Psychology of Men & Masculinity 2010, Vol. 11, No. 3, 208 –224 © 2010 American Psychological Association 1524-9220/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0018255 Masculine Gender Roles and Differentiation: Predictors of Body Image and Self-Objectification in Men Jonathan P. Schwartz Debbie L. Grammas, Roy John Sutherland, Kevin J. Siffert, and Imelda Bush-King New Mexico State University This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. University of Houston Theory and previous research suggests that culture, traditional masculine ideology, and early relational experiences may affect male body image. Thus, the purpose of this study was to examine the influence of ethnicity, gender role conflict, and differentiation of self (DOS) on body image and self-objectification among 202 college men. Results indicated that ethnicity and aspects of DOS were significant predictors of body image dissatisfaction, whereas aspects of gender role conflict and DOS were predictors of self-objectification. A particularly noteworthy finding of the study was that a clear sense of self and a lack of emotional enmeshment are related to low body image concerns and low self-objectification among men. Keywords: masculinity, gender roles, body image, self-objectification, differentiation Historically, research on societal pressure regarding appearance has addressed body-image dissatisfaction and objectification among women (Pope, Phillips, & Olivardia, 2000). Current research suggests that male body-image dissatisfaction is on the rise (Leit, Pope, & Gray, 2001; Morry & Staska, 2001). According to Morry and Staska (2001), men who read fitness magazines are more likely to have a negative body image, engage in self-objectification, internalize the ideal standard of beauty for men (i.e., muscularity), and engage in associated eating disorders. Poor male body image has been associated with several forms of psychological distress, such as anxiety, depression, muscle dysmorphia, shame, and low selfesteem, and negative behaviors such as compulsive exercise, disordered eating, and use of performance-enhancing drugs (Cafri, Strauss, & Thompson, 2002; Cash & Fleming, 2002; Jonathan P. Schwartz, Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology, New Mexico State University; Debbie L. Grammas, Roy John Sutherland, Kevin J. Siffert, and Imelda Bush-King, Department of Educational Psychology, University of Houston. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jonathan P. Schwartz, Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology, 3 MSC 3CEP, P.O. Box 30001, Las Cruces, NM 88003-8001. E-mail: jschwart@nmsu.edu McCreary & Sasse, 2000; Olivardia, Pope, Borowiecki, & Cohane, 2004). Societal pressure on men is qualitatively different from that on women; research suggests that, in general, men feel pressure to become more muscular, whereas women feel pressure to become thinner (Cafri & Thompson, 2004). Accurately measuring unique aspects of male body-image dissatisfaction (Tylka, Bergeron, & Schwartz, 2005), combined with our understanding of the negative implications of societal pressure on men concerning appearance, requires an examination of multiple predictive factors. Previous research has found that a rigid masculine gender role is a positive predictor of male body dissatisfaction (Olivardia et al., 2004; Schwartz & Tylka, 2008; Tylka et al., 2005). In particular, evidence consistently links aspects of masculinity, including socialized masculine gender roles, to male body-image concerns (McCreary, Saucier, & Courtenay, 2005). In fact, being muscular has been identified as a way to externally exhibit masculine identity (Leit et al., 2001; Pope, Olivardia, Gruber, & Borowiecki, 1999). This study adds to the current body of literature by examining multiple predictors of bodyimage satisfaction in men, measuring unique aspects of male body image, and examining self-objectification as an outcome. “Self- 208 This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. BODY IMAGE AND SELF-OBJECTIFICATION objectification” has been defined as internalizing societal messages that view individuals’ value based on external factors, leading to a preoccupation with physical appearance (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997; Fredrickson, Roberts, Noll, Quinn, & Twenge, 1998; Muehlenkamp & Saris-Baglama, 2002). Few studies have examined predictors of selfobjectification in men. Furthermore, there is limited research connecting masculine socialization with body image (Kimmel & Mahalik, 2004; Schwartz & Tylka, 2008). In this study, we examined the construct of gender role conflict (i.e., the intrapersonal or interpersonal conflict/tension created by adopting rigid traditional masculine roles), which is considered to be a result of gender role socialization (O’Neil, Good, & Holmes, 1995) and a predictor of male body image. There is no known research that examines differentiation of self (DOS), a family systems theory related to balancing intimacy and autonomy (Bowen, 1978), as a predictor of male body image. In addition, research using measures specifically created to measure unique male body-image concerns is relatively new (Tylka et al., 2005). The study’s goal was to build on previous research by examining gender role conflict and DOS as predictors of selfobjectification and body-image dissatisfaction in men. Body Dissatisfaction in Men Societal messages have been cited as sources of male body-image disturbances (Cash & Pruzinsky, 2002; Pope, Gruber, et al., 2000; Ricciardelli & McCabe, 2004). According to sociocultural theory, the culture determines the ideal beauty standards and, in turn, how individuals view themselves and others (Thompson, Heinberg, Altabe, & Tantleff-Dunn, 1999). Messages communicated by mass media influence societal standards of beauty (Hargreaves & Tiggemann, 2004); these ideals of beauty are seen in movies and magazines, and on television. In Western cultures (e.g., United States), being thin is the ideal standard of beauty in women; muscularity is the ideal standard for men (Hargreaves & Tiggemann, 2004). Most women, however, cannot achieve the desired level of thinness portrayed in the media, which is one reason for negative body image and eating disorders among women. In contrast, the 209 ideal muscular man has a mesomorphic body shape: wide shoulders, a larger upper body, and a flat abdomen (Leit et al., 2001). Again, this standard of beauty is not attainable for most men and has been implicated in the use of anabolic steroids and other performanceenhancing drugs (Pope, Gruber, et al., 2000). Recent research has indicated that the standard for the optimal male body appearance is changing (Leit et al., 2001; Morry & Staska, 2001). Research has demonstrated that in the last two decades, prototypes of masculinity (Playgirl Centerfolds; G.I. Joes) have increased in muscularity and decreased in body fat in a manner unattainable without the use of steroids (Leit et al.; 2001; Pope et al., 1999). Studies have indicated that, when asked, men chose an ideal male body figure that on average had 25 pounds of additional muscle and about eight pounds less body fat than what they actually had (Olivardia et al., 2004). Men’s ideal body and the body men thought would be preferred by women was significantly more muscular and had less body fat than the average male or women’s actual preference (Cafri et al., 2002; Olivardia et al., 2004). Researchers have found that muscle dissatisfaction is associated with depression and low self-esteem (Cafri et al., 2002; McCreary & Sasse, 2000; Olivardia et al., 2004). Olivardia et al. (2004) found that as men’s Fat Free Mass Index (FFMI; fat free body mass ⫻ height) increased, so did their selfesteem. Moreover, research has found that onefourth of college men surveyed used steroids or other products to improve their appearance (Olivardia et al., 2004). In a college student sample, body builders were much more likely to admit to taking steroids than runners or those engaging in martial arts (Blouin & Goldfield, 1995). The primary reason steroids were used was to enhance looks. Blouin and Goldfield (1995) found that self-esteem and body satisfaction were actually lower among body builders versus other athletes. Similar psychological factors have been found among body builders and women with eating disorders, which lead them to a greater predisposition to engage in destructive behaviors (e.g., unhealthy dieting, diuretics, steroids) to attain the ideal body. Further study is critical for gaining insight as to what places men at a greater risk for these detrimental behaviors. An integral issue in understanding male This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 210 SCHWARTZ ET AL. body-image concerns is the effectively measuring of the unique body-image concerns of men. Body image is a multidimensional construct that encompasses one’s thoughts (i.e., the significance of appearance and one’s beliefs about one’s body) and awareness of one’s body. Many of the scales used to measure body image were designed for women. In the past few years, some scales have been designed to measure male body-image dissatisfaction (Cafri & Thompson, 2004; Tylka et al., 2005). An advantage of these new scales is that they measure