Uploaded by Priyanshi Khandelwal

TOL intro

advertisement
PLANNING:
Planning is the mental process that allows us to choose the necessary actions to reach a
goal, decide the right order, assign each task to the proper cognitive resources, and establish a
plan of action.
Planning has been defined as the identification and organisation of the steps and elements
needed to carry out an intention or achieve a goal (Lezak, 1995).
Planning is the ability to set goals, to monitor performance so as to reach the goals, and to
make corrections in the course adopted, in order to ensure that the goal is attained. Goal setting
involves not only identifying the final goal, but also identifying the intermediate goals which have
to be attained in order to achieve the final goal. The intermediate goals may be in conformity
with the final goal or may be contradictory to the final goal. The essence of planning consists of
attaining a goal through a series of intermediate steps. The subject plans in advance the
complete sequence of moves required to solve the problem, and in order to do so anticipates
the consequences of one or another course of action (Baker et al., 1996)
Neuroscience of planning
● It is generally assumed individuals presented with a complex problem engage in
preliminary planning and that this planning involves prefrontal areas associated with
planning. Supportive evidence comes from patients with damage to prefrontal areas,
who typically have impaired planning and problem solving (Szczepanski & Knight, 2014).
There was increased activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex when participants
solved complex versions of this task. In sum, the prefrontal cortex is important in
planning on many problem-solving tasks. However, many other brain areas are also
involved (Szczepanski & Knight, 2014).
● Dagher et al. (1999) used the Tower of London task in which coloured discs must be
moved one by one from an initial state to match the goal state. There was increased
activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex when participants solved complex versions
of this task. In sum, the prefrontal cortex is important in planning on many
problem-solving tasks. However, many other brain areas are also involved (Szczepanski
& Knight, 2014).
● Crescentini et al. (2012) supported the distinction between plan production and plan
execution using simple versions of the Tower of Hanoi task. The dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex was more active during initial planning than plan execution. In contrast, posterior
temporal areas, inferior frontal regions and dorsolateral premotor cortex were more
activated during plan execution.
●
Lesion studies have shown that left frontal lesions are associated with deficits of
planning (Shallice, 1982). Moreover, the studies have found that the inappropriate
organizational strategies associated with poor planning are greater in bilateral prefrontal
●
lesions(Owens, Brownes, Shakan, Poltrey & Robbins,1990). Imaging studies have found
that increased activation of the left prefrontal cortex is associated with more efficient
planning in terms of longer time to plan and less number of moves.
Planning using the Tower of London test (Morris, Ahmed, Syed & Toone, 1993) activates
a wide network consisting of the dorsal prefrontal cortex is associated with the
components of generating, selecting and/or remembering mental moves(Rowe, Owen,
Johnsrude & Passingham, 2001). Planning is a complex Function with many
components such as speed of processing, mental flexibility, working memory, Regulation
of thought, error correction ability.
Importance of planning
1. In problem solving:
● Newell and Simon (1972) assumed problem solvers typically engage in limited planning
because of the constraints of short-term memory capacity. Patsenko and Altmann (2010)
obtained strong support using Tower of Hanoi problems. Sometimes they added, deleted
or moved discs during participants’ eye movements so they were not directly aware of
the change. These changes only minimally disrupted performance, strongly suggesting
that the participants’ next move was triggered by the current state of the problem rather
than a preformed plan. There are substantial individual differences in planning for
problem-solving tasks. Koppenol-Gonzalez et al. (2010) found with the Tower of London
task that some participants engaged in efficient planning (considerable preplanning of
moves and high performance). In contrast, other participants showed very little evidence
of effective planning (short period of preplanning and numerous errors). Most individual
differences in performance on this task can be explained by the single factor of planning
ability (Debelak et al., 2016). The amount of planning is very flexible. Delaney et al.
(2004) found little evidence of planning on water-jar problems when participants chose
their preferred strategy. However, instructions to generate the complete solution before
making any moves led to detailed planning andfaster problem solution. Morgan and
Patrick (2013) argued that increasing the cost of accessing important task-relevant
information (the goal state) on the Tower of Hanoi task would lead to more planning. It
produced increased planning and also led to problems being solved in fewer moves. If
planning involves deliberate processes, we would expect problem solvers to be
consciously aware of it. Evidence suggesting important problem-solving processes occur
below the level of conscious awareness was reported by Paynter et al. (2010) using
event-related potentials (ERPs; see Glossary). They observed clear differences in the
ERPs associated with correct and incorrect moves early in the problem when no
behavioural evidence indicated participants were making progress.
2. Importance of planning in academic writing:
Writing consists of three phases: planning, sentence generation, and revising (Mayer, 2004).
