi-Xian, Wu, Xiang and Yue. The problem is that when one refers to ‘the Chinese language’, there is no knowing which variety is being referred to without more information. For someone from Hong Kong, their ‘Chinese language’ may be Yue Chinese (aka Cantonese), yet for those from Shanghai, their ‘Chinese language’ may be Wu Chinese (aka Shanghainese). Even more problematic, if these ‘Chinese language’ speakers are educated, they may also speak Mandarin Chinese, the standard language of China; however, neither Yue nor Wu Chinese is mutually intelligible with Mandarin Chinese. Hong Kong is known for its movie productions; the successful 2002 crime-thriller film entitled Mou Gaan Dou (in Yue) ‘Non-stop path’ is known as Wu Jian Dao in Mandarin; its English title is Infernal Affairs. The DVD language choices of this movie include Yue, Mandarin, and English, demonstrating the mutual unintelligibility between the ‘Chinese’ languages. The movie was remade in Hollywood in 2006 as The Departed in American English (directed by Martin Scorsese). When the DVD of the US version was released in different regions like Australia, England, or Singapore, it did not require language choices such as Australian English, British English, or Singapore English because American English is mutually intelligible with other varieties of the ‘English’ language. As mentioned in previous chapters, languages are typically considered to be mutually unintelligible while dialects are mutually intelligible. But these definitions, although often taken for granted, can also be very problematic. A group of mutually intelligible varieties congregated in adjacent geographic areas is called a dialect continuum and there are a number of dialect continuums throughout the world. For example, the rural dialects between geographically adjacent villages of France, Italy, Spain, and Portugal, which never lost their mutual intelligibility, constitute the West Romance dialect continuum. This continuum stretches from the Atlantic coast of France through Italy, Spain, and Portugal (Downes 1998:19). Throughout this area, there is always some intelligibility between the dialects of neighboring locations; however, the intelligibility decreases the further one moves away from any particular location. A dialect continuum usually forms gradually. It is important to remember that a language is never uniform, even within the same dialect-speaking region, and that we have the ability to tolerate certain degrees of variation. Imagine that, while there is some variation in the language in Village A, everybody generally understands one another. One day, some people move to Village B (Stage 1). In the beginning, people in both villages can still understand each other, but as contact becomes less frequent, both groups start to develop their own internal variations that are slightly differently than before (Stage 2). If contact gradually diminishes, speakers in Villages A and B lose their mutually intelligibility and the varieties they speak will eventually become different languages (Stage 3). If the speakers maintain contact with their neighbors, this will eventually result in a dialect continuum (Stage 4). FIGURE: 1STAGE 1 Village A ☺☺☺ Village A 84 Village B ☺☺☺ Language Variation FIGURE 2: STAGE 2 Village B Village A ☻☻☻ FIGURE 3: STAGE 3 Village A Village B FIGURE 4: STAGE 4 3 Village A Village B Village C ☺☺ ☺ ☻☻ ☻ Village D If mutual intelligibility is something that groups dialects together and separates them from a different language, then why are mutually unintelligible varieties such as Yue, Wu, and Mandarin all considered dialects of the same language, Chinese, while mutually intelligible varieties of the West Romance dialect continuum are considered to be dialects of different languages, French, Italian, Spanish and Portuguese? 6.0 VARIATION AT DIFFERENT LINGUISTIC LEVELS. While there are some shortcomings in our definition of ‘language’ and ‘dialect’ in certain cases, this is not to say that these terms are not useful in general. There is little problem recognizing British English and American English as dialects of the same language. However, despite their mutual intelligibility, these English dialects actually differ on many levels of linguistic structure including phonetics, phonology, morphology, syntax, and semantics. Despite the Hawaiian Islands’ relatively small amount of land area there are reported dialect differences in the Hawaiian language as well. According Albert Schütz, a linguistics professor at the University of Hawai‘i, Hawaiian does not distinguish between [t] and [k] sounds (Schütz 1994:14-15). (We will look at this issue in more