unique aspects of male body-image dissatisfaction (e.g., muscularity or the lack thereof). Self-Objectification as Associated With Body Image Self-objectification is an internalization of societal objectification, which causes individuals to scrutinize his or her body (Tiggemann & Kuring, 2004). Self-objectification is related to low self-esteem, body shame, restrained eating, and numerous other psychological costs among women, (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997; Fredrickson et al., 1998). Thus societal pressure and mass media may play a role in men and women’s behaviors, emotions, and attitudes related to their body image (Strelan & Hargreaves, 2005; Wright & Huston, 1983). The literature on self-objectification is limited especially concerning men. The findings examining men and self-objectification seem to indicate that there is a negative correlation between self-objectification and self-esteem, general psychological well-being, and body-image satisfaction (Strelan & Hargreaves, 2005). As note above, much of the current research on self-objectification has focused on women (Tiggemann & Kuring, 2004). However, increased evidence shows that similar selfobjectification processes also occur with men. Male self-objectification is often focused on being young and having a muscular body with broad shoulders, a well-developed upper body, and narrow hips (Leit et al., 2001; Pope, Gruber, et al., 2000). With higher reports of male selfobjectification, researchers have suggested a need for additional research examining the effects of self-objectification on men (Tiggemann & Kuring, 2004). Strelan and Hargreaves (2005) found that men who exercise to enhance their appearance were more likely to report lower body esteem and higher self-objectification. Strelan and Hargreaves also found that these men had lower self-esteem. Moreover, research has indicated that among men whose appearance is related to their self-worth, appearance accounted for 20% of their total psychological self-acceptance (Tager, Good, & Morrison, 2006). Morry and Staska (2001) found that reading fitness magazines contributed to the internalization of social ideals about appearance and consequently to the prediction of eating problems. Gender Role Conflict and Male Body Image/Self-Objectification Research has demonstrated a link between negative body image and psychological distress in men (Tager et al., 2006). Although Western society emphasizes restrictive, physical ideals as a symbol of masculine status (Tager et al., 2006), few studies have examined the role of masculinity in body-image satisfaction. Studies are now beginning to measure the multiple pressures and conflicts resulting from societal pressure to conform to traditional masculine roles (O’Neil, 2008). Gender role conflict (O’Neil, Helms, Gable, David, & Wrightsman, 1986) was conceptualized to explore and measure the cognitive, emotional, and behavioral problems men are subjected to as they try to conform to society’s expectations of male gender roles. There are four dimensions in which gender role conflict occurs: (1) success, power, and competition; (2) restrictive and affectionate behavior between men; (3) restrictive emotionality; and (4) conflict between work and family relations (O’Neil et al., 1986). Gender role conflict has been associated with intrapersonal and interpersonal costs, such as heightened stress levels (Sharpe & Heppner, 1991), neurotic defenses (Mahalik, Cournoyer, DeFranc, Cherry, & Napolitano, 1998), higher rates of substance abuse (Blazina & Watkins, 1996), hostile, rigid interpersonal behaviors (Mahalik, 2000; Robinson & Schwartz, 2004), decreased relationship satisfaction, and intimacy complications (Sharpe & Heppner, 1991). Societal messages for men state that they should be big and muscular, what Mishkind, Rodin, Silberstein, and Striegel-Moore (1986) deem the “muscular mesomorphic” shape. Re- This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. BODY IMAGE AND SELF-OBJECTIFICATION search has also suggested that the drive to be muscular for men is due to the perception that a muscular build is linked to masculinity (Weinke, 1998). The reverse has also been argued: If a man is less muscular (i.e., endomorphic or ectomorphic), he is more feminine (Grogan & Richards, 2002; Klein, 1993; Weinke, 1998). Men may experience negative body perception when attempting to meet gender role ideals (Olivardia, 2001). Research by Shelton and Liljequist (2002) suggests that body-image concerns among men may begin at an early age. Negative peer appraisals may stem from the combination of an alleged nonmasculine appearance (e.g., nonmuscular) and seemingly nonmasculine behavior (e.g., a “feeble” attempt to stand up for one’s self). These appraisals are internalized and may increase body-image dissatisfaction. Kimmel and Mahalik (2004) found that rigid adherence to masculine norms further contributes to masculine body-ideal distress. Research by Mills and D’Alfonso (2007) found that when competing against a woman (and losing in that competition) men had increased negative views of their bodies. Qualitative and quantitative research suggests that men feel the need to become more muscular in order to maintain Western norms of traditional male gender roles (Drummond, 2002; Grogan & Richards, 2002; Klein, 1993; Mishkind et al., 1986; Pope, Philips, & Olivardia, 2000; Weinke, 1998). Mishkind et al. (1986) found that failure to attain gender role expectations led men to displace these shortcomings onto their bodies. DOS and Male Body Image Family relations have been shown to have an impact on body image and disordered eating (Byely, Archibald, Graber, & Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Harworth-Hoeppner, 2000). Research has suggested that parental intrusiveness— or, alternatively, a lack of parental support— contribute to body-image disturbance in men and women (Ata, Ludden, & Lally, 2007; Rorty, Yager, Buckwalter, Rossotto, & Guthrie, 2000). The concept of DOS (sometimes referred to as “individuation”) is a central element in Bowen’s (1978) family theory and provides a tool with which we can examine the influence of early family relational experiences on body image and self-objectification in men. 211 On an intrapsychic level, DOS conceptualizes one’s ability to distinguish between thinking and feeling processes (Bowen, 1978). On an interpersonal level, it denotes one’s ability to experience deep intimacy in close relationships without endangering one’s sense of autonomy or cutting oneself off from significant others. For example, according to Bowen, taking an “I-Position” involves a clearly defined sense of self that is impervious to pressure from others. Research has provided some evidence for this notion in that lower levels of differentiation have been associated with higher levels of chronic anxiety and more symptomatic distress (Skowron & Friedlander, 1998), higher frequencies of health problems, lower psychosocial development, and greater psychological distress (Bray, Harvey, & Williamson, 1987; Harvey, Curry, & Bray, 1991; Jenkins, Buboltz, Schwartz, & Johnson, 2005; Murdock & Gore, 2004; Skowron, 2000; Skowron, Holmes, & Sabatelli, 2003). Bowen (1978) theorized that with higher differentiation, individuals can retain relative autonomy in periods of stress, are more flexible and adaptable, and are more independent of the emotionality about them. Thus, a high DOS should serve as a protective factor against family, peer, and societal pressure. Differentiation allows individuals to cope more effectively with life stresses, social pressure, and pressure in the relational system. Currently, there is no known research examining men’s levels of DOS in relation to body image. However, there is research that has focused on issues of separation–individuation, attachment, and gender role conflict (e.g., Blazina & Watkins, 2000; Schwartz, Waldo, & Higgins, 2004). This research suggests that early familial relational experiences have an impact on identity and subsequent gender role socialization. Furthermore, based on Bowen’s (1978) theory, a theoretical link between DOS and body image can be made. Individuals who have higher differentiation are likely to have a healthier body image and less self-objectification because they typically have a strong identity and an autonomous sense of self in close relationships. In contrast, those that are enmeshed and emotionally reactive in their relationships may overly focus on others, resulting in dissatisfaction with body image and self-objectification. 212 SCHWARTZ ET AL. