The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available at www.emeraldinsight.com/0268-3946.htm No regrets? An investigation of the relationship between being laid off and experiencing career regrets Sherry E. Sullivan Department of Management, College of Business Administration, Bowling Green, Ohio, USA Monica L. Forret Being laid off and career regrets 787 Received November 2006 Revised March 2007; June 2007 Accepted June 2007 Department of Managerial Studies, College of Business, St Ambrose University, Davenport, Iowa, USA, and Lisa A. Mainiero Charles F. Dolan School of Business, Fairfield University, Fairfield, Connecticut, USA Abstract Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to explore the under-examined topic of career regrets. Although much of the careers literature has examined factors that contribute to success, little research has been completed on the regrets individuals may experience as they enact their careers. Design/methodology/approach – A large internet sample of 1,480 professionals was used to examine whether individuals who had been laid off in their careers experienced greater career regrets. Findings – Individuals who had been laid off experienced greater regrets with regard to their political behavior (e.g. lack of networking, not being more politically savvy) and their career choices (e.g. not spending more time with their family, failing to pursue more meaningful work) than individuals who had not been laid off. Research limitations/implications – The findings may have limited generalizability because of the use of a cross-sectional, internet-based survey design. Practical implications – The regrets individuals have may influence their future career behaviors and choices, resulting in different approaches to their work and their lives. These findings may offer some insights to help individuals navigate their careers and make choices that they will be less likely to regret. Originality/value – Although a great deal of research has focused on strategies for career success, relatively little research has examined career missteps or career regrets. It is hoped this exploratory research encourages further study as well as the development of a theory-based model of career regrets. Keywords Careers, Career development, Employment, Redundancy, Individual behaviour, Organizational politics Paper type Research paper The authors would like to thank Hugh Davis for his support of the survey research that underscores this article. We also thank Mike Arthur and Jon Briscoe for their comments on an earlier version of this article as well as Jim Werbel and two anonymous JMP reviewers for their suggestions. Additional support was granted by the Charles F. Dolan School of Business for the development of this manuscript. Portions of this paper were presented at the 2006 Academy of Management Meeting in Atlanta, Georgia. Journal of Managerial Psychology Vol. 22 No. 8, 2007 pp. 787-804 q Emerald Group Publishing Limited 0268-3946 DOI 10.1108/02683940710837723 JMP 22,8 788 Although much anecdotal evidence has been reported in the popular press about people who experience numerous career regrets as well as those who claim to have no regrets at all, little empirical research exists on the regrets individuals may have about their careers. Given the many changes (e.g. frequent downsizings, rapid technological advances, globalization, increased workforce mobility) that have occurred in today’s turbulent career landscape (e.g. Arthur and Rousseau, 1996; Friedman, 2005; Hall, 1996, 2004; Kanter, 2001) studying career regrets is increasingly important because individuals face more career choices and alternatives. Comprehending what type of career regrets individuals experience may permit researchers to better understand how individuals enact their careers, provide greater insights into evolving career theory (e.g. boundaryless, protean career concepts; Arthur et al., 2005; Briscoe et al., 2006), and offer insights to individuals who are trying to navigate an ever more complex work environment. To date, however, there has been little empirical research on the reasons why people might develop career regrets over time. Why has there been little examination of career regrets? Research on careers has tended to focus on upward movement within one or two firms (e.g. Super, 1957) and how to succeed in such a work environment (Eddleston et al., 2004). Past research has focused on repeatedly winning rounds in the career tournament (see Ng et al., 2005, for a review; Rosenbaum, 1979) and how certain factors, like having a mentor (Baugh and Fagenson-Eland, 2005; Forret and de Janasz, 2005; Kram, 1985) can enhance career outcomes. Although workers in Western countries may change jobs much more often than in the past (Arthur and Rousseau, 1996) and increasingly seek intrinsic sources of satisfaction as well as extrinsic rewards (Hall, 1996, 2002), most research still focuses on the linear career model and defines success in terms of rate of promotions and salary increases (Arthur et al., 2005; Feldman, 1989). But not all workers are winners in today’s labor market. Given the widespread lay-offs and under-employment of white collar professionals, it is important that research examines not only the lessons on how to succeed but also the lessons that may be learned from failures, and the regrets individuals may have about their careers. Given the frequency of downsizing (or rightsizing) in the current economic landscape, this area seems ripe for the examination of the possible career regrets of white collar workers. For example, there was an increase in the job loss rate among the more educated (at least 16 years of education) in the 1990s (5.4 percent in 1987-89; 6.9 percent in 1997-1999), with this rate reaching 9.9 percent in the period between 2001 and 2003 (Farber, 2005). The percentage of college graduates who lost their jobs and later were reemployed dropped from 82 percent in 1990 to 74 percent in 1992 to 68 percent in 2002. Also, in sharp contrast to past trends, since 1999 the average decline in earnings for college graduates who lost their jobs was greater than for those with less education (Farber, 2005). Getting laid off is a traumatic event, and may well be one that is likely to elicit feelings of regret as to what individuals could (or should) have done to avoid being placed in such circumstances. The purpose of this paper is to explore the under-researched topic of career regrets against the backdrop of the continually shifting, contemporary career landscape. Our research examines the career regrets that individuals may experience during the course of their careers. We explored two major types of regrets: regrets about not engaging in more effective political behavior at work (e.g. building a stronger network, paying attention to corporate signals); and regrets about not pursuing alternative career choices (e.g. pursuing more meaningful work, focusing more on family). We also investigated whether individuals who had been laid off would be more likely to experience greater regrets as to their political behavior and career choices. Conceptualizing how