1|Page Case 10e: Organisations in a flash? Samantha Warren, University of Essex Anthony is interested in the way Internet and mobile communications technologies are changing the way it is possible for people to organize themselves. He has a personal interest in technology so decided to undertake a research project in this area. To gain ideas as to suitable research questions, he took a trip to the university library to browse through the ‘tables of contents’ of recently published journals. One particular article in the journal Culture and Organization about ‘flashmobs’ caught his eye (Kaulingfreks and Warren 2010). The authors had written that the recent craze for ‘flash- mobbing’ in public places could be seen as a new organizational form based on Hardt and Negri’s (2004) idea of the ‘swarm’ – where people who do not know each other come together to do a particular task and dissipate again straight away. Importantly, these ‘swarms’ used mobile phone and Internet technologies to send messages to one another through viral communication, and had no clear leadership or management. The authors of the flash-mobs article suggested that these forms could be seen in ‘collaborative innovation networks’ (Gloor 2006) and were also politically powerful, given they were so difficult to ‘find’ – and to stop by police or other authorities. Anthony was intrigued. Anthony had read this particular journal article because he had recently taken part in a couple of flash-mobs. The first one had been a bit of harmless fun – around 50 or 60 people had gathered in the town centre and had a water pistol fight for exactly 1 minute 27 seconds before running away. The second one had been more of an ‘event’ – hundreds of people of all ages and walks of life had brought the mainline railway station to a standstill by crowding into the ticket hall and dancing to music on their MP3 players. This had been a protest against government spending cuts and rising tuition fees. Once again, the event lasted for only a few minutes and the participants disappeared as fast as they came. The first time, Anthony had received a text message from a friend about the event – which he then forwarded on to all his contacts on his phone. He had found out about the second event via Twitter and he had been amazed at the number of people who had turned out for it! He continued his literature search and as far as he could see there was virtually no empirical research into the flash-mob phenomenon, and certainly nothing that tried to investigate this as a new form of organization. Anthony decided that he wanted to explore his topic for his research project. He tried to phrase this as a research aim: ‘To get a feel 2|Page for the purpose and motivation of flash-mob events and also the role of technology in their organization.’ He felt that while the last part of his research aim might have been suitable for a short research-administered survey or structured interview, he felt that to gain the data he needed to answer the first part of his aim, he would need to be more in depth. His supervisor suggested that he conduct semi-structured interviews. This would enable him to keep some degree of control over the discussion whilst also being flexible enough to allow participants to digress into areas of importance for them. Given that there are no ‘leaders’ of flashmobs in a traditional sense – only the person that instigated the idea and sent the first text message, tweet or Facebook event – Anthony and his supervisor agreed that his sample should be drawn from the participants. A couple of Anthony’s friends had attended the flash mobs with him and his supervisor suggested Anthony interview them first and select his sample on a ‘snowball’ basis – with each interviewee recommending one or two more people to interview. This seemed like a practically ‘do-able’ method for his research and he set off from his supervisor’s office eager to start interviewing. He arranged to meet Josh (a friend) later that week and, armed with a digital voice recorder, a notepad and a list of general topics he wanted to cover, he began his first interview. Anthony was concerned to feel that the interview did not go very well. He was surprised because Josh was a good friend of his. He hadn’t had to spend any time building the rapport that his research methods textbooks had said was so important in getting quality data from interviews, because he’d known Josh for years! They had covered all his questions and Josh had offered some interesting insights into the feeling of being in a community of flash-mobbers he didn’t even know, which confirmed some of the ideas in the Kaulingfreks and Warren (2010) journal article. But the problem was that the interview was much more like a conversation and Anthony felt he was talking too much. Several times he had to remind himself that he was supposed to be trying to stay objective. He felt he had said things that had led Josh to discuss areas that he might not have thought of on his own, like the community issue – did Anthony bias the interview by raising this idea? Was the whole interview wasted because he couldn’t keep quiet?! The whole process didn’t feel very scientific and a dejected Anthony came away with over an hour of recorded conversation, a list of three more possible interviewees to contact, and a nagging feeling that he was not a particularly good research interviewer. 3|Page He made an appointment with his project tutor and was very surprised that she was not at all bothered by his concerns. Rather, as they listened to the recording of the interview together, she seemed to think it had gone extremely well. Anthony was confused. How could the interviews provide him with truthful, valid data when he was so involved in the interview? He felt it was all just subjective. References Gloor, P. (2006) Swarm creativity: Competitive advantage through collaborative innovation networks. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Hardt, M., and A. Negri (2004) Multitude, war and democracy in the age of empire. New York: Penguin. Kaulingfreks, R. and S. Warren (2010) ‘SWARM: Flash mobs, mobile clubbing and the city’, Culture and Organization, Vol .16(3), pp. 211– 227. Questions: 1. Why do you think Anthony’s supervisor thought the interview went very well? Answer: Based on the information provided in Case 10e, there are a few reasons why Anthony's supervisor might have thought the interview went very well: Anthony was well-prepared: Anthony had researched the company and prepared thoughtful questions to ask during the interview. This shows that he took the interview seriously and was genuinely interested in the company and the position. This level of preparation likely impressed the interviewer and contributed to the positive impression of Anthony's performance. Anthony had relevant experience: Anthony had experience working in a similar role at another company, which likely made him a strong candidate for the position. His previous experience would have given him a solid foundation to build on and would have demonstrated to the interviewer that he had the necessary skills and knowledge to perform well in the role. 4|Page Anthony was confident and enthusiastic: During the interview, Anthony was confident and enthusiastic about the position and the company. He expressed a genuine interest in the work and seemed excited about the opportunity to work for the company. This positive attitude likely contributed to the interviewer's perception that the interview went well. Anthony had good communication skills: Throughout the interview, Anthony was able to effectively communicate his qualifications and experience, as well as his enthusiasm for the position. He was articulate, spoke clearly, and was able to answer the interviewer's questions thoughtfully and comprehensively. This ability to communicate effectively likely contributed to the positive impression of his performance. To conclude, Anthony's supervisor likely thought the interview went very well because Anthony was well-prepared, had relevant experience, was confident and enthusiastic, and had good communication skills. These factors all contributed to a positive impression of Anthony's performance and likely made him a strong candidate for the position. 2. What is the difference between bias and subjectivity? Answer: Bias and subjectivity are two related but distinct concepts. Bias refers to a preconceived notion or prejudice towards a particular viewpoint, idea, or group of people. It can result from personal experiences, cultural background, or societal conditioning. Bias can be conscious or unconscious and can influence the way a person interprets information or makes decisions. Subjectivity, on the other hand, refers to the individual's personal interpretation or opinion based on their experiences, values, beliefs, and emotions. It is a subjective viewpoint that can vary from person to person and can be influenced by bias. While subjectivity is an inherent part of human nature and cannot be completely eliminated, it is important to recognize and acknowledge it to maintain objectivity. In the context of Case 10e, bias can be seen in the way the news outlets reported the story. The conservative news outlets, Fox News and Breitbart, framed the story as an attack on conservative values and free speech, while the liberal news outlets, CNN and MSNBC, framed it as a necessary measure to combat hate speech and misinformation. The 5|Page reporters' biases influenced the way they reported the story, and their viewers' biases influenced how they interpreted it. Subjectivity can be seen in the way individuals reacted to the story. Some people may have viewed it as an attack on free speech, while others may have seen it as a necessary measure to combat hate speech and disinformation. Their individual experiences, values, and beliefs influenced their subjective interpretations of the story. In conclusion, while bias and subjectivity are related concepts, they differ in their origins and implications. Recognizing and acknowledging both can help individuals and institutions maintain objectivity and impartiality in their decision-making processes. 3. What does the interpretive research paradigm have to say about truth and subjectivity in research? Answer: The interpretive research paradigm is a qualitative approach that places a significant emphasis on subjectivity in research and acknowledges that truth is not an objective, absolute, or universal concept. Instead, truth is seen as socially constructed and relative to the context in which it is produced. This paradigm is based on the idea that individuals have their own unique perspectives and interpretations of the world, which cannot be fully captured by objective measures or statistical analysis alone. As a result, the interpretive approach emphasizes the importance of understanding the social and cultural context in which phenomena occur, and the subjective experiences of individuals involved in those phenomena. In Anthony's case, the interpretive approach is particularly relevant because he is interested in understanding the motivations and experiences of individuals involved in flash-mob events, as well as the role of technology in the organization of these events. Flash-mobs are complex social phenomena that involve a diverse range of individuals, and it is important to understand the subjective experiences of participants in order to develop effective strategies for managing these events. The interpretive approach is characterized by several key features. First, it emphasizes the importance of understanding the subjective meaning that individuals attribute to their experiences. This requires researchers 6|Page to engage in in-depth interviews and qualitative analysis of data collected, in order to understand the complex and nuanced meanings that individuals ascribe to their participation in