Part 1: Timeline and essential features of the Development of the English Language from Early Modern English to Contemporary English 1 0 9 8 1 1 7 6 1 1 5 4 1 1 3 2 1 1 1 2 1 0 9 8 7 3 4 5 6 . Phonetics: The study of speech sounds and their production, transmission, and perception. . Phonology: The study of the sound systems of languages, including the patterns and rules for combining sounds. . Morphology: The study of the structure of words and how they are formed. . Syntax: The study of sentence structure and the rules for combining words to form sentences. . Semantics: The study of meaning in language, including how words and phrases acquire meaning, and how meaning is conveyed in sentences. . Pragmatics: The study of how context influences the interpretation of language, including the role of implicature, presupposition, and speech acts. . Discourse analysis: The study of how language is used in different contexts, including the analysis of conversations, written texts, and other forms of communication. . Sociolinguistics: The study of how language is influenced by social factors, including regional, social, and cultural variation in language use. . Language change: The study of how languages evolve over time, including the causes and mechanisms of language change, as well as the effects of language change on language structure and use. . Lexical semantics: The study of how words acquire meaning and how their meaning changes over time. . Etymology: The study of the origins and history of words and how they have changed over time. . Dialectology: The study of regional and social variation in language, including differences in pronunciation, vocabulary, and grammar. . Sociophonetics: The study of the relationship between social factors and speech sounds, including variation in accent and dialect. . Register: The variety of language used in a particular context, such as formal or informal, technical or non-technical, and so on. . Style: The distinctive way in which an individual or group uses languageCollocation: The habitual juxtaposition of a particular word with another word or words with a frequency greater than chance. . Connotation: The emotional or cultural associations that a word carries beyond its literal meaning. . Denotation: The literal or dictionary definition of a word. . Diction: The choice and use of words and phrases in speech or writing. . Idiom: A group of words whose meaning is different from the meanings of the individual words. . Jargon: Specialized language used by a particular group or profession. Early Modern English (1500-1700) Graphology and Orthography: ● Printing press allowed for standardization of spellings and punctuation 1 2 Key Terms Great Vowel Shift led to changes in pronunciation Phonology: ● Great Vowel Shifts led to changes in pronunciation Morphology: ● Development of regular inflectional suffixes for past tense and plural form of nouns Syntax: ● Development of complex sentence structures ● Move towards a more analytical syntax Lexis: ● Vocabulary greatly influenced by Latin and Greek ● New words coined to reflect advances in science and technology Semantics: ● Semantic range of words expanded ● Some words took on new meanings Pragmatics: ● Use of polite language and honorifics became more prevalent Grammar: ● Regularization of past tense ● Use of auxiliary verb "do" ● Loss of inflectional suffix "-th" for third-person singular verbs ● Contemporary English (1800-present) Graphology and Orthography: ● Use of computers and digital technology has led to changes in the way we write and read ○ With the widespread use of computers, most of us now do our writing using word processing software, such as Microsoft Word or Google Docs. This has made it much easier to edit and revise our writing, as well as to produce clean, professionallooking documents. ○ Digital reading: With the rise of e-books and digital reading devices, such as Amazon's Kindle, more and more people are doing their reading on screens rather than on paper. This has led to changes in the way we read, with studies suggesting that digital reading may lead to more skimming and less deep engagement with the text. ○ Globalization: The internet has made it much easier for people all over the world to communicate and share information. This has led to an increase in the number of people using English as a second language and has also led to the development of new varieties of English, such as Singlish (Singaporean English) and Hinglish (Hindi-English). Phonology: ● Pronunciation has remained relatively stable, although there have been some changes due to globalization and the influence of other languages Morphology: ● Loss of many inflectional suffixes ● Greater reliance on word order and auxiliary verbs to convey meaning Syntax: ● More streamlined syntax with emphasis on simplicity and clarity Lexis: ● Vocabulary constantly evolving with new words and phrases added to reflect advances in technology and changes in society Semantics: ● Semantic range of words constantly expanding with some words taking on new meanings and others falling out of use Pragmatics: ● Use of language heavily influenced by social and cultural norms ● Greater awareness of issues related to inclusivity and diversity Grammar: ● Greater degree of standardization with focus on clarity and simplicity ● Variation in use of language depending on region and social class Part 2: Processes of Language Change Language is not a static entity, but a dynamic one that evolves over time. This evolution can be attributed to various processes of language change. In this study guide, we will discuss the processes involved in the formation of new words and how they contribute to the evolution of language. Coinage: Coinage is the process of creating new words from scratch. It involves inventing a completely new word to describe a new object or concept. The new word may be based on an existing word or entirely made up. Examples of coined words include "Google," "Kleenex," and "Xerox." Derivation: Derivation involves the creation of new words by adding affixes to existing words. An affix is a prefix or suffix that alters the meaning of a word. For example, adding the suffix "-ness" to the word "happy" creates the new word "happiness." Similarly, adding the prefix "un-" to the word "happy" creates the new word "unhappy.” Telescoping: Telescoping involves the shortening of words through various processes such as acronyms, compounding, blending, clipping, coalescence, and backformation. ● Acronyms are formed by taking the first letter of each word in a phrase and creating a new word from those letters. Examples of acronyms include NASA, NATO, and AIDS. ● Compounding involves combining two or more words to create a new word. Examples of compound words include "laptop," "hamburger," and "basketball." ● Blending involves combining parts of two or more words to create a new word. Examples of blended words