Research Methods in Business Studies This accessible guide provides clear and practical explanations of key research methods in business studies, presenting a step-by-step approach to data collection, analysis, and problem solving. Readers will learn how to formulate a research question or problem, choose an appropriate research fi method, argue and motivate, collect and analyse data and present ndings in a logical and convincing manner. The authors evaluate various qualitative and quantitative methods and the consequences of their use, guiding readers to a deep understanding of the most appropriate research design for particular questions. Furthermore, the authors provide instructions on how to write reports and dissertations in a clearly structured and concise style. Now in its fi fth edition, this popular textbook includes new and dedicated chapters on data collection for qualitative research, qualitative data analysis, data collection for quantitative research, multiple regression, and additional methods of quantitative analysis. In addition, cases and examples have been updated throughout, increasing the applicability of these research methods across various situations. Pervez Ghauri is Professor of International Business at the University of Birmingham. Pervez is the Editor-in-Chief of the International Business Review and Consulting Editor for the Journal of International Business Studies. Pervez has published more than 30 books and more than a hundred articles in top level journals. Kjell Grønhaug is Professor Emeritus, Norwegian School of Economics. He is Honorary Professor at the Turku School of Economics and Business Adm., Gothenburg School of Business Adm. and Stockholm School of Economics. His publications include 18 authored and co-authored books and some 200 articles in leading European and American journals. Roger Strange is Professor of International Business at the University of Sussex Business School. He is an Associate Editor of the International fi Business Review, a Senior Editor of the Asia Paci c Journal of Management, and on the Editorial Boards of the Journal of World Business, the Global fi Strategy Journal, and the Asia Paci c Business Review. His publications include 12 authored or edited books, and numerous book chapters and articles in leading international journals. Research Methods in Business Studies Pervez Ghauri Kjell Grønhaug University of Birmingham Norwegian School of Economics and Business Administration, Bergen-Sandviken Roger Strange University of Sussex University Printing House, Cambridge CB2 8BS, United Kingdom One Liberty Plaza, 20th Floor, New York, NY 10006, USA 477 Williamstown Road, Port Melbourne, VIC 3207, Australia 314–321, 3rd Floor, Plot 3, Splendor Forum, Jasola District Centre, New Delhi – 110025, India 79 Anson Road, #06-04/06, Singapore 079906 Cambridge University Press is part of the University of Cambridge. It furthers the University’s mission by disseminating knowledge in the pursuit of education, learning, and research at the highest international levels of excellence. www.cambridge.org Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9781108486743 DOI: 10.1017/9781108762427 © Pervez Ghauri, Kjell Grønhaug, and Roger Strange 2020 This publication is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press. First published 2020 Printed in Singapore by Markono Print Media Pte Ltd A catalogue record for this publication is available from the British Library. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Names: Ghauri, Pervez N., 1948– author. | Grønhaug, Kjell, author. | Strange, Roger, author. Title: Research methods in business studies / Pervez Ghauri, University of Birmingham, Kjell Grønhaug, Norwegian School of Economics and Business Administration, Bergen-Sandviken, Roger Strange, University of Sussex. Description: Fifth Edition. | New York : Cambridge University Press, 2020. | Revised edition of the authors’ Research methods in business studies, 2010. Identifiers: LCCN 2019037291 (print) | LCCN 2019037292 (ebook) | ISBN 9781108486743 (hardback) | ISBN 9781108708241 (paperback) | ISBN 9781108762427 (epub) Subjects: LCSH: Management–Research. | Business–Research. Classification: LCC HD30.4 .G49 2020 (print) | LCC HD30.4 (ebook) | DDC 658–dc23 LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2019037291 LC ebook record available athttps://lccn.loc.gov/2019037292 ISBN 978-1-108-48674-3 Hardback ISBN 978-1-108-70824-1 Paperback Cambridge University Press has no responsibility for the persistence or accuracy of URLs for external or third-party internet websites referred to in this publication and does not guarantee that any content on such websites is, or will remain, accurate or appropriate. Contents List of Figures List of Tables Preface Acknowledgements Part I Challenges and Ambiguities of Business Research 1 Introduction page xii xiv xv xviii 1 3 1.1 Purpose of the Book 3 1.2 Readership 5 1.3 On the Use of the Book 7 2 Research in Business Studies 8 2.1 The Nature of Knowledge Development 9 2.2 Why We Do Research 10 2.3 Research versus Common Sense 12 2.4 Different Research Orientations 16 2.5 Induction, Deduction, and Abduction 18 2.6 What Comes First: Theory or Data? 21 2.7 Knowledge Skills and Research 22 2.8 Why Research Never Stops 23 2.9 Research and Ethics 23 2.10 The Researcher and the Respondent 25 2.11 The Researcher’s Moral Responsibility 27 Further Reading 30 Questions 30 Exercises 30 Part II The Research Process 31 3 The Research Process 33 3.1 The Process Perspective 33 3.2 Levels of Research 36 3.3 Research and Knowledge 37 vi Contents 3.4 What Comes First: Theory or Research? 38 3.5 Some Important Issues 39 3.5.1 Concepts 39 3.5.2 Definitions 39 3.5.3 Theory 41 3.5.4 Methods 42 3.6 Concluding Remarks 43 Further Reading 44 Questions 44 Exercises 44 4 Research Problems 45 4.1 Research Ideas 45 4.2 Wrestling with Research Problems 47 4.3 Research Problem and Research Purpose 50 4.4 Models in Research 52 4.4.1 General Concepts and Specific Use 4.4.2 Model Purposes 53 54 4.5 The Role of Reviewing Past Literature 57 Further Reading 60 Questions 60 Exercises 60 5 Research Design 61 5.1 The Design Problem 61 5.2 Problem Structure and Research Design 62 5.2.1 Exploratory Research 63 5.2.2 Descriptive Research 64 5.2.3 Causal Research 64 5.3 The Classic Experiment 66 5.4 Validity Threats 67 5.5 Other Research Designs 71 5.5.1 Cross-Section Data 71 5.5.2 Time-Series Data 72 5.5.3 Longitudinal Studies 73 5.5.4 Panel Data 73 5.6 Requirements in Research Design 5.6.1 Research and Choices 74 76 Further Reading 77 Questions 77 Exercises 77 Contents 6 Measurements 78 6.1 The Operationalization of Variables 78 6.2 Scales of Measurement 81 6.2.1 Nominal Level (Scale) 81 6.2.2 Ordinal Level (Scale) 82 6.2.3 Interval Level (Scale) 82 6.2.4 Ratio Scale 83 6.3 Construct Measurement Using Multi-Item Scales 83 6.4 The Reliability and Validity of Measurements 85 6.5 Improving Your Measurements 87 6.6 Measurements in Qualitative Research 88 Further Reading 91 Questions 91 Exercises 91 Part III Implementation 93 7 Data Collection for Qualitative Research 7.1 Qualitative versus Quantitative Methods 95 96 7.2 Different Qualitative Methods and When to Use Them 98 7.2.1 Historical Review 99 7.2.2 Grounded Theory 100 7.2.3 Case Studies 101 What Is a Case Study? 101 When to Use a Case Study 103 Preparing for a Case Study 104 How to Select the Cases 106 Sampling in Qualitative Research 108 Theoretical Sampling 110 Different Types of Case Study Design 110 7.3 Observations 111 7.4 Communication 114 7.5 Interviews 115 7.5.1 Preparing for an Interview 117 7.5.2 Pre-Interview 120 7.5.3 The Interview 121 7.5.4 Post-Interview 123 7.6 Focus Groups 7.6.1 7.6.2 124 Advantages and Disadvantages of Focus Groups 126 Conducting Focus Groups 126 vii viii Contents Further Reading 128 Questions 128 Exercises 128 8 Qualitative Data Analysis 129 8.1 Characteristics of Qualitative Research 129 8.2 Qualitative Data 130 8.3 Analytical Procedures 131 8.3.1 Analytical Activities 8.3.2 Data Reduction 134 139 8.4 Interpretation 139 8.5 Strategies for Qualitative Analysis 140 8.5.1 Departure from Theory 140 8.5.2 Departure from Data (Observations) 142 8.5.3 Analysing Case Studies 142 8.6 Quantifying Qualitative Data 143 8.7 Validity in Qualitative Research 146 8.8 The Use of Computers in Qualitative Research 146 8.8.1 A Word of Warning 147 8.9 Case Studies and Triangulation 147 8.10 Case Studies and Generalization 150 Further Reading 152 Questions 152 Exercises 152 9 Data Collection for Quantitative Research 9.1 Secondary Data 153 154 9.1.1 The Advantages of Secondary Data 158 9.1.2 The Disadvantages of Secondary Data 158 9.2 Primary Data 9.2.1 The Advantages of Primary Data 9.2.2 The Disadvantages of Primary Data 159 160 160 9.3 Why Take Samples? 161 9.4 Probability Sampling 163 9.4.1 Simple Random Sampling 163 9.4.2 Systematic Sampling 164 9.4.3 Strati ed Random Sampling 164 9.4.4 Cluster Sampling 164 9.4.5 Non-Response Bias fi 9.5 Non-Probability Sampling 165 166 Contents 9.6 Sample Size 168 9.7 Survey Research 169 9.7.1 Survey Administration 171 9.7.2 Questionnaire Design 172 9.7.3 Guidelines for Constructing Questionnaires 175 9.7.4 Improving Response Rates 178 9.8 Experimental Studies 179 Further Reading 181 Questions 181 Exercises 181 10 Description and Preliminary Analysis of Quantitative Data 10.1 How to Describe the Data 182 183 10.1.1 Bar Charts, Histograms, and Scatter Plots 183 10.1.2 Measures of Central Tendency 187 10.1.3 Measures of Dispersion 188 10.1.4 Measures of Skewness 190 10.2 How to Analyse the Data 191 10.2.1 Hypothesis Testing 191 10.2.2 Comparison of Means (Independent Samples) 195 10.2.3 Comparison of Means (Paired Samples) 197 10.2.4 Correlation Coef 199 10.2.5 Correlation Coef cient (Ordinal Variables) 204 10.2.6 Correlations between Nominal and/or Ordinal Variables 206 ficient (Continuous Variables) fi 10.3 The Use of Computers in Quantitative Research 206 Further Reading 208 Questions 208 Exercises 208 11 Multiple Regression 210 11.1 The Determinants of Export Performance 211 11.2 The Regression Model 212 11.2.1 Operationalization of the Variables 213 11.2.2 Functional Form 215 The Linear Relationship 215 The Semi-Logarithmic Relationship 216 The Quadratic Relationship 217 Time Lags 219 11.2.3 11.3 Estimation Methodology 11.3.1 OLS Estimation 219 220 ix x Contents 11.3.2 The Coefficient of Determination 221 11.3.3 The Assumptions Underpinning OLS Estimation 223 The Omission of Relevant Explanatory Variables 224 Multicollinearity 226 11.3.4 The Residual Plot 228 11.3.5 Estimation Methodology with Nominal or Ordinal Dependent Variables 11.4 Hypothesis Testing 11.4.1 Testing for the Significance of Individual Regression 230 231 Coefficients 231 Explanatory Variables 232 11.4.2 Testing for the Joint Significance of Several 12 11.4.3 Testing for Common Method Variance 234 11.4.4 Statistical Significance and Effect Sizes 234 11.5 The Determinants of Export Performance Redux 236 11.6 What Constitutes a Good Regression Model? 239 Further Reading 241 Questions 241 Exercises 241 Additional Methods of Quantitative Analysis 243 12.1 Factor Analysis 243 12.1.1 Exploratory Factor Analysis 244 12.1.2 An Example of Factor Analysis: Burnout on Oil Rigs 246 12.2 Structural Equation Modelling 12.2.1 Estimation of Structural Equation Models 247 250 12.2.2 An Example of Structural Equation Modelling: Green Innovation 12.3 Event Studies 252 253 12.3.1 The Estimation Procedure 254 12.3.2 The Applicability of Event Studies 256 12.3.3 An Example of an Event Study: Cross-Border Acquisitions 13 258 12.4 Final Comments 259 Further Reading 261 Questions 261 Exercises 261 Cross-Cultural Research 262 13.1 The International Dimension 262 Contents 13.2 Data Collection in Some Cross-Cultural Research 265 13.3 Collecting Cross-Cultural Data 267 13.4 Analysis of Cross-Cultural Data 270 13.5 Enhancing Trustworthiness of Cross-Cultural Research 273 Further Reading 275 Questions 275 Exercises 275 14 Writing the Final Report 276 14.1 Purpose and Importance of a Written Presentation 276 14.2 Guidelines 277 14.2.1 Audience 277 14.2.2 Good Communication 278 14.2.3 Language and Flow 279 14.2.4 Form and Style 280 14.2.5 Headings 14.3 281 Structure of the Report 281 14.3.1 Title Page 282 14.3.2 Table of Contents 282 14.3.3 Executive Summary 282 14.3.4 Introduction and Problem Statement 283 14.3.5 Theoretical Background 283 14.3.6 Methodology 284 14.3.7 Findings 284 14.3.8 Conclusions 285 14.3.9 Footnotes 286 14.3.10 Bibliography and References 287 14.4 Oral Presentation 288 14.5 Writing for Publication 290 Further Reading 292 Questions 292 Exercises 292 Appendix A: Sample of Manufacturing Firms, 2018 293 Appendix B: Simple Comparative Tests 299 Appendix C: Formulae 300 References 302 Index 319 xi Figures page 22 2.1 Theory or data as starting point 2.2 The wheel of research 24 2.3 Factors in 29 3.1 The research process 34 3.2 From research topic to research problem 35 3.3 Two levels of research 37 3.4 Production and use of theory 38 3.5 Example 41 3.6 Use of methods 42 4.1 Problem, methods, and research practice 50 4.2 Organizational chart (a model) 54 5.1 The classic experiment 69 6.1 Responses and sense-making 89 7.1 Quantitative and qualitative methods and techniques 98 7.2 Number of 7.3 The data structure 102 7.4 A process model for case research 105 7.5 Basic design for case studies 110 7.6 Choices for collecting primary data through observations 112 7.7 A typology of interviews 115 7.8 Steps in conducting a focus group 127 8.1 Components of data analysis: interactive model 132 8.2 Problem recognition – raw 141 8.3 Case study research: theory building and theory testing 151 9.1 Planning a survey 170 9.2 An example of an open question 172 9.3 An example of a category question 173 9.4 An example of a multiple-choice question 174 9.5 An example of a ranking question 174 9.6 An example of a rating question 175 fluencing research firms in the US transistor industry materials (last purchase) 10.2 firm sizes by region Distribution of fi rm sizes by sector 10.3 Symmetric and skewed distributions 10.1 Distribution of 100 184 184 186 Figures 10.4 Scatter plot of export intensity against firm size 187 10.5 Strong and weak correlations 201 11.1a A positive linear relationship 215 11.1b A negative linear relationship 216 11.2a A positive semi-logarithmic relationship 217 11.2b A negative semi-logarithmic relationship 217 11.3a A U-shaped relationship 218 11.3b An inverse U-shaped relationship 218 11.4 The sample regression line 221 numbers 225 11.5 11.6 The relationship between fire damage and fire fighter A residual plot from a correctly specified regression model 11.7 A residual plot showing an incorrectly specified model 11.8 Residual plots indicating heteroskedasticity 11.9 Scatter plot of the residuals from the OLS regression of export 228 229 229 performance 239 12.1 A path diagram for structural equation modelling 249 12.2 PLS-SEM estimation of green innovation 253 12.3 The time line for event studies 255 12.4 Share price reactions to earnings announcements 257 13.1 Managing the international dimension in the research 13.2 process 263 Steps in operationalizing emic and etic approaches 269 13.3 De fining the unit of analysis 13.4 Cross-country data analysis 272 13.5 Ensuring equivalence in cross-cultural research 274 14.1 An example of a table of contents 283 271 xiii Tables page 18 2.1 Different research orientations 2.2 Ethical issues in the researcher–participant relationship 5.1 Cross-table 65 5.2 Reading of advertisement and purchase 71 5.3 A cross-sectional design 72 5.4 A time-series design 72 5.5 A panel design 74 6.1 Example of the operationalization of nominal variables 82 6.2 Example of the operationalization of ordinal variables 83 6.3 Summary of scales of measurement 84 7.1 The difference in emphasis in qualitative versus quantitative methods 26 97 7.2 Strategies for case selection 109 7.3 Different qualitative approaches 112 8.1 An example of a matrix display in case study analysis 133 8.2 Concept list 144 8.3 The advantages of using NVivo in IB research 148 9.1 Selected publicly available secondary data sources 155 9.2 Selected commercially available secondary data sources 157 10.1 10.2 Summary statistics for the sample of 100 manufacturingfirms Summary statistics for two sub-samples of manufacturingfirms 10.3 Decision criterion for the skewness coefficient 10.4 Possible errors in hypothesis testing 188 189 191 193 10.5 Comparison of firm export intensities by sector 10.6 Correlation matrix 199 11.1 OLS estimation of the export performance model 237 12.1 Example of exploratory factor analysis: job-related burnout 248 196 Preface We are very pleased to present the methods. The fifth edition of our book on research first edition of this book, published in 1995, received consider- able recognition and attention, mainly from scholars in the United Kingdom, Scandinavia, and the Netherlands. The second, third, and fourth editions, published in 2002, 2005, and 2010, were used in several American and Asian schools, in addition to many European schools, and have been translated into several languages, such as Chinese, Portuguese, and Estonian. The fourth edition in particular was widely praised for its direct and concrete approach to research methods not only for business studies but also for social sciences in general. Over the past fifteen years, we personally received many positive comments on the accessibility and directness of the text from our colleagues and the students who used it. But at the same time, we also received a lot of feedback on what was missing from the book and valuable input on how it could be improved. About a year ago we approached Cambridge University Press about a possible fifth edition and at that time we started a more systematic collection of comments from colleagues who have been using the book. The publisher also sent the fourth edition to ten anonymous reviewers and asked for their comments as to the strong and weak points of the book and their recommendations as to how it could be improved. We have thus received an abundance of comments on the previous editions, how the book should be improved, and what a fifth edition should include. In this respect, we would like to thank the following colleagues for their valuable advice and comments: Jose-Pla Barber, Peter Buckley, Tamer Cavusgil, Agnieszka Chidlow, Jeremy Clegg, Ulf Elg, Philippe Gugler, Amjad Hadjikhani, Maria Karafyllia, Saba Khalid, Jorma Larimo, Jean-Paul Lemaire, Leigh Anne Liu, Ulrike Mayrhofer, Niin Nummmela, Rebecca Piekkari, Stefan Schmid, Rudolf Sinkovics, Veronika Tarnovskaya, and Sari Wahyuni. Following the comments from the above-mentioned colleagues, we have worked on the fifth edition and have considerably changed and, hopefully, improved it. Not only have we replaced and added three new chapters and about 50 pages of new text, we have also reorganized the whole book. It is now divided into three parts: (I) Challenges and Ambiguities of Business xvi Preface Research, (II) The Research Process, and (III) Implementation. We have made the language and approach more neutral and have provided additional examples in almost all the sections. We have also invited a third co-author, Professor Roger Strange, for his 25+ years of experience in teaching research methods, particularly quantitative methods. This fifth edition has therefore been thoroughly reworked and reorganized and now has clear guidelines for doing qualitative and quantitative research. This approach is considered useful for students as they can go directly to parts that are relevant to them. We have made several additions to the qualitative and quantitative data analysis sections and how researchers can make their research more trustworthy and reliable. Moreover, new sections have been added on ontological and epistemological considerations while doing research. Part III provides concrete guidelines for designing and conducting quantitative and qualitative methods in our field. and explain which research, We first the two most commonly used research discuss these two types of research method type is more appropriate for which type of research question. Then, qualitative and quantitative data collection and analysis are dealt with in separate chapters, instead of covering both in the same chapters, as was done in the fourth edition. Special attention has been given to international and cross-cultural business research, and the sections on ethical issues have been expanded. A number of examples have been added to make the text more accessible and easy to understand. The last chapter gives section-by-section guidelines for report writing depending on the purpose of the report. A number of new features have been introduced. (1) Boxed examples provide illustrations and help make the book readable to students without a considerable background in statistics. (2) At the end of each chapter, a short list of further reading has been provided for those researchers who want to pursue their research using statistical/qualitative methods. These may be particularly useful to teachers and students who want to probe deeper into a particular topic. These additional readings have been selected with great care to present a balanced and up-to-date view (classical as well as new perspectives) on the different issues discussed in a particular chapter. (3) More questions are posed at the end of each chapter. These can be used to test the reader’s knowledge and could also be used as exam questions. (4) There are exercises at the end of each chapter to encourage discussion and debate in the class. We believe the fifth edition is more comprehensive and better structured, but still to the point and focused, and provides a set of guidelines for research methods in business studies. Preface We take this opportunity to thank our families who afford us time and stimulate us in these endeavours. Diletta Pegoraro, Emily Pickering, and Saad Ghauri deserve our special thanks for helping us in preparing the manuscript and for typing and retyping several versions of the book. Finally, we are grateful to Valerie Appleby at Cambridge University Press, for her enthusiasm for this project, her professional help, and for sending us encouraging emails to fi nish the manuscript on time. Pervez N. Ghauri Birmingham Kjell Grønhaug Bergen Roger Strange Brighton xvii Acknowledgements We are grateful to the following for permission to reproduce copyright material. Figures Figure 2.3 adapted from Social Research Methods, Pearson Education, Inc. (Forcese, Dennis P. and Richer, Stephen 1973) p. 22 © 1973, reprinted by permission of Pearson Education, Inc., Upper Saddle River, NJ; Figure 7.1 adapted from Business Research Projects for Students, Thomson Learning (Jankowicz, A.D. 1991) p. 159; Figure 7.2 fromMastering the Dynamics of Innovation , Harvard Business School (Utterback, J.M. 1994) p. 41, based on data in E. Braun and S. MacDonald,Revolution in Miniature: The History and Impact of Semiconductor Electronics (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press 1978); and J.E. Tilton, International Diffusion of Technology (Washington DC: The Brookings Institution 1971), reprinted by permission of Harvard Business School Press, Copyright © 1994 by the Harvard Business School Publishing Corporation; all rights reserved; Figure 7.4 adapted from Case research in marketing: Opportunities, problems and a process, Journal of Business Research, 12, p. 205 (Bonoma, T.V. 1985); Figure 9.1 fromResearch Methods for Managers, Sage Publications Ltd. (Gill, J. and Johnson, P. 2002) pp. 76 77; Figure 13.2 adapted from How to Write a Research Paper, 2nd edn., Pergamon Press (Berry, R. 1989), reprinted with permission of Ralph Berry; Figures 13.3, 13.4 from International Marketing Research, 2nd edn. (Craig, D.S. & Douglas, S.P. 2000) Copyright© John Wiley and Sons Limited, reprinted with permission; Figure 13.5 from Data equivalence in crosscultural research: A comparison of classical test theory and latent trait theory based approaches , Australasian Marketing Journal, 7(2): 23 38, (Salzberger, T., Sinkovics, R., and Schlegelmich, B. 1999). ‘ ’ – ‘ ’ – Acknowledgements Tables Table 8.2 from ‘Can computerised market models improve strategic decision making?’, Journal of Socio-Economics, Grønhaug, K. 2003), Copyright © 32: 503– 20 (Fuglseth, A.M. and 2003, with permission from Elsevier. Text Epigraph on page 3 from Older ture of the Aging, People and Their Social World: The Subcul- F.A. Davis Company (Rose, A.M. and Peterson, W.A. 1965) p. 11, ISBN: 9780803676206; Epigraph on page 8 from ‘Latent structure analysis’ (Lazarsfeld, P. 1959) (Koch, S. (ed.) 1959), Psychology: A Study of a Science, McGraw-Hill, reproduced with 3, p. 480 permission of The McGraw-Hill Companies; Epigraph on page 54 from ‘The analysis of consumer actions’ (Kornhauser, A. and Lazarsfeld, P.F. 1955), The Social Research, right © Language of pp. 392–403 (Lazarsfeld, P. and Rosenberg, M. (eds.)), Copy- 1955 by the Free Press. Copyright renewed © 1983 by Patricia Kendall Lazarsfeld, all rights reserved, reprinted with the permission of The Free Press, a division of Simon and Schuster, Inc.; Epigraph on page 103 from ‘Case research in marketing: Opportunities, problems and a process ’, of Marketing Research, Journal 12: 199 (Bonoma, T.V., 1985), reprinted with permis- sion by the American Marketing Association; Example 9.1 from Business Research Methods, Oxford University Press (Bryman, A. and Bell, E. 2003) p. 93; Example 8.1 from The Management of Innovations (Burns, T. and Stalker, G.M. 1961) pp. 120–1, by permission of Oxford University Press; Box 13.1 from ‘International comparison of labour disputes’, Trends: Labour Market 195– 201 (Davis, J. 2001), Source: UK Statistics Authority website www.statistics.gov.uk and Business Research Methods, Oxford University Press (Bryman, A. and Bell, E. 2003) p. 227 by permission of Oxford University Press. Crown Copyright material is reproduced with the permission of the fice of Public Sector Information (OPSI). controller, Of In some instances we have been unable to trace the owners of copyright material, and we would appreciate any information that would enable us to do so. xix PART I Challenges and Ambiguities of Business Research .................................................................................. P art I (Chapters 1 and 2) explains the purpose of the book and the challenges related to doing research in business studies. It sets the scene for the rest of the book and explains its perspective, target group, and the purpose of doing research in general. It also takes up the ethical responsibilities of the researcher. 1 Introduction Facts do not simply lie around waiting to be picked up. Facts must be carved out of the continuous web of ongoing reality, must be observed fi within a speci ed frame of reference, must be measured with precision, must be observed where they can be related to other relevant facts. All of this involves methods. R o s e a n d P e t e r s o n (1 9 6 5 : 1 1 ) Purpose of the Book 1.1 The purpose of this book is to help students get rid of the myth that research is ‘too fic scienti ’ or ‘too abstract’ and unnecessary in business studies. It is also intended to help them understand the language and approach of science and research. It will assist them in understanding how properly structured and argued reports can be more convincing and reliable than reports based on a random approach or on common sense. In our opinion, fi a scienti c approach and common sense have much in common. The scien- fic ti approach, however, is a more systematic and controlled treatment of common sense. A layperson uses theories and concepts but in a loose manner. Often, people accept whatever sounds consistent with their beliefs and values: an increase in unemployment is because of immigrants or, in a business setting, the more money you spend on advertising, the more you fi can sell, etc. A scienti c approach, on the other hand, systematically examines assumptions and hypotheses before either believing or discarding them. We have written this book to give practical guidelines for students and researchers. It is based on our own experiences as researchers and as supervisors for numerous PhD and Master’s theses, and that of many colleagues and students in business studies. The fifth edition has also been improved and expanded with help and assistance from our colleagues and students who have been using the first four editions. In this edition, particular attention has been paid to three important issues: the use of qualitative research in business studies, the impact of cross-cultural issues, and ethical issues in research. These issues are dealt with in each and every chapter. A new chapter examines qualitative data analysis, and new chapters and 4 Introduction sections have also been added on quantitative data collection and analysis. Moreover, several examples have been added; these are now boxed to enhance readability. This book is designed to help students understand that a conscious (scien- fic) ti approach is the most appropriate for research and problem-solving projects in business studies. This means helping students understand that before beginning research on a project, they must be aware of what they are doing and what they are not doing. They have to clarify for themselves and for the reader the perspective they have chosen, and they must identify who fit from the study. In other words, researchers must learn how to should bene formulate a problem, how to choose a particular method, and how to argue and motivate. They must also learn how to write a valid and reliable report, which is useful for the purpose of research and for managers or decision makers. We provide students and others involved in research in business studies with clear, hands-on guidelines for doing research. The book deals with the following: • • • the importance of systematic research and problem solving; how to cope with problems associated with doing research; different types of research, the role of the researcher, and the importance of methods and models; • the practicalities of research, such as problem formulation, relating the research to previous studies, choosing a suitable methodology, presenting results and • findings, and drawing conclusions; different methods of data collection and analysis, qualitative as well as quantitative, and their advantages and disadvantages; • which type of method is suitable for which type of research problem and conditions; • • • case study research; the role of the researcher and ethical issues in doing research; how to test the assumptions necessary for the method and techniques being used, and whether these assumptions are valid: in other words, validation of methods and models and not only validation of hypotheses; • the impact of cross-cultural (international) issues on the research process and how to handle these issues; • the practical issues around research in business studies, providing some practical guidelines for questionnaire development, interviewing, and data analysis; • step-by-step guidelines for report writing, the format and writing of a report as a thesis and for publication purposes; • concrete guidelines for oral presentation. 1.2 Readership 1.2 This Readership book is primarily meant for Master of Science (MSc), MBA, and advanced undergraduate students in business studies. It is also meant for PhD students and other researchers at the earlier stages of their research endeavours. Most schools and universities require their graduate and under- fi graduate students to write a thesis or a research report at the nal stage of their studies. These students are the primary target for this text. Students often find the books available either too general or too narrow, dealing with only one aspect of research, such as surveys, interviews, or case studies, fi or else at a higher philosophical level that is often irrelevant and dif cult to comprehend. This book, on the other hand, has an integrative approach and is especially adapted to research in business studies. It will also be highly useful for consultants and business people working with research projects, problem solving, and report writing. Considering the above-mentioned target groups, we have kept the language and discussion simple and accessible. A rather direct and to-the-point stance is taken and a number of examples are given to help the reader understand the point under discussion. – The book is organized in three parts: Part I (Chapters 1 2) dealing with – the challenges of business research, Part II (Chapters 3 6) dealing with the – research process, and Part III (Chapters 7 14) dealing with data collection and analysis, implementation, and report writing In Chapter 2 we discuss the meaning of research with special reference to business studies. The focus here is the difference between research and practical problem solving or common sense, and the different research orien- final tations and knowledge and skills required for research. The part of the chapter deals with ethical issues and responsibilities. In Chapter 3 the role of theory in business research is discussed. Here the focus is on the research process and on the explanation of important concepts, such as theory, models, and knowledge. Chapter 4 deals with the research problem. It is our observa- fi tion that most students of business studies face dif culties in formulating the research problem, and in differentiating between a research problem and a research topic. In this chapter we also discuss the importance of models and systematic thinking in research and the role of reviewing past literature. Chapter 5 deals with research design and problems related to the choice of research design: how the research problem is, and should be, related to the design. Different types of research designs are presented and their use discussed. Examples are used to illustrate the importance and relevance of research design. Problems related to validity and reliability are also dealt with in this chapter. 5 6 Introduction Chapter 6 handles the problems of measurement and the operationalization of research concepts and data. Measurement of empirical research is fi a dif cult task as the quality of the results depends to a large extent upon the measurement procedures used in gathering and analysing the data. The chapter takes us through different types, levels, and scales of measurement. Validity and reliability in measurement are particularly stressed. Some guidelines are provided to improve the measurement. A special section is devoted to measurements in qualitative research. In this new edition, data collection and data analysis for qualitative and quantitative methods have now been clearly separated and are dealt with in different chapters in Part III. While Chapters 7 and 8 deal with qualitative data collection and analysis, Chapters 9, 10, and 11 deal with quantitative data collection qualitative and research analysis. and Chapter Chapter 8 7 thus provides discusses data guidelines on collection designing in and conducting a case study and other types of qualitative research. It discusses when to use qualitative methods and how to analyse data collected through qualitative methods such as case studies and interviews. Chapter 9 thoroughly explains data collection methods for quantitative research methods. It explains the main methods of data collection, such as surveys. Chapter 10 discusses different ways of analysing quantitative data. It explains the role of statistics in data analysis and presentation. Chapter regression 11 provides can be used guidelines for and a quantitative practical data demonstration analysis, while of how Chapter 12 presents a number of other techniques that can be used for quantitative data analysis. The chapter explains and gives examples of the most commonly used statistical techniques for data analysis. Chapter 13 discusses the extra care needed in cases of cross-cultural research, as research involving an unfamiliar environment may complicate the understanding of the research problem, data collection, and data analysis. The chapter also provides guidelines for how to handle multi-country research projects. In these projects, comparability of the data and equivalence of understanding, both of the researcher and the respondents, are of utmost importance. Researchers have to be extra careful in categorization and measurement of cross-cultural data. The chapter deals with each stage of the research project, how it is complicated because of the cross-cultural setting and how each stage can be handled to eliminate items and concepts fl that may cause cultural bias and thereby in uence the fi ndings of the study. Finally, Chapter 14 provides guidelines for writing a report. The process of writing up the fi nal report is often viewed as tiresome work. The report has to be concise, consistent, and convincing. The writing style of the report is also important in order to convince the reader that the report is valid and reliable. 1.3 On the Use of the Book In this chapter the structure of reports is discussed section by section, with examples and illustrations. Guidelines for form and style, for use of footnotes, and the provision of a bibliography are also provided. Different types of reports are discussed, such as a thesis, an oral presentation, and a report written for publication. 1.3 On the Use of the Book A thorough understanding of the objective and the role of theory in practical fi research is needed, so it is recommended that the reader rst scans the whole – text and thoroughly reads Chapters 1 5 before starting work on a business research project. When actually working on a research project, researchers – should read Chapters 6 12, chapters that are relevant for the research project at hand, as this will enable them to decide which research design is the most suitable and which data collection method is most appropriate for their research problem. At this stage they can also get help on how they should go about sampling and collecting data and on which type of analysis will be the most appropriate. While conducting the actual data collection and analysis, the researcher is advised to read the respective chapter from Part III, whichever is relevant. If the researcher is working on an international or cross-cultural research, s/he should go through the chapter on cross-cultural fi research. Finally, before sitting down to write up the report and ndings, the researcher should read Chapter 14, which provides concrete guidelines for report writing for different purposes. An added feature in this fifth edition is that at the end of each chapter a list of further reading is provided. Researchers who want to look more deeply into a particular issue (e.g. on different opinions on the study of management, on ethical issues on secondary data, or how to handle a particular problem caused by cross-cultural data collection) can quickly look up these readings to enhance their understanding. 7 2 Research in Business Studies first, the reader could have If we have mentioned the actual results of investigation labelled these obvious also. Obviously, something is wrong with the entire argument of obviousness. It should really be turned on its head. Since every kind of human reaction is conceivable, it’s of great importance to know which reactions actually occur most frequently and under what conditions; only then will a more advanced social science develop. L a z a r s f e l d (1 9 5 9 : 4 8 0 ) , r e p r o d u c e d with permission of the McGraw-Hill Companies The purpose of this chapter is to explain what we mean by research in business studies and to discuss differences between systematic research and common sense or practical problem solving. Different research orientations fl are also discussed to illustrate the in uence of researchers’ backgrounds and basic beliefs surrounding research methods and processes. We believe that research papers or theses at the Master ’s level, when successfully completed, should demonstrate that the candidate can systematically handle and analyse a problem, arriving at valid conclusions. In other words, it is a professional training process through which we can learn to think and work systematically and develop analytical capabilities. The advantage of systematic thinking and analytical capability is that it contributes to accuracy and a more orderly approach and is reliable in handling research as well as business problems. EXAMPLE 2.1 A business firm experiences declining sales and – of course – managers and employees feel frustrated. Through systematic observations and thinking, it becomes clear that the declining sales are caused by a newly introduced substitute. This results in a systematic effort to develop and introduce a new and improved product offering. The increasingly complex nature of business operations and decision making demands a systematic and thoughtful approach. The importance of research in business studies, in schools, and in businesses has therefore 2.1 The Nature of Knowledge Development EXAMPLE 2.1 (CONT.) increased. Practical problem solving and decision making are (or at least should be) becoming more and more similar to research. Business and marketing research are common activities in medium-sized and larger companies. And most of the decision making in these companies is based on research. For example, whether a company is launching a new product fi rst or trying to enter a new market, it has decide which segment to to undertake some research to target, whether there will be demand for its product (sales forecast), and how to develop a marketing plan for the new product or new market. All this requires research that has to be undertaken by the company itself or by a market research company. 2.1 The Nature of Knowledge Development Debate on the nature of knowledge is not new and dates back to Plato and Aristotle and other ancient cultures; all cultures have been interested in differentiating truth from falsehood or right from wrong. Philosophers therefore often debate the conditions of knowledge, the reliability of sources of knowledge, and the limits of knowledge (see e.g. Moser et al., 1998). In this section, the idea is not to be too philosophical about knowledge development and delve into extensive debate on several ‘isms ’. Our intention is instead to explain what knowledge is, how we acquire it, and how we differentiate between ‘knowledge’ and ‘mere opinion’. We acquire knowledge on our own, in school, at home, and at work, and we believe that it is valuable to have knowledge. However, we also know that we cannot believe what everybody says and that we should have a critical approach to acquiring knowledge. In other words, we need some training in differentiating false from true or what we can believe from what we cannot believe. The theory of knowledge is calledepistemology, which comes from Ancient Greek episteme for knowledge and logos for theory and explanation (Moser et al., 1998). Epistemology is thus the source and the limit and the nature of knowledge. While developing knowledge it is useful to question our own assumptions in addition to ontology (the nature of reality) (Tsoukas and Chia, 2011). Ontology and epistemology together form research philosophies and have fundamental methodological implications (Isaeva et al ., 2015). When choosing research methodologies, we draw upon different ontological and 9 10 Research in Business Studies epistemological assumptions. These assumptions thus underpin our choice of methods. However, in management studies, there is no conclusive agreement on the best philosophy. Philosophically, we cannot be certain that an individual knows what he claims to know. This is called scepticism. According to epistemology, therefore, instead of outright accepting or rejecting knowledge, we seek explanation to be sure what a particular piece of knowledge consists of and how it has been acquired. Methods of acquiring knowledge and explanations thus take a prominent role in the nature of knowledge development. Western definition of knowledge that it should fi ositional knowledge is a justifi ed true belief (Moser et al., 1998), or knowledge is a justifi ed true belief. However, it is still debated whether all three componepistemology philosophers offer a have three components: justi cation, truth, and belief. This means that prop- ents have to be there and whether these components are acquired by sense or experience. In epistemology we use different terminology for knowledge based on sense and knowledge based on experience. Knowledge based on sense is called ‘a priori’ knowledge, while knowledge based on experience is called‘a posteriori’ knowledge, in other words, knowledge acquired after experience. fi This distinction helps in the justi cation of known propositions. The debate about contemporary epistemology and sense (common sense) started during the period of Bertrand Russell (1872 –1970) and G.E. Moore fi (1873–1958). Although there is no common de nition of ‘common sense’, it was believed that a common sense is the beliefs of a group of people about a certain thing. Connecting sense and experience, Russell (1972: 75) believed that ‘all knowledge that asserts existence is empirical, and the only a priori knowledge concerning existence is hypothetical giving connexions among things that exist or may exist’. However, our common sense about knowledge can change; it can be corrected or rejected with the development of knowledge. 2.2 Why We Do Research The basic purpose of research in education is to teach students to work systematically and for them to learn to analyse issues critically before believing in them or acting upon them. However, research is essential for understanding even basic everyday phenomena that need to be handled by individuals and organizations. If we want to buy a car, we do some research, finding out which car fi satis es our needs and where it is available; compare prices at different dealers, or among different cars that fit we our criteria, and so on. In the same manner, a company has to do research while making important decisions, whether it is to reorganize its structure or to 2.2 Why We Do Research merge or take over another company (Hassan and Ghauri, 2014). Hassan and Ghauri believe that many mergers and acquisitions fail due to the fact that proper research was not undertaken while evaluating the target company or each other. Businesses these days are doing systematic research to handle their day-to-day activities (Sekaran, 1992). Businesses are beginning to develop a strategic monitoring program to identify and understand strategies. Any competitors ’ firm strengths, weaknesses and overall business can establish a competitor-analysis system that provides management with essential information about a wide range of strategies that rivals are likely information pro and to pursue. how to The key combine is knowing separate pieces where of to data gather into a relevant coherent file of each competing operation. (Svatko, 1989: 59) We really cannot take decisions on important issues unless we investigate (research) the relevant information more deeply, gathering more information on the particular aspect we are interested in. Then we analyse all this information to make a judgement about the right solution to our problem or answer to our question. In business studies we normally work with problems faced by managers and companies. For example: How to enter a particular fl market? What are the factors that in uence performance in joint venture fl relationships? What are the factors that in uence the successful launch of a new product? Is advertising necessary, or how much advertising is necessary fi to market a certain product? Would acquiring a particular company ful l fi fluence its our strategic objectives? Would the ethical behaviour of a rm in competitive position or not? And so on. Without research, we cannot answer the above or similar questions. As well as learning about systematic information collection and critical analysis, we also need to learn how things work, through research done by others, and then perhaps assess that knowledge to see whether it is applicable to our own problems or situations. Sometimes we need to make or suggest changes to apply what we have found to our own problems. When this process has been done by several researchers, the ideas and theories are tested. Once theories are properly tested, we can even predict the future. We can say with con fidence that in this type of situation/problem, this should be the solution/ answer. Research, therefore, makes our life easier, not only in business but in general. Research can thus be considered as a process of problem solving fi fi for a speci c problem under speci c conditions (Kuhn, 1970). The fundamental purpose of research, however, and we hope everybody can agree, is to improve social life. In business research, the purpose is to understand how and why things happen. 11 12 Research in Business Studies EXAMPLE 2.2 Through systematic research it was found that serious stomach and digestion problems were caused by salmonella bacteria. Research also showed ± that the salmonella bacteria did not survive at a temperature of 80C or higher, resulting in the practice of heating food to that temperature when the presence of such bacteria was suspected. 2.3 Research versus Common Sense There is a common belief that research is an academic activity undertaken by researchers who are not at all familiar with managerial culture and the nature of the problems faced by business managers. At the same time, several studies have revealed that managers do not know how to use research findings and therefore cannot utilize the results and conclusions of research (Gill and Johnson, 2002; Whitley, 1984). In our opinion, research in business studies and managerial problem solving are not much different from each other. Managers need to have some knowledge and evaluation capabilities to understand the consequences of their decisions. In other words, managerial decision making or problem solving, if done systematically, should lead to better decisions and results than those decisions made exclusively through intuition or personal likes and dislikes. Managers must be able to analyse their situations and use investigative approaches to decision making and problem solving. The systematic procedures and approaches to advancing knowledge suggested by the research process also serve as a disciplined and systematic procedure for managerial problem solving. EXAMPLE 2.3 In business life, firms and their managers experience negative surprises, for fi example that customers become dissatis ed and frustrated. An ability to obtain systematic insights into what causes dissatisfaction and frustration is crucial to solve the problem and improve. This can only be done via research. Companies that try to resolve their problems without undertaking any research and understanding the factors that are causing customers to be fi dissatis ed often end up in bankruptcy. We have witnessed several successful companies disappearing from the business landscape: Motorola, Blackberry, and Enron are good examples. 2.3 Research versus Common Sense As a first step, actors in both management and research activities need to decide what they want to achieve. This is followed by collecting relevant information and facts that can help in achieving the first objective. The information collected needs to be analysed and put into a structure that helps to achieve a purpose or initiate different actions. This process, deciding what to do, collecting information, discarding irrelevant information, analysing the relevant information, and arriving at a conclusion or decision in a systematic procedure, is useful for building cumulative knowledge as well as in the personal development of the researcher and manager alike (Ghauri and Firth, 2009; Revans, 1971). Although some scholars differentiate between academic research and research done by companies, for practical problem solving, we believe in ‘trans-disciplinarity ’ in research, where the boundaries of a single contributing discipline can be crossed. The production of knowledge is thus not restricted to academic research. It involves academics, policy makers, and managers, and useful knowledge is developed and exploited more quickly in collaboration than if different types of knowledge were to be developed by different parties (Gibbons et al., 1994; Hadjikhani fi et al., 2012; Tran eld and Starkey, 1998). The purposes of doing research are manifold, such as to describe, explain, analyse, understand, foresee, and criticize already existing knowledge or phenomena in social sciences. The job of a researcher is often that of an observer, and each observation is prone to error; therefore, we go out and research to find a better ‘truth’ or answers to our questions. EXAMPLE 2.4 It is costly to perform transactions. In a classic article,‘The Nature of the Firm’, R.H. Coase (1937) raised the ‘obvious’ question: Why do firms exist? Coase was trained as a neo-classical economist. In neo-classical economics, fi rms do not exist in highly competitive markets. Rather, transactions are fi mediated through markets. Coase explained the existence of rms as being due to ‘frictions’ – or transaction costs. This insight is useful to understand why in some cases firms use the market to perform transactions, whereas in other cases transactions are conducted internally. If the role of a researcher is that of an observer, then what is the difference between an observer who can draw conclusions with common sense and a researcher? The difference is that observations made by the researcher should be systematic, arguable, and challengeable. The researcher explains to us how s/he collects information, argues for the methods used to obtain the 13 14 Research in Business Studies results, and explains their limitations. In an ideal situation, if anybody else made observations using the same methods, they would come up with more or less the same results. The role of the researcher thus becomes very important. When we look and observe, we see differently depending upon our background and what we know and expect. Two different people observing the same object may see two different things. It is thus very important to discuss the object, the observer, and the biases. For this reason, the observer has to explain and convince the reader of the purpose and methods of observation so that the reader can make a judgement about the validity of the results. EXAMPLE 2.5 Research fi ndings show that employees tend to see problems and solutions from the position and tasks they are involved in. For example, a marketing person tends to see marketing problems, a person involved in production sees most problems as production problems. The main reason is that individual actors are constrained by their limited cognitive capacity, that is their limited capacity to notice, make sense of, store, retrieve, and make use of information (data) that is not accessible to them. The usefulness of research is often discussed, especially when it comes to things that seem self-explanatory and common sense. But the very same common sense and self-explanatory beliefs have been proven to be wrong fl through research. Common sense and beliefs, in uenced by our society and culture, provide us with a non-conscious ideology, and we believe in things fl without being aware of the reality. Our common sense is thus in uenced by our background, education, and beliefs. Common sense is thus the most uncommon thing. As Bem (1979: 89) said, ‘Only a very unparochial and intellectual fish is aware that his environment is wet. ’ This is further illus- trated by the following: A man and his son are involved in an automobile accident. The man is killed and the boy, seriously injured, is rushed to the hospital for surgery. The surgeon takes one look at him and says, ‘I am sorry, but I cannot operate on this boy. He is my son.’ (Selltiz et al., 1976: 4) Whenever we tell this story to our students, many of them do not understand the catch. In many societies, the unconscious belief is that a surgeon is 2.3 Research versus Common Sense always a man and therefore we do not even consider that the surgeon might be the mother of that boy. Scienti fic research often challenges these non-conscious ideologies and beliefs by scrutinizing them. Challenging old beliefs, turning things upside down, and creating new beliefs is not always comfortable. For example, in a number of somewhat conservative states, it is not allowed to include Darwin’s evolution theory in the school curriculum, as it challenges the belief promoted by religion about the creation of mankind. Research allows us to correct our misbeliefs, generate new concepts, and broaden our perspectives and perceptions. This is particularly true because research often goes beyond common sense, while common sense considers most things as given. The fundamental difference here is, as mentioned earlier, that research involves systematic methods. The conclusions drawn from research lead to new theories and beliefs. These new theories can challenge our existing beliefs, as illustrated by the following story: A well-known scientist (some say it was Bertrand Russell) once gave a public lecture on astronomy. He described how the earth orbits around the sun and how the sun, in turn, orbits around the center of a vast collection of stars called a galaxy. At the end of the lecture, a little old lady at the back of the room got up flat and said, ‘What you have told us is rubbish. The world is really a plate supported on the back of a giant tortoise. ’ The scientist gave a superior smile before replying, and asked ‘What is the tortoise standing on?’ ‘ You are very clever, young man, very clever,’ said the lady, ‘ But it’s turtles all the way down.’ (Hawkins, 1988: 1) fi The above discussion makes it clear that the difference between a scienti c fi observation and a layperson’s observation is that scienti c research is done systematically and is based on logic and not beliefs: therefore, we stress a logical relationship between cause and effect. Most students in business studies get confused by the terminology used in books on traditional research methods. Although the language and the fic scienti approach fi is somewhat different from common sense, it is not strange or dif cult to comprehend. In fact, quite the contrary, as, when understood, it seems logical and natural. As Whitehead (1911: 10) stated, common sense judgement is is not that the the right new ideas start for research: ‘its shall look like sole the old criterion for ones ’. Research prepares us for new realities. According to one idea, science is a systematic and controlled extension of common sense, as common sense is a series of concepts and conceptual schemes satisfactory for the practical uses of mankind (Conant, 1951: 32 –3). Others believe that these concepts and conceptual schemes can be misleading 15 16 Research in Business Studies in modern times. For example, up until the twentieth century, it was selfevident common sense for many to use punishment as a basic tool of pedagogy. It has, however, been proved that this old view of‘common sense’ may be quite wrong, as rewards seem more effective in aiding learning (Kerlinger, 1964: 4). According to this belief, science and common sense differ in several ways (ibid.): 1. The first difference is that laypersons use They often accept explanations that fit concepts and theories loosely. easily with their beliefs and values, for example that illness is due to sinfulness. Scientists, on the other hand, systematically build up theories and test them for internal as well as external consistency. Moreover, they believe that the concepts they are using are human-made terms that may or may not exhibit a close relation to reality. 2. Laypersons often select theories and test hypotheses, but their evidence fi comes from their own hypotheses. They believe in the evidence as it ts their assumptions. Scientists, on the other hand, test their assumptions and hypotheses systematically and tend to be more careful in their selection and drawing of conclusions. 3. Laypersons do not bother to control phenomena. They do not try to their explanations of control external influences , observed and they accept those assumptions that are consistent with their preconceived biases. They do not try to relate different phenomena. Scientists, on the other hand, are constantly looking for relations among different phenomena. They systematically try to study and control these relations. 4. Finally, laypersons often believe in metaphysical explanations, such as ‘some people are poor because God wants them to be poor ’. Scientists, on the other hand, do not accept metaphysical explanations. They are concerned with things that can be observed and studied. In other words, research is concerned with studying things that can be observed and fi tested. If things can be tested, they can be falsi ed. For example, a person may believe that drinking coffee at night keeps one awake. This assumption (assertion) can be tested. Assuming that people tend to be equally awake whether they drink coffee during the night or not, the stated belief is wrong (cf. Section 12.1). 2.4 Different Research Orientations fl The research process and the research method used are in uenced by the researcher ’s background when it comes to research orientation. A particular 2.4 Different Research Orientations research orientation prescribes the relationship between the methods, data, theories, and values of the researcher. Social knowledge builds one upon fic observations provide new theories, correcting, modifying, another. Scienti extending, and clarifying the older and existing ones. Most methodology books describe ‘originality ’ or ‘original contribution fi to knowledge’ as a basic condition for a scienti c study. Although the demand for originality is perhaps the most controversial, its importance and meaning should not be misunderstood. Students normally believe that topics used by others in their theses should not be studied, because by doing so they would lose originality. We believe ‘originality ’ can describe studies that create a new dimension to already existing knowledge. It implies that there is some novel twist, fresh perspective, new hypothesis or assumption, or new and innovative methods of handling existing knowledge that makes the study a distinctive contribution. In business studies, it is equally possible, or perhaps more useful, to direct research projects towards more sharply delineated tasks. Researchers do not preach or ask whether the social activity observed is good or bad; they just analyse, present, and explain it. In fact, that is the starting point of research: that we have a number of assumptions/speculations, but we should not accept or reject them unless we study them critically and unless we find logical and reliable explanations to accept or reject them. The researcher thus tries to be as objective as possible. It is, however, not always possible to be objective, as put forward by Burrell and Morgan (1979: 225): fi What passes for scienti c knowledge can be shown to be founded upon a set of unstated conventions, beliefs and assumptions, just as every day common-sense knowledge is. The difference between them lies largely in the nature of rules and community which recognises and subscribes to them. The knowledge in both cases is not so much ‘objective ’ as shared. Scholars involves often discuss fic, scienti five objective, research and orientations: measurable (1) Positivism, facts that can which predict the future (Donaldson, 1997); (2) Critical realism, which asserts that a variety of data and methods are acceptable as long as the researcher is as objective and realistic as possible (Bhaskar, 1986); (3) Pragmatism, seeking know- ledge through a variety of methods and their combination in providing a solution to a problem (Elkjaer and Simpson, 2011); (4) Interpretivism, which aims to present full richness with diverse interpretational stories along with contextual factors to enhance our understanding (Crotty, 1998); and fl (5) Post-modernism, where the researcher is radically re exive and critically sensitive in his or her own disciplinary nature of our different orientations, role field, from (Cunliffe, 2003). Considering the inter- researchers in business studies may have objective generalizable problem solving to 17 18 Research in Business Studies Table 2.1 Different research orientations Orientation Explanations and beliefs Positivism A single truth exists that can be explained by law-like causal generalization. Knowledge is objective and is based on observable and measurable facts and relationships. It can predict behaviour. Critical realism Knowledge about social interactions versus physical facts should be approached differently. Researchers can thus use a variety of methods while staying as objective as possible. It leads to explanatory explanations and provides insights into antecedents and causes. It encourages methodological diversity. Pragmatism Knowledge should be relevant and useful to practice. Research should provide solutions to practical problems. It encourages a variety of methodological approaches. Interpretivism Context is important and knowledge is thus subjective. Concepts and theories cannot provide richness and are too simplistic. A variety of explanations and interpretations are encouraged where meanings are more important. It provides fic and in-depth understanding. context-speci Post-modernism Truth and knowledge based on dominant ideologies and fl discourses. Researchers are encouraged to be re exive and examine dominant discourses and reveal the hidden meanings. Source: speci fic Based on Isaeva et al. 2 ( 015). practical problem solving for a company. These different orienta- tions are further explained in Table 2.1. Our epistemological assumptions lead us to a particular research question and methods of data collection and analysis (Burrell and Morgan, 1979). Scholars often logical pluralism, saying that diversity enriches the advocate epistemo- field, and as researchers we should have open minds, in addition to questioning our own assumptions (Isaeva et al., 2015; Knudsen, 2003). 2.5 Induction, Deduction, and Abduction A researcher observes and faithfully records what is seen without any prejudice. Some of these statements of observation are established as true and serve as the basis for theories and laws. There are two ways of establishing what is 2.5 Induction, Deduction, and Abduction true or false and to draw conclusions: induction and deduction. Induction is based on empirical evidence, while deduction is based on logic. induction Through vations. analysis into !fi In this existing ! type ndings ! we draw general conclusions from our empirical obserof research the process findings theory building, as knowledge goes (literature/theories) to from observations are incorporated back improve theories. In this research, thus, theory is the outcome of research (Elg et al., 2017). This type of research is often associated with qualitative research. The process goes from assumption to conclusions and is illustrated as follows: Assumption : Psychiatrists have found that psychological problems in patients depend on their experiences in childhood. Conclusion : All psychological problems are founded in experiences in childhood. It is, however, important to note that we can never be 100 per cent sure about the above inductive conclusions, as these conclusions are based on some empirical observations. Sometimes conclusions based on hundreds of observations can also be wrong. This can be explained by the prediction of election results in a general election. Although the prognosis might be that the Labour Party is going to fi win a UK election, we cannot be sure until we have seen the nal results. For the UK Brexit referendum, all the polls were indicating that the Remain side was going to win, even after the polling stations had closed. However, when the final results came, the Leave side had won. In other words, we can arrive at more or less probable results, but not with 100 per cent certainty. By deduction we mean that we draw conclusions through logical reasoning. In this case, it need not be true in reality, but it is logical. The researcher in this type of research builds/deduces hypotheses from the existing knowledge (literature), which can be subject to empirical scrutiny (testing) and thus can be accepted or rejected. The researcher ’s main job is not only to build hypotheses from existing knowledge but also to present them in operational terms (operationalization), to show how information can be collected to test these hypotheses and the concepts being used (Bryman and Bell, 2003; Chalmers, 1982; Merton, 1967). In this type of research, theory, and hypotheses built on it, comes first fl and in uences the rest of the research process. This type of research is often associated with quantitative research. The process of deduction goes as follows: Assumption Assumption Conclusion : : All metals expand when heated. : Rail tracks are built of metal. Rail tracks will expand when heated. 19 20 Research in Business Studies The above examples explain the difference between induction and deduction. The difference is that in induction, facts acquired through observations lead us to theories and hypotheses, while with deduction (logical reasoning) we accept or reject these theories and hypotheses. This acceptance and rejection then helps us to explain or predict. In the process of research, methods begin with the ideas and facts that we know so far, which lead us to propositions, theories, and predictions. New theories and predictions lead us to new ideas and facts, and a new cycle begins, leading us to new theories. When we use observed facts in generating a theory that is consistent with these facts, we are doing induction. In other words, induction is the process of observing facts to generate a theory, and it is perhaps the first step in scientific methods. While doing research we formulate propositions after observing the relationships between different the variables of our study. Most researchers in business studies go through this method, observing facts or earlier results, that lead them to propositions and later to theories. On the other hand, in deduction we look at the consequences of a theory. There is an established school of thought which believes that the entire research process is initiated by theories. Deduction involves the gathering fi of facts to con rm or disprove hypothesized relationships among variables that have been deduced from existing knowledge. As we can see, discussion on induction and deduction presents us with alternative ways or stages of building theories. Most researchers and scientists believe that they use both types of reasoning in their research. The processes of induction and deduction are not totally exclusive of each other, and induction includes elements of deduction, and vice versa. In both cases, researchers need to know the nature of the existing knowledge. In both cases, however, a great deal of creativity and imagination is demanded from the researcher or investigator. Both induction and deduction demand that we fi go beyond statistical signi cance to systematic data collection, and that we are aware of the sensitive question of the relevance of data to theory or study. Moreover, both demand that the investigator keeps up to date with fi theories and ideas and with scienti c methods. EXAMPLE 2.6 Deductive reasoning – the logical process of deriving a conclusion from a known premise or something known as true. Inductive reasoning – the systematic process of establishing proposition on the basis of observation or particular facts. a general 2.6 What Comes First: Theory or Data? Lately, Abduction often several scholars have been advocating an abductive approach. is not just a combination of induction and deduction, as it is understood. Abduction refers to a theoretical interpretation of an empirical problem that can lead to development of new theories. In other words, abduction is a continuous process that takes place in all phases of the research process where the researcher’s observations are confronted with theoretical assumptions and generate a new view of the phenomenon (Dubois and Gadde 2002; Van Maanen et al., 2007). This means that the original framework and theoretical assumptions of the researcher are continuously fied modi findings. Normally we would develop a field before undertaking research that leads us to one as a result of empirical theoretical view of the or more theoretical lenses through which we observe the reality. In this fi process and in case of unexpected or novel ndings, we need to re-examine our frameworks. Dubois and Gadde (2014) refer to deep probing that leads fi back and forth between framework, data, and research and to context-speci c flexibility and allows us explanations. This abductive approach thus brings us to direct our research towards theoretical insights and learning. 2.6 What Comes First: Theory or Data? It is often assumed that theory should precede data, that is, observations. This impression is easily supported by the way the‘research process’ is illustrated (see Chapter 3), and research often takes place this way. For example, this is the case when a researcher carefully reviews relevant literature and‘sees’ a research opportunity, that there is a gap, a weakness, or unanswered question in present insights. In this case, the researcher usually (but not always) has a clear research problem: for example, do variations in X explain variations in Y? Often, however, this is not the case. The researcher may observe something s/he does not understand, typically resulting in questions such as ‘Why? ’ or ‘Why does this happen?’ For example, the researcher may fi observe a speci c practice, a way of doing things that is counterintuitive or flict in con with what s/he has been exposed to previously. This is often the case in ‘qualitative’ research with a prime emphasis on gaining understanding. A key purpose in this latter case is to‘construct theory’, that is, to come up with an adequate explanation (see Figure 2.1). It should be emphasized that when doing research, interactions between theory and data take place. For example, a researcher conducting a structural fi survey to test a speci c hypothesis gets an unexpected finding, triggering speculations as to why this might be so: that is, the researcher tries to fi theorize to come up with an adequate explanation. The unexpected nding 21 22 Research in Business Studies Theory (1) (2) Data Figure 2.1 shows Theory or data as starting point that the hypothesis (theory) may be wrong, and thus forces the researcher to rethink prior belief. 2.7 Knowledge Skills and Research Basically, we do research because we want to know more about ourselves and the world around us. A number of scholars have explained different ways of knowing (Buchler, 1955; Cohen and Nagel, 1934; Kerlinger, 1964: 6 –9): 1. Method of tenacity , where we hold firmly to the truth or the truth we fi know to be true, as we have always known it to be true. We may nd ways to strengthen our belief even if the proposition or new develop- flat. ments may show otherwise. For example, the world is 2. Method of authority , where it has been established that this is the case. For example, the Bible states that there is a God. In fact, even if such knowledge seems unsound in certain situations, we live our lives according to this authoritative knowledge. 3. A priori method propositions or that method of intuition , where knowing is based on are self-evident or ‘agreeable to reason’. However, something that is self-evident to one person might not be self-evident to another. For example, is it self-evident that US education is inferior to European education – is this ‘agreeable to reason’ (Kerlinger, 1964)? 4. Method of science body ’s final , where we find a way of knowing in which every- conclusion is the same. In other words, there are things whose characters are entirely independent of our opinions about them (Buchler, 1955). The fi scienti c approach to obtaining knowledge has self-correction as one of its characteristics. We must, however, point out that this is a rather deterministic view of science. In our opinion, there are several methods of acquiring knowledge that can be called scienti fic, but this is the most widely accepted view. Some scholars call it objectivity (Kerlinger, 1964). As Polanyi (1958: 4) states, scientists systematically and consciously use the self-corrective aspect of the 2.9 Research and Ethics fic scienti approach. Considering the above, as expressed by Braithwaite (1955: 1): The function of science . . . is to establish general laws covering the behaviours of the empirical events or objects with which the science in question is concerned, and thereby to enable us to connect together our knowledge of separately known events and to make reliable predictions of events as yet unknown. 2.8 Why Research Never Stops As mentioned in an earlier section, when solving problems we need to look at what is already known about that type of situation or problem. In some cases, for example in exploratory research, we make some observations to clarify our research topic or field. What is known in thefield and these observations help us to understand our problems better. They also help us to ask the right questions. Once we have better knowledge of our problem area, we try to look at our field in a rather systematic manner. With the help of this clarified picture, we then build hypotheses or assumptions. Hypothesis building or deriving assumptions leads us to the concepts and constructs we should study to get answers to our questions. These concepts, when put together in relation to each other, lead us to our model. Once we have a clear understanding of our problems, assumptions, and finding concepts, we start thinking about how to go about answers to our questions. At this stage, we have to come up with a research design. And we need to consider how we are going to collect the information needed and what we are going to do with it. In other words, how are we going to analyse that information? Once we have the design, we can proceed to actually collect and analyse data. The analysis will lead us to our interpretation, what we understand from the information we have collected and analysed. Through this interpretation we draw our conclusions, thus improving existing knowledge or helping in practical problem solving. This also means that researchers coming after us, working on the same topic, will start where we left off. In other words, when classifying their problem they will review earlier knowledge, including our study. The never-ending research activity is illustrated in Figure 2.2. 2.9 Research and Ethics fl Ethics are moral principles and values that in uence the way researchers conduct their research activities. In fact, ethics apply to all situations and 23 24 Research in Business Studies Improvement in Observation, theory or problem literature review solving Interpretations Problem and conclusions clarification Assumptions Data analysis and hypotheses Concepts, constructs, Data collection and models Research design Figure 2.2 The wheel of research activities in which there may be actual or potential harm of any kind to anybody (Churchill and Iacobucci, 2015). fi As researchers we have a moral responsibility to explain and nd answers to our questions honestly and accurately. We can point out the strengths of our methods and models but we also have to be open about the weaknesses and reliability of our results. The readers and users of our research reports are often less analytical than we are and believe what we say. They might not understand therefore, the underlying inform our uncertainties readers when our fi and complexities. results or We conclusions should, might be misleading. Also, if not properly clari ed, our results and reports might be misunderstood in a way that creates more problems than solutions to existing problems (Ross and Harris, 1994). The researcher’s ethical responsibility starts with problem formulation. For example, if we want to study the role of subsidies fi performance of companies, it might not be dif cult to find in the export data sources, do an analysis, and come up with some conclusions. On the other hand, if we want to study the role of bribes or other illegal methods in the export fi performance of companies, we might face dif culty in and doing analysis. Moreover, we might finding data sources find ourselves in a moral dilemma 2.10 The Researcher and the Respondent if our study shows unexpected or drastic results. We would then have to ensure that the research did not cause embarrassment or any other disadvantage to people who have provided us with data (information). This leads to another important matter: the extent to which we should collect data from a research population that is unaware they are the subject of our research and who have not given their consent (Saunders et al., 2016). This is a tricky issue. One way of coping – of particular use in experimental research – is debriefing, that is, the subjects are told about the study and its purpose afterwards. Many researchers have problems in deciding whether it is morally appropriate for them to continue doing research while ignoring ethical issues, mainly because they are difficult, time consuming, or do not fit into their research plans. They quite often believe that if they are not doing anything illegal, it is morally acceptable. There is a difference between whether something is legal and whether it is ethical. Many researchers do not even consider evaluating the ethical dimensions of their research. In business studies, awareness of the importance of ethical issues and the responsibilities of the researcher is growing, with increasing debate on social responsibility and consumer well-being, so that there is a danger that business research that is not aware of ethical issues might lose respect or credibility. Companies are also becoming more aware of the impact of our research and can sue researchers for misleading results. For example, Beecham Products sued Yankelovich Clancy Shulman, a research company, for more than $24 million for negligent misrepresentation of research findings, because its market share forecasts were not upheld during the launch of Delicare, a detergent for fine fabrics (Churchill, 1999: 42). The use of the Internet for data collection has also raised several ethical questions regarding invasion of privacy and using information about people without their consent. 2.10 The Researcher and the Respondent The researcher–participant (subject) relationship is the most sensitive one in business studies research. Quite often a researcher struggles to decide whether to inform the participant about the real purpose of the research. They believe that telling the whole truth might result in the participant refusing or being reluctant to give full cooperation. For example, a student doing an internship in a company and working on a thesis about that company’s problem may want to conduct a competition analysis. Where information on competition is not available, s/he wants to interview one or more of the company’s competitors, but is concerned that if s/he reveals that 25 26 Research in Business Studies Table 2.2 Ethical issues in the researcher–participant relationship 1. Preserving participants’ anonymity 2. Avoiding exposing participants to mental stress 3. Not asking participants questions detrimental to their self-interest 4. Being responsible in our use of special equipment and techniques, e.g. tape-recorder, video, or health-hazardous equipment 5. Allowing participants to withdraw at any time during the research process / data collection 6. Not involving participants in research without their consent 7. Being honest with participants 8. Not using coercion to get information 9. Ensuring we don’t deprive participants of their rights, e.g. of self-determination 10. Allowing participants to see the results of the research s/he is doing an internship in one of the competing companies, the others will not give her/him access. In our opinion, we have to be honest and open with the participants and convince them that their position will not be endangered. The violation of ethics is often associated with disguised observations and the use of deception while collecting data. One way to avoid this is to consider whether the participant has a right to know the whole truth or not. Churchill and Iacobucci (2015) present ethical concerns on ten areas of the researcher –participant relationship (see Table 2.2). Ethical issues thus require attention at an early stage of the research process, otherwise a researcher might waste a lot of time and resources only to realize final stage, that the project cannot halfway through, or sometimes even at the be completed due to ethical problems. It is the responsibility of the researcher to assess carefully each and every point mentioned in Table 2.2, and in case of doubt, s/he must take all reasonable precautions to inform and safeguard the respondents. The issue of fi con dentiality is fi particularly important. If the researcher has promised anonymity and con dentiality, s/he has to take extra care that in no way can the identity of the participant or organization be detected. This may involve legal repercussions for the researcher. Deception is ficult another dif issue: this is when researchers intentionally present their research or its objectives as other than what they are. Although these days all universities demand that approval is sought from the university’s ethical approval committee before data collection is started, the best way to improve the researcher – participant relationship in this respect is to consider the following: 1. Evaluate ethical issues right from the beginning of the research project, at the problem-formulation stage. 2.11 The Researcher’s Moral Responsibility 2. If you suspect problems, discuss these with your supervisor, fellow researchers, and potential participants. 3. Provide participants with a complete picture of your research project, its purpose, objectives, and the type of information and access needed. 4. fi Assure participants of anonymity and con dentiality (if required), and that they will not suffer any harm. 5. Use appropriate and simple language when interacting with participants. They might not be aware of the terminology or detail of the research topic, and they might be reluctant to ask. 6. Facilitate understanding and answering of questions. 7. Establish a trustworthy and credible relationship with participants. 8. Where there are any costs involved in answering your questions or providing you with information, you should be responsible for meeting these costs. 9. Assure participants that they will be able to comment on the report before it is made public. 10. fi Assure participants that they will get a copy of the nal report when it is ready. Ethical issues arise particularly at the data collection stage, where a participant cannot be forced or coerced to answer questions. Depending upon the participant method to of data collection, answer. When the conducting researcher surveys, has to persuade researchers are the happy if they get a 30–40 per cent response rate. In interviews, they may have to contact tens of potential participants before they get an interview. Reporting results objectively and honestly is the most important aspect of ethics. The results should be presented in such a way that they do not cause embarrassment, disadvantage, or harm to any of the participants (Saunders, 2011; Zikmund, 1997). Moreover, they must not be distorted to fit your purpose or favour a particular participant/target group or be presented in a fl way that does not re ect reality. Any misinterpretation of data will lead to misleading results and is ethically wrong. 