The Relationship of Procrastination to the Academic Performance of the Students in the City of Mati National High School Review of Related Literature and Studies This section contains readings and literature from different sources like journals, books, websites, articles, thesis and dissertations related to the study. It starts with the definition and determinants of the procrastination and it’s relationship to the academic performance of the students. The independent variable in this study is the procrastination which can be considered as a cause, while the dependent variable is the respective academic performance of the students. The discussion goes further to include other studies as well as there indicators. Procrastination According to Joseph Ferrari, a professor of psychology at DePaul University in Chicago and author of “Still Procrastinating: The No Regret Guide to Getting It Done,” around 20% of U.S. adults are chronic procrastinators. Academic procrastination is very common among university students: almost all occasionally procrastinate in one or another domain of their studies, and approximately every second student regularly procrastinates (Rothblum et al., 1986; Steel, 2007). Considerable attention has been given to procrastination in university setting (Klassen et al., 2008). The student population is especially prone to procrastination, with an estimated prevalence of 50– 95% (Steel, 2007). Procrastination may be defined as ‘the voluntary delay of an intended and necessary and/or [personally] important activity, despite expecting potential negative consequences that outweigh the positive consequences of the delay’ (Klingsieck, 2013, 26). Typical for procrastination is that it is irrational and not imposed by external matters and it is often accompanied by subjective discomfort and negative consequences (Klingsieck, 2013). Procrastination is often associated with several negative factors, such as lower academic performance (Steel et al., 2001), increased stress (Sirois et al., 2003) and poorer mental health (Stead et al., 2010). Therefore, it is necessary to understand the causes and the factors that maintain procrastination in order to be able to reduce it. The challenge is that research in the area of procrastination often lacks a coherent, theoretical explanation of the behaviour (Glick et al., 2014) which has made it difficult to understand the phenomenon and to follow the research (e.g., Klingsieck, 2013; Schraw et al., 2007; Steel, 2007). Therefore, there are prevailing questions regarding the underlying and maintaining mechanisms of procrastination which are yet to be learnt (Katz et al., 2014; Visser et al., 2018). There is little evidence on cultural variations in the prevalence of procrastination. One study (Ferrari, O’Callaghan, & Newbegin, 2005) found that the reported prevalence of avoidant and arousal procrastination was higher in English citizens than U.S. or Australian citizens. They concluded that chronic procrastination was common among westernized, individualistic, Anglophone countries. The core characteristic of procrastination Is the intention-action gap suggesting that the procrastinators often have good intentions, but the challenge lies in the implementation of these intentions (Dewitte and Lens, 2000). Thus, procrastination has traditionally been understood as a self-regulation or time management problem (Wolters et al., 2017). There is a strong body of evidence suggesting that lower levels of self-regulating behaviours are related to higher levels of procrastination, and thus self-regulation is one of the keys to understanding procrastination (Ferrari, 2001). However, Visser et al. (2018) suggest that procrastination is complex behaviour that involves both cognitive and emotional elements as well as evaluations of one’s own competence. Recent research suggests that instead of being purely a self-regulation or time management problem, procrastination is also strongly influenced by psychological factors, such as the low confidence in one’s own abilities to perform (Steel, 2007) and inability to cope with negative emotions that arise in challenging situations referring to the centrality of psychological flexibility in understanding procrastination (Dionne, 2016; Gagnon et al., 2016). In this article, we aim to bring together these central constructs that have usually been addressed separately in previous studies in order to understand the phenomenon of procrastination and its underlying mechanisms better. Researchers have traditionally regarded procrastination as a maladaptive or dysfunctional strategy used in an attempt to cope with conflict or choices (Mann, 1982). Lay and Schouwenburg, 1993, Solomon and Rothblum, 1984 argued that because definitions of procrastination refer to both behavioral delay and psychological distress one should consider the magnitude of procrastination in conjunction with the magnitude of its negative psychological consequences; assumed to be emotional discomfort, including guilt, depression, anxiety or stress. From this perspective procrastination is a wholly dysfunctional behavior. The Procrastination Assessment Scale-Students (PASS; Solomon & Rothblum, 1984), the most widely used scale for measuring procrastination in an academic