include "brunch" (breakfast + lunch) and "smog" (smoke + fog). ● Clipping involves shortening a word by removing one or more syllables. Examples of clipped words include "math" (mathematics) and "phone" (telephone). ● Coalescence involves combining two or more words into a single word by blending the sounds and meanings of the original words. Examples of coalesced words include "smog" (smoke + fog) and "motel" (motor + hotel). ● Backformation involves creating a new word by removing a suffix or prefix from an existing word. For example, the word "editor" was created by removing the "-er" suffix from the word "editorial." Changes through Borrowing: Changes through borrowing involve the adoption of words from other languages. Borrowed words can either retain their original form or be adapted to fit the sound and structure of the borrowing language. Examples of borrowed words in English include "sushi" (Japanese), "entrepreneur" (French), and "kangaroo" (Australian Aboriginal). Changes in Grammar and Style: Changes in grammar and style involve alterations to the way words are used in a language. These changes can include changes in verb endings and the use of modals in verb forms. English has become more reliant on word order instead of inflections. Graphology: Graphology is the study of how the visual aspect of language, including spelling and punctuation, has evolved over time. Changes in graphology can also contribute to the evolution of language. Etymology: the study of the historical relation between a word and the earlier form or forms from which it has developed. ● Amelioration is the process by which the meaning of a word changes to become more positive. A well-known example is the word nice, which, when it first appeared in about 1300 AD, meant clumsy or stupid. ● Pejoration is the reverse process by which the meaning of a word becomes more negative than its meaning in earlier times. For example, silly originally meant blessed. ● Broadening is the process by which a word expands from its original meaning and becomes more general. Examples of this are business, which originally meant being anxious and full of care but has now broadened to include all kinds of work. ● Narrowing is the process by which a word takes on a more restricted meaning. For example, girl originally meant a young person. Part 3: Theories of Language Change Functional Theory (Michael Halliday) . Halliday's functional theory broadly explains language change as a tool which enables changing economic and social functions in society to be carried out. According to Functional Theory, language changes according to the needs of its users. In Functional Theory, lexis is not actively discarded. It evolves until it is of no further use and is then replaced by new words needed such as the need for new technological developments. It proposes that language changes in response to the changing needs of society. Language is seen as a tool that serves a communicative purpose, and it changes according to the needs of its users. The theory argues that language change is motivated by functional needs, such as the need to express new concepts or adapt to changes in social practices. The functional theory is based on the idea that language is a social practice and is shaped by social factors, such as the culture, history, and geography of its speakers. . Reasons for language change: The functional theory argues that language changes for functional reasons, such as to express new concepts or to adapt to changes in social practices. One example of this is the development of new words to describe new technological developments. Another example is the change in the use of pronouns to reflect changes in social attitudes towards gender. Functional needs are the primary driving force behind language change, according to the functional theory. . There are several pieces of evidence that support Halliday's functional theory of language change: ○ Correlation between social change and linguistic change: A study by Trudgill (1974) found a correlation between social change and linguistic change. Trudgill observed that as Norwich, England became more socially diverse in the mid-20th ○ ○ ○ ○ ○ century, there was a corresponding change in the pronunciation of certain vowel sounds by the local population. This supports Halliday's assertion that language changes according to the needs of its users in response to changing social circumstances. The emergence of new terminology: The introduction of new technological and scientific concepts often requires the creation of new words and terminology. For example, the words "internet," "computer virus," and "smartphone" did not exist in the English language until relatively recently. This supports Halliday's claim that language evolves to meet the needs of its users. Adaptation of language to suit different situations: Halliday's functional theory suggests that language is used for a variety of functions, including transactional, interpersonal, and imaginative purposes. Studies have shown that people adapt their language use to suit different situations and contexts. For example, a study by Giles and Powesland (1975) found that people in the Welsh town of Aberystwyth used more Welsh words and phrases when speaking to someone they perceived as Welsh rather than English. This demonstrates how language use can vary depending on the social and cultural context, which supports Halliday's theory that language changes in response to the needs of its users. Changes in language use over time: Linguistic studies have shown that language use changes over time. For example, the use of the word "gay" has shifted in meaning from "happy" to "homosexual" in the last century. This change in meaning is likely due to the changing social attitudes towards homosexuality. This supports Halliday's view that language changes in response to changing social circumstances. Use of language as a tool of power: Halliday's functional theory suggests that language can be used as a tool of power in social interaction. A study by Tannen (1990) found that men and women use language differently in social situations. Men tend to use language in a competitive and confrontational manner, while women tend to use language to build and maintain social relationships. This demonstrates how language use can be influenced by social power dynamics, which supports Halliday's theory that language changes in response to the needs of its users in different social contexts. These studies support the idea that language use is shaped by social factors and is responsive to changing social needs. . While Halliday's functional theory provides valuable insights into the mechanisms behind language change, some elements of the theory do not fully explain language change. ○ Underestimation of the role of individual agency: Halliday's theory puts a strong emphasis on the social functions of language and the needs of the