2.11 The Researcher s Moral Responsibility ’ The moral responsibility of the researcher relates to the social guidelines and constraints upon research techniques and measurements. The researcher has to make a moral judgement about the appropriateness of the research procedures. Although value judgement of research depends on the researcher ’s own perception and interpretation of the findings, this evaluation is also 27 28 Research in Business Studies fluenced in by the researcher’s environment and time period. This has to do findings that conflict with research findings are fi nal, the with whether or not to accept and reveal certain one’s beliefs, customs, or religion. As no researcher has to make a decision on whether the evidence is strong enough to draw certain conclusions from the findings. Exactly how strong is strong enough can be an ethical issue, as results based on trivial information could be misleading (Forcese and Richer, 1973). findings might lead to action that is against the principles of the Research researcher or the funding organization. They may suggest a certain treatment of a labour force or a certain method of decision making to achieve optimal ficiency which is against todays management ethics. The results might infl uence an important decision to be made by policy makers, for example ef ’ regarding mergers and acquisitions, anti-trust measures, or standards setting for a particular industry. Or they might discourage subsequent research on the topic. In these cases, the researcher has to be ethically correct in reporting his or her results and also the methods, techniques, and instruments used, so that readers can make a judgement about the reliability of the findings. fl Figure 2.3 summarizes the factors that may in uence how research is conducted and the acceptance, rejection, concealing, or revealing of its results. As we can see, public interest in a particular issue might encourage a researcher to reveal or conceal results. For example, a study on racial discrimination in the recruitment or promotion policies of companies or public organizations might be a sensitive issue at a particular time, for instance close to elections or when there is a risk of riots. Studies on the labour– management relationship, where the results might be in favour or against the interests of the management conduct. A study might lead to findings might fl in uence a researcher’s that are against government rules and regulations but ethically and morally correct. In a similar way, most researchers have their own interests and biases. Some research is funded by industrial sectors, government institutions, or companies. In many such cases, researchers are dependent on continuous support from these organizations. If case findings are against the interests of these organizations, the researcher may be reluctant to reveal them for fear of losing funding. In some cases, the researcher is under peer pressure to use a certain method (e.g. quantitative methods) even if it is not suitable or reliable for a particular research problem. Peer pressure may also induce a researcher not to reveal the findings of his/her research, especially at a particular time. All these fl factors carry ethical implications for researchers and may in uence procedures, methods, and analysis. Researchers need to be aware of these issues and 2.11 The Researcher’s Moral Responsibility Public interests and biases Company interests and biases Conduct and Government rules findings of research and regulations (tools, procedures, and analysis) Researcher’s own interests and biases Peer pressure fl Figure 2.3 Factors in uencing research Source: Based on Forcese and Richer (1973: 22). © 1973. Reprinted by permission of Pearson Education, Inc., Upper Saddle River, NJ. should take an ethical stance. Most universities and a number of academic associations have drafted codes of ethical conduct in research. Some of these are listed below: Academy of Management (AoM) www.aomonline.org/aom.asp?ID=&page_ ID=54 Market Research Society (MRS)www.mrs.org.uk/code.htm Social Research Association (SRA) www.the-sra.org.uk/ethical.htm American Sociological Association (ASA)www.asanet.org/about/ethics.cfm. 29 30 Research in Business Studies ........................................................................ Further Reading Dubois, A. and Gadde, L.-E. (2014)‘ Systematic combining: A decade later’, Journal of Business Research, 67(6): 1277– 84. Elg, U., Ghauri, P., Child, J., and Collinson, S. (2017) ‘ MNE micro-foundations and routines for building a legitimate and sustainable position in emerging markets’, Journal of Organizational Behavior, 38(9): 1320–37. Van Maanen, J. (1995) ‘ Style as a theory’ , Organizational Science, 7(4): 641–52. Malinowski, B. (1916/1948) Magic, Science and Religion and other Essays, New York: Natural History Press. Pettigrew, A. (1985) ‘ Contextual research: A natural way to link theory and practice ’, in Lawler, E.E., Mohrman, A.M., Mohrman, S.A., Ledford, G.E., Cummings, T.G., and Associates (eds.), Doing Research That Is Useful for Theory and Practice , San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. ........................................................................ Questions 1. A researcher people. The is interested individuals in are sensitive unwilling information to give this from a group information. of This information can, however, be obtained through others or by the use of speci fic techniques (projective techniques – i.e. individuals tend to pro- ject on others what is symptomatic for themselves). Do you think this raises ethical dilemmas? 2. How does research differ from practical problem solving? 3. What are the main differences between inductive and deductive types of research? 4. Abduction is not just a combination of induction and deduction. Comment on this statement. 5. ‘Research never stops.’ Comment on this statement. ........................................................................ Exercises 1. As a marketing manager, you are asked to launch a product (pick a product) in China. Make a plan of how you would collect and analyse information that could help you make a decision for this launch. 2. You have been asked by a company to do a competition analysis for its product in a particular market. As there is no secondary information available, you have also to collect information from competitors of your company. How would you handle the ethical issues in this case? Draft a letter to send to competing companies asking for their assistance. PART II The Research Process .................................................................................. P art II (Chapters 3 to 6) covers the theoretical foundations of research and the important activities involved in conducting research. In the present edition, we have added sections on the ontological and epistemological background and how it fluences our research approach. We talk about identifying a research topic and in framing and structuring the research problem, as well as determining the research design, including a strategy for where to find data and information and how it can be operationalized in the most appropriate way. Thus, we cover the development of adequate measurements and a plan for data collection. The role of the literature review is also discussed. 3 The Research Process fied true belief. If you have good reasons in support of Knowledge is justi the truth of your beliefs, and your belief is true and is based on good reasons, then you have knowledge, according to traditional analysis. M o s e r e t a l . (1 9 9 8 : 1 6 ) This chapter deals with some conceptual (theoretical) foundations of research. Practical business research is often thought of as collecting data from various statistical publications, constructing questionnaires, and analysing data using computers. Research, however, also comprises a variety of important, non-empirical tasks, such as finding or ‘constructing’ a precise problem and developing perspectives or models to represent the problem under scrutiny. In fact, such aspects of research are often the most crucial and skill demanding. The quality of the work done at the conceptual (theoretical) level largely determines the quality of the final empirical research. This is also the case in practical business research. Important topics focused on in this chapter are the research process and the role of concepts and theory. 3.1 The Process Perspective Research is often thought of as aprocess, that is a set of activities unfolding over time. A main reason for considering it so is that research takes time and fi consideration. Insights may be gained gradually, and may also be modi ed and changed over time. It is also useful to look at research as a process with distinct stages, as different stages entail different tasks. This can help researchers to perform these tasks systematically and to understand what is fi fi to be done at a particular stage. For example, we rst have to clearly de ne our research problem and objectives before starting to collect information/ data. Also, we have to think and state what type of data are needed and how best they can be collected before actually doing it. Figure 3.1 illustrates a typical research process or cycle. The illustrated fi process is a simpli ed one. In reality, however, the process is not so orderly and sequential and is rather messy (see e.g. Morgan, 1983; Pettigrew, 1985). 34 The Research Process 1. Choice of research topic 2. Research problem 2a. Problem presentation 3. Research design Literature review 4. Measurement 5. Data 6. Sample/respondents 7. Analyses 8. Writing (completing the report/thesis) 9. Actions Figure 3.1 The research process Researchers should therefore not be surprised or worried if their research process is not as systematic as presented in Figure 3.1, and if in practice they have to go back and forth in the process all the time. For example, at one stage, such as when doing observations, something unexpected may be discovered, resulting in a return to an earlier stage, such as modifying the research problem. Thus feedback loops between the various stages are more common. It should also be noted that the starting point could be some observation that triggers theorizing about the actual problem (see Section 2.6). Research may also lead to new questions, which is why research seemingly never stops (see Section 2.8). The starting point in Figure 3.1 is theresearch topic, that is the phenomenon or theme to be studied (1). For example, you may be interested in how firms organize their activities, how business firms conduct R&D, or how firms enter foreign markets. Choice of research topic is important for several reasons. For example, is the topic worth pursuing, and is it practicable? A research topic is not a research problem. It is usually broader and more general than a (good) research problem, such as what organization structure is most efficient. When moving from the more general research topic to a research problem (2), a more specific question is addressed. For example, you may ask (after having reviewed the literature): Are firms organized in a 3.1 The Process Perspective Research topic Research problem Figure 3.2 From research topic to research problem bureaucratic way less innovative than firms organized in an ‘organic’ way? 1 The relationship between research topic and a research problem is illustrated in Figure 3.2. From the above discussion we see that a research problem is a question. When we have really established what we want to know and how this relates to present insights, we have a clear research problem. This is the point of departure for further research activities. Any problem must be captured or represented. This is done by a set of interrelated concepts, or a ‘model’, implicitly or explicitly (2a in Figure 3.1). fl The way the problem is captured in uences how it is framed and understood. fl How the research problem is captured in uences: • choice of research design; • measurements; • data collection; • sample; • data analysis; • recommendations. fi In Chapter 4 we discuss more fully how to de ne and capture research problems. Research design (3) relates to the choice of strategy to collect the data needed to answer the stated research problem. As will be discussed later, research problems are multiple, and they come in many forms. In some cases fi the purpose is to understand a speci c phenomenon. This will often be the case in ‘qualitative’ research. In other cases, the purpose may be to determine the most adequate action, best mode of market entry, and so on. 1 Inspired by Burns and Stalker (1961). 35 36 The Research Process Inspection of Figure 3.1 shows that after the choice of the overall strategy, the choice of research design follows a series of activities. Data are carriers of information. A variety of data sources are often available; however, the various sources have both advantages and disadvantages. One can also use several data sources in the same research project, which is known as ‘triangulation’. More recently, modern information technology, for example the Internet, has become an important source for gathering the data needed. Choice of data and how to collect them, from whom, and in what way, is important. Such choices are dependent on the type of problem, the information needed and, not least, the data possibilities. Empirical measurements relate to theoretical, unobservable constructs (concepts). For example, ‘power game’ is a concept. How can/should this be captured? Another example is the concept of ‘friend’. How do we recognize that a person is a friend? Good measurements are a prerequisite for highquality empirical research. It is a demanding task to develop good measures. Measurement problems will be dealt with more fully in Chapter 6. Data must be handled, analysed, and interpreted to become meaningful information that can fl in uence subsequent actions and results. Various aspects and methods of analysing data are dealt with in the data analysis chapters for the respective type of research in Part III. Most research efforts are reported in written form, for example as research reports, but also as theses. Craftsmanship is needed to write a good research report (thesis). This is dealt with in Chapter 14. In business, the outcome of research efforts often fl results in or in uences actions. This, however, is beyond the scope of this book, and thus is not dealt with here. 3.2 Levels of Research Going back to Figure 3.1, a distinction can be drawn between activities at the theoretical (conceptual) level (2, 2a) and those at the measurement (empirical) level (4, 5, 6, and 7). Choice of research design may be seen as the‘bridge ’ between activities at the conceptual and empirical levels. The following should be noted: requires activities at the all conceptual research – irrespective of discipline – level. So-called ‘theoretical studies’ deal only with this level. For example, studies in mathematics and pure fi (theoretical) economics primarily relate to speci c problems without seeking empirical evidence. Also, in business studies, important contributions have been made that are primarily ‘theoretical ’ (even though inspired by empirical fl observations) such as the in uential contributions by J.D. Thompson (1967) and J.G. March and H.A. Simon (1958), which have greatly shaped the 3.3 Research and Knowledge Conceptual 1 (theoretical) 2 LEVEL Measurement (empirical) Figure 3.3 Two levels of research thinking of and research in business administration disciplines. Theoretical studies correspond to Circle 1 in Figure 3.3. However, an empirical study – even a study for practical business purposes – requires efforts at the conceptual level. See Circle 2 in Figure 3.3. Bypassing such activities and jumping to the ‘raw empirical data’ is seldom very successful. The fact that this is often done in business does not mean fl that such research is good; rather it re ects lack of insight. 3.3 Research and Knowledge Even if it is not the prime purpose of doing particular research, the main purpose of research is to produce insights or knowledge. Knowledge implies that we ‘know’ something, and that what we know ‘holds true’, that is, the add produced knowledge is valid. Doing research also implies that we to present knowledge: that is, research is done to create new insights. For example, if a business firm conducts a study to examine what buyers emphasize when buying a particular product, this is done to create new insights believed to be important to the firm, so that it can improve its marketing efforts. fi Knowledge can be classi ed in various ways (Naegel, 1961): • theories/models; • concepts; • methods/techniques; • facts. New insights can be acquired in any of the above categories. For example, the researcher may develop a new theory to describe and explain how buyers behave. New methods or techniques can be developed to assist business managers in their decision making, and new facts may be uncovered. For example, before entering a new market, the firm needs knowledge to assess the size of the market and the competitive situation in it; these are prerequisites to developing a marketing plan for the new market. 37 38 The Research Process New insights may be acquired by demonstrating new practical implications of a theory as well: by testing hypotheses derived from theory, and by applying a method to a any research should have an new problem. The important intended contribution, point is that that is to bring or add something new. 3.4 What Comes First: Theory or Research? In the research literature, a distinction is often made between the following two strategies: 1. theory before research; 2. research before theory. In the first case, present knowledge allows for structuring the research problem so that the researcher knows what to look for, what factors are relevant, and what hypotheses should be tested empirically. From the above discussion it follows that, when wrestling with problems, the researcher also makes (or at least should make) use of available knowledge (earlier studies on the topic and its related areas). Figure 3.4 illustrates the two research strategies. In the first case (1), important tasks are to identify relevant concepts, theories, and so on, and to adjust the concepts (theory) to the problem under scrutiny (which also requires a clear understanding of the research problem). In the second case (2), the prime task is to identify relevant factors and construct explanations (theory). This relates to different contexts of research, that is the KNOWLEDGE (‘THEORY’) Theories/models Concepts Methods/techniques Facts (2) (1) Problem Observations/ assumptions Figure 3.4 Production and use of theory 3.5 Some Important Issues fi ‘context of justi cation’ (1) and the ‘context of discovery’ (2) (see Popper (1961) for a lucid discussion). An interesting observation is that route 1 also corresponds to the use of ‘theoretical’ knowledge for practical problems. The user must select adequate theories and methods and adjust them to the actual problem, which is a demanding task. Figure 3.4 shows a broken line between the two strategies, indicating that fi when applying present insights to speci c problems, new observations and new questions may give rise to a search for new explanations, methods, or techniques. 3.5 Some Important Issues 3.5.1 Concepts Concepts are the building blocks of any theory or model (see also Chapter 4). A concept is an abstraction representing an object, a property of an object, or a certain phenomenon. ‘Cost’, ‘income’, ‘market share’, and ‘business strategy’ are all examples of common concepts in business and management disciplines. Concepts are crucial in the researcher’s tool bag. They serve a number of important functions: Concepts are the foundation of communication. Without a set of agreed • concepts, meaningful communication is impossible. Concepts introduce a perspective – a way of looking at the empirical • world. fi cation and generalization. • Concepts are means of classi • Concepts serve as components of theories (models) and thus of explanations and predictions. Concepts are the most critical element in any theory, because theydirect what is captured. For example the concepts ‘cognitive’ and ‘dissonance’ direct the theory of cognitive dissonance, and ‘supply’ and ‘demand’ are key concepts in economic theory. Even though many concepts fluence ), everyday life are ambiguous (e.g. ‘democracy’ and ‘in ’ used in they must be clear and agreed upon to be useful in research. 3.5.2 Clari Definitions fication and precision of concepts are achieved through de fi finitions. Here we will distinguish between two types of de nition, conceptual and operational. 39 40 The Research Process EXAMPLE 3.1 fi The concept of ‘market’ as de ned in marketing literature, that is: all the potential customers sharing a need or want who might be willing and able to engage in exchange to satisfy that need or want. fi In this de nition, ‘customers’ and ‘need ’/‘want’ are among the concepts fi used to de ne the concept of market (Kotler, 1997). Another example is the concept fi ned of ‘industry’ de in the strategy literature as: the group of fi rms producing products that are close substitutes for each other. fi Here ‘ rms’ , ‘products’, and ‘substitutes’ are key concepts to explain industry (Porter, 1980). 1. fi fi De nitions that describe concepts by using other concepts are conceptual de nitions. A useful de nition is that concepts should: • • fi unique attributes or qualities of whatever is defined; not be circular, i.e. must not contain any part of the thing point out fi de fi ned; de ning ‘market exchange’ as ‘exchange taking place in the market’ does not enhance communication; 2. • be stated • use being positively , fi i.e. contain the properties of the concept de ned; clear terms. fi An operational de nition is a set of procedures that describe the activities to be performed to establish empirically the existence or degree of existence fi of what is described by a concept. Operational de nitions are crucial in measurement. They tell us what to do and what to observe in order to bring fi the phenomenon de ned within the range of the researcher ’s experience. EXAMPLE 3.2 fi ‘Market share’ may be de ned operationally as: A company’s sales of products in category X in area A during timet/Total sales of product category X in area A during timet, which also requires fi speci cations of ‘sales’, product category X, area, and time period. fi fi In accounting, ‘sales’ during a speci c time interval is often de ned operationally, as shown in Figure 3.5. 3.5 Some Important Issues EXAMPLE 3.2 (CONT.) Inventory at t 0 Sales Purchases Figure 3.5 Inventory at t 1 Example Or: Sales = Inventory at t0 + Purchases during the period (t0 . . . t1) À Inventory at t1. fi nition gives sales as measured in cost (purchase) prices or in terms of volume (quantity). If measured in sales prices, profit will hopefully be This de present. fi Note that the value de ned differs depending on whether it is measured in volume or value and, if it is based on value, whether the cost or sales value fi is used for ‘sales’ . Operational de nitions will be dealt with in more detail when discussing measurements (see Chapter 6). When we move from the conceptual to the empirical level in research, concepts are converted into variables by mapping them into a set of values. For example, assigning numbers to objects involves the mapping of a set of objects into a set of numbers. A variable is a property that takes two or more values and is subject to change, while a constant has only one value. EXAMPLE 3.3 Construct (concept) Variable Height . . . 150, . . . , 180, . . . cm Gender 1 (= women), 0 (= men) 3.5.3 Theory Theory may be viewed as a system for ordering concepts in a way that produces understanding or insights (Zaltman et al., 1977). A theory 41 42 The Research Process includes more than one concept and how these concepts are linked together. A theory is: fi a set of interrelated concepts, de nitions, and propositions that present a systematic view of specifying relations among variables with the purpose of explaining and predicting phenomena. It is important to note the purposes of theory, that is to explain whether it is related to understanding or prediction. For example, a researcher holds a fi theory of ‘how advertising works’, and uses this theory to allocate the rm’s advertising budget based on a prediction of an outcome resulting from the spending of the advertising money. Also note the notion of‘proposition’, that is an assumed relationship between two concepts, such as between‘performance’ and ‘satisfaction ’. fi The above de nition of theory also claims it should present a systematic view, to enhance explanation and prediction, meaning that the concepts and relationships involved should represent a coherent ‘whole’. fi It is important to notice that theories focus on speci c aspects of the phenomena or problems studied. This is done to capture the actual problem, and (hopefully) understand (solve) it better. On the other hand, some aspects are left out. This is done because human beings have limited cognitive capacity, making it almost impossible to take everything into account at the same time. 3.5.4 Methods Research methods are rules and procedures and can be seen as‘tools or ways of proceeding to solve problems’. Research methods play several roles, such as: • ‘logic’ or ways of reasoning to arrive at solutions; • rules for communication, i.e. to explain how the findings have been achieved; • rules of intersubjectivity, i.e. outsiders should be able to examine and evaluate research findings. Figure 3.6 illustrates the role of methods in arriving at solutions. An important aspect is that there must be a valid reason (or‘theory’) underlying the actual method so that it will result in the‘correct’ solution. Problem/task Solution/answer Method Figure 3.6 Use of methods 3.6 Concluding Remarks Moreover, choice of methods requires an understanding of the actual research problem. In addition, command over the methods and the ability to adequately choose among (and combine) methods are needed. Concluding Remarks 3.6 Research is often associated with constructing and designing questionnaires, measurements, statistical procedures, and so on, which can be subsumed under the umbrella concept ‘research methodology’. Research methodology can be conceived as a system of rules and procedures. Such rules and procedures are important in research for several purposes: Research methodology can be conceived as rules for 1. speci fic logic to acquire insights. Research methodology is important for 2. (in detail) how the researcher has intersubjectivity, obtained his or reasoning, i.e. a i.e. by reporting her finding, the researcher enables others to evaluate it. communication. Research methodology can also be considered as rules for 3. By reporting on the rules and procedures used, others may try to replicate, fi or they can criticize the approach chosen and the reported ndings. Qualifying research requires competence in logical reasoning and analysis. The researcher thus needs to have command over the concepts and research methodology to be used. Research methodology is thus an important tool in fi the researcher’s tool box. Research, however, is also closely related to nding, selecting, structuring, and solving problems. In order to grasp, represent, and understand problems, the construction of concepts, theories, and models is crucial. Theoretical knowledge and the ability to think conceptually are fi important and a prerequisite for doing quali ed research. Problems represent the point problem of departure fluences in in research. subsequent The perception research activities. and structuring Qualifying of a empirical research therefore requires both conceptual and methodological insights. Skills related to topics dealt with in relevant use of other tools in research. this chapter are crucial to making 43 44 The Research Process Further Reading ........................................................................ Douglas, S.P. and Craig, C.S. (1997) ‘The changing dynamic of consumer behavior: implications for cross-cultural research’ , International Journal of Research in Marketing, 14(4): 379– 95. Frankfort-Nachmias, F. and Nachmias, D. (1996) Research Methods in the Social Sciences, 5th edn., London: Edward Arnold. Locke, L.F., Spirduso, W.W., and Silverman, S.J. (2000)Proposals That Work: A Guide for Planning Dissertations and Grant Proposals, 4th edn., Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Questions ........................................................................ 1. Why is research often considered as a process– and not just an outcome? And what is a research process? 2. What do you think are major sources of research problems? 3. Explain and point out the differences between ‘concepts’ and ‘theories ’. 4. How do theories differ from methods? Exercises ........................................................................ 1. A firm wonders whether to increase advertising expenditure for its core products. What do you consider to be the research problem that should fied and answered before it does so? be identi 2. A firm has designed a market test to assess the effect on sales of a particular discount scheme. What do you consider to be the decision problem? 4 Research Problems What we see depends mainly on what we look for. Sir John Lubbock Problems, that is ‘questions’, drive research. Without research questions there would hardly be any research at all. Research problems are not‘given’. They are detected and constructed. How research problems are captured and framed drives subsequent research activities. In normal research situations, we fi rst select a topic and then formulate a research problem within that topic. The process of constructing a research problem is not quite straightforward and often involves a lot of back-and-forth adjustments. In this chapter we particularly focus on how to construct and adequately capture research problems. 4.1 The Research Ideas starting point of any research effort is some attracts attention and initiates speculation. The idea or observation that researcher might have a particular interest in a certain topic, such as consumer behaviour towards imported products, or understanding mergers and acquisitions. However, to make the topic researchable it must be turned into a research question. Because a key purpose of research is to create new insights, there must be something ‘new’, that is, something we do not already know. Even when replicating a previous study there is an aspect of novelty. For example, in a study conducted among large fi fi rms, a speci c budgeting procedure has been observed to be very effective. Is this procedure also effective in middle-sized and smaller fi fi rms? Or do companies perform better after merging with a competing rm? An important source of research ideas is the past literature. Reading the past literature is often also necessary to determine whether the intended research will contain an element of novelty. A common way to construct a research problem is to spot a gap in the existing literature. This may involve resolving existing controversies, integrating different approaches, or 46 Research Problems challenging existing beliefs (Campbell et al., 1982; Alvesson, 2011). Several scholars suggest that formulating a research question should, in addition to fining the domain and topic of the question, include contextual issues and stakeholders infl uence (Silverman, 2001). By presenting the existing literade ’ ture as inadequate, incomplete, or as ignoring a certain perspective, researchers create an opportunity to contribute by showing that the present study would add something that is missing in the current literature (Locke and Golden-Biddle, 1997). Returning to Figure 3.1, it is seen that the literature is consulted throughout the whole research process. For example, the literature is consulted to identify a research problem, to plan sampling, to formulate questions, and to choose a method of analysis. In Section 4.5 we will return to the role of reviewing the past literature in more detail. EXAMPLE 4.1 A researcher is interested in a particular type of decision, captured by the so-called ‘garbage-can models or decisions’, characterized by situations where ‘solutions are searching for problems’. This is similar to the situation where the researcher has a tool, for example linear programming, and is searching for a problem to apply the tool to. Review of the literature reveals that such decisions have primarily been studied and found in public organizations. The researcher raises the question: Do such decisions also take place in private fi rms (organizations)? Once the researcher knows the topic, it is a good idea to look at earlier theses and journal articles on that topic. Many of these present ideas for future research in that particular area. Reading the literature is not the only way to get good research ideas. Reading may not even be the most important way at a very early stage. The newsletter for ACR (Association of Consumer ACR News Research) had an interesting piece ( , 1995) suggesting, first, that good research ideas relate to creativity. To foster creativity one may brainstorm with other people interested in the topic. A common observation is also fl that one idea may generate new ones. So let ideas ow. The first ideas are not always the best ones. Important sources for good research ideas are to: • Observe the real world. Look at how people and organizations are working (or not working) to generate questions as to why things happen the way they do (e.g. go to a shopping first time). everything as if it was for the mall and actively observe 4.2 Wrestling with Research Problems Look for ‘missing holes’ in the literature to see what has really been • addressed and what has been neglected (e.g. Do the studies on mergers and acquisitions deal with between two competing performance? firms?). Do they deal with mergers To improve your research idea as input to your research problem, get feedback. Discuss your research idea with friends, colleagues, your supervisor – and other people who are interested in similar topics. Wrestling with Research Problems 4.2 Wrestling with a research problem involves the search for structure and fication identi of the ‘real ’ problem; that is trying to answer questions such as: What do I want to know? and How do I want to map the problem? first As emphasized, such questions are important in research. The step in (practical) research is therefore to ask the followingtwo questions: 1. What is the actual problem? 2. What is the best way to solve the problem? Although some questions seem trivial in the beginning, the more we read and brainstorm, Answering the the first more we realize that this is not, in fact, the case. question implies that the researcher really knows what s/he wants to know. A common mistake is to go ahead with data collection fi and other ‘practical ’ activities before knowing the speci c research problem. Such an approach often ends up in a situation where ‘a bunch of data is searching for a problem’; time runs out and money is wasted. To avoid misunderstanding, this does not mean that no observations should be done prior to the research. It is often very useful to conduct some preliminary observations (a pilot study) in order to explore and get acquainted with the phenomenon (problem) and arrive at the actual research question. A useful strategy to get hold of the research problem is to ask questions. Good questions have the following characteristics: • They express relationship(s) between two (or more) variables (e.g. Would fi an increase in marketing budget increase our pro ts?). • They are clear, that is what is asked is understood (e.g. Is culture the main problem in the post-merger integration of two competing The advantage of expressing relationships between firms?). variables is that they can be tested. For example, a marketing manager wonders whether the marketing effort should be directed towards large or small households, 47 48 Research Problems depending upon where the propensity to purchase the firm s ’ product is the highest. A possible question is thus: Is there a relationship between household size and propensity to purchase the particular product? fically: Or more speci Are large households more likely to buy the particular product than smaller households? Note that in the above case the following two concepts are included: household size and propensity to purchase. The relationship between the two concepts is a finitions de to hypothesis. measure As discussed in Chapter 3, we use operational concepts example scores on scales, become (constructs). The actual measures, for variables. Given available data, for example data gathered through test marketing in fic area of household size and whether the item was bought or not, the a speci hypothesis can be tested. Suppose that a test marketing programme followed up by a survey study based on a random sample of households shows the following: 1 Household size No. of households Purchase Large 200 50 Small 300 30 Total 500 80 Here it is evident that the propensity to purchase, meaning purchase/ number of households, is higher for large households, that is 50/200 = 0.25 2 and 30/300 = 0.10, respectively. This also allows for statistical testing. An important point is that by expressing relationships as in the above question (hypothesis) falsi fication is allowed (which is at the heart of hypoth- 3 esis testing). An initial research problem is often rather fi vague and general. For example, a small business manager has dif culty in understanding what fluences in 1 2 3 the firm s ’ performance in ‘good ’ and ‘bad’ times. How can this Survey studies are dealt with in Chapter 12. For statistical testing of hypotheses, see Chapter 11. falsifying, A key assumption is that researchers advance knowledge not by verifying, but by i.e. by letting the assumed hypotheses be tested so that they can be rejected. 4.2 Wrestling with Research Problems ambiguous problem be approached? From cost accounting we know the following: fi Pro t = (Price – Variable costs) ± Quantity – Fixed costs Based on this simple equation (model), we may ask several questions, such as: • Do the prices for the • Does the firm use firm s product fluctuate? specifi c raw material ’ a in price? • Does the demand for the which fluctuates highly firm s product fluctuate? ’ By asking such questions, we can narrow the problem down, and concentrate our effort on solving the real problem. But there is more to this, such as what initiates the questions? In the above examples, the questions are all theory driven, that is, existing theory (cf. the above model) is used as the basis for the questions raised. In fact, a prime value of theory is to identify factors (variables) and relate them to each other and examine such relationships to provide explanations. In the above examples, questions were used to‘structure’ the problems. By using existing knowledge the researcher will often be able to structure the problem, so that hypotheses may be derived and tested, for example. Problems may be more or less understood. A distinction is often made between ‘structured’ and ‘unstructured ’ problems. It should be noted that it is not the problems per se, but the understanding of the problems that is more or less structured. The structuredness of the research problem has implications for choice of research design and research methods. Research practice is fluenced by the researcher s perspective on the philosophy of science also in ’ (see Chapter 2), training, and so on. In most cases, a multi-method strategy is used while formulating a research problem, which varies depending on the literature review, managerial practices, and the researcher ’s orientation fl (Brewer and Hunter, 1989). How understanding of the problem in uences choice of research design is dealt with in Chapter 5. The notions of ‘quantitative’ and ‘qualitative’ methods relate partly to differences in problem structure, but also to differences in the philosophy of science perspective held by the researcher, as discussed in Chapter 2 (Figure 4.1). The research problem, that is what the researcher wants to know intuitively, will have an impact on the choice of research approach. For example, the researcher may be interested in how managers think about strategic issues, initiating the question: How can such insights be obtained? The researcher may have some ‘favourite’ methods (e.g. use of surveys) that fl may also in uence their choice of approach. The researcher ’s beliefs about 49 50 Research Problems Philosophy Problem of science Method Research practice Figure 4.1 Problem, methods, and research practice what constitute relevant insights and how such insights may be obtained will fluence the choice of research approach. also in 4.3 The Research Problem and Research Purpose first step towards finding answers is to discover a problem/question. If you are not able to formulate a problem clearly, your answer cannot be correct. In other words, you need to find a problem in your reality (topic/ field). Questions do not exist, they have to be asked: If we drop an apple, it ‘ falls to the ground. This is obvious for everybody – except for Newton’ (Wiedersheim-Paul and Eriksson, 1998: 38). Strauss and Corbin (1990: 34–6) suggest that there are several sources of researchable problems, such as: assigned research problem, as perhaps suggested by the supervisor or professor. This is often useful for students, as it will provide them with a ‘do-able’ and ‘relevant ’ problem. Another source is the technical literature, which can point out a relatively unexplored area or a topic that needs further development. It may also suggest a new approach to solve stimulate an old the problem. researcher The and and professional experiences reading of technical enhance his/her literature curiosity. would also Finally, personal are often the source of a research problem. Professional experience often leads to the judgement that existing practice fi is not ef cient, fair, or equitable. Research into the issue would then correct this situation. While the research problem explains what you are going to investigate, the purpose explains why and how extensive the investigation will be. It also explains what you really want to find out and how your answer can be used. The purpose therefore cannot be stated in one sentence and has to 4.3 Research Problem and Research Purpose be explained in a paragraph or two. It should be clear whether the purpose of the research is to describe, explain, understand, or predict, and for whom. In other words, who can make use of your results? This is important because the reader/supervisor/examiner will check from your result whether you have fi achieved the stated purpose. If the purpose is not clear, then it is dif cult to make that judgement. The researcher needs to revisit the purpose of the research while presenting results and drawing conclusions and explain whether the purpose was achieved as suggested in the earlier sections. In case the original purpose has how not the been achieved, original then purpose the was researcher adjusted or needs fi modi ed to explain during the why and research process, perhaps due to problems in data collection, time constraints, or costs involved. Students often have problems understanding the importance of theory and methodology in writing a good business report or thesis. This frustration is further aggravated by the absence of a suitable text that they can use as a guide to methodological issues in their project work. The message of this fi book is that scienti c methods are a question of consciousness and aware- fi ness, and should not be seen as dif cult, strange, or unnecessary. Research is a process of planning, executing, and investigating in order to find answers to our specific questions. In order to get reliable answers to our questions, we need to do this investigation in a systematic manner, so that it is easier for others to understand the logic of our research and to believe in our report. Students need to understand the basic methodological approaches to management and business research. This understanding helps them in the fi initial stages of their thesis and project work, providing them with con dence and purpose. We do not intend to give students ready-made tools or advocate a particular approach. The idea is to let students understand that there is not one ‘best’ method for business research and that the choice of method depends upon the research problem, the research design, and the purpose of the research. Moreover, it is also dependent on the capabilities and resources available for the particular research. It is more important to first understand what you really want to know than to look for a systematic and easy or quick way to do it. Although there are several schools and opinions on management and on the study of management and other social sciences, our purpose is not to get involved in this discussion (see e.g. Pfeffer, 1993, 1995; Van Maanen, 1995; Holland, 1999). Instead, we want to synthesize and simplify this research to provide practical guidelines to students fi so that they can ef ciently handle their research projects and write good dissertations and project reports. 51 52 Research Problems 4.4 Models in Research In our discussion above, we showed how research problems should be – through questions – mapped into a limited set of factors (variables). When dealing with a research problem, somerepresentation of the problem is needed. This is also the case ‘in real life’. For example, when a business manager instructs employees to smile, the following representation ormodel may be in his mind:‘When my employees smile, the customers feel comfortable and are more willing to buy.’ The manager’s model can be illustrated as follows: Smiling employees ) Customers feel well ) Willingness to buy We all hold such representations/models. Often, however, they are implicit and ambiguous. firms (organizations) advertise quali fications required, such as Another example is the following. When for employees, they usually indicate the educational background, experience, and so on. Firms seek employees to perform. The implicit assumptions or model underlying such ads are thus: fi Criteria (quali cations) e.g. education, experience ) Performance Models play a dominant role in research. They are closely related to the notion of theory, implying a systematic organization of, and relationships between, concepts. Key characteristics of a model are: 1. Representation: the object or phenomenon is represented by the model. The model is not the object or phenomenon itself. 2. fi fi Simpli cation: a model simpli es by reducing the number of factors included. This is done to make it manageable because taking everything into account often becomes impossible. 3. Relationship(s) between the factors included. In research one is very often interested in relationships between factors (constructs), and, as will be discussed later, in causal relationships in particular, because if one fl knows what the cause– effect relationships are, one may in uence the outcome. Above we used the ‘pro fi Pro t = (Price fit planning – Variable ’ model known to business students: cost) ± Quantity – Fixed cost First, this model is a general representation to capture economic aspects firms (and is definitely not the firm itself ). Second, it is clearly a simplifi cation, as a variety of other factors that may in fluence the firm and important to its performance are left out. Note howfew concepts (variables) are used, that 4.4 Models in Research is ‘costs ’ (variable and fixed), ‘quantity ’, ‘price’, and ‘pro fit . Third, it is easily ’ seen that the various factors are related. By changing, say, price, and keeping fi the other factors to the right of the equal sign constant, pro t will change. In research (and this is also the case in practical research), a prime task is to ‘structure’ the problem. This, to a substantial degree, relates to identifying relevant factors and relating them to each other to map and frame the problem under scrutiny. As noted above, research problems may be more or less structured. This has implications for the choice of research strategy. In the research literature, a distinction is often made between ‘theory before research’ and ‘research before theory’. In the first case, important tasks are to identify relevant concepts, theories, and so on, and to adjust the concepts (theory) to the problem under scrutiny, which also requires a clear understanding of the research problem. When this is the case, the researcher knows what to look for, what factors are relevant, and what hypotheses to test empirically. The second strategy (research before theory) starts with observations or gathering of data. A couple of things should be noted before choosing such a strategy, however: 1. There should be a reason for the chosen approach. If relevant knowledge already exists, this easily ends up as ‘reinventing the wheel’ (see the above discussion of understanding the research problem). 2. This approach ‘theory implies ‘theory testing ’. The construction’, knowledge/skill which requirements is different for doing from such research are different but equally demanding, as doing structured theory testing includes use of statistical methods. Most students in business administration have almost no training in such research. If for some no reason the student dislikes, say, statistics, this in way guarantees that s/he can do a good ‘theory-constructing’ study; rather, the opposite will be the case. In the latter case, the prime task is to identify relevant factors and construct explanations (theory). This, however, does not mean that inspecting the literature is irrelevant. Past insights will often allow for ideas or ‘hunches’ on what to look for and how to understand the research problem. 4.4.1 General Concepts and Specific Use It is also important to be aware that theories/methods and concepts are general, meaning that they allow for subsuming a variety of specific research problems, which of course is useful. On the other hand, being general, the 53 54 Research Problems various theories, concepts, or models possess almost no content about the actual problem. The researcher’s task is to give the concepts/theories/ models some content. The notion of the ‘value chain’ is known to any students who have taken a fi fi firm in question. This may include the of the actual firm s activities as well as course in business strategy. To become of any use for the rm, this general term must be adjusted to the speci c identi fication fication and classi ’ linkages between the activities. In order to make use of the general concepts (theories) the researcher must be able toselect, adjust, and apply such tools fi to her or his speci c problem. This is a demanding task, which requires insight and training. Misuse and non-use of relevant knowledge – as fre- flect quently observed – re lack of such skills, and not, as is often believed, ‘practicality’. 4.4.2 Model Purposes Models may be used for a variety of purposes. At the general level, we may distinguish between: 1. description; 2. explanation; 3. prediction/forecasting; 4. guidance of activities. 1. A model may describe how‘things are’. An example of a descriptive model is the organizational chart. Figure 4.2 represents a (naive) description of the formal organization. Note that this general model only containsone A C Figure 4.2 Organizational chart (a model) B 4.4 Models in Research class of variables, that is positions (A, B, and C). The direction of the lines indicates authority–responsibility relationships. A has formal authority over B, B is responsible to A, and B and C are at the same authority level. Are such simple models useful in business research? Description of the firm may, for example, be combined with analysis and formal authority in a assessment of the knowledge and skill requirements of the various positions fi and can thus be used for identi cation of knowledge gaps to be filled. Alternatively, description of the actual formal structure may be compared with some ‘ideal’ structure, which may be used for improving the organizational structure, or such descriptions conducted at different points in time 4,5 may be used to study structural changes. Descriptions come in many forms. Suppose that a researcher wants to describe the informal fi fi organization of a company. This requires a speci ed and detailed de nition of the concept ‘informal organization’, including what aspects should be addressed. For example, the researcher may end up flow in the company, that is, who is interacting fl communication fl ow or from power relationships reflected in studying the communication with whom; or s/he may study who in uences decisions, to uncover potential deviations in the formal organizational chart. Another example is the researcher who wants to study the buying behaviour of a firm s business customers. Depending on the purpose of the study, ’ this might be framed in several ways. For example, the study may focus on what the business firms are buying, or on how the firms business buy. This may include mapping how buying processes are triggered, events taking place and how they are related in the buying processes. 2. A model may explain phenomena. The model of ‘pro fit planning’ fi used earlier may be used for explanation purposes. In this model, pro t is explained by the difference between unit price and variable cost per unit multiplied by quantity sold minus fixed costs. Suppose a researcher wants to explain why some fi firms succeed while others fail in an industry. This requires a de nition of what is meant by fi success. Moreover, it requires an identi cation of the factors and processes that may produce success and failure. 4 fi This relates to different contexts of research, i.e. the‘context of justi cation ’ and ‘context of discovery’ respectively. See Popper (1961) for a lucid discussion. 5 t St , St Comparisons of structural description(s) at different points in time (), 1 2 ... can be seen as a special case of comparative static analysis as frequently used in economics. Changes are inferred by comparing the static descriptions from different points in time. 55 56 Research Problems Lave and March (1993) have proposed the following approach to mapping (model) problems: • Observe some facts (e.g. success or failure offirms). • Look at the facts as if they were the end results of some unknown process (model). Then speculate about the processes that might have produced such a result. deduce other Then • results (implications/consequences/predictions) from the model. Then ask yourself whether these other implications are true and produce • new models if necessary. The main advice from the recommended approach is: • Think ‘process’. • Develop interesting implications. • Look for generality. When working with problems and models it is useful to keep them as simple as possible. More variables should only be included when it is useful, that is, to improve the quality of the explanation. A good model– or, more correctly, its implications – should be testable . For example, a football team has just lost a game. The coach explains the loss as follows:‘I ’m sorry. The problem was that our players didn’t have enough fighting spirit, enough will to win.’ The coach’s (implicit) model can be depicted as follows: f fi Outcome (win/loss) = ( ghting spirit) This model is falsi 3. circular as it explains any outcome. The model can never be fied as it cannot be tested. A model may be used to predict an outcome.Many business studies are preoccupied with prediction/forecasting, for example forecasting of sales, prices, and so on. In their simplest form, such predictions are based on extrapolation of past behaviour (development). For example, the following model has been developed to predict population size in a given area: Pti = Pt 0 ± 1.007 ti ² t ( 0) where: Pti = population size at time ti Pt = population size at a specific point in time, e.g. 1 January 2005 0 Closer inspection of the model shows that it contains only one variable, ti t i.e. time ( – 0). Both Pt 0 and 1.007 are constants. 4.5 The Role of Reviewing Past Literature When calculating different values for (ti – t0), 0, 1, 2, . . . ,n , a smoothly growing curve emerges. Is this a good or a bad model? It only maps a speci fic pattern (depending on the size of the constants), unable to capture the impact of sudden changes, for example unemployment due to emigration or people flocking to the area due to new business opportunities. either a descriptive or an explanatory model must be complemented with arule of choice. For example: 4. • • • A model may be used to guide business decisions: Drop product if (price ² variable cost) < amount k. Drop product if (price ² variable cost) ± quantity < contribution c. After describing market size ² enter market if total sales greater than $X. EXAMPLE 4.2 fl A study shows that students’ satisfaction with a programme is in uenced by: (1) the attention given by the instructor, (2) whether they feel the grading is fi fair, and (3) exam score. The study also reveals that the students have difculties in research understanding how their performance is evaluated. Here the findings indicate that an important task is to clarify and make explicit the evaluation criteria used, and explain how these criteria are applied. 4.5 The Role of Reviewing Past Literature When a business student is conducting a research project as a part of their degree, they are supposed to use ‘relevant theory’, that is they must demonstrate that they can apply relevant parts of the knowledge that they were exposed to in the actual programme. There is nothing‘unpractical’ in this, as all qualified research builds on prior knowledge. To most business students, nagging questions are what to include and how should the literature be reviewed? In order to answer such questions, it should be noted that above we have emphasized the word ‘relevant ’. This means that what is included should be of importance for the study. For researchers using quantitative methods, existing literature has a more specific use as it allows them to discover gaps in understanding. It also helps them to develop theoretical and conceptual frameworks and models and to identify important variables and test the relationships between them. For qualitative studies, quite often 57 58 Research Problems the researcher wants to discover relevant variables and relationships between them and to put together these variables in a new way to enhance our understanding. In this case, the researcher needs to develop the theoretical framework that evolves during the research itself. As the research evolves, relevant elements from previous studies need to be incorporated (Strauss and Corbin, 1990: 51). Moreover, the following should be taken into account: 1. purposes of The prime • • • the literature review are to: frame the problem under scrutiny; identify relevant concepts, methods/techniques and facts; position the study. (Any study should add something ‘new ’.) A useful strategy at the initial stage of the project is for researchers to expose themselves to a variety of sources dealing with the topic, for example by reading journal articles and textbooks, and having discussions with‘experts ’ (such as a supervisor or people from the industry). It should be noted that broad exposure to information and ‘incubation’ are considered important in most creative techniques. Hint: To get quick insights, start with recent state-of-the-art or up-to-date reviews done by others, which are available in most disciplines. Then, after this exposure to information, they should start active questioning to frame the problem. The more precise the problem statement, the better direction for the research activities to follow. This will also help clarify what the 2. the study is supposed to be. Based on the activities in (1) above, the researcher should be able to state the 3. intended contribution of criteria for inclusion in the written literature review. Based on (2), a systematic search for relevant contributions, e.g. by using computerized library services, can be undertaken, and the various sources gathered to supplement the initial search (1). 4. In writing up the literature review, conscious decisions on what to emphasize should be made. If the focus is on mapping the problem, the emphasis may be on prior conceptualizations. If, however, the main focus is on how the variables should be measured, the emphasis should be mainly on measurement procedures used in prior studies. This implies that the literature inspected need the written literature review. What to not necessarily be include should be included in determined by the problem and the criteria used. 5. A literature review should also include evaluation and critique of the sources reviewed. To evaluate and critique, some criteria are needed. 4.5 The Role of Reviewing Past Literature Such criteria can be thought of as ‘ideals ’ with which to compare past research, and they must be found or constructed. For example, a researcher is interested in studying entry modes in foreign markets. The researcher observes that the great majority of past studies have been conducted in Western countries, thus identifying the criterion‘global’ or ‘all countries’. Based on such an evaluation and critique, the researcher ’s own choice of conceptualization and later research design should be argued for. EXAMPLE 4.3 A researcher has observed that the propensity to invest in foreign markets varies across industries. Many theories and perspectives have been applied in the study of foreign direct investments. In this study s/he is particularly interested in whether conditions to perform transactions may vary across industries when they are operating internationally. This gives directions for reviewing theory of transaction cost economics, and empirical studies of fl transaction costs (and in uencing factors) in various industries. NB: The search for and review of literature takes time, not least because it is demanding and time consuming to get hold of the research problem. and thinking often produce fruitful results. Here are some good sources to help youfi nd relevant literature: Therefore, try to get a head start! Reading • state-of-the-art articles/reviews; • databases (the Internet); • conference proceedings/journals; • conference participation/networking; • ‘experts ’; • computer search using keywords – this can be done effectively on the library website of your school or university. 59 60 Research Problems Further Reading ........................................................................ Churchill, G.A. and Iacobucci, D. (2015) Marketing Research: Methodological Foundations , 11th edn., Mason, TN: Earlie Lite Books (Chapter 3). Lilien, G.L., Kotler, P., and Moorthy, K.S. (1992)Marketing Models, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. McCaslin, M.L. and Scott, K.W. (2003) ‘The five question method for framing qualitative research study’, The Qualitative Report, 8(3): 447– 61. Questions ........................................................................ 1. What are the roles of ideas and speculation in research? 2. What are the key characteristics of models? 3. Explain the differences between descriptive and explanatory models. 4. Are decisions in business based on models? Give an example. 5. Why do researchers review existing literature when conducting research? Exercises ........................................................................ 1. You are in charge of a financial institution offering credit. Naturally, you are interested in limiting losses for your institution. Develop a model that can guide your decisions as to whether or not applicants should be allowed credit. 2. You are employed by a firm selling your product directly to private households. It is costly to visit private homes. You have also observed that some households buy at the first visit, some at the second one, and that some households never buy in spite of several visits to their homes. Suggest a model to determine number of visits. Specify your assumptions. 5 Research Design Research designs are master techniques . . . K o r n h a u s e r a n d L a z a r s f e l d (1 9 5 5 : 3 9 6 ) , r e p r i n t e d with permission of The Free Press The research design is the overall plan for relating the conceptual research problem to relevant and practicable empirical research. In other words, the research design provides a plan or a framework for data collection and analysis. It reveals the type of research (e.g. exploratory, descriptive, or causal) and the priorities of the researcher. The research methods, on the other hand, refer to the techniques used to collect and analyse data. 5.1 The Design Problem Empirical research is conducted to answer or elucidate research questions. Poorly formulated research questions will lead to misguided research design. Some advocate an open approach, with no research questions. This is, however, a very risky approach (Bryman and Bell, 2003; Hammersley and Atkinson, 1995). Strategic choice of research design should come up with an approach that allows for solving the research problem in the best possible way – within the given constraints. In other words, a research design should be effective in producing the wanted information within the constraints put on the researcher, for example time, budgetary, and skill constraints. This last point is important, even though too frequently overlooked. In business, research results must often be produced within strict time constraints, limiting the number of possible research options. What is more, the business student usually has a limited amount of time to produce a research report for her or his degree. In most cases, the amount of money available for doing the research is also limited, and the student’s research competence and experience (usually) have their limitations as well. Choice of research design can be conceived of as the overall strategy to fl get the information wanted. This choice in uences the subsequent research 62 Research Design activities, for example what data to collect and how they should be collected. The respected social scientists Arthur Kornhauser and Paul Lazarsfeld once claimed that research design played the role of ‘master techniques’, while statistical analysis of the data collected was termed ‘servant techniques’ (Kornhauser and Lazarsfeld, 1955). Design errors occur too often, often through neglect of the design problem. The typical approach: ‘Let ’s prepare a questionnaire and get some data ’ easily ends up with ‘a bunch of data’, which – after time has run out and the money has been used – leaves the researcher (student) with ‘a bunch of data searching for a problem’ and a research design. Other common mistakes are making wrong or irrelevant design choices, for example by examining a badly understood problem with a very structured design or, as seems even more common as‘qualitative methods’ have become increasingly popular, by examining structured, well-understood problems fi by ‘unstructured ’ methods, making it dif cult to solve the research problem adequately. The importance of the problem–research design relationship is discussed below. EXAMPLE 5.1 Firms are often interested to know what consumers think about their product or service offerings. The research challenge is to design a study that allows access to such information. One way to proceed is to ask consumers directly. However, consumers may not hold explicit views about a firm’s products. Thus a study that gradually uncovers whether the consumers know the products at all and whether they have tried or used the product or service, will probably be preferable. 5.2 Problem Structure and Research Design fi Research problems are in nite, and they come in many forms. Consider the following examples: 1. A political party wants to conduct a poll to examine its share of voters. This is a structured problem. The political party knows what information is wanted, that is, the fraction (or percentage) of voters. 2. An advertising company has produced two sets of copy and wants to know which is the most effective in an advertising campaign. Again, the 5.2 Problem Structure and Research Design research problem is structured. The company wants to know which (if > either) advertisement copy (A or B) is the better, that is whether A B > B, A, or A = B. Moreover, in this case the advertisement is seen as a ‘cause’ that may produce some effect (e.g. awareness, interest, or sales). 3. Company X’s sales have dropped in the last three months. The management does not know why. In this case, the management has made an observation, that the sales are dropping. The management does not know what has caused the decline in sales. This is a more unstructured problem. The above examples show (among other things) that problems may vary in structure. Based on problem structure, we may distinguish between the three main classes of research design: 5.2.1 Research design Problem structure Exploratory Unstructured Descriptive Structured Causal Structured Exploratory Research When the research problem is badly understood, a (more or less) exploratory research design is adequate. An example will illustrate this. Consider your favourite Friday-night TV detective series. Most such stories start with a phone call leading the detective to a dead person, apparently murdered. The problem the detective is confronted with is: Who did it, who is the guilty person (if any)? How should the detective proceed? He (or she) collects data and tries to find a lead. As new information comes up, the picture becomes clearer, and at the end, the detective has found the answers. A key characteristic of the detective’s approach to solving the problem is its fl exibility. As new pieces of information are available, the search for the solution may change direction. But there is more to this: • no Research problems may be more or less understood. There is reason not to use available a priori information. As the detective does, so too may the researcher have ‘suspects’. This is often the case in medical research, where potential causes are examined in a experimental laboratory setting. (This indicates that even experiments can be used in exploratory research: see Section 5.4.) • As with other types of research, exploratory research should be conducted in the best possible way. 63 64 Research Design Exploratory research requires skills, as do all types of research, but the • skill requirements differ. Key skill requirements in exploratory research are often the ability to observe, collect information, and construct explanation, that is theorizing. 5.2.2 Descriptive Research In descriptive research the problem is structured and well understood. Consider the case where a firm wants to look at the ‘size of market M’. The first fi problem as such is clear. What is needed is a classi cation of what is meant by ‘market ’. Is it the number of people – the actual and potential fi fi fi buyers of a speci c product group within a speci c area, within a speci ed time period? Assume agreement on the latter interpretation, that is actual fi fi and potential buyers of a speci c product group (e.g. X) within a speci ed time period (say one year). The researcher ’s task now is to produce this information. What would be the best research strategy? Assume that there is no relevant data publicly available. The researcher plans to collect the data by a survey using personal interviews. A detailed plan must be made with regard to how many and who to interview, that is, a sampling plan. The researcher must also construct questions, that is measurements, to get information about purchase (or use) of the product. Good measurements are crucial in research. Then procedures must be developed for how the interviews should be conducted, questions reported, and so on. All interviews should be conducted in the same way, meaning that the variation in the data collection should be as small as possible. Thus key characteristics of descriptive research are structure, precise rules, and procedures. A good example is the procedure used by medical doctors when examining a person’s height. The person has to take his shoes off, stretch his legs, and look all straight ahead. The same procedure is used for persons measured. Descriptive studies may include more than one variable. For example, for some reason the researcher wants to describe smokers by social class. Again, the researcher is confronted with conceptual and fi de nitional problems. When solved, procedures on how to collect the data must be determined to produce the data needed to answer the research question. In this case, the task can be conceived of as completing the cross-table in Table 5.1. (Preparation and analysis of cross-tables are dealt with in Section 11.4.) 5.2.3 Causal Research In causal research, the problems under scrutiny are also structured. However, in contrast to descriptive research, the researcher is confronted with‘causeand-effect ’ problems, as illustrated in the advertising example earlier. The 5.2 Problem Structure and Research Design Table 5.1 Cross-table Social Class Yes No Total n= Smoke I II III IV Total 100% ( ) 100% ( ) 100% ( ) 100% ( ) 100% ( ) main tasks in such research are to isolate the cause(s), and assess whether and to what extent these cause(s) result(s) in effect(s). Examples of questions in causal research are: Is the medical drug effective? What dose is the most effective? Does the advertising help in achieving greater market share? Such problems are discussed more fully in the following sections. ‘ ’ • • • EXAMPLE 5.2 A sample of middle-aged people is randomly assigned to four weight- reducing programmes: (1) diet, (2) exercise, (3) education about nutrition, or (4) control group (the group not following any of the programmes). The people were weighed on 1 February 2019 and again The findings five months later. show: Groups Diet Exercise Education Control À 4.1 À 6.1 À 1.5 Weight change À5.2 Standard deviation (2.3) (1.5) (3.5) (1.2) n = (30) (30) (30) (30) kg kg kg kg The data report average weight losses, standard deviations, and number of participants in each group. The data show that all groups diet, exercise, and education – on average groups lost – more have lost weight, but the than the control group. Here diet, exercise, and education are seen as potential causes of weight loss. 65 66 Research Design The Classic Experiment 5.3 The ‘classic’ experiment is a useful starting point for thinking about research design, even though – as will be discussed below – it is little used in business studies research. In the simplest form of the classic experiment, the researcher randomly assigns subjects to one of two groups: an experimental group or a control group. The researcher then gives a ‘treatment’ to the experimental group but not to the control group. Subsequently s/he measures some attribute of each of the subjects, and compares the post-treatment mean value for the experimental group with that of the control group: • If the former is larger than the latter, then the researcher may conclude that the treatment has had a positive impact. • If the former is smaller than the latter, then the researcher may conclude that the treatment has had a negative impact. • If the two means are similar, then the researcher may conclude that the treatment has had little or no impact. One possible complication is that the subjects in each group may have different values of the attribute being measured even before the treatment is administered, hence a more sophisticated design would measure the values of the attribute both before and after the treatment. The researcher would then compare the changes in the mean values of the attribute for the experimental group and for the control group – see Example 5.3. EXAMPLE 5.3 In a medical experiment, the subjects in the experimental group were given a new drug to reduce cholesterol, while those in the control group were given a placebo. The cholesterol levels of both groups of subjects were measured (a) before the drugs were administered, and (b) after one month. The results are shown below (lower values indicate better cholesterol levels, as measured in millimoles per litre). Group Mean cholesterol level (mmol/L) Pre-treatment Post-treatment Change Experimental 5.25 5.10 À 0.15 Control 5.12 5.08 À 0.04 5.4 Validity Threats EXAMPLE 5.3 (CONT.) It is apparent that the post-treatment mean cholesterol level in the control group (5.08) is marginally lower than that in the experimental group (5.10). But this comparison is not helpful because the pre-treatment mean cholesterol levels in the two groups were markedly different (5.12 and 5.25). It is more useful to compare the changes in the cholesterol levels between the two groups: this shows that the subjects taking the new drug experienced a mean drop in cholesterol of 0.15 mmol/L, while those taking the placebo only experienced a mean drop of 0.02 mmol/L. The intuition underpinning the design of the classic experiment is straightforward: the researcher is able to manipulate the treatment, and then observes the effects upon the attribute. In Example 5.3, the treatment takes one of just two values (drug or placebo), but it would also be possible to consider various dosages of the drug (e.g. zero, low, medium, high, very high). The drug dosage is referred to as the explanatory variable, while the measured cholesterol level would be termed the dependent variable. We noted above that ‘classic ’ experiments are little used in business studies research. There are three reasons for this. First, the‘subjects’ are sometimes firms, people, but are more often may be recruited ex ante organizations, or even countries. People for experiments, but it is generally not feasible to firms etc. Researchers thus typically have to rely onex post data. Second, fi rms etc. typically have quite different characteristics (e.g. size, age, industry, location, internationalization), hence it is extremely dif ficult to do that with construct matched samples for use as experimental and control groups. Third, most dependent variables in business research are affected by many possible explanatory variables, which the researcher can observe but cannot manipulate. It is thus impossible to assess directly from effects on variables, the as Fortunately, dependent typically these all variable of variables problems may changes will be be in individual changing overcome by ex post at the explanatory the use data the same of time. statistical techniques such as multiple regression analysis – see Chapter 11. 5.4 Validity Threats A key purpose of the experimental design is to isolate and estimate the effect(s) of potential cause(s) (see Section 5.3). The experiment is a‘strong’ design as it 67 68 Research Design allows for manipulation of treatment (cause) before and after measurements fi and thus for identi cation of covariation between treatment (cause) and effect, fi determines time order (cause precedes effect), and offers some con dence in ruling out the effect of other explanations (randomization). The idea of experimental design is useful in many studies. Some examples are pre-test of alternative advertisement copy, studying the effectiveness of various selling strategies, and field tests of marketing programmes. The researcher wants to obtain valid knowledge (see Section 4.2), that is, wants results that are ‘true’. For example, if a study shows that advertisement fi A is more effective than advertisement B, the researcher should be con dent 1 that this is the case. There are many types of validity. In the above adver- internal validity, the question tisement case, the question of validity refers to of whether the results obtained within the fi study are true. In other words, for internal validity we have to be con dent that the causal variation among x is really causing the variations variables suggested by our study is true, that y, at least as one of the influencing factors (Bryman and Bell, 2003). On the external validity refers to the question of whether thefindings can be generalized, for example to other populations, settings, or periods, in other hand, beyond the study at hand. This becomes extremely important in quantitative research. The sampling procedures in this type of research thus become very important, to ensure that the researcher has a representative sample (for a good sample, see e.g. Scase and Goffee, 1989), as only in the case of a representative sample can he or she claim the generalizability of the results. There are several threats to validity (for further discussions see Campbell, 1975 and Cook and Campbell, 1979). 1. fic events external to the study (experiment) that occur at History, speci the same time and which may affect the response (criterion variable). EXAMPLE 5.4 Consider a TV store that reduces prices by 10 per cent and observes a sales increase of 20 per cent. A potential external threat is the announcement of a price increase for TV sets the following month. Note that the experiment (see Figure 5.1), by including one (or more) control group(s), allows for controlling the impact of such effects. 1 For an excellent overview, see Cook and Campbell (1979), Chapter 2. 5.4 Validity Threats Experimental group Pre-test Post-test Difference Figure 5.1 2. Control group R R O1 O3 X X O2 O4 O 2 - O1 O4 - O3 The classic experiment Maturation, processes which are operating within the test units in the study as a function of the passage of time per se. For example, the patient has received a medical drug and recovers. Often patients recover without such treatment. Thus what is the cause of the patient ’s recovery, the medical drug or their immune system? Maturation is a serious threat to validity in many studies. EXAMPLE 5.5 A company recognizes the need for reorganization to enhance performance and survive. It succeeds in doing this. Is the success due to the reorganization per se, or might it be explained by the fact that the reorganization has made the employees aware of the serious situation and motivated them to perform better in order to keep their jobs? 3. Test effect indicates that the experiment itself may affect the observed fi response. For example, a group of employees is chosen for a speci c programme and obtains superior results after the programme period. Is their performance caused by the programme or by the fact that they are the chosen ones and thus motivated to perform? Test effects have been found to be a serious threat to validity, for example in the recognized‘Hawthorne studies’ (see Example 5.6), aimed at explaining the impact of various work conditions. 4. the Selection subjects bias (self-selection) are illustrates this. not (or cannot is be) a serious assigned threat to randomly. validity when Example 5.7 69 70 Research Design EXAMPLE 5.6 During the 1920s and 1930s a number of investigations were carried out in the Hawthorne factory of Western Electric Company in the USA. Some of the workers doing manual tasks were taken away and asked to work in a separate room. A number of changes were introduced (e.g. in heating, fl lighting) to see whether these would in uence workers’ productivity. As the lighting was increased day by day the workers were asked whether they liked it or not. The workers said that they did. Then the investigators changed the lights, but put back the same size bulbs, thus in reality not changing the lighting. The workers perceived that the lighting had been improved and replied favourably. After some days, the investigators started to decrease the light day by day, letting the workers know about the change and asking for their comments. After some further days, the investigators repeated the earlier experience of changing the bulbs but not changing the intensity of light/illumination. Again the workers complained that they did not like the lesser light. As the experiment went on, it was realized that productivity did increase, but irrespective of the changes introduced. It was eventually recognized that the workers were reacting positively to the attention and special treatment accorded to them. The investigators thus concluded that the changes in productivity were not due to changing working condition (increase or decrease in light) but due to favourable circumstances/perceptions created by the experiment itself. These fi ndings contributed to ‘stimulating the human relation’ approach and emphasized how important it is to provide ‘psycho-socio’ support at the workplace. Source: Based on Roethlisberger and Dickson (1939: 17); Van Maanen and Kolb (1985: 6) EXAMPLE 5.7 In assessing advertisement effects, the following procedure has frequently been applied (Colley, 1961). Assume the producer of the shampoo‘Z’ wants to know whether its advertising campaign has been effective. A random sample of people are asked the following questions: Q1. ‘Have you seen any advertisements for shampoos during . . . ’? ‘For what shampoos?’ 5.5 Other Research Designs EXAMPLE 5.7 Q2. (CONT.) ‘Have you bought shampoo during . . . ? ’ If yes, ‘ What brands?’ From the results shown in Table 5.2 it could be argued that 20 per cent of those who have seen the advertisement bought the product, while only 5 per cent of those who did not see the advertisement bought it. Thus the advertisement has ‘contributed’ (20 Is the observed À 5) = 15 per cent. finding valid? It may be, but the result may equally well be explained by other factors, such as selective perception, that is, people who regularly buy brand ‘Z’ are more inclined to see the advertisement for‘Z’. Table 5.2 Reading of advertisement and purchase Seen advertisement for Z Yes (%) No (%) ‘ ’ Bought Z ‘ ’ Yes 20 5 25 No 80 95 175 100 100 200 Total 5.5 Total (%) Other Research Designs The simplest research design involves a single unit of observation (individual, firm, country) at one point in time. Such a one-shot case study may be useful in exploratory research of a new phenomenon, but it suffers from two main weaknesses. First, the focus on a single unit of observation raises questions about the generalizability of any research findings to a wider population. Second, the focus on a single point in time does not allow – for in-depth consideration of cause effect relationships. So what are the alternatives? 5.5.1 Cross-Section Data One possibility is to collect information on many units of observation. This might involve the collection of qualitative information from several case studies, so that the researcher is able to compare and contrast the experiences of the different units and to identify patterns. Or quantitative data on a 71 72 Research Design selection of variables may be collated from a sample for observational units. Table 5.3 depicts such a cross-section design where 2018 data have been collated on five variables (X1, X2, X3, X4, and X5) for a sample of 100firms. The main advantage of a cross-section design is that any researchfindings apply not just to a single observational unit but to a larger sample and may be generalizable to a wider population. The main disadvantage is that the data apply to just one point in time (2018) so it is not possible to track the changes in each variable over time, or the effects of such changes on other variables. 5.5.2 Time-Series Data A second possibility is to collect information on one unit of observation but over a number of time periods (e.g. years). This allows changes to be tracked over time, and possible estimation of cause– effect relationships. Table 5.4 depicts such a time-series design where data have been collated on thefive variables (X1, X2, X3, X4, and X5) for firm #53 over the 20-year period 2000–2019. The researcher might have hypothesized that changes in X1 (the dependent variable) are caused by changes in the other four variables (i.e. X2, X3, X4, and X5), and that these changes take place over time. The time-series design Table 5.3 A cross-sectional design Firm Variable (2018) X1 X2 X3 X4 X5 1 54.3 3.6 234.8 76.4 23.9 2 46.8 3.4 145.7 75.3 45.8 100 63.6 4.5 122.9 78.3 52.3 X4 X5 … Table 5.4 A time-series design Year Variable (Firm #53) X1 X2 X3 2000 45.8 4.6 212.0 80.1 45.2 2001 47.3 4.4 193.6 87.3 45.1 2019 65.4 5.8 176.5 115.3 22.5 … 5.5 Other Research Designs allows these hypotheses to be tested. The main disadvantage of a time-series design is that only one firm (#53) is being considered. Is firm #53 representative of the other 100 firms, or is it a special case? 5.5.3 Longitudinal Studies The third possibility is to collect information on several units of observation over a number of time periods (e.g. years). This is generally referred to as a longitudinal design if the information is primarily qualitative and the number of observational units is fairly small. Lorenzoni and Lipparini (1999) provide an excellent example of a longitudinal study. They were interested in how inter-firm networks with suppliers influence lead firms’ knowledge access and transfer. They focused on the networks of three Italian packaging machine manufacturing firms in Bologna, and looked at structural changes in the inter-firm networks of these firms between two points in time (1988 and 1995). More specifically, they were interested in how various activities and competences were distributed over firm boundaries, why network evolution occurred, and how leveraging occurred. They concluded that lead firms can potentially lower overall production and coordination costs through multiple, repeated, trust-based relationships with key suppliers, and that such relationships may have positive effects on the networks as a whole. The main advantages of longitudinal designs are that they allow the researcher to track processes, to identify time-ordered antecedents and consequences, and to distinguish between short-term and long-term phenomena. The main disadvantages are that longitudinal studies take a long period of time to gather results and are labour-intensive (hence the number of observational units may be small), participants may drop out (subject attrition), time effects may be confounded with time-of-measurement (period) effects, and it may be difficult to pick up causal relationships (because there is no manipulation of explanatory variables). 