detail in the chapter Hawaiian: evolution of an alphabet.) Due to the orthographic representation used to write the Hawaiian language, sounds pronounced either [t] or [k] are all written with the letter k. However, before the writing system became widely used in Hawai‘i, there was some debate as to the proper way to spell certain words. Archibald Campbell, a Scottish sailor who traveled across five oceans from 1806 to 1812, recognized this variation in the Hawaiian language and cataloged the various spellings of the word ‘Kamehameha’ used by different voyagers—Cameamea by Mr. Samwell, the surgeon of the Discovery, who published an account of Captain Cook’s death; Comaamaa by Portlock; Tomyhomyhaw by Meares; Tamaahmaah by Vancouver and Broughton; Tomooma by Langsdorf; Tamahama Thurnbull; etc. (Campbell 1816:210n, cited in Schütz 1994:77). Moreover, Campbell’s editor notes that t and k sounds were “frequently substituted for each other”, such as kanaka or tanata ‘person/people’ (Campbell 1816:227, cited in Schütz 1994:78). When the Hawaiian orthography was developed by a group of American missionaries in the 1820s, the missionaries noticed that the [t] sound was used more commonly at the northwestern end of the Hawaiian Islands on Kaua‘i, while the [k] sound 3 Figures 1 through 4 were modified from Tsz-him Tsui, LING 102 Spring 2006 Lecture Notes. 85 Language Variation was more common at the southeastern end on the Big Island. They finally settled on using the letter k to represent both of these sounds. However this does not mean the t sound is not used by Hawaiian speakers and Schütz (1994) notes that speakers on the island of Ni‘ihau still use t more than k even today. FIGURE 5: MAP OF HAWAI‘I, [t] ~ [k] VARIATION 4 The previous example dealt with variation in pronunciation; however dialects may differ in more ways than just the sounds they use. Morphological variation between dialects can be seen in Standard English’s reflexive pronouns. In standard English, the singular reflexive pronouns are myself, yourself, himself, herself, and itself and the plural reflexive pronouns are ourselves, yourselves, and themselves. Many English dialects, including the southern American English, use hisself and theirselves instead of himself and themselves as in sentences like ‘He laughed at hisself’ or ‘They took good care of theirselves’. Japanese is an instructive language when discussing dialects. Prior to the Meiji Restoration in 1868, commoners were not allowed to travel freely from one region to another and only a small percentage of privileged people could go out of their own provinces. This lack of mobility resulted in regional dialects being preserved, as relatively limited people had a chance to interact with anyone outside of their own area directly. Today’s standard Japanese is based on the Tôkyô dialect; however, there are many distinguished regional dialects such as Ôsaka-ben, Fukuoka-ben, or Tôhoku-ben (ben being the Japanese term for ‘dialect’). The study of dialects is called dialectology and dialectologists compile maps recording the geographic distributions and boundaries of variations in many different features of a language. A geographic boundary marking where a certain linguistic feature is found is known as an isogloss. The set of maps is called a linguistic atlas. The following map indicates the geographic boundary of the regional Japanese dialect called Tôhoku-ben. 4 Courtesy of About.com: Geography,http://geography.about.com/library/blank/blxushi.ht 86 Language Variation FIGURE 6: MAP OF JAPAN, TÔHOKU DIALECT ISOGLOSS 5 The Tôhoku-ben-speaking region consists of the areas of northern Honshû north and east of the bottom end of Fukushima and the northern half of Niigata (e.g. Kanno and Iitoyo 1967/1994; Katô 1958; Tokugawa and Grootaers 1951). This dialect is known for its strong phonological features that differ so that words like sushi and sashimi are pronounced susu and sasumi. Additionally, there are some semantic (lexical) variations in Japanese dialects that are easily observable in Japanese loanwords used in Hawai‘i. Following the Meiji Restoration, the Japanese government encouraged emigration to Hawai‘i, the US mainland, South America, and many Pacific islands. In Hawai‘i, the first to arrive were laborers from the Hiroshima and Yamaguchi prefectures who spoke the southwestern regional Chûgoku-ben. According to historical records, Japanese men in Hawai‘i outnumbered women by a ratio of four to one before 1900 (Clarke 1994:18; Hawaii Hochisha 2001:53; Hiroshima City 2002:1), and consequently many Japanese men, unable to find wives in Hawai‘i, started arranging picture brides from