community, but it does not fully account for the role of individual agency in driving language change. While cultural and social factors are undoubtedly important, individuals can also initiate language change through their own linguistic innovations. ○ Lack of attention to variation and dialects: Halliday's theory focuses on the standard language and does not fully take into account the role of variation and dialects in language change. Variations and dialects have their own rules and conventions, and they can influence language change in different ways. ○ Limited scope for syntactic change: While Halliday's theory recognizes that syntax plays an important role in language, it does not fully account for the complex processes involved in syntactic change. Syntax is an essential aspect of language, and syntactic changes can significantly impact language evolution. ○ ○ Neglect of the influence of other languages: Halliday's theory places a strong emphasis on the internal mechanisms of language change and does not fully account for the influence of other languages on language change. Contact between languages can lead to the adoption of new words, grammatical structures, and pronunciation patterns, which can significantly impact a language’s evolution. Insufficient explanation of sound change: Halliday's theory does not provide a comprehensive explanation for the complex processes involved in sound change. Sound change is an essential aspect of language evolution, and it can occur through a variety of mechanisms, including assimilation, dissimilation, and metathesis. Random Fluctuation Theory (Charles Hockett) . Random Fluctuation Theory relates to spoken language and suggests that change occurs when people pronounce words in a particular style which spreads and becomes seen as the desirable form of discourse. The theory suggests that language change is not necessarily driven by any functional need or cultural transmission, but instead arises due to random fluctuations in the usage of language. In this view, linguistic change is seen as a by-product of the inherently unpredictable nature of communication between individuals. . Reasons for language change: Hockett's theory argues that language change can occur randomly, without any specific cause or purpose, and that it is not always driven by communicative needs or cultural factors. Instead, random fluctuations in language use can sometimes lead to the emergence of new linguistic structures, and these structures can then become part of the language over time. . There is some evidence to support the Random Fluctuation Theory: ○ The observation that language change can occur rapidly and seemingly without reason. For example, the use of "like" as a discourse marker in English (e.g. "I was like, 'no way!'") has increased dramatically in frequency over the past few decades, even though it serves no clear grammatical or semantic purpose. ○ Another piece of evidence comes from studies of Creole languages. Creoles are typically formed when speakers of different languages are forced to communicate with one another, such as in a colonial context. Creoles often have simplified grammar and a reduced vocabulary compared to the languages that their speakers originally spoke. According to the Random Fluctuation Theory, this simplification occurs because the new language is formed by random processes of recombination and simplification. . Some limitations are: ○ Limited explanatory power: Provides a limited explanation for language change, as it does not account for the complex social, cultural, and cognitive factors that shape linguistic variation. It views language change as a random, stochastic process, overlooking the social and cultural contexts that drive linguistic innovations and diffusion. ○ Lack of empirical support: There is little empirical evidence to support the notion that language change occurs primarily through random fluctuations. Most studies of language change suggest that it is driven by a complex interplay of social, cultural, and cognitive factors, including language contact, prestige, social network dynamics, and cognitive processing. ○ Difficulty in accounting for linguistic regularities: The Random Fluctuation ○ ○ theory struggles to explain linguistic regularities, such as the systematic sound changes observed in historical linguistics. While it can account for random noise and variation, it fails to explain the patterns and trends that are characteristic of language change over time. Limited scope: The Random Fluctuation theory focuses primarily on phonological change and does not account for other dimensions of language change, such as syntactic or lexical change. It also does not address the role of language acquisition and learning in driving linguistic change. Cultural Transmission Theory (Clark, Hartl) . Hartl and Clark's Cultural Transmission Theory builds upon Bandura's Social Learning Theory and suggests that language change is primarily driven by the cultural transmission of language norms and practices within a speech community. According to this theory, language change occurs when an individual perceives a benefit in adopting a linguistic innovation or variation. . Reasons for language change: ○ Prestige: speakers may adopt linguistic innovations if they are associated with higher status, wealth, or social influence. For example, in some communities, the use of certain accents or dialects may be associated with higher education or professional success. As a result, younger speakers may adopt these linguistic features to enhance their own perceived prestige. ○ Accommodation: speakers may modify their language use to match that of their conversational partner, particularly in situations where they perceive a need to establish social rapport or demonstrate respect. For example, a speaker may adjust their accent or vocabulary to match a new acquaintance’s or avoid stigmatization in a new social setting. . There is some evidence to support the Random Fluctuation Theory: Hartl and Clark's theory is supported by a range of empirical studies on language change, including sociolinguistic studies of dialect variation and studies of language change in historical contexts. For example, studies have shown that linguistic innovations often originate in social networks with high social interaction and cohesion levels, suggesting that social factors play a key role in driving language change (Eckert, 2012). . Some limitations are: ○ Lack of reach: The theory does not fully account for language change in isolated or small communities with limited exposure to other languages. ○ Community transmission: It does not explain why some changes are more likely to be transmitted than others, and why some changes may occur in one community but not in another. ○ Individual agency: The theory does not account for individual agency in language change, as it primarily focuses on larger social and cultural