5.5.4 Panel Data The final possibility is to collect primarily quantitative data on many units of observation over a number of time periods (e.g. years).2 The use of panel data has expanded in recent years with the improved availability of large datasets and developments in estimation methodology. Panel data blend the inter-individual differences (revealed in cross-section data) and the intraindividual dynamics (revealed in time-series data). The main advantages (Gujarati and Porter, 2009) are that panel designs give the researcher greater 2 Panel data often involve two dimensions, as here (a cross-section dimension and a time-series dimension), but additional dimensions (e.g. a country dimension) may also be added. 73 74 Research Design Table 5.5 A panel design Firm Year 1 Variable X1 X2 X3 X4 X5 2000 33.3 3.4 276.5 65.3 44.4 2001 35.4 3.4 266.9 64.2 44.5 2019 54.3 3.6 234.8 76.4 23.9 2000 21.7 2.5 165.3 60.2 87.3 2001 24.3 2.8 165.2 61.7 85.4 2019 46.8 3.4 145.7 75.3 45.8 2000 55.4 3.4 154.2 58.4 76.7 2001 53.2 3.5 155.3 59.8 75.3 2019 63.6 4.5 122.9 78.3 52.3 human behaviour ... 2 ... … 100 capacity section for or capturing time-series the data, complexity and also of involve larger datasets than cross- (which has fits in terms of statistical hypothesis testing– see Chapter 10). Table 5.5 depicts such a panel design where data have been collated onfi ve variables (X1, X2, X3, X4, and X5) for 100fi rms over the 20-year period 2000–2019. The panel is balanced as each of the 100fi rms has the same number (20) of bene time-series observations, but panels may be unbalanced if data are missing. The main disadvantages with panel designs are that they require much more data collection, and the methodologies for the analysis of the data are more complicated 5.6 – see Gujarati and Porter (2009) for more details. Requirements in Research Design When moving from the research problem at the conceptual level to empirical research, questions arise such as: how to proceed? and how to do it? As noted at the outset of this chapter, the research design represents the overall strategy for gathering the information needed to answer the research problem under scrutiny. 5.6 Requirements in Research Design After thinking through what the research problem is, and if possible how it should be represented (see Section 5.2), and the potential hypotheses derived (if any), the first question to answer is: What requirements should the actual research design satisfy? A few examples will illustrate this point. EXAMPLE 5.8 A study was conducted to explore whether fi rms (managers) within the same industry perceive and interpret their surrounding environments differ- fl ently and, if so, whether this might in uence organizational actions and performance (Grønhaug and Haukedal, 1989). A case study was chosen for the research purpose. The following for the selection of the cases, that • • • criteria were, however, established firms should: belong to the same industry, and be embedded in similar environments; be of approximately the same size; be in an industry where it was possible to identify major environmental change(s); • • at the outset have approximately the same economic resources; and have demonstrated different response(s) to the environmental change(s). The above requirements also demonstrate that even in‘qualitative’ (case) research a priori theorizing can be useful in structuring the research problem (see Chapter 4). Assume a study involving a set of hypotheses, for example: H1: The higher the consumer’ s knowledge about her/his rights, the more likely it is that s/he will complain. Inspection of the above hypothesis shows that two constructs (variables) are included: (1) knowledge about rights and (2) propensity to complain. The first requirement to test this hypothesis is information on these two variables. Moreover, variation in knowledge and propensity is needed. Reading the hypothesis also shows that nothing is said about causation; only covariation between the two constructs (variables) is indicated. Thus a cross-sectional (correlational) design is appropriate. Consider the following hypothesis: H2: An increase in advertising expenditure will lead to an increase in the probability of getting more orders. Again, two variables are present: (1) advertising expenditure and (2) probability of getting orders. Information on these two variables is needed. The 75 76 Research Design hypothesis also expresses a causal relationship, ‘will increase’. In order to demonstrate causality, a design taking the time order between change (increase) in advertising expenditure and change in probability of getting orders is needed. 5.6.1 Research and Choices Research involves choices, problematic choices. Once the design require- fi ments have been speci ed, decisions must be made on how they should be met, and how the information needed should be collected. Important decisions in this respect are: • How should the concepts (variables) be measured (operationalized)? • What type of data are needed? Secondary or primary? • If secondary: what secondary data sources are available? • If primary: how should the data be gathered? Through observation or 3 interviewing? • If interviewing: personal interviews, by phone or face to face? • If interviewing: how should the questions be formulated; structured or unstructured? • Who should be interviewed? How should they be selected (sampling plan)? How many should be included (sample size)? This list of questions is in no way complete, but clearly indicates that research involves choices. Quality research implies conscious, reasonable choices, and the needed skills in performing the activities involved. Several of these questions will be dealt with in the following chapters. 3 fi Primary data refers to data/information that has been collected by the researcher speci cally for the research at hand. Secondary data refers to data/information that has been collected by others for a different purpose, but which the researcher can use for her/his research. The pros and cons of primary and secondary data are discussed in more depth in Chapter 9. Exercises Further Reading ................................................................. Altheide, D.L. and Johnson, J.M. (1998) ‘Criteria for assessing interpretive validity in qualitative research’, in Denzin, N. and Lincoln, Y.S. (eds.), Interpreting Qualitative Materials, Collecting and London: Sage, pp. 283–312. Ghauri, P.N. and Firth, R. (2009) ‘ The formalization of case study research in international business’ , Der Markt, 48(1): 29– 40. Isaeva, N., Bachmann, R., Bristow, A., and Saunders, M.N. (2015)‘ Why the epistemologies of trust researchers matter’, Journal of Trust Research, 5(2): 153–69. Strauss, A. and Corbin, J. (1994) ‘Grounded theory methodology: an overview’, in Denzin, N.K. and Lincoln, Y.S. (eds.), Handbook of Qualitative Research, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 273–85. Questions ................................................................. 1. Describe the key elements of the classic experimental design. 2. What do you consider to be the basic uses of exploratory research? 3. Explain what is meant by: (1) dependent and (2) independent variables. 4. What is meant by: (1) validity and (2) validity threat? 5. How would you decide whether a factor can be considered to be a ‘cause’ of something? Exercises ................................................................. 1. An advertising agency has developed two advertisements to be used in a magazine. It wants to select the most effective before the ad is printed. Design a study allowing the agency to select the most effective one. 2. An internet bank is concerned with improving its services. In particular, fi the bank wants to know if its customers are dissatis ed with current services and the nature of this dissatisfaction. How would you design a study allowing the bank the desired insight? 3. A researcher has observed that some firms in the furniture industry outsource activities such as transportation services while others do not. S/he wants to study: (2) whether outsourcing firms outsource/do not outsource and infl uences fi rms performance. Suggest a study (1) why ’ to enquire into these research questions. 77 6 Measurements The GIGO principle: garbage in, garbage out. Empirical research requires the collection and analysis of data and other information. The quality of the research (and the conclusions derived therefrom) depend upon the collection of appropriate data, the quality of the data collected, and on how well the data are analysed. Quantitative research requires the measurement and enumeration of the variables to be used in the analysis. In this chapter, we will first explain the process of operationalization, by which researchers decide how to measure the theoretical concepts they use. The second section considers different scales of measurement, and highlights some of the implications for empirical analysis. The third section focuses on the measurement of multi-dimensional variables, and the generation of latent constructs. The fourth section addresses how to assess the reliability fifth section and validity of variables and multi-dimensional constructs. The offers some practical suggestions for improving the measurement of the variables used in quantitative research, while the final section is concerned with measurements in qualitative research. 6.1 The Operationalization of Variables In most research problems, we first start with theoretical concepts. These concepts may be straightforward attributes of individuals (e.g. age, gender), firms (e.g. size, industry), countries (e.g. population, living standards), etc. But sometimes the concepts are more complex. Marketers are often interested in customer satisfaction. Many organization scholars ponder the determinants of organizational culture. HR researchers may be concerned about attitudes to work. Those interested in international business debate the merits of firms pursuing international and/or product fi diversi cation strategies. All disciplines make frequent reference to national culture, and to cultural differences between countries. 6.1 The Operationalization of Variables Operationalization involves rules for assigning numbers to the theoretical concepts, and these rules should involve (a) a clear mapping between the theoretical concepts and the corresponding empirical variables; and (b) clear, unambiguous (and ideally objective) definitions of the variables. Sometimes, these mappings and definitions are fairly easy: the age of an individual may be measured in terms of years since birth (or months, or days). But the process is often more problematic. For example: • • • The size of a firm may be assessed in terms of the number of employees (at which point in time?), the value of its assets (again at which point in time?), or its sales/revenues over a given period. Each of these variables is a measure of firm size, but they are not equivalent. What units are being used (numbers of employees; $m; €’000)? The living standards of a country may be assessed in terms of per capita gross domestic product (GDP), per capita gross national income (GNI), or by using broader measures such as the human development index (HDI). All three measures are averages, and may mask important differences between individual citizens. What units are being used ($ or local currency for GDP/GNI; how is the HDI measured)? Customer satisfaction is even more tricky. This is clearly very subjective and also multi-dimensional in that satisfaction may involve considerations of price, functionality, appearance etc. How can data be obtained on customer satisfaction? EXAMPLE 6.1 How could the following theoretical concepts be operationalized and measured? • the gender of an individual? • the industry of a • the extent of a • a country’s national culture? fi rm? fi rm’s fi international diversi cation? EXAMPLE 6.2 The theoretical concept of absorptive capacity (ACAP) refers to the ability of a from firm to identify, assimilate, transform, and exploit knowledge gained external sources (Teece, 2007). Most researchers have typically 79 80 Measurements EXAMPLE 6.2 (CONT.) operationalized absorptive capacity using simple proxy measures such as R&D intensity (R&D expenditure as a % of sales), R&D investments (number of R&D employees), or R&D outputs (number of patents). However, Flatten et al. (2011) queried whether any single measure can capture the complexity of absorptive capacity, and recommended the use of a multi-dimensional construct. They initially identified 53 items which reflected the different shades of meaning of the ACAP concept, but reduced this list to 36 items after three rounds of pre-testing. A questionnaire survey of 285 German executives was then carried out, in which the importance of the various items was assessed using 7-point Likert scales (see Section 9.7.2 for details). Confirmatory factor analysis then established that ACAP did indeed involve four dimensions (i.e. identification; assimilation, transformation, and exploitation), and confirmed that the following 14 items were required to capture these four dimensions adequately: ACAP dimension Questionnaire items Acquisition Please specify to what extent your company uses external Assimilation resources to obtain information: ▪ The search for relevant information concerning our industry is everyday business in our company ▪ Our management motivates the employees to use information sources within our industry ▪ Our management expects that the employees deal with information beyond our industry Please rate to what extent the following statementsfit the communication structure in your company: ▪ In our company ideas and concepts are communicated cross-departmentally ▪ Our management emphasizes cross-departmental support to solve problems ▪ In our company there is a quick informationflow, e.g. if a business unit obtains important information it communicates this information promptly to all other business units or departments ▪ Our management demands periodical crossdepartmental meetings to interchange new developments, problems, and achievements 6.2 Scales of Measurement EXAMPLE 6.2 Transformation (CONT.) fi Please specify to what extent the following statements t the knowledge processing in your company: ▪ Our employees have the ability to structure and to use collected knowledge ▪ Our employees are used to absorb new knowledge as well as to prepare it for further purposes and to make it available ▪ Our employees successfully link existing knowledge with new insights ▪ Our employees are able to apply new knowledge in their practical work Exploitation fit Please specify to what extent the following statements the commercial exploitation of new knowledge in your company: ▪ Our management supports the development of prototypes ▪ Our company regularly reconsiders technologies and adapts them accordant to new knowledge ▪ Our company has the ability to work more effective by adopting new technologies A variety of tests were reported to assess the reliability and validity of these four constructs, including tests for internal consistency and convergent validity. The authors suggest that these questionnaire items and constructs provide managers with a useful tool to assess their fi rms’ strengths and weaknesses with regard to ACAP, and to compare their ACAP with those of their competitors. 6.2 There Scales of Measurement are four measurement scales (or types of data): nominal, ordinal, interval, and ratio. It is important to understand the differences between them because they have implications for the choice of appropriate techniques for statistical analyses (see Chapter 10 for further discussion). 6.2.1 Nominal Level (Scale) fi A nominal scale classi es the subjects into mutually exclusive categories. An example would be nationality (Belgian, German, French, Italian, etc.). 81 82 Measurements Table 6.1 Example of the operationalization of nominal variables Nationality Values of dummy variables DB DG DF Belgian 1 0 0 German 0 1 0 French 0 0 1 Italian 0 0 0 Note: Nationality has j = 4 different categories, hence the variable may be operationalized using (j ± 1) = 3 dummy variables. The values of all three dummy variables (i.e. DB, DG, and DF) are all set to zero for the fourth category (Italian). fi Subjects are classi ed into categories, and these categories are mutually exclusive. Such nominal variables are also called categorical variables, and there is no intrinsic order or ranking to the categories. If there are two possible categories (e.g. gender = male or female), then the variable may be operationalized using a dummy variable (e.g. gender = 1 if female; = 0 if male) that takes one of two possible values. If there are more than two possible categories, then additional dummy variables are required – see Table 6.1. Four nationalities are considered, and three dummy variables are fined: de DB = 1, DG = 0, and DF = 0 for Belgian; DB = 0, DG = 1, and DF = 0 for Germans; DB = 0, DG = 0, and DF = 1 for French; and DB = 0, DG = 0, and DF = 0 for Italians. Each of the four categories thus involves a different combination of the three dummy variables. 6.2.2 Ordinal Level (Scale) An ordinal scale provides numerical ratings for the variable under consider- fi ation: it is the order of the values that is important and signi cant, but the differences between the values do not have a clear interpretation. Variables such as customer satisfaction that have been assessed using Likert scales in a questionnaire survey – see Table 6.2 – are measured on ordinal scales. Clearly, a rating of 3 indicates a higher level of satisfaction than a rating of 2, and a rating of 5 indicates a higher level of satisfaction than a rating of 4. But it is not possible to say how much higher in each case, or whether the difference between the 2 and 3 ratings is greater than, or less than, the difference between the 4 and 5 ratings. 6.2.3 Interval Level (Scale) Interval scales involve continuous variables in which similar differences between the values are equivalent, but the scale does not have a true zero. The oft-cited example is temperature in degrees Celsius. The difference 6.3 Construct Measurement Using Multi-Item Scales Table 6.2 Example of the operationalization of ordinal variables fi Question: how satis ed are you with the product you purchased? fi 1 = very unsatis ed fi 2 = somewhat unsatis ed 3 = neutral fi 4 = somewhat satis ed fi 5 = very satis ed ² ² ² ² ² ² 20 C and 30 C. But is 20 C twice as warm as 10 C? The answer is no, as zero between 10 C and 20 C degrees is 10 degrees, as is the difference between 1 on the Celsius scale is not a true zero where there is no temperature. An alternative example would be measures of intelligence (e.g. IQ). Ratios have no meaning when there is not a true zero: someone with an IQ of 200 is not twice as intelligent as someone with an IQ of 100. 6.2.4 Ratio Scale Ratio scales also involve continuous variables, but the scale does have a natural zero, and hence values can be meaningfully added, subtracted, and firm size. A firm with 60 employees is three firm with 20 employees, it has 40 more employees, and the divided. An example would be times the size of a two firms together employ 80 people. Table 6.3 summarizes these different scales and their characteristics. 6.3 Construct Measurement Using Multi-Item Scales The use of single-item measures has many practical advantages, notably simplicity and ease of data collection. Single-item measures are thus typically favoured when the variable to be operationalized has a clear interpretation (it is narrow in scope, unambiguous, and unidimensional). Many attributes meet these criteria: ‘age’, for instance, may be measured by the fi number of years since birth (individuals) or establishment ( rms). But singleitem scales have often been criticized (notably in the marketing and consumer psychology literatures) for over-simplifying complex constructs. Hence it has become standard practice in many business disciplines to use multi-item scales to measure complex constructs. For example, ‘customer 1 Absolute zero is at ± ² 273.15 C, or zero on the Kelvin scale. 83 84 Measurements Table 6.3 Summary of scales of measurement Features Scale of Measurement Nominal Variables Ordinal categorical Best measure of central tendency ordinal 1 Interval Ratio continuous continuous mode median mean mean no yes yes yes no no yes yes no no no yes no no no yes Data from IQ Age, size 2 Order of values is known Quantifiable differences between values True zero Meaningful ratios between values Typical example Industry fi cation classi questionnaire surveys Notes: 1. Ordinal variables involve numerical values but should not be considered as continuous variables. fi 2. See Section 10.1.2 for de nitions and details of how to calculate means, medians, and modes. fl satisfaction ’ is a complex multi-dimensional construct that is in uenced by many factors. Rather than simply asking customers about their overall satisfaction with a product, it may be preferable to dig deeper and seek views on multiple items such as: fied are you with the price of the product you purchased? satisfied are you with the functionality of the product you • How satis • How purchased? • How fied satis are you with the appearance of the product you purchased? fied are you with the service provided by the sales staff? • How satis • Does your product provide value for money? ‘Customer satisfaction’ may then be measured by combining the responses to these separate items (and maybe other items 2 too) into a single multi- item scale. 2 The combination may be effected simply by summing the responses, or through more sophisticated techniques such as factor analysis – see Chapter 12 for further discussion. 6.4 The Reliability and Validity of Measurements The use of such multi-item scales to measure complex constructs has two main advantages. First, it avoids the need to choose one item from a set of alternative items. Rather than choosing one item rather than another, why not combine them all into an aggregate measure? Second, the use of multiple fi items tends to average out inaccuracies and speci cities that relate to single items, and is thus associated with increased reliability and construct validity. As Churchill and Iacobucci (2015) note, in valid measures, items should have a common core (which increases reliability) but should also contribute some fi unique variance which is not tapped by other items. Each of the ve items listed above is clearly related to the core construct of customer satisfaction, but each is tapping into a different aspect of satisfaction. In combination, the responses to these five items provide a more holistic assessment of customer satisfaction. The use of multi-item scales has been further encouraged by the widespread adoption of structural equation modelling techniques (see Chapter 12 for further details). Diamantopoulos et al. (2012) provide some helpful practical guidelines for researchers in choosing between single-item measures and multi-item scales. 6.4 The Reliability and Validity of Measurements Measurement involves assigning values to variables/constructs. But how do researchers know that these measurements are meaningful? There are two distinct criteria by which researchers evaluate their measures: reliability and 3 construct validity. Reliability refers to the degree to which measurement produces consistent outcomes. A reliable measure is one with the following three attributes: 1. Stability. Suppose the researcher obtains responses to the questions about customer satisfaction this week, and then again next week under similar circumstances from the same respondents. Stability may be – assessed by calculating test retest reliability scores. If the two sets of ratings are similar, then this is an indication of stability over time. If the two sets of ratings are very different, however, this casts doubt on the reliability of both sets. 3 The discussion in this section is primarily concerned with internal validity (i.e. the extent to which the research is properly designed and analysed) rather than external validity (i.e. the extent to which the research findings may be generalized to the real world). Research may – have high internal validity but very low external validity at the same time see Cooper and Schindler (2001) for further discussion. 85 86 Measurements 2. Equivalence . This requires that there is a level of consistency across the fl respondents, so that the measured values are not unduly in uenced by the views of individual respondents. Suppose that the researcher asks fi several experts to assess the growth prospects of a sample of rms. If these assessments are very different, then this casts doubt not just upon the reliability of the responses from each individual but also upon the reliability of the whole exercise as a way of measuring growth prospects. If the assessments are similar, then the researcher will have more con fidence in the overall ratings. Equivalence may be assessed by calculating inter-rater reliability scores. 3. Internal consistency. This requires that the variables used to generate a construct should be measuring similar (though not identical) concepts, and should be highly correlated. In the customer satisfaction example discussed in Section 6.2, it is clear that the various questions cover different dimensions of satisfaction and may be usefully combined to form a customer satisfaction construct. The internal consistency of this ficient construct may then be assessed by calculating Cronbach ’s coef alpha (see Section 12.1.1). The attributes of stability and equivalence are important when the researcher is collecting primary data through experimental studies. Internal consistency is important whenever the researcher has developed multi-item scales, whether using primary or secondary data (see Chapter 9 for further discussion). Reliability is a necessary but not a sufficient condition for validity. A reliable scale may not be valid: my stopwatch may tell me that my walk to work always takes about 15 minutes (and is hence reliable), but it may be slow as the walk takes me 20 minutes (and hence is not valid). Construct validity refers to ‘the extent to which an operationalization measures the concept it purports to measure’ (Zaltman et al, 1977: 44). There are three main aspects of construct validity: 1. Content validity. This refers to the extent to which the construct adequately includes all relevant information. In the example of the measurement of the customer satisfaction construct, do the tions related to satisfaction with price, functionality, five ques- appearance, service, and value for money cover all dimensions of customer satisfaction? Or have important dimensions been omitted, with the result that the construct is a poor measure of the theoretical concept? Content validity may be improved by using (if available) questions that have been used in previous published work, and whose validity has already been established. An additional ex ante check on content validity is to ask a panel of experts for their opinions on whether or not all the 6.5 Improving Your Measurements relevant dimensions have been considered (this is sometimes referred to as face validity). 2. Convergent validity. This refers to the extent to which the construct is correlated with other constructs measuring similar theoretical concepts. Suppose two researchers are both measuring customer satisfaction, but each is using different items in their questionnaires. If the two constructs are highly correlated, then this would be evidence of convergent validity. One way of establishing convergent validity is to carry out robustness tests in empirical studies, using alternative measures of key variables. 3. Divergent validity. This refers to the extent to which the construct is distinguishable from other constructs. Suppose that the researcher wants to assess the factors that affect the choice of country location by US firms investing in the European Union. S/he might hypothesize that both average living standards and average wages in the potential host countries are important determinants: higher living standards mean a more attractive market, while lower wages mean lower production costs. Average living standards might be measured by GDP per capita, but such a measure is likely to be highly correlated with average labour costs. In other words, GDP per capita does not have divergent validity, and alternative measures for both variables will need to be found. 6.5 Improving Your Measurements As noted above, empirical research requires the collection and analysis of data, and the quality of the research depends critically upon the quality of the measurement of the variables and constructs used in the analysis. A necessary prerequisite for good research is thus good measurement. This simple requirement is expressed more pithily in the‘garbage in, garbage out’ quote used at the start of the chapter. So how can measurements be improved? 1. Start by elaborating the theoretical concepts to be used. 2. Consider how each of these theoretical concepts may be operationalized. Here it is useful to consult previously published work because (a) this may provide some useful ideas; (b) it is easier to justify the use of a measure if it has been used in previous studies; and (c) the work may also provide hints to potential sources of data. Be critical in your assessment of this previous work, and do not simply accept measures without question. Consider alternative and possibly better 87 88 Measurements 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. operationalizations of the concepts, and think carefully about the advantages and disadvantages of each option in relation to your research question. For example, if you are researching employmentrelated issues, then the number of employees might be the most appropriate measure of firm size. But if you are researching the determinants of profitability, then a financial measure of firm size such as total revenues might be more appropriate. Are values for the variables readily available in secondary sources? If not, how will the values be generated (e.g. by a questionnaire survey)? Develop the questionnaire instrument, and pre-test it to ensure that the questions are understandable and unambiguous. This is particularly important in the context of cross-cultural studies (see Chapter 13). Can each of the variables be measured directly, or do multi-dimensional constructs need to be calculated? How are the constructs generated? Assess the reliability (stability, equivalence, internal consistency) and validity (content, convergent, divergent) of each of the constructs, and include these assessments in the written report. What scales of measurement are used for each variable/construct? Are they nominal, ordinal, interval, or ratio? In general, it is better to retain as much information as possible in the measures. For example, measure firm size using number of employees (a continuous variable) rather than simply classifying firms as ‘large’ or ‘small’ (a categorical variable). Make a careful note of the units in which your variables are measured, as this will be important in the interpretation of the data and any empirical analyses. For example, is firm size measured by total revenues expressed in $ (e.g. $5 678 123) or in $m (e.g. $5.678123m)? The acid test for any measure is whether it is useful in predicting an outcome that it should be able to predict on the basis of theory, and whether it predicts this outcome better than alternative measures. This is termed predictive validity (Diamantopoulos et al., 2012). 6.6 Measurements in Qualitative Research The research literature deals with measurement problems when it comes to exploratory/qualitative research only to a modest degree. But is the question of measurement irrelevant in such research? The answer is probably no. 6.6 Measurements in Qualitative Research Understanding/ explanation ? ? Conceptual LEVEL Empirical R1, R2, . . . n Figure 6.1 Responses and sense-making 1. In Chapter 5, a key characteristic of exploratory research is that the problem under scrutiny is only partly understood. If the problem is only modestly understood, a prime purpose is to obtain an understanding. Various approaches can be used (see Chapter 8 for more detailed discussion). Assume that the researcher approaches the problem by using semi-structured questions, based on an interview guide (developed by surveying previous studies and so on). As noted above, a key purpose of measurement is to map ‘reality’. When the researcher asks her or his questions s/he gets responses (see Figure 6.1). A detailed example is given in Chapter 8 on qualitative data analysis reporting the study by Burns and Stalker (1961) to capture characteristics of innovative and non-innovative organizations, which they termed ‘organic’ and ‘mechanic’, respectively. The responses, R1 , R2, . . ., are empirical manifestations that the researcher is trying to understand. In this process the researcher will try to relate them to her or his knowledge base, and hopefully produce a reasonable explanation. This may partly be seen as ‘data-driven’ problem solving. However, without use of emerge. Thus concepts a mapping and theory, between an explanation empirical (theory) observations and will never concepts/ theory is taking place. Besides noting that such research requires considerable conceptual skills, which is often overlooked, it should also be noted that the findings. researcher In order 4 supply evidence. should to be handle able such to demonstrate validity claims the the validity researcher and validity claims must infer- have much in common with measurements as discussed above. 4 the He or she should report the questions, responses, infer- ences made, and what supports these inferences. Thus, themapping , ences, of For an excellent discussion, see Kirk and Miller (1986). 89 90 Measurements EXAMPLE 6.3 s Assume a medical doctor is examining a patient with symptoms fi 1, . . . sn . During the examination s/he arrives at a speci c diagnosis and decides on treatment. Has the doctor made some sort of measurement? S/he has observed the symptoms and related them to her or his knowledge base, and thus theory conducted (diagnosis). mapping An expert mistakes constructs valid toms) (relevant and theory between the observer observable who seldom symptoms makes and diagnostic mappings between empirical observations (sympdiagnosis), which corresponds to excellent construct validity. 2. In business studies the researcher often makes use of secondary data. Such data are gathered measurements have by been means used secondary data, one should as of fi speci c well. This always inspect procedures, indicates where that, fi speci c when using and evaluate the data gathering and measurement procedures used. 3. Analysis of written texts, such as annual reports and business maga- zines, and research. taped Even exploratory, and here the transcribed measurement arguments put interviews, problems forward at is are the often used present. outset If of in the this business study is section fi apply. If the research is structured (descriptive or causal), conceptual de n- fi fi itions must be developed, and speci c procedures (operational de nitions) speci fied. The above discussion indicates that measurements are important and must be properly dealt with in qualitative research, where measurement problems have usually been given less attention. Exercises Further Reading ................................................................. Churchill, G.A. and Iacobucci, D. (2015) Marketing Research: Methodological Foundations, 11th edn., Mason, TN: Earlie Lite Books, Inc. (Chapter 7). Ghauri, P.N. (2004) ‘ Designing and conducting case studies in international business research’, in Marshan-Piekkari, R. and Welch, C. (eds.),A Handbook of Qualitative Research Methods for International Business, Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar, pp. 109– 24. Golafshani, N. (2003) ‘Understanding reliability and validity in qualitative research’ , The Qualitative Report, 8(4): 597–606. Questions ................................................................. 5. In Table 6.1, three dummy variables were used to operationalize the nationality variable. Why? Why not just use one variable with the following values: Belgian = 1; German = 2; French = 3; and Italian = 4? 6. Suppose I rank all the 44 students in my class on the basis of their ability, with the best student being rated 1 and the worst student being rated 44. Is this an ordinal scale or an interval scale? 7. What are the main attributes of reliable measures? 8. What is the difference between convergent validity and divergent validity? 9. Give examples of measurements in qualitative research. Exercises ................................................................. 1. Determine whether each of the following variables is measured on a nominal, ordinal, interval, or ratio scale: a. prices on the stock market; b. marital status (married or not married); c. level of education (school, undergraduate degree, postgraduate degree, PhD); personal income; d. e. 2. scores derived from Likert scales. fine De fi each of the following concepts, and develop operational de n- fi itions for the de ned concepts: 3. a. a ‘workaholic’; b. market share; c. product diversi cation. fi How might you measure ‘job satisfaction’ using a questionnaire survey of employees? 91 PART III Implementation .................................................................................. P art III deals with the actual processes of conducting data analysis, examining case – studies, and writing up the report. The chapters 7 ( 14) take you through step by step, providing examples and guidelines. As such, this part can be used as a manual while carrying out the respective activity. This part also provides guidelines for handling cross-cultural research. 7 Data Collection for Qualitative Research Since Hippocrates first presented 14 classic case studies of disease some 2300 years ago, science has proceeded along two divergent knowledge paths. One involves formulating a tentative theory of a phenomenon ‘ writ large’ , deducing implied empirical consequences, and controlling situational events in order to observe the validity of empirical deductions. The second path, less frequently used but equally valid, is to reason from individual and naturally occurring but largely uncontrollable observations towards generalizable inductive principles. B o n o m a (1 9 8 5 : 1 9 9 ) , r e p r i n t e d w i t h p e r m i s s i o n b y t h e American Marketing Association In business studies most researchers need to collect some primary data to answer their research question. Once the researcher has decided to collect information/data through primary sources, s/he has to decide what kind of data collection method to use. As discussed earlier, s/he could do an observation, experiment, interview, or survey. However, the choice of data collection method will depend upon an overall judgement on which type of data are needed for a particular research problem. One important aspect is to identify the unit of analysis. Even in one-country research, where data are firms compared and contrasted, we need to be sure that the consulted are comparable. This is also important for segmentation, new product development, demand forecast, or country analysis for entry strategy studies. fi While de ning the unit of study, several dimensions are important. One is the scope: whether one study will cover a region (e.g. the EU), a country, or a particular area in a city or a country (e.g. north-west England or central fi characteristics of the unit. When we are firms, we should have clear criteria, such as: age, size, revenue, and London). Also, we need to de ne the studying industrial sector. Say we want to study‘Export behaviour of SMEs (small and medium-sized enterprises)’, should we study all firms in that category or only firms? Should we study all firms or only those that have been for at least fi ve years? Moreover, the type of answers we are manufacturing in business looking for will determine which type of analysis we need. At the outset, a researcher has to decide whether s/he wants to use a qualitative or quantitative data collection and analysis method. 96 Data Collection for Qualitative Research Qualitative versus Quantitative Methods 7.1 Alternatives to the standard approach, like unstructured interviewing, tend to be viewed as faulted variants. . . I am arguing, instead, that the standard survey interview is itself essentially faulted and that it therefore cannot serve as the ideal ideological model against which to assess other approaches. (Mishler, 1986: 29) In the literature on research methods there is some discussion on which fic . It is sometimes stated fic and thereby methods or techniques are more suitable or‘scienti ’ that structured and quantitative methods are more ‘scienti ’ better. But in our opinion, methods or techniques are not‘better ’ or ‘scien- fic ti ’ only because they are quantitative. As mentioned earlier, which methods and techniques are most suitable for which research project depends on the research problem and its purpose (Jankowicz, 1991). Several scholars also assert that it depends upon the ontological orientation of the researcher. If the researcher believes in objectivism, that social phenomena and their meaning have an existence that is independent of the researcher, s/he would be inclined to do a positivistic and quantitative type of research. However, in case a researcher believes in constructionism, that social phenomena fi and their meanings are continually being revised and are not de nitive, the researcher will be inclined to use an interpretive and qualitative type of research (Bryman and Bell, 2003). Research methods refer to the systematic, focused, and orderly collection of data for the purpose of obtaining information from them, to solve/answer a particular research problem or question. Methods are different from techniques of data collection. By methods we mean data collection through historical review and analysis, surveys, field experiments, and case studies, while by techniques we mean a step-by-step procedure that we follow in order to gather data and analyse them to find the answers to our research questions. These are concerned more with how to do things than what to do or why to do it. In business studies, we normally use techniques such as structured, semi-structured or unstructured interviews, surveys, and observations (Bennett, 1986; Jankowicz, 1991). The main difference between qualitative and quantitative research is not one of ‘quality’ but of procedure. In qualitative research, arrived at by statistical methods or other procedures findings of are not fi quanti cation. Normally, the basic distinction between quantitative and qualitative research is that quantitative researchers employ measurement and qualitative researchers do not (Bryman and Bell, 2003; Layder, 1993; Sinkovics et al., 2008). The difference between quantitative and qualitative methods and 7.1 Qualitative versus Quantitative Methods fi approaches is not just a question of quanti cation, however, but also a flection of different perspectives on knowledge development and research re objectives. We can do research on behaviour, events, organizational functioning, social environments, interaction, and relationships. In some of these fi studies, data may be quanti ed, but the analysis itself is qualitative, such as with census reports. It is quite common for researchers to collect their data through observations and interviews, which are the methods normally related to qualitative research. But the researcher may code the data collected in such a manner that would allow statistical analysis. In other words, it is quite possible to quantify qualitative data. Qualitative and quantitative methods are therefore not mutually exclusive. The difference is in the overall form and in the emphasis and objectives of the study. These differences in emphasis are illustrated in Table 7.1. Qualitative research is a mixture of the rational, explorative, and intuitive, where the skills and experience of the researcher play an important role in the analysis of data. It is often focused on social process and not on social structures, which is frequently the focus in quantitative research. The skills needed to do qualitative research are thinking abstractly, stepping back and Table 7.1 The difference in emphasis in qualitative versus quantitative methods Qualitative methods Quantitative methods • Emphasis on understanding • Emphasis on testing and veri cation • Focus on understanding from • Focus on facts and reasons for social respondent’ s/informant’ s point of view fi events • Interpretation and rational approach • Logical and critical approach • Observations and measurements in • Controlled measurement natural settings • Subjective ‘ insider view’ and closeness to data • Explorative orientation • Objective ‘outsider view’ distant from data • Hypothetical-deductive; focus on hypothesis testing • Process oriented • Result oriented • Holistic perspective • Particularistic and analytical • Generalization by comparison of • Generalization by population properties and contexts of individual membership organism Source : Based on Reichardt and Cook 1 ( 979). 97 98 Data Collection for Qualitative Research TECHNIQUES TECHNIQUES Qualitative Conversation Structured observation Unstructured and Structured interview semi-structured Structured surveys Attitude scaling interviews, etc. Quantitative Historical Group Case review discussion study Survey Field equipment Experiment METHODS Figure 7.1 Quantitative and qualitative methods and techniques Source : Adapted from Business Research Projects for Students, Thomson Learning (Jankowicz, 1991: 159). analysing situations, recognizing and avoiding biases, obtaining valid and reliable information, having theoretical and social sensitivity, being able to keep analytical distance while utilizing past experience, and having a shrewd sense of observation and interaction (Hagen et al., 2019; Strauss and Corbin, 1990; Van Maanen, 1983). Although most researchers emphasize one or the other, qualitative and quantitative methods can be combined and used in the same study. Many scholars claim that the two approaches are complementary and cannot be used in isolation from each other (for this type of discussion see Jankowicz, 1991; Jones, 1988; Martin, 1988). According to this view, no method is entirely qualitative or quantitative. However, the techniques can be either quantitative or qualitative. Figure 7.1 illustrates this point further. In qualitative methods, although the number of observations is low, several aspects of the problem area can be analysed. Low numbers are also fied justi because we often want to do in-depth studies or provide ‘thick description ’, which is not possible in cases of numerous observations. Qualitative methods are, therefore, most suitable when the objectives of the study demand in-depth insight into a phenomenon. 7.2 Different Qualitative Methods and When to Use Them Research problems focusing on uncovering a person’s experience or behaviour, or when we want to understand a phenomenon about which little is known, are typical examples of qualitative research (Ghauri, 2004; Marshan-Piekkari and fi Welch, 2004). Moreover, when an event or social process is dif cult to study using quantitative methods, qualitative methods are most suitable and can provide intricate details and understanding. Qualitative research is thus common in social and behavioural sciences and among practitioners who want to understand human behaviour and functions. It is suitable for studying organizations, groups, and individuals (Strauss and Corbin, 1990). 7.2 Different Methods and When to Use Them There are three major components of qualitative research (Becker, 1970; Miles and Huberman, 1994; Strauss and Corbin, 1990): 1. data: 2. interpretative or often collected through interviews and observations; analytical procedure: and analyse the data to arrive at report: 3. the techniques to conceptualize findings or theories; written or verbal. In the case of students, the report is written in the form of a thesis or project. In spite of claims that relatively few studies use qualitative methods, it is ficult to find support for the use of qualitative data: not dif Qualitative data are attractive for many reasons: They are rich, full, earthly, holistic, flow real; their face validity seems unimpeachable, they preserve chronological where that is important, and suffer minimally from retrospective distortion; and they, in principle, offer a far more precise way to assess causality in organizational affairs than arcane efforts like cross-lagged correlations. (Miles, 1979: 117) It is generally qualitative accepted methods are that, most for inductive useful, as they and can exploratory lead us to research, hypothesis building and explanations. According to this view, qualitative and quantita- fi tive methods are suitable at different stages or levels of research. At the rst level, the problem is of an unstructured nature and qualitative methods are suitable. At the second level, quantitative methods are most useful, as we want to test different hypotheses that were arrived at through level 1. Quantitative methods allow us to accept or reject these hypotheses in a logical and consistent manner. At the third level, both qualitative and quantitative methods can be used. Often a combination of the two methods is used at this level. As we can see in Figure 7.1, the methods from left to right become more quantitative and use more quantitative techniques. Historical review, group discussions, and case studies are mostly qualitative research methods. These qualitative methods use relatively more qualitative techniques, such as conversations and in-depth, unstructured, or semi-structured interviews. 7.2.1 Historical Review In cases of historical reviews, our job is to describe what happened in the past so that we can understand the present or plan for the future. Here we go through existing records and reports and talk to different people to get as true a picture as possible. The archives are reviewed in an interrogative manner with a particular research question/problem in mind. The main problem in using such a method is that we have to trust human memory, 99 100 Data Collection for Qualitative Research 35 30 25 smrif fo rebmuN Key 20 Entry Exit 15 Total 10 5 0 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 Year (from 1951 to 1968) Figure 7.2 Number of firms in the US transistor industry Source : Utterback, J.M. (1994: 41). Reprinted by permission of Harvard Business School Publishing. Copyright © 2004 by the Harvard Business School Publishing Corporation; all rights reserved. Based on data in E. Braun and S. MacDonald, Revolution in Miniature: The History and Impact of Semiconductor Electronics (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1978) and J.E. Tilton,International Diffusion of Technology (Washington DC: The Brookings Institute, 1971). which records selective parts of reality. It is quite possible that two different people, while going through a certain situation or experience, will record or remember different things; sometimes they make mistakes or misunderstand things. It is therefore important that, when using such a method, we should cross-check one written source with another, or a written source with an interview, or two interviews with each other. In other words, we have to be critical and compare different explanations of the situation or event. For further insight into historical reviews as a research method, we recommend Orbell (1987). Figure 7.2 presents an example of historical review in a particular industry. 7.2.2 Grounded Theory It is often asked whether it is possible to apply a systematic and analytical approach through studying the reality without any pre-assumed understanding, and whether this type of approach can provide us with credible interpretations and conclusions towards theory development. Researchers using grounded theory suggest that instead of developing constructs that can be measured, first speci fied for example in quantitative research, we need to develop some concepts. Concepts are more general and less and can describe and explain a 7.2 Different Methods and When to Use Them phenomenon better. In other words, concepts are precursors to constructs in making sense of our reality (Gioia and Pitre, 1990; Gioia et al., 2012). In this approach, it is considered better to develop concepts before developing constructs for the purpose of theory development. This can be done using the grounded theory approach (Gioia and Chittipeddi, 1991; Glaser and Strauss, 1967). In this type of research, credible interpretations first-order analysis based on can be achieved by systematically presenting a informants’ understanding and terms and then presenting a second-order analysis based on the researcher’s concepts and themes (Hagen et al., 2019; Van Maanen, 1979). This type of research demands clear research question(s) that can lead to some semi-structured questions/interviews to obtain real-time information on a phenomenon or a process. Here the informants are free to talk about their experiences and their actions (Gioia et al., 2012; Morgan, 1983). In this case, the researchers do not provide leading questions and fi should not be afraid of getting too much information. As a rst stance, the informants are leading us to several concepts, while it is in the secondorder analysis that the researcher interprets these concepts into themes. These themes can further lead to aggregate dimensions (Hagen et al., 2019), as suggested in Figure 7.3. This allows the researcher to build a data structure. In this process the researcher can go back and forth between concepts, themes, and dimensions to see whether some new concepts have been discovered. However, it should be noted that the researcher cannot totally ignore earlier research (literature). Particularly at this stage, the researcher needs to compare and contrast his/her analysis with the earlier literature. In other words, at this stage, the data and existing theory work hand in hand (see Hagen et al., 2019 as a good example). This also means that a researcher needs to be informed about earlier research; this is what Gioia et al. (2012: 21) mean by the ‘willing suspension of belief or witting (as opposed to un-witting) ignorance of previous theorizing in the domain of interest ’. This allows for discovery and not reinventing the wheel. 7.2.3 Case Studies What Is a Case Study? The case study approach is often associated with descriptive or exploratory research, but it is not restricted to these areas (Bonoma, 1985; Ghauri, 2004; Yin, 1994). In business studies, case study research is particularly useful when the phenomenon under investigation is fi dif cult to study outside its natural setting and when the concepts and variables under study are dif ficult to quantify. Often this is because there are too many variables to 101 102 Data Collection for Qualitative Research Strategy Concepts Themes Aggregate dimensions Continuous interaction with key partners (e.g. customers, experts) for development and refinement of offering Identification of alternatives/broader scope of Selective customer/partner offerings through customers intimacy and intensity for Discovery of threats and potential market sensing drawbacks through regular contacts Important to listen but avoid being overwhelmed, Selective selection of signals is vital Responsiveness Regular assessment and feedback from customers regarding future and current offering reduces risk of failure/need of adaptation Selective experimentation Better and quicker development/refinement of & testing for innovation and (more) offerings business development Co-creation with customers and key partners Experimentation is a resource-conserving and rapidly resource-generating way to innovate Selection of ‘right’ partners Coordination, cultivation, Trust-based intensive relationships and harmonization of Mobilization of complementary resources multiple stakeholders and Migration and sharing of risks resources Flexibility ‘Doing more with less’ Find new ways to realize objectives (stretch Creative management and resources) extension of resources Flat hierarchy, quick communication, passion, and involvement Figure 7.3 The data structure Source : Based on Hagen et al. 2 ( 019). be considered, which makes experiment or survey methods inappropriate (Bonoma, 1985; Yin, 2009). As mentioned earlier, research in business studies emphasizes the role of deduction, where the validity of all findings depends solely on the quality of logic employed in the study and precise measurement. However, the trade-off between precision and reduced generalizability is not a useful one in many situations. Many phenomena cannot be understood if removed from their social context. In these cases, inductive qualitative approaches are alterna- fi tive methods to scienti c investigation. Case research is based on a process model as suggested by Bonoma (1985). A case study is a description of a management situation and often involves data collection through primary sources such as verbal reports, personal interviews, and observation. In addition, case methods involve data collection through sources such as financial reports and archives, as well as budget and operating statements, including market and competition reports. The case method is not suitable for all types of research. It is the research problem and the objective that decide whether the case method is suitable or not. With the above proviso, the case method is useful for theory development and 7.2 Different Methods and When to Use Them testing. The main feature is the intensity of the study of the object, individual, group, organization, culture, incident, or situation. The researcher needs fi to have suf cient information to characterize, to explain the unique features of the case, as well as to point out the characteristics that are common in several cases. Finally, this approach relies on integrative powers of research: the ability to study an object with many dimensions and then to draw an integrative interpretation. When to Use a Case Study A case study is a preferred approach when‘how’ or ‘why’ questions are to be answered, when the researcher has little control over events, and when the focus is on a current phenomenon in a real-life context (Yin, 2009). Case studies are often of an explanatory, exploratory, or descriptive nature. According to Eisenhardt, case studies are: particularly well-suited to new research areas or research areas for which existing theory seems inadequate. This type of work is highly complementary to incremental theory building from normal science research. The former is useful in early stages of research on a topic or when a fresh perspective is needed, while the latter is useful in later stages of knowledge. (Eisenhardt, 1989: 548– 9) When to use which research approach depends upon: • the type of research questions; • the control of the researcher on behavioural events; • the focus on a current as opposed to a historical phenomenon; • what information is needed; • how this can be obtained. When research questions concern only ‘what?’, for example, ‘What are the ways in which fied. Here justi an effective firm is operated?’, an exploratory study is the objective is to develop hypotheses or propositions, which can later be studied. For an exploratory study, any of the five research strategies can be used. If the questions relate to‘how many?’ or ‘how much?’, survey or archival strategies are favoured. But when ‘how? ’ and ‘why?’ questions are asked, a case study method is favoured as a research strategy. It is quite often stated that the case study method is used when we want to study a single organization and we want to identify factors involved in some aspects or behaviour of an organization or smaller unit, such as a marketing or finance department. However, it is equally possible to study several fi organizations with regard to a set of variables we have already identi ed or assumed. Such case studies are called comparative case studies. In this 103 104 Data Collection for Qualitative Research type of study we ask or study the same questions in a number of organizations and compare them with each other to draw conclusions. The purpose of data collection in the comparative case study method is to compare the phenomenon (e.g. strategy formation) in different cases in a systematic way, to explore different dimensions of our research issues, or to examine different levels of research variables. In a survey, on the other hand, we are more concerned with the sampling of different organizations as we want to generalize our findings to all other organizations of the same type (Jankowicz, 1991). Yin (2009) compares the case study method with experiments, and suggests three situations where case study is the preferred method: 1. fi If we want to follow a theory that speci es a particular set of outcomes in some particular situation, and if we find a firm which finds itself in that particular situation, we can use the case study method for a critical test of theory and its applicability to the organization. 2. fi If we want to study some speci c characteristics of a rare or extreme situation in which an organization finds itself, we can use the case study method to compare and contrast. 3. If we want to study a situation or an organization which has rarely been studied and is unique in its nature, we can use the case study method. In this case, we hope to learn something new and important. As most case studies are done through a review of existing historical material and records plus interviews, the case study method is quite similar to historical review, but it is different in the sense that here we have a possibility of direct observation and interaction. As mentioned earlier, we would like to make it explicit that the case study method is not synonymous with qualitative research or methods. A case study may very well involve quantitative methods or even be entirely quantitative. In many cases, especially in business studies, students method to use first decide which – for example, case study or survey – and then formulate their problem. We have been advocating that it is the research problem and the research objectives that dictate the type of method that should be used. Here fi lies a dilemma: should we decide the method rst, or should our problem lead us to the method? Of course, the latter should be the case, as most problems and research objectives clearly suggest one form of data collection over another. Preparing for a Case Study When preparing to undertake the case study method for research, a theory/ – data/theory revision cycle, as suggested by Bonoma (1985: 204 6), is quite 7.2 Different Methods and When to Use Them Disconfirmation gnidnatsrednu fo segatS Model/theory creation Prediction Design Drift Early Middle Late Stages of the research project Figure 7.4 A process model for case research Source: Based on Bonoma (1985: 205). Copyright © 1985, with permission from Elsevier. useful (see Figure 7.4). This revision process goes through four stages:‘drift ’, ‘design’, ‘prediction ’, and ‘discon firmation . ’ At the beginning of a research project the researcher is in a ‘drift ’ stage and is trying to learn the area of research and the concepts and terminology in the field. This drifting leads him/her to a priori notions about the phenom- enon and how it operates. This stage widens the perspective of the researcher fi and often leads to modi cation in the basic research questions (Van Maanen, 1983). In fact, most research methods involve this ‘drift’ phase in the early stages of the research project. A researcher moves to ‘design ’: the choice of strategy to collect the data needed to answer/elucidate the research question plus the stage when s/he starts developing tentative explanations of the observations so far. At this fi stage, the researcher assesses and re nes major areas of the research project as suggested by the drift stage. This facilitates the conceptualization of the research problem. At some time, s/he may have to go back to the drift stage, to get a better understanding. ‘Prediction’ occurs in the middle-to-late stage of the project. By this stage, the researcher has a good understanding of the factors according to which the case information may be grouped and can proceed with further case fi construction and analysis. The researcher proceeds con dently, compiling more cases with the purpose of drawing conclusions. As a result, s/he can even develop some tentative explanations. Similarly, s/he may realize that 105 106 Data Collection for Qualitative Research some generalizations are not very general and are valid only in particular circumstances, settings, industries, or The final firmation , stage, ‘discon ’ firms. refers to further testing and analysis of the results suggested by the prediction stage. This can be done by applying the results to other or broader sets of cases. The idea is to apply the concepts to totally different cases or situations to test the generalizability of the results. As we can see, these four stages do not really form a rigid fi or xed hierarchy but rather represent an iterative evolution towards understanding (Bonoma, 1985: 205). Coming to more practical details, once our research problem suggests case study as the preferred approach, we should deal with questions such as: What are the skills needed? What types of a priori assumptions do we have? How do we select the cases? How many cases shall we include in our study? How shall we conduct the case study? As far as the skills required are concerned, this is not one of the easiest types of research to do, as it demands special skills from the researcher. Some fic training for speci cases is necessary, especially if the researcher has no previous experience of conducting case studies, or a research assistant is used when there are several cases. Unlike in surveys, where data collection is more routinized, in case studies the researcher has to be skilled in the dynamics of a case and should be able to take advantage of opportunities offered during the data collection. While collecting data through semi-structured interviews a researcher must be able to control the situation, ask the right questions, adapt to new or unexpected situations, and develop trust. All these skills can be learnt if the researcher is aware of them beforehand. Yin (1994: 67) recommends case study training as a seminar experience, especially when many cases and several researchers are involved. Moreover, he recommends a protocol to arrive at the final version. One purpose of these seminars and protocols should be to discuss potential problems and how to handle them. Such training may also reveal weaknesses in the research problem, in the study design, and even in the capabilities of the researchers. All these can be improved if detected. It is also recommended that a pilot study is conducted before the data collection starts. This serves as a rehearsal for the data collection procedure, indicating the time it takes and any problems it can cause or that may arise. How to Select the Cases As in other methods of data collection, it is important to decide the target fi population that is to be used for the investigation. It includes those rms, individuals, groups, or elements that will be represented in the study. The 7.2 Different Methods and When to Use Them next stage is to assess the accessible population, the population to which we can get access (Cooper, 1984). Out of this accessible population we have to firms select one or a few cases, objects, or for the study, financial for the study. The time available resources for travelling, and other practical issues are of great importance. For example, depending upon how much time we have, the type of organization or company we select for our study would be different. If we have very little time available, we should perhaps study a firm, as in these firms the communication lines are shorter and faster, they are more fl exible, and it is easier to get overall or in-depth information. On the other hand, if we are studying a speci fic and complex issue, we should perhaps study a bigger fi rm, because these firms experience complex smaller problems and have in-house expertise that can provide us with in-depth information on the particular issue (van der Meer-Kooistra, 1993). The cases should also correspond to our theoretical framework and the variables we are studying. For example, if we are studying the behaviour of industrial buyers, we have to firms select that are dealing purchasing. Once we have selected a firm, with industrial marketing and we should select a manager who is involved in the process of marketing and purchasing. An interview with the firm s ’ public relations manager or an accountant would not provide us with the information we are looking for. In bigger organizations it is very important to select the right department, section, or individual. It is not a question of interviewing the most important individuals, but of interviewing the right person from an organization: right from the point of view of our research questions and study variables. Finding the right person is sometimes a long process. However, when we are able to establish contact with a key, or highly placed, manager, our goal should be to take his or her help in identifying the right person. Students often ask how many cases they should include in their study. The fi answer to this question is very dif cult as there is no upper or lower limit to the number. Often one case is enough. As Mintzberg says, ‘What, for example, is wrong with a sample size of one? Why should researchers have to apologize for them? Should Piaget apologize for studying his own children, a physicist for splitting only one atom? ’ (1979: 583). It is the research fl problem and the research objectives that in uence the number and choice of cases to be studied. Moreover, what is meant by‘a case’? For example, if we fi are trying to understand the process of decision making in a rm, we may study several decisions on different issues, important/unimportant, novel/ routine decisions, etc., in the among important factors same organization. (see, e.g., Campbell, This will provide variability 1975, who argues for the richness of detail within a single case by looking for many implications of the ideas under study). 107 108 Data Collection for Qualitative Research Hint: There is no upper or lower limit with regard to the number of cases to be included in a study. Sampling in Qualitative Research Sampling is primarily associated with quantitative research, emphasizing estimation of various parameters, testing hypotheses, and so on. Samples consisting of one or more units of observations (and analysis) are, however, always applied in qualitative research. In quantitative empirical research an important purpose is to arrive at statistically valid conclusions, for example that on a statistical basis we can conclude that‘X’ and ‘Y’ covary. In other words, statistical conclusion validity plays a major role. For example, if a study of the effectiveness of a new medical drug cannot demonstrate that the drug is effective (or more effective than another drug), no conclusions can be drawn on either internal or external validity. In qualitative research the purpose is seldom to arrive at statistically valid conclusions (even though it is possible), but rather to understand, gain insights, and create explanations (theory). Therefore, in qualitative research, sampling issues are also important, such as who and how many should be included. EXAMPLE 7.1 fl In a qualitative study the objective is to gain insights into who inuences purchase decisions. To obtain these insights the researcher has to consider who is involved, and who possesses (and is willing to share) the needed information, which implies selecting the most relevant respondents (subjects). In this effort the researcher may start with one person, for example fi the manager of the research department, and by asking about speci c purchases, and also asking: ‘Were other people involved?’, will thus grad- fl ually uncover participation and in uences in buying decisions. EXAMPLE 7.2 Focus groups are often used to gain insights into various perspectives and opinions. How many focus group interviews should be conducted? Let us assume that the researcher starts with one focus group, and the data are transcribed and analysed. Then s/he conducts another focus group 7.2 Different Methods and When to Use Them EXAMPLE 7.2 (CONT.) fi interview, and that also uncovers points of view not present in the rst one. The researcher continues the procedure until no new opinions/points of view are uncovered. Most scholars suggest that in qualitative research‘purposeful sampling’ is most suitable. This means that we should select information-rich cases for in-depth study to achieve insight and understanding rather than to achieve generalizability (Patton 2014). Some of the most commonly used purposeful case selections are: critical case, comparative cases, criterion-based cases, heterogeneous cases, homogeneous cases, typical case, and snowball case selection. These most common case selection strategies and what they entail are presented in Table 7.2. Table 7.2 Strategies for case selection Selection strategy Explanation Critical case Where an insight into a single critical case can provide maximum and logical explanation that can be true for all other cases in that category Comparative When cases are selected so that they can be compared and cases contrasted to achieve maximum understanding Criteria-based When cases are selected based on pre-decided criteria, often to cases compare the cases that meet criteria with those that do not meet criteria Heterogeneous When we select cases that are clearly different from each other, cases for example to understand the impact of a certain factor on the success of manufacturing versus services firms Homogeneous When we select cases that are clearly similar, for example, cases cases from manufacturing as well as the same industry such as automobiles Typical cases When we select cases that are considered normal/average to achieve better insight and understanding Snowball case When we select cases that can lead us to further cases, suggested selection by respondents, that are dif cult to approach on our own or are fi connected to earlier cases Source: Based on Patton (2014). 109 110 Data Collection for Qualitative Research Theoretical Sampling In any study, variability is needed. Consider a study designed to examine the (potential) relationship between organizational form and innovativeness. In order to study this research question, firms (organizations) representing a range of organizational forms need to be included. Let us also assume that F the researcher, based on a review of the literature, knows that forms, F 2 fi 1 , . . . exist. This insight is then useful when deciding on which rms (organ- izations) should be included. We see here that the sample units are chosen for theoretical reasons. Different Types of Case Study Design Yin 2 (1994) provides four types of case study design and presents a  2 matrix (see Figure 7.5), suggesting that single and multiple case studies re flect different design considerations. The four types of research design are as follows: 1. single case design, holistic; 2. single case design, embedded; 3. multiple case design, holistic; 4. multiple case design, embedded. As we can see, the primary distinction is between single and multiple case designs. We should therefore decide, at an early stage, whether we are going to use single or multiple case design. Single case is appropriate when a particular case is critical and we want to use it for testing an established theory. It is a critical case because it meets all the conditions necessary to con firm, challenge, or extend the theory. Another situation is when a single case is an extreme or a unique case. Finally, a single case design is appropriate when a case is revelatory. This means that we can observe and study a phenomenon that was previously not accessible, and which can provide useful insight. We can also use single case design in other situations, such Single case design Multiple case designs Holistic (single unit of analysis) Type 1 Type 3 Embedded (multiple units of analysis) Type 2 Type 4 Figure 7.5 Basic design for case studies Source : Yin (1994: 46). Copyright © 1994 by Sage Publications. Reprinted by permission of Sage Publications, Inc. 7.3 Observations fi as a pilot study or an exploratory study that serves as a rst step to a later, more comprehensive, study (Yin, 2009). Multiple case study design is considered more appropriate for studies which do not involve rare, critical, or revelatory cases. In this approach we should be clear that every case must serve a particular purpose in the study. In other words, we have to justify the selection of each case. However, as flexible pointed out earlier, case study design is often fi fi and can be modi ed with proper justi cation. The use of a case study method depends also upon the type of study we are doing, whether it is inductive or deductive, and also upon whether we are fi looking for speci c or general explanations. In the case of an inductive fi approach and speci c explanation, we may use the single case. On the other hand, if we are doing a study with a deductive approach, we can use a case study at an early stage to develop our hypotheses or propositions. If we are doing a study with an inductive approach but are looking for general explanations, then we should use a multiple case method. However, if we are doing a study with a deductive approach and are looking for generalizations, then the case study is a less recommended method. We suggest that students using this method should consult Yin (2003) for further guidance.The above- mentioned approaches to qualitative methods are summarized in Table 7.3, where we can see the characteristics of these different approaches. 7.3 Observations Observation as a data collection tool entails listening and watching other people’s behaviour in a way that allows some type of learning and analytical interpretation. The main advantage is that we can collect first-hand infor- mation in a natural setting. Moreover, we can interpret and understand the observed behaviour, attitude, and situation more accurately, and capture the dynamics of social behaviour in a way that is not possible through questionnaires and interviews. The main disadvantage is that most observations are made by individuals fi who systematically observe and record a phenomenon, and it is dif cult fi to translate the events or happenings into scienti cally useful information. This is particularly important when the purpose is to generalize from these observations. Here questions about validity and reliability become very important and need to be answered satisfactorily. When we collect data through observations, we have to make a number of choices regarding, for example, participatory versus non-participatory, laboratory versus tings. This is further illustrated by Figure 7.6. field set- 111 112 Data Collection for Qualitative Research Table 7.3 Different qualitative approaches Features Historic review Focus group Grounded theory Case study Focus Description of Gathering Developing a theory Developing in-depth events information from a from the data in the description through group of people fi eld one or more cases Type of To tell stories of Explaining a lived Grounding a theory Providing an in-depth research experiences phenomenon in the view of the understanding from a participants case or cases Discipline Humanities, Philosophy, Sociology, Psychology, law, history, psychology, management political science, sociology, education medicine, literature management Unit of Studying one Studying several Study a process, Study an event, analysis or more individuals who action, or programme, activity incidents/ share the interaction between more than one individuals experience individuals individual Data Documents and Group discussion Interviews with Multiple sources, collection interviews and observations several individuals interviews, – observations, documents ficant Data Storytelling, Seeking signi Generating a theory Developing detailed analysis developing statement, illustrated by data analysis from one or themes, structure, and chronologies description more cases Source: Based on Creswell (2007). Observations Human Laboratory Figure 7.6 Mechanical Field Laboratory Field Choices for collecting primary data through observations In participant or fi eld observation, the observer is a natural part of the situation or event. The researcher is part of a company or organization and decides to study the same organization in one way or another. Sometimes fi a researcher speci cally joins an organization to be able to observe it as a 7.3 Observations participant. In this case, the observation is not hidden or disguised: the people who are being observed know that they are being observed and by whom. In business studies, participant observation can enable researchers to have access to what people actually do instead of what they might claim they do. One danger of participatory observation is that the observers can be so fluenced in by the event, situation, culture, or everyday lives of the subjects that they become unable to take a neutral view of events and situations. On the other hand, observers may be so ethnocentric that they are not able to observe or analyse the situation because they believe the subjects have an inferior culture or that the observer knows best (Douglas, 1976). A contrived setting refers to a method where reactions are observed in a controlled setting in a laboratory or in other virtual reality. In this method, the researcher is better able to control the observation without any disturb- fi ances. Moreover, it could be more ef cient and less time consuming. In non-participant observations, natural setting but is not part of the the observer or researcher situation her/himself. fl observes a It has been reported in several studies that people’s behaviour may be in uenced by a non-participatory observer, but only in the beginning; people get used to it in a very short time. One way to overcome this problem is to observe under disguise. For example, in a buying or selling situation an observer can act as a potential customer or salesperson. Observations for research should be planned systematically in direct relevance to research questions. However, these observations can be simple and straightforward, quite often in a natural setting, event, or occurrence. An example is observing customers in a supermarket while they make their choices in picking up a particular product category or brand. In the case of a mechanical method of observation, the same observation can be done by placing a video camera overlooking a particular section of the supermarket. Another way employed by a number of companies is to use their hotline statistics to understand consumer behaviour and future trends. These calls are recorded and later analysed in terms of the type of questions asked and the types of problems mentioned by the customers. However, a researcher has to consider the ethical aspects of this method of data collection. Do the customers/subjects know that they are being observed? If not, then is it ethically correct, or might the recording of their behaviour harm them in some way or violate their personal integrity? In the case of human observation, an observer follows his or her own values and expectations and is also limited by his or her own constraints. The subjects can catch those values and constraints by placing the researcher in some class or category. Most subjects who are alert to these issues can do so by the dress, manner, or language used. For example, while studying the 113 114 Data Collection for Qualitative Research fi behaviour of a labour force, if the observer is classi ed as anti-working class, fl this can in uence the behaviour of the observed (Phillips, 1966). Some scholars agree that data collected through observation are more objective and accurate, as this method is independent of respondents ’ unwillingness or inability to provide the information needed by the researcher. For example, respondents are often careful in replying to sensitive or embarrassing questions. Observation of the actual behaviour as it occurs fl re ects respondents’ behaviour more accurately (Churchill and Iacobucci, 2015). As mentioned earlier, observation as a data collection method should not be fl selected just because it is easier. This choice is highly in uenced by the research problem and the research design as well as the researcher ’s skills, capabilities, and nature, and the characteristics of the subjects to be observed. 