individuals make sense of their own careers – the successes and the failures, with their associated regrets about past behaviors and choices they made – is more imperative than ever given that careers are no longer viewed as bounded by a single organization, industry, profession, or even country (Arthur and Rousseau, 1996) nor are they solely characterized by upward advancement (Cappelli, 2000) or extrinsic rewards (Hall, 1996; Heslin, 2005). Individuals, especially those without a long-term relationship with an organization, are increasingly responsible for their own career management (Hall, 1996; Mallon and Walton, 2005) and may particularly benefit from insights about how to avoid career regrets when possible, or learn from their own regrets or the regrets experienced by others. To address the lack of research on career regrets, this article begins by reviewing past research on regrets (e.g. consumer and personal regrets) and detailing specific hypotheses on the relationship between being laid off and experiencing career regrets. This is followed by our research methods and analyses to test these hypotheses. Finally, we present our results and discuss the implications for future scholarship and practice. Regrets The emotion of regret has been defined as remorse about outcomes of a decision different from the ones hoped for (Baron, 2000; Zeelenberg et al., 1998) or which compares unfavorably with real or imagined outcomes of alternatives not chosen (van Dijk and Zeelenberg, 2005). Scholars in a variety of disciplines, such as marketing, medicine, sexuality, psychology and economics, have studied different types of regrets, ranging from regrets about consumer purchases (Inman and Zeelenberg, 2002; Lin and Huang, 2006; Reynolds et al., 2006; Zeelenberg et al., 2006, Zeelenberg and Pieters, 2004), health choices (Abraham and Sheeran, 2004; Chapman and Coups, 2006; Connolly and Reb, 2005), sexual relationships (Oswalt et al., 2005; Roese et al., 2006), economic decisions (Connolly and Butler, 2006), actions during conflicts and negotiations (Van Kleef et al., 2006), and other life choices (Roese and Summerville, 2005). Who is most prone to regrets (Markman et al., 2002, 2005), how even individuals who experience exceptional accomplishments are not immune to regrets (Medvec et al., 1995; Roese and Summerville, 2005), and how regrets affect customer satisfaction and behaviors (e.g. Zeelenberg et al., 2006) have also been examined. Overall, the research suggests that the greatest regrets stem from situations in which individuals view the greatest possibility for change or corrective action (Roese and Summerville, 2005). In a meta-analysis of 11 regret-ranking studies, Americans ranked education as their number one regret, perhaps because various educational opportunities (e.g. community college, professional certifications, equivalency exams, distance learning) are seen as available to most individuals. Americans ranked careers as their number two regret, with romance, parenting, the self, and leisure, respectively, rounding out the top six (Roese and Summerville, 2005). Although little research has focused on business regrets (see Markman et al., 2002, for a study of entrepreneurs), the idea of reducing potential regrets, and the subsequent Being laid off and career regrets 789 JMP 22,8 790 negative outcomes (e.g. dysfunctional turnover, job dissatisfaction), is the underlying foundation of a number of organizational processes. For instance, the use of realistic job previews during the organizational entry process to increase retention and job satisfaction (Blenkinsopp and Zdunczyk, 2005; Wanous, 1992) and the use of training to reduce the culture shock that often leads expatriate managers to prematurely terminate international job assignments (e.g. Black and Gregersen, 1999) are two such examples. In the next section, we hope to contribute to a greater understanding of the experiences of individuals in today’s changing work environment by specifically discussing career regrets and suggesting hypotheses for examining this under-researched topic. Career regrets Because we could find no published studies specifically on career regrets, our approach to understanding regrets within the context of careers is exploratory and focuses on two major types of regrets. The first type we examined is those regrets associated with political behaviors. Given the long-held view of careers as a linear progression up the corporate ladder where success is rewarded with additional pay and promotional opportunities (e.g. Super, 1957), individuals may regret not being more savvy to corporate politics, including choosing allies more carefully and effectively networking, to enhance their career advancement and associated extrinsic outcomes (Forret and Dougherty, 2004; Judge and Bretz, 1994). The second type we examined is those regrets associated with career choices. In contrast to the traditional models, more recent models of careers suggest that individuals are redefining career success beyond extrinsic outcomes (Briscoe et al., 2006; Hall and Chandler, 2005). People may regret not engaging in behaviors that would have provided enhanced personal or spiritual fulfillment such as going back to school and pursuing a new career path, leaving the corporate world and becoming self-employed, or creating a better work/family balance for themselves (Friedman and Greenhaus, 2000; Hall, 1996; Heslin, 2005; Powell and Mainiero, 1992). To explore this issue further, we examined the career event of experiencing a layoff with these two types of regrets. We suggest that individuals who have been laid off from an organization are more likely to have regrets about their political behavior as well as regrets about their career choices than those who have not been downsized. The psychological contract between employee and employer has changed (Cappelli, 2000; Hall, 1996) and even employers known for their promise of life-long employment have engaged in downsizing and rightsizing (Rousseau and Wade-Benzoni, 1995). Research on the effects of layoffs, on both those terminated and those who remain, report many negative outcomes (Brockner et al., 1992; Leana and Feldman, 1991, 1994; Power, 2006). Besides the loss of income and status associated with losing one’s job, Feldman and Leana (2000) found that downsized managers have lower trust and are more cynical about their careers. Having misplaced their trust in former employers, the negative attitudes of laid off workers spillover and adversely impact their perceptions of their careers. Because of societal definitions of success, with the explicit notion that “up is the only way” (Kaye, 2002), individuals experiencing layoffs may feel like losers, even those who previously had been successful executives (Kovach, 1989; McCall et al., 1988a, b). For instance, Shipper and Dillard (2000) found that individuals blamed themselves for not taking the necessary actions to prevent career derailment. Therefore, individuals who were laid off may regret not engaging in political behaviors that may have prevented their dismissal. They may also regret the time invested in jobs that “died” and wish they had made different career choices such as devoting more time elsewhere (e.g. family, personal pursuits) or choosing a different career path (e.g. self-employment). Therefore we hypothesize: Being laid off and career regrets H1. Individuals who report they were laid off will experience greater regrets about their political behaviors than those who were not laid off. 791 H2. Individuals who report they were laid off will experience greater regrets about their career choices than those who were not laid off. Method Survey administration and sample Our survey was posted on a web site of one of the largest established marketing research panels in the USA. This firm is able to employ a variety of statistics and structural methods to ensure varied sample composition and sufficient response rates, thereby increasing the likelihood of obtaining a representative sample. The firm draws on approximately 10,000 individuals of various ages, races, backgrounds, industries, and job titles. These individuals have agreed to participate in research conducted by the firm for a fee, with respondents required to participate in two surveys per month to maintain an active status. By participating in surveys, respondents’ logins are also entered into a drawing for a chance to win a $100.00 cash prize. Respondents are not required to purchase products for market research purposes. Typically, surveys are made available on the web site until percentages and numbers as requested by the researcher are obtained; we made no requests so as to avoid biasing the sample in any way. Moreover, the posted survey contained only our questions. The firm reported that the response rate for the survey was 22 percent, which they considered very good, given typical response rates. After removing those who were self-employed (n ¼ 167), our sample size was 1,480 and was almost evenly split between men (n ¼ 764) and women (n ¼ 716). Respondents reported their ethnic status as Caucasian (88 percent), African-American (4 percent), Asian (3 percent), Hispanic or Latin American (2 percent), Native American (1 percent), and Other/Prefer Not to Say (2 percent); 31 percent were between 18-34 years old, 30 percent between 35-44, 28 percent between 45-54, and 11 percent were 55 and older; 46 percent of the respondents reported having children in the household, and 11 percent reported having eldercare responsibilities. The educational level of the respondents varied: 14 percent were high school graduates; 65 percent attended some college or graduated from college; and 21 percent had completed some graduate work or held a postgraduate degree. Respondents worked in a wide variety of industries, ranging from education (13 percent), government (11 percent), healthcare (10 percent), retail (9 percent), finance/banking (5 percent), computers (4 percent), telecommunications (3 percent), engineering/ architecture (3 percent), and others (42 percent). The respondents worked an average of 42 hours per week. Of the respondents, 28 percent (n ¼ 412) reported having been laid off. On average, respondents were 36 years old and had 11 years of employment at the time they were laid off. JMP 22,8 792 Dependent variables Because no prior empirical research had been completed on career regrets, we developed a 12-item scale to measure political behavior regrets and career choice regrets based on a review of the careers literature. Using a Likert scale ranging from strongly disagree ¼ 1 to strongly agree ¼ 5, respondents indicated their level of agreement with the 12 items listed in Table I. To examine whether the two types of career regrets were distinct constructs, a principal components factor analysis with a varimax rotation was performed on the scale. Examination of the scree plot and the eigenvalue-greater-than-1 criteria suggested that two factors be retained. The factor analysis results are shown in Table I. Next, scales were created by calculating the means of the items that: loaded 0.40 or greater on that factor; did not load 0.40 or greater on any other factor; and loaded on that factor at least 0.10 higher than on any other factor. Such procedures have been used in other research on careers (e.g. Forret and Dougherty, 2001). Item Table I. Rotated factor analysis of career regrets scale 1. I wish I had been more successful at networking in general, as I have lost many career opportunities due to my inability to build a strong network 2. I wish I had been more of a strong corporate player. Instead I often chose a more innovative or entrepreneurial path than my boss appreciated 3. I wish I had paid full attention to corporate signals and political changes in the corporations in which I worked. My inattention has caused setbacks in my career from time to time. 4. I wish I had changed jobs more often 5. I wish I had taken a career interruption and/or moved out of the workforce for a period of time to better serve the needs of my family 6. I wish I had gone back to school and restarted another career path that was better suited to me 7. I wish I could have worked harder than I did, but it was not worth compromising my family or personal needs to work so many hours on the job 8. I wish I had chosen my friends and allies more carefully, as their lack of support has cost me a measure of success in my career 9. I wish I had developed stronger skills at managing others along the way 10. I wish I had been more of a maverick and did work that suited me rather than doing what the boss says 11. I wish I had made my own path and did work that suited me creatively or entrepreneurially rather than working for a corporation 12. I wish I had stayed focused on my family as my priority Political behavior regrets Career choice regrets 0.80 0.18 0.63 0.29 0.73 0.36 0.21 0.47 0.06 0.80 0.26 0.52 0.28 0.56 0.77 0.18 0.63 0.24 0.37 0.56 0.33 0.54 0.09 0.71 Note: The numbers in italic represent items used to create the scale for that factor Factor 1 was labeled political behavior regrets. It is comprised of five items (i.e. items 1, 2, 3, 8, and 9 in Table I) and measures the extent to which respondents wished they had behaved differently (e.g. built a stronger network, paid attention to corporate signals, developed stronger managerial skills) in order to advance up the corporate ladder and gain more extrinsic rewards. The coefficient alpha for this scale was 0.81. Factor 2 was labeled career choice regrets. It is comprised of seven items (i.e. items 4, 5, 6, 7, 10, 11, and 12 in Table I) and measures the extent to which respondents wished they had taken more action (e.g. pursued more creative work, went back to school, focused more on family) that would have resulted in enhanced personal satisfaction and fulfillment. The coefficient alpha for this scale is 0.77. Independent variable Respondents indicated whether they had ever been laid off (0 ¼ no, 1 ¼ yes). Control variables Control variables for the study were grouped into two categories: personal background variables and human capital variables. The personal background variables consisted of gender (0 ¼ male, 1 ¼ female), age (1 ¼ 18-24, 2 ¼ 25-34, 3 ¼ 35-44, 4 ¼ 45-54, 5 ¼ 55-64, and 