factors. ○ Linguistic constraint: The theory does not fully address the role of linguistic constraints and the influence of linguistic structure on language change. ○ Adoptive advantage: The theory also does not account for language change in situations where there is no clear benefit or adaptive advantage to the change. Substratum Theory . Substratum Theory is a linguistic theory that explains language change through the . influence of pre-existing languages on a new dominant language. According to this theory, when speakers of different languages come into contact with each other, they will interact and influence each other's languages. In situations where one language is dominant, the other languages may have an effect on it, leading to changes in the dominant language. . Substratum Theory suggests that the substrate languages can leave an imprint on the dominant language, resulting in changes such as phonological shifts, borrowing of words, and grammatical constructions. For example, when the Romans conquered England in the first century AD, the Latin language influenced the English language. This is evidenced by the large number of Latin loanwords in English, such as "butter" (from Latin "butyrum") and "calendar" (from Latin "calendarium"). . There is some evidence to support the Substratum Theory from historical and sociolinguistic studies. For example, the influence of the Celtic language on the English language has been observed in Old English texts, which show a number of grammatical constructions and loanwords from Celtic languages. In addition, studies of creole languages, which are formed when speakers of different languages come into contact with each other, have also provided evidence for Substratum Theory. For example, Haitian Creole, which is spoken in Haiti, is based on French but also has influences from West African and other languages. ● Some limitations are: ○ Lack of direct evidence: One limitation of the Substratum Theory is the lack of direct evidence that substrate languages have influenced the development of dominant languages. While there may be similarities between the languages, it can be difficult to determine whether these are due to direct influence or coincidental similarities. ○ Difficulty in determining causation: It can also be difficult to determine the extent to which a substrate language has influenced the dominant language, as there may be other factors at play. For example, trade or cultural contact may have led to the borrowing of words or grammatical structures, rather than direct influence from a substrate language. ○ Not applicable to all language change: This may not be applicable to all instances of language change. For example, it may not explain the development of new words or the evolution of pronunciation within a single language community. ○ Ignores internal factors: The theory places a heavy emphasis on external influences, such as contact between different language communities, and may therefore neglect internal factors that also contribute to language change. For example, changes in social attitudes or cultural practices within a single community may lead to changes in language use and structure. Theory of Lexical Gaps . The Theory of Lexical Gaps is a linguistic theory that proposes that language change occurs as a result of the need to fill gaps in the lexicon. The theory suggests that speakers of a language may encounter new concepts or experiences that do not have corresponding words in their language, and thus must create new words to fill these lexical gaps. This process of creating new words to fill gaps in the lexicon is known as lexical innovation. . One reason why the Theory of Lexical Gaps is proposed as a cause of language change is that language is constantly adapting to reflect changes in society and culture. As new concepts and experiences emerge, speakers need to be able to communicate about them, which can lead to the creation of new words or the adaptation of existing ones. This process of creating and adapting words to reflect changes in society and culture is known as lexicalization. . Evidence in support of the Theory of Lexical Gaps comes from the fact that new words are constantly being created and added to languages. For example, the rise of technology has led to the creation of new words like "emoji," "selfie," and "hashtag," which did not exist in the lexicon just a few decades ago. Similarly, changes in social attitudes have led to the creation of new words like "genderfluid," "nonbinary," and "transgender," which reflect new concepts and experiences that were not widely understood or discussed in the past. . Some limitations are: ○ Inability to explain why some words are more likely to catch on: Some lexical innovations become part of the standard lexicon, while others fall out of use or are only used in certain contexts. ○ Does not fully account for the role of language contact in language change: When speakers of different languages come into contact, they may borrow words or structures from one another, which can lead to changes in both languages. ○ Why some languages change more rapidly than others: Some languages may be more open to lexical innovation and adaptation, while others may be more resistant to change. Factors like language prestige and standardization may play a role in this. The S-Curve Model . The S-Curve model is a theory that describes the way language changes over time. It is based on the idea that language change follows an S-shaped curve, with an initial period of slow change, followed by a rapid period of change, and then a levelling off as the change becomes the norm. This model is based on Chen who asserted that users would pick up a language change at a certain rate before spreading into wider language usage and then slowing. . The S-Curve model gives a number of reasons for language change, including: ○ Social factors: Changes in language are often driven by changes in society, such as shifts in demographics or changes in social norms. For example, the rise of feminism in the 20th century led to changes in language use, such as the adoption of gender-neutral terms. ○ Contact with other languages: When two or more languages come into contact, they can influence each other and lead to changes in the languages involved. For example, English has borrowed many words from other languages over the centuries, such as "sushi" from Japanese and "croissant" from French. ○ Language standardization: As a language becomes more standardized, there may be pressure to conform to certain rules or norms, which can lead to changes in the language over time. For example, the rise of the printing press in the 15th century led to increased standardization of written languages, which in turn led to changes in pronunciation and grammar. . There is evidence to support the S-Curve model from studies of language change over time. For example, a study by Labov in