7.4 Communication For primary data, the researcher has to decide whether to communicate with the respondents/subjects or just to observe them. Communication does not have to be direct or face to face. We could send our questions by post or email and ask for answers to be sent back to us in the same manner. We could also use more personal methods and meet with the respondents/ subjects face to face and ask questions. The instrument used for this type of data collection is called a survey or questionnaire. A questionnaire can be structured, unstructured, or semistructured. advance In and a structured respondents questionnaire, have to the answer questions from a set are of decided in predetermined answers. In the case of an unstructured questionnaire, the questions are only roughly scripted in advance and there are no predetermined answers; the respondent can reply in his or her own words. A questionnaire where the questions are predetermined but the respondents can use their own words to answer is a semi-structured questionnaire (Churchill and Iacobucci, 2015). Communication thus refers to collection of data by asking those who have experienced a particular phenomenon to explain it to the researcher. In qualitative research, there are three main ways to collect data through this method: 1. personal interview; 2. telephone interview; 3. email interview. Many students interviews. and business researchers collect their data through 7.5 Interviews 7.5 Interviews Interviews demand real interaction between the researcher and the respond- fi ent. To be able to run the interview ef ciently and without any disturbances, the researcher needs to know the respondent and his/her background, values, and expectations. At present, we have fi enough accumulated experience available to be able to carry out ef cient interviews. Interviews are often considered the best data collection methods (see Figure 7.7). first In research we use two types of interview. The structured is survey research or interviews, where a standard format of interview is used with an emphasis on fixed response categories and systematic sampling and loading procedures combined with quantitative measures and statistical methods. The second type is unstructured interviews, where the respondent is given almost full liberty to discuss reactions, opinions, and behaviour on a particular issue. The interviewer is there just to give lead questions and to record the responses in order later to understand‘how’ and ‘why’. The questions and answers are often unstructured and are not systematically coded beforehand. In this section, we deal with this type of interview for qualitative research. In the literature there is some discussion on semi-structured interviews, which differ from both unstructured and structured interviews. They differ from the former in the sense that the topics and issues to be covered, sample sizes, people to be interviewed, and questions to be asked have been determined beforehand. They also differ in how we plan to minimize bias arising from the sequence in which we address subject matter, from any inadvertent omission of questions, from unrepresentative sampling, and from an uncontrolled over- or under-representation of sub-groups among our respondents. In semi-structured interviews we handle this bias by careful design of the technique itself. Semi-structured and unstructured interviews differ from structured interviews in that they demand greater skills from the interviewer, as in semi-structured and unstructured interviews we often obtain information about personal, attitudinal, and value-laden material, which calls for social sensitivity. An advantage of structured interviews lies in the uniformity in the behaviour of interviewers, as those other than the researcher can replicate the interview in similar situations. Unstructured interviews are considered Interviews Interview by mail Figure 7.7 A typology of interviews Interview by phone Personal interview 115 116 Data Collection for Qualitative Research advantageous in the context of discovery because the researcher/interviewer who is well acquainted with the research questions/area can ask follow-up questions and enrich the data so collected (Alvesson, 2011; Phillips, 1966). There is an abundance of literature available on structured vs. unstructured or semi-structured interviews when it comes to the question form and respondent understanding (e.g. Alvesson, 2011; Beza, 1984; Bryman and Bell, 2015; Fowler and Mangione, 1990; Mishler, 1986; Schuman and Presser, 1976). Anybody interested in this discussion should consult any of the references cited, as our purpose here is to provide some guidelines for interviewing. For the purpose of this book, interviews refer to face-to-face verbal exchanges, in which one person, the interviewer, attempts to obtain information or opinions or beliefs from another person, the interviewee. The advantage of in-depth unstructured interviews is that we can gain a more accurate and clear picture of a respondent ’s position or behaviour. This is possible because of open-ended questions and because respondents are free to answer according to their own thinking, as we have not constrained answers to only a few alternatives. This is also true in the case of complicated or sensitive issues, where the interviewer can ask for further elaboration of answers and attitudes. This method of data collection is highly suitable for exploratory and inductive types of study as it matches their purposes very well. EXAMPLE 7.3 An interviewer starts by asking: Could you please tell me about your experiences with your new computer? fi rst Respondent: At I had a lot of trouble. Interviewer: What trouble? Respondent: It was dif cult to operate, it was different from my old fi computer. Interviewer: How? Could you please explain? fl This hypothetical example re ects that the respondent feels unsure and uncomfortable learning to use a new computer. The disadvantage of in-depth interviews is that they demand a skilled and cautious interviewer. The interviewer should have a complete understanding of the research problem, its purpose, and what information is being sought. The course of the interview is decided by the skills of the interviewer when asking questions and probing further with supplementary questions. The 7.5 Interviews know-how and skills of the interviewer are thus of the utmost importance. Interviews can also take a long time – longer than questionnaires – and may even require several filling sessions in structured with the same fi respondent (Churchill and Iacobucci, 2015). In addition, interviews are dif cult to interpret and analyse. The researcher ’s own background may greatly fluence in the interpretations, thereby causing problems of objectivity. Depending upon which type of analysis we want to do, coding of in-depth fi interviews is a dif cult task in spite of improved techniques and systems. 7.5.1 Preparing for an Interview The first steps in preparing for an interview are to: (1) analyse your research problem, (2) understand what information you really need to have from an interviewee, and (3) see who would be able to provide you with that information. The clearer the problem statement is, the easier it is to know what to ask. It is understood that the purpose of data collection through interviews is to obtain valid information from the most appropriate person. In other words, you should clearly know what you want to ask as well as who can provide the most relevant and valid information on those issues. Moreover, the interviewee should be willing to answer your questions truthfully. For example, can you expect everyone to be honest on very sensitive matters? The next step is to draft an interview guide or interview questions. These questions should be compared with the research problem several times, partly to test the consistency between the two and partly to see whether find questions are thorough and precise enough to allow you to the out what you want. It is very useful to let somebody else (perhaps your supervisor) see the problem statement and the questions to be asked in the interview to check this congruence. After the above scrutiny, a first draft of the interview questions should be prepared. This draft has to be pre-tested as a pilot study. While the above scrutiny dealt with the researcher’s understanding of the research problem and the interview questions, this test checks the understanding of the interviewee regarding the research problem and interview questions. Such pilot research also provides first-hand insight into what might be called the ‘cultural endowment’ of the informants. After this pilot study, where a few respondents (say three to five) have been asked to read the research problem and the interview questions and have also answered the questions and have fi commented on their understanding of them, you can prepare the nal draft of the interview guide and questions. At this point, particular attention has to be given to the approach you are going to use: for instance, before contacting the interviewee you have to decide how much time the interview should take. The pilot study can help you 117 118 Data Collection for Qualitative Research to determine the time needed for your questions. You must consider that business executives work with the belief that ‘time is money’ and might refuse to give an interview because of the shortage of time. Our experience shows that an interview should not take more than one and a half hours, and ideally it should take around an hour. However, the total meeting time may be two hours or more. In many cases, the interview is preceded or followed by a factory visit or lunch. In open-ended interviews you can get a lot of information during these interactions. In this respect, it is very important to realize that the interviewer has to create a situation where the respondent willingly offers time. If the respond- fi ent is not suf ciently motivated to provide you with time, there will be little motivation to answer your questions and to be an interviewee. This can jeopardize the whole purpose of the interview. Once you have considered all aspects and prepared the interview guide, you should approach the person(s) you want to interview. Here you can use the telephone or write a letter/email, perhaps both. In the letter/email, for example, you can explain the purpose of your study, provide a short problem statement, and describe the type of information you are interested in collecting. In this letter you may also mention that you will be calling very soon (next Monday, next week, etc.) to request an appointment for an interview. You should also mention how much time you think the interview will take. You must remember that you cannot demand a certain time, day, or week for the interview: you have to adapt to the interviewee ’s schedule, and not the other way around. Here is an example of the conversation for getting an appointment for an interview: EXAMPLE 7.4 ‘Hello, my name is . . . ’Im working on my Master’s thesis on new product development. Last week I sent you a letter with some information on my project, which I think will be of interest to your company, and I would like very much to have an interview with you. ’ Manager: It sounds interesting, but it is very hectic here, and ’m I short of time. Student: I understand the situation, but the interview will not take more than an hour and I can adjust to your time table. Manager: fi Well, let me see, can you meet me at my of ce on Monday at 9am? Student: Yes, thank you very much. I will be there on time. 7.5 Interviews One important issue that you must be clear on before you contact the record the information you interviewee is how you are going to will get. It is widely accepted that voice-recording is a useful method. The disadvantages with tape-recording are that the respondent might be reluctant to answer sensitive questions, or might not even answer some questions. There is also a risk that the interviewer might cease to listen carefully, believing that all the information is going on to the tape to be listened to later in a more relaxed environment. It is therefore recommended that you also take notes even if you will be tape-recording the interview (Alvesson, 2011; Ghauri, 2004). inform When you make the approach for an appointment, you will have to the respondent whether you will be using a voice-recorder or not. In fact, instead of informing, you should ask whether you can use the recorder. This issue automatically leads to the question of fi confidentiality. You will have to ask if the interview is to be treated con dentially and you will have to give your promise (undertaking or personal fi guarantee) that all information you receive will be kept con dential. If necessary, you may send a written fi assurance of con dentiality, signed by yourself and your supervisor or any other responsible person from the school, for example the director of research. When making an appointment, you should remember that you have to create a reason or a reward for the respondent – why should they answer your questions? What is in it for them? For example, you can mention that the results of the study will be provided to the respondent, or that you hope the results will help in analysing a competitive/managerial position that fi he/she/the company in question would bene t from. Or perhaps the study would help the industry or the country or it would help policy makers in their job and thereby indirectly the firm in question. Another way to show your appreciation is to take a token gift such as a pen or a coffee mug with your school’s logo. Now that you have made the appointment, it is time to decide who is going to conduct the interview: One person or more? Will the same person be conducting all the interviews or will different people interview different companies/managers? If several interviewers are used, they have to be trained. Once you have taken an appointment and cleared matters such as the amount of time to be used, recording or not, etc., you should send a confirmation letter about the appointment, thanking the interviewee for giving you the opportunity to visit them and saying you look forward to seeing them on the agreed date and time. This is necessary to avoid any misunderstandings on date or timing, and to remind the interviewee. Before closing the preparation section, we would like to discuss what you need to do before you start making appointments and interviewing. It is very important to analyse, discuss, and consider resources available to you for 119 120 Data Collection for Qualitative Research performing these interviews. You have to consider all the costs: for example travelling costs, the time necessary for the interviews, and also the time you need to process the interviews. Sometimes researchers start a very ambitious interview process and after a couple of interviews, or after about 50 per cent of their planned interviews, they give up due to travelling expenses, the time consumed, or overload of information. We have seen several students start their research interviewing, spending two to four hours at each interview, recording everything, and ending up with several hours of information. But when they started listening to the recorded material (which is a very time-consuming and tedious job), they did not know what to do with all the information, and in fact reported that they had used less than half of the interviews they recorded. The best way to handle this issue is to discuss these matters with your supervisor before you start interviewing and stick to a model or a plan for the interviews. 7.5.2 Pre-Interview The appointment has been made with the respondent and now, together with your supervisor, consider the you data have to collection review the dimension, questionnaire. how you are Here going you to must use the responses for the analysis in your report, and how you want to present the information you are gathering. You also have to schedule your time properly. plan your If you are having more than one interview per day, you should time. This is particularly important when you are in a foreign environment or if you are in a strange city, as you must also consider how much time you need to arrive at the location from your hotel, especially in rush hours. The interviewees have limited time and are always very busy. If you have already informed them that you need one hour of their time, you cannot arrive half an hour late and give the excuse that the journey took longer than you fi thought, or that you were stuck in traf c, or you could not Another important issue is one of social conventions, find a taxi. meaning how you behave in the interview, how you should dress, and so on. It is advisable to be reasonably formal. If you go in shabby clothes or worn-out jeans you may not give a positive impression to the interviewee about your seriousness and the fact that you can do worthwhile research and thereby might be of some help to the company. It is also wise not to‘party ’ the night before. It would not be a good idea to go for an interview smelling of alcohol or half-asleep. Finally, we believe that interviewing is a skill you should practise rehearse or with regard to understanding the time needed and your own argu- ments and questions, etc. Find a ‘victim ’, such as a friend, and practise. If there is the slightest chance that the respondent might misunderstand something, then they probably will. Moreover, it goes without saying that you 7.5 Interviews must check and recheck the equipment you are going to use in the interview, such as the voice- or video-recorder. Hint: Dress properly and check your timing, questions, and recording material. 7.5.3 The Interview The fi to rst important issue here is to introduce the study and its purpose and orient the respondents. The interviewer should be able to answer clearly all fi the questions the respondents might have, such as: Who will bene t from the study? When will the fi nal report be ready? Will they get a report or not? fi Moreover, the interviewer should reinforce the con dentiality, if required, to the respondent’s satisfaction. At this stage, it is important to realize that the respondent is asking the questions and the interviewer must provide satisfactory answers. These introductory fi ve to ten minutes can be a determining factor in how the rest of the interview goes. The language used in these early minutes and in the subsequent inter- simple and under- action is of great importance. The interviewer must use standable language, being extra careful when using certain terminology or concepts from a particular discipline, such as quite normal that working in the fi business executives, fi nance or management. It is although they might have been eld for several years, will not be familiar with textbook terminology. This point is even more important when the interview is taking place in another environment or country. In that case, it is not only the terminology you have to be careful about, but also the language. You must be sure that the language and the level of language you are using is compatible with the respondent’s knowledge and use of the same language. The interviewer, irrespective of questioning entirely to the informant to provide answers to technique, must leave it questions. In other words, the questions should not be asked in a leading or directive manner, as this puts pressure on the respondent to give the answer s/he thinks you want to hear. For example, do not pose questions such as:‘You must have realized that . . .’ or ‘How could you . . .?’ Moreover, it is important that every now and then the interviewer expresses an understanding of what the interviewee is saying. A nod or a‘hmm’ from time to time gives the impression that the interviewer understands what is meant. For the interviewee to keep on asking and answering questions, the interviewer must show interest and enthusiasm in the respondents and their ‘story’. 121 122 Data Collection for Qualitative Research Although it is advised that the interviewees are given full freedom to express personal meanings and give their own answers, it is quite important that the interviewer controls the situation to get the relevant information. Therefore, control with some care is necessary, not only to get the relevant information but also to manage the time. The interviewee must be given reasonable time for each question and should not be interrupted. However, business executives or other respondents often like to talk at length about their experiences and know-how, especially about positive events, and should therefore be controlled, but with care. Here is a conversation example to start an interview: EXAMPLE 7.5 Interviewer: You have allowed one hour for this interview. ’ve I read annual reports and inspected your homepage. Could you fi rst tell me about what you consider to be the major reasons for your company’s success in the past? firm’s Manager: [starts explaining in detail the Interviewer: This is interesting, but could you tell me a little more about the dif ficult history and his role.] period you just mentioned? And so on . . . Controlling time is very important, as the interviewee has given you a certain time and may be interested in talking a lot on each issue. You must fi ensure that you have answers to all your questions within the speci ed time. If the time agreed upon beforehand was between 9 and 10.30 am, it is quite possible that at 10.30 the interviewee has to go to a meeting and just stops talking and asks you to leave, or a number of people enter the room to have a meeting, giving you only one option, to leave. You also have to, in a way, That is why we stress taking develop a relationship with great care in the the interviewee. opening minutes of the interview. You must be able to give the impression of a serious, trustworthy, and friendly person. The relationship can also be developed by expressing interest in the interviewee’s position and opinion and by appreciating his/ her point of view. The better the relationship between the interviewer and the interviewee, the more open the responses and the more useful the information you will get. This is also important in case you have not been able to get all the information due to time or any other reason, or if you would like to obtain some additional information later. If the interviewee 7.5 Interviews enjoyed with talking you or to you, offering s/he you a should factory not mind visit or having lunch. another These meeting are excellent opportunities to develop trust, friendship, and a positive relationship with the interviewee. You should be careful about sensitive questions. Many times it is just a question of phrasing or using the right language to make the questions less sensitive. Sometimes, the questions are of a sensitive nature but must still be fi asked. Here the interviewee should not be pressured to provide a de nite yes or no (admit it . . . ) answer. Questions regarding why a certain strategy or plan failed, about competitors and their success, or about some financial issues can be of this nature. For example, when interviewing a bank manager, the following question may be quite sensitive:‘Who is responsible for fi all the bad debts reported by your branch/of ce? ’ The same question can be asked in another way; for example: ‘What, in your opinion, are the factors fi that caused the bad debts reported by your branch/of ce?’ It is advisable to avoid any direct questions on who was responsible for a certain blunder fl or miscalculation. Questions regarding intra-organizational con icts should be asked with some care and with indirect language. 7.5.4 Post-Interview After coming back from the interview, you should write ant points down the import- that were discussed as well as notes on the practical details. This can include whether you were able to get all the answers you wanted or how much time it took, some opinions on the respondent (such as very open or reserved person), and your perception of your interaction and relationship with the interviewee. All these details will help you later when you listen to the recorded tape or when you sit down to write up the information you collected. Most of all, it will help you in case you need to get additional information. In case the interview was not recorded, it is recommended that you go through your notes and write a complete descriptive report of the interview immediately (or as soon as possible) after the interview. The second thing you must do is to write a thank-you letter to the respondent. You might also send some further information on your research project which you might have promised or realized in the interview that they would like to have. Reporting or transcribing an interview is an important and tedious job. As mentioned earlier, interviews that are not recorded should be written as a narrative story as soon as possible. Recorded interviews always need some supporting material to remind you of the situation and the feeling of the 123 124 Data Collection for Qualitative Research interview. The best way is first to write down all the information recorded in the same order, and later develop a descriptive report of the interview relevant for the study. In this second stage you can discard all irrelevant talk and information. Sometimes it is useful to send this descriptive report to the interviewee for comments. You might have misunderstood something or perhaps you are not sure what the respondent really wanted to say. Depending upon your relationship with the interviewees, they might like to see what they said and, quite often they will give additional information or clarify their message voluntarily. Hint: Go through your interview notes promptly. 7.6 Focus Groups Another qualitative method mentioned above is group discussion (also called a focus group). In this research method, the researcher can get together with several respondents at the same time and initiate discussion on a small (focused) number of issues (Bryman and Bell, 2003; Stewart and Shamdasani, 1990). The opinions of respondents are considered as information and analysed later. This method differs from other methods such as in-depth interviews in that here the interaction is not only between the interviewer and the respondent but also among the respondents. It is also considered a relatively cheap and convenient way of gathering information from several respondents in a short time. In business studies, focus groups can be particularly useful in the following types of study (Stewart and Shamdasani, 1990: 15): obtaining general background about a topic; generating research hypotheses; stimulating new ideas and creative concepts; diagnosing problems/success factors for a new product, service or programme; generating impressions of products, programmes, services or institutions/ firms; learning how respondents talk about the phenomenon, which may help in designing questionnaires or other instruments; interpreting previously obtained quantitative data. 7.6 Focus Groups fl Here we should be aware of the in uence the group itself will have on fl the discussion and information that is exchanged. The discussion is in uenced by the size of the group, its composition, the personalities of the people involved, geographical the the roles arrangement interviewer and the they of are the group asked meeting, or to and individuals. play, the physical the ‘chemistry’ It is thus and between apparent that information gathered by this method will be different from information gathered through historical review and case studies. However, this method is widely used in some research cultures, such as consumer behaviour (Kent, 1993). As a data collection method, focus groups take many different forms, such as discussion groups, focused interviews, group interviews, and group research, and they are often used in business studies, for example for new product development, programme evaluation, marketing, advertising, and communication. The size of the group interviewed should be small, normally from six to around ten people, who will discuss a particular topic or issue under the direction of a moderator, who keeps the discussion on track (focused). Too small (e.g. < > 5) or too large (e.g. 10) numbers can make the focus group ineffective as the participation of individuals can become too fragmented or too little. These groups are arranged and the discussion may last from half an hour to around two hours. The moderator can then observe the interview or group, sometimes without disturbing the discussion. The moderator plays an important role in keeping the discussion on the focus issue and in ensuring that it goes smoothly. The role of the moderator is to secure interaction between the focus group members and see that they address the topics believed to be important. Prior to running a focus group, a list of topics or keywords is usually prepared. The moderator ’s task is to ensure the discussion covers all the topics, but not necessarily in the listed order. The amount and the nature of direction provided by the moderator will fluence in the quality and depth of the data collected. In the worst case, the group discussion might not cover the topic or issue at all. The moderator must therefore carefully consider the structure and nature of direction in relation to the research questions and purpose. Although focus groups can provide quantitative data, they are mostly used for collecting qualitative data. One of the conditions is that there should be some homogeneity among the individuals in each focus group, as this will encourage more in-depth and open discussion (Cowley, 2000; Merton et al., 1956). In the case where data are to be collected from different groups of people, it is often advised that separate focus groups be arranged for different subsets of the population. 125 126 Data Collection for Qualitative Research Advantages and Disadvantages of Focus Groups 7.6.1 One advantage of the focus group is that it produces very rich and in- depth data expressed in respondents’ own words and reactions, which is ficult normally dif to obtain using other methods such as surveys. It allows the researcher to understand why people feel or behave the way they do. It allows him or her to probe into respondents ’ views through discussion and reasoning, as in a focus group people can argue with each other ’s views. This reveals how people really think about main advantage, however, is that it is a quick, different flexible, issues. The and inexpensive method of data collection. It gives the researcher a chance to observe the reactions of people in open and free conversation with each other. Further advantages include the fact that it allows the researcher to interact directly with respondents, and to react and build upon the discussion as it goes. The disadvantages are that this type of data collection makes it very ficult to summarize and categorize the information gathered. And in the case of an unskilled moderator, it may be dif ficult to get really useful information. Other disadvantages include the fact that it can be dif ficult dif to gather people at a location, and the small numbers who are willing might not be representative of the population under study. The responses of the fl group members are not independent of one another and might be in uenced by each other or by a dominant group member. The live participation and observation warranted. The may lead moderator the researcher may bias the to have greater respondents, faith than knowingly is or unknowingly. 7.6.2 Conducting Focus Groups Once a clear statement of the problem has been generated (see Figure 7.8), it is important to identify the sample from those who are representative of the larger population. Representativeness is most important in this case, as we fi will be observing only a few individuals. Having done this, we need to nd an appropriate moderator and, with his or her assistance (or keeping that individual in mind as well as our research objectives), formulate an interview guide and questions, and decide the structure of the focus group and exactly how it will be run. Normally in focus groups researchers record data on paper or in a notepad. It is, however, becoming quite common to use video recorders that enable researchers to view and analyse the data later. In the case of several focus groups, the records and notes are analysed; they then provide a good picture to the researcher. 7.6 Focus Groups Formulation of research questions Identification of the sample Identification of the moderator Generation of interview guide/questions and structure Recruiting the sample Conducting the group Interpretation and analysis Writing the report Figure 7.8 Steps in conducting a focus group Source: Stewart and Shamdasani (1990: 20). Copyright © 1990 by Sage Publications. Reprinted by permission of Sage Publications, Inc. 127 128 Data Collection for Qualitative Research Further Reading ........................................................................ Fontana, A. and Frey, J.H. (1994) ‘Interviewing: The art of science’, in Denzin, N.K. and Lincoln, Y.S. (eds.), Handbook of Qualitative Research , Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 361–76. Hagen, B., Zucchella, A., and Ghauri, P. (2019)‘ From fragile to agile: Marketing as a key driver of entrepreneurial internationalization’ , Review International Marketing , 36(2): 260–88. Mason, J. (2002) ‘ Qualitative interviewing: asking, listening and interpreting’ , in May, T. (ed.), Qualitative Research in Action , London: Sage, pp. 225–41. Questions ........................................................................ 1. When are: (1) quantitative and (2) qualitative research approaches most appropriate? 2. What do you consider to be the major reasons for doing historical reviews? 3. For what research purposes do you consider focus group interviewing is more appropriate? 4. What is (are) the difference(s) between single case and multiple case study designs? When do you consider: (1) single, (2) multiple case designs to be most appropriate? 5. ‘Develop a relationship with the interviewee.’ Comment. Exercises ........................................................................ 1. You want to understand consumer behaviour towards use of mobile telephones. You have decided to do that through focus group interviewing. Prepare a focus group interview plan to gain insights into how a group of people experience and use a product (e.g. a mobile phone). Draft a letter to explain your plan to send to the target group. 2. Draft a letter to send to companies you want to interview/study using the case study method for your research. Formulate a research problem and try to explain it to companies, also convincing them to participate in your study. 3. Referring to Exercise (1) above: suppose you are trying to understand consumer behaviour around mobile phones through a survey. Formulate a precise research problem and develop a questionnaire that you can use for this data collection. 4. Visit a store or a marketplace. Observe how people behave and shop. 8 Qualitative Data Analysis Quality, not quantity is my measure. Douglas Jerrold, quotation from L. Henry, Quotations for all Occasions, Best New York: Fawcett Premier, 1995 Qualitative research poses fic speci analytical challenges. This chapter addresses important characteristics of qualitative research and qualitative data. Strategies and procedures to handle the analytical challenges are also dealt with, as well as validity and reliability issues in qualitative research. 8.1 Characteristics of Qualitative Research Previously we have emphasized that a key purpose of qualitative research is to understand and gain insights (see Section 2.5). Qualitative research is particularly relevant when prior insights about the phenomenon under scrutiny are modest, implying that qualitative research tends to be exploratory and flexible because the problems tend to be ‘unstructured ’. Even though qualitative research may allow for tests of hypotheses, the main emphasis is usually on gaining insights and constructing explanations or theory. In the terminology of Popper (1961), qualitative research is particularly relevant in the ‘context of discovery’ (see Sections 5.2, 8.2). Qualita- fi tive research approaches are multiple and no agreed-upon classi cation of the various approaches or methods exists. Van Maanen (1979: 520) also claims that [the] label qualitative methods has no precise meaning in any of the social sciences. It is at best an umbrella term covering an array of interpretative techniques which seek to describe, decode, translate, and otherwise come to terms with the meaning, not the frequency, of certain more or less naturally occurring phenomena in the social world. 130 Qualitative Data Analysis The distinction between quantitative and qualitative data is not an easy one – and disagreements prevail. As noted above, qualitative research tends to be more explorative and unstructured, with emphasis on understanding, while quantitative research tends more to emphasize descriptions and testing of derived hypotheses. In ‘traditional quantitative’ research, data collection and analysis are usually sequential. By contrast, in qualitative research, data collection and analysis are often conducted simultaneously in an interactive way: collected data are analysed, which raises new (evidence) clari fied. questions, are initiating collected and further analysed, data the collection. problem As more becomes data gradually fi An analogy is the effort of a detective trying to nd the person guilty of a crime. Qualitative Data 8.2 Data are carriers of information. To become information they must, however, be interpreted. A common observation is that in qualitative studies, the researcher easily becomes overwhelmed by the masses of data. One reason for this is that when the research problem is badly (modestly) understood, many data are collected, of which a large proportion may be irrelevant. Thus, redundancy of data is often the case. Miles (1979: 590) has even termed qualitative data as ‘attractive are usually associated with numbers. data fic possess speci As nuisance’. noted Quantitative above, numbers characteristics that make them very useful for analytical fi purposes. They can be seen as an ef cient way to represent information and meanings. Often, however, the research problem is less well understood and the underlying concepts may be ambiguous. Assigning numbers to something we do not know and understand makes no sense. In such situations, qualitative data may be useful to gain insights and understand concepts. Qualitative data come in many forms, such as pictures, texts, and verbal reports. Such data are rich, full, earthy, holistic, and‘real’ (Miles, 1979: 590). Such data are in many ways closer to the problem/phenomenon studied. In a famous study, Burns and Stalker (1961) were interested to understand why some firms were very innovative while others were not, despite thefirms belonging to the same industry, being of approximately the same size, and located in the same region. In this case they had adequate information about fi the dependent variable, innovativeness, that is, the extent to which the rms 8.3 Analytical Procedures EXAMPLE 8.1 Management System ‘Mechanistic’ ‘Organic’ – specialized differentiation of – contributive nature of special knowledge functional tasks and experience to common tasks – abstract nature of individual – ‘realistic’ nature of individual task set by tasks the situation adjustment and continuous fi – immediate superiors rede nition of individual tasks fi – precise de nition of rights and obligations – spread of commitment – network structures of control, authority, – translation of rights and and communication obligations into responsibilities – hierarchic structure of control, – lateral direction of communication – information and advice rather than authority, and communication instructions – vertical interaction – instructions by superiors Source: Burns and Stalker (1961: 120–1) introduced novel products and patents. Their insights regarding why the firms differed in this way were, however, highly limited. Detailed study of a sample of highly innovative and non-innovative two groups of firms differed firms revealed that the flow of innovativefirms as greatly in organizational structure and information (see Example 8.1). They termed the highly ‘organic’ and the non-innovative as ‘mechanistic ’. These terms are summary labels of the characteristics shown in Example 8.1. Inspection of Example 8.1 shows that meanings are expressed through fi words: the non-standardized collected data here are classi ed into two broad categories, ‘organic’ and ‘mechanistic’, and the analysis is conducted through conceptualization. The two labels ‘organic’ and ‘mechanistic’ are used as flow of ‘shorthand’ to capture the differences in organizational structure and information for the two groups of firms, innovative and non-innovative respectively. 8.3 Analytical Procedures A key – if not the key – purpose of analysis is to understand and gain insights from the collected data, or, as stated by Marshall and Rossman (1995: 111), 131 132 Qualitative Data Analysis Data collection Data display Data reduction Conclusions: drawing/verifying Figure 8.1 Components of data analysis: interactive model Source : Miles and Huberman (1994: 23) ‘data analysis is the process of bringing order, structure and meaning to the mass of collected data’. As noted above, in qualitative studies, the researcher is often overwhelmed by the mass of data. A key characteristic of analysis is the dividing up or breaking down of some complex whole into its constituent parts (i.e. from the Greek, ana-, ‘up, throughout’, and lyein, ‘to loosen’). Through analytical operations the researcher dissects, reduces, sorts, and reconstitutes data. Researchers use analysis to manipulate data to gain understanding, clarify problems, and test hypotheses. No single, agreedupon approach to qualitative data analysis exists. However, it is possible to give some rather general and helpful advice. For example, in their recognized ‘source book’ Miles and Huberman (1994) distinguish between the following components in qualitative data analysis (see Figure 8.1): • data reduction; • data display; • conclusion drawing and veri cation. Data reduction fi refers to the process of selecting, focusing, simplifying, fi abstracting, and transforming the data that appear in writing up eld notes or transcriptions. For example, to be manageable and to give meaning, lengthy transcripts from focus group interviews must not only be read, the researcher must also focus, simplify, and abstract to create meaning from the mass of words. 8.3 Analytical Procedures Table 8.1 133 An example of a matrix display in case study analysis Atmosphere Cooperation and con Process Power and flict Distance dependence Expectation Offer Informal meetings Final offer Planning for negotiations Formal negotiations I Internal meetings Formal negotiations II Source: Ghauri and Firth (2009) fi In this phase the researcher generates categories and identi es themes and patterns. As categories and patterns between them in the data are gradually uncovered, understanding or explanation of the phenomenon studied may emerge. However, the researcher should be extremely careful and critical in this phase, because other explanations are in most cases possible. This is important, because the key purpose is to arrive at valid explanations (see Kirk and Miller, 1986, for an excellent discussion). Ghauri and Firth (2009) suggest that matrices can be used to display data and to explain the relationship between different variables (see Table 8.1 in Section 8.5.3). Data display is an organized, compressed assembly of information that permits drawing conclusions and taking action. Data display can be in the form of strategy a data process matrix, may figures, display and the so on. major For events example, and/or a study actors in of a that process. In the following sections such activities will be dealt with in greater detail. We first describe various analytical activities, and then turn to interpretation, which is at the heart of qualitative research. 134 Qualitative Data Analysis 8.3.1 Analytical Activities Here we distinguish between: • categorization; • abstraction; • comparison; • dimensionalization; • integration; • iteration; • refutation. 1 Categorization is the process of classifying units of data. Qualitative researchers categorize data during the process of coding. The essence of categorization is identifying a chunk or unit of data (e.g. a passage of text of any length) as belonging to, representing, or being an example of some more general phenomenon. Categorization involves naming, or giving labels to, instances of the phenomenon found in the data. A passage categorized fi with a speci c label may be a few words or many pages long. A passage may also exemplify different categories of interest to the analyst and thus have many labels. Usually some part of the text, for example a transcribed interview, will contain no meaningful information and remain uncategorized. Codes serve as shorthand devices to label, separate, compile, and organize data. Coding of qualitative data differs somewhat from the coding of quantitative data, for example survey data. When handling quantitative data, coding is mostly a way of managing the data, while in qualitative data analysis – in particular in situations where the analysis departs from inspection of the collected data (see Section 8.5.2)– coding is an important first step in arriving at understanding and generating theory. Several approaches to coding exist. Probably the most elaborate is that developed by Strauss and Corbin (1990) drawing on the‘grounded theory’ approach (see Section 8.5). These authors distinguish between three types of coding: open coding, axial coding, and selective coding. • Open coding – ‘the process of breaking down, examining, comparing, conceptualizing and categorizing data’ (1990: 61); this process of coding yields concepts, which are later to be grouped and turned into categories. 1 This section builds on Spiggle (1994). 8.3 Analytical Procedures • Axial coding – ‘a set of procedures whereby data are put back together in new ways after open coding, by making connections between categories ’ (1990: 96). This is done by linking codes to contexts, to consequences, to patterns of interaction, and to causes. • Selective coding – ‘the procedure of selecting the core category, system- atically relating it to other categories, validating those relationships, and filling in categories (1990: 116). A that need core category further fi re nement and development’ is the central issue or focus around which all other categories are integrated. It is what Strauss and Corbin call the storyline that frames the account. The three types of coding are really different levels of coding, and each relates to a different point in the elaboration of categories in grounded theory. Qualitative researchers do not generally specify the unit of analysis (although it is done in some cases). Rather they categorize a chunk of data on the basis of its coherent meaning– its standing on its own – and not by an arbitrary designation of grammar. It is also quite common in qualitative fi research for initial categories and codes to be changed and re ned during the research process. Abstraction builds on categorization. It surpasses categorization in that it collapses more empirically grounded categories into higher-order con- fi ceptual constructs. Abstraction goes beyond the identi cation of patterns fi in the data. It groups previously identi ed categories into more general conceptual classes. Abstraction includes both incorporating more concrete categories into fewer, more general, ones, and recognizing that a unit of data is an empirical indicator of a more general construct of interest. For example, a researcher studying consumers’ purchasing behaviour might, based on transcribed interviews, make categories for ‘reading of advertisements ’, ‘brochures ’, ‘ store visit’, and ‘asking for advice’, and then relate these categories to a more general and abstract one– ‘information search ’. Abstract constructs encompass several more concrete instances found in fi the data that share certain common features. The theoretical signi cance of a construct partly depends on its relationship with other constructs. The reason amount of search importance of the purchase is that most research is relational. For example: Is purchase situation related to the Comparison explores differences in a ? and similarities across incidents within the data currently collected and provides guidelines for collecting additional data. 135 136 Qualitative Data Analysis Comparison begins in the initial stage of the analysis as one categorizes and abstracts data. While categorizing, the investigator notes general simi- fi larities in the speci c empirical instances in the data and labels them as representing the same category. As the analysis proceeds, the investigator may conduct comparisons in a systematic way. EXAMPLE 8.2 A researcher is studying purchasing behaviour in a business firm. Among other things, s/he is observing various search and evaluation activities for the purchase of the same products. To examine whether search and evaluation activities are related to purchase experience, s/he makes the following comparison: Purchase product A: First Last Bid Yes No Price evaluation Yes No Brochures No Yes No Advertisements No Yes No fi Here we see that more search and evaluation are conducted in the rst than fi in the last purchase. This may indicate learning and simpli cation of purchases through experience. Glaser and Strauss (1967) pioneered the constant comparative method as an analytical procedure in which the analyst compares incidents in the data with other incidents appearing to belong to the same category, exploring their similarities and differences. As the analysis proceeds and categories develop, the investigator compares incidents in the data with appropriate emergent categories. Comparison processes also guide subsequent data collection. As investigators identify categories, constructs and preliminary propositions, and fi conceptual linkages from the initial analyses, they may de ne subsequent individuals or groups to sample that maximize or minimize differences 8.3 Analytical Procedures between them on variables of interest. This procedure allows them to control for, or manipulate, similarities and differences in conditions, outcomes, or information characteristics in a way that is analogous to that in experimental and survey designs. EXAMPLE 8.3 A researcher is doing a detailed tracking of budgeting in small, privately owned restaurants and notes that the restaurants do not prepare formal budgets (as prescribed in the textbooks). S/he wonders whether this is something particular to this industry and proceeds to collect the same information from other small, privately owned service findings, s/he may proceed example larger fi rms or fi rms from on the observed fi rm, for This way of proceeding is an example of fi rms. Depending to include other types of manufacturing purposive or industries. theoretical sampling. Dimensionalization involves identifying properties of categories and con- fi structs. Once a category has been de ned, the researcher may explore its attributes or characteristics along one or more dimensions. The properties represent conceptual dimensions that vary empirically in the data across the incidents depicting the constructs. Strauss and Corbin (1990: 72) offer the following hypothetical example: Category Properties Dimensional range (applied to each incident) Watching frequency often . . . never extent more . . . less intensity high . . . low duration long . . . short In this example, the category ‘watching’ is described by four properties, each one dimensionalized along a continuum. 137 138 Qualitative Data Analysis Dimensionalization is important for theory construction in the following ways. By systematically exploring empirical variations across incidents representing a construct, the researcher can clarify and enrich the conceptual fication meaning. Identi the researcher to of properties and their dimensions can explore and fi de ne relationships across also help categories and constructs. Integration is important in efforts to build theory based on data, and departs by noting in the data that certain conditions, contexts, strategies, and outcomes tend to cluster together. Integration requires the mapping of relationships between conceptual elements. Iteration involves moving through data collection and analysis in such a way that preceding operations shape subsequent ones. In doing so, the researcher fi does not perform speci c research stages in a sequential order, but moves back and forth between stages. An example is simultaneous data collection and data analysis as emphasized above. This is in contrast to quantitative research, where the various stages tend to be performed sequentially. Refutation involves deliberately subjecting one’s emerging inferences – categories, constructs, propositions, or conceptual framework – to empirical scrutiny. To do so, the researcher may make use of a negative case or negative fi incident to discon rm the emerging analysis. A main reason for trying to firm is to avoid the so-called confirmation bias discon Hint: ’ (Bazerman, 1998). ‘ It is important to note that assumptions and hypotheses are not tested by seeking supporting evidence but by seeking information that allows for fi discon rmation. EXAMPLE 8.4 In their well-known book In Search of Excellence, Peters and Waterman fi (1982) made an effort to come up with characteristics for excellent rms, such as that successful fi rms were headed by managers who walked around – talking to the employees. In their study they only included successful fi rms, at that time – which makes it impossible to conclude whether the sug- gested criteria hold true. To hold true, the probability of success should be higher when a given characteristic is present, the characteristic is not present, P(S/C i). P(S/ci), compared with when 8.4 Interpretation 8.3.2 Data Reduction The above analytical operations – categorization, abstraction, comparison, dimensionalization, integration, iteration, and refutation – provide a way of managing analysis of qualitative data. Even though the researcher cannot program these operations in a mechanical fashion, s/he can proceedsystem- atically. One way of doing this is to read through all the data before rereading records for each case. Alternatively, the researcher can read and retract the record for each case before going to subsequent cases. Also, as one categorizes, abstracts, compares, and integrates, one can move through the data horizontally (e.g. grouping indicators of categories), or vertically (e.g. grouping records of cases), or both sequentially. Tabulations can be helpful in promoting systematic comparisons, for example listing incidents that represent a construct across cases. The purpose of proceeding systematically is to stimulate a complete analysis and minimize potential distortion from selective use of the data. Record. In analysing qualitative data, the researcher is strongly encouraged to keep records charts, and of the analysis in process in the form of memos, journals, other documents. Records may be of two types: those that condense, summarize, and integrate data displays and documents thatfacili- tate the analysis, and records that preserve the construction of inference. Such records can be memos about insights and ideas, notes, and summaries. Report. In both quantitative and qualitative research it is common to report the procedures applied for collecting and analysing data. A reason for doing so is that others should realistically be able to follow and evaluate the underlying logic. 8.4 Interpretation Analytical procedures are applied to manipulate data. Through interpretation, the researcher makes sense of the collected (and manipulated) data. Compared with data analysis, interpretation tends to be more intuitive and subjective. Interpretation may occur in many ways, and involves the use of some ‘lenses’, that is, some conceptual or theoretical ‘glasses ’. A key point is that sense is made of data by using some concepts, perspective, or theory. One example is the use of metaphors. A metaphor requires us to suspend our ordinary frame of reference for viewing some aspect of the world‘as is’, and, instead, view it ‘as if’. In using a metaphor as a lens we seek correspondence, 139 140 Qualitative Data Analysis similarities, and identities between domains. One domain is compared with or seen in terms of another. The comparison involves a mapping of elements of one domain on to those of the other. Metaphors not only play an important role in research but are also of crucial importance for how people make sense of their observations and experiences (for an excellent discussion, see Lakoff and Johnson, 1980). Grasping the meanings of others. In qualitative research the investigator often attempts to understand informants by grasping a concept, an experience, or an idea in their terms. In doing so, the researcher grasps the informants ’ meaning by ‘translating’ their sayings into her/his own insights and understanding. This implies seeking correspondence between the informants ’ and the researcher’s experiences and understanding. Seeking patterns in meaning. Researchers often represent the meaning and experiences of informants as forming coherent patterns. They do this by fi aggregating them into larger wholes and identifying uni ed themes by which the individual informants construct their worlds and more generalized patterns that characterize their sample. 8.5 Strategies for Qualitative Analysis From the above discussion, it is apparent that there are several approaches to analysing qualitative data. A distinction can be made between whether the research departs from existing theory (deductive), or whether the analyses mainly depart from the observations/data (inductive). 8.5.1 Departure from Theory In qualitative research the investigator may – and often does – depart from some theory. exploratory, This may sound discovery-oriented strange. However, research with a even focus when on conducting understanding and construction of explanations, the investigator often holds prior assumptions and ‘hunches’ about critical factors and relationships. By taking such assumptions and hunches into account, the research may fi bene t from theorizing at the outset, which can be helpful to get some direction for how to proceed (cf. Zaltman et al., 1982). Two things should be noted. First, the a priori assumptions and hunches should guide and direct, not dictate, the research. Second, even though some a priori theory may exist, it is assumed that a priori insights should not allow for complete structuring and explan- fi ation of the research problem. If so, the research could probably bene t from structured tests of hypotheses (see Section 5.2). 8.5 Strategies for Qualitative Analysis EXAMPLE 8.5 Firms make use of technologies, and these can be described in various ways. One way is the flexibility of a firm’ s production technology, that is, how fi the technology allows for by using specific input. In a study, a researcher asked whether technological fl exibility infl uences fi rms’ buying strategies. Here the researcher believed that technological flexibility did influence firms’ many types of input can produce a speci c output, and how many outputs buying strategies, but s/he did not know exactly how, and therefore conducted a discovery-oriented qualitative study. Inspection of Figure differently in the two 8.2 firms. shows that problem recognition occurs very In Alpha, information about expected increases in the prices is the stimulus for the purchasing department to start the search fi for raw materials. It should also be noted that in this rm no alternative for the type of raw materials used exists. In Beta, the buying process is triggered by a sales order. In this fi rm, multiple alternatives for raw materials were perceived to be possible. Is it possible to explain the two apparently different fl buying strategies re ected in Figure 8.2? The actual study focused on the role of technological fl exibility, here conceived as bindings between input and output possibilities. In Alpha, the perceived technological possibilities of changing to other combinations of input and output factors were few. Thus the ‘proactive’ behaviour in Alpha can be seen as a way of‘buffering’ against environmental changes. In Beta, a high degree of technological iour. Its flexibility fl exibility allows for ‘reactive’ behav- allows for immediate adjustments (Grønhaug, 1999). (1) Alpha Environmental information - Sales budget expected increase in prices (start) Purchase dept Order (Sales) (2) Beta Headquarters production (Specification) (start) Purchase dept Figure 8.2 Problem recognition – raw materials (last purchase) 141 142 Qualitative Data Analysis 8.5.2 Departure from Data (Observations) The opposite strategy is to start from the data and let inspection of the data result in explanation. In its extreme form this is what has been termed grounded theory, that is ‘theory derived from data, systematically gathered and analysed’ (Strauss and Corbin, 1990: 121). An important point is that theory is needed in any type of research. To make sense of data (observations), theory is needed. In fact, the quality of qualitative research is highly dependent on the investigator’s ability to observe, conceptualize, and theorize. As noted above, qualitative research implies intensive interplay between data (observations) and theory (see Section 2.5). Over the years, the original grounded theory (Glaser and Strauss, 1967) has fied. been modi It has also been criticized. One objection is that theory- neutral observations are hardly feasible, and what we‘see’ when conducting fl research is in uenced by multiple factors, one of which is what we already know about the social world being studied. (For more details, see Bryman, 2001; Flyvbjerg, 2006; Gioia et al., 2012.) 8.5.3 Analysing Case Studies One way to analyse data collected through case study methods is to look for commonalities and differences, for example in situations where there are many cases. As stated by Boyd et al. (1985: 51): In one study to improve the productivity of the sales force of a particular company, the investigator studied intensively two or three of the best sales representatives and two or three of the worst. Data was collected on the background and experience of each representative and then several days were spent making sales calls with them. As a result, a hypothesis was developed that checking the stock of retailers and suggesting items on which they were low were the most important differences between the successful and the poor sales representatives. find answers to our questions As illustrated by the above example, we can by comparing different cases. In some cases, comparing the best and worst cases is the most suitable approach. In the same manner, we could analyse the performance of several units/branches of a company by comparing them with each other. Campbell (1975) and Ghauri (2004) refer to‘pattern matching’ where information assumptions. If we can from find several cases could be related to a priori fi a systematic pattern, we can accept or con rm our assumption or proposition. We do not need statistical testing to come to fi the conclusions. The requirement is that the pattern has to be suf ciently systematic. Patterns can be found through single case analysis and then 8.6 Quantifying Qualitative Data fi through case comparison. It can be bene cial to use a matrix to present data and to show the analysis and patterns in case study research as this helps to show the relationship between different concepts fluences what. This is illustrated in Table 8.1. and to establish what in In the case of descriptive case studies, this does not mean that the purpose is to collect and present only the facts. As put by Ferber et al. (1964: 153): Facts do not lead anywhere. Indeed facts, as facts, are the commonest, cheapest and most useless of all commodities. Anyone with a questionnaire can gather thousands of facts a day – and probably not find much real use for them. What makes facts practical and valuable is the glue of explanation and understanding, the framework of theory, the tie-rod of conjecture. Only when facts can be fleshed to a skeletal theory do they become meaningful in the solution of problems. fi In descriptive research, the researcher has to work with speci c research problems, propositions, or hypotheses. While exploratory study is characterized by requires a flexibility, clear descriptive fi speci cation study of the can who, be considered what, fi why, more and rigid. how of It the research problem. For further details of speci c techniques for data analysis for descriptive studies, the reader should look into Miles and Huberman (1994). Quantifying Qualitative Data 8.6 fication can be relevant in qualitative research. This may be the case, Quanti for example, when the researcher wants to capture the frequency of certain fl events. To gain insights into whether computer models in uence strategic decision making, a study was conducted in which a management team was asked to solve a strategic task with and without a computer tool. The team was taped and video-taped while solving the task. A part of the analysis was to examine the concepts used (because concepts held and used relate to what is captured). Table 8.2 reports the concepts mentioned and used before (without the computer) and after (with the computer). In this case, the researchers knew that managers hold concepts, but not what concepts they hold and use. fi fi fi Also, assume that an identi ed concept is gradually re ned and de ned. Over time, quantitative measures may be developed to capture the underlying phenomenon empirically (see Chapter 6). Hypotheses may also be tested statistically in qualitative research. 143 144 Qualitative Data Analysis Table 8.2 Concept list Concepts Number of concepts Number of concepts Mentioned Used in discussions Before After Before After Management of the Norwegian shelf 7 1 7 1 Goals 2 0 2 0 Role of the authorities 5 1 5 1 Producer economic results 4 7 2 7 Pro 1 1 1 1 Price 1 1 1 1 Income 0 2 0 2 Cost 2 3 0 3 Transportation costs 1 1 1 Production costs 1 1 1 1 1 fit Marginal costs Supply (production) 18 13 17 13 Producer behaviour 4 3 4 3 Actors 8 7 8 7 1 1 0 1 Volumes 5 2 5 2 Production capacity 1 1 1 1 Production volume 1 1 1 1 Surplus/scarcity 1 1 Seasonal variation (winter sale) 1 1 Take back gas clause 1 1 Demand (consumption) 9 16 7 16 Actors 5 13 3 13 Import quota 0 1 0 1 Growth 2 1 2 1 Gas fields 8.6 Quantifying Qualitative Data 145 Table 8.2 (cont.) Concepts Number of concepts Number of concepts Substitutes 2 0 2 0 Volumes 0 1 0 1 0 3 0 3 Trade pattern – changes Norway 1/Norway 2 1 1 Norway 1 and 2/other producers 1 1 Market share 1 1 Pipelines 1 4 Landing node 1 1 1 1 Capacity 1 1 Utilization 1 1 Shadow price 1 1 1 Number of superordinate categories Number of concepts Source : 0 4 5 6 4 6 39 44 33 44 Fuglseth and Grønhaug (2003). Copyright © 2003, with permission from Elsevier. EXAMPLE 8.6 fi An anthropologist observes a speci c behaviour among a group of people and advances the assumption (hypothesis) that this is due to a particular condition. When the fi speci c condition occurs, nothing happens. The researcher drops the initial hypothesis and advances a second one, and so on. At his nth trial to advance a hypothesis, the observation is as expected. fi Moreover, every time the expected condition is present, the speci c behaviour is observed. Assume that the researcher has observed the expected behaviour in sent. With fi ve fi subsequent situations when the speci c condition is pre- no prior information, this implies the probability outcome happening by chance is: of such an P(B = 5) = 0.031, p = 0.5, n = 5; that is, the likelihood of making such an observation by chance is a little higher than 3 per cent. 146 Qualitative Data Analysis Validity in Qualitative Research 8.7 In earlier chapters we emphasized the importance of validity, and that validity is also required in qualitative research (see Sections 5.4 and 6.4). Validity concerns are challenging to handle. In qualitative research the following types of validity are often emphasized: • descriptive; • interpretative; • theoretical; • generalizable. Descriptive validity refers to the degree to which the actual description holds true. For example, is the description of a strategy process or the buying process in Figure 8.2 correct: that is, does it capture the buying process in Alpha and Beta as it took place? Interpretative validity refers to how good the interpretation is. Is the expressed interpretation the correct one? For example, is the explanation regarding technological Theoretical validity flexibility suggested above valid? refers to the adequacy of our suggested ‘theory’ or explanation. For example, based on a substantial research effort, a researcher suggests an explanation (a theory) of how firms cope with external uncer- tainties. Does the suggested theory hold true? By generalizable validity we mean to what extent thefindings from a study can be generalized to other settings. For example, the suggested explanation of how firms among small cope with external uncertainties was firms based on observation in one industry. Can the explanation be generalized to hold true also for large firms or firms in other industries? Validity claims: It is not enough only to talk about validity. Validity must also be demonstrated. For example, in a study you claim that a power game ‘ was going on in the company’. The term ‘power game’ must be explained (on finitions, conceptual de power game really see Chapter 3). Moreover, evidence that the claimed was going on must be reported. Validity claims are responses to the question: How can I trust you? 8.8 The Use of Computers in Qualitative Research Analysis of qualitative data is tedious and time consuming. However, over the past 15 years, various software programs have been designed to facilitate data analysis, assisted known qualitative collectively data analysis by the acronym software). Some CAQDAS (computer- well-known computer programs for qualitative research are ATLAS.ti, NVivo and QSR NUD*IST. 8.9 Case Studies and Triangulation These software packages are able to systematically organize and analyse qualitative data. NVivo, for example, allows for grouping and linking of concepts support through the use codes, of nodes, NVivo to master-lists, enhance the and trees. Several trustworthiness of scholars qualitative analysis (Kelle, 1997; Miles and Huberman, 1994; Sinkovics et al., 2008). It not only ficient, ef makes the coding and retrieval of text much faster and more but also enhances the transparency of the analysis and supports the theory building beyond description, and allows us to avoid the criticism of anecdotalism in qualitative research (Silverman, 1993). NVivo has become the most widely used CAQDAS as it provides procedural advantages in the management of textual data (Ghauri and Firth, 2009; King et al., 1994). The decision to use NVivo or not should be taken at an early stage, before data collection. When using NVivo, data collection should be standardized. Data collected through interviews should be digitally recorded and transcribed. Data collected through multiple sources, secondary and primary, should be gathered in one place. This will facilitate the data analysis when data are organized, coded, and reduced into a manageable system so that interpretations can be made to draw conclusions. NVivo facilitates this process through nodes, and makes it easier to examine the nodes at any time and bring them together, even from different cases. It shows the frequency of different concepts in all cases. Thus, in a final stage it helps in the interpretation and building of models, propos- itions, and theories (Ghauri and Firth, 2009; Sinkovics et al., 2008). Table 8.3 summarizes the fi bene ts of using NVivo in international business (IB) research. 8.8.1 A Word of Warning To make proper use of such programs, the researcher must of course know how to analyse data and have command over how to use the program. Probably even more important is to have an intimate personal knowledge of the data, so that appropriate themes, nodes, codes, and responses can be inserted at the outset of data interpretation. Such knowledge can best come from personal involvement in the entire process, from data collection through to analysis. Moreover, the researcher needs to get some training in the particular program used. 8.9 Case Studies and Triangulation Triangulation refers to the combination of methodologies in the study of the same phenomenon. Through triangulation we can improve the accuracy of our judgements and thereby our results, by collecting data through different 147 148 Qualitative Data Analysis Table 8.3 The advantages of using NVivo in IB research Characteristics of IB research Implications for analysis of case studies Advantage of using NVivo Data saturation – advances Case studies can become All types of data can be in technology mean the increasingly large and less neatly stored and organized researcher can collect more manageable with a single program, data on a global scale increasing clarity and focus of analysis Research may be undertaken Interpretations of cases may NVivo offers a single by a variety of different differ between researchers and platform in which all researchers based in ideas may not be researchers can work with different countries communicable across borders individual passwords. Data in real time in order to cross- can then easily be emailed so check the data researchers can view/alter others ’ ideas in an iterative process which is less subjective Language barriers/difference Case studies may not be Newer versions of NVivo in language of data collected comparable, and validity of will accept languages other data may be compromised than English. Different codes and categories can be used to show which language was used and retain equivalence of the data The unit of analysis often Case studies must recognize NVivo allows categorization differs i.e. HQ/subsidiary/ the differences in units of of units of analysis, and local analysis and compare issues direct comparisons between according to separate accounts them are possible on a single from individual units screen Time constraints of Case study analysis may be put NVivo allows initial analysis cross-border research on hold until ALL data are of the data even if collection collected, compromising the is not totally complete as quality of analysis alterations can easily be firm made at a later date, rather than the analysis having to be repeated IB research is Case studies can be rich with NVivo can store all sources multidisciplinary and likely data however, linking different of data, including text, to make greater use of a sources may be dif cult webpages, electronic variety of sources of data fi files, videos, pictures, and audio files, and link them together in terms of their coding Source : Ghauri and Firth (2009: 36). 8.9 Case Studies and Triangulation methods or even by collecting different kinds of data on the subject matter of our study. The discussion on validity is particularly relevant here. Sometimes to enhance the validity of our research we need to collect or analyse data through triangulation. In cases where correctness or precision is important, it is quite logical to collect information through different methods and from different angles. The following story illustrates what we mean. Three blind men were asked to describe an elephant by touching or feeling only a part of it. We can well imagine what they might have described by touching different parts of an elephant, such as a foot, an ear, or a trunk. This illustrates that, in many cases, one method alone cannot be enough to explain or describe a phenomenon, and we need to use a multi-method approach to capture the whole reality. The use of multiple methods or triangulation is not new and can be traced back to Campbell and Fiske (1959), who argued that to ensure validation one should use more than one method. The main advantage of triangulation, however, is that it can produce a more complete, holistic, and contextual portrait of the object under study. This is particularly important in the case study method as you need to check and validate the information you receive fi from various sources. For example, you can check the performance of a rm or a project claimed by an interviewee through examining the annual accounts or archives or by interviewing another manager or the company auditors. However, there are some fi problems with triangulation. For example, sometimes it can be dif cult to judge whether the results from different methods are consistent or not. A second problem arises when the different methods come up with contradictory results. Sometimes researchers may prefer or emphasize one method over another: for example, quantitative versus qualitative. However, all research methods have advantages and disadvantages when it comes to different research problems. Our conclusion is that triangulation or the use of a multi-method approach on the same study object can be useful even if we do not get the same results. It can lead us to a better understanding or to new questions that can be answered by later research. Triangulation is particularly useful in international business research to check for method bias. Using multiple methods to examine the same phe- fi nomenon and checking for consistencies enhances our con dence in the analysis and results. Here we could compare the data and results obtained through an experiment and with those gained through interviews. Convergence of results obtained through these different methods will enhance their reliability and validity. We could also compare results obtained through qualitative methods with statistical analysis. 149 150 Qualitative Data Analysis Case Studies and Generalization 8.10 fi Case studies are often considered less rigorous because their ndings cannot be generalized (Hagg and Hedlund, 1979; Hillebrand et al., 2001). Although fl this view is widely accepted, a number of fundamental and in uential studies in our field have been based on case studies. For example, Johanson and fi Vahlne’s (1977) study on the internationalization process of rms is based on fl four cases and has been one of the most in uential studies in international business. Some scholars also argue that case studies should be judged not on the basis of generalization, but on the basis of transferability and comparability (Chreim et al., 2007; Sinkovics et al., 2008). fi Case studies are generally de ned as ‘an empirical enquiry that investigates a contemporary phenomenon in depth and within its real-life context, especially when the boundaries between the phenomenon and the context fi are not clearly evident’ (Yin, 2009: 18). The generalization is often de ned as ‘a general statement or proposition made by drawing on inference from observation of the particular’ (Schwandt, 1997: 57). Generalization is thus fi an act of inference from speci c observation to general statements (Cohen and Nagel, 1934; Tsang, 2014). We follow Tsang (2014) in keeping our fi discussion on logic rather than on any speci c philosophical perspective such as positivism or interpretivism. According to Tsang (2014), the results of case studies may in fact be more generalizable than quantitative studies based on large survey data analysed by statistical methods. He particularly discusses three aspects of generalization: fi theoretical generalization, falsi cation, and empirical generalization. Case studies are particularly suitable for theoretical generalization (theory building) fi and falsi cation (theory testing) and not particularly inferior to quantitative studies in empirical generalization. However, different case study designs may fluence in the strength of generalization claims. For example, single case versus multiple cases and cross-sectional cases versus longitudinal cases. The evidence from multiple cases is considered more compelling than from a single case and a longitudinal case study provides more convincing evidence for theoretical generalization than a cross-sectional case (Tsang, 2014). In the same manner, falsi fication concerns theory testing: existing hypotheses or theory can be rejected on the basis of a case study. The famous story of ‘all swans are white’ was considered true for some time till just one fi observation of a black swan falsi ed the earlier statement/theory (Flyvbjerg, fi 2006). In empirical generalization, the idea is that ndings from a case can be applicable to the population from which the case is drawn (within a population). This means that to generate empirical generalization, it is better to choose cases that are representative of the particular population (Gobo, 2004). Figure 8.3 summarizes the relationship between case studies, theory 8.10 Case Studies and Generalization Aspect of Generalization Empirical generalization Identification of empirical regularities Theoretical generalization Theory Building Falsification Theory Testing Figure 8.3 Case study research: theory building and theory testing Source: Based on Flyvbjerg (2006). development, and theory testing. As we can see, the relationship between theory building and theory testing is quite close. However, case study research normally aims to achieve one or the other (Tsang, 2014). fi The above discussion deals with and clari es the common objection and a prevailing view that case studies cannot provide generalizable results. This view has been a major factor in many researchers avoiding case study research. Our discussion reveals that, in addition to Yin ’s (2009) internal validity, discuss construct the type validity and reliability, of generalization their the study case can study researcher can provide and (Gibbert Ruigrok, 2010; Tsang, 2014). Moreover, case study researchers can claim that they seek to investigate phenomena in their context and not independent of context (Gibbert et al., 2008). 151 152 Qualitative Data Analysis Further Reading ........................................................................ Ghauri, P. (2004) ‘ Designing and conducting case studies in international business research’ , in Marshan-Piekkari, R. and Welch, C. (eds.),A Handbook of Qualitative Research Methods for International Business, Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar, pp. 109– 24. Miles, M.B. and Huberman, A.M. (1994) Qualitative Data Analysis, 2nd edn., Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Sinkovics, R.R., Penz, E., and Ghauri, P.N. (2008)‘Enhancing the trustworthiness of qualitative research in international business’, Management International Review, 48(6): 689–714. Tsang, E.W.K. (2014) ‘ Generalizing from research findings: The merits of case studies’, International Journal of Management Reviews, 16(4): 369–83. Questions ........................................................................ 1. What do you consider to be the key characteristics of qualitative research? 2. Explain what is meant by coding. Also explain the role of coding in analysing qualitative data. 3. What is meant by suggesting that qualitative data are an ‘attractive nuisance ’? 4. What do you think are the main advantages and disadvantages in departing from theory in qualitative research? 5. How can triangulation make our research more reliable? 6. What is meant by theoretical generalization? Give examples. Exercises ........................................................................ 1. You wonder whether young women and young men differ in their evaluations and behaviours in their purchase of a new car. Suggest how you would design a study to explore your research problem. Also explain what data you will gather and how you would do this to solve your research problem. 2. Assume you decide to gather information through (modestly) unstructured interviews. Prepare, conduct, and tape-record one such interview. Transcribe the interview, read, code, and analyse it. 9 Data Collection for Quantitative Research The goal is to turn data into information, and information into insight. Carly Fiorina In Chapter 6, we considered various issues related to the measurement of the variables to be used in quantitative data analysis. Here we turn our attention to the collection of the data. A distinction is usually made between primary data and secondary data. Secondary data are data that have already been collected by government • agencies, market research agencies, firms, or other organizations or individuals, and which are publicly available. For the researcher, the main advantage of using secondary data is that they have already been collected, and the researcher simply needs to collate the data (often this may just involve downloading from a website) in a suitable format for subsequent analysis. Primary data • are data that are collected by the researcher through fi surveys, interviews, or experiments speci cally for the research problem that is being studied. This means that the researcher is able to specify exactly which data should be collected and how much, and is able of the personally to verify the data. In this chapter, first we provide a more detailed discussion advantages and disadvantages of collecting and using secondary data and highlight some important secondary data sources. The next section then considers the advantages and disadvantages of collecting and using primary data. The following four sections are devoted to sampling. With secondary data, the researcher is obliged to accept the data that are publicly fl available, and is not able to in uence how the data are collected or how much data are collected. In contrast, the researcher collecting primary data needs to decide whether to survey the entire population or just a sample, to choose an appropriate sampling procedure, and to determine the sample size that will assure a empirical analysis. The satisfactory final level of precision in the subsequent two sections are then devoted to undertaking 154 Data Collection for Quantitative Research the two most common methods of primary data collection, viz: questionnaire surveys and experiments. Secondary Data 9.1 The first step for any researcher should be to look for secondary data sources relevant to the research project, before going out to collect primary data. As Churchill (1999: 215) advises, ‘Do not bypass secondary data. Begin with secondary data, and only when the secondary data are exhausted or show diminishing returns, proceed to primary data.’ Secondary data can help researchers in several ways: • answering research questions or solving research problems; • helping in problem formulation and devising more concrete and focused research questions; deciding on the appropriateness of particular research methods or sug- • gesting better research methods; providing benchmarking methods and other • findings that can be com- pared later on with the results from the study at hand. five broad categories. 1 Secondary data sources may be grouped into there is an enormous amount of data that is publicly First, available via the websites of supranational organizations (e.g. the World Bank, the World Trade Organization, UNCTAD, OECD) and independent research institutes (e.g. the Heritage Foundation, Transparency International) – see Table 9.1 for details. Most of these sources present data at a fairly high level of aggregation (i.e. country-level, or industry-level), though the national statistics bureaux of individual countries may be able to provide more disaggregated data. Examples would fi include the Bureau of Economic Analysis (United States), the Of ce for National Statistics (ONC, United Kingdom), the National Bureau of Statistics (China), and the Ministry of Statistics and Programme Implementation (India). Trade organizations and chambers of commerce may also be good sources of more disaggregated data. Second, more detailed 1 firm-level data may be found in internal Some secondary data may be available from may be undertaking research for a firms ’ annual accounts, but sources. For instance, the researcher firm and may have been provided with data on suppliers, customers, employees, marketing plans, invoices, minutes, production costs, and perhaps on competitors. A prudent researcher should treat this data with care, as only favourable data external may have been provided. Our emphasis in this chapter, however, is on data from sources where the researcher is able to collect whatever data s/he requires. 9.1 Secondary Data Table 9.1 155 Selected publicly available secondary data sources Organization Bank for Website www.bis.org Data available * Country-level data on international International banking, liquidity, credit, external debt, Settlements foreign exchange, consumer prices, property prices, etc. CIA www.cia.gov * World factbook * Country reports * Administrative, transportation etc. maps Columbia Center on http://ccsi.columbia.edu fi * Inward and outward FDI pro les for Sustainable selected countries Development * Data on leading MNEs for selected emerging economies European Union www.europa.eu * Country-level data for the Member States * Basic information about life and business in the European Union * Public opinion polls Heritage www.heritage.org * International trade freedom index www.ilo.org * Country-level data on formal and Foundation International Labour informal employment, hours of work, Organization labour costs, labour productivity, labour migration, etc. * Labour, social security and human rights legislation International www.imf.org Monetary Fund fl * Country-level data on trade, FDI, in ation rates, commodity prices, exchange rates, etc. * World Economic Outlook database * Links to many other relevant databases MSU-CIBER www.ciber.msu.edu * Market potential indices * Business climate rankings * The globalEDGE database OECD www.oecd.org * Country-level data for many countries, not just member countries * Economic surveys * Economic forecast summaries * Labour force statistics * Activities of multinational enterprises (AMNE) database 156 Data Collection for Quantitative Research Table 9.1 (cont.) Organization Data available Website Property Rights Alliance www .propertyrightsalliance.org * International property rights index Transparency International UNCTAD www.transparency.org * International corruption perceptions index * Country-level data for all countries on economic trends, international trade, FDI, migrants remittances, population, commodities, information economy, creative economy, and maritime transport * Country profiles www.unctadstat.unctad.org ’ World Bank www.worldbank.org World Economic Forum www.weforum.org World Trade Organization www.wto.org * Country-level data on agriculture & rural development, aid effectiveness, climate change, economy & growth, education, energy & mining, environment, external debt, financial sector, gender, health, infrastructure, poverty, private sector, public sector, science & technology, social development, social protection & labour, trade, and urban development. * Ease of doing business index * Worldwide governance indicators (voice & accountability, political stability, government effectiveness, regulatory quality, rule of law, control of corruption) * Global competitiveness rankings * Country-level indices on environmental footprints, inclusive development, gender gap, and income equality * Country-level data on international trade, tariff barriers, non-tariff measures, and global value chains * Value-added trade statistics there are several organizations (e.g. Bureau van Dijk) that collate such information into online databases which may be accessed on payment of a subscription. Bloomberg provides real-time market data for individualfirms. In addition, there are dozens of private organizations who can provide offthe-peg market research reports on different product sectors in different countries for a fee see Table 9.2 for a non-exclusive list. Third, useful data – 9.1 Secondary Data Table 9.2 Selected commercially available secondary data sources Organization Website Data available AC Nielsen Bloomberg Bureau van Dijk www.nielsen.com www.bloomberg.com www.bvdinfo.com * Expertise in consumer measurement * Firm-level real-time market data * Orbis database contains fi nancial, ownership, and M&A activity etc. data on 300m firms worldwide * Fame database contains fi nancial, ownership, and M&A activity etc. data on 11m firms in the UK & Ireland * Zephyr database contains detailed information on M&A, IPO, private equity and venture capital deals Euromonitor International Financial Times www.euromonitor .com www.ft.com * Market research information for many products in many countries * Several firm-level databases (e.g. on mergers and acquisitions) Ipsos www.ipsos.com Market Research .com Mintel www.marketresearch .com * Market research * Opinion polls * Market research reports for many products in many countries * Surveys of global market trends * Market research reports for many products in many countries * Surveys of global market trends www.mintel.com may be obtainable directly from other researchers, or via their personal websites. Some academic journals insist that authors publish their datasets so that others may check or replicate their results. Two examples of personal websites are (a) the site of Geert Hofstede w( ww.hofstede-insights.com), which provides data on his dimensions of national culture (i.e. power distance, uncertainty avoidance, individualism vs collectivism, masculinity vs femininity, long-term orientation, indulgence vs restraint) and enables the calculation of cultural distances between pairs of countries; and (b) the site of Pankaj Ghemawat (www.ghemawat.com), which enables the calculation not just of cultural distances, but also administrative, geographic, and economic distances between countries according to his CAGE model (Ghemawat, 2001). Fourth, big data is a fashionable contemporary term. Many firms, banks, social media sites, and search engines routinely collect huge amounts of data 157 158 Data Collection for Quantitative Research firms. These data are about the activities and preferences of individuals and collated and analysed for their own commercial purposes but may be made available to bona fide researchers. Last but not least, it is always possible to track down additional data sources through the use of search engines such as Google or Yahoo. 