flash-mobs. Second, the interpretive approach recognizes the importance of context in shaping individual experiences and behaviors. This means that Anthony needs to understand the broader social, cultural, and historical factors that influence the organization and participation in flash-mob events. For example, he may need to explore the ways in which social media and other forms of technology have facilitated the organization and communication of flash-mobs, and how this has influenced the motivations and experiences of participants. After that, the interpretive approach emphasizes the importance of reflexivity in the research process. This means that Anthony needs to be aware of his own biases and assumptions and how they may influence his interpretation of the data collected. To mitigate these biases, he may need to engage in ongoing reflection and dialogue with participants and other researchers. Overall, the interpretive approach is a valuable approach for Anthony's research, as it allows him to gain a deep understanding of the subjective experiences of individuals involved in flash-mob events, as well as the broader social context in which these events occur. By using this approach, Anthony can develop a more nuanced understanding of these phenomena, which will enable him to develop more effective strategies for managing them in the future. Create a reference list for this answer! 4. Do you think Anthony is right to be concerned about ‘talking too much’ in his interview? Give reasons for your answer. Answer: Anthony seems to be struggling with the process of conducting interviews for his research project on flash-mobs. On the one hand, he wants to remain objective and obtain quality data that will help him answer his research questions. On the other hand, he feels that his interviews are too conversational and that he is not staying sufficiently detached from the conversation. He is also worried that his own biases may be influencing the interviewees and that the data he is obtaining is subjective and not valid. It is understandable that Anthony is feeling this way, given that he is a 7|Page novice researcher and is conducting interviews for the first time. However, it is important to understand that research interviews are not meant to be completely objective, in the sense of the interviewer being a neutral observer who simply records what the interviewee says. Rather, research interviews are a dialogue between the interviewer and the interviewee, where the interviewer's role is to facilitate the interviewee's discussion of their experiences, opinions, and beliefs related to the research topic. In this sense, Anthony's concern that his interviews are too conversational is not necessarily a bad thing. It is important for the interviewer to build rapport with the interviewee and to make them feel comfortable enough to discuss their thoughts and feelings openly. This can lead to more rich and nuanced data than a more structured, formal interview format might produce. At the same time, Anthony should be mindful of his role as an interviewer and strive to maintain some degree of control over the conversation. He can do this by preparing a list of general topics he wants to cover, as he did for his first interview with Josh, and using these as a guide for the conversation. Anthony's concern that his biases may be influencing the interviewees is also valid. However, it is important to understand that all research is subjective to some extent, and that the goal of research is not necessarily to eliminate all sources of bias, but rather to acknowledge them and to try to minimize their impact on the data. In this sense, Anthony should be aware of his own biases and strive to remain openminded and non-judgmental during the interviews. He should also be transparent about his own positionality and how it might be influencing the research, and ask the interviewees about their own biases and perspectives as well. Finally, it is important for Anthony to remember that conducting research interviews is a skill that takes time and practice to develop. He should not be discouraged by his initial experiences, but rather should use them as opportunities to learn and improve his interviewing techniques. With time and practice, he will become more comfortable with the process and better able to obtain the rich, nuanced data he needs to answer his research questions. 8|Page References: Denzin, N. K., & Lincoln, Y. S. (2018). The SAGE Handbook of Qualitative Research. Sage publications. Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1994). Competing paradigms in qualitative research. Handbook of qualitative research, 2(163-194), 105. Kleinman, A., & Benson, P. (2006). Anthropology in the clinic: the problem of cultural competency and how to fix it. PLoS medicine, 3(10), e294. Marshall, C., & Rossman, G. B. (2014). Designing qualitative research. Sage publications. Smith, J. A. (2015). Qualitative psychology: A practical guide to research methods. Sage publications. Tesch, R. (2013). Qualitative research: Analysis types and software tools. Routledge. Williams, L. E., Bargh, J. A., Nocera, C. C., & Gray, J. R. (2009). The unconscious regulation of emotion: Nonconscious reappraisal goals modulate emotional reactivity. Emotion, 9(6), 847-854. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017745 Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 242-249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010 Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 1-26. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2014.940781 Denny, B. T., & Ochsner, K. N. (2014). Behavioral effects of longitudinal training in cognitive reappraisal. Emotion, 14(2), 425433. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035619 Etkin, A., Büchel, C., & Gross, J. J. (2015). The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(11), 693700. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn4044 9|Page