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Race/Ethnicity It is important to consider ethnic diversity when looking at body-image concerns in men, because culture influences the perception of physical attractiveness (Heinberg, Thompson, & Stormer, 1995). According to sociocultural theory, culture affects individuals’ ideal standards of beauty for themselves and others (Thompson et al., 1999). In addition, the use of a more ethnically diverse sample has been cited in the literature as an area for further inquiry (Cafri et al., 2002; McCreary et al., 2005; Vartanian, Giant, & Passino, 2001). Research that has examined racial differences in bodyimage concerns among men has yielded inconsistent results (Ricciardelli, McCabe, Williams, & Thompson, 2007). When comparing African American and White men, some authors have found that African American men tend to be more satisfied with their bodies (Miller, Gleaves, Hirsch, Green, Snow, & Chanda, 2000; Smith, Thompson, Raczynski, & Hilner, 1999). However, there was no difference in body satisfaction when comparing these two groups after controlling for body mass index (Russell, 2002). When comparing Asian American and White men, in general Asian American men felt that their current body was smaller than ideal, which did not hold true for White men (Barnett, Keel, & Conoscenti, 2002). This is in contrast to Pope, Phillips, et al. (2000), who found that a majority of White men felt the ideal body was more muscular and had less fat than their actual body. Some authors also have found Asian American men tend to have greater body dissatisfaction than White men (Edman, Yates, & Aruguete, 2005) whereas others have found no differences (Miller et al., 2000). When African American, White, and Latino men were compared, there were no differences in body-image dissatisfaction (Demarest & Allen, 2000; Miller et al., 2000). In contrast, when comparing African American, White, Latino, and Asian American men, African American men tended to be the most satisfied with their bodies, and there were no differences among the remaining racial/ ethnic groups (Mayville, Katz, Gipson, & Cabral, 1999). This study, focusing on the body-image concerns of Latino, Asian American, African American, and White men, not only examines differences in body image, but also considers how race/ethnicity may interact with other predictors of body image. Present Study The study’s goal was to add to previous research by examining masculine gender roles and individuation from the family of origin association with male body image and selfobjectification. The study will explore whether masculine gender role conflict and DOS will predict body-image dissatisfaction and selfobjectification. It is hypothesized that when controlling for race/ethnicity, conflict resulting from rigid adherence to masculine gender roles will lead to body-image dissatisfaction and selfobjectification. It is also hypothesized that when controlling for race/ethnicity and gender role conflict, body-image dissatisfaction and selfobjectification will be predicted by a lack of DOS. Method Participants A total of 202 undergraduate heterosexual men (M age ⫽ 22.08 years; range ⫽ 18 – 65; SD ⫽ 3.88, M weight ⫽ 170.19 pounds, M height ⫽ 69.25 in.) from a large Southern university participated. Participants were recruited from undergraduate classes in education and psychology and received extra credit for their participation. Racial/ethnic characteristics: 37.3% were Asian American, 27.7% were White, 14.5% were Latino, and 13.6% were African American. Eighty percent of participants were single, 6% were engaged, 8.7% were married or partnered, 1.2% were separated or divorced, fewer than 1.3% were widowed, and 2.8% reported their relationship status as “other.” With regard to socioeconomic status, 4.2% identified themselves as members of the upper class, 14.8% identified as uppermiddle class, 60% identified as middle class, and 19.5% identified as members of the working class. Seventeen percent of participants identified as college freshmen or high school seniors, 28.9% were sophomores in college, 17.5% were juniors, 32.3% were seniors, 1.6% were postbaccalaureate students, and fewer than 1% identified as “other” or grad- BODY IMAGE AND SELF-OBJECTIFICATION uate student. The majority of the participants reported exercising at least once a week. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Procedures A script describing the study was read to potential participants; those that chose to participate completed an informed consent form in class. The researchers retained the signed consent and provided the participant with a copy that included a link to complete the survey online. Participants completed an additional informed consent online and the surveys in counterbalanced order using an online data collection program (Survey Monkey). Participants were able to complete the surveys at their convenience at their home computers or in a computer lab at the university. Measures Gender Role Conflict Scale (GRCS). The GRCS (O’Neil et al., 1986) is a 37-item scale that uses a 6-point Likert-type scale indicating to what degree the participants agree with the given statements (e.g., 1 ⫽ strongly disagree to 6 ⫽ strongly agree). It is designed to measure negative conflict resulting from restrictive and rigid gender roles with the following four factors: (1) success, power, and competition (SPC), for example, “I like to feel superior to other people”; (2) restrictive emotionality (RE), for example, “I often have trouble finding the words to describe how I am feeling”; (3) restrictive affectionate behavior between men (RABBM), for example, “I am sometimes hesitant to show my affection toward men because of how others may perceive me”; and (4) conflict between work and family (CBWF), for example, “My school or my work often disrupts other parts of my life (e.g., home, family, health, leisure).” Higher scores indicate greater gender role conflict. Positive correlations were demonstrated between the GRCS and other measures of men’s attitudes about masculinity (Good et al., 1995), supporting its construct validity. O’Neil et al. (1986) reported internal consistency for the four factors, with alphas ranging from .75 to .85 and test-retest reliabilities ranging from .72 to .86. In this study, internal reliability coefficients were .94 for RE, .90 for SPC, .89 for RABBM, and .86 for CBWF. 213 Differentiation of Self Inventory (DSI). The DSI (Skowron & Friedlander, 1998) was created to measure one’s level of differentiation based on the following four factors: I-position (IP), emotional reactivity (ER), emotional cutoff (EC), and fusion with others (FO). The DSI is a 43-item scale that uses a 6-point Likert-type scale indicating to what degree the participants agree with the given statements (e.g., 1 ⫽ low differentiation to 6 ⫽ high differentiation). Higher scores on the DSI reflect greater levels of differentiation and subsequently, less ER, EC, FO, and a greater ability to take “I” positions in relationships. The DSI measures one’s level of differentiation with the following four factors: (1) ER, for example, “When someone close to me disappoints me, I withdraw from him or her for a time”; (2) IP, for example, “No matter what happens to me, I know that I’ll never lose my sense of who I am”; (3) EC, for example, “Our relationship might be better if my spouse or partner would give me the space I need”; and (4) FO, for example, “I worry about people close to me getting hurt, sick, or upset.” Construct validity of the DSI was supported by high correlations with measures of anxiety and psychological distress (Skowron & Friedlander, 1998). Skowron and Friedlander reported internal consistency for the four factors, with alphas for the entire inventory at .88, ER at .88, IP at .85, EC at .79, and FO at .70. Test–retest alphas ranged from .74 to .84. In this study, internal reliability coefficients were .90 for ER, .88 for IP, .80 for EC, and .74 for FO. The Male Body Attitude Scale (MBAS). The MBAS (Tylka et al., 2005) is an instrument consisting of 24 items used to assess body dissatisfaction in men. Respondents rate three body areas (muscularity, body fat, height) on a 6-point scale that ranges from 1 (always) to 6 (never). Factor analysis yielded three subscales: muscularity (13 items; e.g., “I think my arms should be larger”); low body fat (nine items; e.g., “I think I have too much fat on my body”); and height (two items, e.g., “I am satisfied with my height”). Total scores can also be determined by taking the average of all the items of the instrument. Higher total and subscale scores indicate a greater degree of satisfaction. Concurrent, convergent, and discriminant validity have been supported (Tylka et al., 2005). Tylka et al. (2005) reported Cronbach alphas as .92 for the total scale, .90 for muscularity, .94 for low This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 214 SCHWARTZ ET AL. body fat, and .85 for height. Evidence of convergent validity has been found because the MBAS total score, muscularity scores, and low body fat scores have been related to the Body Esteem Scale, and the muscularity score was related to the muscular behaviors subscale of the Drive for Muscularity Scale. Support for discriminant validity has been shown as the MBAS total score and subscales were not related to impression management. Construct validity was shown because the MBAS total score and subscale scores were related to self-esteem, and the MBAS total score and low body fat subscale scores were related to eating disorder symptomology. In this study, internal reliability coefficients were .93 for muscularity, .92 for low body fat, and .88 for height. Self-Objectification Questionnaire. The Self-Objectification Questionnaire (Noll, 1996; Noll & Fredrickson, 1998) is a 10-item scale that requires the participants to rank the items (e.g., 0 ⫽ least impact, 10 ⫽ greatest impact) indicating how strongly the statements