6 ¼ 65 and older), children in the household (0 ¼ no, 1 ¼ yes), eldercare responsibilities (0 ¼ no, 1 ¼ yes), and household income (1 ¼ less than $20,000, 2 ¼ $20,000-29,000, 3 ¼ $30,000-39,000, 4 ¼ $40,000-49,000, 5 ¼ $50,000-59,000, 6 ¼ $60,000-74,000, 7 ¼ $75,000-99,000, 8 ¼ $100,000-199,000, and 9 ¼ $200,000 and above). The human capital variables measured were education (on a scale from 1 ¼ some high school to 7 ¼ post graduate degree), and the number of hours respondents worked in a typical week. Both categories of control variables were selected for inclusion in the study as they provide insight as to the resources and constraints that might influence people’s careers and are consistent with past research. Results The correlations, means, and standard deviations of the study variables are shown in Table II. Being laid off was significantly associated with both political behavior regrets (r ¼ 0:18) and career choice regrets (r ¼ 0:08) in the hypothesized direction. A t-test was computed to examine the differences in means for the two types of regrets based upon whether the respondent had been laid off. The mean of political behavior regrets was 2.80 for those who were laid off and 2.43 for those who had not been laid off. In addition, the mean of career choice regrets was 2.59 for those who were laid off and 2.44 for those who had not been laid off. Both t-tests were statistically significant. Further examination of the correlation matrix shows that those who were laid off were more likely to be male, older, without children living in the household, at a lower income level, and less educated. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were performed, with the personal background control variables entered as the first block, followed by the human capital control variables in the second block, and the laid off variable in the third block. The results of these regression analyses are shown in Table III. Being laid off was significantly related to experiencing greater political behavior regrets (b ¼ 0:19) and career choice regrets (b ¼ 0:09), providing support for H1 and H2, respectively. Being laid off and career regrets 793 Table II. Correlations, means, and standard deviations of study variables 0.92 0.81 0.45 0.50 1.12 0.50 0.31 2.09 1.62 11.46 (0.81) 0.62 * * 0.18 * * 2 0.07 * * 2 0.03 2 0.02 2 0.01 2 0.03 0.03 0.03 1 (0.77) 0.08 * * 0.05 20.03 0.13 * * 0.03 20.07 * * 20.13 * * 20.04 2 – 20.13 * * 0.19 * * 20.08 * * 0.03 20.05 * 20.07 * * 0.01 3 – 2 0.28 * * 0.08 * * 0.04 2 0.11 * * 2 0.05 2 0.27 * * 4 – 2 0.06 * 0.08 * * 0.13 * * 2 0.01 0.03 5 Notes: The numbers on the diagonal represent the coefficient alpha for the scale; *p , 0.05; * *p , 001 2.53 2.48 0.28 0.48 3.12 0.46 0.11 4.86 4.19 42.26 SD – 0.00 0.07 * * 20.05 20.08 * * 6 – 0.01 0.02 20.01 7 – 0.33 * * 0.16 * * 8 794 1. Political behavior regrets 2. Career choice regrets 3. Laid off (0 ¼ N, 1 ¼ Y) 4. Gender (0 ¼ M, 1 ¼ F) 5. Age 6. Children (0 ¼ N, 1 ¼ Y) 7. Elder care (0 ¼ N, 1 ¼ Y) 8. Household income 9. Education 10. Hours worked Mean – 0.11 * * 9 – 10 JMP 22,8 Political behavior regrets Unique DR2 Career choice regrets Unique DR2 20.07 * 20.07 * * 0.00 20.01 20.03 0.01 * 0.02 2 0.03 0.13 * * 0.03 2 0.03 0.02 * * Human capital variables Education Hours worked 0.05 0.00 0.00 2 0.11 * * 2 0.01 0.01 * * Independent variable Laid off (0 ¼ N, 1 ¼ Y) 0.19 * * 0.03 * * 0.09 * * 0.01 * * R2 Adjusted R 2 0.04 * * 0.04 Personal background variables Gender (0 ¼ M, 1 ¼ F) Age Children (0 ¼ N, 1 ¼ Y) Elder Care (0 ¼ N, 1 ¼ Y) Household Income Being laid off and career regrets 795 0.04 * * 0.04 Notes: The regression coefficients shown are standardized; *p , 0.05; * *p , 0.01 The control variables presented some interesting statistically significant results. Males and younger respondents were more likely to experience political behavior regrets. In addition, those with children and those with less education were more likely to experience regrets pertaining to their career choices. Overall, while both regression models were significant and the laid off variable contributed unique incremental variance, we note that the percentage of variance accounted for in both models was low (i.e. R 2 ¼ 0:04). Discussion Our research examines career regrets that individuals may experience during the course of their careers. We explored two major types of regrets: regrets about not engaging in more effective political behavior at work (e.g. building a stronger network, paying attention to corporate signals); and regrets about not pursuing alternative career choices (e.g. pursuing more meaningful work, focusing more on family). We investigated whether individuals who had been laid off would be more likely to experience greater regrets as to their political behavior and career choices. In light of the stress and turmoil associated with downsizing, it seemed appropriate that the topic of career regrets be examined so a better understanding of the complexities of how careers are enacted in the contemporary work landscape can be gleaned. Consistent with previous research that illustrates the negative effects of job loss (e.g. Leana and Feldman, 1994), the present study’s findings supported both H1 and H2 in that individuals who had been laid off experienced greater regrets associated with both political behaviors and career choices. Individuals who have been laid off may engage in self-blame as they reflect upon actions they could have taken to enhance their job security. Additionally, they may feel their efforts to help their organization achieve success were wasted, and their time and energies could have been better spent Table III. Regression equations JMP 22,8 796 elsewhere (e.g. pursuing self-employment options or going back to school to further their education). Those who were laid off were more likely to have greater regrets about their political behavior and, to a lesser extent, their career choices. These individuals’ regrets about their political behaviors are consistent with the large number of studies that have examined the importance of political behavior to career success (e.g. Judge and Bretz, 1994; Perrewé and Nelson, 2004). Individuals may believe that they have greater control over their political behaviors, thinking that political skills such as networking and choosing allies can be more readily accomplished. In contrast, individuals may believe they have less control over their career choices, thinking that alternatives are constrained by factors including their spouse’s job mobility, child and elder care demands, availability of resources for continued training and education, and the difficulties associated with balancing work and nonwork concerns. We also noted some interesting findings related to the control variables used in this study. Our results showed that men and younger individuals were more likely to experience regrets about their political behavior. Whereas research examining gender and age as antecedents of political behavior has tended to be nonsupportive (Kacmar and Baron, 1999) and few gender differences in networking behavior have been found (Forret and Dougherty, 2001), it appears