However— like the similar stages we discussed in connection with spoken language—these
tasks often overlap in time (Kellogg, 1994, 1996; Ransdell & Levy, 1999). For example, you may
be planning your overall writing strategy while you generate parts of several sentences. Most
people begin a formal writing project by generating a list of ideas; this process is called
prewriting. Prewriting is difficult and strategic—very different from many relatively automatic
language tasks (Collins, 1998; Torrance et al., 1996). As you can imagine, students differ
enormously in the quality of the ideas they generate during this phase (Bruning et al., 1999).
According to the research, good writers are more likely than poor writers to spend high-quality
time in planning during prewriting (Hayes, 1989). Some people prefer to outline a paper before
they begin to write (Kellogg, 1998; McCutchen et al., 2008). An outline may help you avoid
overloaded attention. You’ve probably had the experience of beginning to write a paper, only to
find that each of several interrelated ideas needs to be placed first! An outline can help you sort
these ideas into an orderly, linear sequence, although not all writers find that an outline
is helpful (Engle, 2011).
Writing involves proposing or planning, translating, transcribing, and evaluating and revising the
text that has been produced. Shifts from one writing process to another depend on a monitor or
control system. Good writers use acknowledge-transforming rather than knowledge-telling
strategy and devote more time to revision. Expert writers attain a knowledge-crafting stage
emphasising the reader’s needs. The working memory system (especially the central executive)
is heavily involved in the writing process. Word processing often enhances writing quality,
probably by encouraging revision and thinking processes.
Pauses account for over half of writing time. Medimorec and Risko (2017) analysed the pause
data of students writing narrative essays (about a memorable day) and argumentative essays
(about mobile-phone use in schools). Pauses occurred most often at paragraph boundaries,
followed by sentence boundaries, suggesting they often indicate planning processes.
Limpo and Alves (2018) studied key aspects of writing dynamics using the triple-task technique.
Limpo and Alves (2018) found that Firstly, time spent on planning is reduced during the course
of writing. Second, the time spent on the revising process increased over time. Third, all three
writing processes (i.e., planning, translating and revising) occurred during all phases of the
writing process. Fourth, reaction times to the occasional beeps were slowed most during
revising and least during translating. These findings indicate that revising was the most
cognitively demanding process, followed by planning and translating in that order. Thus, as Levy
and Ransdell (1995) found, episodes of planning and revising were shorter than translating or
text generation.
Role of planning: planning-control model
Glover (2004) proposed a planning-control model of goal-directed action towards objects.
According to this model, we initially use a planning system followed by a control system but the
two systems often overlap in time. Here are the main features of the two systems:
(1) Planning system
● It is used mostly before the initiation of movement.
● It selects an appropriate target (e.g., cup of coffee), decides how it should be grasped and
works out the timing of the movement.
● It is influenced by factors such as the individual’s goals, the nature of the target object, the
visual context and various cognitive processes.
● It is relatively slow because it uses much information and is influenced by conscious
processes. It is used during the carrying out of a movement.
● It ensures movements are accurate, making adjustments, if necessary, based on visual
feedback. Efference copy (see Glossary) is used to compare actual with desired movement.
Proprioception is also involved.
● It is influenced by the target object’s spatial characteristics (e.g., size; shape; orientation) but
not by the surrounding context.
● It is fairly fast because it uses little information and is not susceptible to conscious influence.
According to the planning-control model, most errors in human action stem from the planning
system. In contrast, the control system typically ensures actions are accurate and achieve their
goal. Many visual illusions occur because of the influence of visual context. Since information
about visual context is used only by the planning system, responses to visual illusions should
typically be inaccurate if they depend on the planning system but accurate if they depend on the
control system. There are similarities between the planning-control model and Milner and
Goodale’s perception action model. However, Glover (2004) focused more on the processing
changes occurring during action performance.
Findings
Glover et al. (2012) compared the brain areas involved in planning and control using a planning
condition (prepare to reach and grasp an object but remain still) and a control condition (reach
out immediately for the object). There was practically no overlap in the brain areas associated
with planning and control. This finding supports the model’s assumption that planning and
control processes are separate.
According to the planning-control model, various factors (e.g., semantic properties of the visual
scene) influence the planning process associated with goal-directed movements but not the
subsequent control process. This prediction was tested by Namdar et al. (2014). Participants
grasped an object in front of them using their thumb and index finger. The object had a
task-irrelevant digit (1, 2, 8 or 9) on it. As predicted, numerically larger digits led to larger grip
apertures during the first half of the movement trajectory but not the second half (involving the
control process).
According to Glover (2004), action planning involves conscious processing followed by rapid
non-conscious processing during action control. These theoretical assumptions can be tested
by requiring participants to carry out a second task while performing an action towards an
object. According to the model, this second task should disrupt planning but not control.
However, Hesse et al. (2012) found a second task disrupted planning and control when
participants made grasping movements towards objects. Thus, planning and control can both
require attention resources.