context, is a representative procrastination inventory based on the assumption that procrastination is dysfunctional. It consists of items asking students to report the frequency with which they procrastinate, the extent to which procrastination causes them a problem and their desire to stop procrastination in six specified academic domains; it also includes items designed to elicit reasons for procrastination. Decisional Procrastination Scale (DP; Mann, 1982) is based on the conflict theory of decision making (Janis & Mann, 1977), according to which procrastination is a maladaptive coping behavior (Ferrari, Johnson, & McCown, 1995). The Tuckman Procrastination Scale (TPS; Tuckman, 1991) assesses academic procrastination resulting from inability to self-regulate or control task schedules (Ferrari et al., 1995) is another inventory designed to measure procrastination as a maladaptive behavior (Hensley, 2014). Time Management For students to better manage their curriculum and achieve learning objectives, time management behaviors or skills are argued to improve the positive academic output. The aim of the study is to find the impact of time management on the academic performance of students among the diagnostic radiology technology students at KAU. This study had a cross-sectional survey-based study design. It was conducted among students of diagnostic radiology technology department at King Abdul-Aziz University from Sep. 2020 to Feb. 2021. For this study, 152 students were targeted among which 142 completed the questionnaire, making the response rate of 93.4%. Among 142 participants, 75 (52.8%) were females and 52 (36.6%) were in the 2018 batch. Majority of the students i.e., 107 (75.4%) had GPA between 4.5 – 5 in 2020. Whereas, 37.3% agreed or strongly agreed that they manage their time. Here, 69.2% students with 4.5 – 5 GPA strongly agreed that they meet their deadline (p value = 0.005) and 36.7% students with 4 – 4.5 GPA strongly believed that their academic performance decreased due to mis planning (p value = 0.005). Around 71 (66.3%) students with 4.5 – 5 GPA agreed or strongly agreed to make to do list or calendar (p value 0.047). In conclusion, according to student’s perception, preplanning their studies had been beneficial for their academic performance. However, less than half of the students agreed that they manage their time There are students are expected to perform well with excellence in everything that they do, most especially in the academic aspect. One factor that dictates the quality of performance of a student is his/her punctuality that is, getting to school on time or being tardy. As cited by Nakpodia and Dafiaghor (2011), “lateness” can be defined as the “situation where an individual arrives after the proper, scheduled or usual time (Oxford Advance Learners’ Dictionary, 5 th ed., 1995), Lauby (2009) puts it as a term used to describe “people not showing up on time” and Breeze et al. (2010) contributed by saying that, lateness is synonymous with “tardiness”, which implies being slow to act or slow to respond, thus not meeting up with proper or usual timing. Lastly, Weade (2004) defined tardiness as “being late for any measurable length of time past the stated or scheduled start time for work or school.” Gender As conflicting results for the association between procrastination and some demographic characteristics were consistently found, the potential factors to impact these results have also sparked much interests in current study (Pychyl et al., 2002; Özer et al., 2009; Steel and Ferrari, 2013). Svartdal and Steel (2017) have pioneered the examination and revision for five mainstreaming scales, and indicated the variability of psychometric quality for different measurements (Svartdal and Steel, 2017). In addition to this field, results based by heterogeneous measuring tools were found in elsewhere (Dawis, 2000; Marsh et al., 2013). In this vein, it lead us to assume that the conflicting results may be moderated by different scales. Further, procrastination type is also noteworthy to be an alternative moderator. On the basis of temporal motivation theory, the main factor to promote one postponing off is the inadequate motivations (Steel, 2007). A robust body of studies provided solid evidence to claim the interaction effect of gender and motivation, which demonstrated that female posed high intrinsic motivation than male in academic activities (e.g., reading, L2 learning, Rusillo and Arias, 2004; Kissau, 2006; Hakan and Münire, 2014). Despite no straightforward evidence, such interaction may bring about the specific sexdifferences between academic procrastination and other ones. Lastly, the educational stage stress should be taken into account to explain the heterogeneous results for sex-differences in procrastination. It is well-known to us for the close association between perceived stress and procrastination, with more stresses for stronger procrastination tendency (Stead et al., 2010; Sirois and Tosti, 2012). On the other hand, more stresses were perceived for students in high educational stages than others, and make undergraduates more prone to procrastinate (Pascoe et al., 2020). Given the significant gender-difference (Barnett et al., 1987), we are interested in probing