that men and younger individuals regret not being more savvy about workplace politics and altering their behavior. Perhaps men experience greater regrets about political behavior than women because they perceive a level playing field and make internal attributions regarding the source of their regrets. In contrast, women may perceive that the workplace, especially in male-dominated industries, provides fewer opportunities for them regardless of their political skills. The finding that younger individuals had more political regrets than older individuals is somewhat surprising. One might expect that older individuals would have more behaviors to regret while perceiving less time to recover from errors and make things right. However, older individuals may have learned mental strategies for more quickly overcoming disappointments or to recognize the futility of regrets especially in terms of things that cannot be changed. In addition to gender and age, there were noteworthy findings associated with two other control variables. Having less education and having children, respectively, were associated with greater career choice regrets. Those with less education may have experienced blocked opportunities and feel they should have chosen a different path. In addition, one interpretation for the finding regarding children is that organizations have not made sufficient inroads for employees to satisfactorily balance work and family needs (Burud and Tumolo, 2004). As such, individuals expressing regrets as to their career choices may desire alternatives such as pursuing a new career path or self-employment to enhance their flexibility and give them more control over how they manage work and nonwork demands. Research on women entrepreneurs, for instance, has found that many women leave corporations in order to build companies that permit them to nurture their family while experiencing career challenge (Moore, 2002) or to create companies that promote better work/life balance (Mainiero and Sullivan, 2005, 2006). Limitations of the study Although this study suggests a number of interesting lines for future research, several limitations must be noted when considering its findings. Despite support for both hypotheses, the amount of variance explained in career regrets by the laid off variable was small. This was an exploratory study, and the limited effects may reflect the need for a better measure of career regrets or the inclusion of other variables that predict career regrets. Future research on the career regret scales would be helpful for demonstrating construct validity. In addition, future research should include other variables that examine the circumstances and perceptions surrounding a layoff. For instance, if individuals were involved in massive downsizing efforts by their organizations, they may attribute being laid off more to the fault of their organization as opposed to their own lack of political savvy or choice of career. Future research should examine how the individual’s perceived cause of his/her lay-off as well as the number of coworkers also laid off may affect regrets. Furthermore, how commonplace layoffs are in the industry and among an individual’s family and friends may also affect an individual’s career regrets and account for additional variance. Second, there have been some questions about the generalizability of internet-based surveys (Birnbaum, 2004; Couper, 2000). We had strong reasons, however, for choosing this technology. One reason for our use of this emerging research technology is the ability to access a large and varied survey population. Our survey was listed on the web site of one of the largest established marketing research panels in the USA, and this firm is able to employ a variety of methods to ensure varied sample composition and sufficient response rates, thereby increasing the likelihood of obtaining a representative sample. Another reason for our use of this technology is that recent research has found that web-based samples compare favorably to traditional survey methods. Although internet samples may not be fully representative of the population at large, they are usually more diverse than samples published in highly selective psychology journals utilizing student populations (Gosling et al., 2004). For example, Gosling and associates (2004) reported that internet samples were more representative than traditional pencil-and-paper samples with respect to gender, socioeconomic status, geographic location, and age, and were about as representative as traditional samples with regard to race. Other researchers (e.g. Berrens et al., 2003, 2004) have found internet samples to be comparable to large-scale telephone samples in terms of representing the general population. Third, like most of the research in our field, our study was cross-sectional. Because data were collected at one point in time no statements about causality can be made. Future longitudinal studies concerning the development of career regrets over time would help us better comprehend how career regrets might vary throughout the life span. Future research should also consider other variables (e.g. number of times laid off, stage in life when layoff occurred, organizational level, number of promotions, locus of control and other personality factors) that were not included in this study but may explain additional variance. For instance, career satisfaction was not measured. It may be that those who engaged in regretful thinking were less satisfied with their careers (or their lives in general). Likewise, there may be other factors such as personality traits or differences in whether individuals evaluate regrets in relation to other-referent or self-referent criteria, and how salient different career outcomes are to individuals that may influence the degree to which individuals regret the past (see Heslin, 2005). Also, in the spirit of enhancing research through multiple methods, we recommend that qualitative studies be completed. Such qualitative studies may Being laid off and career regrets 797 JMP 22,8 798 provide a richer understanding of career regrets across the lifespan and be the basis for further theory development. Fourth, because one survey was used to collect the data, our findings may be subject to common method variance. However, the presence of demographic and otherwise verifiable information (e.g. getting laid off) makes common method variance less of a concern (Crampton and Wagner, 1994; Podsakoff and Organ, 1986). To test for common method variance, we conducted Harman’s one-factor test by including the study variables in an unrotated factor analysis (Podsakoff and Organ, 1986). The results broke into several factors, providing some evidence against method variance problems in the data. Implications and directions for future research Despite these potential limitations, this study makes several contributions to the literature on careers. First, while having regrets about one’s career is a premise of many of the established developmental career theories (Sullivan and Crocitto, 2007) to our knowledge this is the first study to empirically explore career