the 1960s found that the use of the "r" sound in certain words in New York City English was spreading rapidly through the population, following the pattern of an S-shaped curve. Other studies have found similar patterns of change in other languages. . Some limitations are: ○ Does not account for all types of language change: The S-curve model is most useful for explaining changes in the use of specific words or phrases. It may not be as useful for understanding broader linguistic changes, such as shifts in grammar or pronunciation. ○ Does not explain why certain innovations are adopted: The S-curve model can help explain the spread of linguistic innovations, but it does not provide an explanation for why certain innovations are adopted in the first place. Other theories, such as Cultural Transmission Theory, may be more useful for understanding the motivations behind language change. ○ Assumes a uniform rate of adoption: The S-curve model assumes that the rate of adoption of a linguistic innovation is uniform across all members of a speech community. In reality, the rate of adoption may vary depending on factors such as social status or geographic location. Part 4: Language change – progress or decay? Introduction: Language is a dynamic and ever-changing system that evolves over time. While some linguists view language change as progress, others see it as decay. Jean Aitcheson's book, Language Change: Progress or Decay? (1981), taps into this linguistic debate and explores the two opposing viewpoints: the prescriptive approach and the descriptive approach. This study guide aims to provide an in-depth analysis of the two viewpoints and their arguments. Section 1: The Prescriptive Approach 1.1 Definition: The prescriptive approach is a viewpoint that advocates for a 'gold standard' of the English Language that should be preserved. 1.2 Key features: ● The focus is on maintaining a fixed standard of language use. ● There are rules for grammar, vocabulary, and pronunciation that should be followed. ● Deviations from the standard are considered incorrect or improper. ● The goal is to preserve the language and prevent it from changing over time. 1.3 Arguments: ● The prescriptive approach advocates for clear and precise communication, which is essential for effective communication. ● A standard language is necessary for social cohesion, and deviations from the standard can lead to misunderstandings and communication breakdowns. ● A fixed language standard is essential for educational purposes, such as teaching grammar and syntax. 1.4 Criticisms: ● ● ● The prescriptive approach is often seen as elitist, as it values the language of the upper classes over that of the lower classes. It can be seen as an attempt to impose a single, dominant language on diverse communities, which can be oppressive. The prescriptive approach is often based on arbitrary rules and conventions that do not reflect the way language is actually used. Section 2: The Descriptive Approach 2.1 Definition: The descriptive approach is a viewpoint that advocates for the evolution of language as the product of its users, with no fixed standard. 2.2 Key features: ● The focus is on describing how language is used rather than prescribing how it should be used. ● There is no fixed standard, and language use evolves over time. ● All varieties of language are considered equal and valid, including non-standard and regional dialects. ● The goal is to document language use and understand how it evolves over time. 2.3 Arguments: ● The descriptive approach acknowledges that language is constantly changing and evolving, and that this change is necessary for communication to remain effective. ● All varieties of language are considered equal and valid, and there is no hierarchy of language varieties. ● The descriptive approach values linguistic diversity and the richness of different dialects and varieties of language. 2.4 Criticisms: ● The descriptive approach can be seen as promoting linguistic relativism, in which all language varieties are considered equal, regardless of their quality or effectiveness. ● It can lead to a lack of clarity and precision in communication, as there are no fixed rules for language use. ● It can be challenging to teach language effectively when there is no fixed standard to follow. Conclusion: In conclusion, the prescriptive approach advocates for a fixed standard of language use, while the descriptive approach acknowledges the evolution and diversity of language over time. Both viewpoints have their strengths and weaknesses, and there is no clear winner in the debate. The study of language change is essential for understanding how language evolves and how it can be used effectively in communication. Language Change and Standards of English 1.1 Introduction ● Language is constantly changing and we encounter different varieties of spoken and written English in our daily lives. ● Some people believe that language change is a decline in the standards of English, while others see it as a natural process that reflects the ways in which people use language to communicate and express themselves. 1.2 Prescriptivism vs Descriptivism Prescriptivist view: ○ The prescriptivist view of language emphasizes the importance of following rules and adhering to standard grammar and vocabulary. ○ Prescriptivists believe that there is a "correct" way to use language and that deviations from this standard are incorrect or "bad" English. ○ This view tends to prioritize the language of the elite or powerful, often at the expense of regional or minority dialects. ○ Prescriptivist attitudes can lead to resistance to language change and the use of language that deviates from standard grammar and vocabulary. Descriptivist view: ○ The descriptivist view of language recognizes that language is constantly changing and evolving. ○ Descriptivists believe that all varieties of language are valid and that there is no inherently "correct" way to use language. ○ This approach prioritizes clear communication and mutual understanding over adherence to a fixed standard. ○ Descriptivist attitudes can lead to greater acceptance of language change and the use of non-standard forms of language. Impact on language change: ○ Prescriptivist attitudes can lead to resistance to language change and the use of nonstandard forms of language. ○ This can create a divide between speakers of different dialects or varieties of English. ○ Descriptivist attitudes can lead to greater acceptance of language change and the use of non-standard forms of language. ○ This can create a more inclusive and diverse linguistic community. ○ Both prescriptivist and descriptivist views play a role in language change and evolution. The tension between these two views can lead to debates and discussions about language use and norms. 