into how the educational stage may influences procrastination and even the interaction of gender and procrastination. Males Procrastinate More Than Females A total of 193 papers (k = 193) were includeincludeed to pool effects for revealing gender differences in both general and academic procrastination, with 102,484 participants [47,901 males (46.73%), 54,583 females (53.27%)]. The findings derived from both Q and I2 tests showed a high level of between-study heterogeneity [Q (192) = 1,266.78, p < 0.001, I2 = 84.84] and thus indicated that the REM is more suitable here. The REM results demonstrated that the procrastination tendency was significantly higher in males than in females (r = 0.042, 95% CI: 0.023–0.056, z = 4.785, p < 0.001) (see Supplementary Table 1 and Figure 2). No publication bias was found in this meta-analysis (Egger’s test t = 0.38, p = 0.70; Begg test tau = 0.02, p = 0.65; fail-safe N = 7,226) (see the funnel plot in Supplementary Figure 1). The results of the modified Newcastle-Ottawa quality control assessment demonstrated good quality for the included studies and showed high scorer reliability based on the Spearman test (Qs = 4.48, r = 0.99, p < 0.001). In particular, this study provided robust statistical evidence that males procrastinate more than females (n = 102,484, k = 193; r = 0.042, 95%, p < 0.001) in both general and academic procrastination, which was consistent with previous investigations suggesting a relationship between them (Pychyl et al., 2000; Steel, 2007; Gröpel and Steel, 2008). There might be promising evidence suggesting a causal role of demographic features (i.e., gender) in procrastination. Males were found to possess a lower level of selfcontrol, which is a key determinant of procrastination (Tewksbury and Higgins, 2006; Ward et al., 2018). As a result, males may tend to procrastinate more due to a lack of goal-directed processing ability and an inability to suppress tempting stimuli (Pychyl et al., 2000; Ferrari, 2001; Steel, 2007; Steel and Klingsieck, 2016). Similarly, males also have a higher level of impulsivity than do females (Cross et al., 2011). A large number of studies have demonstrated that procrastination is positively associated with impulsivity from the behavior, neural variance and behavioral genetics perspectives (Steel and König, 2006; Gustavson et al., 2014; Liu and Feng, 2017), suggesting that males may procrastinate more than females as a result of intrinsic neurobiological factors. Furthermore, a previous meta-analysis found that females score higher on effortful control than males and that effortful control is closely related to procrastination as well (Else-Quest et al., 2006; Lian et al., 2018). This may explain why females procrastinate less than males. On the other hand, existing studies have provided insights into the evolutionary origins of procrastination, suggesting that procrastination can be considered a strategy frequently used throughout life to deal with unpredictable circumstances (Steel, 2007; Chen and Chang, 2016; Chen and Qu, 2017). Compared to females, males were identified to be more sensitive to unpredictable environments, in which they are able to adapt and quickly develop a strategy to succeed using their past experience (Del Giudice, 2009; Jonason et al., 2017). Thus, males’ procrastination tendencies may be stronger than those of females as an evolutionary consequence. Notwithstanding that, the external ecological validity should be mentioned to discuss above results. Notably, there were no any statistical considerations aiming at the ecological validity of this meta-analysis though the substantial heterogeneity for these included studies was found (Andrade, 2018). Despite statistical significance, above explanations to support our findings that the males procrastinate more than females were largely grounded post hoc evidence. There were evidence not enough to validate whether this conclusion could be generalizable elsewhere. Given that, extending this conclusion should be quite careful. On balance, the results demonstrated that males procrastinate more than females, not only because of their low levels of self-control and effortful control and high levels of impulsivity but also because of human evolutionary influences. (Andrade, 2018). Grade Level The procrastination serves as major cumbersome of many students in secondary schools in Nigeria, because they fond of postponing what they are expected culture right time, for instance many students spend much time on social media instead of read their books, even procrastination affect the reading culture among the students in Nigeria. (Adeniyi Adewale, 2019). Therefore, many students prefer to postpone the tasks and do pleasure things which they can not benefit from it .For example, many students fail public examination (WAEC, GCE, NECO) e.t.c yearly because they like to postpone the time to read their books, but they lure to spend all their time on social media .This prevail among the secondary schools students both Urban, Rural and River area in Nigeria. (Adeniyi Adewale, 2019). In all secondary schools in Nigeria there is problem of inability of the students to perform well because many time they are engaged much in social media instead to read their books. it