their hometowns, especially between 1908 and 1923 (Hawaii Hochisha 2001:61; Odo 1998:109). This naturally contributed to maintaining the proportions of Japanese dialects—from the beginning of the Japanese immigration phase until even after WWII, immigrants from Hiroshima and Yamaguchi outnumbered others. Naturally, more Chûgoku-ben terms came into Hawai‘i than any other Japanese dialects during the plantation period. Examples of lexical variation from Chûgoku-ben found in Hawai‘i include bôbora or bôbura ‘pumpkin’ (originally from Portuguese Cambodia abóbora ‘Cambodian pumpkin’, Kanbe 1998:147), waya ‘chaotic’ (Tomosada 2000:133), bocha ‘to bathe’ (The National Language Institute of Japan 1979:145), habuteru ‘to pout’ (Tomosada 2000:99), and obâ-ban or bâ-ban 5 Courtesy of Shigenobu Aoki, http://aoki2.si.gunma-u.ac.jp/map/map.cgi 87 Language Variation (Chiyonobu 1935:58). Due to the historical factors, Japanese spoken in Hawai‘i is heavily influenced by Chûgoku-ben rather than the standard variety (e.g. Fukazawa and Hiramoto 2004). 7.0 SOCIAL VARIATION. Additional factors contributing to the formation of various dialects of a language, other than the regional or geographic factors discussed above, include socioeconomic class, age, gender, and ethnicity. 7.1 SOCIOECONOMIC CLASS. In the same way that physical separation results in a geographical distribution of dialects, factors that affect socioeconomic class can give rise to the social dialects of a language. Some groundbreaking work on social variation was conducted by William Labov, the father of variationist sociolinguistics. One of his more widely known studies on socioeconomic class focuses on the speech of sales assistants in three Manhattan department stores: the highly prestigious Saks, mildly prestigious Macy’s and the low-class S. Klein (Labov 1972). These rankings were based on the stores’ locations, prices, merchandise, and advertisement media. As Labov was interested in the sales associates’ articulations of the final or preconsonantal [r], data collectors, disguised as customers, asked unwitting store clerks a question that was designed to elicit the answer ‘fourth floor’. To obtain both casual and emphatic speech styles, the data collectors pretended that they did not hear the answer the first time so that the clerks would repeat their answer more carefully and emphatically. The study found that the sales assistants from Saks used r the most while those from Klein’s used it the least, and those from Macy’s showed the greatest increase in use when they were asked to repeat the answer. The results from the department store study highlight that the frequency of use of the prestige variable, final or preconsonantal [r], varied with level of formality (casual or emphatic) and social class (scales of the stores). TABLE 1: PERCENTAGES OF ‘R-FULLNESS’ (ADOPTED FROM LABOV 1972/1997:175) Casual Saks Macy’s S. Klein Fourth 30 27 5 Emphatic Floor 63 44 8 Fourth 40 22 13 Floor 64 61 18 7.2 AGE. Another pioneering study done by Labov (1963) on the island of Martha’s Vineyard explores the effects of age on variation. Martha’s Vineyard lies about three miles off the coast of Massachusetts and at the time of Labov’s study had a permanent population of only six thousand people. Every summer, thousands of tourists visit the island with the results that summer tourism replaced fishing and whaling as the dominant industry. At the time of Labov’s fieldwork, only about 150 of the island’s inhabitants still engaged in fishing, forming a close-knit social group. In his study, Labov focused on pronunciations of the diphthongs [aw] (house or how) and [aj] (why or wide) in the onset position of certain words. In the New England dialect, these diphthongs are often centralized and pronounced [əw] and [əj]. Labov interviewed a number of speakers from different ages and ethnic groups on the island. His findings indicate that the younger speakers (under 30 years of age), especially the ones engaged in the summer tourism or the ones who considered leaving the island at some point, did not use the standard New England pronunciation ([əw] and [əy]) as much, preferring instead the pronunciation associated with standard English ([aw] and [ay]). The heaviest users of the New England pronunciation were from the working middle age population (31~45 years old) and were mainly fishermen. These people resented the infringement of summer tourists on the traditional island way of living and were committed to staying on Martha’s Vineyard despite the declining fishing business. 