9.1.1 The Advantages of Secondary Data The major advantage of using secondary data is obviously the enormous savings in time and money. As noted above, huge amounts of secondary data are freely available on a wide variety of variables. There may be a cost involved for access to some commercial data, but this is unlikely to be signi ficant relative to the costs the researcher would incur in collecting the data by her/himself. Furthermore, much of the data published by government departments and supranational organizations will have been compiled and checked by specialists, and may reasonably be assumed to be collected on a consistent basis. This is particularly useful when data are presented for several different figures are comparable. countries, where it is imperative to ensure that the 9.1.2 The Disadvantages of Secondary Data There are also possible drawbacks to using secondary data. The first and main disadvantage is that the data were collected for purposes other than those of the researcher, hence the data may not map exactly on to the theoretical concepts in which the researcher is interested. For example, international business scholars often use data on foreign direct investment (FDI) stocks as proxy measures for the importance of multinational enterprises (MNE) in host countries. But FDI data relate to (net) funds transferred from the home country of the MNE to the host country, and do not take account of finance (e.g. though local equity or debt, or retained earnings) raised locally in the host country. Such data thus provide biased measures of filiate activity (Beugelsdijk et al., 2010; Kerner, 2014). MNE af The second disadvantage relates to the accuracy and reliability of the raw data. The ‘true and fair’ concept in auditing requires that firms financial ’ financial performance and situation of the firm, and are free of significant misrepresentations. But financial statements statements faithfully represent the can be massaged in both legal ways (e.g. to minimize tax liabilities) and illegal ways, as numerous financial scandals (e.g. Enron) testify. Finally, even when collated data are published by reputable organizations (such as those listed in Tables 9.1 and 9.2), there is often no way of checking how (if at all) the raw data were checked and cleaned before being published. The researcher is thus obliged to take responsibility for the data used (and the 9.2 Primary Data findings from any analysis) without being fully cognizant of their authenticity (Cooper and Schindler, 2001). 9.2 Primary Data Sometimes suitable secondary data sources are not available (as with many of the types of data listed below), or are inappropriate (for the reasons outlined in Section 9.1.2), or the researcher requires data for a new group of firms or individuals. Then the researcher is obliged to collect primary data though surveys, interviews, or experiments. Primary data come in many types: • Data on attributes. This category refers to demographic or socio- economic data, such as age, education level, profession, marital status, gender, classi income, fication or social class. These data are used for cross- of information as, for example, in market segmentation and positioning. Does the purchase/usage of a product depend upon age group? • Data on personality and lifestyle. This category refers to personality data fically, to and, more speci data that may be related to individual behav- fi iour. For example, different types of consumers might be identi ed: proactive vs reactive shoppers, or traditional vs innovative shoppers. • Data on attitudes and opinions. This category refers to individuals ’ fic products, ideas, objects, or issues, or to the individ- opinions on speci ual ’s cognitive behaviour. Such data may reveal future usage of particular products. • Data on awareness and knowledge. This category includes data on what is known about particular products, issues, or business activities. Such data are required if the researcher wants to find out what factors motiv- ate consumers to buy products, or the factors that stimulate opinions on issues, or the factors that motivate different activities. • Data on intentions. The data in this category relate to future actions and might, for example, be used to compare the intended purchases of consumers with their actual buying behaviour. • Data on motivations. The data in this category may reveal the motives or fluence stimuli that in consumers’ or managers’ behaviour. Motives are more stable than behaviour, and such data may help the researcher both understand past behaviour and predict future behaviour. • Data on behaviour. The data in this category relate to what the respondents have done or will be doing. 159 160 Data Collection for Quantitative Research 9.2.1 The Advantages of Primary Data There are three advantages associated with the collection of primary data. The first and main advantage is that the researcher is able to collect data with the exact objectives/questions of the project in mind, and hence is able to generate data that map exactly on to the theoretical concepts in which s/he is interested. Example #1: government policy makers are often interested in the factors that attract inward foreign direct investment. Secondary data on many potential factors (e.g. market size, labour quality, technological capabilities) are readily available, but, crucially, data on the motivations (i.e. market-seeking, efficiency-seeking, asset-seeking, natural resource-seeking) of the investing firms (Dunning and Lundan, 2008) are not, and thus require primary data collection. Example #2: market researchers often want to know about the attributes, personalities, lifestyles, and buying behaviours of potential customers when evaluating future market demand. Such data are typically not available from secondary sources, hence primary data collection is required. Example #3: many researchers in international entrepreneurship are interested in why some SMEs internationalize quickly and/or extensively (so-called ‘born globals’), while others do not, and believe that part of the explanation may be linked to the experientialknowledge of the firms and the personality characteristics of the founders. Again such data can not be obtained from secondary sources, so primary data collection is required. The second advantage is that the researcher can choose exactly who to approach for the required data, and how many units of observation are to be surveyed. In example #1, the most appropriate respondents would be the CEOs or board members of the investingfirms. In example #2, the appropriate respondents would be a sample of potential customers. In example #3, the appropriate respondents would be the founders of the SMEs. The issue of sample size will be discussed in Section 9.6 below. The third advantage is that the data will be collected directly by the researcher, or by her/his trusted nominee, who will be aware of the objectives of the study and possible nuances of interpretation, and should thus be able to collect accurate responses and thus have greater confidence in the final results. 9.2.2 The Disadvantages of Primary Data There are two main disadvantages with primary data collection. Thefirst is that the process of data collection is often slow and expensive, which can be a major issue if timely analysis is required or the researcher has a limited budget. The second is that it may be difficult to obtain suitable access or build a dataset of suitable size. Manyfirms (especially large, successful, and well-known firms) receive dozens of requests every year to participate in questionnaire surveys or to provide interviews with senior executives: most 9.3 Why Take Samples? are refused or simply ignored because the executives do not have the time, or because they do not feel the project merits attention. Unfortunately, though perhaps understandably, access is even more problematic for research students than for more senior researchers. Response rates are often low, and this may lead to small sample sizes (with problems for statistical inference) and/ or non-response bias (see Section 9.4.5). 9.3 Why Take Samples? One possibility when carrying out a research project is to survey the entire population of interest. The term population, as used here, refers to all possible units of observation, and these units may be people, countries, depending upon the context of the firms, project. For products, or instance, the project might involve the problems faced by SMEs wishing to export from their home country. Now, even if the home country was fairly small (e.g. the Netherlands), the number of SMEs is likely to be large (maybe tens of thousands). So, although it might in principle be possible to survey the population and collect primary data from all the SMEs within the Netherlands, such an approach would have two obvious drawbacks. First, such a survey would require a considerable amount of resources (including labour and money) to visit each SME and collect the requisite data, and such resources may not be available. Second, such a survey would require a considerable amount of time not just for data collection but for subsequent data processing, hence the findings from such a project may be delayed to an unacceptable degree. Hence it is often preferable to undertake a sample survey, in which a subset of the units of observation from the population 2 are selected for investigation. Sample surveys are cheaper and quicker than population surveys, but it is essential that the sample is selected in such a way that it is representative of fi the population from which it is drawn so that any ndings may be generalized to the population. In the example above, it would be important to select SMEs with a range of sizes, capabilities, and industries if the sample was to be representative of all Dutch SMEs. In assessing representativeness, ideally the researcher would like to have access to a sampling frame listing all the units in the population so that the characteristics of the sample and the population could be compared. See Example 9.1 for details of the basic terminology used in sampling. 2 Sample surveys are almost inevitable when collecting primary data, but it is often feasible to survey many more units of observation when using secondary data. 161 162 Data Collection for Quantitative Research EXAMPLE 9.1 • Population – the universe of units from which the sample is to be selected. The term ‘ units’ is employed because it is not necessarily people who are being sampled – the researcher may want to sample from a universe of nations, cities, regions, firms, etc. Thus, ‘population’ has a much broader meaning than the everyday use of the term, whereby it tends to be associated with a nation’s entire population. • Sample – the segment of the population that is selected for investigation. It is a subset of the population. The method of selection may be based on a probability or a non-probability approach (see below). • Sampling frame – the listing of all units in the population from which the sample will be selected. • fl Representative sample – a sample that re ects the population accurately so that it is a microcosm of the population. • Probability sample – a sample that has been selected using random selection so that each unit in the population has a known chance of being selected. It is generally assumed that a representative sample is more likely to be the outcome when this method of selection from the population is employed. The aim of probability sampling is to keep sampling error (see below) to a minimum. • Non-probability sample – a sample that has not been selected using a random selection method. Essentially, this implies that some units in the population are more likely to be selected than others. • Sampling error – the difference between a sample and the population from which it is selected, even though a probability sample has been selected. • Non-sampling error – differences between the population and the fi sample that arise either from de ciencies in the sampling approach, such as an inadequate sampling frame or non-response (see below), or from fl such problems as poor question wording, poor interviewing, or awed processing of data. • Non-response – a source of non-sampling error that is particularly likely to happen when individuals are being sampled. It occurs whenever some members of the sample refuse to cooperate, cannot be contacted, or for some reason cannot supply the required data (for example, because of mental incapacity). Source: Bryman and Bell (2003: 93) 9.4 Probability Sampling A sampling frame is essential for probability sampling, so that each unit has a known non-zero probability of being included in the sample. This then allows the researcher not only to construct a representative sample, but also to make fi statistical inferences about the population on the basis of the survey ndings. But often such a sampling frame does not exist or may be out of date, in which case it is not possible to calculate the probability that any one unit will appear in the sample. In such cases, the researcher has to rely on non-probability fi sampling and s/he will have less con dence when deriving inferences about the population. In our example above, there may be an up-to-date list of all Dutch SMEs, but such a list will quickly become inaccurate as new SMEs are established and old SMEs develop, are acquired, or go out of business. 9.4 Probability Sampling There are several techniques of probability sampling, of which the most widely used are: • • • • simple random sampling; systematic sampling; strati fied random sampling; cluster sampling. To illustrate the differences between these techniques of probability sampling, suppose the researcher wishes to interview some of the (approx) 2 million students studying at UK universities (of which there are about 130) as part of a market research project. Further suppose that an accurate sampling frame exists of these 2 million students. 9.4.1 Simple Random Sampling A key characteristic of simple random samples is that all units (here the 2m students) have the same probability of being included in the sample. Thus the researcher might select 1 000 students at random from the sampling frame for interview, with each student having a 0.0005 chance of being selected. Clearly this procedure results in a substantial reduction in the numbers of students being interviewed, with commensurate savings in the time and resources required. However, such a procedure still has two major drawbacks. First, the random selection of students might still give rise to an unrepresentative sample in terms of, say, gender or undergraduate/postgraduate. Second, it is highly likely that most (if not all) of the 130 universities will be represented in the sample: this may be a good thing, but the downside is that the researcher still has to visit each and every university to carry out the market research. 163 164 Data Collection for Quantitative Research 9.4.2 Systematic Sampling fi A systematic sample involves selecting every nth unit after a random rst choice. In our market research example, the researcher might visit various first halls of residence at the 130 universities, choose the rooms at random, and then select every nth room thereafter until 1 000 students have been interviewed. The procedure would be random (assuming students were randomly allocated to halls), and simple to undertake; indeed, a full sampling frame would not even be needed. Other applications might involve checking for defects on production lines by examining every 100th product. The main disadvantage is the danger of hidden periodicities. 9.4.3 Strati fied Random Sampling fied A strati sample is a probability sample where the population may be divided into mutually exclusive and exhaustive subsets, and where simple random samples of units are chosen independently from each subset. The fi idea of strati ed sampling is to ensure that every part of the population, that is every stratum, gets satisfactory representation. This is especially important if the means (or proportions or whatever we want to estimate) are very different in the different strata. The result will be a smaller sampling variation: i.e. more stable results in repeated samples than we would get by using simple random sampling. Sampling from the different strata can be either fi proportional or disproportional. A proportionate strati ed sample involves a uniform sampling fraction in each strata. A disproportionate fi strati ed sample involves a variable sampling fraction. In our market research example, the researcher might believe that the results will depend upon the students’ gender, undergraduate/postgraduate status, and possibly other characteristics. In this case, the strata are gender and status, and the researcher can improve the representativeness of the sample by selecting appropriate proportions of students (based on the sam- fi pling frame) in each category. The main advantages of strati ed random sampling are that the results have greater precision for the same sample size (or alternatively the same precision with a smaller sample size), and separate results may be generated for each stratum. 9.4.4 Cluster Sampling Cluster sampling involves first selecting large groups of units (clusters) and then selecting the sample units from the chosen clusters. If the researcher examines all units in the selected clusters, the procedure is called one-stage cluster sampling. If a sample of units is selected randomly from within the selected clusters, the procedure is known as two-stage cluster sampling. 9.4 Probability Sampling One advantage of cluster sampling is that the researcher does not need a complete sampling frame, just knowledge of all potential clusters and sampling frames for the selected clusters. A second advantage is that it will generally be easier and quicker to survey units in geographically proximate clusters than in the more dispersed population. In our market research example, the researcher might select the Universities of Birmingham and Sussex as two clusters, and then randomly survey 500 students in each. If the student bodies in Birmingham and Sussex are similar to those at other UK universities in demographic characteristics and attitudes, then the researcher will maintain the precision of the survey while saving a lot of time and expense as s/he will only need to visit two universities. 9.4.5 Non-Response Bias A serious potential threat to the validity of results from sampling surveys is non-response. When some units that have been drawn for inclusion in the sample do not respond to our questions, the effective sample size is reduced. But this is not the main problem, since it can easily be remedied. If we need a sample of 400 units and we expect a 50 per cent response rate, we could take a sample of 800 units to counteract the non-response. The real problem with non-response is that those who do not respond are usually different from those who do respond, and thus there will be no – this is guarantee that the sample is representative of the population termed non-response bias. To take an extreme example, suppose we use a mail questionnaire to learn something about consumption patterns for alcoholic beverages. The majority of the real drinkers will probably not respond, for several reasons, but they make up an important part of the whole picture. Therefore it is very important to get responses also from the drinkers who have been picked out by our sampling procedure. For further details regarding how to deal with the problem of non-response bias, see Churchill – (1995: 652 88). Non-response and other sampling errors are often more problematic than statistical errors. EXAMPLE 9.2 Non-response is widely believed to be a major reason for the discrepancy fi between the survey-based estimates of alcohol consumption and of cial (taxbased) estimates. Lahaut et al. (2002) first undertook a mail questionnaire survey in 1999 on the alcohol consumption of 1 000 people in Rotterdam. The response rate in this primary survey was 44 per cent after two reminders. 165 166 Data Collection for Quantitative Research EXAMPLE 9.2 (CONT.) They then undertook a secondary follow-up study involving house visits. The fi key results were that several categories were signi cantly under-represented in the primary survey. In particular, abstainers were under-represented, fi whereas frequent excessive drinkers were not signi cantly under-represented. Furthermore, the under-representation of abstainers was greater for females than for males, for people younger than 35 years, and for the Dutch than for the non-Dutch group. They concluded that the results of the secondary study revealed a serious non-response bias in the primary survey, and that this bias cannot be corrected by weighting data on the basis of socio-demographic variables. They recommend that researchers should try to minimize nonresponse bias by, for example, developing more appealing survey materials. 9.5 Non-Probability Sampling In many practical cases, a comprehensive and accurate sampling frame may not exist, hence the researcher may be obliged to adopt some form of nonprobability sampling. The disadvantages are that it is then not possible to calculate the probability for each unit being included in the sample, there is no way of guaranteeing a representative sample, and hence the researcher fi will have less con dence when drawing inferences about the population. The main advantages are that non-probability sampling techniques are generally easier to administer, and may provide valid insights. There are three major forms of non-probability sampling used by business researchers: • Convenience sampling. Select whatever units are conveniently avail- able. In our market research example, the researcher might simply interview those students s/he knows personally or their friends. • Judgement (purposive) sampling. Here the researcher selects units sub- jectively in an attempt to obtain a sample that appears in her/his judgement to be representative of the population. Thus the researcher might try to ensure that the sample included a mix of male/female and undergraduate/postgraduate students. • Quota sampling. fl that re The main aim of quota sampling is to select a sample ects the main characteristics of the population. Our researcher might well have some knowledge about the gender composition to ensure a proper representation. 9.5 Non-Probability Sampling EXAMPLE 9.3 Quota sampling is widely used by political polling organizations (e.g. Gallup, Ipsos MORI, YouGov). The first step is to identify the key characteristics of the population that might have systematic influences on voting behaviour: say gender (male; female); age (under 20; 20–39; 40–59; 60 and over); and annual income level (under £25K; £25–74K; over £75K). Suppose that it is known that the population is distributed as follows: Gender Male Female 45% 55% Age (years) < 20 20 – 39 40 – 59 > 60 Annual income (£) 20% 40% 30% 10% < £25K £25 > – 74K £75K 40% 50% 10% The quotas for each age-gender-income combination could then be calculated by cross-multiplying the appropriate percentages in the population distribution. For example, 20% of the population is aged under 20 years; 45% is male; and 40% has annual income less that £25K. Hence our sample should include a quota of 3.6% (= 20% 45%  40%) of males under the age of 20 years, with annual incomes less than £25K. The full quota sample would then be constructed as follows: Age (years) Gender Annual income (£) < £25K < 20 Male Female 20 – 39 Male Female 40 – 59 Male Female > 60 Male Female Totals 3.6% 4.4% 7.2% 8.8% 5.4% 6.6% 1.8% 2.2% 40% £25 – 74K 4.5% 5.5% 9.0% 11.0% 6.75% 8.25% 2.25% 2.75% 50% Totals > £75K 0.9% 1.1% 1.8% 2.2% 1.35% 1.65% 0.45% 0.55% 10% 20% 40% 30% 10% 167 168 Data Collection for Quantitative Research EXAMPLE 9.3 Thus if the (CONT.) polling organization wished to survey a sample of 1 000 individuals, it would interview the following: Age (years) Gender Annual income (£) < < 20 £25K £25 – 74K Totals > £75K Male 36 45 9 Female 44 55 11 Male 72 90 18 Female 88 110 22 Male 54 68 Female 66 83 Male 18 23 Female 22 27 400 500 200 20 – 39 400 40 – 59 1 1 14 300 > 60 Totals 1. The exact figures 1 1 1 1 16 1 5 1 100 5 100 1 000 have been rounded to an appropriate integer. The polling organization would thus need to interview 36 males under the age of 20 years, with annual incomes less than £25K; 44 females under the age of 20 years, with annual incomes less than £25K; and so on. The sample would be ‘roughly’ representative of the population as long as the categories were independent, and there were no other systematic factors confounding the results. The main advantage is that the organization does not need a full sampling frame, just knowledge of the key characteristics of the population 9.6 Sample Size An important and frequently asked question is: what sample size is needed? Beware of simplistic answers such as that a sample size of 30 is satisfactory, or that the sample should be greater than a given proportion (e.g. 5%) of the population. A more helpful answer is that it depends upon five main considerations: How do you plan to analyse the data? If your intention is just to use the sample mean as an estimate of the population mean (see Chapter 10), 1. 9.7 Survey Research 2. 3. 4. 5. then a relatively small sample should suffice. But if you want to estimate the parameters of a multiple regression model with several explanatory variables (see Chapter 11), then a much larger sample will be required to generate enough degrees of freedom. What level of do you require in your estimates? If greater precision is required (e.g. Æ2% of the true population mean rather than Æ4%), then a larger sample size will be needed. Furthermore, a doubling of the required level of precision (as above) will require a quadrupling of the required sample size. What is the in the values of the sample data (as measured by the standard deviation – see Chapter 10)? If the values are expected to be similar, then a relatively small sample will suffice. But the greater the variability, the larger the sample size that will be required for any given level of precision. All samples involve making estimates of the population parameters and there is always a risk that the sample was unrepresentative and hence the sample estimate would not be accurate. How much confidence do you require in your prediction? The greater the confidence, the greater the required sample size. What is your budget and timescale? The involved in terms of time, labour, and other resources typically rise with sample size. Generally speaking, a larger sample is preferable to a smaller sample in statistical analysis, but larger samples are more expensive and slow or difficult to collate, particularly if primary data are to be collected. Sometimes it may be necessary to forego extensive data collection and settle for restricted but more timely data collation and analysis. 9.7 precision expected variability costs Survey Research Survey research refers to the collection of primary data using questionnaires and/or semi-structured interviews.3 Survey research is an effective tool for obtaining opinions, attitudes, and descriptions as well as for capturing cause-and-effect relationships. Sample surveys are popular in business studies research, and may be analytic or descriptive– see Figure 9.1. According to Simons (1987), analytic surveys may be used to test a theory by taking the logic into thefield, for example to understand the relationship 3 Semi-structured interviews typically involve a list of pre-determined questions, but the researcher is also able to digress when further clarification is required or promising issues are raised. 169 170 Data Collection for Quantitative Research Conceptualize and structure the research problem 1. Consider the aims of the research 2. Review the current state of knowledge 3. Assess the various resources available Analytic survey? Descriptive survey? Identify the phenomena whose variance you wish to describe Identify the independent, dependent, and extraneous variables Determine the sampling strategy by defining the research population and designing a means of accessing a representative (random) sample Are the data to be collected through one approach to respondents? Or does the nature of the research problem require the repeated contact of a single sample or several equivalent samples? Interviewer-administered questionnaire/schedule Respondent-completed/ postally administered questionnaire Figure 9.1 Planning a survey Source : – Gill and Johnson (2002: 76 7). Copyright Sage Publication Ltd. Reproduced by permission of Sage Publications Ltd. between accounting control systems and business performance. In this type of survey, we have to put the emphasis on specifying the dependent, independent, and control variables. We should also give due attention to and bene fit from existing literature, theory, and research while conceptualizing and structuring our research. The review of literature is therefore of the utmost importance. In analytical surveys, dependent, independent, and control variables are controlled through statistical techniques such as multiple regression (see Chapter 11). The questions and variables included in such a survey thus need careful conceptualization and measurement scales. In contrast, descriptive surveys are concerned with identifying the phenomena whose variance we wish to describe. The survey is concerned with fi fixed particular characteristics of a speci c population or subjects, either at a point in time or at varying times for comparative purposes. Here the focus is more on a representative sample of the relevant population than on 9.7 Survey Research the analytical design, as we are concerned principally with accuracy of the findings and whether they can be generalized. Even in these surveys, a review of earlier research and literature is important to determine what kind of questions are to be included in the questionnaire. In business studies, descriptive surveys are often used to obtain consumer attitudes towards a certain product and to ascertain views and opinions of employees in an organization (Reeves and Harper, 1981). These surveys are often used to understand the behaviour of employees with regard to motivation, job satisfaction, and grievances. 9.7.1 Survey Administration One key issue for the researcher is how the survey will be administered. There are four main methods for obtaining survey data, each of which has advantages and disadvantages: • mail questionnaires; • email or website questionnaires; • personal interviews; • telephone/video interviews. The main advantages of mail questionnaires are that costs are low, espe- cially when the sample respondents are geographically dispersed. The respondents also enjoy a high level of anonymity and have time to give considered responses to the questions. The respondents ’ answers will not be fluenced in by the characteristics or technique of the interviewer. The main disadvantages are that mail questionnaires require simple, easily understood questions and instructions, and the researcher does not have the opportunity to clarify answers or request additional information. Response rates are often low, and the researcher often does not know who actually completes the questionnaire. The main advantages of email or website questionnaires are similar to fi those of mail questionnaires, with the added bene t that answers may be coded automatically. The main disadvantages are also similar, with response rates often even lower. Some questionnaires are rejected as spam, while others are easily deleted. Respondents must be computer literate while the researcher must know the email addresses of potential respondents in order to distribute the questionnaires. Anonymity may be compromised if the completed questionnaires reveal information about the respondents ’ email addresses. There are many websites on the Internet (e.g. SurveyMonkey) which you can use to disseminate surveys. Semi-structured personal interviews typically require answers to questions as might be found on a questionnaire. The main advantages of such personal 171 172 Data Collection for Quantitative Research interviews are that they allow greater flexibility in the survey process, with the researcher able to react to answers and to collect additional detail from respondents, including spontaneous reactions and tangential or background information. Once agreed, response rates tend to be high, as is the degree to which researcher the questionnaire also has control is completed over who with answers usable the information. questions, where The the interview is conducted, and the order in which questions are answered. The main disadvantages are the higher cost, especially when the respondents are geographically dispersed. Anonymity is also compromised and respondents may not answer the questions honestly, while they may also be affected by the interviewer’s personal characteristics or technique. Telephone/video interviews tend to be more structured than personal interviews, with less opportunity for collecting supplementary information. Costs are dispersed much lower, respondents and and respondents hence can undertake easily many reach geographically interviews in short a period of time. The main disadvantages are the lack of personal contact and a reluctance by many respondents to discuss sensitive issues. 9.7.2 Questionnaire Design The main objective of a questionnaire survey (whether administered by mail or in person) is to obtain data that enables the researcher to provide answers to the research questions posed in her/his project. This objective highlights the need for the researcher to think carefully about the questions to be included (content, format, and sequence), about the wording of the questions, and about how the data will be analysed before the survey is administered. A common mistake by inexperienced researchers is to administer a survey, and then seek the help of a statistician to analyse the data. Most questionnaires include a mixture of open and closed questions. Open questions allow the respondent to provide his/her own answers without any prompting from the researcher – see Figure 9.2. Please list up to four features you like about product X 1 ………………….. Figure 9.2 2 ………………….. 3 ………………….. 4 ………………….. An example of an open question 9.7 Survey Research Open questions are useful in exploratory research, when the researcher may not have a clear idea of possible responses, or when the researcher wants fi to elicit detailed responses. But it is often dif cult to code the responses to such questions consistently, as each respondent may use different terminology. Hence open questions should be used sparingly. In contrast, the general format of closed questions is for the researcher to list all possible answers, and to instruct the respondent to choose from this list. The main advantage of using closed questions is that the responses can be easily coded, and their analysis is straightforward. The main disadvantage fi is that the researcher speci es the set of possible answers and may omit other valid alternatives. Sometimes closed questions also include‘Don’t know’ or ‘No comment’ options to provide an escape route for respondents who wish to avoid providing a response, either because they do not know the answer or because they do not want to answer the question because of its sensitive nature. If no such escape route is provided, then the respondents are obliged to choose from the list of possible answers and this may bias the results. Closed questions may have several formats: • Category questions. The respondent is invited to select one response from a fi nite set of mutually exclusive categories – see Figure 9.3 for an example. The resultant data are measured on a nominal scale. • Multiple-choice questions. The respondent is invited to select one or more responses from a predetermined set of possibilities– see Figure 9.4 What is your age? (tick one category only) Under 21 21 – 30 31 – 40 41 – 50 51 – 60 61 or over Figure 9.3 An example of a category question 173 174 Data Collection for Quantitative Research Which of the following features attracted you to product X? (tick as many features as apply) Low price Functionality Appearance Value for money After-sales service Figure 9.4 An example of a multiple-choice question Rank the following features of product X using the values 1 to 5 where: 1 = the feature you like the most 5 = the feature you like the least Low price Functionality Appearance Value for money After-sales service Figure 9.5 An example of a ranking question for an example. Note that respondents simply list the attractive features but do not put them in order of importance. • Ranking questions. The respondent is invited to list the possible options in order of preference – see Figure 9.5 for an example. The resultant are measured on an ordinal scale. data 9.7 Survey Research Strongly Disagree Neither Agree Disagree nor Disagree Agree Strongly Agree Product X provides good value for money Figure 9.6 • An example of a rating question Rating questions. The respondent is invited to express her/his opinion about how much s/he agrees or disagrees with a particular statement– see fi Figure 9.6 for an example. The scale typically involves a nite number of pre-coded levels, each with its own label, and where the middle level is ‘neutral’. In Figure 9.6, there are five levels, which are labelled ‘strongly disagree ’, ‘disagree’, ‘neither agree nor disagree’, ‘agree’, and ‘strongly disagree ’, hence we have a 5-point scale. The responses can then be fied with a value of 1 allocated to the lowest level, and 5 allocated quanti to the highest level. Sometimes 7-point scales are used, possibly adding additional levels with labels such as ‘very strongly disagree’ and ‘very 4 strongly agree’. Such rating scales are often referred to as Likert scales, although there are other types of rating scale (e.g. semantic differential rating scales). The data obtained from such Likert scales are ordinal, though researchers often treat them as though they were measured on interval scales. It is more acceptable to assume that the data are continu5 ous when the responses to several questionnaire items are combined. 9.7.3 Guidelines for Constructing Questionnaires The precise wording of the questions is crucial for achieving the maximum validity of the data collected through questionnaire surveys. This is illustrated though the following (possibly apocryphal) story provided by Sudman and Bradburn (1989: 8): Two priests, a Dominican and a Jesuit, are discussing whether it is a sin to smoke and pray at the same time. After failing to reach a conclusion, each goes off to 4 5 The scale is named after the Swedish psychologist, Rensis Likert. Sometimes the scales are referred to as Likert-type scales. There is considerable debate as to whether items measured on a Likert scale should be considered as ordinal variables (Wu and Leung, 2017). Strictly speaking, single items should be treated as ordinal variables. Often, however, several items are combined and averaged (see Section 6.3) to form a multi-item scale which allows for non-integer scores, and many researchers then treat such scales as if they were continuous variables. This is acceptable in student dissertations, but may not find favour with the reviewers at top-quality journals. 175 176 Data Collection for Quantitative Research consult his respective superior. The next week they meet again. The Dominican says, ‘ Well, what did your superior say?’ The Jesuit responds, ‘ He said it was all right’ . ‘That ’s funny’, the Dominican replies, ‘my superior said it was a sin’. Jesuit: ‘What did you ask him?’ Reply: ‘I asked him if it was all right to smoke while praying’. ‘Oh ’ , said the Jesuit, ‘ I asked my superior if it was all right to pray while smoking’. Here we provide some guidelines for the construction of questionnaires. 1. The questions must be asked in very simple and concise language. One should consider the respondents’ background when it comes to educational level, cultural background, knowledge, and acquaintance with the subject matter. The questions should then be adjusted and adapted to the above-mentioned characteristics of the respondents. Not only the questions but also the possible answers provided (in the case of structured questionnaires) should use clear and unambiguous language. 2. We should be rather conservative as to the level of knowledge, education, etc. necessary for the respondents to answer the questionnaire. We should not put unrealistic demands on the respondents’ know-how, memory, and willingness to respond. 3. dimension or aspect. If we mix Each question should deal with only one fi up several dimensions or aspects in one question, it will be dif cult for respondents to explain their behaviour or to answer yes or no. In other words, one cannot ask one question about more than one variable or dimension of the study. Each variable and dimension should be covered by a separate question. It is quite common to ask several questions on one variable or to have each question cover different dimensions of the subject matter. In other words, avoid ‘double-barrelled’ questions, such as: ‘What is the turnover of your company and how much of that comes from export?’ or ‘What is your educational background and how long have you been working in this position?’ One way to do this is not to use ‘and ’ in any question. 4. The questions should be formulated in such a way that there is no escape route in the questions. We should not offer an option such as ‘Don’t know’. 5. Moreover, the questions should be specific and not too general in nature so that the respondent does not give several answers. If we must have general questions, we should check the understanding of these questions through another question. The more fi speci c and concise the questions are, the easier it is for us to interpret the questions and answers in different categories and then draw conclusions. 9.7 Survey Research 6. The questions should not be of a suggestive nature, directing the respondent towards an answer or a specific opinion. For example, we cannot ask a high-tech or a pharmaceutical company the following question: Do you consider R&D important for your type of company? Questions should be formulated in a polite and soft language. They should not irritate, offend, or provoke the respondents. It is very important to place the sensitive questions, if any, at the right place in the questionnaire, so that the respondents can understand why that particular question is being asked. But, in any case, there must be a logical and systematic sequence of questions to avoid misunderstandings and to ensure a high response rate. We must keep in mind that the respondents are doing us a favour by answering the questions. The language and words used in the questions should bestraightforward and should not have double or hidden meanings, otherwise respondents may answer the questions with a different understanding and thereby contribute negatively to the conclusions of the study. Another risk is that the respondent will leave the question unanswered if s/he is not sure of the questions meaning. For example, it will be quite difficult for a respondent to understand what you want to know by the following question: What type of structure does your company have for export activities? or What are the major barriers to entry faced by your company in international markets? If one is using complex wording or concepts, a note describing or clarifying the exact meaning of the text should be added to the question. Several authors (e.g. Cannell et al., 1981) have advocated that an explanation or argument as to why that particular question is being asked gives a better response rate. Questions should be placed in a logical order. The easy-to-answer questions and positive types of questions should be placedfirst. If we place the complicated or difficult questions first for example questions for which the respondents need to consult books or managers/colleagues they might get the impression that all the questions are of that nature and thus refrain from responding at all. The same is true for sensitive questions. There should also be a logical order from general to specific questions. The layout of the questionnaire is also important. It should lookneat and tidy as this can influence respondents willingness to answer. The questionnaire should be formatted and printed in a way that does not look frightening due to its length or complexity to understand and answer. ‘ 7. 8. ’ ’ ‘ ’ ‘ ’ 9. – – 10. ’ 177 178 Data Collection for Quantitative Research Last but not least, we should go through the questionnaire critically or 11. have a friend, colleague, or adviser do this and give comments. The best five way to handle this is to do a pre-test on three to real firms or respondents. In such a pre-test we should check whether the above- fi mentioned issues such as understanding, the level of dif culty, the willingness to answer sensitive questions, and the time it takes to answer the questionnaire are as we wish. 9.7.4 Improving Response Rates Poor response rates are commonplace, particularly with questionnaire surveys undertaken by mail, email, or via websites. There are various practices that the researcher can try in order to improve response rates: • Write a cover letter that introduces the researcher and her/his status and fi af liation. Explain succinctly the purpose and relevance of the research fi first thing that the potential respondents will read, and it project, and guarantee the con dentiality of any responses. This cover letter will be the is vital that its content and tone encourages the respondents to look at the questionnaire itself. • Consider carefully the format of the questionnaire. Limit yourself to only those questions that are necessary for the research project, and keep the questionnaire as short as possible. Think about the layout and appearance of the questionnaire, and even the colour of the paper. Perhaps use images and different colours to make the questionnaire more attractive. Provide clear instructions to respondents about how to answer each question. • If possible, notify possible respondents in advance in person or by telephone so that they are more likely to participate when they receive the survey instrument. • Sometimes a sponsor may be useful. This might be a government department or an industry organization that is willing to encourage its members to participate. For students, it may be helpful if their supervisor is willing to supply a letter of support. In other cases, the use of a sponsor may be counterproductive. Suppose you are trying to elicit views on fi corruption, then it would probably be unwise to have any of cial government organization as a sponsor! • fi Offer some form of inducement to participate. This might be nancial (if funds are available), a promise to provide a summary report of the research, or simply a reiteration of the importance of the project. • Provide to clear minimize instructions the cost and for the hassle return for of the potential survey instrument respondents. This 9.8 Experimental Studies might involve a stamped addressed envelope for the return of mail questionnaires, or dedicated email addresses/websites for online questionnaires. Experimental Studies 9.8 Survey research is an important source of primary data based on events and actors in the ‘real world’. In contrast, experimental studies allow the collection of methods primary have data in long more been fi arti cial popular in controlled settings. psychology and, Experimental more recently, in economics (Bandiera et al., 2016), while applications in strategy and business studies are becoming more frequent (Chatterji et al., 2016; Croson et al., 2007; Di Stefano and Gutierrez, 2018; Kraus et al., 2016; van Witteloostuijn, 2015; Zellmer-Bruhn et field experiments and/or al., 2016). Experiments may involve laboratory fi experiments. The former refers to any arti cial experimental setting, which might involve alternative scenarios, vignettes (Aguinis and Bradley, 2014), or policy capturing (Connelly et al., 2016), and fi which takes place in a computer laboratory or classroom. In contrast, eld experiments though the involve novel situations laboratory and field are strategies still being contrived, piloted hence the in actual situations, distinction between experiments is often not clearcut (Harrison and List, 2004). The advantages of controlled experiments over observational research based on real-world data are several (Croson et al., 2007). First, the experiments can be designed to generate data that capture the relevant theoretical constructs, while real-world data tends to be noisy in that often only rough proxies are available. Second, the experiments can be designed to isolate the key mechanisms that the researcher wishes to study, and to remove the confounding effects that are inevitably present with real-world data. Third, additional controls may be introduced to take into account factors that may interact with the main effects. Fourth, experimental methods are able to establish causal relationships more conclusively by comparing effects with and without just provides a particular evidence of stimulus, whereas statistical observational association. Fifth, research often experiments may be replicated: other researchers can undertake the same experiment and verify the findings independently, or the same experiment may be carried out in a different setting or with different subjects. Taken together, these advantages mean that experimental methods can be very useful in testing the predictions predictions. of theories, and for comparing competing theories and 179 180 Data Collection for Quantitative Research first is that There are two disadvantages with controlled experiments. The the experimental context is necessarily abstract and unrealistic in that many fl potential real-world in uences are not considered. This gives rise to claims that controlled experiments lack external validity (Falk and Heckman, 2009). Advocates of experimental methods acknowledge this criticism, but suggest not only that experimental methods can be useful complements to observational research but also that experimental and observational studies often show high correlations between estimated effect sizes. As Croson et al. (2007: 176) suggest, ‘when research methods that emphasize internal validity and those that emphasize external validity provide a consistent set of results, it increases not only the reliability of the outcome, but also leads to a stronger set of conclusions.’ The second disadvantage is more practical. Often it is dif ficult to recruit actual managers as subjects, and researchers instead use MBA or undergraduate students whose experience of, and expertise in, work settings are typically less than those of managers. This raises issues about how subjects are selected, and whether they are representative. Exercises Further Reading ................................................................. Brace, I. (2018) Questionnaire Design: How to Plan, Structure and Write Survey Material for Effective Market Research, 4th edn., London: Kogan Page. Chatterji, A., Findley, M., Jensen, N., Meier, S., and Nielson, D. (2016)‘ Field experiments in strategy research’ , Strategic Management Journal, 37(1): 116–32. Cooper, D.R. and Schindler, P.S. (2013)Business Research Methods, 12th edn., Boston MA: McGraw-Hill. Sudman, S. and Bradburn, N. (1982)Asking Questions: A Practical Guide to Questionnaire Design, San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Questions ................................................................. 1. Explain the difference between secondary and primary data. 2. What are the advantages and disadvantages of secondary data? 3. Is all secondary data on a particular topic suitable for a research project on that topic? 4. ‘First explore secondary data, only when secondary data sources are exhausted, then you can plan to collect primary data. ’ Comment. 5. What do you understand by the terms ‘population’ and ‘sample’? 6. What are the main considerations involved in choosing an appropriate sample size for survey research? 7. Are the data obtained from Likert rating scales ordinal or interval? 8. What are the advantages of controlled experiments over observational research based on real-world data? What are the disadvantages of controlled experiments? Exercises ................................................................. 1. Suppose you wish to undertake a questionnaire survey of manufacturing firms to ascertain the extent of their adoption of 3D printing technologies. Draft a fi cover letter to accompany the questionnaire to be sent to the CEOs of the rms. 2. Design a questionnaire survey to assess customer satisfaction with the service provided in a local restaurant. Include items related to different aspects of the service provided, and use Likert scales to provide the assessments. Include some relevant category questions. 3. firms may have different motivations (i.e. In Section 9.2.1, we suggested that fi market-seeking, ef ciency-seeking, asset-seeking, natural resource-seeking) for investing in foreign countries. Suggest some questions that you could firms to discover their motivations? [Note: you pose to the CEOs of investing should not assume that the CEOs will understand the terms‘market-seeking’, ficiency-seeking , ‘ef ’ ‘asset-seeking’, and ‘natural resource-seeking’.] 181 10 Description and Preliminary Analysis of Quantitative Data The greatest moments are those when you see the result pop up in a graph or in your statistics analysis: that moment you realise you know something no one else does and you get the pleasure of thinking about how to tell them. Emily Oster The appropriate method of data analysis depends upon a variety of factors fi that have been speci ed in the research question and as part of the research design (see Chapter 5). Indeed, it cannot be stressed too strongly that the before researcher should consider how s/he intends to analyse the data makes s/he final decisions about the research design. All too often (especially with inexperienced researchers), research strategies are decided, data are collected, and only then is attention given to how the data are to be analysed – only to find that the requisite data have not been collected to provide answers to the research questions. As the renowned scientist and statistician Sir Ronald Fisher pithily remarked, ‘To consult the statistician after an experiment is finished is often merely to ask him to conduct a post mortem examination. He can perhaps say what the experiment died of. ’ One key issue is whether the data are qualitative or quantitative, and this depends upon the underlying research approach. If the research approach is deductive, then most of the data are likely to be expressed as numbers, and the key issue will be selecting the appropriate statistical techniques for describing and analysing the data. In this chapter, we will concentrate on techniques for describing quantitative data and for providing simple preliminary analyses. Much statistical analysis involves explaining and predicting relationships between variables: a comprehensive review of all possible techniques is beyond the scope of this book, so we will concentrate on providing an intuitive primer on the use of regression analysis in Chapter 11. In Chapter 12, we will provide introductions to factor analysis, structural equation modelling, and event study analysis. If the approach is inductive, then most of the data are likely to be qualitative (text, interview transcripts, pictures, etc.) in nature and the purpose of the analysis will be to bring some order, structure, and meaning to this mass of data. 10.1 How to Describe the Data 10.1 How to Describe the Data 10.1.1 Bar Charts, Histograms, and Scatter Plots fi Often data are collated in a spreadsheet, and it can be dif cult to identify patterns in the data or to compare the values of two or more variables. Consider the dataset in Appendix A, which contains data for a sample of 100 imaginary manufacturing (years since establishment), firms CAPS on SIZE (number of employees), AGE (capabilities, measured on a subjective REGION (north, south, east, or west), SECTOR (electronic, automobile, or textile), and export intensity (export sales as percentage of total sales). REGION and SECTOR are both nominal (categorical) variables while SIZE, AGE, CAPS, and export intensity are all continuous variables. Two values for export intensity are provided for each fi rm: one (EXP18) for 2018 and a second (EXP19) for 2019. In addition, a panel of industry experts have also ranked the 100 firms according to their growth potential (GROW ) over the next fi ve years: GROW is an ordinal variable with the rank 1 indi– scale: 1 7), cating the the firm judged to have the most potential, and the rank 100 indicating firm judged to have the least potential. One way of visualizing the data for the two categorical variables is to present the data in the form of bar charts, where the categories are presented on the x-axis of the chart while some metric for each category is presented 1 on the y-axis. Suppose we are interested in how firm size varies across the four regions and across the three sectors. The relative height of each bar – see corresponds to the mean value of the chosen variable in each category Figure 10.1 for the size distribution of the for the size distribution of the mean size of firms firms by region, and Figure 10.2 by sector. Figure 10.1 shows that the firms is greatest in the south region, while the mean size of the firms in the east region is less than 100. Figure 10.2 shows that the automobile fi rms are larger, on average, than the electronicfi rms, which are in turn larger than the textile fi rms. A histogram provides a graphical representation of the distribution of the sample observations for a single continuous variable. Several discrete inter- fi vals are speci ed on the x-axis for the values of the variable, while the frequencies with which the values fall within these intervals are measured on the y-axis. Vertical bars then depict how many sample observations fall within each interval. A histogram differs from a bar chart in that it presents 1 Alternatively, the categories may be presented on the y-axis with the metric on the x-axis. See Example 10.1 for a sophisticated bar chart in this format. Pie charts are alternatives to bar charts. 183 184 Description and Preliminary Analysis 150 EZIS naeM 100 50 0 NORTH EAST SOUTH WEST REGION Figure 10.1 Distribution of firm sizes by region 150 EZIS naeM 100 50 0 ELECTRONIC AUTOMOBILE TEXTILE SECTOR Figure 10.2 Distribution of firm sizes by sector EXAMPLE 10.1 In the United States, government shutdowns occur when the necessary legislation to allocate funds for the operation of government agencies is not enacted. This may be because Congress fails to pass the legislation or because the President vetoes the legislation. In these circumstances, the federal government is obliged to shut down the affected agencies and put the employees on temporary unpaid leave. Shutdowns typically occur when Congress and the President cannot agree on a budget, but may also happen when one or the other is trying 10.1 How to Describe the Data EXAMPLE 10.1 (CONT.) to force through other political objectives. Murse (2019) reports that the Republican majority in the House of Representatives triggered a 16-day shutdown in 2013 in an unsuccessful attempt to force President Barack Obama to repeal the Affordable Care Act. More recently, the Democratic – majority in Congress triggered a 34-day shutdown in 2018 19 by refusing to approve funding for the plan of President Donald Trump for a border wall along the US border with Mexico. There 1976 have –2018: Jimmy been 21 US government shutdowns over the period – 1 under President Gerald Ford (1974 77); 5 under President Carter – (1977 81); 8 under President Ronald Reagan – (1981 89); 1 under President George Bush (1989-93); 2 under President Bill Clinton (1993 –2001); none under President George W. Bush; 1 under President – Barack Obama (2009 2017); and 3 to date under President Donald Trump (2017 –). The bar chart below depicts these shutdowns visually. The chart might be further enhanced by highlighting the terms of the various presidents, and the periods when Republican (Democratic) presidents are confronted by Democratic (Republican) majorities in Congress. US Government Shutdowns, 1976–2018 2018 Dec 22 2018 Jan 20 2018 Feb 9 2013 Oct 1 1995 Dec 16 1995 Nov 14 1990 Oct 5 1987 Dec 18 1986 Oct 16 1984 Oct 3 1984 Sept 30 1983 Nov 10 1982 Dec 17 1982 Sept 30 1981 Nov 20 1979 Sept 30 1978 Sept 30 1977 Nov 30 1977 Oct 31 1977 Sept 30 1976 Sept 30 1 16 nwodtuhs fo etad tratS 2 5 3 3 2 11 8 8 10 0 21 5 3 1 1 1 34 3 5 10 18 12 15 20 Days 25 30 Source : Murse, T. (2019) ‘All 21 government shutdowns in US history’, ThoughtCo, 25 May 2019. Available at: thoughtco.com/government-shutdown-history-3368274. 35 40 185 186 Description and Preliminary Analysis Symmetric Distribution Skewed Distribution Mean Median Mode Figure 10.3 Mode Median Mean Symmetric and skewed distributions the distribution of data for a single continuous variable, while the bar chart compares metrics for different categorical variables. A histogram plots the observations on a single variable from a (small) finite sample, and may provide a rough idea of the underlying probability fi distribution of the variable in a large (possibly in nite) population. Different 2 variables exhibit different probability distributions. For example, the roll of a dice gives rise to a uniform distribution where each possible outcome (1, 2, . . . 6) has an equal probability (i.e. 1/6) of occurrence. In business – applications, we often encounter variables that follow a normal distribution see Figure 10.3. Such variables exhibit a symmetrical bell-shaped distribution around a mid-point which corresponds to the mean, median, and mode of the variable (see Section 10.1.2). The distribution of some variables is not symmetric but is skewed (to the right, as depicted in Figure 10.3, or to the left): in such cases the mean is less than the median which is less than the mode. A scatter plot is a graph in which the values of two continuous variables are measured along the two axes: each observation is plotted as a point whose x-y coordinates correspond to the values of the two variables. Additional variables may also be added by highlighting the points using different colours or indicators. Scatter plots are useful in identifying patterns in the data, such as whether there may be a relationship (linear or non-linear) between the variables. Figure 10.4 presents a scatter plot of the observations on firm size (SIZE) and export intensity (EXP18) from Appendix A. Although there is considerable variability in the data, the plot suggests clearly that export intensity tends to fall as firm size increases. Such charts and plots provide useful visual representations of the data, but they are cumbersome and of little use in statistical analysis. We need to be 2 Common distributions include uniform, binomial, Bernoulli, normal, Poisson, and exponential. For further details: http://blog.cloudera.com/blog/2015/12/commonprobability-distributions-the-data-scientists-crib-sheet/. 10.1 How to Describe the Data 90 80 )%( ytisnetnI tropxE 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 0 50 100 150 200 250 300 Firm Size (employees) Figure 10.4 Scatter plot of export intensity against firm size able to summarize the key characteristics of each variable in a small number of summary statistics that measure central tendency (average), dispersion, and skewness. 10.1.2 Measures of Central Tendency Three measures of central tendency are commonly used in practice: mean, n observations on variable X: ± Þ of these observations may depicted as X , X , . . . X , X . The mean value ðX median, and mode. Suppose we have a sample of 1 2 n-1 n a variable is calculated by adding together all the observations in the sample, XX and then dividing by the total number of observations. In formal terms: X± ¼ 1 n n i¼1 i¼ 1 n ðX 1 þ X 2 þ . . . . . . . . . . . þ . X n± 1 þ XnÞ (10.1) where Xi = value of observation i of the variable n = number of observations in the sample eg X The horizontal bar over the top of a variableð : : ± Þ is used to denote the mean value. The sigma symbol Σ is used as a shorthand way to denote a n observations on the X variable from i n shown at the top of the sigma symbol). Any letter (herei, but t is often used for time-series summation: here we are summing the i X1 ( = 1 shown at the bottom of the sigma symbol) to X ( = n data) can be used for the index, and the index can run from any integer to any other integer to denote a sub-sample. Mean values may be calculated for continuous variables but not for categorical or ordinal variables. As shown in Table 10.1, the mean size of 187 188 Description and Preliminary Analysis Table 10.1 Summary statistics for the sample of 100 manufacturingfirms Standard SIZE AGE CAPS Region Sector Export intensity Coef ficient of Mean Median deviation variation Skewness 114 31 4.3 n/a n/a 38.0 111.5 29 4.35 n/a n/a 37.75 59.0 16.1 1.5 n/a n/a 13.8 0.52 0.52 0.35 n/a n/a 0.36 0.458 0.335 ±0.253 n/a n/a 0.295 41.4 43.45 15.2 0.37 ±0.041 n/a 50 n/a n/a n/a (EXP18) Export intensity (EXP19) GROW Source: Appendix A the 100 firms in the sample is 114 employees, the mean age is 31 years, mean capabilities is 4.3, mean export intensity in 2018 was 38.0%, and mean export intensity in 2019 was 41.4%. The median value of a variable is determined by ranking all the observations from smallest to largest, and then selecting the value of the middleranking observation. Median values may be calculated for continuous variables, and for variables measured using ordinal scales. As shown in Table 10.1, the median size of the 100firms in the sample is 111.5 employees, the median age is 29 years, median capabilities is 4.35, and the median export intensity is 37.75% in 2018. If the variables are normally distributed, then the median values should be similar to the mean values (Figure 10.3). The median value of the variable is 50. Finally, the mode value of a variable is simply defined as the value that occurs most frequently in the sample. GROW 10.1.3 Measures of Dispersion Two samples may share the same mean value, yet the distributions of the data may be completely different. In Table 10.2, we reproduce the data on for the first 10 firms (sub-sample A) and the last 10 firms (sub-sample B) in our sample of 100 manufacturing firms. Both samples have the same mean value (120 employees), but the size distributions of the two samples is obviously quite different: the 10firms in sample B are all of similar size, while the 10 firms in sample A have very different sizes. SIZE 10.1 How to Describe the Data Table 10.2 Summary statistics for two sub-samples of manufacturing Sample A Mean firms Sample B Deviation Sum 189 Deviation from the Squared from the Squared Size mean deviation Size mean deviation 243 +123 15 129 122 +2 4 67 ±53 2 809 105 ±15 225 79 ±41 1 681 118 ±2 4 134 +14 196 126 +6 36 207 +87 7 569 131 +11 121 114 ±6 36 115 ±5 25 33 ± 87 7 569 117 ±3 9 65 ± 55 3 025 122 +2 4 163 +43 1 849 128 +8 64 95 ± 25 625 116 ±4 16 0 40 688 0 508 120 120 Variance 4 068.8 50.8 Standard 63.8 7.1 0.53 0.059 deviation Coef fi cient of variation Source: Appendix A 3 One measure of dispersion is the variance. This is derived by taking the deviations of each observation in the sample from the mean, then squaring Var) is all the deviations, and summing the squared deviations. The variance ( then calculated by dividing this sum by the total number of observations in the sample: Varð XÞ ¼ 3 1 n XX n i¼1 ð i± X± Þ 2 (10.2) Many measures of dispersion exist, both for continuous and for categorical variables. See Weisberg (1992) and Frankfort-Nachmias and Nachmias (1996) for excellent discussions. Here we will focus on measures of dispersion based on the mean. 190 Description and Preliminary Analysis How may the variance be interpreted? If all the values in the sample are similar (as in the case of sample B), then each value is close to the mean, hence all the deviations are relatively small number (here small VarB and the variance in the sample is a = 50.8). If, in contrast, the values in the sample are quite dissimilar (as in the case of sample A), then few values are close to the mean, most deviations are quite large, and the variance in the sample is a relatively large number (here VarA = 4068.8). The variance measures the total variability in the sample and is expressed not in the units of the original variable but in squared units. A second s measure of dispersion is the standard deviation ( ) which is calculated by taking the square root of the variance, and is expressed in the units of the pffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi original variable: sð XÞ ¼ Var ðXÞ If two variables have very different means, (10.3) then a comparison of their standard deviations is not meaningful: a standard deviation of 3 is quite substantial if the mean of the variable is only 5, but is almost negligible if the mean is 105. A final fi CV measure of dispersion is thus the coef cient of variation ( ), which is calculated by dividing the standard deviation by the mean of the variable: CV ð XÞ ¼ sðX±XÞ (10.4) The three measures of dispersion are clearly closely related and, notwithstanding the differences in their calculation, all may be interpreted in similar ways: large values indicate a high degree of variability; small values indicate a low degree of variability. 10.1.4 Measures of Skewness Normal distributions are assumed in many (parametric) statistical tests, but many variables exhibit other distributions which may invalidate these tests and may require the use of non-parametric tests. Tests for normality are shown in most basic statistical textbooks, but will not be covered here. Skewness refers to the asymmetry of the distribution of a variable about its mean. One simple measure which might indicate a non-normal distribution fi SK is the Fisher-Pearson coef cient of skewness ( 4 4 ), calculated as follows: Some computer packages use a variant of this formula which takes sample size into account. 10.2 How to Analyse the Data Table 10.3 Decision criterion for the skewness coefficient Data are Data are Data are fairly Data are Data are highly moderately symmetric moderately highly skewed skewed skewed skewed ± 1.0 – 0.5 + 0.5 + 1.0 SK ðXÞ ¼ Pni X i ± X± Þ =n SDð XÞ ¼1 3 ð (10.5) 3 fi A normally distributed variable will have a skewness coef cient close to fi zero. A large value of the coef cient re flects a mean that is quite different from the median, and is evidence of skewness: a large negative value of the ficient coef indicates the distribution is skewed to the left, while a large fi positive value of the coef cient indicates that the distribution is skewed to the right. Some rough rules of thumb for deciding whether the skewness ficient is large or small are shown in Table 10.3. It is apparent from Table 10.1 that all five continuous coef variables fi in Appendix A have data that may be described as fairly symmetric as all ve fi skewness coef cients fall within the range [± 0.5, +0.5]. However, these ficients do not confirm normality. coef 10.2 How to Analyse the Data 10.2.1 Hypothesis Testing In most quantitative research, the intention is to move beyond simple description of the sample data and to test various hypotheses about the variables and the relationships between variables. At this point, it is important to reiterate the distinction between a sample and a population (see fi Section 9.3). A population is the entirety of a de ned set of firms, people, countries etc, and may be described by reference to various parameters. In principle, the researcher could survey the entire population but, if the population is large, this might involve a considerable expenditure of time, effort, 5 and money. So the researcher might instead survey a sample, or subset of the population, and generate sample statistics as described in Section 10.1. Such sample statistics might then be used to make inferences about the unknown population parameters. For example, the data in Appendix A relate 5 fi Some populations are in nite, in which case a full survey is not feasible. 191 192 Description and Preliminary Analysis firms, and this sample may well be drawn from a much larger population of, say, 10 000+fi rms in the economy. to a sample of 100 manufacturing We can use the sample statistics to make estimates of the population estimates . parameters but it is important to emphasize that the statistics are firms in our sample is 114 employees, For example, the mean size of the 100 but this sample mean is only an estimate of the population mean size of the firms. The sample mean will provide a good estimate of the popula- 10 000+ tion mean if the sample is representative of the population, but this requires that the sampling procedure has been done in such a way as to avoid potential bias and to maximize precision (see Chapter 9). A larger sample size will generally improve the precision of the estimate, but larger samples are more costly to collect. A hypothesis test involves making a statement hypothesis (H0) – about – referred to as the null an unknown population parameter, and then seeing whether the sample data are consistent with this null hypothesis or not. If not, we reject the null hypothesis in favour of an alternative hypothesis (H ). A fi For instance, we might be interested in comparing the mean sizes of rms in two samples (see Sections 10.2.2. and 10.2.3 below) to see if they come from populations of similar-sized firm firms: the null hypothesis is thus that the mean sizes in the two populations are equal, but this will be rejected if the mean firm sizes in the two samples are markedly different. All hypothesis tests involve the same four basic steps even though, as we will see, the formulae and other details vary according to the exact nature of the test. These steps are as follows: Step 1: • Specify the null and alternative hypotheses. It is vital that an explicit statement is made of the null hypothesis (and alternative hypothesis) otherwise there is often confusion about the result of the test. Step 2: • Step 3: • Calculate the test statistic. The test statistic is calculated on the basis of appropriate sample data. Find the critical value from tables. The sampling distribution of the test statistic theoretical distribution (e.g. normal, t, F, χ 2 will follow a given ), and all statistics text- books provide tabulated values for the most common distributions. • This critical value will typically depend, among other things, upon fi (a) the degrees of freedom in the sample, and (b) the signi cance level speci • fied by the researcher (see below). The degrees of freedom depend in part upon the number of observations in the sample. More observations mean more degrees of freedom, and more powerful hypothesis tests. 10.2 How to Analyse the Data Table 10.4 Possible errors in hypothesis testing Result of hypothesis test Do not reject H0 State of nature Reject H0 H0 is true Correct Type I error H0 is false Type II error Correct Step 4: Specify the test criterion. • The test criterion is a decision rule for rejecting or not rejecting the null hypothesis, based upon a comparison of the calculated test statistic and the critical value. • Explain the outcome of the hypothesis test in words. fi What is the signi cance level of a hypothesis test? In setting up the test, the researcher hopes that the sample is representative of the population, and that the test criterion provides a robust rule for deciding whether or not to reject the null hypothesis. However, it is important to emphasize again that there is always the possibility of drawing an ‘unrepresentative’ sample and making a decision error when using sample data to test a hypothesis about an (unknown) population parameter. Two types of error are possible: 1. The researcher rejects the null hypothesis on the basis of the hypothesis test, but the null hypothesis is in fact true. This is called a Type I error. 2. The researcher does not reject the null hypothesis on the basis of the hypothesis test, but the null hypothesis is in fact false. This is called a Type II error. The possible outcomes of the hypothesis test are summarized in Table 10.4: the ‘state of nature’ refers to the true value of the unknown population parameter. Example 10.2 illustrates the idea of Type I and Type II errors in the context of a legal trial. EXAMPLE 10.2 Suppose you (the reader) have been accused of a crime. Probably only you know whether you are guilty of the crime or innocent. This is the‘state of nature’ . Nevertheless, you have been arrested and brought to trial for the crime. In most jurisdictions, the default position at the trial (the null hypoth- fi esis) is that the accused person is innocent, unless the evidence is suf cient 193 194 Description and Preliminary Analysis EXAMPLE 10.2 (CONT.) to convince the judge/jury that the accused is guilty (the alternative hypothesis). There are four possible outcomes to the trial: Result of trial Innocent Guilty State of nature Innocent Guilty Correct decision Type I error Type II error Correct decision Now society would like the judge/jury to find you guilty if you did indeed fi commit the crime (the south-east quadrant), and to nd you innocent if you did not commit the crime (the north-west quadrant). But the judge/jury may fi make two types of errors. One error (Type I error) is to nd you guilty even though you are innocent; the other is to fi nd you innocent (Type II error) even though you are guilty. Both these errors are regrettable from the point of view of society, though you would be happy in the latter case. One way of minimizing the probability of making a Type I error would be to require (very) high standards of proof for guilty verdicts in trials, but this is likely to result in some guilty people being found innocent (i.e. more Type II errors). Similarly, one way of minimizing the probability of making a Type II error would be to lower the standards of proof in trials, but this might result in some innocent people being found guilty (i.e. more Type I errors). It is only possible to reduce the probability of one incorrect decision at the expense of increasing the probability of the other. In hypothesis testing, the probability of making a Type I error is termed the significance level (denoted as α ) and this significance level is specified by the researcher as part of the research design. In business research, researchers typically choose one of the following significance levels: • α = 0.05. This means that there is a 1 in 20 chance of making a Type I error. Hypothesis tests that result in the null hypothesis being rejected are said to be significant, or significant at the 5% level. • α = 0.01. This means that there is a 1 in 100 chance of making a Type I error. Hypothesis tests that result in the null hypothesis being rejected are said to be highly significant, or significant at the 1% level. • α = 0.10. This means that there is a 1 in 10 chance of making a Type I error. Hypothesis tests that result in the null hypothesis being rejected are said to be significant at the 10% level. 10.2 How to Analyse the Data The probability of making a Type II error in a hypothesis test is denoted byβ, and the power of the test is then (1 β). Multicollinearity (see Section 11.3.3) reduces the power of hypothesis tests in regression analysis. For a given sample size, it is only possible to reduce the probability of one type of error at the expense of increasing the probability of the other. The only way to reduce simultaneously both the probability of a Type I error and the probability of a Type II error is to increase the sample size. Hypothesis testing can be something of a drudge if the calculations are undertaken manually, and the researcher has to identify the appropriate critical values from tables. Fortunately, many computer packages are readily available that take care of this drudgery (see Section 10.3), and these packages can be instructed to calculate a range of sample statistics (mean, standard deviation, skewness, etc.) and to carry out a large array of hypothesis tests. Typically, the output from these packages will report the sample estimate and also the p-value associated with the chosen hypothesis test: this p-value is the calculated probability that the null hypothesis is true. A small p-value indicates a low probability that the null hypothesis is true, and may be interpreted as evidence against the null hypothesis. If the p-value< 0.05, this is equivalent to rejecting the null hypothesis at the 5% level of significance (α = 5%); if the p-value< 0.01, this is equivalent to rejecting the null hypothesis at the 1% level of significance (α = 1%). Generally, it is more helpful to show p-values (which show the exact probability that the null hypothesis is true) rather than to report the results of hypothesis tests with researcher-determined levels of significance. We will illustrate the mechanics of hypothesis testing below in the context of (a) comparing the means of two samples; and (b) checking the correlations between pairs of variables. – 10.2.2 Comparison of Means (Independent Samples) One of the most common hypothesis tests is to compare the mean values of two samples, or of two sub-samples. The appropriate test depends upon whether or not the data are normally distributed and whether the samples are independent or paired see Appendix B for details. Here we will illustrate the procedure (student s t-test) for comparing the means of two independent samples where the data are normally distributed. Consider again the data on firm export intensity from Appendix A, which have been divided into three sub-samples based on the sector of the firms see Table 10.5. All three sub-samples are independent in that each firm is categorized to just one of the three sectors, and the three sub-samples contain different numbers of observations. The researcher might well be interested in knowing whether the mean export intensity of the firms in the automobile sector is significantly different – ’ – 195 196 Description and Preliminary Analysis Table 10.5 Comparison of firm export intensities by sector Electrical Automobile Textile 29 40.0 14.5 28 28.9 10.0 43 44.6 12.2 Number of observations Mean Standard deviation Total 100 38.0 13.8 from the mean export intensity of thefirms in the textile sector.6 Clearly the sample means are different (automobile = 29.0; textile = 44.6), but are the population means significantly different7 from each other? In more formal terms, we specify the null hypothesis that the two population means are equal to each other, and set up the test so that we only reject this hypothesis if there is a small (α = 5%) probability if it is correct. Step 1: Specify the null and alternative hypotheses H0 : μ 1 = μ2 HA : μ 1 6¼ μ 2 where μ1 = population mean firm export intensity in sector1 (automobile) μ2 = population mean fi rm export intensity in sector2 (textile) Step 2: Calculate the test statistic ± ± ± ± ± ð ±1 ± ±2 Þ ± ± ð28: 9 ± 44:6 Þ± ± ± ¼± ± ¼ þ5: 69 ± ± ± ð ± 1 ± ± 2Þ ± 2 :76 X X SE X X where X± 1 ¼ sample mean firm export intensity in sector1 ðautomobileÞ X± 2 ¼ sample mean firm export intensity in sector2 ðtextileÞ SEðX± 1 ± X± 2 Þ is the standard error of the difference between the two means The numerator of this expression shows the difference between the two sample means, while the denominator is the standard error of this difference (a measure of the precision of the estimated difference).8 Note that the 6 7 8 Or whether the mean export intensity of thefirms in any sector is different from the mean export intensity of the firms in any other sector. In this chapter, we are only testing hypotheses about whether two means are significantly different from each other: these are known as two-tailed tests. There are analogous onetailed tests that test whether one mean is significantly larger (or smaller) than another mean. More detail about how to evaluate this formula is provided in Appendix C. 10.2 How to Analyse the Data standard error will be larger the smaller the number of observations in the two samples and the more variation there is in the data. If there is a ‘large ’ difference between the sample means, and/or the • standard error is small, then the test statistic will have a‘large’ value – this may be interpreted as evidence against the null hypothesis. If there is a ‘small’ difference between the sample means, and/or the • standard error is large, then the test statistic will have a‘small’ value – this may be interpreted as evidence in favour of the null hypothesis. But what is a ‘large ’ and what is a ‘small’ value of the test statistic? Step 3: Find the critical value from t-tables We know that the test statistic follows a t-distribution, hence the critical value is: t∗n n 1 þ 2 ±2 t∗ ¼ 71 ¼ 1:99 for α ¼ 5% where n = number of sample observ ations in sector1 (automobile) n = number of sample observations in sector2 (textile) n + n + 2 = degrees of freedom α = level of significance 1 2 1 2 Note that the critical value depends upon the combined number of observations fi fi α (n1 and n2) in the two sub-samples, and upon the speci ed signi cance level ( ). Step 4: Specify the test criterion ± ± If SEXX± XX± ± ± ± ð 1 ± 2Þ ± ± ± > ± ð 1 ± 2 Þ± 1:99 ± 1 ± X± 2Þ ±±± X If SEðX± ± X± Þ± ² 1 99 1 2 ± ± ð ± ± : Reject H0 Do not reject H0 firms is ficantly greater than the mean export intensity of the automobilefirms. As 5.69 > 1.99, reject H0. The mean export intensity of the textile signi 10.2.3 Comparison of Means (Paired Samples) Here we will illustrate the procedure for comparing the means of two paired samples (paired t-test) where the data are normally distributed. Consider again the data on export intensity from Appendix A. EXP18 fi measures the export intensity of the sample of 100 manufacturing rms in 2018 while 100 firms EXP19 measures the export intensity of the same sample of fi in 2019: i.e. the samples are paired and, by de nition, the two samples contain the same number of observations. 