makes the students to perform woefully on their studies, it also contribute to the inability of reading culture. (Adeniyi Adewale, 2019) Academic Performance The attitudes of students are important to be understood toward in their school and learning, learning can be affected by different factors (Candeias, Rebelo, and Oliveira, 2008). Honour students are more likely to perform and participate in non academic activities than non-honour students (Wolfensberger, 2004). The attitudes of students toward school and learning is dependent on their school, school level, age, and parents (Candeias, Rebelo, Oliveira, and Mendes, 2010). Students who are low in their self-esteem but higher interdependent level with others are students who think with positive attitudes toward psychological help (Yeh, 2002). The attitudes of students are related to their academic scores for instance with directly affect their academic performance (Janssen, 2014). Students who hold neutral and positive ones are less verbal than the students who hold negative attitudes, because their attitudes can affect the achievement in academic performances (Schau, 2005). It is good for the students to develop their positive mental attitude while they are still in the early years of college for them to be ready to broaden their responsibility and maturity and to be more confident and independent (Laguador, 2013). The attitudes of the students towards learning in mathematics can improve the student’s achievement (Khun-Inkeeree, Omar-Fauzee, and Othman, 2016). The attitudes of the students have a strong impact in their learning toward in academic performances (Cahill, McDaniel, Frey, Hynes, Recipe, Zhao, and Trousil, 2018). Academic achievement is predicted by attitude in learning (Veresova and Mala, 2016). The cognitive variables of teachers are the most Important than any other, because it can predict the academic performance of the students through their questions it can help the students to think critically (Fehintola, 2014). Perseverance is one the attitudes that considered a strength of courage that reflects to the academic performances of a students (Littman-Ovadia and Lavy, 2015). Motivation, tenacity, trustworthiness, and perseverance are some of the attitudes that can lead the students to success (Heckman and Rubinstein, 2001). Character has an effect in grades and in interpersonal characteristics (Kern and Bowling III, ). Time management is one of the attitudes that is proven variable of success (Renzulli, 2015). Even though effort is related to the attitude of students, it is still fails to be an important in predicting the academic achievement of the students (Li, 2012). Avoidance to trouble is associated to high academic scores (Bowen, Hopson, Rose, and Glennie, 2012). Those students who care likely to gain understanding are the students who talk informally to the faculty members (Komarraju, Musulkin, Bhattacharya, 2010). Motivated or less motivated students can still gain high grade point average (Janssen and O’Brien, 2014). The students who are high achievers are the students who have positive study attitude (Sarwar, Bashir, and Alam, 2010). The academic success depends only in cognitive aspect as well as the emotional abilities (Nasir and Masrur, 2010). Teachers who are passionate can influence the performance of the students (Mart, 2013). The relationship of the students toward in their peers, families, and teachers can affect their achievement on their academic performances (Murray- Harvey, 2010). The instructor’s positive reinforcement can affect their attitudes towards their study habits (Rhoads and deHaan, 2013). The students who decide responsibly achieves more (Greenberg, Weissberg, O’Brien, Zins, Fredericks, Resnik, and Elias, 2003). Students expect for their future as manifested in their positively increased views (Twenge and Campbell, 2008). Health conscious students adapt using strategies which lessen stress (Hamaideh, 2011). Environmental forces conflict with freedom and self-determination (Lefcourt, 2014). The positive and negative feedback of the participants can affect the evaluations of the students towards in their academic performances (Guenther and Alicke, 2007). Students who are actively and positively thinking are less lonely students which may directly correlation with students performances (Wang, Chen, Chen, Li, Harari, Tignor, Zhou, Ben-Zeev, and Campbell, 2014). Grades Grading policies have a direct effect on the grades that students receive, it is extremely important that schools carefully consider what practices best measures students performance (Reeves, 2008). Grading practices have long be a controversial issue among educators and academics. Through grades are accepts as a standard and inherent part of education system, there is some disagreement as to what exactly is the function of grades. There has been much debate over whether grades should be designed to communicate a student’s performance in variety of areas, including behaviour and participation or whether they should just represent a student’s proficiency in a given subject. Some educators have even questioned the value of using grades at all, claiming that using extrinsic rewards to reinforce learning teaches students to care more about