1.3 Language is an ever-changing phenomenon. To study its changes, a systematic method of storage and retrieval of the language is essential. This is because: ○ Language changes occur gradually over time: Language changes can take place over a long period of time, and it can be difficult to identify them without a systematic record of the language. ○ Multiple forms of a language exist: Languages are not uniform, and different varieties and dialects exist. Without a systematic method of recording and retrieving these different forms of a language, it would be difficult to study how they evolve over time. ○ Accurate documentation is necessary: For accurate analysis of language change, it is important to have accurate and reliable documentation of the language. This documentation must be comprehensive and detailed enough to capture the nuances of the language. ○ Helps with identifying patterns: Having a systematic record of a language can help researchers identify patterns and trends in language change. This can be useful in predicting future changes and understanding the factors that drive language change. ○ Allows for comparison: A systematic record of language enables researchers to compare different versions of a language across time and space. This can help identify similarities and differences between dialects and can lead to a better understanding of language change. Part 5: Child Language Acquisition I. Features of Child Language Acquisition ○ Virtuous errors: errors made by children that demonstrate a grasp of language rules and structures, even if they are not yet fully developed. ○ Overextension/underextension: when a child uses a word to refer to a broader or narrower category than adults would. ○ Overgeneralization: when a child applies a grammatical rule too broadly, such as adding "-ed" to irregular verbs (e.g. "runned" instead of "ran"). II. Features of Child-Directed Speech (CDS) ○ High degree of repetition: caregivers often repeat words or phrases to aid in the child's language acquisition. ○ Longer and more frequent pauses: caregivers use pauses to allow the child time to process language and formulate a response. ○ Frequent use of the child's name: using the child's name in speech helps to engage their attention and foster language development. III. Theories of Child Language Acquisition ○ Nativism: the idea that humans have an innate ability to learn language and that the brain is pre-wired for language acquisition. ○ Behaviourism: the theory that language is acquired through reinforcement and conditioning, with children learning through imitation and repetition. ○ Social Interactionism: the idea that language acquisition is the result of social interaction and communication, with children learning through conversation and joint attention. Stages of first language acquisition in Children There are four main stages of first language acquisition in children. These are: . The babbling stage (3-8 months): Children first start to recognise and produce sounds eg 'bababa'. They don't yet produce any recognisable words but they are experimenting with their newfound voice! . The one-word stage (9-18 months): The one-word stage is when babies start to say their first recognisable words, eg using the word 'dog' to describe all fluffy animals. . The two-word stage (18-24 months): The two-word stage is when children start communicating using two-word phrases. For example, 'dog woof', meaning 'the dog is barking', or 'mummy home', meaning mummy is home. . The multi-word stage (telegraphic stage) (24-30 months): The multi-word stage is when children start to use longer sentences, more complex sentences. For example, ‘Mummy and Chloe go school now'. The Babbling Stage The babbling stage is the first significant stage of language acquisition in children, occurring from around 4-6 months until about 12 months of age. During this stage, the child hears speech syllables (sounds that make up spoken language) from its environment and caregivers and attempts to imitate by repeating them. There are two types of babbling: canonical babbling and variegated babbling. ● Canonical babbling is the type of babbling that emerges first. It consists of the same syllables being repeated over and over e.g. a baby saying 'ga ga ga', 'ba ba ba', or a similar string of repeated syllables. ● Variegated babbling is when different syllables are used in the babbling sequence. Instead of using one syllable repeatedly, the child uses a variety e.g. 'ga ba da' or 'ma da pa'. This occurs around two months after canonical babbling starts, at around eight months of age. Children may also begin to use intonation that resembles actual speech at this stage, while still only producing meaningless sounds. The Holophrastic (One-word) Stage The holophrastic stage of language acquisition, also known as the ‘one-word stage’, typically occurs around the age of 12 to 18 months. ● At this stage, children have identified which words and combinations of syllables are the most effective for communicating and may attempt to communicate a full sentence's worth of information. ● For example, a child may say 'dada' which could mean anything from ‘I want dad’ to ‘Where is dad?’. This is known as holophrasis. A child's first word will often resemble a babble and, while they may hear and understand a wide range of sounds, they can still only produce a limited range themselves. These words are known as proto words. Despite sounding like babbles, they still work as words because the child has assigned meaning to them. Children may also use real words and typically adapt them to suit their speaking ability. Sometimes these words are used incorrectly as the child attempts to learn and use them. For example, they may call every animal a 'cat' if they grew up with one. The Two-word Stage The two-word stage occurs at around 18 months of age. At this stage, children are able to use two words in the right grammatical order. However, the words they use tend to exclusively be content words (words that hold and convey meaning) and they often leave out function words (words that hold a sentence together, such as articles, prepositions, etc.) ● At this stage, the child’s vocabulary starts at around 50 words and consists mostly of common nouns and verbs. These often come from things their caregivers have said or things in their immediate environment. ● Typically, as the child progresses through the two-word stage, the ‘word spurt’ occurs, which is a relatively short period during which the child's vocabulary grows much larger. Most children know 50 words by around 17 months of age, but by 24 months they may know up to over 600. For example, a child might see a dog jump over the fence and simply say ‘Dog jump’ instead of ‘A dog jumped over the fence.’ The order is correct and they say the most important word, but the lack of functional words and tense use makes the information very context-dependent, much like in the holophrastic stage. The Multi-word Stage The multi-word stage of language acquisition in children can be broken up into two distinct sub-stages: the early multi-word stage and the later multi-word stage. Children move on from two-word phrases and begin to form short sentences of around three, four, and five words, and eventually even more. They also begin to use more and more function words and are able to form more complex sentences. Children typically progress rapidly through this stage as they understand many of their language basics already. The early multi-word stage ● The early part of this stage is sometimes called the ‘telegraphic stage’ as the children's sentences seem to resemble telegram messages due to their simplicity. The telegraphic stage takes place from around 24 to 30 months of age. ● Children mostly ignore function words in favour of using the most important content words and usually start using negatives (no, not, can't, etc.). They also tend to ask more questions about their surroundings. ● For example, a child might say ‘no want veggies’ instead of ‘I don't want vegetables with my food.’ While children at this substage still don't use function words in their own sentences, many do understand when others use them. The later multi-word stage ● The later multi-word stage, also known as the complex stage, is the final part of language acquisition. It starts at around 30 months of age and has no fixed endpoint. ● At this stage, children start to use a variety of function words and there is a great increase in the amount of words children can use. Their sentence structures also become a lot more complex and varied. ● Children in this stage have a concrete sense of time, quantity, and the ability to engage in simple reasoning. This means they can talk confidently in different tenses, and verbally explain ideas such as putting ‘some’ or ‘all’ of their toys away. They can also start to explain why and how they think or feel things, and may also ask others. As children reach the age of five and above, their ability to use and understand language becomes more or less fluent. Many children still struggle with pronunciation, but they are able to understand when others use these sounds. Eventually, older children gain the ability to confidently read, write, and explore a variety of new topics and ideas. Typically, school will also help children to further develop their linguistic skills. Part 6: Theories of language acquisition What is Cognitive Theory? Cognitive theory suggests that children go through stages of language development. Theorist Jean Piaget emphasised that we can only move through the stages of language learning as our brains and cognitive processes develop. In other words, children have to understand certain concepts before producing the language to describe them. Theorist Eric Lenneberg argued that there is a critical period between two years old and puberty in which children need to learn language, otherwise, it cannot be learned sufficiently well. What is Behavioural Theory (Imitation Theory)? Behavioural theory, often called 'Imitation Theory', suggests that people are a product of their environment. Theorist BF Skinner proposed that children 'imitate' their caregivers and modify their language use through a process called 'operant conditioning'. This is where children are either rewarded for desired behaviour (correct language) or punished for undesired behaviour (mistakes). What is Nativist Theory and Language Acquisition Device? Nativist theory, sometimes referred to as the 'innateness theory', was first proposed by Noam Chomsky. It states that children are born with an innate ability to learn language and that they already have a "language acquisition device" (LAD) in their brain (this is a theoretical device; it doesn't really exist!). He argued that certain errors (eg 'I runned') are evidence that children actively 'construct' language rather than just imitating caregivers. What is Interactionist Theory? Interactionist theory emphasises the importance of caregivers in child language acquisition. Theorist Jerome Bruner argued that children do have an innate ability to learn language however they require lots of regular interaction with caregivers to achieve full fluency. This linguistic support from caregivers is often called 'scaffolding' or a Language Acquisition Support System (LASS). Caregivers may also use child-directed speech (CDS) that helps a child learn. For example, caregivers will often use a higher pitch, simplified words, and lots of repetitive questioning when talking to a child. These aids are said to enhance communication between the child and caregiver. What are Halliday's functions of language? Michael Halliday suggested seven stages that show how the functions of a child's language become more complex with age. In other words, children express themselves better and better as time goes by. These stages include: ● ● ● ● ● ● ● Stage 1- Instrumental Stage (language for basic needs eg food) Stage 2- Regulatory Stage (language to influence others eg commands) Stage 3- Interactive Stage (language to form relationships eg 'love you') Stage 4- Personal Stage (language to express feelings or opinions eg 'me sad') Stage 5- Informative Stage (language to communicate information) Stage 6- Heuristic Stage (language to learn and explore eg questions) Stage 7- Imaginative Stage (language used to imagine things) How do we apply these theories? Babies and young children say all kinds of funny things such; 'I runned to school' and 'I swimmed really fast'. These may sound ridiculous to us but these errors suggest that children are learning common English grammar rules. Take the examples' I danced ',' I walked ', and' I learned'- why do these make sense but not 'I runned '? Theorists who believe that language is innate, such as nativists and interactionists, argue that these errors are virtuous errors. They believe that children build up a set of internal grammar rules and apply them to their own language; for example 'the suffix -ed means past tense'. If there is an error, children will modify their internal rules, learning that 'ran' is correct instead. Cognitive theorists may argue that the child has not reached the level of cognition required to understand the use of irregular verbs. However, as adults don't say 'runned' we cannot apply the behaviourist theory, which suggests that children imitate carers. Part 7: The seven functions of children’s language as discussed by Halliday . The instrumental function of language The instrumental function of language refers to when language is used to fulfil a need, such as requiring food, drink, or comfort ○ For example, the phrases 'I want', 'Can I have', and 'I need' are all examples of instrumental language. ○ When a child is thirsty they might say something like 'I want bottle'. If the carer gives them the bottle then their needs have been met through their use of language. . The regulatory function of language When a speaker commands, persuades or requests something