197 198 Description and Preliminary Analysis Suppose the home country of these SMEs negotiated a free trade area (FTA) with its neighbouring countries, and this agreement came into force on 1 January 2019. The researcher might well be interested in knowing whether the introduction of this FTA had a significant effect upon the export performance of SMEs in the home country. Clearly, the sample means are different ( EXP 18 ¼ 38 0; EXP 19 ¼ 41 4), but are the population means significantly different from each other?9 In more formal terms, we specify the null hypothesis that the two population means areequal to each other, and set up the test so that we only reject this hypothesis if there is a small α( = 5%) probability that it is correct. Please note that the test procedure follows the same steps as in Section 10.2.2, but that the detail of the test is rather different. : : Step 1: Specify the null and alternative hypotheses H0 : μ 1 = μ2 HA : μ 1 6¼ μ 2 where μ1 = population mean export intensity in2018 μ2 = population mean export intensity in2019 Step 2: Calculate the test statistic ± ± ± ± ±d ± ± ± ¼ ± SEðd± Þ ± ± ð38:0 ± ± ± ± 41:4Þ ±± ± ¼ 0:63 þ5 :40 where ±d ¼² mean difference between the paired observations ³ ± SE d ¼ standard error of the mean difference Step 3: Find the critical value from t-tables We know that the test statistic follows a t-distribution, and that the critical value is: t∗n±1 ¼ t∗99 ¼ 1:98 for α ¼ 5% Note that the critical value depends upon the number of paired observations (n) in the two sub-samples, and upon the specified significance level (α). Step 4: Test criterion If 9 See footnote 7. ± ± ± ± ±d ± ± ± > ± SEð±d Þ 1 :98 Reject H0 10.2 How to Analyse the Data ± ± ± ± ± d± ±± If ² 1: 98 SEð±d Þ± Do not reject H0 As 5.40 < 1.98, reject H0. The mean export intensity of the signi 199 firms in 2019 is ficantly greater than the mean export intensity in 2018. fi 10.2.4 Correlation Coef cient (Continuous Variables) fi A correlation coef cient measures the degree of association between two fi variables, X and Y. The choice of appropriate correlation coef cient depends upon the scale of measurement for the variables. If both variables are continuous, then the researcher should use the Pearson (product-moment) ficient (r): correlation coef sXY r¼ sX ³ sY where sXY = sample covariance of X and Y sX = sample standard deviation of X sY = sample standard deviation of Y fi There will be a different correlation coef cient for each pair of variables and, if there are several variables of interest, these are normally presented in the form of a correlation matrix. Table 10.6 shows such a matrix for the continuous variables in the dataset in Appendix A. fi fi The statistical signi cance of each correlation coef cient may be tested as follows. Consider the correlation between firm SIZE and CAPS. Step 1: Specify the null and alternative hypotheses H0 : ρ = 0 HA : ρ 6¼ 0 where ρ is the population correlation coefficient between SIZE and CAPS Table 10.6 Correlation matrix SIZE AGE SIZE 1.000 AGE 0.257** 1.000 CAPS 0.146 0.006 CAPS EXP18 1.000 EXP18 ± 0.507** ±0.064 0.353** 1.000 EXP19 ± 0.445** 0.008 0.306** 0.909** p < 0.05; ** if p < 0.01. Note: The p-values are denoted as follows: * if EXP19 1.000 200 Description and Preliminary Analysis Step 2: Calculate the test statistic r where n rffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi n±2 1 ± r2 ¼ 0 146 : rffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi 100 ± 2 1 ± 0:021 ¼ 1 46 : = number of paired observations in the sample Step 3: Find the critical value from t-tables We know that the test statistic follows a t-distribution, hence the critical value is: t∗n±2 ¼ t∗98 ¼ 1 98 for α ¼ 5% : Step 4: Test criterion If r If r rffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi n±2 1 ± r2 rffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi n±2 1 ± r2 > 1:98 Reject H0 ² 1 98 Do not reject H0 : As 1.46 1.98, do not reject H0 . There is no evidence of a linear correlation between firm SIZE and CAPS at the 5% level of significance. How may the Pearson correlation coefficient be interpreted? • r is a measure of the linear association between two variables, X and Y– i.e. the degree to which the two variables move together and are collinear. • The value of r lies between ±1 and +1: ○ If r ´ +1 then there is a strong positive linear association between X and Y; ○ If r ´ ±1 then there is a strong negative linear association between X and Y; ○ If r ´ 0 then there is no linear association between X and Y. • A value of r ´ 0 implies the absence of a linear association, but the two variables may have a non-linear (curvilinear) association. • Is the value of r statistically significant? Check the p-value. • r is not a measure of the strength of any relationship between the variables – see Figure 10.5 – which depicts a scatter plot of two variables (Y1 and Y2) against X. The Y1 values are identified by the darker dots and the Y2 values by the lighter dots. The values of X and Y1 are highly correlated, even though there appears to be only a weak relationship between them. In contrast, the values of X and Y2 are weakly correlated, though there appears to be a much stronger relationship between them. < 10.2 How to Analyse the Data 201 300 250 200 Y 150 100 50 0 0 2 4 6 X 8 10 12 Y1 Y2 Figure 10.5 • Strong and weak correlations A value of cause r = µ1 implies a strong linear association, but doesnot imply a –effect relationship between X and Y or vice versa. It may be that fl X causes Y, or that Y causes X, or that both are in uenced by other variables. • fi By themselves, correlation coef cients provide little insight. But the rvalues do provide useful information about the degree of collinearity between pairs of continuous variables. If r is high (say > 0.7), then multicollinearity (see Section 11.3.3) may well be a problem in regression For this reason, regression analysis analysis. first present and discuss the continuous data in a most empirical studies that involve fl correlation matrix, and also consider variance in ation factors (see Section 11.3.3). fi The correlation coef cient between signi ficant firms (r = +0.909, p < 0.01): EXP18 and EXP19 is large and very this is unsurprising as we would expect that export a high proportion of sales one year to also export a high fi proportion the following year. There is also a positive and highly signi cant p < 0.01) firm SIZE and firm AGE, suggesting that older fi rms tend to be larger than younger firms. Interestingly, the correlations between capabilities (CAPS) and fi rm SIZE (r = +0.146, p > 0.05) and between CAPS and firm AGE (r = +0.006, p > 0.05) are both statistically insignificant. Finally, 2018 export intensity (EXP18) is highly positively correlation (r = +0.257, between CAPS (r = +0.306, p < 0.01), highly negatively correlated with firm SIZE (r = ± 0.507, p < 0.01) reflecting the finding in Figure 10.4, and uncorrelated with fi rm AGE (r = ± 0.064, p > 0.05). correlated with 202 Description and Preliminary Analysis EXAMPLE 10.3 The term ‘institutions’ refers to the formal rules (e.g. constitutions, laws, and regulations) and informal constraints (norms of behaviour, conventions, and self-imposed codes of conduct), and together with their enforcement mechanisms, they set the ‘rules of the game’ to which firms must adapt in different economies (North, 1990). It is usually assumed that advanced economies are characterized by high-quality institutions, whereas many emerging and developing economies suffer from institutional voids (Khanna and Palepu, 1997) and are characterized by increased transaction costs, market vulnerabilities to large macroeconomic and political instabilities, under-developed or missing infrastructures, and opportunistic behaviour, bribery, and corruption. How to measure the quality of a country ’s institutions? One perspective is provided by the Worldwide Governance Indicators (WGI) project of the fi World Bank, which identi es six dimensions: • Voice and accountability (VA) captures perceptions of the extent to which a country’s citizens are able to participate in selecting their government, as well as freedom of expression, freedom of association, and a free media. • Political stability and absence of violence (PS) measures perceptions of the likelihood of political instability or politically motivated violence, including terrorism. • Government effectiveness (GE) captures perceptions of the quality of public services, the quality of the civil service and the degree of its independence from political pressures, the quality of policy formulation and implementation, and the credibility of the government ’s commitment to such policies. • Regulatory quality (RQ) captures perceptions of the ability of the government to formulate and implement sound policies and regulations that permit and promote private-sector development. • Rule of law (RL) refl ects perceptions of the extent to which agents have con fidence in and abide by the rules of society, and in particular the quality of contract enforcement, property rights, the police, and the courts, as well as the likelihood of crime and violence. • Control of corruption (CC) reflects perceptions of the extent to which public power is exercised for private gain, including both petty and grand forms of corruption, as well as ‘capture’ of the state by elites and private interests. The World Bank has reported country-level data on these six dimensions since 1996 at its website (http://info.worldbank.org/governance/wgi): the 10.2 How to Analyse the Data EXAMPLE 10.3 (CONT.) latest data relate to 215 separate countries/territories. Below we reproduce the 2017 data on these six variables for just 12 countries, and then calculate fi the Pearson correlation coef cients between these six variables. It clear that all six dimensions are highly collinear with the other dimensions: all values of r > fi 0.672, and all are highly statistically signi cant. Worldwide Governance Indicators, 2017 VA PS ± ± Cameroon ± ± ± ± ± 1.05 China 1.50 India 0.39 ± ± Iran 1.30 Iraq 1.05 GE ± 1.08 0.82 0.25 0.42 0.83 0.09 ± ± 0.93 2.33 0.19 1.27 RQ RL ± ± ± ± ± ± ± 0.82 0.15 0.25 1.02 0.26 0.00 ± ± 1.20 1.20 0.68 1.64 CC ± ± ± ± ± 1.18 0.27 0.24 0.81 1.37 Italy 1.05 0.24 0.50 0.70 0.32 0.19 Japan 1.01 1.12 1.62 1.37 1.57 1.52 0.16 0.84 0.68 0.41 0.03 0.03 0.20 ± ± Malaysia 0.40 Mexico ± 0.08 United ± 0.65 ± 0.57 ± 0.93 1.33 0.26 1.41 1.71 1.68 1.84 1.05 0.30 1.55 1.63 1.64 1.38 Kingdom United States Zimbabwe ± Note: Scores range from ± 1.20 ± 0.77 1.19 ± ± 1.56 1.38 ± 1.27 ± 2.5 (poor governance) to +2.5 (good governance). ficients Pearson correlation coef VA PS GE RQ RL VA 1.000 PS 0.672 1.000 GE 0.760 0.890 1.000 RQ 0.864 0.824 0.945 1.000 RL 0.842 0.869 0.977 0.953 1.000 CC 0.832 0.832 0.945 0.928 0.984 CC 1.000 203 204 Description and Preliminary Analysis fi 10.2.5 Correlation Coef cient (Ordinal Variables) fi As noted above, the choice of appropriate correlation coef cient depends upon the scale of measurement for the variables. If one or both variables are measured on ordinal scales, then the researcher should use the Spearman correlation ficient coef (rS). Unlike variables measured on interval/ratio scales, where equal differences between values have equivalent meanings (e.g. the difference between 9 and 10 years on the age scale is equivalent to the difference between 49 and 50 years), no such equivalence exists for the rankings on an ordinal scale. All that is known is that a ranking of 1 is better (or worse) than a ranking of 2, and that a ranking of 2 is better (or worse) than a ranking of 3, but the rankings do not tell us how much better (or worse) in fi Pd each case. The Spearman correlation coef cient is calculated as follows: rs ¼ 1 ± 6 2 i n ðn2 ± 1Þ where di = X i ± Yi Xi = rank of ith item with respect to variable X Yi = rank of ith item with respect to variable Y n = number of items being ranked find out if there is any significant correlation between the export intensity (EXP18) of our sample of 100 manufacturing fi rms and the growth potential (GROW) of these firms as judged by the panel of Suppose we want to industry experts. GROW is an ordinal variable, and the calculated value of fi ±0.155. the Spearman correlation coef cient is fi fi The statistical signi cance of this correlation coef cient may be tested as follows. Step 1: Specify the null and alternative hypotheses H0 : ρ = 0 HA : ρ 6¼ 0 where ρ is the population correlation coefficient Step 2: Calculate the test statistic Z ¼ rS ,rffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi 1 n±1 ,rffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ¼ ±0: 155 1 ¼ 100 ± 1 where n = number of paired observations in the sample ±0: 016 10.2 How to Analyse the Data Step 3: Find the critical value from normal distribution tables We know that the test statistic follows a normal distribution, hence the critical value is: Z∗ = 1.96 for α = 5% ±±± ,rffi ffi ffi1ffi ffi ffi ±±± If ±± r n ± 1 ±± ±±± ,rffi ffi ffi1ffi ffi ffi ffi±±± If ±± r n ± 1 ±± Step 4: Test criterion S 1 96 Reject H0 ² 1 96 Reject H0 > S : : As 0.016 < 1.96, we do not reject H0. There is no evidence of a correlation firms and their export intensity. between the growth potential of the fi How should the Spearman correlation coef cient be interpreted? • r S is a measure of a monotonic association between two variables, X and Y – i.e. the degree to which the two variables move together in either a positive linear. • or a negative direction. The value of rS lies between ○ ○ If The association is not necessarily 10 rS ´ +1 then there ± 1 and +1: is a strong positive monotonic association is a strong negative monotonic association between X and Y. If rS ´± 1 then there between X and Y. ○ • If rS ´ 0 then there is no monotonic association between X and Y. ficant? Check the p-value. Is the value of rS statistically signi GROW is an ordinal variable where small values indicate high potential and low values indicate low potential. The potential tend calculated Spearman correlation ficient is ±0.155: the negative sign suggests that those thefirms judged coef to have the best growth to be those with higher fi export intensities, but the correlation is weak and statistically insigni cant (rS = ± 10 0.155, p > 0.05). A monotonic association between X and Y is one where the values of Y always increase as the values of X increase (or always decrease as the values of X increase), though not necessarily linearly. More formally, the first derivative of a monotonic function does not change sign over the range of X values. 205 206 Description and Preliminary Analysis 10.2.6 Correlations between Nominal and/or Ordinal Variables fi Neither the Pearson nor the Spearman correlation coef cients have any meaning if one or both of the variables are measured on nominal scales. In such cases, the researcher should instead present contingency tables and use χ 2 tests for independence. The use of contingency tables is not common in business research and so will not be discussed here, but there is an excellent introduction in Field (2018). 10.3 Many The Use of Computers in Quantitative Research excellent statistical analysis packages (e.g. SPSS, SAS, Stata, MATLAB, R) are commercially available or accessible in most universities for use on personal computers, and spreadsheet packages (e.g. Excel) also include some basic functionality. These packages cover a wide range of statistical procedures and permit rapid and comprehensive analysis even of large datasets. But, and it is an important but, this powerful functionality does not remove the need for intelligent input from the researcher: • At the heart of all statistical analysis, there is a research question often fi formulated as a hypothesis test. The researcher speci es this research question, and this in turn determines the data that s/he needs to collect. firms, countries, etc. are now readily Huge amounts of secondary data on available online, but the researcher must identify which data are necessary for the analysis and whether primary data are also required. • The researcher is responsible for selecting, cleaning, and inputting the necessary data into the package. If the input data are incorrect or are otherwise imperfect, then any statistical output will be compromised. Bear in mind the well-known adage ‘garbage in, garbage out’. • The researcher must instruct the package on which statistical procedure to use, with the choice depending upon a variety of factors, including the type of data (nominal, ordinal, continuous) and the type of analysis required. • All packages provide huge amounts of output and diagnostic statistics, and the researcher must decide how much of this output is relevant and fi how much s/he should usefully include in the nal report. Do not simply cut and paste entire tables from the computer output into the report, but be selective: ○ Produce tables for the report that contain only the essential numbers and statistics from the computer output. Simple tables (and simple 10.3 The Use of Computers in Quantitative Research diagrams) are much more effective than cluttered tables containing fl large amounts of super uous information. The late Hans Rosling was ○ ○ a genius at presenting complex statistical data in an accessible form. Only include diagnostic statistics on which comment will be provided in the text of the report. fi Most packages produce estimates with many signi cant figures, often as many as eight to ten. These estimates suggest a level of accuracy that may not be warranted, so the researcher has to make a judgement fi figures should be included. Our suggessignifi cant fi gures are usually suitable in most about how many signi cant tion is that three situations: thus an estimate of 0.03466974 should be reported as 0.0347, while an estimate of 3 462 974 should be reported as 3.47 million. • Last but not least, computer packages can generate large quantities of output but they do not interpret this output. This is where the expertise of the researcher is most important, both in discussing the results of the statistical analysis and in drawing appropriate conclusions. Do not make the same mistake as the PhD candidate who, when quizzed on the statistical analysis in his thesis during the oral examination, replied that he did not know because someone in the Statistics Department had done the analysis for him. The candidate did not pass the examination! 207 208 Description and Preliminary Analysis Further Reading ........................................................................ Field, A. (2018) Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics Discovering Statistics Using SAS Business Data Analysis Using Excel , 5th edn., London: Sage. Field, A. and Miles, J. (2010) , London: Sage. Whigham, D. (2010) , Oxford: Oxford University Press. Questions ........................................................................ 1. What are population parameters, and what are sample statistics? How do they differ? 2. What is the relationship between the standard deviation, the variance, fi and the coef cient of variation of a variable? 3. When is it appropriate to assess the degree of association between two fi variables (X and Y) using (a) the Pearson correlation coef cient, and fi (b) the Spearman correlation coef cient? fi 4. What do you understand by the level of signi cance of a hypothesis test? 5. What is the difference between a Type I error and a Type II error in a hypothesis test? Exercises ........................................................................ 1. Consider the data in the table below: X1 X2 X3 X4 X5 X6 2 5 6 11 9 8 a. XX ¼ Enumerate the following expression: 6 t¼ b. t ? 1 XX ¼ Enumerate the following expression: 5 t¼ c. t ? 3 XX ¼ Enumerate the following expression: 6 2 t¼ t ? 1 d. Express the sum of these X values (3 + 6 + 9 + 12 + 15 + 18 + 21) using the sigma notation. Exercises 2. Enter the data from Appendix A in an Excel spreadsheet (or a statistical package of your choice). Calculate the means and the standard deviations for the export intensities (EXP18) of firms in each of the four regions (i.e. north, south, east, and west). 3. Enter the data from Appendix A in an Excel spreadsheet (or a statistical fi package of your choice). Prepare a scatter plot of the observations of rm capabilities (CAPS) and export intensity (EXP18). Does this plot suggest there is a positive (negative) association between firm capabilities and export intensity? 4. Consider the data in the table below. Calculate the value of the Pearson fi fi correlation coef cient between X and Y and test for its statistical signi cance. What do you conclude? X 20 8 12 4 17 10 18 7 14 Y 63 37 31 59 44 32 49 41 33 X 3 11 19 15 6 9 13 5 16 Y 67 31 56 36 46 34 31 53 39 Now prepare a scatter plot of the data. What type of association is there between X and Y? 5. In Section 10.2.6, the estimated Spearman correlation ficient coef between growth potential (GROW ) and export intensity (EXP18) was fi statistically insigni cant. a. Calculate the change in export intensity ( 2019 (i.e. EXP19 6. Spearman correlation fi coef cient between 2018 and between GROW and b. Derive c. Produce a scatter plot of the values ofGROW and d. What do you conclude? ΔEXP. the – EXP18). ΔEXP) ΔEXP. Suppose your computer output provides you with the following estimates of means: a. Mean = 0.87459313. How would you report this mean to three sig- ficant figures? ni b. Mean = 1 956 288.26. How would you report this mean to four sig- ficant figures? ni c. Mean = 0.0000458762. signi ficant figures? How would you report this mean to two 209 11 Multiple Regression Models differ from theories in that a theory ’s role is explanation, whereas a model ’s role is representation. B l u m b e r g e t a l . (2 0 0 8 ) The most commonly used technique for the analysis of quantitative data in business research is regression analysis. It is the ‘go-to method’ in data analytics, according to Gallo (2015). In particular, it is a powerful technique for understanding the relationships between variables and which variables have the most impact, and for prediction. For example, suppose you want to understand the student-related factors that lead to good performance in the Research Methods examination. Many factors are potentially important, such as intelligence, hours of revision, lecture attendance, age, and many others. Maybe you have a hunch that female students do better than male first students. The potential factors step is (variables) to collect and on data final for each student examination on all performance. these But which of these variables has the greatest impact on examination performance, and which has the least impact? Can I predict my own performance based upon my own intelligence, hours of revision, lecture attendance, age, and gender? This is where multiple regression is useful as it allows the researcher to separate out and quantify the impacts of the multiple potential variables. In more formal terms, the basic idea underpinning multiple regression is that the researcher wants to explain the variation in the values of a depend- fi ent variable (Y) across selected observational units (which might be rms, or individuals, or countries, etc.). We might hypothesize the following model where the dependent variable is a function (f ) of several explanatory variables (X1, X2, X3 . . . Xk): Y i = f( X i, X i, X 1 2 i, . . .Xki ) 3 where the subscript i (i = 1, 2 . . . n) refers to the observational unit. (11.1) 11.1 The Determinants of Export Performance This model envisages a set of causal relationships between the explanatory variables and the dependent variable rather than simple correlations between the variables. The likely direction of causation is established on the basis of theory and of an understanding of the mechanisms through which each – cause effect relationship operates. Here the researcher must draw upon the existing academic literature on the topic, and his/her own experiences of these mechanisms in practice. So far, so good – but how important are each of these explanatory variables in explaining the variation in the dependent variable? In a natural experiment (as described in Section 5.3), the researcher would simply generate a dataset by altering the values of each explanatory variable (e.g. X 1) in turn while keeping the values of the other explanatory variables (e.g. X to 2 Xk) constant, and then recording what happens to the values of the dependent variable. But such a methodology is generally not feasible in business research because the data on the dependent and explanatory variables are observed in the real world, and it is simply not possible to set the values of 1 chosen variables while allowing the values of other variables to change. separate However, multiple regression analysis permits the estimation of the effects of X , X . . .X k upon Y, even though it is not possible to run controlled 2 3 natural experiments. The Determinants of Export Performance 11.1 Suppose, for example, a researcher is interested in explaining the variation in 2 export performance across a cross-sectional sample of firms – a popular research topic in the marketing and international business literature (see Bernard et al., 2007; Chetty and Hamilton, 1993; Sousa et al., 2008; Zou and Stan, 1998 for excellent literature reviews). Now a careful reading of this literature suggests that several fl firm-specific characteristics have been shown to in uence export performance, including size, age, and a range of technological and managerial capabilities (Krammer et al., 2018). In addition, fi it is likely that exporting will be more common among all rms in some industrial sectors (e.g. those producing small, high value-to-weight products) than 1 2 in other sectors (e.g. those producing large, low value-to-weight See Section 9.8 on experimental methods. In a cross-sectional sample, the data all relate to the same time period. The data in Appendix A relate to the year 2018. 211 212 Multiple Regression firms products), and among all in certain regions of the exporting country (e.g. those with access to ports) than in other regions (e.g. those that are landlocked or inaccessible). Finally, various general characteristics of the busi3 ness environment in the exporting country may be important. In formal terms, the researcher might hypothesize the following model of export 4 performance: EXP i = f(SIZEi, AGEi, CAPSi, REGIONi, SECTORi) (11.2) where EXPi = export performance of firm i = age of firm i fi rm i SIZEi = size of AGEi CAPSi = the capabilities of REGIONi = region of firm i fi rm i SECTORi = industrial sector of firm i Appendix A contains data on these variables for an imaginary sample of 100 manufacturing firms. It should be noted thatEXP, SIZE, AGE, and CAPS are all continuous variables, while region and sector are both categorical variables with four (north, south, east, west) and three (electronic, automobile, textile) categories respectively. 11.2 The The Regression Model general model of export performance set out in equation 11.2 needs further detail before it may be estimated by multiple regression. This fi involves (a) the operationalization of the variables, (b) the speci cation of the functional form of the relationships between the dependent variable and each of the explanatory variables, and (c) the fi speci cation of any time lags. 3 fi These country-level characteristics would be important if the rms in the sample came from several different countries and/or if the researcher was interested in changes in export performance over time (i.e. if the researcher was using time-series or panel data). Here the firms from the same exporting country, and hence firms are faced with the same home-country business environment. This is a very simplified model as our intention here is to illustrate the application of multiple data relate to a cross-sectional sample of 4 all regression analysis rather than to make a substantive contribution to the literature on the determinants of export performance. 11.2 The Regression Model 11.2.1 Operationalization of the Variables Some variables are relatively straightforward to operationalize. For instance, the AGE of the firm may be measured by the number of years since the firm was established, though alternatively age might be measured in terms of months. But some of the other variables are more problematic: • Export performance (EXP) is often measured by export intensity (export sales as a % of total sales), but some empirical studies use a dichotomous variable for export propensity (= 1 if the firm exports; = 0 if the firm does not export). Firm size (SIZE) has been measured by the number of employees in Appendix A. Other common measures of firm size are annual sales revenues or total assets . These measures are not necessarily highly correlated with each other: firms producing labour-intensive products will tend to have more employees, and firms producing capitalintensive products will tend to have more assets for the same amount of sales revenue. The researcher must thus think carefully about the most appropriate measure to use in any model, as the choice of measure will have an impact on the results obtained from the regression analysis. How are firm capabilities (CAPS) assessed? The literature identifies numerous capabilities that might be useful for exporting firms to possess, including technological, managerial, marketing, networking, and organizational capabilities, as well as dynamic capabilities. But which capabilities are important in the context of exporting? Furthermore, how are capabilities measured? It is difficult to think of readily available secondary data that would provide measures for each of these capabilities, so often researchers derive more subjective measures from appropriate items in questionnaire surveys. Typically this results in measures based on Likert scales, where the possible values are constrained to a limited range and are expressed in undefined units. The data on CAPS in Appendix A are captured on a 7-point scale, and do not distinguish between different types of capability. Region is a nominal (categorical) variable, which may take four possible values (north, south, east, west). There is no ordering to these categories, so it would be arbitrary to convert the categories into a single numerical variable with four different values. Instead, we have to define a set of (j ± 1) dummy variables to capture the separate effects of the j regions, as follows: ‘ ‘ • ’ ’ ‘ ’ ‘ ’ • • ‘ ’ 213 214 Multiple Regression Firms REGION DNORTH DSOUTH DWEST 1–18 North 1 0 1 19–34 South 0 1 0 35–77 East 0 0 0 78–100 West 0 0 1 Source: Appendix A. NOTES : 1. REGION may take j = 4 different values, hence we de fi ne j ± 1 = 3 dummy variables (DNORTH, DSOUTH, and DWEST). 2. EAST has been selected as the‘base’ region, for which all three dummy variables have a zero value. • SECTOR is also a nominal (categorical) variable, which may take three possible values (electrical, automobile, textile). There is no ordering to these categories, so it would be arbitrary to convert the categories into a single numerical variable with three different values. Instead, we have to define a set of (j ±1) dummy variables to capture the separate effects of the j regions, as follows: Firms SECTOR DELEC DTEXT 1–10; 19–28; 35–38; 78–82 Electrical 1 0 11–15; 29–31; 39–45; 83–95 Automobile 0 0 16–18; 32–34; 46–77; 96–100 Textile 0 1 Source: Appendix A. 1. SECTOR may take j = 3 different values, hence we de fine j ± 1 = 2 dummy variables (DELEC and DTEXT). 2. AUTO has been selected as the‘base’ region, for which both dummy variables have a zero value. NOTES : The model of export performance may thus be respecified as; EXPi = f(SIZEi, AGEi, CAPSi, DNORTHi, DSOUTHi, DWESTi, DELECi, DTEXTi) (11.3) Finally, it is important to take careful note of the units in which each of the variables is measured, as both the results from the regression analysis and their interpretation will vary accordingly. For example, data on sales revenue might be expressed either as $56 452 785, as $56.45m or as $0.05645bn, while export intensity might be expressed as 34.5% or as 0.345. It is advisable to provide a table in the final report that lists the exact definitions of all variables used in the analysis, and also the units in which they are measured. 11.2 The Regression Model 11.2.2 Functional Form fication involves not only choosing the explanatory variables to Model speci include in the regression model, but also choosing the functional form of the relationship between each explanatory variable and the dependent variable (Y). The choice of functional form should be based on economic theory rather than on statistical fit. The mathematical properties of the functional form should be examined and compared with what is known about the nature of the relationship between the variables. The Linear Relationship One option is to hypothesize a (positive) relationship, such as that depicted in Figure 11.1a between Y and one of the explanatory X i . Figure 11.1b shows a negative linear relationship. If linear relationships are hypothesized between the dependent variable and all the explanatory variables, then the general model in equation 11.1 5 may be written as: Yi = β 1 X + β2 2 i + β3 X3i + β4 X4i + . . .+ βkXki + ε i (11.4) where β is the intercept term (k ± 1) = number of explanatory variables β , β , . . .. . .βk are the partial regression coefficients εi = disturbance term 1 2 3 Y t cep r e t In Slope = bk Xk Figure 11.1a 5 A positive linear relationship We only consider ‘additive ’ models where each explanatory variable is assumed to have only a direct and separate effect on the dependent variable. It is also possible to model interactions between the explanatory variables, but this is beyond the scope of this textbook. 215 216 Multiple Regression Y Slope = -bk Xk Figure 11.1b A negative linear relationship The hypothesized relationship is stochastic (not deterministic), hence the fi 6 presence of the disturbance term in the model. The partial regression coef - β cients ( 2. βk) .. measure the marginal effects upon the dependent variable (Y) of unit changes in the corresponding explanatory variables (X , X3 . . . Xk), 2 assuming all other explanatory variables are held constant. A linear relationship implies that the slope of the relationship between Xk and the dependent variable is constant whether or not X takes a small value, k a large value, or a value in between. slope ¼ ΔY ¼ βk ΔX k In certain circumstances, this may be a realistic assumption. However, in other linear circumstances, relationship it may between 7 non-linear relationships, Y be more and Xk. realistic There to are hypothesize numerous a non- possible but here we will just highlight two: the semi- logarithmic relationship and the quadratic relationship. The Semi-Logarithmic Relationship Often the relationship between the dependent variable (Y) and the explanatory variables will be subject to diminishing returns, in the sense that unit increases in each explanatory variable will give rise to small and smaller increments to Y. The relationship between Y and each of the explanatory variables (say Xk) will be as depicted in Figure 11.2a (positive relationship) and b (negative relationship). 6 In a deterministic model, the values of the dependent variable may be predicted precisely from the values of the explanatory variables. In contrast, stochastic models allow for the fi fl possibility of some (unspeci ed) random in uences on the dependent variable, and these fl random in uences are represented by the disturbance term. 7 See Gujarati and Porter (2009) for an extensive discussion of alternative functional forms. 11.2 The Regression Model Y Slope = k 1 Xk b Xk Figure 11.2a A positive semi-logarithmic relationship Y Slope = ± k 1 Xk b Xk Figure 11.2b A negative semi-logarithmic relationship If semi-logarithmic relationships are hypothesized between the dependent variable and all the explanatory variables, then the general model in equation 11.1 may be written as: Yi = β 1 lnX + β2 2 i + β3 lnX3i + β4 lnX4i + . . . + βklnXki + εt A semi-logarithmic relationship implies that the (11.5) slope of the relationship between Xk and the dependent variable varies with the level of X : k slope ¼ β If k < β k > ΔY 1 ¼ βk ΔX k X ki 0 then the positive impact of Xk on Y decreases as Xk gets larger. If 0 then the negative impact of X on Y decreases as Xk gets larger. k The Quadratic Relationship In a quadratic relationship, the shape of the relationship between Y and X 2 is U-shaped or inverse U-shaped, depending upon the signs of the two partial fi β regression coef cients ( 2 and β – see Figure 11.3a and b respectively). 3 217 218 Multiple Regression Y slope = ΔY = ΔX 2 b2 + 2b3X 3i X2 Figure 11.3a A U-shaped relationship Y ΔY slope = = ΔX 2 b2 + 2b3X3 i X2 Figure 11.3b An inverse U-shaped relationship If a quadratic relationship is hypothesized between the dependent variable and just one of the explanatory variables (say X ), then the general model in 2 equation 11.1 may be written as: Yi ¼ β þ β X i þ β X i þ εt 2 1 2 2 A quadratic relationship implies that the 3 2 slope of the (11.6) relationship between X2 and the dependent variable varies with the level of X : 2 slope ¼ If β 2 < 0 and Y, while if β 2 > β 3 > 2 ¼ β þ 2β X i 2 3 3 0 then there is a U-shaped relationship between X and 2 0 and between X2 and Y. ΔY ΔX β 3 < 0 then there is an inverse U-shaped relationship 11.3 Estimation Methodology Haans et al. (2016) provide an excellent guide to theorizing about quadratic relationships, while Lind and Mehlum (2010) offer a rigorous three-step procedure for testing for the existence of such relationships. 11.2.3 A Time Lags regression model depicts the – hypothesized cause effect relationships between the dependent variable and the various explanatory variables. In many instances, it is appropriate to use data for all these variables for the same period or at the same point in time. For instance, it is reasonable to use firm SIZE, AGE, CAPS, REGION, 2018 data for and SECTOR in order to model export intensity in 2018. But it will be important to take explicit account of time lags in two sets of circumstances: • When there is an obvious lag between a change in the explanatory variable (the cause) and a likely change in the dependent variable (the effect). For instance, a firm may decide to invest in a new production facility based upon its assessment of current market conditions. But there will be a lag between the investment decision and the actual commissioning of the facility and the generation of output. If this lag were about one year, then it would be appropriate to model the change in production capacity in year t as a function of market conditions in year t( • ± 1). When the change in the explanatory variable has a prolonged impact upon the dependent variable rather than just a one-off impact. For instance, RandD expenditure tends to have an impact on innovation over a period of several years. 11.3 Estimation Methodology A crucial consideration in deciding upon the appropriate estimation methodology is the scale on which the dependent variable is measured. In this section, we will concentrate on the estimation of regression models where 8 the dependent variable is a continuous variable – here the default method- ology is ordinary least squares (OLS). 8 See section 11.3.6 for a brief discussion of the estimation methods to be used when the dependent variable is a nominal variable or an ordinal variable. 219 220 Multiple Regression OLS Estimation 11.3.1 For the sake of simplicity (and so the regression model can be depicted in a twodimensional figure), consider a simple (population) linear regression model with just one explanatory variable (X). Equation 11.4 can then be written as: Yi = β 1 Xi + εi + β2 (11.7) where Yi = dependent variable Xi = explanatory variable εi = disturbance term β and β are ( unknown)population parameters 1 2 In order to estimate the (unknown) population parameters, the researcher collects a sample of n observations on the dependent and explanatory variables, viz: Y , X ), (Y , X ( These 1 1 2 2), Y ,X ( 3 3), X n, Yn) . . .. . .. . ., ( n observations are depicted by the dots in Figure 11.4. Now OLS is a mathematical procedure to estimate the ‘best line’ through this set of observations: 9 this ‘best line’ is known as the sample regression line, and is depicted as: Y^ i ¼ β^ 1 þβ ^ 2 Xi (11.8) where Y^i ¼ predicted value of Yi Xi = explanatory variable ^ β and ^ β are sample estimates of the parameters 1 2 The sample regression line may be used to generate a predicted value of the dependent variable ± Y^ p ² fi for any speci ed value of the explanatory variable Xp), but this predicted value will generally not be the same as the actual value of Yp) associated with Xp see Figure 11.4. The difference ( the dependent variable ( – between the actual and the predicted values is termed the residual: residual ¼ ei ¼ Y i ± Y^ i (11.9) n observations, for the simple There is a residual associated with each of the reason 9 that the sample regression line does not provide a complete OLS will generate the best line when there is a dependent variable and one explanatory variable, and a k-dimensional surface when there are (k ± 1) explanatory variables, as hypothesized in equation 11.4. 11.3 Estimation Methodology Y Yp Yp Xp Figure 11.4 X The sample regression line explanation of the variation in the dependent variable: it is merely the best line through the observations, with some observations lying above the line, some below the line, and few (if any) actually on the line. The OLS procedure estimates the best line through the sample of observa- fi tions by calculating the values of the sample coef cients ^1 ðβ and ^β 2 Þ that SSE). This sum of squared residuals is minimize the sum of squared residuals ( a measure of how much of the variance in the dependent variable is not X e X ±Y Y ² explained by the sample regression model. SSE ¼ n n i ± ^i 2 i¼1 i ¼ i¼1 2 (11.10) We have provided here an intuitive explanation of OLS estimation in a simple model involving the dependent variable and just one explanatory variable. The same principles apply when estimating a multiple regression model with several explanatory variables (as in equation 11.4), though the fi formulae which may be used to calculate estimates of the sample coef cients fi will differ. The precision of the coef cient estimates is measured by their fl standard errors: smaller standard errors re ect more precise estimates, and fl larger standard errors re ect less precise estimates. Generally, standard errors may be reduced (and hence precision increased) by increasing the sample size used to generate the regression results or ensuring that the sample includes a wide range of values of the explanatory variables. Fortunately, all computer fi packages routinely provide the coef cient estimates and their standard errors as part of their output, so we will not present the formulae here. 11.3.2 Recall the The Coefficient of Determination that variance one of in the the objectives observed of values regression of the analysis dependent is to variable explain (Y). This 221 222 Multiple Regression variance is termed the total sum of squares (SST) and may be calculated as follows: SST ¼ where Y± ¼ XY n i¼1 ±Þ ð i ±Y 2 (11.11) mean value of the dependent variableðY Þ The amount of variance explained by the regression model is termed the X ±Y Y² regression sum of squares (SSR), and this may be calculated: SSR ¼ n i¼1 ^i ± ± 2 (11.12) where Y^i ¼ fitted value of Yi It can be shown mathematically that: SST = SSR + SSE In other words, the total variance (11.13) (SST) in the observed values of the dependent variable can be decomposed into two components: one component is the variance explained by the regression model (SSR), and the other component is the variance that is unexplained (SSE). Thus a measure of how well the regression model explains the variance in the observed values of the fi R dependent variable is provided by the coef cient of determination ( R 2 ¼ 2 ): variation explained by the regression total variation R 2 ¼ SSR SSE ¼1± SST SST (11.14) fi How may the coef cient of determination be interpreted? • The value of R 2 must lie between zero and one. A higher value implies a better model. • R 2 =1 implies that changes in the values of the explanatory variables explain all the variance in the dependent variable (Y). • R 2 = 0 implies that changes in the values of the explanatory variables explain none of the variance in the dependent variable (Y). The model is useless. • The inclusion of additional explanatory variables in the regression model, even if they have minimal effect upon the dependent variable, 2 2 will never reduce R . At worst, R will remain the same if the additional variable has zero explanatory power. 11.3 Estimation Methodology 2 Some researchers thus also report the adjusted R : • Adjusted R ¼ R± ¼ R ± k ± n±k 2 The value of • R± 2 2 1 2 ± 1 ±R 2 ² (11.15) will fall if irrelevant explanatory variables are added to the regression model, hence it can be used to compare different models (with the same dependent variable). 11.3.3 The Assumptions Underpinning OLS Estimation Above we described OLS as the default estimation method when the dependent variable is continuous. But OLS is only an appropriate estimation method if six basic assumptions about the multiple regression model hold: A1: The regression model is linear in the parameters, and is correctly • speci A2: • fied. The explanatory variables are uncorrelated with the terms (no disturbance term. • A3: The expected value of the disturbance term is zero. • A4: There is no correlation between disturbance autocorrelation). • A5: The disturbance terms have a constant variance (homoskedasticity). • A6: No explanatory variable is a perfect linear function of any other explanatory variable(s). If these assumptions hold, then the Gauss-Markov theorem shows that OLS 10 provides unbiased estimates of the regression parameters. But if one or more of these assumptions do not hold, then OLS estimates may be biased and alternative estimation methods may be preferable. What do these assumptions mean, and how can we tell if they do not hold? • A1 requires that the functional form of the relationship be correctly speci fied (see Section 11.2.2), and that all relevant explanatory variables are included in the regression model (see ‘Omission of Relevant Explanatory Variables’ below). Omission of relevant explanatory variables may lead to biased parameter estimates – see below. • A2 means that the explanatory variables must be exogenous in the regression model. If some of the explanatory variables are endogenous, 10 Strictly speaking, the Gauss-Markov theorem proves that OLS provides the best linear unbiased estimators – see Gujarati and Porter (2009) for further details. A seventh assumption, that the disturbance term in normally distributed (A7), is also required for the hypothesis tests shown in Section 11.3.3 to be valid. 223 224 Multiple Regression • • • • and the values of these variables are determined at the same time as the values of the dependent variables, then the researcher should specify a simultaneous equation system. An alternative estimation method such as two-stage least squares (2SLS) should then be used. A3 is essential, but is met as long as there is an intercept term β(1) included in the regression model. A4 requires that there is no systematic relationship between the disturbance terms. This assumption is often violated in time-series models, and causes OLS to generate imprecise estimates of the parameters. An alternative estimation method such as Cochrane-Orcutt should then be used. A5 means that the variance of the disturbance terms is constant. This assumption is often violated in cross-section models, and causes OLS to generate imprecise estimates of the parameters. An alternative estimation method such as weighted least squares (WLS) should then be used. If A6 is violated, then the regression model exhibits perfect multicollinearity and OLS is unable to distinguish the effects of one explanatory variable from the effects of the others. Perfect multicollinearity means that it is impossible to generate estimates of the regression parameters, but even high levels of collinearity (see ‘Multicollinearity’ below) between the explanatory variables are a problem as this results in imprecise estimates of the parameters. Possible remedies are discussed below. A comprehensive discussion of these alternative estimation methodologies is beyond the scope of this textbook, and the interested researcher is encouraged to consult a basic econometrics textbook such as Gujarati and Porter (2009). Further explanation of the problems caused by omitted variables and by multicollinearity are, however, necessary. The Omission of Relevant Explanatory Variables Suppose the researcher forgets to include all the relevant explanatory variables when s/he first specifies the regression model. Or s/he does not include a variable because the data were hard to collect. If this omitted variable is correlated with one of the included explanatory variables, then the estimated coefficient of the latter will ‘pick up the effect’ of the omitted variable and will be biased. If the omitted and included variables are positively (negatively) correlated, then the bias will be positive (negative). This is omitted variable bias. Consider the following hypothetical example of the determinants of the monetary value of damage (measured in $m) arising from a sample of building fires. The researcher may hypothesize that the extent of thefire damage will be influenced by the number of fire fighters who attend the fire and by the scale of the fires. The researcher first graphs the data on 11.3 Estimation Methodology fire damage ($m) Number of firefighters Figure 11.5 fire The relationship between fire damage and firefighter numbers damage and on firefighter numbers for a sample of fires, and obtains a scatter plot such as shown in Figure 11.5. Clearly there is a strong positive fire damage. Furthermore, OLS estimation of a simple regression model withfi re damage as the dependent variable and the number offi refi ghters as the explanatory correlation between firefighter numbers and the extent of the variable would yield a sample regression line such as that depicted in Figure 11.5. This apparently shows that more damage – firefighters a surprising conclusion! led to greater fi The problem is that the regression model is incorrectly speci ed, and that a fi relevant explanatory variable (the scale of the res) has been omitted from the firefighters and the scale of thefires are likely to be highly positively correlated: biggerfi res require more fi refighters to deal with them. The estimated coeffi cient of the included variable (the number of fire fighters) in the regression model picks up the effect of the omitted variable (the scale of the fire), and generates biased parameter estimates. In this hypothetical example, the incorrect specification is easy to diagnose regression model. Now the number of ‘ ’ and it is clear that the regression model should include both explanatory variables (i.e. the number of firefighters and the scale of the fire): this will result in an estimated positive relationship between the latter and the extent of the fire damage, and an estimated negative relationship between the former and the extent of the fire damage (as would be expected). But, in fi fi many real situations, the incorrect speci cation may only be identi ed by careful consideration of the relevant literature and of the underlying mechanisms at work (see Section 11.6). 225 226 Multiple Regression Multicollinearity Multicollinearity is a matter of degree (rather than being a particular state), and it is only a problem when there are high levels of collinearity between several explanatory variables in a regression model. In such circumstances, the estimates of the parameters of the collinear explanatory variables are fi imprecise: i.e. it is dif cult to separate out the individual effects of the explanatory variables, and these variables may well be judged statistically insigni ficant. High values (> 0.7) of the relevant Pearson correlation coeffi- cients (see Section 10.2.4) provide an indication of potential multicollinearity problems, and this is why it is always a good idea to present a correlation matrix (see Table 10.6) as part of the preliminary analysis of a dataset. fl Another diagnostic tool is to calculate a variance in ation factor (VIF) for > each explanatory variable: a rough rule of thumb is that a VIF 11 10 suggests serious multicollinearity (O’Brien, 2007). What can be done about multicollinearity? • If possible, try to increase the size of the sample. This will reduce the size of the standard errors and hence increase the power of any hypothesis tests. • A flawed remedy would involve dropping one or more of the collinear explanatory variables from the regression model or, equivalently, estimating several regression models each including just one of the collinear explanatory variables. For example, we highlighted in Example 10.3 the fi high correlations between the six governance dimensions identi ed by the World Bank. Many researchers have tried to estimate regression models incorporating all six dimensions, have been thwarted by the high correlations, have resorted to estimating six separate regressions each fi including just one of the dimensions, and have then reported signi cant fl effects due to all six dimensions. But such an approach is awed because these estimates will all suffer from omitted variable bias. • Combine the collinear explanatory variables into a single variable or index. This new variable must have some theoretical rationale, and must not be a purely arbitrary combination. Thus the mean of the six governance dimensions might be used as a single ‘governance’ indicator: this would avoid the problem of multicollinearity but would not allow the estimation of the effects of the individual variables. Sometimes a simple mean would not be appropriate as, for example, when the variables to be 11 combined have different orders of magnitude Most computer packages may be instructed to calculate VIFs. (in which cases a 11.3 Estimation Methodology weighted mean would be required) or are inversely correlated. A more sophisticated technique for combining explanatory variables is factor analysis (see Section 12.1). EXAMPLE 11.1 A commonly used construct, particularly in the international business literature, is the cultural distance between pairs of countries. Hofstede (2001) identi • fi es six distinct dimensions of national culture: The power distance dimension (PDI) focuses on how societies deal with inequalities in wealth and power between people. • The uncertainty avoidance dimension (UAI) focuses on how different societies socialize their members into accepting ambiguous situations and tolerating uncertainty. • The individualism versus collectivism dimension (IDV) focuses on the relationship between the individual and the community. • The masculinity versus femininity dimension (MAS) focuses on the relationship between gender and work roles. • Long-term orientation (LTO) stands for the fostering in society of pragmatic virtues oriented to future rewards. • fi Indulgent societies (IND) allow relatively free grati cation of basic and fi natural human desires, while restrained societies suppress grati cation of needs and regulate it by means of strict social norms. Consider the United Kingdom and China: their scores (scores range from 1 to 120) on these six dimensions are as follows: United Kingdom China PDI UAI IDV MAS LTO IND 35 35 89 66 51 69 80 30 20 66 87 24 What is the cultural distance between the UK and China? • It would be a mistake to calculate the average scores for each country (UK = 57.5; China = 51.2) and conclude that the two countries were culturally similar because of their similar scores. Clearly the countries are quite different, with China outscoring the UK on the PDI and LTO dimensions, and the UK outscoring China on the UAI, IDV, and IND dimensions. Furthermore, there is more variability in the scores for some dimensions than for others. 227 228 Multiple Regression EXAMPLE 11.1 • (CONT.) Most researchers thus use the Kogut and Singh (1988) formula, the Mahalanobis formula (Berry et al., 2010), or variance-based measures (Beugelsdijk et al., 2015) to combine the scores for the different dimensions. The Kogut and Singh formula considers the absolute values X h±I ² i of the differences in the scores, and also their variability: Dij ¼ where • K k ¼1 ik ± I jk =V k =K 2 Dij = cultural distance between country i and country k Iik = score for country i on dimension k Ijk = score for country j on dimension k Vk = variance of scores for dimension k K = number of dimensions This value for the cultural distance between the UK and China (xxx) should be interpreted in comparison to the distances between other pairs of countries: each distance by itself has little meaning. 11.3.4 The Residual Plot ±Y ² One very useful diagnostic tool is to produce a scatter plot of the residuals from the regression (ei) against the ^i fitted values of the dependent variable fi . If the regression model has been correctly speci ed, then this plot – should show a random pattern around a mean value of zero see Figure 11.6. But if there is a clearly discernible pattern to the residuals (as in Figure 11.7), or the spread of the residuals becomes larger (smaller) with larger laudiseR dezidradnatS Predicted Figure 11.6 A residual plot from a correctly specified regression model fitted Residuals 11.3 Estimation Methodology 0 Independents Figure 11.7 A residual plot showing an incorrectly specified model Residuals that show an increasing trend slaudiseR Residuals that show a decreasing trend slaudiseR Constant variance slaudiseR Fitted values Figure 11.8 Residual plots indicating heteroskedasticity values of the dependent variable (as in Figure 11.8), then something may well be amiss. In the case of Figure 11.7, the problem may be caused by an fi omitted explanatory variable, by an incorrectly speci ed functional form, or by autocorrelation between the disturbance terms. In the case of 229 230 Multiple Regression Figure 11.8, the problem is probably caused by a violation of the assumption of homoskedasticity (see Section 11.3.3). The residual plot alerts the researcher to these possibilities, and to the need for further diagnosis. Estimation Methodology with Nominal or Ordinal Dependent Variables 11.3.5 If the dependent variable is a continuous variable, then OLS regression or one of its variants will be the appropriate methodology. But often the researcher may want to estimate regression models where the dependent variable is 12 measured on a nominal or an ordinal scale. Often business decisions involve the choice between discrete alternatives, such as the decision whether to make a fi green eld investment or to acquire an existing facility, or the choice between several alternative locations for an investment. In such cases, the dependent variable is measured on a nominal scale, and the appropriate estimation methodology is multinomial logit regression (if there are more than two alternatives) or binomial logit regression (if there are just two alternatives). For example, Majocchi and Strange (2007) modelled the determinants of the choice of location in Eastern Europe for a sample of foreign direct investment (FDI) projects by Italian multinational enterprises (MNEs). Seven possible locations were considered (i.e. Bulgaria, the Czech Republic, Hungary, Poland, Romania, Slovakia, and Slovenia), and the authors reported that important determinants were host country market size and growth, the availability of labour, the quality of infrastructure, agglomeration economies, the extent of trade, financial and market liberalization, and (negatively) the openness to foreign banks. The dependent variable in this study was the choice between seven discrete alternatives, and there was no ordering to these alternatives. In other cases, there may be an ordering between the alternatives, in which case the appropriate methodology is ordinal logistic regression. For example, Carter and Van Auken (2005) modelled the determinants of the use of various bootstrapping financing techniques (i.e. delaying payments, minimizing financing, and firms in Iowa. The extent to which the accounts receivable, minimizing investment, private owner sharing resources) in a sample of small firms used each technique was assessed on a 5-point Likert scale, using data from a questionnaire survey, and hence was an ordinal variable. The model related the extent to which the 12 firms used each technique to three categories We provide here a very short introduction to these estimation methodologies. The interested reader is referred to the more complete descriptions provided in Gujarati and Porter (2009) or Hensher et al. (2005). 11.4 Hypothesis Testing of perceived business constraints and opportunities (risk, ability, effort) as well as the age of the 11.4 firm. Hypothesis Testing fi OLS (and other estimation methods) provide estimates of the coef cients in the regression model together with other diagnostic statistics, but these are just estimates. Hence the next step in any analysis is to formulate various hypotheses about the corresponding population parameters, and test these hypotheses using the results from the sample regression analysis. fi Suppose we have estimated the regression model speci ed in equation 11.4, and have generated the following regression equation: Y^ i ¼ ^β þ 1 β 2 X2i þ ^ β 3 X3i þ ^ β 4X 4i þ . . . þ ^ β k X ki ^ SEð^β Þ SEð^β 1 2 Þ SEð^β 3 SEð^β Þ 4 (11.16) SEð^β k Þ Þ where Y^ i ¼ predicted value of Yi ^ β to ^ β k are the estimated regression coefficients SEð^β Þ to SEð^β kÞ are the estimated standard errors of the regression coefficients R = coefficient of determination SSE = sum of squared residuals 1 1 2 Numerous possible hypothesis tests are available, but here we concentrate first on three of the most commonly used. We will outline each of the test procedures in principle, and then illustrate and interpret each of the tests in Section 11.5 using the data on the determinants of export performance. 11.4.1 Testing for the Significance of Individual Regression Coefficients fi Each of the estimated regression coef cients provides an estimate of the true value of the corresponding population parameter, and the standard errors provide an idea of how precise these estimates are. This test involves testing to see whether changes in any single explanatory variable (say X 2) have a ficant impact upon the dependent variable, in which case, the estimated regression coeffi cient should have a value that is signi ficantly different signi from zero. The test can, however, be used to test whether the value of the estimated regression fied value (β). speci fi coef cient ð^ β 2Þ is signi ficantly different from any 231 232 Multiple Regression Step 1: Specify the null hypothesis H0 : β2 = β = 0 where β is the regression coefficent associated with X where β is the hypothesized value of β 2 2 2 Step 2: Calculate the test statistic ^ ðβ 2Þ SEð^β 2 Þ where ^β ¼ estimated coefficient associated with X SEðβ^ Þ ¼ standard error of ^β 2 2 2 2 Step 3: Find the critical value from t-tables We know that the test statistic follows a t-distribution, and that the critical value is: for α ¼ 5% t∗n k ± where n = number of observations in the sample k = number of coefficients in the regression model (n ± k) = degrees of freedom Step 4: Test criterion If If 11.4.2 ^ ³ ³ ³ ³ ³ ðβ 2 Þ ³ ³ ³ ³ ð β 2Þ ³ > t ∗n k SE ^ ^ ³ ³ ³ ³ ³ ð β 2Þ ³ ³ ³ ² ³ ðβ 2 Þ³ SE ^ Reject H0 ± t∗n k ± Do not reject H0 Testing for the Joint Significance of Several Explanatory Variables Suppose that we have estimated the general model in equation 11.4, and we want to test whether a subset of the explanatory variables (say X 2 and X3) together have a fi signi cant joint impact upon the dependent variable, 11.4 Hypothesis Testing whether ^ ðβ 2 or ^Þ and β 3 not each of the estimated individual regression fi fi coef cients are signi cantly different from zero. Step 1: Specify the null hypothesis In formal terms, we differentiate between the maintained hypothesis (H m) fi – as speci ed in equation 11.4 – and the restricted hypothesis (Hr), which depicts the model if the null hypothesis (H ) is true: 0 Hm : Y i = β 1 X i+ β X i +β X + β2 2 3 3 H :β 0 Hr : Y i = β 1 + 2 4 4 i+ ... + βk Xki + εt = β3 = 0 β4X4i + ... + βk Xki + εt Step 2: Calculate the test statistic fi The regression models speci ed in the maintained and restricted hypotheses are now both estimated and, in each case, a note is made of the sum of squared residuals (SSE). Recall from Section 11.3.1 that the SSE in any regression model is a measure of how much of the variance in the dependent variable is not explained by the model: a low value of SSE is thus preferable fi to a high value of SSE. Now the model speci ed in Hr must necessarily have a fi higher value of SSE than the model speci ed in Hm because it includes fewer explanatory variables (see Section 11.3.2), but the hypothesis test addresses fi the issue of whether the former model has signi cantly less explanatory power than the latter model. ðSSE r ± SSEmÞ=r SSEm= ðn ± k Þ where n = number of sample observations k = number of parameters in Hm r = number of restrictions in H (here = 2) r and (n ± k) are the degrees of freedom 0 Step 3: Find the critical value from F-tables We know that the test statistic follows an F-distribution, and that the critical value is: F ∗r n±k , for α ¼ 5% 233 234 Multiple Regression Step 4: Test criterion r ± SSE m Þ= r If ðSSE > F∗ rn SSEm=ðn ± kÞ , r ± SSEm Þ=r ∗ If ðSSE ² Fr n SSE m=ðn ± kÞ , Reject H k ± k ± 0 Do not reject H 0 A rejection of the null hypothesis means that the explanatory variables (here X2 and X3) excluded in the restricted hypothesis actually have a jointly signi ficant effect upon the dependent variable, hence their exclusion is not warranted. It may be that these explanatory variables are not individually signi ficant fi (see Section 11.4.1), but together they have a jointly signi cant effect. Such a finding is common when there is a high degree of collinearity between the variables (see Section 11.3.3). 11.4.3 Testing for Common Method Variance Common method variance is often a potential problem in business research when the data on the dependent and explanatory variables are drawn from self-report questionnaire surveys. Common method variance refers to the fl fact that the respondents to such surveys tend to in ate to a similar degree the ratings they assign to the items measuring the dependent and explanatory signi variables, ficant and this leads to correlations between the spurious variables. and possibly Most statistically authorities (see, for ex ante example, Chang et al., 2010; Podsakoff et al., 2003) recommend measures to minimize possible common method bias, but common method bias may also be diagnosed ex post by Harman’s single factor test. This involves an exploratory factor analysis (see Section 12.1.1) involving all of the constructs derived from the questionnaire survey to ascertain whether a single factor accounts for a sizeable proportion of the covariance between the constructs. If it does, then common method bias is present and any conclusions from the analysis of the data must be interpreted accordingly. 11.4.4 Statistical Significance and Effect Sizes One of the most important objectives of regression analysis is to determine which of the hypothesized explanatory variables have the greatest effects 11.4 Hypothesis Testing upon the dependent variable, and to quantity the magnitude of these effects with statements such as ‘an x% rise in variable X will give rise to an expected y% rise (fall) in the value of variable Y ’. Such statements in turn lead to questions about how feasible in practice is an x% increase in X, and how meaningful in practice is a y% change in Y? If the answers to these questions are ‘not feasible’ and ‘not meaningful’, then this raises issues about the (lack of ) contribution of the research. Often, however, researchers do not comment upon the effect sizes associated with each of the explanatory variables in their regression models, but fi simply report the levels of statistical signi cance (the p-values). Cohen fi (1994: 1001) noted that most researchers ‘know that statistically signi cant fi does not mean plain English signi cant, but if one reads the literature, one finding reported in the Results section studded with asterisks implicitly becomes in the Discussion section highly signi ficant, or very highly significant, important, big! This is regrettable as statistical signifi cance does not necessarily predict effect size, as statistical signi ficance depends upon both effect size and sample size. If the sample is suf ficiently often discovers a ’ large (and many samples are huge due to the ease of downloading large quantities of data from commercial databases, and will become ever larger with the greater availability of big data), then statistical tests will almost fi always report signi cant effects – even if the effect size is small and unim- portant. We would strongly recommend that all researchers include com- fi ments about both the statistical signi cance and the effect sizes of their regression results. As Ellis (2010) comments, the relevant questions are how big is the effect and what does it mean and for whom? Ellis (2010: 1587) further suggests that ‘effect size indexes are meaningless unless they can be contextualized against some frame of reference. At a minimum, authors should interpret their results in the context of current evidence. Does the observed effect differ from what others have found and, if so, why and by how much.’ One final warning is to be aware of the units in which each of the variables are measured, and not to be bamboozled by the absolute sizes of the model, estimated firm regression coef ficients. In the export performance size is measured in terms of the number of employees: mean firm size is 114 employees coeffi cient (see Table 11.1) (Table 10.1) and the estimated regression is approximately ±0.12. If firm instead been expressed in thousands of employees, the mean fi size had firm size would have been 0.114 and the estimated regression coef cient would have been ±120. 235 236 Multiple Regression 11.5 The Determinants of Export Performance Redux We now return to the export performance model we outlined in Section 11.2, and discuss the findings that come from its estimation using OLS regression. The general form of the model was set out in equation 11.2 (repeated here for convenience). EXP i = f(SIZEi, AGEi, CAPSi, REGIONi, SECTORi) (11.2) where EXPi = export performance of fi rm i (%) SIZEi = size of firm i (number of employees) AGE' = age of firm i (years ) CAPSi = the capabilities of fi rm i (1 ± 7 scale) REGIONi = region of firm i SECTORi = industrial sector of firm i EXP, SIZE, AGE, and CAPS are all continuous variables, while REGION and SECTOR are both categorical variables with four (North, South, East, West) and three (electronic, automobile, textile) categories respectively. The two categorical variables were operationalized using three (DNORTH, DSOUTH, DWEST) and two (DELEC, DTEXT) dummy variables respectively (see Section 11.2.1). Furthermore, we hypothesize linear relationships between the dependent variable (EXP) and each of the explanatory variables, hence the model to be estimated is: EXP i ¼ β þ β SIZE i þ β AGE i þ β CAPSi þ β DNORTH i þ β DSOUTH i þβ DWEST i þ β DELEC i þ β DTEXT i þ εi 1 2 7 3 8 4 5 6 9 (11.17) where β1 is the intercept term β2, β3 , β9 are the partial regression coefficients εi = disturbance term . . .. . . Four different variants (1±4) of this model were estimated and, to aid the interpretation of the empirical results, the estimated coefficients etc. are normally tabulated as in Table 11.1. Model (1) just includes the three continuous explanatory variables but excludes the dummy variables. The value of the coefficient of determination (R2) is 0.449, while the sum of squared residuals (SSE) is 10 443. The coefficient of CAPS is positive (+4.11) and is highly statistically significant (p < 0.01), while the coefficient of SIZE 11.5 The Determinants of Export Performance Redux Table 11.1 OLS estimation of the export performance model Model (1) Intercept 34.0** (3.9) ± SIZE AGE CAPS 2 3 Model (2) Model (3) Model (4) 35.1** 26.8** 28.9** (3.9) (4.3) (4.5) ± ± ± (0.019) (0.019) (0.019) (0.019) 0.074 0.070 0.066 0.051 (0.067) (0.069) (0.066) (0.069) 4.11** 4.29** 3.86** 4.04** (0.72) (0.71) (0.69) (0.70) 0.139** 0.133** DNORTH 0.114** ± (2.92) (3.25) ± ± (3.12) (3.34) ± ± (2.71) (2.94) 1.02 DWEST 6.08* DELEC DTEXT n 2 SSE 3.16 6.03* (2.70) (2.94) 9.05** 7.34* (2.62) (2.94) 100 100 100 0.449 0.487 0.512 0.525 10 443 9 727 9 257 9 015 < fi 0.05; ** if p < 0.01. 3. The standard errors of the regression coef cients are in brackets. ± fi 0.139) and also highly signi cant (p < ficient of AGE (+0.074) is insignificant (p = 0.276). 0.01). However, the coef In model (2), the three dummy variables DNORTH ( , DSOUTH , DWEST ) related to region are included. The inclusion of three additional explanatory fi variables necessarily leads to an improvement in the coef cient of determin2 ation (R ) to 0.487, and to a fall in the sum of squared residuals (SSE) to fi 9 727. But are these changes statistically signi cant? An F-test (see Section firms that the fall in the SSE is not significant (F = 2.28, p > 0.05), 11.4.2) con and that the inclusion of the threeREGION dummy variables has not led to a ficant 0.31 100 2. The p-values are denoted as follows: * ifp signi 4.31 5.98* Notes: 1. The dependent variable is export intensity E ( XP18). is negative ( 0.115** ± 5.31 DSOUTH R 237 improvement in the explanatory power of the regression model. In model (3), the two dummy variables DELEC ( , DTEXT ) together 238 Multiple Regression with the three continuous variables are included. In comparison to model (1), the inclusion of the two dummy variables leads to an improvement in the ficient of determination (R ) to 0.512, and to a fall in SSE to 9 257. But are these changes statistically signifi cant? An F-test (see Section 11.4.2) confi rms that the fall in the SSE is signi ficant (F = 6.02, p < 0.01), and that 2 coef the inclusion of the two SECTOR dummy variables has led to a jointly highly ficant improvement in the explanatory power of the regression model. Finally we estimate the full model, as speci fied in equation 11.17, which signi includes the three continuous variables and both sets of dummy variables. In comparison to model (1), the inclusion of all the dummy variables leads to fi 2 an improvement in the coef cient of determination (R ) to 0.525, and to a fall in SSE to 9 015. The model thus explains 52.5 per cent of the variance in EXP18). the dependent variable ( SECTOR The results of the F-tests suggest that the fi dummy variables add signi cantly to the explanatory power of the model, but that the REGION dummy variables do not. Here the researcher must make a choice between reporting the empirical results for the full model (4), or only dummy for the variables. grounds that model Some even (3) which researchers fi fi excludes the prefer fi insigni cant coef cients the REGION insigni cant former approach are interesting, on while the others favour the latter on the grounds of parsimony. ´ fi µ Here we consider the full model (4). The possession of rm-level capabilities CAPS) ( fi has a positive and very signi cant effect export intensity. Recall that interpretation of fi the CAPS coef cient ^ β 4 ¼ 4:04; p < 0:01 on is measured on a 7-point scale, so the size is not straightforward other than to state that every 1-point improvement in capabilities is expected to give rise to a 4 and per very larger cent rise fi signi cant ´ in export effect ^ β intensity. 2 ¼± Firm 0:115; size ( ) µSIZE p < 0:01 on has a export negative intensity: firms tend to have lower export intensity than smallerfirms, with each additional ten employees associated with an expected 1.15 per cent lower export intensity. Firm age AGE) ( is statistically fi insigni cant, as are the three REGION dummy variables. The two SECTOR dummy variables (i.e. DELEC and DTEXT ) are not ´ only jointly significant µ as reported ´ above, but also indiµ ^ vidually significant β ¼ þ6:03; p < 0:05 and ^ β ¼ þ7:34; p < 0:05 . 8 9 Recall from Section 11.2.1 that the automobile sector has been designated as fi the base sector, hence these two coef cients estimate that, on average, firms of given size, age, and capabilities in the electrical sector export 6.03 per cent more firms in the textile sector export 7.34 per cent more of their sales than similarfi rms in the of their sales than similar firms in the automobile sector, while automobile sector. These empirical results should be compared with those findings should be noted while explanations should be provided for dissimilarfindings and results reported in previous studies of export performance: similar 11.6 What Constitutes a Good Regression Model? 3 Residuals 2 1 0 –1 –2 –3 Figure 11.9 –2 –1 0 Fitted values 1 2 Scatter plot of the residuals from the OLS regression of export performance that run counter to theoretical preconceptions. Finally, we provide the scatter plot of the residuals against the fitted values of EXP18 in Figure 11.9: the observations appear to be randomly distributed with no obvious evidence of fi model misspeci cation. 11.6 What Constitutes a Good Regression Model? OLS is a mathematical procedure, and computer packages will always pro- fi duce output (estimated coef cients, standard errors, etc.) no matter how good or how poor is the underlying model provided by the researcher (the ‘garbage in, garbage out’ principle). So what are the essential features of a good regression model? First, the model should be theoretically grounded in that good reasons should be explicitly provided for the inclusion of each of the hypothesized explanatory variables, and for the mechanisms by which each is expected to have an impact upon the dependent variable. If some of the ficients turn out to have the wrong signs, then the researcher should investigate possible misspecifi cation of the model and/or provide convincing explanations for the perverse fi ndings. Second, the model should estimated coef ‘ ’ have good explanatory power and account for a‘considerable’ proportion of the variance in the dependent variable. There are no hard-and-fast rules as to what constitutes a ‘considerable’ proportion, but a regression model that has little explanatory power is not particularly useful. Third, the model should be parsimonious in that it should be as simple as possible while still providing fi satisfactory explanatory power: statistically signi cant variables should be 239 240 Multiple Regression included explicitly in the model, while variables that have only minor effects should be excluded and captured by the disturbance term. The researcher will thus need to take the following into account in deriving final regression model, discussing the empirical results, and interpreting the importance of the findings: the • R determination ( • fi the explanatory power of the model as measured by the coef cient of signi 2 ) (see Section 11.3.2); ficant correlations between the explanatory variables, as reported in a correlation matrix (see Section 10.2.4); • fi fi the statistical signi cance of each of the regression coef cients, as tested by an array of appropriate t-tests and F-tests (see Sections 11.4.1 and 11.4.2); • a comparison of the signs and magnitudes of the estimated regression ficients with a priori theoretical expectations; coef • an examination of the residuals from the regression estimation (see fi Section 11.3.4), together with various formal diagnostic tests, to con rm fi the correct speci cation of the model (particular attention should be given to outliers, though removal of such observations from the dataset is not recommended unless the observations are clearly wrong); • a substantive assessment of the marginal effects on the dependent variable of changing each of the explanatory variables (see Section 11.4.4). Are these effects feasible/likely and/or substantial? What are the implications for policy and for A final word. Regression firm management? analysis (like any statistical analysis) involves plenty of routine calculations and formal tests, but there is art as well as science in that the researcher has to exercise his/her judgement in deciding fi not only the speci cation of the initial model but also how to adapt and interpret the model in response to the various statistical tests. Exercises Further Reading ................................................................. Gallo, A. (2015) ‘A refresher on regression analysis’, Harvard Business Review, 4 November, online: https://hbr.org/2015/11/a-refresher-on-regression-analysis. Gujarati, D.N. and Porter, D.C. (2009) Basic Econometrics, 5th edn., New York: McGraw-Hill. Sarstedt, M. and Mooi, E. (2014) ‘Regression analysis’ , in Sarstedt, M. and Mooi, E. (eds.), A Concise Guide to Market Research, 2nd edn., Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, pp. 193–233. Studenmund, A.H. (2017) Using Econometrics: A Practical Guide, 7th edn., London: Pearson. Questions ................................................................. 1. Suggest alternative measures to operationalize a concept such as the ‘international experience’ of a firm. What are the strengths or weak- nesses of the alternative measures? 2. 3. Suggest examples of relationships between variables that are likely to be: a. linear; b. positive semi-logarithmic; c. quadratic (U-shaped); d. quadratic (inverse U-shaped). What is omitted variable bias, when is it likely to occur, and why is it a problem? 4. What is the relationship between the total sum of squares (SST), the regression sum of squares (SSR), and the sum of squared residuals (SSE) in multiple regression analysis? 5. What is common method variance, when is it likely to occur, and why is it a problem? Exercises ................................................................. 1. Enter the data from Appendix A in an Excel spreadsheet (or a statistical package of your choice). Estimate the export performance model assuming that there are semi-logarithmic relationships betweenEXP18 and the three continuous explanatory variables (i.e. SIZE, AGE , and CAPS). Has this improved the explanatory power of the model? 241 242 Multiple Regression 2. 3. In Section 11.5, we reported that the inclusion of the two SECTOR dummy variables (i.e. DELEC, DTEXT) in the export performance model (3) had led to a highly significant (p < 0.01) improvement in the explanatory power of the regression model. Confirm this finding using the hypothesis test set out in Section 11.4.2. Consider the estimated export performance model in Table 11.1 (model 4). What is the predicted value of export intensity E( XP18): For an automobile firm located in the north that has 200 employees, is 40 years old, and scores 5.0 for capabilities? For a textile firm located in the south that has 50 employees, is 15 years old, and scores 2.5 for capabilities? For an electrical firm located in the east that has 120 employees, is 25 years old, and scores 4.0 for capabilities? Consider the estimated export performance model in Table 11.1 (model 4). EXP18 is measured as a percentage (mean = 38.0%), while AGE is measured in years (mean = 31 years). What will be the estimated coefficient of AGE if the variable is measured in months (i.e. mean = 372)? What will happen to its statistical significance? What will be the estimated coefficient of AGE if EXP18 is measured as a fraction (i.e. mean = 0.38)? What will happen to its statistical significance? a. b. c. 4. a. b. 12 Additional Methods of Quantitative Analysis An approximate answer to the right problem is worth a good deal more than an exact answer to an approximate problem. John Tukey A huge array of statistical methods are available, of variable levels of sophistication, and a comprehensive survey would be well beyond the scope of this textbook. Here we outline three methods which are widely used in business studies research, viz: factor analysis, structural equation modelling, and event study analysis. In each case, we explain the key elements of each method, the underlying intuition, and how to interpret the results, and then provide an example from the business literature. 12.1 Factor Analysis Factor analysis is a mathematical technique that may be used to reduce a large number of (observed) variables into a smaller number of (unobserved) factors while still accounting for as much of the variance in the observed variables as possible. Factor analysis has two main applications in business studies research: • The first application relates to the analysis of the responses from ques- tionnaire surveys, when the researcher wants to fl find out whether the answers to several questionnaire items re ect some underlying latent variable. Indeed, it is considered good practice in questionnaire design to include multiple items when trying to measure respondents ’ opinions or attitudes. • 1 The second application relates to multiple regression analyses, when the researcher has included several explanatory variables which are highly collinear (see Example 10.3 for evidence of the high correlations between 1 fi Single-item scales have considerable random measurement error, cannot discriminate nely between attributes, and cannot fully represent complex theoretical concepts. In short, they are less valid, less accurate, and less reliable than multiple-item scales. 244 Additional Methods of Quantitative Analysis the six World Bank worldwide governance indicators). Multicollinearity is thus a potential issue, and factor analysis may be used to combine the original variables into a lesser number of orthogonal factors. fi fi The factor analysis may be con rmatory or exploratory. In con rmatory factor analysis, the researcher starts by specifying the number of factors and which variables load on to (are associated with) which factors, and will typically be trying to validate or replicate previous studies. In contrast, the researcher undertaking an exploratory factor analysis will typically only have some general expectations about the relationships between the vari- fi ables, and hence does not want a priori to impose a xed number of factors or to specify which variables load on to which variables. Rather, the number of factors, and their interpretation by the researcher, emerge from the factor analysis. In this chapter, we will outline the basic ideas underpinning exploratory factor analysis, and refer the interested reader to Brown (2014) fi for further explanation of con rmatory factor analysis. 12.1.1 Exploratory Factor Analysis Exploratory factor analysis usually involves three steps. The first step is factor extraction through which initial estimates of the factors are obtained 2 from principal components analysis (PCA). fi The coef cients that relate the variables to the factors are displayed in a factor matrix. However, it is usually dif ficult to interpret the meaning of the factors based on this matrix because most factors will be correlated with most variables, hence the second step involves transformation of the initial matrix into one that is easier 3 to interpret by a process called factor rotation. The purpose of rotation is to achieve a simple structure in which each factor has‘meaningful’ nonzero loadings for only some of the variables (so that interpretation of the factors is possible) and each variable has‘meaningful’ non-zero loadings for only a few factors (which allows the factors to be differentiated from each other). Normally a loading is considered ‘meaningful’ if it has an absolute 2 A number of possible methods of factor extraction are available, but principal components fi analysis (PCA) is the most common. PCA proceeds by identifying the rst factor (component) as the linear weighted combination of the variables that accounts for the highest amount of fi variance in the dataset. The process is then repeated, with a second factor identied which accounts for the highest amount of the remaining variance. The process is repeated until the number of factors is equal to the number of original variables, each accounting for progressively smaller proportions of the total variance. The researcher then chooses the number of factors that account for a satisfactory amount of the variance. Other methods of 3 factor analysis include common factor analysis, image factoring, and alpha factoring. Several different types of rotation are available, but the most commonly used method is varimax rotation. 