88 Language Variation TABLE 2: PERCENTAGES OF CENTRALIZATION (ADOPTED FROM LABOV 1963:167) Age (ay) (aw) 75+ 25 22 61~74 35 37 46~60 62 44 31~45 81 88 14~30 37 46 Labov’s (1963) report shows that a group of fishermen started to exaggerate the vowel centralization pronunciation in order to establish themselves as a local group as opposed to the summer visitors. For these people, the centralized pronunciation had an emblematic function signaling the difference between insiders and outsiders. 7.3 GENDER. Japanese is known to have gender-exclusive expressions and there are certain linguistic characteristics reserved for Japanese women’s language, which are expected to be used by ideal women. Japanese women’s language features include sentence final particles, pronouns, lexical items, etc., and these speech styles have been studied extensively. The sentence final particles, for instance ze and zo, are considered to masculine while kashira and nanoyo are feminine. However, as pointed out by the anthropological linguist Miyako Inoue (2003), highly idealized Japanese women’s language became the province of imagined characters like Minnie Mouse or Barbie, as no real Japanese women would speak in such a manner. Although the concept of Japanese women’s language exists, it has become so idealistic that real speakers do not bother to maintain its demanding linguistic standards in their everyday speech. On the other hand, fictional female characters often employ Japanese women’s language to mark their femininity. In the popular anime Cowboy Bebop, the main female characters are developed through both artwork and language use. This story takes place in the year 2071, largely on the spaceship Bebop, and follows the adventures of a group of bounty hunters. The main female character, Faye Valentine, has the body of a model and in an unrealistically feminine manner, employing sentence final particles like nanoyo and kashira, discussed above. Other female characters are similarly tailor-made to suit their roles. Older and less attractive characters can be counted on to use less feminine, less proper language. FIGURES 7 & 8: FAYE VALENTINE (A MODEL WOMAN IN THE HEGEMONIC HETEROSEXUAL IDEAL, LEFT), AND ANNIE (AN UNATTRACTIVE WOMAN IN THE HEGEMONIC HETEROSEXUAL IDEAL, RIGHT) 6 6 Figures 7, 8, and 9: Copyright© SUNRISE. Permission for reprint received 2009. All rights reserved. 89 Language Variation As with Faye, the main male characters, Spike and Jet, are similarly constructed through a combination of artwork and language. They are both physically and mentally skilled, although both have prosthetic body parts due to previous injuries. Jet is an experienced mechanic and capable programmer who loves bonsai gardening, and Spike is a martial artist, master of Bruce Lee’s Jeet Kune Do. In addition, not only do Spike and Jet speak in an idealized rough, masculine register, they are extraordinary pilots and excellent fighters. Overall findings of this study show that characters of Cowboy Bebop demonstrate ideal Japanese gendered speech styles (Hiramoto 2010). FIGURE 9: ED, SPIKE, JET, AND FAYE 90 Language Variation 8.0 CONCLUSION. This chapter has presented a broad overview of language variation, especially as it pertains to the fields of sociolinguistics and anthropological linguistics. Two important concepts, speech community and community of practice, were introduced as tools to aid researchers in the analysis of their data. A researcher dealing with large amount of data, such as a Census, may employ the notion of a speech community to answer research questions. By employing this method, the distributions and boundaries of regional dialects may be depicted. Conversely, researchers interested in observing the language use of a specific group of people, the community of practice concept will allow them to address such questions as what types of linguistic variety are used by a certain group of people in order to build solidarity among themselves. It is important to keep in mind that there is no one standard language. Standard language exists only as an ideology. Take, for example, Standard English. While elementary school teachers may claim that they use Standard English, their use of English may vary significantly from that used in other parts of the United States or other English speaking areas. Some may claim that British English is the standard English, but there are many different dialects, both regional and social, spoken in today’s UK. For example, the Geico gecko may be using the ‘cockney’ variety, while Queen Elizabeth may be using the Queen’s English. A sociolinguist’s goal is not to decide which variety is more prestigious. Rather, sociolinguistics tend to focus on how these many varieties are employed by