regrets. Future studies of how retirees reflect back upon their careers and perceive how political tactics used or ignored or career choices made led to good or bad outcomes may provide further insights into how and why career regrets develop and test commonly held beliefs about career regrets (e.g. greater regrets for actions not taken, than for actions taken that ended in failure). Future research should also explore the consequences of career regrets for both individuals and organizations. For instance, experiencing career regrets may cause individuals to lose their self-confidence and result in feelings of resignation or being “stuck” in their jobs. From an organizational perspective, such feelings may translate into lower performance levels, reduced organizational citizenship behavior, decreased trust and increased cynicism if employees believe that their career opportunities are limited. A second contribution of this study is that unlike much of the literature, it focuses on career regrets rather than career success. Research on traditional careers tends to focus on success and most of the writings on nontraditional careers (e.g. boundaryless, protean) have also emphasized the positive aspects of making career transitions such as the learning benefits derived by working for many different employers (Heslin, 2005; Sullivan, 1999). For instance, nontraditional career theories tend to assume that workers can easily transition from job to job, organization to organization and the like; in other words, these theories suggest that individuals are what Atkinson (1984) calls “plug-in workers.” Individuals, however, must not only have transferable knowledge in their field in order to be hired by these different employers but also must rapidly gain an understanding of the setting in which they work (Blenkinsopp and Zdunczyk, 2005). Personally experiencing being laid off is likely to be accompanied by a second-guessing of one’s past behavior and career moves, which may shake one’s confidence and make it even more difficult to transition to a new employer. Our emphasis on political behavior and career choice regrets is thus a departure from past research examining success factors. A third contribution of this exploratory study is that it is the first, to our knowledge, to examine career regrets, and suggests future avenues of research. Despite increased conceptualization of nontraditional careers, research has still tended to focus on traditional careers and career success rather than career missteps (Arthur et al., 2005). By examining career regrets, we hoped to shed further light on the complexities of contemporary careers, both the good and the bad. Although this study focused on the repercussions of experiencing a layoff, we suggest that future research consider the possible negative aspects of nontraditional careers (e.g. obstacles to remaining employable, stress of repeatedly seeking out project work or adapting to new employers) and examine the potential for career regrets related to different types of career transitions and events. For instance, while many nontraditional career theories assume that people have equal chances to pursue alternative career paths, in reality this is not always the case. Societal norms and expectations may restrict women’s occupational, educational, and job choices while other factors such as discrimination, sexual harassment and work policies may limit their advancement opportunities, resulting in women having less physical mobility than men. In contrast, men may perceive fewer career possibilities than women (i.e. have less psychological mobility) because societal norms may put them into mental straitjackets, requiring them to conform to traditional work and family (i.e. breadwinner) roles (Mainiero and Sullivan, 2006; Sullivan and Arthur, 2006). Future research should examine obstacles that may deter career choices or tactics used and how other factors, such as cultural norms and individual differences, may also limit the extent to which people can enact nontraditional careers. Scholars should also examine how organizational actions (or lack of response) and other players in an individual’s career (e.g. managers, colleagues, and mentors), may positively or negatively affect the regrets experienced. It is our hope that this study of career regrets encourages scholars to take a closer look at assumptions about the ease of career transitions, factors that could affect the transition process, and how regrets about past career strategies and choices made may influence future career decisions (e.g. perceptions of psychological and physical mobility). Moreover, we hope this study encourages scholars to conceptualize how career regrets occur and develop theory-based models to explain the process of career regrets over the lifespan. In conclusion, the study of career regrets represents a productive and fruitful arena for future research on the subject of careers. The regrets individuals have may influence their future career behaviors and choices, resulting in different approaches to their work and their lives. These findings may offer some insights to help individuals navigate their careers and make choices that they will be less likely to regret. Moreover, the regrets individuals experience may provide career counselors and coaches with a better understanding of how careers unfold. Such knowledge may also help provide more effective career counseling for those individuals who have experienced career missteps. Furthermore, longitudinal studies of career regrets may help us to understand why the proverbial “path not taken” could serve as an impetus for life and career decisions that individuals must reconcile over time. References Abraham, C. and Sheeran, P. (2004), “Deciding to exercise: the role of anticipated regret”, British Journal of Health Psychology, Vol. 9 No. 2, pp. 269-78. Arthur, M.B. and Rousseau, D.M. (1996), The Boundaryless Career, Oxford University Press, New York, NY. Being laid off and career regrets 799 JMP 22,8 800 Arthur, M.B., Khapova, S.N. and Wilderom, C.P.M. (2005), “Career success in a boundaryless career world”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 26 No. 2, pp. 177-202. Atkinson, J. (1984), “Managing strategies for flexible organizations”, Personnel Management, Vol. 16 No. 8, pp. 28-31. Baron, R.A. (2000), “Counterfactual thinking and venture formation: the potential effect of thinking about what might have been”, Journal of Business Venturing, Vol. 15, pp. 275-94. Baugh, S.G. and Fagenson-Eland, E.A. (2005), “Boundaryless mentoring: an exploratory study of the functions provided by internal versus external organizational mentors”, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Vol. 35, pp. 939-55. Berrens, R.P., Bohara, A.K., Jenkins-Smith, H.C., Silva, C.L. and Weimer, D.L. (2003), “The advent of internet surveys for political research: a comparison of telephone and internet samples”, Political Analysis, Vol. 11, pp. 1-22. Berrens, R.P., Bohara, A.K., Jenkins-Smith, H.C., Silva, C.L. and Weimer, D.L. (2004), “Telephone versus internet samples for a national advisory referendum: are the underlying