Uploaded by sallen2556

paying more

advertisement
Of particular interest is the part where the findings show that people
routinely overpay and/or buy more when pricing is broken into chunks
instead of aggregated into a single simple price. So I guess we can
thank ourselves for being morons when it comes to simple math and
the psychological impact on our decisions.
The last paragraph might be my favorite piece:
Our instincts as consumers are unfortunately the opposite: We punish
transparency, think we’re clever enough to figure out any complexity,
get sucked in by low offers then upgrade our ambitions, and conclude
it’s too much hassle to start over. When we shop that way, we have
mostly ourselves to blame.
Here's the article in full:
Who’s to Blame for All Those Hidden Fees? We Are
Four tests show why ‘drip pricing’ is so effective at getting us to pay
up
Add-on fees are driving consumers crazy. From restaurants and hotels
to concerts and food delivery, we are increasingly shown a low price
online, only to click through and find a range of fees that yield a much
higher price at checkout.
Everyone says they hate these fees, but four experiments illustrate
why “drip pricing,” as it’s called by researchers and regulators, is so
effective at getting us to pay more. Note that this isn’t about
emotional blackmail, as with tipping: Drip pricing isn’t negotiable. Nor
does it explain why the cost of living might seem higher than the
official data suggests: The consumer-price index reflects these fees
and taxes.
The experiments explain why the fees proliferate. The conclusion:
Consumers themselves are to blame. “Even when we know the fees
are coming, we underestimate their magnitude,” said Vicki Morwitz, a
marketing professor at Columbia University who studies consumer
psychology.
Consumers punish transparency
The term drip pricing was popularized by a 2012 Federal Trade
Commission conference. Its spread is associated with the proliferation
of airline fees after the Sept. 11, 2001, terrorist attacks. Yet an
example of the phenomenon that long predates 2001 is stores’
practice of listing goods without the sales tax, which gets added at
checkout.
Why not include the sales tax with the sticker price? A 2009 paper
from economist Raj Chetty, then at the University of California,
Berkeley, and co-authors, showed consumers punish that sort of
transparency.
A grocery store let the authors tag some products with the familiar
pretax price and some with the total price including tax. For example,
a hair brush’s price tag showed $5.79 before tax, and beneath that
$6.22 with the tax. Store managers predicted the transparency would
be a disaster, and permitted the experiment for only three weeks and
three product groups.
The managers were right. Sales volume dropped about 8% for
products with price tags that included the tax than a control group
without the tax.
This isn’t because shoppers didn’t know the tax rate or which items
were taxable. In fact, 75% of shoppers surveyed knew the sales tax
within 0.5 percentage point, and most knew what goods were taxable.
So the tax-inclusive price tag didn’t give them new information; it was
just that transparent reminders turned some people off.
People prefer ‘costly complexity’
For several years, Abigail Sussman at the University of Chicago and
colleagues have run a series of experiments asking participants to find
the best deal for a range of purchases, such as wedding venues,
prepaid cards, university tuition schedules, cellphone plans or home
closing costs.
They can then choose between complex or simplified disclosures. For
example, the complex disclosure for prepaid cards breaks down the
final price into things such as “initial fees,” “card acquisition fees,”
“card activation fees,” “service fees” and “administration fees.” The
simplified disclosure combines all these fees as a single “initial fee.”
If you realized this complexity tricks you into paying more for
equivalent products, you’re in the minority. Some 70% of people
preferred the complex disclosure, said Sussman. They believe that it’s
more transparent and that they can calculate the total cost by
themselves.
They’re wrong. Even when participants are offered cash to identify the
cheaper option, people botch the math (costly complexity!) and pick
more expensive options.
Lower prices prompt shoppers to upgrade
In 2013, the website StubHub, which resells event tickets, attempted
to do away with hidden fees, citing research about how hated they are.
Its new “all-in pricing” prominently displayed the total ticket cost from
the beginning of searches. The strategy failed to boost business or
attract more customers.
In 2015, shortly before abandoning all-in prices, StubHub did an
experiment—described several years later by economists who obtained
the data—where half of shoppers saw all-in pricing, and half saw the
lower base price with taxes and fees only added at the end. The latter
strategy boosted revenue 20%.
Shoppers didn’t just buy more tickets. When they saw lower prices
initially, they opted for better seats. By the end of the checkout
process, they were committed.
“When people get to the end of the process, there’s a variety of
psychological reasons they’re locked in,” said Morwitz. “They
overestimate the cost of starting over, they underestimate the
benefits.” Maybe they’re just excited about the purchase, or reluctant
to admit they could have made a mistake, she said.
On Thursday, two of the biggest U.S. ticket sellers, Ticketmaster and
SeatGeek announced a switch to more transparent pricing. Some
companies in the ticketing industry have said they would support all-in
prices if they were mandated for everyone. The StubHub example
shows why it’s hard for one company to buck an industrywide practice.
People think fees, like taxes, don’t alter relative prices
Morwitz and co-authors Shelle Santana of Bentley University and
Steven Dallas of Duke University recruited people to book airlines and
hotels for vacations, sometimes with rewards for finding the lowest
rate.
People went for the low base prices, of course. After all the additional
fees were dripped on, they had the option, not to mention incentive, to
start over and look for a cheaper alternative. Here’s the twist: Most
opted not to, erroneously believing drip prices worked sort of like
taxes, affecting the base price by a constant, fixed amount, the
authors concluded in their 2020 paper for Marketing Science.
It isn’t true. Drip prices can vary substantially between sellers, said
Morwitz, and the lowest base price won’t necessarily be the lowest
final price.
“For consumers, it’s hard,” said Morwitz. “You have to look at the
prices carefully, don’t make any decisions until you see the total and
be willing to restart the search.”
Our instincts as consumers are unfortunately the opposite: We punish
transparency, think we’re clever enough to figure out any complexity,
get sucked in by low offers then upgrade our ambitions, and conclude
it’s too much hassle to start over. When we shop that way, we have
mostly ourselves to blame.
Download