12.1 Factor Analysis value of greater than 0.40 (Field, 2018). The number of factors extracted will be equal to the number of observed variables being analysed, but typically fi most of the variance will be accounted for by the rst few factors. It is for the researcher to decide how many factors are retained, and s/he will be guided by (a) whether the factors have substantive conceptual meanings drawing on what the variables have in common, and (b) whether the solution accounts 4 for a ‘satisfactory ’ proportion of the variance. This ‘satisfactory’ proportion might be proportion fi de ned of the in terms variance, of a or minimum by (e.g. stipulating fi 50 per that cent) each cumulative retained factor should account for more than a speci ed proportion (say 4 per cent) of the variation in the dataset. The proportion of the variance in each observed variable explained by the extracted factors is called the communality of the variable. Communalities can range from zero to one, with zero indicating that the factors explain none of the variance, and one indicating that all the fi variance is explained by the common factors. The third and nal step is to calculate factor scores for each observational unit, which may then be used in further data analysis. These factor scores are obtained by multiplying the factor loadings by the values of the original variables, and are thus weighted linear combinations of the original variables that load on each factor: positive values denote above-average scores, and negative values denote below-average scores. Finally, each factor is thus a composite derived from several of the original variables, but how reliable are these composite scores? This reliability may be assessed by considering the internal consistency of each of the factors, or how well correlated the values of the underlying variables are. A high degree of internal consistency is desirable because it speaks directly to the ability of the researcher to interpret the composite fl score as a true re ection of the variables. The internal consistency of each of ficient the factors is typically measured by Cronbach ’s coef alpha, whose values fall in the 0–1 range: values over 0.7 are considered ‘good’, while values over 0.6 are ‘acceptable’ (Field, 2018). Factors with lower values of 5 Cronbach alpha are unreliable, and should not be used. 4 5 Other criteria might involve looking at the eigenvalues or using scree plots. Robust factor analysis requires large data samples so that the factor solutions are stable (MacCallum et al., 1999). The literature contains various recommendations about the minimum size of the sample to obtain stable factor solutions. Some people offer simple rules of thumb, such as that the sample size should be at least ten times the number of variables. Others note that the appropriate sample size will depend upon study-related features, such as the communalities of the variables: if the communalities are high, then a smaller sample size is acceptable. 245 246 Additional Methods of Quantitative Analysis EXAMPLE 12.1 fi The identi cation of personality factors has a long history in the psychology fi literature. Cattell (1943a, 1943b) identi ed various personality traits, and this list was later extended by Tupes and Christal (1992/1961) to 35 trait variables: they also used factor analysis to reduce these 35 traits to only 5 underlying factors, and showed that these factors were replicable. Subsequent empirical research has enlarged the number of trait variables and has culminated in the widely accepted Five Factor Theory (McCrae and Costa, 1997, 1999), which posits • fi ve structural personality tendencies: Openness concerns an individual’s willingness to try new things, to be vulnerable, and the ability to think outside the box. • Conscientiousness is the tendency to control impulses and act in socially acceptable ways that facilitate goal-directed behaviour. • Extraversion concerns where individuals draw their energy and how they interact with others. Extroverts tend to draw energy from interacting with others, while introverts get tired from interacting with others and replenish their energy from solitude. • Agreeableness concerns how well people interact and get along with others. • fi Neuroticism concerns an individual’s con dence and being comfortable in their own skin. It encompasses the individual’s emotional stability and general temper. McCrae and Costa (1997) further claim that these tendencies are universal and transcultural. 12.1.2 An Example of Factor Analysis: Burnout on Oil Rigs Job-related burnout is a unique type of stress syndrome, characterized by emotional exhaustion, depersonalization, and a diminished sense of personal accomplishment. It is fl a serious problem both for employers and, more importantly, for the af icted workers for whom burnout may have negative impacts upon both their working lives and their family lives. Hellesøy et al. (2000) examined job-related burnout among the workers on offshore oildrilling platforms in the North Sea. This work is unique in several ways. It involves working in cycles, two weeks off-shore interrupted by two-week periods with no job-related obligations on-shore. The working day on the platforms lasts 12 hours, even on weekends and traditional holidays. The working environment is dangerous. Working conditions are rough, and 12.2 Structural Equation Modelling often so are the weather conditions. Fires and blow-outs with fatal consequences are not unusual. Transportation to and from the platforms is by helicopter, and there have been fatal transportation accidents. The authors administered a questionnaire survey to investigate burnout. The survey included 19 items, each assessed using a 5-point scale, and 2 061 usable responses were received. The authors then conducted an exploratory factor analysis of the survey data to uncover the latent dimensions of burnout: 5 items were omitted, hence the original 19 items were reduced to 14. The rotated factor matrix is shown in Table 12.1, with only the loadings for the first four factors being included and only factor loadings greater than fi 0.4 being reported. Inspection of the four items that load heavily on the rst factor shows that these capture the respondents ’ lack of relationships with others. The authors thus term this factor ‘alienation’. The three items that fl load heavily on the second factor re ect lack of focus and motivation, and the authors term this factor ‘focus loss’. The four items that load heavily on fl the third factor re ect the respondents’ feelings of vulnerability and depression, and the authors term this factor ‘depression ’. Finally, the three items that load on the fourth factor relate to worrying about home, and this factor is termed ‘worry ’. Hence the authors conclude that job-related burnout has four distinct dimensions (factors). The first factor accounts for 34.9 per cent of the total variance, with the other three factors accounting for 4.6, 4.1, and 2.2 per cent of the variance. Together these four factors thus account for almost 46 per cent of the total variance. The authors note that some of the communalities are modest, and they suggest this is due to the skewness of the responses to some of the question- fi naire items. The computed values of Cronbach ’s coef cient alpha for all four factors were above the ‘good’ level of 0.7, suggesting that these dimensions were internally consistent. 12.2 Structural Equation Modelling Structural equation modelling (SEM) is a general modelling technique which may be viewed as a combination of factor analysis and regression analysis. SEM allows the researcher to analyse simultaneously all the paths between observable indicator variables and multiple outcome variables, rather than estimating a set of regression equations individually. SEM has been described as a second-generation multivariate analysis technique (Fornell, 1987), and its use is common in research on strategic management (Hair et al., 2012a; Shook et al., 2004), marketing (Hair et al., 2012a), management information systems (Hair et al., 2012b; Ringle et al., 2012), family business 247 248 Additional Methods of Quantitative Analysis Table 12.1 Example of exploratory factor analysis: job-related burnout Communality Feel lonesome even together with other people Feel everything is a great effort Never feel close to another person Feel that nobody understands or cares Difficulties in remembering Difficulties in deciding Difficulties in concentrating Become easily irritated Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Alienation Focus loss Depression Worry 0.65 0.74 0.41 0.41 0.41 0.70 0.46 0.59 0.36 0.51 0.45 0.61 0.71 0.76 0.47 0.57 Feel depressed 0.63 0.51 Feel easily hurt 0.47 0.51 Feel tense 0.53 0.52 Worries about home when away Worrying about home affects work Difficulties in sleeping due to worry Percentage of total variance Cronbach alpha 0.62 0.77 0.57 0.69 0.58 0.50 34.9 0.78 4.6 4.1 2.2 0.73 0.80 0.73 Source : Hellesøy et al. (2000). 12.2 Structural Equation Modelling V1 V2 LAT1 V3 V4 OUT1 V5 V6 V7 LAT2 MED V8 OUT2 V9 V10 LAT3 V11 MOD1 V12 MOD2 Figure 12.1 A path diagram for structural equation modelling (Sarstedt et al., 2014a, 2014b), operations management (Peng and Lai, 2012), accounting (Lee et al., 2011), and international business (Hult et al., 2006; Richter et al., 2016). A SEM model is often initially visualized in a path diagram which depicts the expected relationships between the variables and consists of circles and/ or boxes connected by arrows. Observed indicator variables are often represented by square or rectangular boxes, while latent (unobserved) variables are represented by circles or ellipses. Single-headed arrows denote causal relationships in the model, with the arrows showing the direction of causation. For example, in Figure 12.1, the model includes 12 observed indicator V1–V12 ) which are assumed to load on three latent constructs (LAT1 , LAT2 , and LAT3): V1 –V3 load on LAT1, V4–V8 load on LAT2 , and V9– V12 load on LAT3. These latent constructs are assumed to have an impact upon the two outcome variables O ( UT1 and OUT2) but (a) these impacts are mediated by an intervening variable M ( ED), and (b) the relationships between LAT1 –LAT3 and MED are moderated by two moderating variables (MOD1 and MOD2). Mediation here refers to a situation where the path relationship between two variables (e.g. between LAT1 and variables ( 6 6 A SEM does not necessarily include mediating and/or moderating variables. The outcome, mediating, and moderating variables may be latent constructs (as depicted in Figure 12.1), or may be observed variables. 249 250 Additional Methods of Quantitative Analysis OUT1 ) is effected through the intervention of a third variable MED ( ). In contrast, moderation refers to a situation where the strength of the path relationship between two variables (e.g. LAT1 and MED ) depends upon the values of a third variable (or variables). Direct relationships are typically depicted by solid arrows, while the moderating effects are depicted by dashed arrows. The path diagram then has to be converted into a set of equations which specify the model for input into one of the many available computer programs. 7 In the language of SEM, the relationships between the observed indicator variables and their corresponding constructs are known as measurement (outer) models, while the relationships between the constructs are known as the structural (inner) model. As Hair et al. (2014: 110 –11) emphasize, the fication ‘sound speci of the outer models is crucial because the relationships hypothesized in the inner model are only as valid and reliable as the outer models’. 12.2.1 Estimation of Structural Equation Models Now the model set out in Figure 12.1 could in principle be estimated by a sequential combination of factor analyses and regression 8 analyses. The main problem with such an approach is that each set of relationships in the model is estimated separately, and the measurement errors are aggregated in the residual error term. In contrast, the entire model (including the factor analyses and all the hypothesized relationships between the variables) are estimated simultaneously with SEM modelling. This has a number of advantages: 1. SEM allows for a more precise estimation of the effects of the latent and moderating variables on the mediating and outcome variables because the algorithm can distinguish variance arising from the imperfect meas- fi urement of variables from variance arising from the misspeci cation of the theoretical model. 2. The impact of the latent, mediating, and moderating variables upon more than one outcome variable may be assessed simultaneously. 7 8 Popular programs include AMOS, EQS, LISREL and SmartPLS. The latent constructs and the moderating variables are exogenous in the model (i.e. they are not affected by other variables in the model)– they do not have arrows pointing at them. In contrast, the mediating variable and the outcome variables are endogenous (i.e. they are affected by other variables in the model). The model in Figure 12.1 is also recursive, in that it does not include any feedback loops between the variables (which would be depicted by double-headed arrows). 12.2 Structural Equation Modelling Two complementary procedures are available for estimating SEM models, viz: covariance-based SEM (CB-SEM) and partial least squares SEM (PLS-SEM). The two procedures differ in their statistical methods, have different data requirements, and the choice between them requires an alignment of the research objective with the amount of existing knowledge (Hair et al., 2011; Richter et al., 2016; Sosik et al., 2009). Thus CB-SEM is the more appropriate procedure when there is a solid or strong theoretical foundation fi for the proposed model and con rmatory modelling is required. The correctness of the theoretical model is the basic assumption that underpins the CBSEM procedure, model parameters are estimated by minimizing the difference between the empirical covariance matrix and the covariance matrix deter- fi fit statistics are computed to evaluate the extent to which the empirical datafit the theoretical model. In mined by the theoretical model, and goodness-of- t contrast, the PLS-SEM procedure is more appropriate when the constructs and fi the relationships in the model are not unambiguously de ned in advance and the approach is more exploratory. PLS-SEM maximizes the amount of variance explained by the observed variables, and is useful for accurately predicting individual scores on the latent variables. Furthermore, PLS-SEM is better suited to the estimation of complex models or relationships, as long as the sample is of fi suf cient speaking, the use size. As of empirical Hair et methods al. (2014: 116) in business suggest ‘Broadly applications has two objectives: prediction and explanation. . . Application of CB-SEM typically overlooks a key objective of empirical studies, which is prediction. The solution to this inherent weakness is the use of PLS-SEM. ’ In conclusion, Richter fi et al. (2016: 382) suggest that ‘authors are advised to make use of the bene ts offered by PLS-SEM in research situations characterized by theorizing and prediction-oriented goals rather than by strong theory ’. How should the results from a PLS-SEM be interpreted? • The reliability of each of the latent constructs in the measurement models should be evaluated using Cronbach’s fi coef cient alpha (see Section 12.1.1) or the composite reliability (CR) measure. The CR statistic provides an alternative and more general measure of reliability than Cronbach alpha, though it is not so widely cited in the literature. CR values also fall in the 0– 1 range: values over 0.7 are considered to indicate good reliability (Hair et al., 1998). • The quality of each of the latent constructs should be assessed by the average variance extracted (AVE), which measures how much of the total variance is accounted for by the variance in the construct. The AVE is equivalent to the communality of a factor, and values above 0.5 are considered acceptable (Hair et al., 1998). 251 252 Additional Methods of Quantitative Analysis • goodness-of-fi t The overall the root-mean-square comparative RMSEA • > fit error of approximation (RMSEA), and index (CFI). Recommended thresholds for good 0.08 and CFI ± by fit the are 0.90. quality of the paths in The of the structural model may be assessed by the structural model are based on their ability to predict the endogenous constructs. This may be assessed by the coef- ficient of determination (R ) and the estimated path coefficients. The coeffi cient of determination is a measure of the predictive accuracy of each of the paths in the structural model. The values of the path coef ficients are standardized in the range from ² ( 1 to +1 ), with coefficients closer to +1 representing strong positive relationships and coef ficients 2 ² closer to 1 indicating strong negative relationships. Standard errors may be obtained using bootstrapping, and then used to test the path ficients for statistical significance. coef 12.2.2 An Example of Structural Equation Modelling: Green Innovation Kawai et al. (2018) investigated the extent of green innovation by the subsidiaries of Japanese multinational enterprises (MNEs) in overseas host countries. They differentiated between two outcome variables (i.e. green product innovation and green process innovation), and suggested that such fi first-mover innovations brought potential bene ts (e.g. positive image, premium pricing, improved product design, and advantages) for the innovating firms, but that the firms also incurred private costs/obstacles (e.g. uncertain returns, competition from existing (dirtier) products/processes, customer reluctance hence additional marketing costs). They argued that the social fits of innovation often outweighed the private benefits to the innovating fi rm, and thus outside stakeholders have incentives to exert pressure on firms to undertake more innovation. Furthermore, they suggested that three bene types of local stakeholder (regulatory, market, and societal) pressures in the host countries enhance green innovation, and that these pressures are mediated by the firms ’ implementation of formal environmental management systems (EMS). The authors thus estimated a structural equation model using PLS-SEM and the results are summarized in Figure 12.2: the two outcome variables (i.e. green product innovation and green process innovation), the mediating variable (i.e. EMS implementation), and the predictor variables (i.e. regulatory stakeholder pressure, market stakeholder pressure, and societal stakeholder pressure) are all latent constructs using data on multiple items from a Europe. questionnaire The survey relationships of 123 between subsidiaries the three in types North of America local and stakeholder 12.3 Event Studies Pressure from Multiple Stakeholder Groups Proactive Environmental Initiatives Green Innovation Performance Global Institutional Pressure Macro-level Micro-level Regulatory n.s. R b =0.127** Stakeholder 2 = 0.433 Pressure Green Product n.s. 2 n.s. Market Stakeholder Pressure b=0.429*** R = 0.149 Innovation EMS b=0.183** Implementation R 2 = 0.484 b =0.615*** Green Process Societal Innovation n.s. Stakeholder Direct Effects Pressure Moderating Effects Figure 12.2 PLS-SEM estimation of green innovation pressure and EMS implementation by the subsidiaries were hypothesized to be moderated by global institutional pressures. The authors reported that the CR values for all the latent constructs were larger than the threshold value of 0.70, and that all the Cronbach alpha values were larger than 0.64. The quality of each of the constructs was con firmed fi by all the AVE values being larger than 0.50. The overall t of 2 the model was satisfactory: CFI = 0.958; RMSEA = 0.0628. The R values for fi the individual pathways were acceptable. As regards the path coef cients, β = 0.183, p < 0.05) effect on fi market stakeholder pressures have a signi cant ( EMS implementation, whereas the effects of both regulatory stakeholder and fi societal stakeholder pressures are insigni cant, and EMS implementation is β in turn positively associated with both green product innovation ( = 0.429, p < 0.01) β and green process innovation ( = 0.615, fi p < 0.01). Furthermore, global institutional pressures have a signi cant positive moderating effect β = 0.127, p < 0.05) ( on the relationship between market stakeholder pres- sures and EMS implementation. 12.3 Event Studies Event studies are used for assessing the short-term stock market reactions to firm-speci fic events such as announcements of earnings, new CEO appointments, major acquisitions, etc. The methodology is founded on the ef ficient markets hypothesis (Fama et al., 1969), and assumes that thefi rm s share ’ 253 254 Additional Methods of Quantitative Analysis fl price at any point in time re ects all publicly available information. The fi basic idea is that if investors believe that speci c unexpected events are value-enhancing, then the share price will increase and shareholder wealth will rise. Alternatively, if investors believe the event is value-destroying (e.g. a law suit), then developed in the the share price will finance fall. Event studies were originally literature (Corrado, 2011; MacKinlay, 1997), but applications are now found in many other areas of business research, including management (McWilliams and Siegel, 1997), marketing (Johnston, 2007; Sorescu et al., 2017), anti-trust (Cichello and Lamdin, 2006), information systems (Konchitchki and O’Leary, 2011), and operations and supply chain management (Ding et al., 2018). 12.3.1 The first The Estimation Procedure fi step in the estimation procedure is to select a sample of rms that have experienced the event (e.g. a new CEO appointment) under investigation. This selection is not completely straightforward as it is essential that none of the firms in the sample has experienced any other significant value- enhancing event during the period of the study (see details below), otherwise the effects will be confounded. The second step in this procedure is to specify an event window during which the changes in the share prices resulting from the event are to be assessed. If the event takes place on day T, then a typical event window might include both the day of the announcement and the following day: i.e. the event window would be [T, T+1]. An event window that is too short may not capture the full extent of the resulting share price increase, while an event window that is too long may include the effects of other information becoming available. Sometimes the event window includes days before the ² announcement – e.g. [T 3, T+1] – in order to capture any possible effects due to anticipation of the announcement or information leakage about the announcement. The third step in the procedure is to specify an estimation window during which a market model is estimated to predict the expected share prices for each firm window in the absence be quite will of long, fi signi cant say 120 events. days Typically from ² the [T 130, estimation ² T 10], and will include a buffer zone (washout period) before the start of the event window so minimize any possible effects due to information leakage. Longer estimation windows mean more observations and potentially more precise estimates of the model parameters, but the window must not include any ficant other signi in Figure 12.3. events. The time line for a typical event study is depicted 12.3 Event Studies Estimation window Buffer zone Event window T-130 Post-event window T-10 T T+1 Time Figure 12.3 The time line for event studies fi The fourth step is the estimation of the market model for each rm, 9 which is typically expressed in the form of returns (i.e. the share price as a percentage of the amount invested) as follows: Rit = αi + βi Rmt + εt (12.1) where Rit return for firm i on day t Rmt return on a market portfolio on day t αi constant term for firm i βit captures the systematic risk associated with the stock offirm i = = = The choice of an appropriate market portfolio is at the discretion of the researcher and will depend in part upon the empirical context of the study. If US firms are being considered, the SandP500 index might be relevant. Other possible portfolios might include the FTSE100 index in the UK, the DAX index in Germany, the Hang Seng index in Hong Kong, and the MSCI emerging markets index. The estimation (by OLS) of the market model for each estimation of expected returns ± ² ARit ) for each calculate the abnormal returns ( R^ it on each day. The firm fifth allows the step is then to firm, as the difference between the realized return and the expected return on the same day. ARit ¼ Rit ² ³ ^αi þ ^β i Rmt ´ (12.2) where ^α i and ^β i are the OLS coefficient estimates 9 Other models may be used to generate expected returns, including the constant mean model, the capital asset pricing model (CAPM), and the arbitrage pricing model. 255 256 Additional Methods of Quantitative Analysis The sixth and CARi returns ( final ) for each step is then to calculate the cumulative abnormal firm over the event window: this CAR measures the increase in the market value of the firm as a direct result of the event. X AR t ¼T þ1 CARi ¼ If the event is perceived by it t¼T the (12.3) market as value-enhancing (e.g. a major acquisition), then the CAR will be positive; if the event is perceived to be value-destroying (e.g. a corporate scandal) then the CAR will be negative. The usefulness of event studies arises from the assumption that share flect prices re the economic ex post is all relevant information, and hence it is possible to estimate no impact assessment need to of any after formalize event immediately collecting a model months of involving rather data. all than making Furthermore, possible there determinants fl an of the impact, as it is assumed that all the relevant effects are re ected in the share prices. Once the estimation procedure has been completed, the researcher may use the cumulative abnormal return (CAR) data in a variety of ways: • To test whether the event has had a fi signi cant fi impact, by testing whether the mean CAR is signi cantly different from zero using the Corrado rank test (Corrado, 2011). • fi To compare the mean CARs for different sub-groups of rms. MacKinlay (1997) compared the share price reactions of firms reporting strong fits, normal earnings, or a loss in the earnings– see Figure 12.4. His results show clearly that fi rms which reported good news were rewarded pro with higher CARs, especially on the event day. • To investigate possible determinants of variations in the CARs between different 12.3.2 firms; see section 12.3.3 for an example. The Applicability of Event Studies Event studies are widely applicable in situations where the following conditions are met (McWilliams and Siegel, 1997): • fi Markets are ef cient, so that it may be assumed that share prices react fi quickly to all available information related to the pro tability of the fi firm. If markets are less ef cient (as perhaps in developing countries), then there may be abnormal returns after the event window that capture second-thought price effects (Zhu and Malhotra, 2008). 12.3 Event Studies 0.025 0.02 0.015 0.01 CAR 0.005 0 –0.005 –0.01 –0.015 –0.02 –0.025 –21 –18 –15 –12 –9 –6 –3 0 3 Event Time Good News Firms Figure 12.4 6 9 No News Firms 12 15 18 21 Bad News Firms Share price reactions to earnings announcements The event being studied is not anticipated, and hence the information • being disseminated is ‘new’. This condition may be problematic in situ- fl ations where the event re ects past performance, either good (e.g. the announcement of an award) or bad (e.g. the announcement of an impending law suit). There are no serious confounding effects during the event window or the • estimation window that could distort the results. The need to have a sample of ‘clean’ data that is clear of serious confounding effects is especially problematic, and this may result in small sample sizes, with consequent repercussions for assumptions about normality and the choice of hypothesis tests. The results from event studies are also dependent upon appropriate, and to some extent arbitrary, choices being made regarding the length of the event and estimation windows, and the market portfolio. researchers effects, to need to justify McWilliams report their how choices and they Siegel control regarding the (1997) for emphasize possible length of that confounding the event and estimation windows, and to explain how in theory investors are likely to react to reaction. the event and why this behaviour should deliver a stock price 257 258 Additional Methods of Quantitative Analysis EXAMPLE 12.2 Porter et al. (2004: 8) suggest that a key role of a company CEO ‘is to sell the strategy and shape how analysts and shareholders look at the company ’. If investors are uninformed about the company strategy, the resulting information asymmetries create uncertainty which has a negative impact upon share prices and the company’s costs of capital. Whittington et al. (2016) thus argue that CEOs should provide public presentations in which they inform investors about the broad strategies of their companies. They further suggest that the information asymmetries, and hence the potential fi bene ts, are greatest the more the CEO is unfamiliar to the investors– as in the case of new CEOs, and when new CEOs come from outside the company. They tested their hypotheses using event study analysis and data on 876 presentations made by the CEOs of US companies over the period 2000 – 2010. They found that share prices rose by 1.6 per cent on average after these presentations, but that the reactions were stronger the more the CEO was unfamiliar to investors. Thus the share price gains following presentations by new CEOs were 5.3 per cent; but they were 9.3 per cent for external, within-industry new CEOs, and were 12.4 per cent for external, outside-of-industry new CEOs. They concluded that such presentations were not ‘cheap talk’, but effective forms of impression management, especially in the uncertain months following the appointment of a new CEO. Furthermore, they report that the timing of presentations can make a difference, with greater effects if the presentations take place within the Park (2004) notes first 100 days than within thefi rst 200 days. that most event studies have analysed corporate fi announcements in a single country, but suggests that stock returns for rms fi fluences such as share price movements in other countries as a result involved in international business to a signi cant degree may be affected by global in of the global integration of equity markets. He offers guidance to the researcher on the major challenges and solutions pertaining to the use of the event study method in multi-country settings: these include the development of a stock return model, the lack of synchronism in stock market trading hours, and the differences in institutional environments between countries. 12.3.3 An Example of an Event Study: Cross-Border Acquisitions Ning et al. (2014) carried out an event study to assess the share price reactions to 335 announcements of cross-border acquisitions by Chinese MNEs listed on the Hong Kong Stock Exchange. They used a 90-day estima- ² tion window (T ² 120, T 31), various (2-day, 3-day, 5-day, and 11-day) event windows, and the Hang Seng index as the market portfolio. They 12.4 Final Comments ficant positive CARs of about 1 per cent reported signi for each of the event windows, and concluded that investors perceived the acquisitions as valueenhancing strategies. The main objective of their study, however, was to investigate the effects on shareholder wealth creation of several dimensions of MNE ownership structure and corporate control, and of various internal control mechanisms. They thus estimated the following multiple regression model with CAR as the dependent variable: CAR i = β 1 OWNi + β CONTROLi + β ICRi + β INDi + εi + β2 3 4 (12.4) 5 where CARi cumulative abnormal return for firm i OWNi variables related to the ownership structure offi rm i CONTROLi dummy variables about the identity of the controlling shareholder ICRi variables related to the internal control mechanisms offirm i INDi independent control variables forfirm i εi disturbance term = =3 =3 =7 =8 = OWN) The three variables ( related to the ownership structure were the percentages of shares held by the largest shareholder, by other blockholders (with at least 10 per cent shareholding), and by institutional investors. The CONTROL) related to the identity of the controlling three dummy variables ( shareholder, controlled, signi or fied whether the founder-controlled. firm The fi was seven state-controlled, ICR) variables ( foreign- related to internal control mechanisms, and identi ed board size, board independence, CEO duality, the number of non-executive directors, the size of supervisory board, audit committee independence, and the fees paid to auditing firms. The authors concluded that investors react positively to the presence of presence large shareholders, but negatively to the of institutional shareholders. There is also a negative impact if the largest shareholder is either the state or the corporate founder which, they suggest, is because the investors perceive potential – principal principal agency flicts con in such ownership/control constellations and discount equity prices accordingly. They also found that board independence and size had positive effects on share prices, but that large supervisory boards were associated with negative reactions. 12.4 Final Comments We have concentrated in this chapter on three methods that are widely used in business research, viz: factor analysis, structural equation modelling, and 259 260 Additional Methods of Quantitative Analysis event studies. Other methods are also available, including discriminant analysis (Eisenbeis, 1977) and cluster analysis (Ketchen and Shook, 1996; Punj and Stewart, 1983; Sarstedt and Mooi, 2019). The objective in discriminant analysis is to derive a linear combination of two or more independent variables – the discriminant function – that will best discriminate between fi two or more groups that have been de ned in advance. An example might be the use of financial ratios to predict corporate success or failure. A second example might involve the use of selected location attributes to predict firms firm-specific characteristics and ’ location choices (Chen and Chen, 1998). In contrast, the objective of cluster analysis is to discover natural groups within a sample based on the sample data, with the grouping based upon similarities or distances (dissimilarities) in n-dimensional space. An example would be to group customers into market segments based upon their personal characteristics (e.g. age, gender, ethnicity, social class). Exercises Further Reading ................................................................. Brown, T.A. (2014) Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research , 2nd edn., New York and London: Guilford Press. Chatterji, A., Findley, M., Jensen, N., Meier, S., and Nielson, D. (2016)‘ Field Strategic Management Journal Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics experiments in strategy research’ , Field, A. (2018) , 37(1): 116–32. , 5th edn., London: Sage. Questions ................................................................. 1. What are the main objectives of factor analysis? 2. What is the difference between moderation and mediation in structural equation modelling? 3. What is the cumulative abnormal return in event studies? Exercises ................................................................. 1. Read the article capabilities. 2. by Ramaswami et al. (2009) on market-based fi a. How do the authors de ne and measure market-based capabilities? b. How do they assess the validity and reliability of these measures? In addition to market-based capabilities, what other capabilities may be important for a. firm performance? Do a literature review to identify important firm-level capabilities and how they are measured. b. What are the limitations of subjective measures of capabilities, and how may these be resolved? 3. five-personality- Use any online facility (e.g. www.truity.com/test/bigtest) to assess your personality scores according to the Five Factors model. a. Which questionnaire items do you think load on to which factors? b. Repeat the analysis (if possible) with friends, and discuss the differences between your scores. 4. Reese and Robins (2017) investigate whether companies earn abnormal returns when their shares are added to the SandP500 index. Download the spreadsheet that accompanies the article (see link on p. 211, note 1), and carry out the event study as directed. 261 13 Cross-Cultural Research Collecting information from international markets is by no means a simple matter. Complexity of collecting data demands a clearer research problem, comparable sample and equivalence of questions asked. C r a i g a n d D o u g l a s (2 0 0 0 : 3 ) 13.1 The International Dimension In cases of international or cross-cultural research we need to take extra care in each and every stage of the process. Research involving unfamiliar environmental and cultural differences may complicate the understanding of the research problem, and researchers often fail to anticipate the impact of local cultures on the questions asked. This also has to do with deciding the scope and limits of the problem. In some cultures, a broader scope is necessary to cover the necessary variables. For example, concepts such as‘supermarket’ have different meanings in different markets. In Japan, a supermarket usually occupies two or three stories and sells groceries, daily necessities, and clothing on respective floors. Some even sell furniture and electronics, stationery, and sporting goods (Ghauri and Cateora, 2014). The availability of data/statistical information on exports or imports of a particular product may also be different. Even if it is available, in some countries it might not be up to date or reliable. Many countries do not have government agencies that fi collect and maintain up-to-date data. In some countries private rms collect and sell data, or the researchers may have to collect primary data themselves. It is not possible to use data gathered in one market for another market. This is important for researchers as well as managers doing research in different markets, as illustrated by Example 13.1. Comparability of data is, however, the main issue in international/crosscultural research. This is due not just to the availability of data but also to the manner in which data are collected and analysed (Chidlow et al., 2015). Researchers have to be extra careful in categorization and measurement of cross-cultural data. The international dimension explained earlier is added in Figure 13.1. of the research process 13.1 The International Dimension The process 1. Topic and research problem 2. Research design and plan 3. Data collection and measurement 4. Data analysis and interpretation 5. Presentation of the findings and report The international dimension Ensure comparability of concepts Determine scope Check the influence of culture Identify comparability differences Select methodology with minimum comparability problems Check practical issues, budget, time, etc. Attach weight to eliminate cultural differences Identify local biases Check reliability of data Check understanding of scales for comparability Watch for comparability issues Ensure that unexpected findings are not due to local biases Adjust interpretation level to different countries/cultures/markets Think of international readers and communicate accordingly Watch for language and terminology Avoid culturally sensitive conclusions Check implications for different markets Figure 13.1 Managing the international dimension in the research process Source: Based on Ghauri and Cateora (2014: 118) As illustrated in the fi gure, the researcher must be careful and adjust her/ his approach at each and every stage of the research process when doing international/cross-cultural research. It is appropriate to eliminate items or concepts that are found to be biased in one or more countries involved in the research. However, we must still be careful to ensure that a meaningful comparison between the countries can be made. This will increase the 263 264 Cross-Cultural Research probability that the results obtained are comparable and equivalent (Craig and Douglas, 2000). EXAMPLE 13.1 The ‘Habits and Practices’ study by Procter & Gamble consisted of home visits and discussion groups (qualitative research) to understand how the Egyptian housewife did her laundry. The study wanted to discover her likes, dislikes, and habits (the company’s knowledge of laundry practices in Egypt had been limited to automatic washing machines). From this study it was determined that the Egyptian consumer goes through a very laborious washing process to achieve the desired results. Among the 95 per cent of homes that washed in a non-automatic washing machine or by hand, the process consisted of soaking, boiling, bleaching, and washing each load several times. Several products were used in the process; bar soaps orflakes were added to the main wash along with liquid bleach and bluing to enhance the cleaning performance of the poor-quality locally produced powders. These findings highlighted the potential for a high-performing detergent that would accomplish everything that currently required several products. The decision was made to proceed with the development and introduction of a superior-performing, high-suds granular detergent. Once the basic product concept (i.e. one product instead of several to do laundry) was decided on, the company needed to determine the best components for a marketing mix to introduce the new product. The company went back to focus groups to assess reactions to different brand names, to get ideas about the appeal and relevant wording for promotions, and to test various price ranges and package design and size. Information derived from focus group encounters helped the company eliminate ideas with low consumer appeal and to focus on those that triggered the most interest. Further, the groups helped refine advertising and promotional wording to ensure clarity of communication through the use of everyday consumer language. At the end of this stage, the company had well-defined ideas garnered from several focus groups, but did not have a‘feel’ for the rest of those in the target market. Would they respond the same way as the focus groups? To answer this question the company proceeded to the next step, a research programme to validate the relative appeal of the concepts generated from focus groups with a survey (quantitative research) of a large sample from the target market. Additionally, brand name, price, size, and the product’s intended benefits were tested in large sample surveys. Information gathered in the final surveys provided the company with the specific information used 13.2 Data Collection in Cross-Cultural Research EXAMPLE 13.1 (CONT.) to develop a marketing programme that led to a successful product introduction and brand recognition for Ariel throughout Egypt. Source: Based on Ghauri and Cateora (2014) 13.2 Data Collection in Some Cross-Cultural Research fi It is sometimes dif cult to classify these data in ways that are consistent with the study at hand. The variables might have been fi de ned differently or the measurement unit could have been totally different and would, therefore, make the comparison invalid. For example, when studying the export behav- firms, we could use a number of studies undertaken in different findings. After a closer look, however, we might realize that smaller firms had been defined differently in iour of smaller countries and compare the results with our ‘ ’ the different locations – different measurement units had been used to deter- fi mine the size (small, medium, or large). Some studies de ned size in terms of fi sales, some in terms of number of employees, some in terms of pro t, and some fi in terms of square metres of occupied space, as in the case of retailing rms. Moreover, even if two studies used the same measurement unit, the terms fi fi of de nition were often different. In a study in Norway, for example, rms fi with 200– 499 employees were de ned as medium sized, while in the USA firms with fewer than 500 employees were defined as smaller firms. In such a comparison, if the US study concluded that smallerfirms depend highly on unsolicited orders for their initial export, we could not compare thisfi nding with our fi ndings in Norway by saying, Consistent with the US study, smaller fi rms in Norway also depend heavily on unsolicited orders for their initial export, while medium-sized fi rms are much more aggressive and do ‘ not depend upon unsolicited orders for their initial exports. ’ These types of difference are quite common, and researchers using secondary data or comparing and supporting their findings with the help of these data should be aware of the problems and make any comparisons with some caution. One way to ameliorate the situation is to discuss the differences and the relevance of secondary data to our own study, looking at the validity of the comparison and how it should be understood (see Example 13.2). Although we have mentioned cost saving as one of the advantages of using secondary data, this cannot always apply. For example, using secondary data compiled by a commercial organization might be quite expensive. In this case the researcher will have to compare the cost of collecting compared with the price of purchasing the secondary data. primary data 265 266 Cross-Cultural Research Another problem with secondary data is that it is the responsibility of the researcher to ensure that data are accurate; inaccuracies cannot be blamed on the secondary source. When referring to secondary data, the researcher has to consult and refer to the original source and not rely on an intermediate or third-hand report, because it is only the original source that can provide the required information on the quality of data as it will describe the process of data collection and analysis. Also, it is the researcher ’s responsibility to check whether fi ndings presented by another researcher are based on primary or secondary data. This can be checked by examining the internal consistency of the report being consulted. It is therefore important always to check the fi original source of data. Example 13.2 illustrates the dif culties in making fi cross-cultural comparison using of cial statistics. EXAMPLE 13.2 Jackie Davis (2001) carried out an international comparison of labour disputes and stoppages through strike action in 23 OECD countries between 1990 and 1999 using statistical data collected at a national level. However, the article is careful to point out the limitations of such an analysis for the following reasons: • fi Voluntary noti cation. In most of the countries, governments rely on employers notifying them of any disputes, which they are then able to con • firm through media reports. Failure to measure full effects. None of the countries records the full effects of stoppages at work, for example measured in terms of lost working time in companies that are not involved in the dispute but are unable to work because of a shortage of materials caused by the strike. • Different thresholds for inclusion. In the UK, disputes involving fewer than ten employees or lasting less than one day are excluded from the recorded figures. In other countries the thresholds for inclusion are particularly high. For example, in the USA, records include only disputes involving more than 1 000 workers. This can make comparison of strike fi rates between countries particularly dif cult. • Exclusion of certain industrial sectors. Some of the countries exclude the effects of disputes in certain sectors: for example, Portugal omits the public sector and general strikes. • Changes in the way figures are recorded. For example, France has fi changed the way it records lost working days, thus making it dif cult to make comparisons over time. 13.3 Collecting Cross-Cultural Data EXAMPLE 13.2 • (CONT.) Indirectly involved workers. There are differences between the countries in recording those workers who are unable to work because others at their workplace are on strike. Half of the countries, including France, the Netherlands, and New Zealand, attempt to include these workers in the statistics, but the other half, including Italy and Japan, do not. • Dispute rates affected by a small number of very large strikes. Some countries can appear to have very high labour dispute rates in one particular year because of one strike involving a large number of workers. In France, for example, there was a strike in 1995 involving all of the public sector. These differences lead some countries, such as the USA or Japan, to record a lower number of working days lost through labour disputes than, say, the UK or Germany, simply because of the different methods used for compiling statistics in the individual countries. This means that cross-cultural comparisons using nationally collected statistics need to be made with a degree of caution. Source: Davis (2001); Bryman and Bell (2003: 227). Reproduced under the terms of the Click-Use Licence. 13.3 Collecting Cross-Cultural Data Before collecting primary data, however, you need to consider a number of fi issues. First you need to de ne the relevant unit of analysis, that is who is the right person/group/organization to be consulted/studied. This is particularly important in an international research setting, as the complexity of environmental and contextual factors may have a major impact on the topic/area of research. According to some views, cross-cultural research can be viewed as a quasi-experiment in which data are collected from different contexts that may or may not be equivalent (Campbell and Stanley, 1966; Craig and Douglas, 2000). The unit of analysis and a particular research question may lead to different research design and data sources. Here the comparability of data collected from different countries/cultures also needs to be evaluated. The data source, and the information collected, should represent the same meaning and interpretation. The data collected from different countries should also be equally reliable and accurate. Diversity in culture, language, and 267 268 Cross-Cultural Research methods of communication between the respondents and also between the researchers (if more than one researcher is involved) can lead to misunderstandings and misinterpretations (see Andreasen, 1990; Craig and Douglas, 2000 and Lonner and Adamopoulos, 1997). In cross-cultural and international research projects, we need to handle this issue and develop equivalent or comparable measures and interpret- find ations of the data to be collected. In the literature, we two different approaches to handling this issue. The emic approach believes that cultures are different, and issues related to one culture should be understood through that particular cultural context. Consequently, our questions and interpret- fi ations should be adjusted to the speci c culture to arrive at the correct understanding (Pike, 1966). The etic approach advocates constructing measures that are ‘culture free’ and can be used in cross-cultural studies without posing equivalence or comparability problems (Triandis, 1972). In this situation, constructs and measures developed in one country can be used without any, or with minimum, adjustment to cultural differences. These two approaches present two extremes, and many authors recommend that while doing cross-cultural research you need to raise this issue and try to use an etic approach as far as possible, by preparing constructs and measures that are comparable. This can also be achieved by using already established constructs and measures (Craig and Douglas, 2000; Schwartz, 1992; Wind and Douglas, 1982). In many situations a combination of emic and etic approaches is advisable (Berry, 1989). Here it is suggested that you first conduct research in your own culture and then apply the construct or measurement instrument to study behaviour in the other culture (imposed etic). Behaviour is then studied in the other culture within its own context, using an emic approach in both cultures. These two observations are then brought together towards an etic approach and are interpreted in a comparable manner. A derived etic approach is thus possible based on common aspects and features. This is further explained in Figure 13.2. For data collection in a multi-culture or multi-context study, it is important to understand the differences between culture and behaviour. Quite often when dealing with a culture very different from their own, researchers collect and interpret data according to their own self-reference criteria (Ghauri and Cateora, 2006). Qualitative data collection methods can be useful in such cases, as they often use unstructured questions that can be changed, translated, and reformulated more easily. Qualitative research allows researchers to explore and probe deeply into attitudes towards product classes, brands, trends, and behaviour. It also helps to reduce the psychic distance between the researcher and the respondent, 13.3 Collecting Cross-Cultural Data Step Research activity Culture A (own) 1. Begin research in own culture EMIC A 2. Transport to other culture 3. Discover other culture 4. Compare two cultures 5-1. Comparison not possible 5-2. Comparison possible Culture B (other) IMPOSED ETIC EMIC B EMIC A EMIC B EMIC A EMIC A EMIC B EMIC B Derived etic Figure 13.2 Steps in operationalizing emic and etic approaches Source: Based on Berry (1989), reproduced with permission of Ralph Berry especially in cross-cultural research (Craig and Douglas, 2000). Qualitative research thus provides a better understanding of a given context and its underlying motivations, values, and attitudes. Data collected from a small fi number of carefully selected samples on relevant issues can be suf cient in this case (Patton, 1990). In cross-cultural research, behaviour can be observed quite easily, for example shopping behaviour in supermarkets, department stores, or bazaars. The researcher can observe whether bargaining takes place or not, how long each transaction takes, the conditions of sales and customers ’ reactions, the reaction of customers to special offers or promotions, and so on. The observation can be made personally by the researcher or by placing a video 269 270 Cross-Cultural Research camera in the supermarket. This type of research can provide useful insight into how people purchase a certain product in different cultures or fl how different environments, cultures, and promotions in uence the buying behaviour of customers. The data collected from different countries/cultures can then easily be compared (Craig and Douglas, 2000). Observations, however, are often criticized because their interpretation can be rather subjective. In international research it is particularly problematic if the researcher who is going to interpret the data is not familiar with the cultural conditions in which the data are collected. This can, however, be handled through collection of data by several researchers familiar with different cultures, and through interpretation of data through a common and systematic analytical framework. In international or cross-cultural research it is important that the instru- fi ment is adapted to the speci c culture in which it is used and not to just one of the cultures in the study. The design and administration of the instrument needs adaptation according to the education levels and other background of respondents. The formulation of questions, the questionnaire as a whole, and fl the response format often in uence comprehension and response accuracy in different cultures (Craig and Douglas, 2000; Malhotra, 1996; Schwartz and Sudman, 1996). Concepts such as ‘supermarket’, ‘household ’, ‘occupation’, etc. may have different meanings in the USA, Sweden, India, or China. All these need to be explained so that they are correctly understood and interpreted. The response format should encourage correct and clear answers. The researcher should check and ensure that everybody question in the same manner: in other words, understands the that everybody draws the same meaning from the questions. This is particularly important in questions or questionnaires that are translated from one language to another. One way to deal with this is to have an expert translate the questions, for example from English to Norwegian, and then have another expert translate the text back from Norwegian to English. The researcher can then clearly see if there has been any change in the meaning. The discrepancies should be corrected with the help of experts from both sides. There are several methods to handle the language issue in international research. For more details, see Ghauri and Cateora’s (2014) chapter on international marketing research. 13.4 Analysis of Cross-Cultural Data Multi-country or multi-cultural data can be involved in quantitative and qualitative research. Analysis of data in these situations becomes more 13.4 Analysis of Cross-Cultural Data complex due to the existence of multiple units of analysis. In this case the analysis can be conducted in different phases: first within-country analysis and then cross-country analysis. In international research the purpose might be not only to understand each country/market involved but also to assess the extent to which strategies or decisions can be standardized across different markets (Craig and Douglas, 2000). In qualitative research this issue is particularly important, as one way of handling cross-cultural differences is to collect data through less structured research. In international business research, the focus shifts from withincountry to between-countries variables. Thus country becomes the unit of analysis, and it becomes very important that the variables chosen in each country are representative of that country. Figure 13.3 explains the unit of analysis discussion. In cross-cultural or international business research, equivalences of the unit of analysis need to be considered. Moreover, this should be clearly fined de and explained so that it is not mixed up and is distinct from other units. Figure 13.3 explains that we need to consider three distinct aspects – geographic scope of the and the situational context, (Craig and Douglas, 2000: 142 3): the unit, the criteria for membership meaning in the unit, the particular socio-cultural setting. fi Geographic scope helps us to de ne the geographic boundaries of the unit we intend to study. In international business the most commonly used unit is the country, and most secondary data are collected on a country-by-country Situational context Micro Macro Age Criteria for membership Interest Gender Education Region Country City Area Geographic scope fi Figure 13.3 De ning the unit of analysis Source: Based on Craig and Douglas (2000). Copyright © John Wiley and Sons Limited. Reprinted with permission. 271 272 Cross-Cultural Research basis. Increasingly, however, research is being done on a regional basis, such as the European Union, NAFTA, or ASEAN. Sometimes we also undertake research in cities or in particular industries (e.g. airlines, banking, or retailing) in a certain country or due to the size of the countries/companies (e.g. measured as GDP, revenue, or number of employees). Criteria for membership has to be determined for the unit to be studied. Whether it is individuals or organizations, we need to decide some criteria for their inclusion. For individuals it could be age or gender, and for organiza- fi tions it could be businesses or non-pro t organizations. For organizations, we could include criteria such as size, industry, origin, internationality, etc. Situational context can range from broad socio-cultural settings (macrocontext) to a fi speci c situation (micro-context). At the macro-level our geographic unit is a country, and we should identify country characteristics fl that might in uence the behaviour of the respondent. In the same manner, if we are studying cities, we need to differentiate between rural and urban areas. Moreover, whether we are conducting the study in business settings or fl in social (e.g. at home) settings might have some in uence on the data collected. This means that we need to analyse data within each country and across different countries. Moreover, we need to examine the structure or – relationship between variables. Craig and Douglas (2000: 292 3) present this issue as illustrated by Figure 13.4. first There are two phases in data analysis. In the phase we analyse data within each country. In the second we examine and compare data between find similarities and differences. In the first phase, the relationship between different variables is studied (e.g. factors in fluencing consumer behaviour). In the second stage, we compare thefi ndings from the countries involved, to different countries. In qualitative research, the observations in each country Difference in Within-country Cross-country analysis analysis Level Structure Figure 13.4 Cross-country data analysis Source : Based on Craig and Douglas 2 ( 000: 293). Copyright © John Wiley and Sons Limited. Reprinted with permission. 13.5 Enhancing Trustworthiness might be based on subjective judgement and experience. While comparing data from different countries we can apply relatively objective techniques to discover the similarities and differences. Here we can focus either on the level of the variables or on the structure of For level issues, we need to find out whether there is a ficant difference in a certain variable in the countries compared. If there is a difference, then the researcher should try tofi nd out why this is so. In structural issues, the researcher is trying to find out whether there is a the variables. signi difference in the relationship between two variables between the countries fl compared, for example whether high income and gender in uence a certain consumer behaviour for a certain product. This type of analysis is more complex, and the complexity increases with the number of variables. Analy- fi sis in this case demands more sophisticated techniques and clear de nition of the unit of analysis that is consistent across countries (Douglas and Craig, 1997; van de Vijver and Leung, 1997). The level of variables between countries can be analysed using crosstabulation and other statistical regression and analysis of techniques. variance are Techniques useful ways such to as analyse multiple data in international business research. Enhancing Trustworthiness of Cross-Cultural Research 13.5 Driven by globalization and relocation of companies ’ activities all over the globe, there is an increasing emphasis on comparative international research and how to make it more trustworthy or reliable. Cross-cultural perspectives that were used in many other disciplines, such as psychology and ethnography, are now adopted and extended by management scholars. In quantitative research there are established methods to handle and analyse cross-cultural data and dealing with equivalence and comparability issues, as objectivity and validity in data collection is easier to enforce (Chidlow et al., 2015). For qualitative data collection and analysis, it is important to establish conformability, credibility, and trustworthiness. Some researchers claim that CAQDAS, computer-assisted methods such as NVivo, can assist researchers in enhancing this trustworthiness. NVivo facilitates coding and categorization and can assist in preparing matrix graphs. However, if you are planning to use NVivo for data collection and analysis, we suggest you look at Sinkovics et al. (2008) and Ghauri and Firth (2009). For cross-cultural research, however, it is important to be careful right from the start and at each and every stage of the research process, as suggested by Figure 13.5. The question of equivalence, that the respondents have understood 273 274 Cross-Cultural Research Equivalence of data in international research Comparability of data Data analysis Equivalence of data handling Response translation Response categories Data preparation Data collection Equivalence of research methods Data collection Stimuli Equivalence of research units Definition Selection Equiv. of research administration Timing Interaction Problem definition Equivalence of research topics Functional equivalence Conceptual equivalence Category equivalence Equivalence of international research Figure 13.5 Ensuring equivalence in cross-cultural research Source : – Salzberger et al. (1999: 23 8). your questions in the same way in each and every country/culture and have responded researcher. de fi nition in the This stage. same has to In the way be across dealt same cultures, with right way, at the is the responsibility from the start, data collection at the stage, of the problem if you are collecting data/information from several countries, the methods of data collection, the unit of study you have chosen, and the timing have to be comparable. It is quite common for a researcher to conduct the data collection in the home country and then decide to collect data from another country after a year using the same questionnaire. The equivalence and comparability of data in this case is highly questionable. Also, the method of data collection is important; for example, was the researcher present at the time of data collection through or data not? Later, reduction when data and are sorted categorization, it and prepared is important for analysis, to obtain the real meaning. Dictionary translation is often not very helpful as this does not explain the intensity or the seriousness of the response. This has to be considered throughout the data analysis stage. Exercises Further Reading ................................................................. Ghauri, P.N. and Firth, R. (2009) ‘ The formalization of case study research in international business’ , Der Markt: Journal for Marketing, 48: 29–40. Grisar-Kassé, K. (2004) ‘The role of negative personal experiences in cross-cultural case study research: failure or opportunity’, in Marshan-Piekkari, R. and Welch, C. (eds.), A Handbook of Qualitative Research Methods for International Business, Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar, pp. 144 – 61. Sinkovics, R., Penz, E., and Ghauri, P.N. (2008)‘Enhancing trustworthiness of qualitative research in international business’, Management International Review, 48(6): 689–714. Questions ................................................................. 1. Is cross-cultural research different from research in the same cultural setting? Discuss. 2. What is meant by the equivalence of the unit of analysis? Explain with examples. 3. Explain the two concepts emic and etic. How can we handle these issues in cross-cultural research? 4. How can a cross-cultural research project be most meaningful? What adjustments are necessary to accommodate cultural differences? Exercises ................................................................. 1. firm wants to go into a new market and is analysing Russia and India as possible candidates. Do you think the fi rm can collect demographic A data on the two markets and compare them to see which market is more suitable? Advise the company on how it can enhance the comparability of the data collected in the two markets. 2. Discuss the emic and etic approach of data collection for a multi-country research project that you have to undertake. Select three countries of your choice and explain how you can use these approaches. 3. You are working on a qualitative research project that involves interviewing several firms to understand how companies evaluate a new market to enter. Suggest ways to enhance the reliability of your results. 275 14 Writing the Final Report Everything should be as simple as it can be, but not simpler. Albert Einstein After completing the data collection and data analysis, you have to put the research problem, the data collected, and the ent, and persuasive report. Fortunately, findings into a logical, consist- as with research proposals and methodology, research reports conform to a fairly standard format. In many cases, an oral presentation is also made to present your findings and research. We deal with both of these aspects. 14.1 Purpose and Importance of a Written Presentation Before starting to write the final report, you should consider its purpose and to whom it is addressed. Research reports typically start with an executive summary providing the important points from the report. After the summary, a preface is normally written where we explain the course of the study and thank individuals and organizations who have been helpful in data collection, funding, etc. If the report is a Master’s thesis, it may look different from a report that is fi presented to a company/manager on a speci c issue. In the latter case, the report can be very concise and to the point, so that the manager can easily comprehend it and use the results in decision making. On the other hand, if the report is a thesis or project work, it has to be more detailed and written in a systematic manner. In this case, all the details regarding problem formulation, data collection, the method of analysis, and conclusion should be presented. In fact, we would argue that a report written to‘sell’ an idea to management should also be very detailed and convincing with regard to the fi concepts used, comparison with present systems and ideas, and the bene ts of the new system/ideas (Sekaran, 1992). We believe that a report needs to be detailed, argumentative, and convincing in both cases. Another type of report is an academic paper written with the purpose of publication. We will deal with this type of report separately. 14.2 Guidelines You must also realize that your report, project work, or thesis will be assessed according to the criteria set by your institution or supervisor. Before working on the (Bloom et final report, you must revisit these criteria. Several authors al., 1971; Easterby-Smith et al., 1991; Saunders et al., 2016) provide educational objectives for different levels of research and report. For lower levels, your report should demonstrate understanding and knowledge of the topic. At a higher level, your report should demonstrate not only that you have knowledge of the topic but also that you can apply it to a concrete problem, having analysed the situation. At a still higher level, in addition to the above you have to demonstrate that you can synthesize, evaluate, and analyse to draw conclusions. The process of writing up a report is complex and sometimes tedious work. findings and arguments presented in a The report should be concise, and the convincing and consistent way. It is also important to present the research methodology and results in such a way that the reader can judge the validity and relevance of the findings. Hint: A report needs to be argumentative and convincing, whether it is to be submitted to a company or as an academic thesis. We should give an account of our methods ’ weaknesses and strengths and the necessary details so that readers can make their own judgements on the validity and reliability of our study and reader that we have, certain problem findings. We should convince the first of all, done our job as a researcher, investigating a area with systematic data collection and data analysis, presented in a logical, easy-to-read and understandable report. Second, we must show that we have followed the technically correct and consistent fi method expected of a quali ed report, that our hypotheses and conclusions are properly supported by existing literature and empirical evidence, and that there is a logical congruence between different parts of the report. We should also be concerned that our report gives due credit to earlier studies we have used and that we refer to all the sources in a proper manner. These two aspects are very important in fi quali ed report writing and are therefore treated separately in this chapter. 14.2 Guidelines 14.2.1 Audience The best reports are those that are written with a particular audience in mind. The contents, the length, the terminology used, the focus, and the 277 278 Writing the Final Report presentation of data and results all depend upon the audience. In writing a fi report for a company/manager the tone should be concise and ef cient and the writing and use of terminology should be according to their background, considering how much they already know about the topic. In this case, an executive summary becomes very important, as it provides a summary of the report in a direct and brief manner. Managers can then go into the report fully or into only those parts of it they feel appropriate. In the case of a thesis, you need to check the rules and regulations of your particular school or supervisor. The guidelines provided later in this chapter on the structure of the report are the most commonly used. The audience fluences in the overall presentation, extensive use of tables and to decide which tables and as figures and some audiences are distracted by by footnotes. In this case, you have figures are necessary in the text and which should go into appendices, where those who are interested can look at them. Any fl restriction on the number of pages also in uences the above. Quite often there is a maximum page limit but no minimum limit, which shows that a concise report is more useful than an extensive one. In any case, the audience determines the type, the level, and the complexity of the report. Researchers fi must make every effort to acquaint themselves with the speci c preferences of their audience. They should not consider these preferences unalterable, but any deviation from them should be made with reason and not from ignorance (Boyd et al., 1989; Churchill and Iacobucci, 2015). 14.2.2 Good Communication It goes without saying that the report should be readable. Scholars often suggest that the report has to be clear, concise, coherent, focused, lively, exciting, meaningful, and without pedantry (Rubin and Rubin, 1995; Sekaran, 1992). The main idea is that the text should provide a clear explanation and should entice the readers, making them interested and curious to read the whole report. The assumptions and methods should be clearly stated fl and explained. There should be a natural and smooth ow from one part of the report to the next. Many researchers use a pedantic writing style to impress the reader with their sophisticated concepts and terminology. In our opinion, such a report does not communicate well. The reader should flow through the report without any hindrance or being stuck in understanding diffi cult terminology. The format of the report enhances its readability; the use of headings and subheadings takes the reader through the report step by step. The reader should understand what you are saying and where you are going. As a good communicator, you can ‘program ’ the reader’s mind so that s/he reads your report as you want it to be read or as you believe readers would like to read it. 14.2 Guidelines Do not expect your reader to read between the lines; you have to use direct and simple language. Hint: You can ‘ program’ a reader’s mind as to how he or she reads your report. 14.2.3 Language and Flow It is not easy to write clear and simple sentences. As Elliot (1980: 18 – 19) says: Think what you want to say. Write your sentence. Then strip it of all adverbs and adjectives. Reduce the sentence to its skeleton. Let the verbs and nouns do the work. If your skeleton sentence does not express your thought precisely, you have got the wrong verb or noun. Dig for the right one. Nouns and verbs carry the guns in good writing; adjectives and adverbs are decorative camp followers. ficult language and terminology does not really impress the reader Using dif fluences the readability of the report adversely. Good writing does not but in mean setting obstacles for the reader to overcome. flow Sentences and paragraphs should be kept fairly short. The of argu- ments should run logically through the report. A concept or a terminology should be explained when first mentioned. In our opinion, the fewer the words, the better the report. Several short sentences are better than one long sentence. Do not torture the reader by fi explaining your dif culties in collecting data or interviewing prominent business executives; just say what you have done and how you have collected your data. If you are using a concept to explain a certain situation, use the same concept every time you want to explain that situation. In speech, when we explain a certain situation we repeat facts and stories to make sure that the necessary listener in has written heard and language, understood and you what should we mean. always This avoid is not repeating things. Reports and theses are never judged according to their length or weight. Normally we do not start a sentence or a paragraph with‘but ’, ‘because’, or a number. If it is necessary to start a sentence with a number, it should be expressed in words: for example ‘Fifty per cent’ instead of ‘50 per cent’. Whatever rules are used, the most important aspect is to be consistent in writing, spelling (for example UK English versus US English), terminology, use of headings, and figures and tables. 279 280 Writing the Final Report Hint: Using fi dif cult language and terminology does not really impress the reader. first The report, especially the descriptive part, should be written in the person. Some people believe that impersonal language conveys objectivity and suggest writing in the third person. We, however, advise that the observer/researchers be referred to as ‘I ’ or ‘we’ and that participants or respondents, etc. be referred to as ‘we ’ or ‘them ’. 14.2.4 Form and Style The question of form and style is often more a matter of likes and dislikes or individual style than a matter of rules. However, the following are common mistakes and should be avoided (Grønhaug, 1985: 71): Telegraphic style. This means that a report is written in sentences which • are not connected to each other with any logical consistency. Long and complicated sentences. Reports are sometimes written in long • sentences that are not comprehensible without being read several times. Writing short and concise sentences is, however, a good way of writing reports. Usage of terminology and differences between spoken and written lan- • guage. When using textbook or other terminology, you should be sure that the reader will understand their use in the same way. It is quite common for students to use a spoken language form in their report. In report writing you should follow the rules of writing and avoid using a spoken or slang version. Shortage • of tables, fi gures, or other illustrations. You should try to simplify, highlight, and complement the important and interesting parts figures and tables or other illustrations. It is important that these tables and figures should not replace the text. of the report with to point out They are used to complement or further explain the text or the point being made. Our experience as researchers is that we have to write and rewrite reports at least four or five times before they are in a final shape. This is the working draft, and after writing this you should go through the material again, along with the data and findings, to see if there is something that has been missed that should be included in the report. Then read the draft again and start correcting it as you read. Add the information you think is missing and delete the information you feel is irrelevant or repetitive. At 14.3 Structure of the Report the same time, check that the information is correctly placed; if it is not, reshuf fle before the text. This process will need to be repeated three to four times you have an almost-ready report. Finally, you should check the language and ideally have someone else read the report. It is quite common that, having read and reread the report several times, you become blind to drawbacks or mistakes. Hint: Before submitting your report, have someone else read it through. The appearance is very important. It does not matter how well written a report is if it does not look good; it leaves a bad impression on the reader. Here the title page, the margins and the structure of the report are very important. It ones. common It is is easier to read practice to double-spaced use double reports spacing than and single-spaced indent first the word of each paragraph. In the case of single-spaced reports, double-space between the paragraphs to mark the new paragraphs. Regardless of whether the report is single-spaced or double-spaced, the following documents should always be typed single-spaced: letters, displayed quotations, footnotes (but use double spacing between them), tables, and figures (Murphy and Hildebrandt, 1988). 14.2.5 Headings The headings serve as an outline of a report and should be clear, meaningful, and consistent. Headings are often numbered to highlight different heading levels, but numbers are not essential. It is possible to use different styles or type to make these levels explicit. The most important aspect is not to use too many levels of heading, as the reader should be able to differentiate easily between different levels of headings and subheadings. 14.3 Structure of the Report The following outline is a commonly used format for a research report. The chosen format is, however, dependent upon the reader for whom the report is prepared. One common format is as follows: 1. title page; 2. table of contents; 3. executive summary; 4. introduction and problem statement; 281 282 Writing the Final Report 5. theoretical background; 6. methodology; 7. findings or empirical study; 8. conclusions and recommendations; 9. footnotes; 10. bibliography or references; 11. appendix. 14.3.1 Title Page The title page should indicate the topic of research and the name(s) of the researcher(s) (authors). Second, it should indicate the name of the organization where the report has been prepared and for what programme, if appropriate; for example, ‘MSc Thesis for King’s College London’. If it is a research project ordered or demanded by a company or any other organization, the company ’s or organization’s name should also appear on the title page. Moreover, if the project has received any financial help from an institute other than the school or university, this name should also appear on the title page. 14.3.2 Table of Contents The table of contents lists the contents of the report with page numbers. Here, the headings and subheadings are presented. The reader should be able to differentiate the headings and subheadings both in the table of contents and in the report (see Figure 14.1). The table of contents should also include tables and figures with page numbers. 14.3.3 Executive Summary A summary provides the important aspects of each part of the report. It is often stated that a summary should be self-suf readers, especially business executives, ficient often read because most of the only the summary (Churchill, 1999). The summary should thus highlight the whole report. In addition, it should be based on the main report and should not include any new material. The data you have collected, presented, and analysed are often buried in the middle or at the end of the report and have to be brought forward in the summary so that the reader can quickly and without any trouble perceive the message of the report (Bolsky, 1988). The length of a summary depends upon the complexity of the original material. Normally you should reduce material in a summary to, at the most, 5 per cent of the report’s full length or to a couple of pages. An ideal length is two to three pages. 14.3 Structure of the Report Contents List of figures ix List of screenshots x List of boxes xi List of tables xii Preface xiii Guided tour of the book xv Publisher’s acknowledgements xvii PART I CHALLENGES AND AMBIGUITIES OF BUSINESS RESEARCH 1 1 INTRODUCTION 3 1.1 Purpose of the book 3 1.2 Readership 4 1.3 On the use of the book 5 RESEARCH IN BUSINESS STUDIES 7 2.1 The nature of knowledge development 7 2.2 Why we do research 8 2 Figure 14.1 14.3.4 An example of a table of contents Introduction and Problem Statement An introduction to a research report should present what the study is about and what the purpose of the study is. This background will provide the reader with the necessary information to understand and comprehend the rest of the report. The objectives and the purpose of the study should be clearly mentioned in this section. After reading this section, the reader should have complete information on what the report deals with, why you are studying a particular problem and what can be expected in the rest of the report. Here fi all the unfamiliar terms should be clari ed and the concepts of the research problems fi de ned. Some account of what has already been done in this research or problem area is also presented. This section should also explain how the report is organized, so that the reader ’s mind is programmed accordingly. 14.3.5 Theoretical Background Here the theoretical background to the problem area as well as to the study design is presented. If we are using hypotheses or a priori assumptions there 283 284 Writing the Final Report fi has to be proper reasoning with the help of previous studies and ndings. Depending on the research orientation, as covered in Chapters 2 and 3, the importance of theory and its use is different. It is therefore important to be consistent in your report, and you should check this section with your research orientation and design. Methodology 14.3.6 In this section you inform the reader of your research design, whether it is exploratory, descriptive, or causal, and why a particular design was chosen. You should state the design requirements and how they are met. Different research designs require different methods, as explained in Chapter 5. The research designs, exploratory, descriptive, or causal, would also suggest whether you should use qualitative or quantitative methods for your data fl collection and analysis. This then in uences the structure of your report. You have to ensure these matters are all consistent. You should also inform the reader about your primary fi and secondary sources of data, along with arguments and justi cations. When discussing the primary sources, you should explain how you collected information and discuss your studies, how population the cases and were sampling, selected and and in the why. case When of in-depth writing about case data collection, you have to explain what you have done, how you did it, and why you have used this particular method. You should also explain which methods of data analysis have been used: if statistical methods, why these particular methods; if qualitative methods, then how you operationalized the different concepts, where the models came from, and what types of conclusion could be drawn through this type of analysis. Findings 14.3.7 The empirical study, what you have found out from your data collection, is fi presented here. This section is often a major part of the report as the ndings fi are presented in detail with supportive tables and gures. Here you have to fi refer back to your research questions or hypotheses and present your ndings in accordance with these in a systematic, structured, and logical manner. The fi ndings, tables, and fi gures should follow a systematic chronological or psychological order. The most important job is to prune out irrelevant information and fi ndings. How to arrange and present the fi fi ndings of your study is a dif cult issue. There are no rules for this, but you should consider the purpose of the study and the report when it comes to what you want to communicate and to whom. We suggest that researchers be following methods of presentation: systematic and choose one of the 14.3 Structure of the Report • By order of occurrence. Here you present the findings chronologically. This is particularly suitable when you are working with case studies or when you have a process or longitudinal approach: for example, when fl explaining the process of negotiations and factors in uencing this process (see, for example, Ghauri, 2004). • By criteria or topics. You may use your own headings – for example from the questionnaire or problem statement – as a format to present the findings. You may have some criteria on what affects what – for example independent and dependent variables – which can be used as headings to present the hypotheses 1, findings. 2, You might number your research questions or 3 . . . and then discuss each of these in the same sequence. • By order of location. You can present your findings from different parts of the country or world in different sections and use these as headings: for example findings from the south, east, or central parts of the country, findings from different countries or continents. By order of importance. Quite often it is advised that you presentfindings in order of importance. The most important and interesting fi ndings should be presented first, followed by the less important issues. When or • listing criteria, you can list them according to their importance. 