from someone else, this is known as a regulatory language function. Regulatory language controls the listener's behaviour (the speaker adopts a commanding tone). Examples include: ○ 'Let's go home now.' ○ 'You need to finish that work by tomorrow so get on with it.' ○ 'Can you give me the report from yesterday?' As evident in these examples of Halliday's functions, the listener has restricted agency as the speaker has taken the dominant position in the conversation. . Interactive This example of Halliday's function is how we form relationships with others as it encompasses the communicative use of language. It is how we relay our thoughts and emotions, strengthening bonds with those around us. Interactional language examples include phrases like 'I love you mum' or 'Thank you so much', revealing the emotions and opinions of the speaker. . Personal functions This function defines how we refer to ourselves and express our personal opinions, our identity, and our feelings. A child may communicate their opinions and emotions in a simplistic way, using phrases like 'me good' or 'me happy'. As well as expressing personal opinions and emotions, the personal function of language also encompasses how we use language to learn more about our surroundings, by requesting information about it. This is known as the 'personal function of language', as we gain information which benefits ourselves and our understanding of society. Questions like 'what's that?' or 'what does that mean?' are examples of personal language function. The next three language functions that are part of Halliday's functions of language describe how children adapt to their environment through learning language . Heuristic This term refers to language associated with discovery and explanation, usually in the form of questions or a running commentary (when the child talks about what they are doing as they are doing it). For example, a lot of children talk to themselves when they are younger (some people still do this as adults!) to explain what they are doing, to themselves. This helps them understand their actions in relation to the world around them. Children tend to do this when they are playing. ○ They say things like 'The horsey goes over to the dinosaur and says hello, but he doesn't say hello back because he isn't being friendly. The wizard felt bad for the horsey and came and said hello to her. Now they are best friends'. Another example of children narrating is persistent questioning. ○ It is not uncommon for children to continuously ask 'What's that?', 'What does that do?', or 'Why?' in response to an adult giving them an instruction. It also occurs when adults are talking about a topic children don't understand. . Representational/informative Similar to heuristic and personal functions, representational language occurs when we request information. However, it differs from 'heuristic' and 'personal' functions as it also refers to when we relay information. In other words, it describes the exchange of information between two or more people. ○ For example, questions like 'what's that?' and 'what does that do?' are representational; if this question is answered it leads to an exchange of information. ○ Another example of representational language is when people relay information. Imagine someone telling a story, such as, 'I was walking down the street and a cat jumped out in front of me. It really made me jump! 'This is an example of representational language because it relates information about events. . Imaginative When children tell stories and create imaginary friends or concepts in their heads, it is an imaginative way of using language. Imaginative language usually occurs in leisure or play scenarios. ○ Remember when you were a child playing in the playground? Did you and your friends imagine you were somewhere else, such as in a house or a spaceship? Children do this to make games more interesting. Children adopt suitable characters to act out with their friends. For example, if you pretend you are in space you remove yourself from the playground environment and into a fantasy world - much more exciting! Halliday's theory of language Based on his own child, Halliday argued that children communicate and learn a language before they can speak. There are a few things that Halliday believed impact how a child learns its first language: ● ● ● ● ● Halliday suggests social interaction is key for child language learning as it is vital for them to see how language functions in society. That way they can learn how to be members of society themselves. Halliday views language as not only a mode of communication but a cultural code we need to understand to fit in with our surroundings: 'Language is the main channel through which the patterns of living are transmitted to him, through which he learns to act as a member of a society... [and] to adopt its' culture'. This quote reveals Halliday's view that language is the key to learning how to become a member of society rather than just a method of communicating. Rather than the language learning process starting when children start to speak, Halliday argued that as soon as children can cry or make facial expressions language is present. This is because children can communicate their feelings through facial expressions and actions. Children learn to do things to get a reaction, meaning they can communicate their emotions and use language to get what they want before they can talk. Children often throw small objects or start to cry to get a reaction from their caregivers. Halliday believes we are always making choices in our language and communication. Nature vs Nurture in Language Acquisition The debate between nature vs nurture in language acquisition is an ongoing one, with evidence supporting both sides. It is generally agreed that both genetic and environmental factors play a role in language acquisition. Nature: ● The idea that language acquisition is innate and predetermined by genetic factors. ● The theory suggests that the human brain has an inherent capacity to learn language. ● The famous linguist Noam Chomsky proposed the theory of Universal Grammar, which suggests that all human languages share a common structure. ● According to this theory, children are born with an innate understanding of this structure and can use it to acquire any language they are exposed to. Nurture: ● The idea that language acquisition is a result of environmental factors, such as social interaction, exposure to language, and learning from caregivers. ● This theory suggests that children learn language through imitation, reinforcement, and shaping. ● Behaviorist B.F. Skinner proposed the theory of Operant Conditioning, which suggests that language is learned through a process of reward and punishment. ● The Social Interactionist theory suggests that children learn language through their social interactions with others and the feedback they receive from them.