affect their physical self-concept. There are two subscales: Appearance-based items, for example, “What rank do you assign to weight”; and Competence-based items, for example, “What rank do you assign to physical coordination.” The difference between the cumulative scores on the appearance rankings and the competence rankings provides the overall score. The higher the overall score, the higher the individual’s level of self-objectification. Evidence for construct validity was found by Noll and Fredrickson (1998) on the basis of moderate correlations with scores on the Body Image Assessment (Williamson, Davis, Bennett, Goreczny, & Gleaves, 1985) and the Appearance Anxiety Questionnaire (Dion, Dion, & Keelan, 1990). Results Preliminary Analyses No outliers were identified within the data set. Skewness and kurtosis for each measure were evaluated via significance tests and the visual appearance of the measure distributions. No substantial violations existed (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996); therefore, no measures were transformed. Means, standard deviations, and range for all measures are reported in Table 1. The means were similar to other comparable samples for all the instruments. The correlation matrix is presented in Table 2. Examination of the correlations reveals a portion of the predictor variables (DOS, gender role conflict) correlate in the hypothesized direction. For example, less ER (positively) and more IP (positively) are both moderately correlated with the MBAS subscale of Low Body Fat. The only gender role conflict factors that correlated with the criterion variables were SPC and RABBM, which positively correlated with the Low Body Fat subscale. To explore whether racial/ethnic differences within the sample were related to scores on the study’s key measures, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted. Of seven possible race/ethnicity choices, there were suf- Table 1 Descriptive Statistics (n ⫽ 201) Variables Range Minimum Maximum M SD Emotional reactivity I-position Emotional cutoff Fusion SPC Restrictive emotionality RABBM Work and family conflict Self-objectification Muscularity Low body fat Height 36.00 48.00 50.00 35.00 65.00 50.00 40.00 30.00 55.00 45.00 38.00 10.00 25.00 16.00 17.00 14.00 13.00 10.00 8.00 6.00 ⫺36 10.00 8.00 2.00 61.00 64.00 67.00 49.00 78.00 60.00 48.00 36.00 36 55.00 46.00 12.00 39.35 43.94 46.79 28.70 52.21 31.84 27.37 22.64 3.41 35.24 29.83 7.14 6.73 9.42 9.25 6.58 12.18 10.69 8.75 6.59 12.13 10.38 9.76 2.23 Note. SPC ⫽ success, power, and competition; RABBM ⫽ restrictive affectionate behavior between men. BODY IMAGE AND SELF-OBJECTIFICATION 215 Table 2 Correlation Matrix (n ⫽ 201) Variable This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12. Emotional reactivity I-position Emotional cutoff Fusion SPC Restrictive emotionality RABBM Conflict between work and family Self-objectification Muscularity Low body fat Height 1 2 3 4 5 — .12ⴱⴱⴱ — .31ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.15ⴱⴱⴱ — .51ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.41ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.56ⴱⴱⴱ — ⫺.27ⴱⴱⴱ .26ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.33ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.23ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.29 ⫺.12 ⫺.19ⴱⴱⴱ .10 ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.65 ⫺.03 ⫺.42ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.10 ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.34 .09 ⫺.35 ⫺.07 ⫺.09 ⫺.03 .07 .21ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.03 ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ .22 .27 .12 .20ⴱⴱⴱ .13 .10 ⴱⴱ ⫺.18 .02 ⫺.07 ⫺.07 ⫺.04 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 — .48ⴱⴱⴱ .52ⴱⴱⴱ — .64ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ .56 ⫺.08 .05 .14ⴱ ⫺.08 .45 ⫺.01 .05 .01 ⫺.04 — .36ⴱⴱⴱ — .08 .09 — .04 .09 .14ⴱ — ⴱ .16 .02 .24ⴱⴱⴱ .35ⴱⴱⴱ — ⫺.05 ⫺.09 .13 .24ⴱⴱⴱ .60ⴱⴱⴱ — Note. SPC ⫽ success, power, and competition; RABBM ⫽ restrictive affectionate behavior between men. p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001. ⴱ ficient participants in four groups for this analysis, White (n ⫽ 60), African American (n ⫽ 29), Asian American (n ⫽ 74), Latino, n ⫽ 28) on scores of muscularity, low body fat, height, and self-objectification. The MANOVA indicated that the multivariate effect was not significant, Wilk’s ⌳ ⫽ .974 F(185.00, 450.39) ⫽ .543, p ⫽ .84, 2 ⫽ .009. This indicates that there were no significant differences between ethnic groups for the four dependent variables. Even though the MANOVA showed no significant differences among ethnicities, we were interested in determining if ethnicity would explain variance when combined with the other variables, thus it was added to the regression equation (Meyers, Gamst, & Guarinno, 2006). To control for the impact of race/ethnicity, it was included in the regression. Race/ethnicity was dummy coded, and White was used as the reference group. Main Analyses To test the hypotheses that controlling for race/ethnicity, high gender role conflict, and low DOS would predict negative body image in men and self-objectification, hierarchical regression analyses were conducted. Because there is previous literature supporting a relationship between race/ethnicity and the criterion variables, these predictors were entered in the first step. Thus, controlling for race/ethnicity, gender role conflict was entered in the second block (based on previous research supporting the relation between gender role conflict and body image) (e.g., Tylka et al., 2005). Because this is the first known study examining DOS and body image/self-objectification, it was added as a last step to examine if it added variance to the prediction of the criterion variables. Thus, four separate hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted to determine if gender role conflict and DOS would be significant predictors of muscularity, low body fat, height, or self-objectification after controlling for the effects of ethnicity. In each regression, race/ ethnicity (which was dummy coded) was entered at the first step, followed by the Gender Role Conflict subscales, which were in turn, were followed by the DOS subscales. The results yielded by the hierarchical regression analyses are discussed below. Prediction of muscularity. See Table 3 for results of the regression analysis for the Muscularity subscale. After controlling for the effects of ethnicity, the four subscales of Gender Role Conflict accounted for 7% of the variance, F(7, 194) ⫽ 1.21, p ⬎ .05, and did not account for significant unique variance on this criterion measure. However, in step 3, with the addition of the four subscales of DOS, the full model accounted for 11% of the variance, F(7, 194) ⫽ 2.10, p ⬍ .05, and Asian American ethnicity was a significant negative predictor ( ⫽ ⫺.19, p ⬍ .05), indicating that being Asian American was associated with concern 216 SCHWARTZ ET AL. Table 3 Summary of Hierarchical Regression for Muscularity (n ⫽ 201) This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Variable Step 1 Latino Black Asian Step 2 Latino Black Asian SPC Restrictive emotionality RABBM Conflict between work and family Step 3 Latino Black Asian SPC Restrictive emotionality RABBM Conflict between work and family Emotional recativity I-position Emotional cutoff Fusion B SE B  ⫺3.88 ⫺0.32 ⫺3.92 2.25 2.31 1.67 ⫺0.14 ⫺0.01 ⫺0.19ⴱ ⫺3.69 ⫺0.48 ⫺4.23 ⫺0.03 0.02 0.04 2.27 2.33 1.79 0.08 0.09 0.12 ⫺0.13 ⫺0.02 ⫺0.20ⴱ ⫺0.04 0.02 0.03 0.12 0.14 0.07 ⫺3.73 ⫺0.61 ⫺4.06 ⫺0.1 0.06 ⫺0.02 2.24 2.38 1.81 0.08 0.11 0.12 ⫺0.13 ⫺0.13 ⫺0.19ⴱ ⫺0.11 0.06 ⫺0.2 0.14 0.18 0.29 ⫺0.14 0.04 0.14 0.15 0.10 0.11 0.16 0.08 0.12 0.27ⴱⴱ ⫺0.13 0.03 Note. SPC ⫽ success, power, and competition; RABBM ⫽ restrictive affectionate behavior between men. ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. about muscularity and IP was a significant positive predictor ( ⫽ .27, p ⬍ .01), indicating that an IP was associated with a lack of concern about muscularity. Prediction of low body fat. See Table 4 for results of the regression analysis for the Low Body Fat subscale. After controlling for the effects of ethnicity the four subscales of Gender Role Conflict accounted for 8% of the variance, F(7, 194) ⫽ 1.87, p ⬍ .05, and Restrictive Emotionality was a significant negative predictor ( ⫽ ⫺.19, p ⬍ .05), indicating that high restrictive emotionality was associated with a concern about low body fat. RABBM was a significant positive predictor ( ⫽ .23, p ⬍ .05), indicating that high RABBM is associated with a lack of concern about low body fat. In step 3, with the addition of the four subscales of DOS, the full model accounted for 17% of the variance, F(7, 194) ⫽ 3.33, p ⬍ .001, and ER ( ⫽ .25, p ⬍ .01) and IP ( ⫽ .23, p ⬍ .05) were significant positive predictors of the Low Body Fat subscale, indicating that high IP and low ER are associated with a lack of concern about low body fat. Prediction of height. See Table 5 for results of the regression analysis for the Height subscale. After controlling for the effects of ethnicity, the four subscales of Gender Role Conflict accounted for 3% of the variance, F(7, 194) ⫽ .69, p ⬎ .05, and did not account for significant unique variance on this criterion measure. In step 3, with the addition of the four subscales of DOS, the full model accounted for 13% of the variance, F(7, 194) ⫽ 1.83, p ⬍ .05, and Fusion was a significant negative predictor ( ⫽ ⫺.19, p ⬍ .05), indicating that high fusion is associated with a lack of concern about height and ER was a significant positive predictor ( ⫽ .27, p ⬍ .01), indicating that low emotional Table 4 Summary of Hierarchical Regression for Low Body Fat (n ⫽ 201) Variable Step 1 Latino Black Asian Step 2 Latino Black Asian SPC Restrictive emotionality RABBM Conflict between work and family Step 3 Latino Black Asian SPC Restrictive emotionality RABBM Conflict between work and family Emotional reactivity I-position Emotional cutoff Fusion  B SE B ⫺1.44 1.59 ⫺1.58 2.13 2.19 1.58 ⫺0.05 0.06 ⫺0.08 ⫺1.56 1.59 ⫺2.07 0.1 ⫺0.17 0.25 2.12 2.17 1.66 0.08 0.08 0.11 ⫺0.06 0.06 ⫺0.11 0.12 ⫺0.19ⴱ 0.23ⴱ ⫺0.1 0.13 ⫺0.06 ⫺1.75 1.51 ⫺1.1 0.06 ⫺0.01 0.18 2.04 2.17 1.64 0.08 0.1 0.11 ⫺0.06 0.05 ⫺0.06 0.07 ⫺0.01 0.16 ⫺0.02 0.36 0.22 0.04 ⫺0.09 0.12 0.14 0.09 0.1 0.15 ⫺0.01 0.25ⴱⴱ 0.23ⴱ 0.05 ⫺0.06 Note. SPC ⫽ success, power, and competition; RABBM ⫽ restrictive affectionate behavior between men. R2 ⫽ .03 for Step 1; ⴱR2 ⫽ .08 for Step 2; ⴱⴱR2 ⫽ .17 for Step 3 (⌬R2 ⫽ .09). ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. BODY IMAGE AND SELF-OBJECTIFICATION Table 5 Summary of Hierarchical Regression for Height (n ⫽ 201) This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Variable Step 1 Latino Black Asian Step 2 Latino Black Asian SPC Restrictive emotionality RABBM Conflict between work and family Step 3 Latino Black Asian SPC RE RABBM Conflict between work and family Emotional reactivity I-position Emotional cutoff Fusion  217 194) ⫽ 3.27, p ⬍ .05, and SPC ( ⫽ ⫺.29, p ⬍ .001) and IP ( ⫽ ⫺.22, p ⬍ .05), ER ( ⫽ ⫺.27, p ⬍ .01), and EC ( ⫽ ⫺.21, p ⬍ .05) were significant negative predictors, indicating that low SPC conflict, IP and high EC, and ER are associated with a high self-objectification. B SE B ⫺0.25 0.37 ⫺0.25 0.5 0.51 0.37 ⫺0.04 0.06 ⫺0.09 ⫺0.31 0.36 ⫺0.49 ⫺0.01 0.5 0.52 0.4 0.02 ⫺0.05 0.05 ⫺0.12 ⫺0.07 0.01 ⫺0.01 0.02 0.03 0.04 ⫺0.01 ⫺0.02 0.03 ⫺0.06 Previous literature has found that negative body image is consistently associated with psychological distress in men (Tager et al., 2006). It appears that body image can be an integral component of male identity, particularly when men feel pressure to adhere to rigid masculine norms (Jacobi & Cash, 1994). Previous research has not examined the process of individuation in 0.3 0.39 ⫺0.28 ⫺0.02 0.03 ⫺0.01 0.5 0.53 0.4 0.02 0.02 0.03 ⫺0.05 0.06 ⫺0.06 0.1 0.15 ⫺0.05 Table 6 Summary of Hierarchical Regression for Self-Objectification (n ⫽ 201) ⫺0.01 0.09 0.03 0.03 ⫺0.09 0.03 0.03 0.22 0.02 0.04 ⫺0.02 0.27ⴱⴱ 0.11 0.02 ⫺0.19ⴱ Note. SPC ⫽ success, power, and competition; RABBM ⫽ restrictive affectionate behavior between men. R2 ⫽ .02 for Step 1; R2 ⫽ .03 for Step 2; ⴱR2 ⫽ .13 for Step 3 (⌬R2 ⫽ .10). ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. reactivity is associated with a lack of concern about height. Prediction of self-objectification. See Table 6 for results of the regression analysis for Self-Objectification. After controlling for the effects of ethnicity the four subscales of Gender Role Conflict accounted for 9% of the variance, F(7, 194) ⫽ 1.88, p ⬎ .05, and SPC was a significant negative predictor ( ⫽ ⫺.24, p ⬍ .01) indicating that low success, power, and competition conflict is associated with high self-objectification, and that RABBM was a significant positive predictor ( ⫽ .21, p ⬍ .05), indicating that high restrictive affectionate behavior between men conflict is associated with high self-objectification. In step 3, with the addition of the four subscales of DOS, the full model accounted for 19% of the variance, F(7, Discussion Variable Step 1 Latino Black Asian Step 2 Latino Black Asian SPC Restrictive emotionality RABBM Conflict between work and family Step 3 Latino Black Asian SPC Restrictive emotionality RABBM Conflict between work and family Emotional reactivity I-position Emotional cutoff Fusion B SE B  1.09 1.05 3.82 2.75 2.83 2.07 0.03 0.03 0.17ⴱ 0.94 0.46 2.32 ⫺0.26 2.73 2.8 2.17 0.1 0.03 0.01 0.09 ⫺0.24ⴱⴱ ⫺0.13 0.3 0.11 0.14 ⫺0.12 0.21ⴱ 0.32 0.17 0.13 0.95 ⫺0.18 2.45 ⫺0.32 2.64 2.81 2.14 0.1 0.03 ⫺0.01 0.1 ⫺0.29ⴱⴱ ⫺0.11 0.23 0.13 0.14 ⫺0.09 0.15 0.32 ⫺0.5 ⫺0.3 ⫺0.28 ⫺0.08 0.16 0.18 0.12 0.13 0.19 0.13 ⫺0.27ⴱⴱ ⫺0.22ⴱ ⫺0.21ⴱ ⫺0.04 Note. SPC ⫽ success, power, and competition; RABBM ⫽ restrictive affectionate behavior between men. R2 ⫽ .06 for Step 1; ⴱⴱR2 ⫽ .09 for Step 2; ⴱⴱⴱR2 ⫽ .19 for Step 3 (⌬R2 ⫽ .19). ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 218 SCHWARTZ ET AL. college students as predictors of body satisfaction and objectification. The purpose of this study was to add to the current literature in two ways: First, when controlling for ethnic/racial group membership, to clarify the relationship between masculine gender role conflict and body image and self-objectification in men; second, to control for ethnic/racial group differences and masculinity and examine whether DOS (Bowen, 1978) would significantly add to the prediction of body image and self-objectification in men. These hypotheses were partially supported. Because of the sample’s diversity, race/ ethnicity was controlled for in the prediction of body image and self-objectification. Limited previous research had contradictory findings related to ethnic/racial groups and male body image (Ricciardelli et al., 2007). In this study, only participants who identified as Asian American were associated with dissatisfaction with one’s body (as related to muscularity). In addition, although previous literature has suggested a stable relation between aspects of masculinity and male body image (Jacobi & Cash, 1994; Olivardia, 2001; O’Dea & Abraham, 1999), the examination of gender role conflict as a predictor of body image was only partially supported. Finally, DOS was found to be predictive of body image and self-objectification. In particular, this study found that an individuated identity and a lack of ER and sensitivity were related to a positive body image and low selfobjectification. Prediction of Muscularity After controlling for the effects of race/ ethnicity, gender role conflict was not a significant predictor of muscularity. This was surprising because previous research had shown that a man’s desire to be more muscular is related to feelings of conflict regarding success, power, and competition, as well as difficulty balancing work and family (McCreary et al., 2005; Olivardia, 2001). In addition, muscularity has been found to be one attribute that distinguishes men from women, and a way to assert one’s masculinity or power over other men (Pope, Phillips et al., 2000). However, being Asian American was a significant positive predictor of muscle dissatisfaction. It is important to note that when examining mean differences between ethnic/racial groups on body image there were no differences. Thus, this finding should be interpreted cautiously because it may be a statistical artifact. However, this finding is consistent with research conducted by Barnett et al. (2001), which found that Asian American men were less satisfied with their bodies compared with White men. However, results of a meta-analysis on men and body image indicated that the research comparing Asian American men with White men is inconsistent (Ricciardelli et al., 2007). The majority of the articles reviewed did indicate that Asian American men had greater body concerns, but some research indicated that there was no difference between the groups or that Asian American men had lesser bodyimage concerns then Whites. However, a variety of measures were used to examine bodyimage dissatisfaction across the articles, thus the results may not measure the same construct. Our results may be due to measuring a specific aspect of male body-image dissatisfaction: muscularity. It is also important to note that this study did not examine within-group differences and the findings may be due to ethnic identity, racial identity, or acculturation factors. When DOS was added to the regression, the overall model served as a significant predictor of muscle dissatisfaction. Being Asian American continued to be a positive predictor of muscle dissatisfaction. In addition, the ability to take an IP (i.e., a defined and autonomous sense of self) was a negative predictor, indicating that those who are able to take a healthy IP were less focused on being muscular. It is interesting to note that being Asian American and not taking an IP were related to muscle dissatisfaction. As stated above, this finding may be affected by cultural factors, such as collectivistic versus individualistic cultural values. Thus, those from a collectivistic culture who internalize societal pressure to look a certain way may not have autonomous and alternative values about their body. Because there is no research examining DOS, and limited research on cultural differences in relation to male body image, it is difficult to form accurate conclusions. Results have shown that those who are able to take an I-position have greater psychological health and less anxiety than those who engage in emotional reactivity, cutoff, or fusion with others (Skowron & Friedlander, 1998). In addition, those that can take an I-position experience greater auton- BODY IMAGE AND SELF-OBJECTIFICATION omy and are not easily pressured by outside influences. Thus, it is possible that these individuals are able to resist pressure from society to conform to an ideal body type. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Low Body Fat After controlling for ethnicity, gender role conflict was a significant predictor of pressure to have lower body fat. Specifically, gender role conflict related to the inability to express or understand emotions and, alternatively, a lack of conflict concerning affectionate behavior with other men was associated with dissatisfaction with one’s amount of body fat. This finding appears somewhat contradictory. This finding is consistent with the hypothesis that restricting emotions leads to body-image pressure. Restrictive emotionality has consistently been found to be the gender role conflict factor with the strongest relationship to psychological distress (Good et al., 1995), so it was not surprising it predicted dissatisfaction with one’s amount of body fat. Restricting emotions truncates communication and expression and may lead men to overemphasize external factors such as body image. Having low body fat is one way to control how others view you. In addition, having low body fat may be used as a form of intrapsychic control. Controlling the body may defend against uncomfortable emotions, such as anxiety, that come with being overweight. The fact that restrictive affectionate behavior between men was associated with greater satisfaction with one’s body fat was contradictory to our hypotheses. Although there is evidence that low body fat is a prevalent concern in male body image (Tylka et al., 2005), there is evidence that men who feel pressure to adhere to traditional gender roles desire to be muscular and big, with thinness denoting femininity (O’Dea & Abraham, 1999; Olivardia, 2001). Restrictive affectionate behavior between men measures an aspect of homophobia in which men fear appearing gay in their interactions with other men (O’Neil et al., 1995). It may be that this leads men to want to appear bigger and fear that having low body fat will lead to a feminine appearance. When DOS was added to the regression, only the DOS factors of IP were negative predictors and ER was a positive predictor of dissatisfaction with one’s body fat. Similar to the finding 219 on muscularity, it appears that an individuated sense of self is a protective factor from dissatisfaction with one’s body fat. On the other hand, a tendency to be emotionally sensitive and reactive appears to be a risk factor for body fat dissatisfaction. ER is theoretically derived from enmeshment with others (Bowen, 1978) and may relate to increased attention and reactivity to what others think. Previous research has found that, particularly for men, the ability to openly express emotions is a key to psychological health (Good et al., 1995). It may be that even men who are expressing emotions in an unhealthy and reactive way or restricting emotions are at risk to overly focus on body-image issues related to low body fat. Height After controlling for the effects of ethnicity, gender role conflict was not a significant predictor of dissatisfaction with height. When DOS was added to the regression, the overall model served as a significant predictor of dissatisfaction with height. Specifically, those high in emotional reactivity and low in fusion with others were more dissatisfied with height. Because satisfaction with height is not measured in most body-image measures it is difficult to interpret these findings. It may be that because height is not malleable, those who are more ER may experience more conflict about height. Cash, Theriault, and Annis (2004) found that those who had a preoccupied style of attachment (i.e., anxiously focused on others) were less satisfied with their body; however, dissatisfaction with one’s height was not specifically measured. In contrast, people who are fused with others were more satisfied with height. It is important to note that there was no significant correlation between fusion and height; this finding needs to be replicated and should be interpreted cautiously. Those who experience fusion tend to be enmeshed with others, demonstrating a lack of individuality and autonomy, and seek approval at all costs (Peleg-Popko, 2002). It may be possible that people who are fused with others may not be concerned about height, because it is not something that can be changed. Future research is needed to make meaningful interpretations of this finding. 220 SCHWARTZ ET AL. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. Self-Objectification Self-objectification has been defined as internalizing societal messages that view individuals’ value in terms of external factors, leading to a preoccupation with physical appearance (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997; Fredrickson et al., 1998; Muehlenkamp & Saris-Baglama, 2002). In this study, after controlling for race/ ethnicity, it was found that aspects of gender role conflict were significant predictors of selfobjectification. Specifically, an overemphasis on achievement, competition, and power and control over others negatively predicted selfobjectification. In contrast, discomfort with the expression of affectionate behavior in relation to other men positively predicted self-objectification. Thus men who have difficulty making close emotional connections with other men were more likely to self-objectify. When DOS was added, the gender role conflict factor of Success, Power, and Competition was still a negative predictor of self-objectification. This is a surprising finding; it was hypothesized that high gender role conflict would predict selfobjectification. Previous research that found self-objectification related to appearance increases exercise and a drive for muscularity (McCreary et al., 2005; Strelan & Hargreaves, 2005), which may accompany a focus on success and competition with others. It may be that a preoccupation with success and achievement, particularly in an academic environment, leads to less focus on external appearance. It is important to note that this finding could be connected to developmental issues related to being a college student. The findings on DOS factors were that high ER and EC and low IP predicted self-objectification. It appears, therefore, that men who have a healthy sense of self and do not either seek emotional distance in relationships or react emotionally are less likely to internalize societal objectification. Thus, it appears that emotional reactivity to others and being emotionally distant relate to being overly focused on appearance, whereas having an autonomous identity while remaining connected to others is associated with a lower level of self-objectification. Previous research has suggested that the strongest influence of self-objectification in men is the media (Morrison, Morrison, & Hopkins, 2003; Strelan & Hargreaves, 2005). It is important to note that examining self-objectifica- tion in men is relatively new (Strelan & Hargreaves, 2005; Tiggemann & Kuring, 2004) and the results should be replicated. This study suggests that future research should examine the impact of relationships on male self-objectification. Summary and Implications Overall, the consistent findings of this study suggest that for men, having an autonomous sense of self and not being overly emotionally reactive in relation to others are protective factors against body-image concerns and selfobjectification. Previous research has found that alexithimia, or the restriction of emotions, is related to multiple forms of distress in men (Good et al., 1995). In addition, body image has been conceptualized as an external symbol of masculinity (Olivardia et al., 2004). ER, theorized as unhealthy and related to a fusion with others, may lead men, when combined with a lack of an autonomous sense of self, to overemphasizing external factors such as body image. It appears that counselors treating men with body-image concerns should focus on their interpersonal relations with others. In particular, it appears that having an autonomous sense of self (i.e., an identity separate from others while maintaining connected) and finding a balance between restricting emotions and emotional reactivity is important. Interpersonal approaches to therapy (e.g., Bowen, 1978; Teyber, 1992) would allow men to actively find a balance between intimacy and autonomy in relationships as they explore body-image issues. Men’s relationships with each other may be particularly important to examine. It may be that a lack of differentiation leads men to base their perception of themselves on others, resulting in distorted body image. Moreover, healthy differentiation would lead to a healthy balance between affect and cognition (Bowen, 1978). Interpersonally focused therapy may assist men in not reacting impulsively (e.g., with steroids or testosterone) to body-image concerns. This is particularly important considering the multiple pressures coming from the media, society, and family proscribing hegemonic masculinity and an ideal body