14.3.8 Conclusions In this section evaluated facts are discussed, but these evaluations should only be made from the data presented in the earlier sections; the author ’s fl biases and desires should not in uence these conclusions. You should state your conclusions systematically for each study objective, research question, or hypothesis. The best way is to refer back to the objectives or research questions and check whether or not you have provided conclusions for each of these. If the data collected and the analysis do not provide enough information or support for you to draw conclusions, you should clearly state that. fi It is important to be speci c and concise. We suggest the following format for conclusions: ‘Based on our findings, our conclusions are: . . . ’ or ‘We have found that: . . .’ Recommendations for future research or implications for business executives should be based on the findings and conclusions. There should be a logical congruence between conclusions and implications or recommendations. If recommendations or implications are given throughout the report, 285 286 Writing the Final Report for example while presenting findings, these should be summarized and highlighted in this section. We suggest the following format: ‘ The implications of C1 are:. . .’ 14.3.9 Footnotes As mentioned earlier, the value of a report also depends upon argumentation and sources. Proper credit to these sources is given in two ways: the bibliography or references listed at the end, and the footnotes or references given fi throughout the paper. The footnotes speci cally and individually document the facts and opinions referred to in the report (Berry, 1989). In most reports and theses, footnotes are used primarily for three purposes: 1. to credit the source or earlier study you have used; 2. to direct the reader to another section of the same report, also referred to as cross-references; 3. to explain, discuss, or provide additional information on a particular concept or issue. In research reports, a separate reference list or bibliography is provided. In that case, you only need to mention the surname of the author and year of publication or source in footnotes or bracketed within the text at the appropriate point, as in this book. fi The type of information that should be documented is a dif cult question. A fact that a reader already knows need not be documented: for example, the year when the Second World War ended. However, for facts that are not common knowledge you have to inform the reader how you arrived at a certain point. It is also important for the sake of intersubjectivity. For example, the result of a study on the buying behaviour of a certain segment needs to be documented with the year of the study because the buying behaviour might change at different times. Behaviour can change due to awareness about environmental pollution, oil crises, or any other major incident. Whenever you bring in and use a paraphrase or quotation, it has to be documented with a footnote, together with the page number in the book. This goes for articles or any other source you have used: for example,‘Grønhaug (1985: 18)’. The most important rule is to use the same method throughout the report; different forms should not be mixed up in the same report. When using the form where footnotes are presented at the end of the report, these should come before the list of references or bibliography. Some authors advise using different forms for different types of footnotes: for example, footnotes where you only want to document and mention the source might be gathered at the 14.3 Structure of the Report end, while footnotes where you need to explain or discuss a concept should be mentioned at the bottom of the page (Berry, 1989). It is advised that one consistent method should be used for all types. There should also be internal consistency. For example, if footnotes are given at the bottom of the page, they should be consistently separated from the main text with double spacing (double the spacing in the text). The footnotes themselves should be single-spaced, with a double space between two footnotes. The margins and numbers should also be consistently at the same place, not only on the same page but throughout the report. For crossreferenced footnotes or references to other parts of the same report, you should avoid referring to later pages, as the reader is not yet aware of what is coming. On the other hand, it is acceptable to refer to previous pages: for example, ‘see p. 10’. 14.3.10 Bibliography and References Bibliographies are lists of books (and other material) on a particular subject at least all the sources that have been cited in the report. A list of references, on the other hand, includesonly those sources cited and and should include should not include books and other sources consulted but not used. All references should be listed in alphabetical order with authors ’ surnames coming come first. If there is no author, the issuing organization ’s name should first in the alphabetical order: for example the European Commission, editorial in a periodical, The Economist. In the case of a article in a periodical, the author’s name should come first. or, in the case of an reference to an The most popular format is to place the first word of the first line (e.g. surname) at the left-hand margin, while the rest of the lines are indented by several spaces. Remember, however, that for footnotes, especially those at the fi foot of the page, the opposite system is often used: the rst word of the first line is indented, while the rest of the lines start at the left-hand margin. This system is used for each and every source. The bibliography or references, like the footnotes, should be typed in single spacing while giving double spacing between references. Where there is more than one author in a source, it is permissible to reverse the names of all authors. However, it is quite common first author s name for the sake of alphabetical order and of the authors with their first name (or initials) first. Both to reverse just the mention the rest ’ systems are correct, but for the sake of consistency, you should use only one system in the same report. The titles of books, names of journals, periodicals, and newspapers, and titles of published government reports are often underlined, italicized, or typed with capital letters. In the case of edited books, if the reference comes from a chapter written by another author, the reference should start with the 287 288 Writing the Final Report author’s name and not with the editor’s name, and the title of the edited volume or book should be underlined or italicized. The title of articles and chapters from edited books and journals should be indicated by quotation marks (‘. . .’) and be typed exactly as the original. If the title is in a different language, such as Dutch or Norwegian, it should be mentioned in the original language. Some common examples of references are provided here: The Internationalization of the Firm Buckley, P. and Ghauri, P. (eds.) (1999), 2nd edn., London: Thomson. Ghauri, P.N. and Usunier, J.-C. (1996), , International Business Negotiations , Oxford: Elsevier. Grønhaug, R. and Haukedal, W. (1989), ‘Environmental imagery and strategic action ’, Scandinavian Journal of Management , vol. 4, nos 1–2: 5–17. Dunning, J. (1980), ‘Towards an eclectic theory of international production: some empirical tests’, Journal of International Business Studies , Spring/ Summer, 1: 9–31. United Nations Center on Transnational Corporations (1985), Accounting and Reporting Issues : 1984 review, New International York: United Nations Publications. As mentioned earlier, there are several methods and styles that are correct in listing the bibliography. The most important thing, however, is to use one form throughout a report and not to mix up different styles. Hint: For the format and structure of the report, also check the rules of your institution. 14.4 Oral Presentation Often you have to present your report in front of not only your supervisor or examiner but also company executives or members of an evaluation committee: people who have not been involved in your project. As in writing a report, the first important issue is to know your audience, as that will decide the level of your presentation. It should not be too technical or academic in nature, especially if people from outside (companies, etc.) are present. The presentation must be organized in the same manner as the written fi report. First, the main objectives and the speci c research questions should be presented, as well as why these questions are interesting or worth your study. You should relate your study to earlier work on the topic and 14.4 Oral Presentation 289 demonstrate how your study differs from that work. You should state the importance of your study as well as your limitations and constraints. Second, you have to explain ‘how’ you have done the study: how you have collected the information and how you have analysed it. Here again you should explain your limitations and constraints. This will adjust the expectation level of the audience as well as help them to make their own judgement as to the reliability of your study and its conclusions. Finally, the fi ndings and conclusions can be presented in a simple and straightforward unexpected way. results These and conclusions other should surprises. The be discussed conclusions in cases of also be should related to your objectives and research questions. If you have not been able to answer research all your project questions, regarding this fi speci c should be company explained. problems, In you the case need to of a also present your recommendations or solutions and argue why, in your opinion, these are the best solutions or recommendations. The most important issue, however, is the presentation itself: how you are going to use PowerPoint presentations, etc. We recommend that you use a minimum amount fi Technical or dif of text, preferably just points, and then talk about it. cult terminology and concepts should be avoided and, if fi used, should be explained. Using illustrations, tables, and strategy to support your arguments, though fi gures is a good gures and tables with lots of numbers and characters should be avoided. If used, the part of the table/ fi explaining charts facilitates gure your point should understanding. be highlighted. Listeners in this Use case of can graphics easily see and the whole picture and make comparisons more quickly and easily. Otherwise, fi they will have to make such evaluations themselves, which is often dif cult, if not impossible, during the presentation. Use simple and direct language when presenting, speak slowly, and articulate your words properly – say the whole words. Do not rush through the presentation in order to say a lot of things. If possible, make copies of your slides and distribute them. Time management is one of the most important aspects of your presentation. Rehearse your presentation so that you can explain the issues clearly without the need to rush. Do not forget that one of the major purposes of the presentation is for the audience to ask questions, so you should plan question and answer time into your presentation. In the case of a 30-minute presentation, we suggest you divide your time as follows: • 5 minutes to present the objectives and the research question; also, to relate your study to earlier work and explain its importance; • 5 minutes to present how you have carried out the study, your information sources and arguments; 290 Writing the Final Report findings and conclusions; • 10 minutes to present the • 10 minutes for questions and answers. Hint: Rehearse your presentation and, while presenting, speak slowly and clearly. 14.5 Writing for Publication If you are writing a report with the intention of getting it published in a journal, the report needs a different structure. The first thing to consider is the journal you are aiming at. Most journals demand a certain format for the papers footnotes submitted and to references, The starting point is thus paper and look through them with and first the regard to emphasis length, on headings, use methodological of rigour. to decide where you want to publish your your target publication’s criteria and format requirements. Most academic journals look for well-focused research that can contribute something to existing knowledge in the particular field. Many journals also value contributions towards managerial practice, so-called managerial implications. In business studies, the knowledge developed by researchers is rather more quickly applied to practice than in many fields. other The research, whether developing theories or testing existing ones, contributes considerably towards the furthering of knowledge. This is the main objective particular of most field. Considering the academic above, a journals: report to written further for knowledge publication in needs establish the objective of study and emphasize the gap it will fill new to knowledge knowledge in the it will lead to. Second, it has to be related their first to or the existing field. Third, the methodology needs much more argumen- tative explanation to convince the reader that, for the particular research problem, this is the most appropriate method. Here the discussion on internal validity, for example of the measurement instrument, and external validity, for example the generalizability or managerial implications, is much more important than in other reports (Huff, 1999). A report written for publication need not have headings and paragraphs on all the above issues, but nevertheless the argument in the methodology section and on measurement should satisfy all concerns regarding them. 14.5 Writing for Publication A paper/report written for publication is normally written with the following headings: • abstract, which explains the research questions, the contribution, the methods used, and • findings; introduction, which introduces the topic and explains the research problem and objective, why this study is needed, and what contribution it will make; • literature review, either as a separate section or while building/presenting hypotheses and propositions; • method/model/measurement, where you explain how you have done the study (data collection and data analysis) with all the argumentation mentioned earlier; • findings, where the results are presented according to the method chosen (quantitatively or qualitatively); • discussion and conclusion, where the conclusions are drawn out of the findings and unexpected or peculiar results are discussed. Finally, with or without a heading, the contributions (theoretical as well as practical) of the study are presented in a convincing manner. A reference list is attached at the end, following the formatting instructions of the journal or the publisher. All the references mentioned in the report, no more, no less. Once the report is ready it should be read by at least a couple of colleagues, especially your supervisor, if you have one, or a senior colleague. These colleagues should have some experience of reviewing papers for the journal being considered, or for similar journals. The criticisms and comments given by these colleagues should be taken seriously and the paper should be revised before you send it to the journal. 291 292 Writing the Final Report Further Reading ........................................................................ Birkinshaw, J. (2004) Publishing qualitative research in international business, in Marshan-Piekkari, R. and Welch, C. (eds.),A Handbook of Qualitative Research Methods for International Business, Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar, pp. 570 84. Fisher, C. (2010) Researching and Writing a Dissertation: An Essential Guide for Business Students, 2nd edn., London: FT & Prentice Hall. Miles, M.B. and Huberman, A.M. (1994) Qualitative Data Analysis, 2nd edn., Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage (Chapter 12). Rousseau, D.M. (1995) Publishing from a reviewer s perspective , in Cummings, L.L. and Frost, P.J. (eds.), Publishing in the Organizational Sciences, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 151 63. ‘ ’ – ‘ ’ ’ – Questions ........................................................................ 1. 2. 3. What are the differences between writing a company report and writing a thesis? What are the most important issues to be considered when writing the final report for a research project? Is writing for publication different from writing up a thesis? How? Exercises ........................................................................ 1. 2. 3. Pick up a Master s thesis from the library and prepare a 15-minute presentation. Pick up a Master s/PhD dissertation and write a two-page executive summary. Working in groups of two, read each other a report and give critical comments regarding the structure of the report and the languageflow. ’ ’ APPENDICES APPENDIX A Sample of Manufacturing Firms, 2018 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 93 89 2 33 52 74 6 91 19 98 53 62 82 001 28 24 13 5.81 9.02 9.57 4.91 3.93 9.71 9.86 3.35 7.82 0.54 8.92 5.24 8.72 7.12 7.51 5.53 1.04 3.21 5.32 5.76 7.61 0.03 6.51 6.45 6.54 7.92 0.54 6.32 2.53 6.32 5.32 4.81 0.43 5.53 CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA TXET TXET HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON 5.5 4.1 9.6 4.2 9.4 2.2 5.2 5.6 4.3 7.3 8.3 9.2 6.4 3.5 8.5 7.5 5.3 54 44 91 22 65 24 22 33 43 5 12 43 92 4 5 51 01 342 76 97 431 702 411 33 56 361 59 322 102 781 88 431 55 76 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 01 11 21 31 41 51 61 71 81PXE rotceS noigeR SPAC EGA EZIS )elacs )seeyolp 7–1( )sraey( -me( 0 91PXE mriF 1 WORG )CELE )TXET )TSEW )HTUOS )HTRON = = = = = )selas )selas rotceS rotceS noigeR noigeR noigeR )001 – 1 fo % sa fo % sa fi 1=( fi 1=( fi 1=( fi 1=( fi 1=( gniknar( stropxe( stropxe( CELED TXETD TSEWD HTUOSD HTROND Appendix A: Sample of Manufacturing Firms, 2018 294 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 93 89 2 33 52 74 6 91 19 98 53 62 82 001 28 24 13 69 5.81 9.02 9.57 4.91 3.93 9.71 9.86 3.35 7.82 0.54 8.92 5.24 8.72 7.12 7.51 5.53 1.04 4.76 3.21 5.32 5.76 7.61 0.03 6.51 6.45 6.54 7.92 0.54 6.32 2.53 6.32 5.32 4.81 0.43 5.53 3.67 CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA TXET TXET TXET HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON HTRON 5.5 4.1 9.6 4.2 9.4 2.2 5.2 5.6 4.3 7.3 8.3 9.2 6.4 3.5 8.5 7.5 5.3 8.6 54 44 91 22 65 24 22 33 43 5 12 43 92 4 5 51 01 9 342 76 97 431 702 411 33 56 361 59 322 102 781 88 431 55 76 43 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 01 11 21 31 41 51 61 71 81 81PXE rotceS noigeR SPAC EGA EZIS )elacs 7– 1( )sraey( )seeyolpme( 0 91PXE mriF 1 WORG )CELE )TXET )TSEW )HTUOS )HTRON = = = = = )selas )selas rotceS rotceS noigeR noigeR noigeR )001 – 1 fo % sa fo % sa fi 1=( fi 1=( fi 1=( fi 1=( fi 1=( gniknar( stropxe( stropxe( CELED TXETD TSEWD HTUOSD HTROND 295 Appendix A: Sample of Manufacturing Firms, 2018 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 45 94 68 97 78 11 48 01 42 3 22 06 15 58 73 18 81 63 35 26 37 27 72 9.83 3.91 6.44 7.34 6.83 4.54 3.84 0.66 0.94 3.54 6.32 5.71 5.21 8.44 1.36 6.33 3.54 9.56 8.94 6.43 7.64 3.34 0.83 4.53 3.71 2.34 8.34 6.63 0.32 2.54 5.95 9.34 6.22 8.42 3.81 4.41 6.34 7.45 9.45 6.33 9.95 3.54 8.43 1.14 6.73 8.43 CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE OTUA OTUA OTUA TXET TXET TXET CELE CELE CELE CELE OTUA OTUA OTUA HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS HTUOS TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE 2.1 9.1 5.2 8.5 2.6 4.4 3.5 3.6 3.3 9.2 8.5 3.1 4.1 5.4 1.5 3.6 3.4 3.5 4.4 0.4 5.3 5 7.5 44 55 32 43 26 44 92 62 45 23 86 23 51 22 55 33 84 82 62 34 22 4 41 211 651 451 76 822 761 561 001 58 941 542 891 551 45 32 67 321 781 33 831 56 431 902 91 02 12 22 32 42 52 62 72 82 92 03 13 23 33 43 53 63 73 83 93 04 14 Appendix A: Sample of Manufacturing Firms, 2018 296 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 38 65 43 09 87 05 54 14 85 44 9 08 51 71 4 79 96 12 0.62 6.72 1.03 6.21 3.56 2.45 2.05 1.17 8.47 2.64 3.24 0.45 1.24 6.54 0.64 4.83 7.83 0.26 5.32 8.32 8.52 9.52 4.56 3.45 6.54 6.46 7.47 0.24 7.53 4.65 7.63 7.93 5.93 8.33 2.54 4.65 OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE 5.2 5.2 6.3 8.3 2.6 3.5 3 1.6 3.6 2.2 8.3 4.3 6.1 4.1 2 3.6 5.2 3.4 3 25 35 12 14 81 83 72 63 33 26 41 21 92 92 13 72 52 85 54 871 651 69 33 54 37 28 22 16 43 65 87 901 321 83 871 24 34 44 54 64 74 84 94 05 15 25 35 45 55 65 75 85 95 81PXE rotceS noigeR SPAC EGA EZIS )elacs 7–1( )sraey( )seeyolpme( 0 91PXE mriF 0 WORG )CELE )TXET )TSEW )HTUOS )HTRON = = = = = )selas )selas rotceS rotceS noigeR noigeR noigeR )001 – 1 fo % sa fo % sa fi 1=( fi 1=( fi 1=( fi 1=( fi 1=( gniknar( stropxe( stropxe( CELED TXETD TSEWD HTUOSD HTROND 297 Appendix A: Sample of Manufacturing Firms, 2018 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 16 04 25 47 23 07 29 76 49 64 56 17 46 7 57 99 32 02 75 03 34 84 5 9.34 4.95 1.63 1.63 3.04 0.15 3.52 6.54 6.82 6.63 1.05 8.82 5.24 4.55 4.25 3.33 9.35 0.84 9.71 9.74 4.53 5.54 1.56 0.54 0.35 6.53 3.13 6.53 5.45 6.82 7.93 2.23 5.23 0.44 3.33 6.83 4.84 6.74 6.53 0.94 4.24 9.71 3.34 6.33 3.34 4.94 TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET CELE CELE CELE CELE CELE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSAE TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW 3.4 8.3 9.2 8.4 2.5 7.5 6.5 7.6 4.2 3.2 4 4 1.6 8.2 3.5 7.4 5.5 3.4 8.4 1.4 3.3 3 3.6 72 41 21 43 82 62 92 24 73 71 81 62 56 56 23 4 5 9 82 63 22 54 56 76 56 221 451 341 65 891 102 38 99 46 111 381 47 57 82 48 56 102 631 44 55 66 06 16 26 36 46 56 66 76 86 96 07 17 27 37 47 57 67 77 87 97 08 18 28 Appendix A: Sample of Manufacturing Firms, 2018 298 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 88 66 36 31 77 67 1 39 59 61 92 86 41 8 83 55 21 95 0.9 1.54 0.03 7.43 7.82 5.44 1.45 0.5 2.32 8.55 1.24 3.64 6.34 5.56 4.35 0.74 1.44 2.43 5.9 1.93 4.03 6.82 2.52 2.93 8.93 2.01 8.82 4.94 3.83 8.04 9.73 5.85 6.74 7.24 3.81 7.33 OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA OTUA TXET TXET TXET TXET TXET TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW TSEW 8.4 2.5 6.5 2.6 5 3.3 6.4 4 7.3 6.4 8.3 2.5 8.5 4.4 5.3 2.5 9.3 1.6 45 22 72 32 91 34 74 92 33 73 15 16 33 92 73 14 7 9 842 55 541 781 87 33 441 622 221 501 811 621 131 511 711 221 821 611 38 48 58 68 78 88 98 09 19 29 39 49 59 69 79 89 99 001 81PXE rotceS noigeR SPAC EGA EZIS )elacs 7–1( )sraey( )seeyolpme( 0 91PXE mriF 0 WORG )CELE )TXET )TSEW )HTUOS )HTRON = = = = = )selas )selas rotceS rotceS noigeR noigeR noigeR )001 – 1 fo % sa fo % sa fi 1=( fi 1=( fi 1=( fi 1=( fi 1=( gniknar( stropxe( stropxe( CELED TXETD TSEWD HTUOSD HTROND APPENDIX B Simple Comparative Tests Often the researcher will want to compare the means of several populations by using random samples of data drawn from each. The appropriate method of analysis depends upon: the underlying distribution of the data (whether it is normally distributed or not); whether the samples contain independent or matched (paired) observations; the number of means being compared. If the data are normally distributed, then the followingparametric tests are appropriate: • • • Observations Number of means 2 Independent Student s t-test Matched (paired) Paired t-test >2 One-way ANOVA Randomized block ANOVA ’ Note: The ANOVA will need to be supplemented with an appropriate multiple comparisons test (e.g. LSD, Tukey). This is because there are 3 possible comparisons if 3 means are being compared (i.e. A & B, A & C, and B & C); 6 possible comparisons if 4 means are being compared (i.e. A & B, A & C, A & D, B & C, B & D, and C & D); 10 possible comparisons if 5 means are being compared, etc. If the data are not normally distributed, or the data are ordinal, then the following non-parametric tests are appropriate: Observations Number of means 2 Independent Mann-Whitney test Matched (paired) Wilcoxon signed-ranks test >2 Kruskal-Wallis test Kruskal-Wallis test Parametric tests are generally more powerful than non-parametric tests, and hence should be used as long as the underlying assumptions hold. Many parametric methods are robust if sample sizes are reasonably large (i.e. n > 30), in that they are valid even if the underlying distributional assumptions are not met. APPENDIX C Formulae Comparison of Means (Independent Samples) ........................................................................ rffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi The standard error of the difference between two means is: SEðX± 1 ± X± 2 Þ ¼ sp where s2p 1 n1 þ n1 2 2 2 ¼ ðn1 ± n11Þsþ1 þn2ð±n22± 1Þs2 s21 ¼ var ðX 1 Þ s22 ¼ var ðX 2 Þ Comparison of Means (Paired Samples) ........................................................................ The standard error of the mean difference: ffiffi ffiffi sffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffiXffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi s 0:335 SEð±d Þ ¼ pd ¼ p ¼ 0:137 n 6 where sd ¼ 1 n n ± 1 i¼1 di = X1i ± X2i ðdi ± ±dÞ2 Appendix C: Formulae Correlation Coefficient (Continuous Variables) ................................................................. sXY ¼ XX X Y Y n sffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi Xffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi n 1 ±1 sX ¼ sY ¼ i¼1 1 n±1 ð i± n i¼1 ± Þð i ± ± Þ ±Þ ðX i ± X 2 sffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi Xffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi ffi 1 n±1 where sXY = sample covariance of and Y sX = sample standard deviation of X sY = sample standard deviation of Y n i¼ 1 2 ±Þ ðY i ± Y 301 References ACR News (1995) ‘Where’s the big idea?’, ACR News, December, p. 13. Aguinis, H. and Bradley, K.J. (2014) ‘Best practice recommendations for designing and implementing experimental vignette methodology studies’, Organizational Research Methods, 17(4): 351–71. Altheide, D.L. and Johnson, J.M. (1998) ‘Criteria for assessing interpretive validity in qualitative research’, in Denzin, N. and Lincoln, Y.S. (eds.), Collecting and Interpreting Qualitative Materials, London: Sage, pp. 283–312. Alvesson, M. (2011) Interpreting Interviews, London: Sage. Andreasen, A.R. (1990) ‘Cultural interpretation: A critical consumer research issue for the 1990s’, Advances in Consumer Research, 3: 185–96. de Saint Antonine’, Lyon: La Bacconnière. Bennett, R. (1986) ‘Meaning and method in management research’, Graduate Management Research, 3(3). Bernard, A.B., Jensen, J.B., Redding, S J., and Schott, P.K. (2007) ‘Firms in international trade’, Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21(3): 105–30. Berry, H., Guillén, M., and Zhou, N. (2010) ‘An institutional approach to cross-national distance’, Journal of International Business Studies, 41(9): 1460–80. Berry, R. (1989) How to Write a Research Paper, 2nd edn., Oxford: Pergamon Press. Beugelsdijk, S., Hennart, J.-F., Slangen, A., and Smeets, R. (2010) ‘Why and how FDI stocks are a biased measure of MNE affiliate activity’, Bandiera, O., Barankay, I., and Rasul, I. (2016) ‘Field experiments with firms’, Journal of Economic Journal of International Business Studies, 41: 1444–59. Beugelsdijk, S., Maseland, R., Onrust, M., Perspectives, 25(3): 63–82. Bazerman, M. (1998) Judgment in Managerial Decision Making, 4th van Hoorn, A., and Slangen, A. (2015) ‘Cultural distance in international business and edn., New York: Wiley. Becker, H.S. (1970) Sociological Work, Chicago, IL: Aldine. Bem, J. (1979) Désir et savoir dans management: From mean-based to variance-based measures’, International Journal of Human Resource Management, l’oeuvre de Flaubert: ‘La tentation 26(2): 165–91. References Beza, T. (1984) Psaumes mis en vers Français (1551–62), Geneva: Droz. Bhaskar, R. (1986) Scientific Realism and Human Emancipation, London: Verso. Birkinshaw, J. (2004) ‘Publishing Market Research, 4th edn., London: Kogan Page. Braithwaite, R.B. (1955) Scientific Explanation: A Study of the Function of Theory, Probability and Law in Science, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. qualitative research in international business’, in Marshan-Piekkari, R. and Welch, C. (eds.), A Handbook of Brewer, J. and Hunter, A. (1989) Multimethod Research: A Synthesis of Styles, Sage Library of Social Qualitative Research Methods for International Business, Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar, pp. 570–84. Research, 175, Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Brown, T.A. (2014) Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Applied Research, 2nd Bloom, B.S., Hastings, J.T., and Madaus, G.F. (1971) Handbook on Formative and Summative Evaluation of edn., New York and London: Guilford Press. Bryman, A. (2001) Social Research Student Learning, New York: McGraw-Hill. Blumberg, B., Cooper, D.R., and Methods, Oxford: Oxford University Press. Bryman, A. and Bell, E. (2003) Business Schindler, P.S. (2008) Business Research Methods, 2nd European edn., London: McGraw-Hill. Bolsky, M.I. (1988) Better Scientific and Technical Writing, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Bonoma, T.V. (1985) ‘Case research in marketing: Opportunities, problems, and a process’, Journal of Business Research, 12: 199–208. Research Methods, Oxford: Oxford University Press. Bryman, A. and Bell, E., (2015) Business Research Methods, New York: Oxford University Press. Buchler, J. (ed.) (1955) Philosophical Writings of Peirce, New York: Dover. Burns, T. and Stalker, G.M. (1961) The Management of Innovations, Boyd, W.B., Westfall, R., and Stasch, S.F. (1985) Marketing Research: Text and Cases, 6th edn., Homewood, IL: London: Tavistock. Burrell, G. and Morgan, G. (1979) Sociological Paradigms and Irwin. Boyd, W.B., Westfall, R., and Stasch, S.F. (1989) Marketing Research: Text Organizational Analysis, London: Heinemann. Campbell, D.T. (1975) ‘Degrees of and Cases, 7th edn., Homewood, IL: Irwin. Brace, I. (2018) Questionnaire Design: How to Plan, Structure and Write freedom and the case study’, Comparative Political Studies, 8(2): 173–93. Campbell, D.T. and Fiske, W. (1959) Survey Material for Effective ‘Convergent and discriminant 303 304 References validation by the multitrailmultimethod matrix’, Psychological Bulletin, 56: 81–105. Campbell, D.T. and Stanley, J.C. (1966) Experimental and Quasi Experimental Designs for Research, Chicago, IL: Rand Journal of International Business Studies, 41(2): 178–84. Chatterji, A., Findley, M., Jensen, N., Meier, S., and Nielson, D. (2016) ‘Field experiments in strategy research’, Strategic Management Journal, 37(1): 116–32. McNally. Campbell, J.P., Daft, R.L., and Hulin, C. (1982) What to Study: Generating Chen, H. and Chen, T.-Y. (1998) ‘Network linkages and location choice in foreign direct investment’, and Developing Research Questions, Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Cannell, C.F., Miller, P.U., and Oksenberg, L. (1981) ‘Research on Journal of International Business Studies, 29(3): 445–67. Chetty, S.K. and Hamilton, R.T. (1993) ‘Firm-level determinants of export interviewing techniques’, in S. Leinhardt (ed.), Sociological Methodology, San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass, pp. 389–437. Carter, R.B. and Van Auken, H. (2005) ‘Bootstrap financing and owners’ perceptions of their business constraints and opportunities’, Entrepreneurship & Regional performance: a meta-analysis’, International Marketing Review, 10(3): 26–35. Chidlow, A., Ghauri, P.N., Yeniyurt, S., and Cavusgil, S.T. (2015) ‘Establishing rigor in mail-survey procedures in international business research’, Journal of World Business, 50(1): 26–35. Development, 17(2): 129–44. Cattell, R.B. (1943a) ‘The description of personality. 1. Foundations of trait measurement’, Psychological Chreim, S., William, B.E., and Hinings, C.R. (2007) ‘Interlevel influences on the reconstruction of professional role identity’, Academy of Review, 50(6): 559–94. Cattell, R.B. (1943b) ‘The description of personality: Basic traits resolved Management Journal, 50(6): 1515–39. Churchill, G.A. (1995) Marketing into clusters’, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 38(4): 476–506. Research: Methodological Foundations, 6th edn., Fort Worth, TX: Dryden Press. Chalmers, A.F. (1982) What Is This Thing Called Science?, 2nd edn., Philadelphia, PA: Open University Churchill, G.A. (1999) Marketing Research: Methodological Foundation, 7th edn., Fort Worth Press. Chang, S.-J., van Witteloostuijn, A., and Eden, L. (2010) ‘From the editors: Common method variance in TX, London: Dryden Press. Churchill, G.A. and Iacobucci, D. (2015) Marketing Research: Methodological Foundations, 11th international business research’, edn., Mason, TN: Earlie Lite Book. References Cichello, M. and Lamdin, D.J. (2006) ‘Event studies and the analysis of antitrust’, International Journal of the Economics of Business, 13(2): 229–45. Coase, R.H. (1937) ‘The nature of the firm’, Economica, 4(16): 386–405. Corrado, C.J. (2011) ‘Event studies: a methodology review’, Accounting and Finance, 51(1): 207–34. Cowley, J.C.P. (2000) ‘Strategic qualitative focus group research: define and articulate our skills or we will be replaced by others’, Cohen, J. (1994) ‘The earth is round (p < .05)’, American Psychologist, 49(12): 997–1003. International Journal of Market Research, 37(7): 903–30. Craig, D.S. and Douglas, S.P. (2000) Cohen, M. and Nagel, E. (1934) An Introduction to Logic and Scientific Method, New York: American Advertising International Marketing Research, 2nd edn., Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons. Creswell, J.W. (2007) Qualitative Inquiry Association. Colley, R.A. (1961) Defining Advertising Goals for Measured Advertising and Research Design: Choosing Among Five Approaches, 2nd edn., Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Results, New York: American Advertising Association. Conant, J. (1951) Science and Common Croson, R., Anand, J., and Agarwal, R. (2007) ‘Using experiments in corporate strategy research’, Sense, New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Connelly, B.L., Ketchen, D.J., Gangloff, European Management Review, 4(3): 173–81. Crotty, M. (1998) The Foundations of K.A., and Shook, C.L. (2016) ‘Investor perceptions of CEO successor selection in the wake of integrity and competence failures: a policy capturing study’, Strategic Management Journal, 37: 2135–51. Social Research: Meaning and Perspective in the Research Process, London: Sage. Cunliffe, A.L. (2003) ‘Reflexive inquiry in organizational research: Questions and possibilities’, Human Relations, 56(8): 983–1003. Cook, T.D. and Campbell, D.T. (1979) Quasi-Experimentation: Design and Analysis Issues for Field Settings, Davis, J. (2001) ‘International comparison of labour disputes’, Labour Market Trends, April, Chicago, IL: Rand McNally College Publication Co. Cooper, D. and Schindler, P. (2001) pp. 195–201. Denzin, N.K. and Lincoln, Y.S. (eds.) (1994) Handbook of Qualitative Business Research Methods, Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill. Cooper, H.M. (1984) The Integrative Research Review: A Systematic Research, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Di Stefano, G. and Gutierrez, C. (2018) ‘Under a magnifying glass: On the use of experiments in strategy Approach, Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. research’, Strategic Organization, 305 306 References online: https://doi.org/10.1177/ 1476127018803840. Diamantopoulos, A., Sarstedt, M., Fuchs, C., Wilczynski, P., and Global Economy, Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar. Easterby-Smith, M., Thorpe, R., and Lowe, A. (1991) Management Kaiser, S. (2012) ‘Guidelines for choosing between multi-item and single-item scales for construct Research, London: Sage. Eisenbeis, R.A. (1977) ‘Pitfalls in the application of discriminant analysis measurement: a predictive validity perspective’, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Sciences, in business, finance, and economics’, Journal of Finance, 32(3): 875–900. 40(3): 434–49. Ding, L., Lam, H.K.S., Cheng, T.C.E., and Zhou, H. (2018) ‘A review of shortterm event studies in operations and supply chain management’, International Journal of Production Economics, 200(C): 329–42. Donaldson, L. (1997) ‘Derek Pugh: Scientific revolutionary in organization studies’, in Clark, T. (ed.), Essays in Honour of Derek Pugh, Aldershot: Ashgate, pp. 23–43. Douglas, J. (1976) Investigating Social Research, Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Douglas, S.P. and Craig, C.S. (1997) ‘The changing dynamic of consumer behavior: Implications for crosscultural research’, International Journal of Research in Marketing, 14(4): 379–95. Dubois, A. and Gadde. L.-E. (2002) ‘Systematic combining: An abductive approach to case research’, Journal of Business Research, 55(7): 553-60. Dubois, A. and Gadde, L.-E. (2014) ‘Systematic combining: A decade later’, Journal of Business Research, 67(6): 1277–84. Dunning, J.H. and Lundan, S.M. (2008) Multinational Enterprises and the Eisenhardt, K.M. (1989) ‘Building theories from case study research’, Academy of Management Review, 14(4): 532–50. Elg, U., Ghauri, P, Child, J., and Collinson, S. (2017) ‘MNE microfoundations and routines for building a legitimate and sustainable position in emerging markets’, Journal of Organizational Behavior, (38(9): 1320–37. Elkjaer, B. and Simpson, B. (2011) ‘Pragmatism: A lived and living philosophy. What can it offer to contemporary organization theory?’, in Tsoukas, H. and Chia, R. (eds.), Philosophy and Organization Theory, Vol. 32, Bradford: Emerald, pp. 55–84. Elliot, J. (1980) ‘How hard it is to write easily’, View Point: The Ogilvy & Mathew Magazine, 2, pp. 18–19. Ellis, P.D. (2010) ‘Effect sizes and the interpretation of research results in international business’, Journal of International Business Studies, 41(9): 1581–8. Falk, A. and Heckman, J.J. (2009) ‘Lab experiments are a major source of knowledge in the social sciences’, Science, 326(5952): 535–8. References Fama, E.F., Fisher, L., Jensen, M.C., and Roll, R. (1969) ‘The adjustment of stock prices to new information’, International Economic Review, 10(1): 1–21. Ferber, R., Blankertz, D.F., and Hollander, S. Jr. (1964) Marketing Research, American Marketing Association, pp. 407–50. Fowler, F.J. and Mangione, T.W. (1990) Standardized Survey Interviewing: Minimizing Interviewer-Related Error, Newbury Park, CA: Sage. New York: Ronald Press. Field, A. (2018) Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics, 5th Frankfort-Nachmias, F. and Nachmias, D. (1996) Research Methods in the Social Sciences, 5th edn., London: edn., London: Sage. Fisher, C. (2010) Researching and Writing a Dissertation: An Essential Guide for Business Edward Arnold. Fuglseth, A.M. and Grønhaug, K. (2003) ‘Can computerized market models improve strategic decision Students, 2nd edn., London: FT & Prentice Hall. Flatten, T.C., Engelen, A., Zahra, S.A., making?’, Journal of SocioEconomics, 32: 503–20. Gallo, A. (2015) ‘A refresher on and Brettel, M. (2011) ‘A measure of absorptive capacity: scale development and validation’, European Management Journal, 29(2): 98–116. Flyvbjerg, B. (2006) ‘Five regression analysis’, Harvard Business Review, 4 November, online: https://hbr.org/2015/11/arefresher-on-regression-analysis. Ghauri, P.N. (2004) ‘Designing and conducting case studies in misunderstandings about casestudy research’, Qualitative Inquiry, 12(1): 219–45. Fontana, A. and Frey, J.H. (1994) international business research’, in Marshan-Piekkari, R. and Welch, C. (eds.), A Handbook of Qualitative Research Methods for International ‘Interviewing: The art of science’, in Denzin, N.K. and Lincoln, Y.S. (eds.), Handbook of Qualitative Business, Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar, pp. 109–24. Ghauri, P.N. and Cateora, P. (2006) Research, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 361–76. Forcese, D.P. and Richer, S. (1973) Social International Marketing: European Edition, London: McGraw-Hill. Ghauri, P.N. and Cateora, P. (2014) Research Methods, Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Fornell, C.G. (1987) ‘A second generation International Marketing, 4th edn., London: McGraw-Hill. Ghauri, P.N. and Firth, R. (2009) ‘The of multivariate analysis: classification of methods and implications for marketing research’, in Houston, M.J. (ed.), Review of Marketing, Chicago: formalization of case study research in international business’, Der Markt: Journal fur Marketing, 48: 29–40. Ghemawat, P. (2001) ‘Distance still matters: The hard reality of global 307 308 References expansion’, Harvard Business Review, 79(8): 137–47. Gibbert, M. and Ruigrok, W. (2010) ‘The “what” and “how” of case study rigor: Three strategies based on published work’, Organization Research Methods, 13: 710–37. Gibbert, M., Ruigrok, W., and Wicki, B. (2008) ‘What passes as a rigorous case study?’, Strategic Management Journal, 29(13): 1465–74. Gibbons, M., Limoges, C., Nowotny, H., Schwartzman, S., Scott, P., and Trow, M. (1994) The New Production of Knowledge, London: Sage. Gill, J. and Johnson, P. (2002) Research Methods for Managers, 3rd edn., London: Paul Chapman. Gioia, D. and Chittipeddi, K. (1991) Sensemaking and sensegiving in Silverman, D. (eds.), Qualitative Research Practice, London: Sage, pp. 405–26. Golafshani, N. (2003) ‘Understanding reliability and validity in qualitative research’, The Qualitative Report, 8(4): 597–606. Grisar-Kassé, K. (2004) ‘The role of negative personal experiences in cross-cultural case study research: failure or opportunity’, in MarshanPiekkari, R. and Welch, C. (eds.) A Handbook of Qualitative Research Methods for International Business, Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar, pp. 144–61. Grønhaug, K. (1985) ‘Problemer i empirisk forskning’, in NHH/RSF, Methoder og perspectiver i okonomisk-administrativ strategic change initiation, Strategic Management Journal, 12(6): 433–48. Forskning, Oslo: Universitetsforlag. Grønhaug, K. (1999) ‘Technological flexibility and organisational Gioia, D.A. and Pitre, E. (1990) ‘Multiparadigm perspectives on theory building’, Academy of Management Review, 15, 584–602. buying strategies’, European Journal of Purchasing and Supply Management, 51: 13–22. Grønhaug, K. and Haukedal, W. (1989) Gioia, D., Corley, A., Kevin, G., and Hamilton, A. L. (2012) Seeking qualitative rigor in inductive research: Notes on the Gioia methodology, Organisational Research Methods, 16(1): 15–31. Glaser, B.G. and Strauss, A.L. (1967) The Discovery of Grounded Theory: Strategies for Qualitative Research, Chicago, IL: Aldine. Gobo, G. (2004) ‘Sampling, representativeness and generalizability’, in Seale, C., Gobo, G., Gubrium, J.F., and ‘Environmental imagery and strategic action’, Scandinavian Journal of Management, 4(1/2): 5–17. Gujarati, D.N. and Porter, D.C. (2009) Basic Econometrics, 5th edn., New York: McGraw-Hill. Haans, R.F.J., Pieters, C., and He, Z.-L. (2016) ‘Thinking about U: theorizing and testing U- and inverted U-shaped relationships in strategy research’, Strategic Management Journal, 37(x): 1177–95. References Hadjikhani, A., Elg, U., and Ghauri, P. (eds.) (2012) Business, Society and Politics, Bingley: Emerald. Hagen, B., Zucchella, A., and Ghauri, P. (2019) ‘From fragile to agile: Marketing as a key driver of entrepreneurial tool in business research’, European Business Review, 26(2): 106–21. Hammersley, M. and Atkinson, P. (1995) Ethnography: Principles in Practice, London: Routledge. Harrison, G.W. and List, J.A. (2004) ‘Field experiments’, Journal of internationalization’, International Marketing Review, 36(2): 260–88. Hagg, I. and Hedlund, G. (1978) ‘Case Economic Literature, 42(4): 1009–55. Hassan, I. and Ghauri, P. (2014) studies in accounting research’, Accounting, Organization and Society, 4(1-2): 135–43. Hair, J.F., Anderson, R.E., Tatham, R.G., Evaluating Companies for Mergers and Acquisitions, Bingley: Emerald. Hawkins, S.W. (1988) A Brief History of Time: From the Big Bang to Black and Black, W.C. (1998) Multivariate Data Analysis, 5th edn, Harlow and Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Holes, Toronto: Bantam Books. Hellesøy, O., Grønhaug, K., and Kvitastein, O. (2000) ‘Burnout: Hair, J.F., Ringle, C.M., and Sarstedt, M. (2011) ‘PLS-SEM: indeed a silver bullet’, Journal of Marketing Theory Conceptual issues and empirical findings from a new research setting’, Scandinavian Journal of and Practice, 19(2): 139–52. Hair, J.F., Sarstedt, M., Pieper, T.M., and Ringle, C.M. (2012a) ‘The use of Management, 16(3): 233–47. Hensher, D.A., Rose, J.M., and Greene, W.H. (2005) Applied Choice partial least squares structural equation modeling in strategic management research: A review of past practices and Analysis: A Primer. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Hillebrand, B., Kok, R., and Biemans, W. (2001) ‘Theory testing using case recommendations for future applications’, Long Range Planning, 45(5): 320–40. studies: A comment on Johnston, Leach and Liu’, Industrial Marketing Management, 30: 651–7. Hair, J.F., Sarstedt, M., Ringle, C.M., and Mena, J.A. (2012b) ‘An assessment of the use of partial least squares structural equation modeling in marketing research’, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 40(3): 414–33. Hair, J.F., Sarstedt, M., Hopkins, L., and Kuppelwieser, V.G. (2014) ‘Partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM): An emerging Hofstede, G. (2001) Culture’s Consequences: Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions, and Organizations across Nations, 2nd edn., Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Holland, R. (1999) ‘Reflexivity’, Human Relations, 52(4): 463–85. Huff, S.A. (1999) Writing for Publication, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Hult, G.T.M., Ketchen, D.J. Jr, Cui, A.S., Prud’homme, A.M., Seggi, S.H., 309 310 References Stanko, M.A., Xu, A.S., and Cavusgil, S.T. (2006) ‘An assessment of the use of structural equation modeling in international business research’, Research Methodology in Strategy and Management, 3: 385–415. Kelle, U. (1997) ‘Theory building in qualitative research and computer programs for management of textual data’, Social Research Online, 2(2): www.Socresonline .org.uk/2/2/1.html. Kent, R.K. (1993) Marketing Research in Isaeva, N., Bachmann, R., Bristow, A., and Saunders, M.N. (2015) ‘Why the epistemologies of trust researchers Action, London: Routledge. Kerlinger, F.N. (1964) Foundations of Behavioural Research: Educational matter’, Journal of Trust Research, 5(2): 153–69. Jankowicz, A.D. (1991) Business Research Projects for Students, and Psychological Inquiry, London: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Kerner, A. (2014) ‘What do we talk about when we talk about foreign direct London: Chapman & Hall. Johanson, J. and Vahlne, J.-E. (1977) ‘The internationalization process of investment?’, International Studies Quarterly, 58(4): 804–15. Ketchen, D.J. Jr. and Shook, C.L. (1996) the firm – a model of knowledge development and increasing foreign market commitments’, Journal of ‘The application of cluster analysis in strategic management research: an analysis and critique’, Strategic International Business Studies, 8(1): 23–32. Johnston, M.A. (2007) ‘A review of the Management Journal, 17(6): 441–58. Khanna, T. and Palepu, K.G. (1997) ‘Why application of event studies in marketing’, Academy of Marketing Science Review, 11(4): 1–31. Jones, M.O. (1988) ‘In search of focused strategies may be wrong for emerging markets’, Harvard Business Review, 75(4): 41–51. King, G., Kelohane, R.O., and Verba, S. meaning: using qualitative methods in research and applications’, in Jones, M.O., Moore, M.D., and Snyder, R.C. (eds.), Inside Organisations: Understanding the Human Dimension, London: Sage, (1994) Designing Social Enquiry: Scientific Inference in Qualitative Research, Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Kirk, J. and Miller, M.L. (1986) Reliability and Validity in Qualitative pp. 31–48. Kawai, N., Strange, R., and Zucchella, A. (2018) ‘Stakeholder pressures, the Research, Sage University Paper Series on Qualitative Applications in Social Sciences, No. 01, Newbury adoption of environmental management systems, and green innovation in MNC overseas subsidiaries’, International Business Park, CA: Sage. Knudsen, C. (2003) ‘Pluralism, scientific progress, and the structure of organization theory’, in Tsoukas, H. Review, 27(5): 933–46. and Knudsen, C. (eds.), The Oxford References Handbook of Organization Theory: Meta-Theoretical Perspectives, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 262–86. Lahaut, V.M.H.C.J., Jansen, H.A.M., van de Mheen, D., and Garretsen, H.F.L. (2002) ‘Non-response bias in a sample survey of alcohol Kogut, B. and Singh, H. (1988) ‘The effects of national culture on the choice of entry mode’, Journal of consumption’, Alcohol and Alcoholism, 37(3): 256–60. Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. (1980) International Business Studies, 9(3): 411–32. Konchitchki, Y. and O’Leary, D.E. (2011) Metaphors We Live By, Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Lave, C.A. and March, J.G. (1993) An ‘Event study methodologies in information systems research’, International Journal of Accounting Information Systems, 12(2): Introduction to Models in the Social Sciences, 2nd edn., New York: Harper & Row. Layder, D. (1993) New Strategies in 99–115. Kornhauser, A. and Lazarsfeld, P.F. (1955) ‘The analysis of consumer Social Research, Cambridge: Polity Press. Lazarsfeld, P. (1959) ‘Latent structure actions’, in Lazarsfeld, P. and Rosenberg, M. (eds.), The Language of Social Research, Glencoe, IL: Free analysis’, in Koch, S. (ed.), Psychology: A Study of a Science, vol. 3, New York and London: Press, pp. 392–403. Kotler, P. (1997) Marketing Management, 9th edn., Englewood Cliffs, NJ: McGraw-Hill. Lee, L., Petter, S., Fayard, D., and Robinson, S. (2011) ‘On the use of Prentice Hall. Krammer, S.M.S., Strange, R., and Lashitew, A. (2018) ‘The determinants of export performance partial least squares path modeling in accounting research’, International Journal of Accounting Information Systems, 12(4): in BRIC countries: The influence of firm resources and the institutional environment’, International 305–28. Lilien, G.L., Kotler, P., and Moorthy, K.S. (1992) Marketing Models, Business Review, 27(1): 218–30. Kraus, S., Meier, F., and Niemand, T. (2016) ‘Experimental methods in entrepreneurship research: the status quo’, International Journal of Entrepreneurial Behavior and Research, 22(6): 958–83. Kuhn, T. (1970) The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, 2nd edn., Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Lind, J.T. and Mehlum, H. (2010) ‘With or without U? The appropriate test for a U-shaped relationship’, Oxford Bulletin of Economics and Statistics, 72(1): 109–18. Locke, K. and Golden-Biddle, K. (1997) ‘Constructing opportunities of contribution: Constructing intertextual coherence and “problematizing” in organizational 311 312 References studies’, Academy of Management Journal, 40(5): 1023–62. Locke, L.F., Spirduso, W.W., and Silverman, S.J. (2000) Proposals That Work: A Guide for Planning Dissertations and Grant Proposals, 4th edn., Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Lonner, W.J. and Adamopoulos, J. (1997) ‘Culture as an antecedent to behaviour’, in Berry, J.W., Poortinga, Y.H., and Panday, J. (eds.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology, vol. 2, Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon, pp. 43–83. Lorenzoni, G. and Lipparini, A. (1999) ‘The leveraging of interfirm relationships as a distinctive organizational capability: A longitudinal study’, Strategic Management Journal, 20(4): 317–38. MacCallum, R.C., Widaman, K.F., Zhang, March, J.G. and Simon, H.A. (1958) Organizations, New York: Wiley. Marshall, C. and Rossman, G.B. (1995) Designing Qualitative Research, 2nd edn., London: Sage. Marshan-Piekkari, R. and Welch, C. (2004) A Handbook of Qualitative Research Methods for International Business, Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar. Martin, C. (1988) Computers and Senior Managers: Top Management’s Response to Interactive Computing, Manchester: NCC. Mason, J. (2002) ‘Qualitative interviewing: Asking, listening and interpreting’, in May, T. (ed.), Qualitative Research in Action, London: Sage, pp. 225–41. McCaslin, M.L. and Scott, K.W. (2003) ‘The five question method for framing qualitative research study’, The Qualitative Report, S., and Hong, S. (1999) ‘Sample size in factor analysis’, Psychological Methods, 4: 84–99. MacKinlay, A.C. (1997) ‘Event studies in 8(3): 447–61. McCrae, R.R. and Costa, P.T. (1997) ‘Personality trait structure as a human universal’, American economics and finance’, Journal of Economic Literature, 35(1): 13–39. Majocchi, A. and Strange, R. (2007) ‘The Psychologist, 52(5): 509–16. McCrae, R.R. and Costa, P.T. (1999) ‘A five-factor theory of personality,’ FDI location decision: Does liberalization matter?’, Transnational Corporations, in Pervin, L.A. and John, O.P. (eds.), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research, New York: Guilford 16(2): 1–40. Malhotra, N. (1996) Marketing Research: An Applied Orientation, 2nd edn., Press, pp. 139–53. McWilliams, A. and Siegel, D. (1997) ‘Event studies in management Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Malinowski, B. (1916/1948) Magic, Science and Religion and other Essays, New York: Natural History research: Theoretical and empirical issues’, Academy of Management Journal, 40(3): 626–57. Meer-Kooistra, J. van der (1993) Press. Coordineren, Motiveren en References verrekenen, Wisselwerking Naegel, E. (1961) The Structure of tussen omgeving, onderneming en mensen, Groningen: WoltersNoordhoff. Science, New York: Harcourt, Brace & World. Ning, L., Kuo, J.-M., Strange, R., and Merton, R.K. (1967) On Theoretical Sociology, New York: Free Press. Merton, R.K., Fisk, M., and Kendall, P.L. (1956) The Focused Interview: A Manual of Problems and Procedures, New York: Free Wang, B. (2014) ‘International investors’ reaction to cross-border acquisitions by emerging market multinationals’, International Business Review, 23(4): 811–23. North, D.C. (1990) Institutions, Press. Miles, M.B. (1979) ‘Qualitative data as attractive nuisance: The problem of analysis’, Administrative Science Institutional Change and Economic Performance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. O’Brien, R.M. (2007) ‘A caution Quarterly, 24(4): 590–601. Miles, M.B. and Huberman, A.M. (1994) Qualitative Data Analysis, 2nd edn., regarding rules of thumb for variance inflation factors’, Quality & Quantity, 41: 673–90. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Mintzberg, H. (1979) ‘An emerging strategy of “direct” research’, Orbell, J. (1987) A Guide to Tracing the History of a Business, Aldershot: Gower Press. Administrative Science Quarterly, 24(4): 582–9. Mishler, E.G. (1986) Research Park, N.K. (2004) ‘A guide to using event study methods in multi-country settings’, Strategic Management Interviewing, London: Harvard University Press. Morgan, G. (1983), Beyond Method: Strategies for Social Research, Journal, 25(7): 655–68. Patton, M.Q. (1990) Qualitative Evaluation and Research Methods, 2nd edn., Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Moser, P.K., Mulder, D., and Trout, J.D. (1998) The Theory of Knowledge: Patton, M.Q. (2014) ‘Purposeful sampling and case selections: Overview of strategies and options’, A Thematic Introduction, Oxford: Oxford University Press. Murphy, H.A. and Hildebrandt, H.W. in Patton, M.Q. (ed.), Qualitative Research & Evaluation Methods, 4th edn., pp. 264–72. (1988) Effective Business Communications, 5th edn., New York: McGraw-Hill. Peng, D.X. and Lai, F. (2012) ‘Using partial least squares in operations management research: A practical Murse, T. (2019) ‘All 21 government shutdowns in US history’, ThoughtCo, 25 May 2019. Available at: thoughtco.com/government- guideline and summary of past research’, Journal of Operations Management, 30(6): 467–80. Peters, T.J. and Waterman, R.H. (1982) In shutdown-history-3368274. Search of Excellence: Lessons from 313 314 References America’s Best Run Companies, Porter, M.E. (1980) Competitive Strategy, New York: Harper & Row. Pettigrew, A. (1985) ‘Contextual research: A natural way to link New York: Free Press. Porter, M.E., Lorsch, J.W., and Nohria, N. (2004) ‘Seven surprises for new theory and practice’, in Lawler, E.E., Mohrman, A.M., Mohrman, S.A., Ledford, G.E., Cummings, T.G., and CEOs’, Harvard Business Review, 82(10): 62–75. Punj, G. and Stewart, D.W. (1983) Associates (eds.), Doing Research That Is Useful for Theory and Practice, San Francisco, CA: Jossey- ‘Cluster analysis in marketing research: Review and suggestions for application’, Journal of Bass, pp. 222–74. Pfeffer, J. (1993) ‘Barriers to the advance of organizational science: paradigm development as a dependent Marketing Research, 20(2): 134–48. Ramaswami, S.N., Srivastava, R.K., and Bhargava, M. (2009) ‘Market-based capabilities and financial variable’, Academy of Management Review, 18(4): 599–620. Pfeffer, J. (1995) ‘Morality, performance of firms: Insights into marketing’s contribution to firm value’, Journal of the Academy of reproducibility and the persistence of styles of theory’, Organization Science, 6(6): 681–93. Phillips, B. (1966) Social Research: Strategy and Tactics, New York: Macmillan. Pike, K. (1966) Language in Relation to a Unified Theory of Structure of Human Behaviour, The Hague: Mouton. Podsakoff, P.M., Mackenzie, S.B., Lee, J.-Y., and Podsakoff, N.P. (2003) ‘Common method biases in behaviour research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies’, Journal of Marketing Science, 37(2): 97–116. Reese, W.A. and Robins, R.P. (2017) ‘Performing an event study: An exercise for finance students’, Journal of Economic Education, 48(3): 206–15. Reeves, T.K. and Harper, D. (1981) Surveys at Work: A Practitioner’s Guide, London: McGraw-Hill. Reichardt, C.S. and Cook, T.D. (1979) ‘Beyond qualitative versus quantitative methods’, in Cook, T.D. and Reichardt, C.S. (eds.), Qualitative and Quantitative Methods in Evaluation Research, Beverly Hills, CA: Sage, pp. 7–32. Applied Psychology, 88(5): 879–903. Polanyi, M. (1958) Personal Knowledge, Revans, R.W. (1971) Developing Effective Managers, London: Longman. Richter, N.F., Sinkovics, R.R., Ringle, Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Popper, K.R. (1961) The Logic of Scientific Discovery, New York: C.M., and Schlägel, C. (2016) ‘A critical look at the use of SEM in international business research’, International Marketing Review, Science Edition. 33(3): 376–404. References Ringle, C.M., Sarstedt, M., and Straub, D.W. (2012) ‘A critical look at the use of PLS-SEM in MIS Quarterly’, MIS Quarterly, 36(1): 3–14. Roethlisberger, F.J. and Dickson, W.J. (1939) Management and the Worker, Cambridge, MA: Harvard Berlin and Heidelberg: SpringerVerlag, pp. 193–233. Sarstedt, M. and Mooi, E. (2019) ‘Cluster analysis’, in Sarstedt, M. and Mooi, E. (eds.), A Concise Guide to Market Research. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. University Press. Rose, A.M. and Peterson, W.A. (1965) Older People and Their Social Sarstedt, M., Ringle, C.M., and Hair, J.F. (2014a) ‘PLS-SEM: Looking back and moving forward’, Long Range World: The Subculture of the Aging, Philadelphia, PA: Davis. Ross, N.P. and Harris, S. (1994) ‘What are the ethical responsibilities of model Planning, 47(3): 132–7. Sarstedt, M., Ringle, C.M., Henseler, J., and Hair, J.F. (2014b) ‘On the emancipation of PLS-SEM: builders?’, in Wallace, W.A. (ed.), Ethics in Models, Oxford: Pergamon Press, pp. 161–6. A commentary on Rigdon (2012)’, Long Range Planning, 47(3): 154–60. Rousseau, D.M. (1995) ‘Publishing from a reviewer’s perspective’, in Cummings, L.L. and Frost, P.J. Saunders, M.N.K. (2011) ‘The management researcher as practitioner’, in Cassell, C. and Lee, (eds.), Publishing in the Organizational Sciences, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 151–63. W.J. (eds.), Challenges and Controversies in Management Research, London: Taylor & Francis, Rubin, H.J. and Rubin, I.S. (1995) Qualitative Interviewing: The Art of Hearing Data, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. pp. 243–56. Saunders, M.N.K., Lewis, P., and Thornhill, A. (2016) Research Methods for Business Students, 7th Russell, B. (1972) The Collected Stories, London: Unwin. Salzberger, T., Sinkovics, R., and edn., New York: Pearson Education. Scase, R. and Goffee, R. (1989) Reluctant Managers, London: Unwin Hyman. Schlegelmilch, B. (1999) ‘Data equivalence in crosscultural research: A comparison of Schuman, H. and Presser, S. (1976) Questions and Answers in Attitude Surveys: Experiments on Question classical test theory and latent trait theory based approaches’, Australasian Marketing Journal Form, Wording, and Context, New York: Academic Press. Schwandt, T.A. (1997) Qualitative 7(2): 23–38. Sarstedt, M. and Mooi, E. (2014) ‘Regression analysis,’ in Sarstedt, M. and Mooi, E. (eds,), A Concise Inquiry, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Schwartz, N. and Sudman, S. (eds.) (1996) Answering Questions: Methodology for Determining Guide to Market Research, 2nd edn., Cognitive and Communicative 315 316 References Processes in Survey Research, San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Schwartz, S.H. (1992) ‘Universals in the content and structure of values’, in Zane, M. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 25, New York: Academic Press. Sekaran, U. (1992) Womanpower: Managing in Times of Demographic Turbulence, London: Sage. Selltiz, C., Wrightsman, S., and Cook, S.W. (1976) Research Methods in Social Relations, 3rd edn., New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Shook, C.L., Ketchen, D.J. Jr., Hult, G.T.M., and Kacmar, K.M. (2004) ‘An assessment of the use of structural equation modeling in strategic management research’, Strategic Management Journal, 25: 397–404. Silverman, D. (1993) Interpreting Qualitative Data: Methods of overview’, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 45(2): 186–207. Sosik, J.J., Kahai, S.S., and Piovoso, M.J. (2009) ‘Silver bullet or voodoo statistics? A primer for using the partial least squares data analytic technique in group and organization research’, Group and Organization Management, 34(1): 5–36. Sousa, C.D.A., Martínez-López, F.J., and Coelho, F. (2008) ‘The determinants of export performance: A review of the research in the literature between 1998 and 2005’, International Journal of Management Reviews, 10(4): 343–74. Spiggle, S. (1994) ‘Analysis and interpretation of qualitative data in consumer research’, Journal of Consumer Research, 21 Analysing Talk, Text and Interaction, London: Sage. Silverman, D. (2001) Interpreting Qualitative Data, London: Sage. (December): 491–503. Stewart, D.W. and Shamdasani, P.N. (1990) Focus Groups: Theory and Practice, Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Simons, R. (1987) ‘Accounting control systems and business strategy: An empirical analysis’, Accounting, Strauss, A. and Corbin, J. (1990) Basics of Qualitative Research: Grounded Theory Procedures and Organisation and Society, 12(4): 357–74. Sinkovics, R., Penz, E., and Ghauri, P. Techniques, Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Strauss, A. and Corbin, J. (1994) (2008) ‘Enhancing the trustworthiness of qualitative research in international business’, Management International Review, 48(6): 689–714. Sorescu, A., Warren, N.L., and Ertekin, L. (2017) ‘Event study methodology in the marketing literature: An ‘Grounded theory methodology: An overview’, in Denzin, N.K. and Lincoln, Y.S. (eds.), Handbook of Qualitative Research, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, pp. 273–85. Studenmund, A.H. (2017) Using Econometrics: A Practical Guide, 7th edn., London: Pearson. References Sudman, S. and Bradburn, N. (1982) Asking Questions: A Practical Guide to Questionnaire Design. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Sudman, S. and Bradburn, N.M. (1989) Asking Questions, San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Svatko, J.E. (1989) ‘Working with consultants’, Small Business Reports, 14(2): 58–68. Personality, 60(2): 225–51. (Originally published as a technical report in 1961.) Utterback, J.M. (1994) Mastering the Dynamics of Innovation, Boston, MA: Harvard University Press. Van Maanen, J. (1979) ‘Reclaiming qualitative methods for organizational research: A preface’, Administrative Science Quarterly, Teece, D.J. (2007) ‘Explicating dynamic capabilities: The nature and microfoundations of (sustainable) enterprise performance’, Strategic 24(4): 520–6. Van Maanen, J. (ed.) (1983) Qualitative Methodology, Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Management Journal 28: 1319–50. Thompson, J.D. (1967) Organizations in Action: Social Science Bases of Van Maanen, J. (1995) ‘Style as a theory’, Organizational Science, 7(4): 641–52. Administrative Theory, New York: McGraw-Hill. Tranfield, D. and Starkey, K. (1998) ‘The Van Maanen, J. and Kolb, D. (1985) ‘The professional apprentice: Observations on fieldwork roles in nature of social organisation and promotion of management research: towards policy’, British two organizational settings’, in Research in the Sociology of Organizations, vol. 4, New York: Journal of Management, 9(4): 341–53. Triandis, H.C. (1972) The Analysis of Subjective Culture, New York: JAI Press. Van Maanen, J., Sorensen, J.B., and Mitchell, T. (2007) ‘The interplay between theory and method’, The Wiley. Tsang, E.W.K. (2014) ‘Generalizing from research findings: The merits of Academy of Management Review 32(4): 1145–54. Van Witteloostuijn, A. (2015) ‘Toward case studies’, International Journal of Management Review, 16(4): 369–83. experimental international business’, Cross Cultural Management, 22(4): 530–44. Tsoukas, H. and Chia, R. (eds.) (2011) Philosophy and Organization Theory: Research in the Sociology of Vijver, F. van de and Leung, K. (1997) Methods and Data Analysis for Cross-Cultural Research, Thousand Organizations, vol. 32, Bradford: Emerald. Tupes, E.C. and Christal, R.E. (1992/ 1961) ‘Recurrent personality factors Oaks, CA: Sage. Weisberg, H.F. (1992) Central Tendency and Variability, Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences, based on trait ratings’, Journal of 83, Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 317 318 References Whigham, D. (2010) Business Data Yin, R.K. (2009) Case Study Research: Analysis Using Excel. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Whitehead, A.N. (1911) An Introduction Design and Methods, 4th edn., Los Angeles, CA: Sage. Zaltman, G., Lemasters, K., and to Mathematics, London: Williams & Norgate. Whitley, R. (1984) ‘The scientific status of management research as a practically oriented social science’, Journal of Management Studies, 21(4): 369–90. Whittington, R., Yakis-Douglas, B., and Ahn, K. (2016) ‘Cheap talk? Strategy presentations as a form of chief executive officer impression management’, Strategic Management Journal, 37(12): 2413–24. Wiedersheim-Paul, F. and Eriksson, T. (1998) Att Utreda Forska och Rapportera, Malmo: Liber. Wind, Y. and Douglas, S.P. (1982) ‘Comparative consumer research: the next frontier’, Management Decisions, 4: 24–35. Wu, H. and Leung, S.-O. (2017) ‘Can Likert scales be treated as interval scales? A simulation study’, Journal of Social Service Research, 43(4): 527–32. Yin, R.K. (1994) Case Study Research: Design and Methods, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Yin, R.K. (2003) Case Study Research, 3rd edn., London: Sage. Heffering, M. (1982) Theory Construction in Marketing, New York: Wiley. Zaltman, G., Pinson, C.R.A., and Angelmar, R. (1977) Metatheory and Consumer Research, New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Zellmer-Bruhn, M., Caliguiri, P., and Thomas, D.C. (2016) ‘From the editors: experimental designs in international business research’, Journal of International Business Studies, 47(4): 393–407. Zhu, P. and Malhotra, S. (2008) ‘Announcement effect and price pressure: An empirical study of cross-border acquisitions by Indian firms’, International Research Journal of Finance and Economics, 13: 24–41. Zikmund, W.G. (1997) Business Research Methods, 5th edn., Fort Worth, TX: Dryden Press. Zou, S. and Stan, S. (1998) ‘The determinants of export performance: A review of the empirical literature between 1987 and 1997’, International Marketing Review, 15(5): 333–56. Index a priori method, 22, 75, 140 sampling, 108 a posteriori knowledge, 10 selection of cases, 106 abduction, 21 selection strategies, 109 absorptive capacity (ACAP), 79 single design, 110 abstraction, 135 skills required, 106 accessible population, 107 theoretical sampling, 110 alternative hypothesis, 194 training for, 106 analytic surveys, 169 triangulation, 147 assigned research problem, 50 history, 149 ATLAS.ti, 146 attractive nuisance, 130 average variance extracted (AVE), 251 axial coding, 135 method bias, checking, 149 two-step, 149 when to use, 103 categorical variables, 183, 236 mean values not applicable, 187 bar charts, 183 bias no intrinsic order or ranking, 82 categorization, 134 common method bias, 234 category questions, 173 method bias, 149 causation non-response bias, 165 omitted variable bias, 224 semi-structured interviews, 115 bibliography, 287 binomial logit regression, 230 likely direction, 211 classic experiment model, 66 intuition underpinning, 67 limited use, 67 simplest form, 66 bootstrapping, 252 clear instructions for return of survey capabilities (CAPS), 213, 238 closed questions, 173 CAQDAS (computer-assisted cluster sampling, 164 instrument, 178 qualitative data analysis software), Cochrane-Orcutt method, 224 146, 273 codes, 134 analysis, 142 ficient of determination (R ), 252 coef ficient of variation (CV), 190 comparative, 103 collinear explanatory variables, 226 deductive, 111 common method bias, 234 de common method variance, 234 case studies fi nition, 101 design stage, 105 discon firmation stage, 106 2 coef common sense de finition, 10 drift stage, 105 non-conscious ideology, 14 focus groups, 108 research, versus, 12 13 generalization, 150 science, differences between, 16 – inductive, 111 communality of variables, 245 matrix analysis, 143 communication, 114 multiple design, 111 comparative prediction stage, 105 comparison, 135 fit index (CFI), 252 320 Index comparison of means observation, 269 paired samples, 197 responsibility for accuracy, 266 composite reliability (CR) values, 251, 258 same meaning drawn from question, computers 270 qualitative research, use in fi unit of analysis, de ning, 267 knowledge required, 147 data comparability, 262 program examples, 146 emic approach, 268 concept list, 143 enhancing trustworthiness, 273 concepts computer assistance, 273 critical role, 39 equivalence, 273 functions of, 39 questionnaires, 274 finitions, 40 conceptual de etic approach, 268 conclusions, 285 unit of analysis, 270 fidentiality, 26, 119 con firmatory factor analysis, 244 con criteria for membership, 272 geographic, 271 constant comparative method, 136 situational context, 272 construct theory, 21 cross-sectional design, 71 construct validity, 86 cross-table content validity, 86 social class, 64 context of discovery, 39 cultural distance between countries, 227 context of justi cation, 38 cumulative abnormal returns (CAR), 256 continuous variables, 183, 236 customer satisfaction, 79, 83 fi ficient, 301 correlation coef interval scale, 82 data analysis median values, 188 ratio scales, 83 case studies, control of corruption (CC), quality of case studies focus groups, 124 institutions, 202 advantages, 126 convenience sampling, 166 conducting, 126 convergent validity, 87 disadvantages, 126 ficient correlation coef interviews continuous variables, 199 ordinal variables, 204 see data collection controlling, 122 data display, 133 correlation matrix, 199 data reduction, 132 correlations deception, 26 Pearson (product-moment) correlation coef ficient, 200 strong and weak, 200 deduction, 20 process, 19 deductive case studies, 111 covariance-based SEM (CB-SEM), 251 deductive reasoning, 20 cover letter, 178 de critical realism, 18 ficient alpha, 245, 247 Cronbach’s coef cross-cultural research data analysis fi nitions, 39 conceptual, 40 operational, 40 degrees of freedom, 192 demonstrated validity, 146 two phases, 272 data collection, 265 descriptive surveys, 170 descriptive validity, 146 costs obtaining, 265 dimensionalization, 137 culture and behaviour, understanding, disturbance term, 216, 223 268 fi nitions, 265 differing de constant variance, 224 no systematic relationship, 224 emic and etic approaches, 268 divergent validity, 87 instrument adapted to culture, 270 dummy variables, 213–14, 237 Index fication, 48, 150 emic approach, 268 falsi empirical generalization, 150 feedback loops, 34 epistemology, 9 debate, 10 knowledge based on sense and experience, 10 field experiments, 179 figures shortage of in report, 280 final report equivalence, 86, 273 oral presentation, 288 estimation window, 254 structure, 281 ethics, 23 bibliography, 287 legality and illegality, 25 conclusions, 285 moral responsibility, 24, 27 executive summary, 282 fluencing, 28 factors in fi ndings, 284 negligence and legal action, 25 footnotes, 286 problem formulation, 24 introduction, 283 public interest, 28 methodology, 284 –participant (subject) relationship, 26–7 researcher problem statement, 283 references, 287 etic approach, 268 table of contents, 282 event studies theoretical background, 283 time limits, 254 event study analysis, 253 title page, 282 writing, 276 applicability, 256 appearance, 281 cross-border acquisitions, 258 assessment criteria, 277 estimation procedure, 249 audience, 277 event window, 254 for publication, 290 executive summary, 282 form and style, 280 expected variability, 169 format, 278 experimental studies, 179 headings, 281 advantages, 179 language and disadvantages, 180 purpose, 276 fl ow, 279 explanatory power, 222, 233, 239 readability, 278 explanatory variables, 211 strengths and weaknesses of methods, collinear, 226 277 common method variance, 234 findings, 284 effect sizes, 235 Fisher-Pearson coef multiple regression model, 221 omission of, 224 fi cance, 234 fi cance, 232 statistical signi testing joint signi exploratory factor analysis, 244 job-related burnout on oil rigs, 246 export performance, 213 external validity, 68 ficient of skewness (SK) calculating, 190 decision criterion, 191 focus groups, 108, 124, 126 advantages, 126 conducting, 126 disadvantages, 126 footnotes, 286 format questionnaire, 178 face validity, 86 frictions, 13 factor analysis con firmatory, 244 garbage-can models or decisions, 46 exploratory, 244, 246 Gauss-Markov theorem, 223 two main applications, 243 generalizable validity, 146 factor extraction, 244 factor rotation, 244 factor scores, 245 government effectiveness (GE), quality of institutions, 202 grounded theory, 100, 142 321 322 Index heteroskedasticity, 230 structured, 115 histogram, 183 time limits, 117 historical review, 99 time management, 122 homoskedasticity, 223 unstructured, 115 hypothesis testing, 231 introduction, 283 common method variance, 234 inverse U-shaped relationship, 217 effect sizes, 234 iteration, 138 explanatory variables, 232 four basic steps, 192 fi judgement (purposive) sampling, 166 individual regression coef cients, 231 knowledge development, 9 independent samples comparison of means, 195 individualism versus collectivism dimension (IDV), 227 laboratory experiments, 179 Likert scales, 82, 175, 230 – linear relationship, 215 16 inducement, 178 living standards, 79 induction, 20 long and complicated sentences, 280 process, 19 longitudinal design, 73 inductive case studies, 111 long-term orientation (LTO), 227 inductive reasoning, 20 indulgent societies (IND), 227 mail questionnaires, 171 inferences, 191 maintained hypothesis, 233 integration, 138 mapping, 41, 89 90 intended contribution of research, 38, 58 market model – internal consistency, 86, 245, 287 internal validity, 68 international research, estimation individual see cross-cultural research interpretative validity, 146 interpretivism, 18 firms, 255 ordinary least squares (OLS), 255 window, 254 masculinity versus femininity dimension (MAS), 227 interval scale, 82 matrix display, case analysis, 143 interviews mean, 187 advantages, 116 measures of dispersion, 188 analysis of research problem, 117 median, 188 con method bias, 149 contacting subjects, 118 method of authority, 22 controlling, 122 method of intuition, 22 disadvantages, 116 method of science, 22 drafting questions, 117 method of tenacity, 22 initial discussion, 121 methodology, 284 language style, 121 mode, 188 leading questions, 121 moral responsibility, 24, 27 fi dentiality issues, 119 confi rmation letter, 119 pilot study, 117 common, 234 fluencing, 9 factors in post-interview notes, 123 multicollinearity, 226 pre-interview preparation, 120 multi-item scales, 83 recording information, 119 relationship with interviewee, 122 resource availability, 119 construct measurement advantages, 84 SEM, and, 85 reward for respondents, 119 multinomial logit regression, 230 semi-structured, 115 multiple choice questions, 173 sensitive questions, 123 multiple design case studies, 111 Index multiple regression non-sampling error, 162 estimation methodology, 219 coef ficient of determination, 221 cultural distance between countries, 227 normal distribution, 186 notify possible respondents in advance, 178 null hypothesis, 192 multicollinearity, 226 nominal or ordinal dependent observations variables, 230 advantages and disadvantages, 111 omission of relevant explanatory variables, 224 contrived setting, 113 field observation, 112 ordinary least squares (OLS), 219, 223 human, 113 residual scatter plot, 228, 239 mechanical method, 113 export performance non-participant, 113 determinants, 211 omitted variable bias, 224 linear relationship, 215 one-shot case study, 71 operationalization of variables, 213 – ontology, 9 ordinary least squares (OLS), 236 8 open coding, 134 quadratic relationship, 217 open questions, 172 semi-logarithmic relationship, 216 operational de time lags, 219 operationalization of variables, 79, 213 fi nitions, 40 features of a good model, 239 oral presentation of report, 288 hypothesis testing, 231 ordinal logistic regression, 230 common method variance, 234 ordinal scale, 82 effect sizes, 234 ordinal variables ficient, 204 explanatory variables, 232 correlation coef individual regression coef cients, 231 nominal variables, relationship between, fi fi statistical signi cance, 234 206 operationalization, 82 – NVivo, 146, 273 ordinary least squares (OLS), 219, 236 8 national culture individualism versus collectivism dimension (IDV), 227 paired t-test, 197 panel data, 73 indulgent societies (IND), 227 parameters long-term orientation (LTO), 227 masculinity versus femininity dimension (MAS), 227 population, 191 estimates only, 192 null hypothesis, 192 power distance dimension (PDI), 227 parametric tests, 299 uncertainty avoidance dimension (UAI), parsimonious model, 239 227 parsimony, 238 – negative linear relationship, 215 16 negative semi-logarithmic relationship, 217 nominal scale, 81 nominal variables operationalization, 82 ordinal variables, relationship between, 206 non-normal distribution, 190 partial least squares SEM (PLS-SEM), 251 green innovation, 253 interpreting, 251 past literature, 45 importance for study, 57 intended contribution of study, 58 pattern matching, 142 Pearson (product-moment) correlation coef ficient, 199–200, 203 non-parametric tests, 299 peer pressure, 28 non-probability sampling, 162, 166 personal and professional experiences, 50 non-response, 162 personal interviews, 171 non-response bias, 161 pilot study, 117 323 324 Index political stability and absence of violence (PS), quality of institutions, 202 population departure from theory, 140 dimensionalization, 137 display, 133 accessible, 107 gaining insights, 131 de grasping the meanings of others, 140 finition, 161, 191 empirical generalization, 150 grounded theory, 142 linear regression model, 220 integration, 138 non-response bias, 165 interpretation, 139 null hypothesis, 192 iteration, 138 representative sample, 126 problems, 130 sample size, 168 quantifying, 143 strati record keeping, 139 fied sample, 164 target, 106 reduction, 132 positive linear relationship, 215 refutation, 138 positivism, 18 reporting procedures, 139 post-modernism, 18 seeking patterns in meanings, 140 power distance dimension (PDI), 227 pragmatism, 18 tabulations, 139 data collection, 97 pre-interview preparation, 120 advantages, 99 primary data, 153 case studies, see case studies three advantages, 160 communication, 114 two main disadvantages, 160 grounded theory, 100 types, 159 historical review, 99 – probability sampling, 162 3 interviews, see interviews cluster sampling, 164 low number of observations, 98 non-probability sampling, 166 observations. non-response bias, 165 qualitative versus quantitative simple random sampling, 163 strati fied random sampling, 164 systematic sampling, 164 problem formulation, 24 see observations methods, 96 qualitative versus quantitative methods, difference in emphasis, 97 qualitative versus quantitative problem statement, 283 methods, procedure, 96 problem structure, 62 summary, 111 causal research, 64 techniques, 98 descriptive research, 64 three major components, 99 explanatory research, 63 unit of study, 95 public interest, 28 main emphasis, 129 purpose of book, 3 traditional model, 130 p-value, 195 validity, 146 quality of institutions, 202 QSR NUD*IST, 146 Worldwide Governance Indicators – quadratic relationship, 217 18 qualitative research computers, use of knowledge required, 147 program examples, 146 data analysis abstraction, 135 categorization, 134 codes, 134 comparison, 135 constant comparative method, 136 – (WGI), 202 3 quantitative data comparison of means independent samples, 195 computers, use of, 206 continuous variables ficient, 199 correlation coef data analysis event study analysis, see event study analysis factor analysis, see factor analysis Index – hypothesis testing, 191 5 representative sample, 162 structural equation modelling (SEM), research see structural equation modelling (SEM) abduction, 21 common sense, versus, 13, data description – bar charts, 183 deduction, 19 20 histogram, 183 deductive reasoning, 20 mean, 187 different orientations measures of dispersion, 188, 190 measures of skewness, 190 median, 188 process, 16 summary, 18 ethics, 23 mode, 188 legality and illegality, 25 – scatter plot, 186 ordinal variables see also common sense moral responsibility, 24, 27 8 ficient, 204 correlation coef paired samples negligence and legal action, 25 problem formulation, 24 public interest, 28 comparison of means, 197 variables, correlations between, 206 quantitative research induction, 19 inductive reasoning, 20 knowledge skills, 22 data collection making judgements, 11 experimental studies, see ongoing process, 23 experimental studies primary data, 153, purpose of, 13 see also primary data qualitative, see qualitative research see quantitative research quantitative, see sampling see secondary data research, see survey research fi rst, 21 sampling, theory or data secondary data, understanding phenomena, 10 survey techniques, 98 research design choice of, 36 questionnaires choice of model, 61 – construction guidelines, 175 6 classic experiment model, 66 design, 172 intuition underpinning, 67 improving response rates, 178 limited use, 67 types, 171 simplest form, 66 – quota sampling, 166 7 cross-sectional design, 71 errors, 62 ranking questions, 174 longitudinal, 73 rating questions, 175 panel data, 73 ratio scale, 83 problem structure, 62 readership of book, 5 causal research, 64 record keeping, 139 descriptive research, 64 redundancy of data, 130 references, 287 refutation, 138 a priori method, 75 REGION variable, 213 choices and decisions, 76 regression analysis, 210 explanatory variables, effect of, 234 judegment, and, 240 multiple regression, explanatory research, 63 requirements, 74 see multiple regression time-series data, 72 validity threats, 67 types, 68 research measurement improving, 87 regression sum of squares (SSR), 222 interval scale, 82 regulatory quality (RQ), quality of mapping, 89 institutions, 202 multi-item scale, 83 325 326 Index research measurement (cont.) advantages, 85 SEM and, 85 theory before research, 38 topic and problem, relationship, 35 researcher nominal scale, 81 operationalization of variables, 78 ordinal scale, 82 ratio scale, 83 –participant (subject) relationship, 25 – ethical concerns, 26 7 residual scatter plot, 228 OLS regression of export performance, reliability 239 construct validity, 86 restricted hypothesis, 233 content validity, 86 root-mean-square error of approximation convergent validity, 87 divergent validity, 87 (RMSEA), 252 rule of law (RL), quality of institutions, 202 three attributes, 85 research problems capturing, 35 ideas sample regression line, 220 sampling basic terminology, 161 past literature, 45 cluster sampling, 164 sources, 46 frame, 162 3 identifying, 47 – non-probability sampling, 166 asking questions, 47 non-response bias, 165 sources of problems, 50 probability, 163 structured/unstructured, 49 sample size, 168 vague and general, 48 simple random sampling, 163 models in research, 52 descriptive, 54 strati explanatory, 55 sampling error, 162 guidance, 57 scatter plot, 186 key characteristics, 52 scepticism, 10 predictive/forecasting, 56 secondary data, 153 structured, 53 advantages, 158 value chain, 54 disadvantages, 158 past literature selected commercially available sources, importance for study, 57 intended contribution of study, 58 purpose, 50 research process concepts 157 selected publicly available sources, 158 usefulness of, 154 SECTOR variable, 214 selective coding, 135 – critical role, 39 semi-logarithmic relationship, 216 17 functions of, 39 semi-structured interviews, 115, 171 cross-cultural dimension, managing, 262 de fied random sampling, 164 systematic sampling, 164 finitions, 39 bias, 115 sigma, 187 ficance level, 193–4 distinct stages, 33 signi empirical measurements, 36 simple random sampling, 163 ethical issues, 26 single design case studies, 110 illustrative example, 33 size of knowledge, producing, 37 skewed distributions, 191 levels of research, 36 skewness, 190 methods, 42 research before theory, 38 theory de fi nition, 41 purpose, 42 firm, 79, 213 ficient (SK) Fisher-Pearson coef calculating, 190 decision criterion, 191 slope of the relationship linear, 216 Index quadratic, 218 semi-logarithmic, 217 time-series data, 72, 74, 187 fi title page, 282 Spearman correlation coef cient, 204–5 total sum of squares (SST), 222 spoken and written language, differences, transaction costs, 280 sponsor, 178 stability, 85 standard deviation, 190 see frictions trans-disciplinarity in research, 13 triangulation, 36, 147 history, 149 method bias, checking, 149 state of nature, 193 two-step case study, 149 strati Type I error, 193–4 fied random sampling, 164 structural equation modelling (SEM), 247 Type II error, 193, 195 estimating, 250 advantages, 250 uncertainty avoidance dimension (UAI), 227 complementary procedures, 251 uniform distribution, 186 green innovation, 252 unit of analysis path diagram, 250 criteria for membership, 272 visualization of, 249 cross-country research, 272 fining, 271 structured interviews, 115 de structured problems, 62 geographic scope, 271 student’ s t-test, 195 sum of squared residuals (SSE), 221 survey research analytic, 169 descriptive, 170 situational context, 272 units of observation cross-sectional design, 71 longitudinal design, 73 panel data, 73 email or website questionnaires, 171 unstructured interviews, 115 mail questionnaires, 171 unstructured problems, 63 questionnaires, 171 use of terminology, 280 construction guidelines, 175–6 use of book, 7 design, 172 U-shaped relationship, 215 improving response rates, 178 semi-structured interviews, 171 telephone/video interviews, 172 validity qualitative research, 146 systematic sampling, 164 validity threats, 67 table of contents, 282 value chain, 54 tables variance types, 68 shortage of in report, 280 average variance extracted (AVE), 251 tabulations, 139 common method, 234 target population, 106 disturbance terms, 224 technical literature, 50 in telegraphic style, 280 regression sum of squares (SSR), 222 telephone/video interviews, 172 theoretical background, 283 theoretical generalization, 150 flation factor (VIF), 226 total sum of squares (SST), 222 voice and accountability (VA), quality of institutions, 202 theoretical sampling, 110 theoretical studies, 36 weighted least squares (WLS) method, 224 theoretical validity, 146 World Bank theoretically grounded regression model, 239 Worldwide Governance Indicators (WGI), 202–3 327