stated preferences the same?”, Applied Economics Letters, Vol. 11, pp. 173-6. Birnbaum, M.H. (2004), “Methodological and ethical issues in conducting social psychological research via the internet”, in Sansone, C., Morf, C.C. and Painter, A.T. (Eds), Handbook of Methods in Social Psychology, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA, pp. 359-82. Black, J.S. and Gregersen, H.B. (1999), “The right way to manage expats”, Harvard Business Review, Vol. 77 No. 2, pp. 52-63. Blenkinsopp, J. and Zdunczyk, K. (2005), “Making sense of mistakes in managerial careers”, Career Development International, Vol. 10 No. 5, pp. 359-74. Briscoe, J.P., Hall, D.T. and DeMuth, R.L.F. (2006), “Protean and boundaryless careers: an empirical exploration”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 69 No. 1, pp. 30-47. Brockner, J., Grover, S., Reed, T. and DeWitt, R.L. (1992), “Layoffs, job insecurity, and survivors’ work effort”, Academy of Management Journal, Vol. 35, pp. 413-25. Burud, S. and Tumolo, M. (2004), Leveraging the New Human Capital, Davies-Black, Palo Alto, CA. Cappelli, P. (2000), The New Deal at Work, Harvard Business School Press, Boston, MA. Chapman, G.B. and Coups, E.J. (2006), “Emotions and preventive health behavior: worry, regret, and influenza vaccination”, Health Psychology, Vol. 25 No. 1, pp. 82-90. Connolly, T. and Butler, D. (2006), “Regrets in economic and psychological theories of choice”, Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, Vol. 19, pp. 139-54. Connolly, T. and Reb, J. (2005), “Regret in cancer-related decisions”, Health Psychology, Vol. 24 No. 4, pp. 29-34. Couper, M.P. (2000), “Web surveys: a review of issues and approaches”, Public Opinion Quarterly, Vol. 64, pp. 464-94. Crampton, S. and Wagner, J. III (1994), “Percept-percept inflation in microorganizational research: an investigation of prevalence and effect”, Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 79, pp. 67-76. Eddleston, K.A., Baldridge, D.C. and Veiga, J.F. (2004), “Toward modeling the predictors of managerial career success: does gender matter?”, Journal of Managerial Psychology, Vol. 19 No. 4, pp. 360-85. Farber, H.S. (2005), “What do we know about job loss in the US? Evidence from the displaced workers survey, 1984-2004”, working paper #498, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, available at: www.irs.princeton.edu/pubs/pdfs/498.pdf (accessed 03-11-07). Feldman, D.C. (1989), “Careers in organizations: recent trends and future directions”, Journal of Management, Vol. 15 No. 2, pp. 135-56. Feldman, D.C. and Leana, C.R. (2000), “A study of reemployment challenges after downsizing”, Organizational Dynamics, Vol. 29 No. 1, pp. 64-75. Forret, M. and Dougherty, T.D. (2001), “Correlates of networking behavior for managerial and professional employees”, Group and Organization Management, Vol. 26 No. 3, pp. 283-311. Forret, M.L. and Dougherty, T.W. (2004), “Networking behaviors and career outcomes: differences for men and women?”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 25, pp. 1-19. Forret, M. and de Janasz, S. (2005), “Perceptions of an organization’s culture for work and family: do mentors make a difference?”, Career Development International, Vol. 10 Nos 6/7, pp. 478-92. Friedman, S.D. and Greenhaus, J.H. (2000), Work and Family – Allies or Enemies? What Happens When Business Professionals Confront Life Choices, Oxford University Press, New York, NY. Friedman, T. (2005), The World is Flat: A Brief History of the 21st Century, Farrar, Strauss & Giroux, New York, NY. Gosling, S.D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S. and John, O.P. (2004), “Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about internet questionnaires”, American Psychologist, Vol. 59, pp. 93-104. Hall, D.T. (1996), The Career is Dead – Long Live the Career, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA. Hall, D.T. (2002), Careers In and Out of Organizations, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA. Hall, D.T. (2004), “The protean career: a quarter-century journey”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 65, pp. 1-13. Hall, D.T. and Chandler, D.E. (2005), “Psychological success: when the career is a calling”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 26, pp. 155-76. Heslin, P.A. (2005), “Conceptualizing and evaluating career success”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 26, pp. 113-36. Inman, J.J. and Zeelenberg, M. (2002), “Regret in repeat purchase versus switching decisions: the attenuating role of decision justifiability”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 29 No. 1, pp. 116-28. Judge, T.A. and Bretz, R.D. (1994), “Political influence behavior and career success”, Journal of Management, Vol. 20, pp. 43-65. Kacmar, K.M. and Baron, R.A. (1999), “Organizational politics: the state of the field, links to related processes, and an agenda for future research”, in Ferris, G.R. (Ed.), Research in Personnel and Human Resource Management, JAI Press, Greenwich, CT, pp. 1-39. Kanter, R.M. (2001), Evolve! Succeeding in the Digital Culture of Tomorrow, Harvard Business School Press, Boston, MA. Kaye, B. (2002), Up is Not the Only Way, Davies-Black, Palo Alto, CA. Kovach, B.E. (1989), “Successful derailment: what fast-trackers can learn while they’re off the track”, Organizational Dynamics, Vol. 18, pp. 33-7. Kram, K.E. (1985), Mentoring at Work: Developmental Relationships in Organizational Life, Scott Foresman, Glenview, IL. Leana, C.R. and Feldman, D.C. (1991), “Gender differences in responses to unemployment”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 38, pp. 65-77. Leana, C.R. and Feldman, D.C. (1994), “The psychology of job loss”, in Ferris, G.R. (Ed.), Research in Personnel and Human Resource Management, JAI Press, Greenwich, CT, pp. 271-302. Being laid off and career regrets 801 JMP 22,8 802 Lin, C. and Huang, W. (2006), “The influence of unawareness set and order on effects in consumer regret”, Journal of Business and Psychology, Vol. 21 No. 2, pp. 293-311. McCall, M.W., Lombardo, M.M. and McCauley, C.D. (1988a), The Dynamics of Management Derailment, Tech report 34, Center for Creative Leadership, Greensboro, NC. McCall, M.W., Lombardo, M.M. and Morrison, A. (1988b), The Lessons of Experience: How Successful Executives Develop on the Job, Lexington Books, New York, NY. Mainiero, L.A. and Sullivan, S.E. (2005), “Kaleidoscope careers: an alternative explanation for the opt-out revolution”, Academy of Management Executive, Vol. 19 No. 1, pp. 106-23. Mainiero, L.A. and Sullivan, S.E. (2006), The Opt-out Revolt: Why People are Leaving Companies to Create Kaleidoscope Careers, Davies-Black, Palo Alto, CA. Mallon, M. and Walton, S. (2005), “Career and learning: the ins and the outs of it”, Career Development International, Vol. 34 No. 4, pp. 468-87. Markman, G.D., Balkin, D.B. and Baron, R.A. (2002), “Inventors and new venture formation: the effects of general self-efficacy and regretful thinking”, Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, Vol. 27 No. 2, pp. 149-65. Markman, G.D., Baron, R.A. and Balkin, D.B. (2005), “Are perseverance and self-efficacy