their performance on assessment than on what they learn (Edwards, 1999). The grading practices used by many teachers are designed to communicate student’s performance in a number of areas, including both academic achievement and behavioral factors such as student effort, conduct and attitude (Allen, 2005). When teachers assign grades, especially final grades, they are communicating a number of messages to students with single mark. According to Zoeckler (2007), teachers often attempt to communicate message that include; level of expectations, level of academic achievement, encouragement and disappointment. Educators often use grades as both a punishment for bad behaviour and a motivational tool for good behaviour (Wormeli, 2006). However. Some educators now recommend that grades should not be based on behaviour and other non-academic factor, but only on student’s mastery of the material in a given subject. Grading students on what they know and can do, and not on other factors, will help teachers provide students and parents with specific feedback on what learning areas need improvement (Andy, 2011). Grading only on achievement is a key elements of standards-based grading, a practice that is gaining in popularity that focuses solely on student's proficiency on well-defined course objectives (Scriffiny, 2008). Relationship Between Procrastination and the Academic Performance of the students Results from qualitative studies exemplify the negative role of freedom in the study situation in several ways, as too little regulations in studies (Grunschel et al., 2013), low degree of external structure (Klingsieck et al., 2013), or insufficient direction of lecturers (Patrzek et al., 2012). Overall, students reported feeling lost and overwhelmed by the task of planning a whole course of studies, a semester, or even an exam phase on their own. Thus, students lacking self-management skills such as planning and prioritizing tasks (e.g., Lay and Schouwenburg, 1993) and metacognitive learning strategies (e.g., Wolters, 2003; Howell and Watson, 2007) should feel particularly lost when facing a situation with a large degree of freedom. The autonomy associated with a large degree of freedom in the study situation makes the student particularly vulnerable if skills are low (Low focus on study skills training) and if the student fails to develop good habits and routines. Habits help people accomplish more and procrastination. There is a considerable body of empirical research on the relationship between procrastination and performance, particularly academic performance. The results have, however been inconsistent. Researchers have reported negative effects of procrastination on learning and achievement, such as lower grades and course withdrawals (e.g. Aremu et al., 2011, Balkis, 2013). The time pressure resulting from procrastination can reduce accuracy and punctuality, and on this basis it can be argued that procrastination will negatively influence researchers have suggested that the lack of consistency in research on the relationship between procrastination and performance is probably the result of using contaminated self-report data (e.g. Rotenstein et al., 2009, Steel et al., 2001). Previous studies have relied on self-report measures of procrastination, which are only weakly related to external indicators of procrastination (Rotenstein et al., 2009). One study (Steel et al., 2001) reported that the correlation between observed or externally assessed procrastination and selfreported procrastination was 0.35 while the correlation between observed procrastination and course grade was −0.87; the correlation between self-reported procrastination and course grade was only −0.36.(Kyung K. 2015). Various indices of academic performance including self-reported GPA, examination grades, assignment grades etc. have been used to examine the relationship between procrastination and academic performance. Some researchers have reported that the relationship between procrastination and academic performance depends on the choice of performance indicator, for example Tice and Roy (1998) found that the correlation between procrastination and academic performance varied from −.26 to approximately −.66 depending on whether academic performance was indexed using various examination or assignment grades. Jackson, Weiss, Lundquist, and Hooper (2003) found that procrastination, measured using Tuckman’s scale, was negatively correlated with cumulative grade point average (GPA) but was not associated with American College Test score (ACT). We hypothesized that the choice of academic performance indicator would affect the observed relationship between procrastination and academic performance. Other studies have failed to detect an association between procrastination and academic performance (e.g. Seo, 2011, Solomon and Rothblum, 1984) or even reported that procrastination had a positive effect on academic achievement (e.g. Brinthaupt and Shin, 2001, Schraw and Wadkins, 2007). It has been suggested that students of greater ability procrastinate more than those with lower ability (Ferrari, 1991). Ferrari concluded that procrastination tended to increase during the course of a student’s academic career, as learning became more self-regulated.