image. Investing in close relationships with others, particularly men who do not proscribe to rigid body standards or criticize others, may be a protective factor for bodyimage concerns. This may suggest the impor- BODY IMAGE AND SELF-OBJECTIFICATION This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. tance of educating men on supporting each other in relation to body-image issues. In addition, consistent with feminist therapy (Brabeck & Brown, 1997), raising men’s social consciousness on the messages they are receiving from society may allow those men to challenge those messages, thus differentiating and leading to a more realistic body image. Limitations The Differentiation of Self Inventory (Skowron & Friedlander, 1998) was designed to be used with those 25 and older. The average age of our sample was 22.08 years, so it is possible that the results would be different if the participants sampled were a few years older. Furthermore, there may be a generational or developmental factors related to body-image perception; because of our limited sample, we were not able to assess age as a factor. There is evidence that pressure on men to have an ideal body has increased recently (Leit et al., 2001; Morry & Staska, 2001); thus, a longitudinal or crosssectional design might have explicated age as a factor. Because the measurement of male body image is relatively new, more studies are needed to show that the scales are reliable and valid. One strength of our sample is the ethnic diversity of the men. Most other studies consist solely of, or have a large majority of, White men. This is particularly important because body image and DOS may be culture-based phenomena. Future studies could further explore the role of culture, ethnic identity, and worldview in the prediction of body image and self-objectification. Because of previous findings that found differences in body-image satisfaction based on sexual orientation (Morrison, Morrison, & Sager, 2004), only heterosexual men were included in this study. Future studies could examine differences in the variables based on sexual orientation. Another limitation of the current study was that self-report measures were used, thus it is possible that results do not represent the actual feelings of the participants. Data was collected on college men and therefore results may not generalize to other populations. Because a regression analysis was used, no causal explanations can be made regarding the findings. Future research could measure body mass index and examine actual eating and exercise behavior. 221 Examining current peer and romantic relationships separate from the family of origin also may help understand the causes of body-image distress. Finally, the results of this study suggest that for men, emotional variables and relational factors may be important in body image and self-objectification research. Future research could more directly measure emotional and relational aspects and their impact on body-image variables. References Ata, R. N., Ludden, A. B., & Lally, M. (2007). The effects of gender and family, friend, and media influences on eating behaviors and body image during adolescence. Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 36, 1024 –1037. Barnett, H. L., Keel, K. K., & Conoscenti, L. M. (2002). Body type preferences in Asian and Caucasian college students. Sex Roles, 45(11/12), 867– 878. Blazina, C., & Watkins, E., Jr. (1996). Masculine gender role conflict: Effects on college men’s psychological well-being, chemical substance usage, and attitudes toward help-seeking. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 43, 461– 465. Blazina, C., & Watkins, E., Jr. (2000). Separation/ individuation, parental attachment, and male gender role conflict: Attitudes toward the feminine and the fragile masculine self. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 1, 126 –132. Blouin, A. G., & Goldfield, G. S. (1995). Body image and steroid use in male bodybuilders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18, 159 –165. Bowen, M. (1978). Family therapy in clinical practice. New York: Jason Aronson. Brabeck, M., & Brown, L. (1997). Feminist theory and psychological practice. Shaping the future of feminist psychology: Education, research, and practice (pp. 15–35). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Bray, J. H., Harvey, D. M., & Williamson, D. S. (1987). Intergenerational family relationships: An evaluation of theory and measurement. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 24, 516 –528. Byely, L., Archibald, A. B., Graber, J., & BrooksGunn, J. (2000). A prospective study of familial and social influence on girls’ body image and dieting. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 155–164. Cafri, G., Strauss, J., & Thompson, J. K. (2002). Male body image: Satisfaction and its relationship to well-being using the somatomorphic matrix. International Journal of Men’s Health, 1, 215– 231. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 222 SCHWARTZ ET AL. Cafri, G., & Thompson, J. K. (2004). Evaluating the convergence of muscle appearance attitude measures. Assessment, 11, 224 –229. Cash, T. F., & Fleming, E. C. (2002). Body image and social relations. In T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of theory, research, and clinical practice (pp. 277–286). New York: Guilford Press. Cash, T. F., & Pruzinsky, T. (2002). Future challenges for body image theory, research, and clinical, practice. In T. F. Cash & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body images: A handbook of theory, research, and clinical practice (pp. 509 –516). New York: Guilford Press. Cash, T. F., Theriault, J., & Annis, N. M. (2004). Body image in an interpersonal context: Adult attachment, fear of intimacy, and social anxiety. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology, 23, 89 – 103. Demarest, J., & Allen, R. (2000). Body image: Gender, ethnic, and age differences. Journal of Social Psychology, 140, 465– 472. Dion, K. L., Dion, K. K., & Keelan, J. P. (1990). Appearance anxiety as a dimension of social, and sexual functioning. Sex Roles, 52, 220 –225. Drummond, M. J. N. (2002). Men, body image, and eating disorders. International Journal of Men’s Health, 1, 89 –103. Edman, J. L., Yates, A., & Aruguete, M. S. (2005). Negative emotion and disordered eating among obese college students. Eating Behaviors, 6, 308 – 317. Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. (1997). Objectification theory: Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173–206. Fredrickson, B. L., Roberts, T., Noll, S. M., Quinn, D. M., & Twenge, J. M. (1998). That swimsuit becomes you. Sex differences in self-objectification, restrained eating, and math performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 269 –284. Good, G. E., Robertson, J. M., O’Neil, J. M., Fitzgerald, L. F., Stevens, M., DeBord, K. A., et al. (1995). Male gender role conflict: Psychometric issues and relations to psychological distress. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 3–10. Grogan, S., & Richards, H. (2002). Body image: Focus groups with boys and men. Men & Masculinities, 4, 219 –232. Hargreaves, D. A., & Tiggemann, M. (2004). Idealized media images and adolescent body image: “Comparing” boys and girls. Body Image, 1, 351– 361. Harvey, D. M., Curry, C. J., & Bray, J. H. (1991). Individuation and intimacy in intergenerational relationships and health: Patterns across two generations. Journal of Family Psychology, 5, 204 –236. Heinberg, L. J., Thompson, J. K., & Stormer, S. (1995). Development and validation of the sociocultural attitudes towards appearance questionnaire. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 81– 89. Jacobi, L., & Cash, T. F. (1994). In pursuit of the perfect appearance: Discrepancies among selfideal percepts of multiple physical attributes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 379 –396. Jenkins, S. M., Buboltz, W. C., Schwartz, J. P., & Johnson, P. (2005). Differentiation of self and psychosocial development. Contemporary Family Therapy, 27, 251–261. Kimmel, S. B., & Mahalik, J. R. (2004). Measuring masculine body ideal distress. International Journal of Men’s Health, 3, 1–10. Klein, A. M. (1993). Little big men: Body building subculture and gender construction. Albany, NY: SUNY Press. Leit, R. A., Pope, H. G., & Gray, J. J. (2001). Cultural expectations of muscularity in men: The evolution of playgirl centerfolds. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 29, 90 –93. Mahalik, J. R. (2000). Gender role conflict in men as a predictor of self-ratings of behavior on the Interpersonal Circle. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology, 19, 276 –292. Mahalik, J. R., Cournoyer, R. J., DeFranc, W., Cherry, M., & Napolitano, J. M. (1998). Men’s gender role conflict and use of psychological defenses. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45, 247–255. Mayville, S., Katz, R. C., Gipson, M. T., & Cabral, K. (1999). Assessing the prevalence of body dysmorphic disorder in an ethnically diverse group of adolescents. Journal of Child & Family Studies, 8, 357–362. McCreary, D. R., & Sasse, D. (2000). An exploration of the drive for muscularity in adolescent boys and girls. Journal of American College Health, 48, 297–304. McCreary, D. R., Saucier, D. M., & Courtenay, W. H. (2005). The drive for muscularity and masculinity: Testing the associations among genderrole traits, behaviors, attitudes and conflict. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 6, 83–94. Meyers, L. S., Gamst, G., & Guarinno, A. J. (2006). Applied multivariate research: Design and interpretation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Miller, K. J., Gleaves, D. H., Hirsch, T. G., Green, B. A., Snow, A. C., & Chanda, C. C. (2000). Comparisons of body image dimensions by race/ ethnicity and gender in a university population. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 27, 310 –316. Mills, J. S., & D’Alfonso, S. R. (2007). Competition and male body image: Increased drive for muscu- This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. BODY IMAGE AND SELF-OBJECTIFICATION larity following failure to a female. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology, 26, 505–518. Mishkind, M. E., Rodin, J., Silberstein, L. R., & Striegel-Moore, R. H. (1986). The embodiment of masculinity: Cultural, psychological and behavioral dimensions. American Behavioral Scientist, 29, 545–562. Morrison, M., Morrison, S., & Sager, N. (2004). Does body satisfaction differ between gay men and lesbian women and heterosexual men and women? A meta-analytic review. Body Image, 1, 127–138. Morrison, T. G., Morrison, M. A., & Hopkins, C. (2003). Striving for bodily perfection? An exploration of the drive for muscularity in Canadian men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 4, 111– 120. Morry, M. M., & Staska, S. L. (2001). Magazine exposure: Internalization, self-objectification, eating attitude, and body satisfaction in male and female university students. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 33, 269 –279. Muehlenkamp, J. J., & Saris-Baglama, R. N. (2002). Self-objectification and its psychological outcomes for college women. Psychology of Women, 26, 371–379. Murdock, N. L., & Gore, P. A. (2004). Stress, coping, and differentiation of self: A test of Bowen theory. Contemporary Family Therapy, 26, 319 –335. Noll, S. M. (1996). The relationship between sexual objectification and disordered eating: Correlational and experimental tests of body shame as a mediator. Doctoral dissertation, Duke University, Durham, NC. Noll, S. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). A mediational model lnking self-objectification, body same and disordered eating. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 22, 623– 636. O’Dea, J. A., & Abraham, S. (1999). Onset of disordered eating attitudes and behaviors in early adolescence: Interplay of pubertal status, gender, weight, and age. Adolescence, 34, 671– 679. Olivardia, R. (2001). Mirror, mirror on the wall, who’s the largest of them all? The features and phenomenology of muscle dysmorphia. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 9, 254 –259. Olivardia, R., Pope, H. G., Borowiecki, J. J., & Cohane, G. H. (2004). Biceps and body image: The relationship between muscularity and selfesteem, depression, and eating disorder symptoms. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 5, 112–120. O’Neil, J. M. (2008). Summarizing 25 years of research on men’s gender role conflict using the Gender Role Conflict Scale: New research paradigms and clinical implications. Counseling Psychologist, 36, 358 – 445. O’Neil, J. M., Good, G. E., & Holmes, S. (1995). Fifteen years of theory and research on men’s gender role conflict: New paradigms for empirical 223 research. In R. Levant & W. Pollack (Eds.), A new psychology of men (pp. 164 –206). New York: Basic Books. O’Neil, J. M., Helms, B., Gable, R., David, L., & Wrightsman, L. (1986). Gender Role Conflict Scale (GRCS): College men’s fear of femininity. Sex Roles, 14, 335–350. Peleg-Popko, O. (2002). Bowen theory: A study of differentiation of self, social anxiety and physiological symptoms. Contemporary Family Therapy, 24, 355–369. Pope, H. G., Gruber, A. J., Mangweth, B., Bureau, B., deCol, C., & Jouvent, R., et al. (2000). Body image perception among men in three countries. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1297–1301. Pope, H. G., Olivardia, R., Gruber, A., & Borowiecki, J. (1999). Evolving ideals of male body image as seen through action toys. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 65–72. Pope, H. G., Phillips, K. A., & Olivardia, R. (2000). The Adonis complex: The secret crisis of male body obsession. New York: Free Press. Ricciardelli, L. A., & McCabe, M. P. (2004). A biopsychosocial model of disordered eating and the pursuit of muscularity in adolescent boys. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 179 –205. Ricciardelli, L. A., McCabe, M. P., Williams, R. J., & Thompson, K. (2007). The role of ethnicity and culture in body image and disordered eating among males. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 582– 606. Robinson, D. T., & Schwartz, J. P. (2004). Relationship between gender role conflict and attitudes toward women and African Americans. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 5, 65–71. Rorty, M., Yager, J., Buckwalter, J. G., Rossotto, E., & Guthrie, D. (2000). Development and validation of the parental intrusiveness rating scale among bulimic and comparison women. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 188 –201. Russell, W. D. (2002). Comparison of self-esteem, body satisfaction, and social physique anxiety across males of different exercise frequency and racial background. Journal of Sport Behavior, 25, 74 –90. Schwartz, J. P., & Tylka, T. L. (2008). Exploring entitlement as a moderator and mediator of the relationship between masculine gender role conflict and men’s body esteem. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 9, 67– 81. Schwartz, J. P., Waldo, M., & Higgins, A. J. (2004). Attachment styles: Relationship to masculine gender role conflict in college men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 5, 143–146. Sharpe, M. J., & Heppner, P. P. (1991). Gender role, gender-role conflict, and psychological well-being in men. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 323–330. This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers. This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly. 224 SCHWARTZ ET AL. Shelton, S., & Liljequist, L. (2002). Characteristics and behaviors associated with body image in male domestic violence offenders. Eating Behaviors, 3, 217–227. Skowron, E. A. (2000). The role of differentiation of self in marital adjustment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47, 229 –237. Skowron, E. A., & Friedlander, M. L. (1998). The differentiation of self inventory: Development and initial validation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45, 235–246. Skowron, E. A., Holmes, S. E., & Sabatelli, R. M. (2003). Deconstructing differentiation: Self regulation, interdependent relating, and well-being in adulthood. Contemporary Family Therapy: An International Journal, 25, 111–129. Smith, D. E., Thompson, J. K., Raczynski, J. M., & Hilner, J. E. (1999). Body image among men and women in a biracial cohort: The CARDIA study. Smith International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 71– 82. Strelan, P., & Hargreaves, D. (2005). Reasons for exercise and body esteem: Men’s responses to self-objectification. Sex Roles, 53, 495–503. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). New York: Harper Collins. Tager, D., Good, G. E., & Morrison, J. B. (2006). Our bodies, ourselves revisited: Male body image and psychological well-being. International Journal of Men’s Health, 5, 228 –237. Teyber, E. (1992). Interpersonal process in psychotherapy: A guide for clinical training (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson Brooks/Cole. Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (1999). The scope of body im- age disturbance: The big picture. In J. K. Thompson, L. J. Heinberg, & M. Altabe (Eds.), Exacting beauty: Theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance (pp. 19 –50). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Tiggemann, M., & Kuring, J. (2004). The role of body objectification in disordered eating and depressed mood. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 299 –311. Tylka, T. L., Bergeron, D., & Schwartz, J. P. (2005). Development and psychometric evaluation of the Male Body Attitudes Scale (MBAS). Body Image, 2, 161–175. Vartanian, L. R., Giant, C. L., & Passino, R. M. (2001). “Ally McBeal vs. Arnold Schwarzenegger”: Comparing mass media, interpersonal feedback and gender as predictors of satisfaction with body thinness and muscularity. Social Behavior & Personality, 29, 711–723. Weinke, C. (1998). Negotiating the male body: Men, masculinity, and cultural ideals. Journal of Men’s Studies, 6, 255–282. Williamson, D. A., Davis, C. J., Bennett, S. M., Goreczny, A. J., & Gleaves, D. H. (1985). Development of a simple procedure for assessing body image disturbance. Behavioral Assessment, 11, 433– 446. Wright, J. C., & Huston, A. C. (1983). A matter of form: Potentials of television for young viewers. American Psychologist, 38, 835– 843. Received August 22, 2008 Revision received October 21, 2009 Accepted October 21, 2009 䡲