costless? Assessing entrepreneurs’ regretful thinking”, Journal of Organizational Behavior, Vol. 26, pp. 1-19. Medvec, V.H., Madley, S.F. and Gilovich, T. (1995), “When less is more: counterfactual thinking and satisfaction among Olympic medalists”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 69, pp. 603-10. Moore, D.P. (2002), Careerpreneurs: Lessons From Leading Women Entrepreneurs on Building a Career Without Boundaries, Davies-Black, Palo Alto, CA. Ng, T.W.H., Eby, L.T., Sorensen, K.L. and Feldman, D.C. (2005), “Predictors of objective and subjective career success: a meta-analysis”, Personnel Psychology, Vol. 58, pp. 367-408. Oswalt, S.B., Cameron, K.A. and Koob, J.J. (2005), “Sexual regret in college students”, Archives of Sexual Behavior, Vol. 34 No. 6, pp. 663-9. Perrewé, P.L. and Nelson, D.L. (2004), “Gender and career success: the facilitative role of political skill”, Organizational Dynamics, Vol. 33 No. 4, pp. 366-78. Podsakoff, P.M. and Organ, D.W. (1986), “Self-reports in organizational research: problems and prospects”, Journal of Management, Vol. 12, pp. 531-44. Powell, G.N. and Mainiero, L.A. (1992), “Cross-currents in the river of time: conceptualizing the complexities of women’s careers”, Journal of Management, Vol. 18, pp. 215-37. Power, S.J. (2006), The Mid-career Success Guide: Planning for the Second Half of Your Working Life, Praeger, Westport, CT. Reynolds, K.E., Folse, J.A. and Jones, M. (2006), “Search regret: antecedents and consequences”, Journal of Retailing, Vol. 82 No. 4, pp. 339-48. Roese, N.J. and Summerville, A. (2005), “What we regret most . . . and why”, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 31 No. 9, pp. 1273-85. Roese, N.J., Pennington, G.L., Coleman, J., Janicki, M., Li, N.P. and Kenrick, D.T. (2006), “Sex differences in regrets: all for love or some for lust?”, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 32 No. 6, pp. 770-80. Rosenbaum, J. (1979), “Tournament mobility: career patterns in a corporation”, Administrative Science Quarterly, Vol. 24 No. 6, pp. 220-41. Rousseau, D. and Wade-Benzoni, K. (1995), “Changing individual-organization attachments”, in Howard, A. (Ed.), Changing Nature of Work, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA, pp. 290-321. Shipper, F. and Dillard, J.E. (2000), “A study of impending derailment and recovery of middle managers across career stages”, Human Resource Management, Vol. 39, pp. 331-45. Sullivan, S.E. (1999), “The changing nature of careers: a review and research agenda”, Journal of Management, Vol. 25, pp. 457-84. Sullivan, S. and Arthur, M. (2006), “The evolution of the boundaryless career concept: examining physical and psychological mobility”, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Vol. 69 No. 1, pp. 19-29. Sullivan, S.E. and Crocitto, M. (2007), “Developmental career theories”, in Peiperl, M. and Gunz, H. (Eds), Handbook of Career Studies, Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA. Super, D. (1957), Psychology of Careers, Harper & Brothers, New York, NY. Van Kleef, G.A., De Dreu, C.K. and Manstead, A.S. (2006), “Supplication and appeasement in conflict and negotiation: the interpersonal effects of disappointment, worry, guilt, and regret”, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 91 No. 1, pp. 124-42. Wanous, J.P. (1992), Organizational Entry: Recruitment, Selection, Orientation and Socialization of Newcomers, Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA. Zeelenberg, M. and Pieters, R. (2004), “Beyond valence in customer dissatisfaction: a review and new findings on behavioral responses to regret and disappointment in failed services”, Journal of Business Research, Vol. 57 No. 4, pp. 445-55. Zeelenberg, M., van Dijk, W.W., Der Pligt, J.V., Manstead, A.S., Empelen, P.V., Dimitri, R. and Pepihn, V. (1998), “Emotional reactions to the outcomes of decisions: the role of counterfactual thought in the experience of regret and disappointment”, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Vol. 75 No. 2, pp. 117-41. Zeelenberg, M., Nijstad, B.A., van Putten, M. and van Dijk, E. (2006), “Inaction inertia, regret, and valuation: a closer look”, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Vol. 101, pp. 89-104. van Dijk, E. and Zeelenberg, M. (2005), “On the psychology of if only: regret and the comparison between factual and counterfactual outcomes”, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, Vol. 97 No. 2, pp. 152-60. About the authors Sherry E. Sullivan (PhD, The Ohio State University) served as Program Chair of the Careers Division of the Academy of Management, was recognized with the Gender and Diversity in Management’s Janet Chusmir Award for mentoring and service (2002) and is a Fellow of the Southern Management Association. She has published in journals including the Academy of Management Executive, Journal of Applied Psychology, Career Development International, Journal of Management, and Journal of Vocational Behaviour. She is co-author (with Lisa Mainiero) of The Opt-Out Revolt: Why People are Leaving Companies to Create Kaleidoscope Careers (Davies-Black Publishers, 2006) and co-editor (with Yehuda Baruch and Haze Schepmyer) of Winning Reviews: A Guide for Evaluating Scholarly Writing (Palgrave Macmillian, 2006). She is the corresponding author and can be contacted at: ssulliv@bgsu.edu Monica L. Forret (PhD, University of Missouri) is a full professor at St Ambrose University and is the Director of the Doctor of Business Administration Program. She has published numerous articles in journals including the Journal of Organizational Behavior, Group and Organization Management, Journal of Vocational Behavior, Organizational Dynamics, Career Development International, Journal of Business and Psychology, and Leadership & Organization Development Journal. Her research interests include networking, mentoring, career decisions, and recruitment processes. She has served on the board of the Careers Division of the Academy of Management, as the Human Resources/Careers Track Chair of the Midwest Academy of Being laid off and career regrets 803 JMP 22,8 804 Management, and is an Editorial Board Member for the Journal of Leadership and Organizational Studies. Lisa A. Mainiero (PhD, Yale University) has published numerous papers in journals such as Administrative Science Quarterly, Academy of Management Review, Academy of Management Executive, Group and Organization Management, Journal of Management, Organizational Dynamics, and Harvard Business Review. She was elected as Program Chair for the Women In Management Division of the Academy of Management, and has also served as Secretary and Proceedings Editor for the Eastern Academy of Management and on the Board for the Careers Division of the Academy of Management. She is the author of several books, including Office Romance and is co-author (with Sherry Sullivan) of The Opt-Out Revolt: Why People are Leaving Companies to Create Kaleidoscope Careers (Davies-Black Publishers, 2006). To purchase reprints of this article please e-mail: reprints@emeraldinsight.com Or visit our web site for further details: www.emeraldinsight.com/reprints