speakers; that is why sociolinguists take notice of ‘covert’ versus ‘overt’ prestige. Differences between social and regional/geographic varieties have also been raised. In the traditional view, regional or geographic variations tend to change slowly due to immigration. We have also discussed why it is hard to separate the concept of dialect from the concept of language, as variations which are 91 Language Variation classified as dialects of the same language may not be mutually intelligible, as was the case with Mandarin, Cantonese and Shanghainese. Language variation and linguistic change motivated by social factors have been studied extensively by William Labov, who has conducted a number of groundbreaking studies. This chapter summarizes a few of his pioneering works. Labov’s most notable results are that factors other thanthose that are languageinternal, such as socioeconomic classes, age, gender, etc., can be correlated with the linguistic variables people use. Many researchers in sociolinguistics and anthropological linguistics have been following in his footsteps to find more about language variation across languages. DVDs Cowboy Bebop: Complete Session Collection. 2003. [video] Directed by Shinichiro Watanabe. USA: Pioneer Video. [6 videodiscs (10 hr., 50 min.)] Mou Gaan Dou (Wu Jian Dao Infernal Affairs). 2004. [video] Directed by Siu Fai Mak & Wai-keung Lau. USA: Buena Vista, Miramax Home Entertainment. [1 videodisc (1 hr., 41 min.)] The Departed. 2007. [video] Directed by Martin Scorsese. USA: Warner Home Video. [1 videodisc (2 hr., 31 min.)] References Campbell, Archibald. 1816. A voyage around the world from 1806 to 1812 … Edinburgh: Constable; London: Longman; Glasgow: Smith. Chiyonobu, Naohisa. 1935. Bingo-koku, Miyoshi-chô no hôgen. Hôgen:5:54-61. Clarke, Joan. 1994. Family traditions in Hawai‘i. Honolulu: Namkoong Publishing. Downes, William. 1998. Language and Society 2nd ed. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Eckert, Penny and Sally McConnell-Ginet. 1992. Think practically and look locally: language and gender as community based practice. Annual Review of Anthropology 21:461-490. Fukazawa, Seiji and Mie Hiramoto. 2004. Chûgoku dialect terms that remain in Hawaii Creole English. Bulletin of the Graduate School of Education, Hiroshima University, Part II, Vol. 53. 163-171. Kanbe, Hiroyasu. 1998. Hiroshima-ken no kotoba. In Teruo Hirayama et al. (eds.) Nihon no kotoba shiriizu 34. Tôkyô: Meiji Shoin. 121-233. Kanno, Hiroomi and Kiichi Iitoyo. 1967/1994. Gengo seikatsu. In Fumio Inoue, Kôichi Shinozaki, Takashi Kobayashi & Takuichirô Ônishi (eds.), Tôhoku Hôgen-kô vol.3: Iwate-ken, Miyagi-ken, Fukushima-ken, 312-347. Tôkyô: Yumani Shobô. Katô, Minoru. 1958/1966. Niigata-ken ni okeru Tôhoku hôgen teki onin to Echigo gogen teki onin no kyôkaichitai. In Fumio Inoue, Kôichi Shinozaki, Takashi Kobayashi & Takuichirô Ônishi (eds.), Hokuriku Hôgen-kô vol.1: Hokuriku ippan, Niigata-ken, 6376. Tôkyô: Yumani Shobô. Honolulu Star-Bulletin. 1997. Niihau: Island at a crossroad special report. http://archives.starbulletin.com/1997/07/14/features/story3.html 92 Language Variation Hawaii Hochisha (ed.) 2001. Aroha nenkan: Hawai no subete, 11th ed. Honolulu: Hawaii Hochisha. Hiramoto, Mie. 2010. Anime and intertextualities: Hegemonic identities in Cowboy Bebop. Pragmatics and Society 1: 235-257. Hiroshima City. (ed.) 2002. Furusato wa Hiroshima. Hiroshima: Sankô Shuppan. Inoue, Miyako. 2003. Speech without a speaking body: ‘Japanese women’s language’ in translation. Language & Communication 23: 315-330. Labov, William. 1963. The social motivation of a sound change. Word 19:273-309. Labov, William. 1972/1977. The social Stratification of (r) in New York City department stores. In Nikolas Coupland & Adam Jaworski (ed.), Sociolinguistics: a reader. New York: St. Martin’s Press. 168-178. Nagara, Susumu. 1972. Pidgin English of Japanese in Hawaii. Honolulu: University of Hawai‘i Press. National Language Institute of Japan, the. (ed.) 1979. Data concerning the change of individual kinship terms into age group terms. Linguistic sociological study on the kinship vocabulary of Japanese dialects (1). Tôkyô: The National Language Research Institute of Japan. 133-170. Odo, Franklin. 1998. Hawai no nisei. In Yukuji Okita (ed.), Hawaii nikkei shakai no bunka to sono henyô, 108-126. Tôkyô: Nakanisshya. Schütz, Albert J. (1994), The voices of Eden: a history of Hawaiian language studie., Honolulu: University of Hawai‘i Press Tokugawa, Munemasa and Willem Grootaers. 1951. Hogenchirigaku zushû. Tôkyô: Akiyama Shoten. Tomosada, Kenji. (ed.) 2000. Hiroshima no omoshiro hôgen-shû. Takamatsu: Shôrin-sha. 93