According to the model, visual illusions occur because misleading visual context influences the
initial planning system rather than the later control system. Roberts et al. (2013) required
participants to make rapid reaching movements to a Müller-Lyer figure. Vision was available
only during the first 200 ms of movement or the last 200ms. The findings were opposite to those
predicted theoretically – performance was more accurate with early vision than late vision.
Elliott et al. (2017) explained the above findings with their multiple process models. According to
this model, performance was good when early vision was available because of a control system
known as impulse control.
Impulse control “entails an early, and continuing, comparison of expected sensory
consequences to perceived sensory consequences to regulate limb direction and velocity during
the distance-covering phase of the movement”
Evaluation
Glover’s (2004) planning-control model has proved successful in various ways. First, it
successfully developed the common assumption that motor movements towards an object
involve successive planning and control processes. Second, the assumption that cognitive
processes are important in action planning is correct. Third, there is evidence (e.g., Glover et al.,
2012) that separate brain areas are involved in planning and control.
What are the model’s limitations? First, the planning system involves several very different
processes: “goal determination; target identification and selection; analysis of object affordances
[potential object uses]; timing; and computation of the metrical properties of the target such as
its size, shape, orientation and position relative to the body” (Glover et al., 2012). This diversity
sheds doubt on the assumption there is a single planning system.
Second, the model argues control occurs late during object-directed movements and is
influenced by visual feedback. However, there appears to be a second control process (called
impulse control by Elliott et al., 2017) operating throughout the movement trajectory and not
influenced by visual feedback.
Third, and related to the second point, the model presents an over simplified picture of the
processes involved in goal-directed action. More specifically, the processing involved in
producing goal-directed movements is far more complex than implied by the notion of a
planning process followed by a control process. For example, planning and control processes
are often so intermixed that “the distinction between movement planning and movement control
is blurred” (Gallivan et al., 2018).
Fourth, complex decision-making processes are often involved when individuals plan
goal-directed actions in the real world. For example, when planning, tennis players must often
decide between a simple shot minimising energy expenditure and risk or injury or a more
ambitious shot that might immediately win the current point (Gallivan et al., 2018).
Fifth, the model is designed to account for planning and control processes when only one object
is present or of interest. In contrast, visual scenes in everyday life are often far more complex
and contain several objects of potential relevance (see below).
Role of planning: changing action plans
We all have considerable experience of changing, modifying and abandoning action plans with
respect to objects in the environment. How do we resolve competition among action plans?
According to Song (2017)
“Critical is the existence of parallel motor planning processes, which allow efficient and timely
changes.” What evidence indicates we often process information about several different
potential actions simultaneously? Suppose participants are given the task of reaching rapidly
towards a target in the presence of distractors (Song & Nakayama, 2008). On some trials, their
reach is initially directed towards the target. On other trials, their initial reach is directed towards
a distracter but this is corrected in mid-flight producing a strongly curved trajectory. Song and
Nakayama’s key finding was that corrective movements occurred very rapidly following the
onset of the initial movement. This finding strongly implies that the corrective movement had
been planned prior to execution of the initial incorrect movement.
Song (2017) discussed several other studies where similar findings were obtained. He
concluded, “The sensori-motor system generates multiple competing plans in parallel before
actions are initiated this concurrent processing enables us to efficiently resolve competition and
select one appropriate action rapidly”
Factors that influence planning
Ability to represent the problem mentally (Nitschke et al., 2012) influences planning; participants
who take the time to clearly represent the problem tend to plan better and arrive at the solution
faster.
Instructions given to plan ensures better planning (Delaney et al., 2004); participants who have
been instructed to plan the entire task before making a move performed better.
Having access to important task-relevant information (eg., the goal state) improves planning
(Morgan and Patrick, 2013); participants who were clear about the goal state planned to reach
the goal more easily.
Problem solving ability is correlated to planning ability.
Damage to the prefrontal cortex results in poorer planning (Goel and Grafman, 1995, Colvin et.
al. 2001).
The Tower of London Test
Planning is tested using the Tower of London Test (Shallice, 1982). The initial TOL was devised
by Tim Shallice (1982) as an improvement upon the Tower of Hanoi (TOH) task. Shallice
employed his TOL to assess planning in patients with frontal lobe lesions.
The test evaluates the subject's ability to plan and anticipate the results of their actions to
achieve a predetermined goal. The test consists of two identical wooden boards. Each board
measures 38 cms long and 13 cms wide. Each board is fitted with 3 round pegs of different
sizes. The first peg is 18 cms inheight, the second is 11 cms in height and the third is 7 cms in
height. There are three wooden balls, painted red, green and blue respectively. Each ball has a
bore in the middle. The tallest can hold 3 balls. The second tallest can hold two balls, while the
shortest can hold one ball. peg.
Download