Exploring the Perceptions and Emotions of U.S. Investors Using Geographical Diversification as an Investment Strategy Dissertation Manuscript Submitted to Northcentral University Graduate Faculty of the School of Business Management and Technology in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY by SAMUEL ANTWI San Diego, California January 2017 ProQuest Number: 10256767 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. ProQuest 10256767 Published by ProQuest LLC (2017 ). Copyright of the Dissertation is held by the Author. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, MI 48106 - 1346 Approval Page Exploring the Perceptions and Emotions of U.S. Investors Using Geographical Diversification as an Investment Strategy By Samuel Antwi Approved by: Chair: Vanessa Claus, Ph.D. Certified by: Dean of School: Peter Bemski, Ph.D. Date ii Abstract The Behavioral Finance Theory suggests that investors’ financial decision-making is influenced by psychological factors. This study was designed to address the problem of investors’ continued use of the modern portfolio theory (MPT) strategy of geographical diversification, as an investment strategy, in spite of the evidence that the strategy is not effective, which results in increased cost, decreased returns, and increased risks. The purpose of this qualitative single case study was to explore U.S. investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding their continued use of geographical diversification as an investment strategy. Ten participants who were at least 18 years old, lived or work in the Washington D.C. Metropolitan area, and diversify their investments across different countries participated in this study. Semi-structured interview questions and journaling were used to collect data from the individual participants. The data was manually coded into themes for analysis. The study revealed that participants had positive emotional feeling when using geographical strategy and thought having positive emotional feelings during investment decision-making would lead to positive investment behavior. Moreover, participants perceived geographical diversification in real estate and oil industries on emerging and growing markets helped increase investment returns and reduce investment risks. The findings of this study are consistent with literature on behavioral finance theory, as pertaining to participants’ perceptions and emotions about the use of geographical diversification as a strategy and psychological biases of the decision making-process. The researcher recommends that investors critically assess their emotional feelings and regulate those emotions during decision-making in order to make beneficial investment decisions. Investors should assess the emerging and growing real iii estate and oil markets before diversifying their portfolios, on such markets, in order to increase returns and reduce risks. The researcher recommends that future quantitative studies should be conducted to evaluate the effectiveness of geographical diversification on the real estate and oil markets of the emerging and growing countries in Africa, Asia, Eastern Europe, and South America. Finally, investors should recognize, analyze, and control their emotional feelings, perceptions, and psychological biases in order to make increasingly beneficial investment decisions. iv Acknowledgements I am grateful to the Almighty God for granting me the wisdom, protection, and strength throughout my schooling. I would like to express my sincere appreciation to my Dissertation Committee Chair, Dr. Vanessa Ann Claus. Without Dr. Claus’ timely support and guidance, this dissertation would not have come into fruition. I would also like to acknowledge the invaluable contributions of Dr. Frank Bearden, the Subject Matter Expert, Dr. Verrill, the Academic Reader, Dr. Joan Saunders, my financial management specialization professor, and all the professors who mentored me. I will be forever indebted to Dr. Stephanie Wallio for her motherly support throughout the dissertation process. I would also like to show my appreciation to my family for their understanding throughout my academic journey. I want to acknowledge the amazing support provided by my son, Samuel Antwi, Jr., my mother, Madam Rebecca Boahemaa, my father, Mr. Paul Antwi of blessed memory, my siblings, and my uncles, Mr. Jonas Owusu-Banahene and Dr. John Kwabena Kwakye. Lastly, I would like to express profound gratitude to my friends and colleagues, especially, Nora Frimpong, Anita Arthur, Gloria Amankwah-Sarpong, and Dr. Christosla Anguelov for their insight and constructive feedback. v Table of Contents Chapter 1: Introduction ....................................................................................................... 1 Background ................................................................................................................... 3 Statement of the Problem .............................................................................................. 5 Purpose of the Study ..................................................................................................... 6 Theoretical/Conceptual Framework Overview ............................................................. 7 Research Questions ..................................................................................................... 10 Nature of the Study ..................................................................................................... 11 Significance of the Study ............................................................................................ 13 Definition of Key Terms ............................................................................................. 15 Summary ..................................................................................................................... 16 Chapter 2: Literature Review ............................................................................................ 18 Theoretical/Conceptual Framework............................................................................ 19 MPT as an Investment Strategy .................................................................................. 21 Understanding Investor Behavior ............................................................................... 32 Problems and Limitations of the Previous Studies ..................................................... 62 Summary ..................................................................................................................... 65 Chapter 3: Research Method ............................................................................................. 68 Research Design.......................................................................................................... 69 Population/Sample ..................................................................................................... 72 Materials/Instrumentation .......................................................................................... 74 Study Procedures ....................................................................................................... 79 Data Collection and Analysis...................................................................................... 81 Assumptions................................................................................................................ 84 Limitations .................................................................................................................. 85 Delimitations ............................................................................................................... 86 Ethical Assurances ...................................................................................................... 86 Summary ..................................................................................................................... 88 Chapter 4: Findings ........................................................................................................... 91 Trustworthiness of Data……………………………………………………………...91 Results ......................................................................................................................... 92 Evaluation of Findings .............................................................................................. 108 Summary ................................................................................................................... 118 Chapter 5: Implications, Recommendations, and Conclusions ...................................... 121 Implications............................................................................................................... 122 Recommendations for application ............................................................................ 140 vi Recommendations for future research ...................................................................... 143 Conclusions ................................................................................................................ 144 References ....................................................................................................................... 147 Appendixes ..................................................................................................................... 161 Appendix A: Recruitment Notice on Social Media Network Websites ......................... 162 Appendix B: Recruitment and Screening Email Response............................................. 163 Appendix C: Informed Consent ...................................................................................... 164 Appendix D: Participation Confirmation Email ............................................................. 167 Appendix E: Email Response to Unqualified Applicant ................................................ 168 Appendix F: Participation Waiting List Email ............................................................... 169 Appendix G: Participation not Needed due to Data Saturation Email............................ 170 Appendix H: Interview Questions .................................................................................. 171 Appendix I: Journaling Instructions................................................................................ 173 Appendix J: Journal Template ........................................................................................ 174 Appendix K: Member Validation of Recorded Response Email .................................... 175 vii List of Tables Table 1 Theme Surmised from Processing and Analysis of the ResearchQuestion 1…....93 Table 2 Theme Surmised from Processing and Analysis of the Research Question 2…...97 Table 3 Theme Surmised from Processing and Analysis of the Research Question 3….101 Table 4 Theme Surmised from Processing and Analysis of the Research Question 4….105 viii 1 Chapter 1: Introduction According to Delcoure (2010), Hyoyoun and Wook (2013), and Resnik (2010), diversification of financial assets across different geographical areas remains one of the most common approaches employed by financial investors and financial corporations to maximize investment returns and reduce financial investments risks. The modern portfolio theory (MPT) assumes that financial investors geographically diversify their financial assets to maximize returns and minimize risk (Markowitz, 1959; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010; Resnik, 2010). According to Hyoyoun and Wook (2013), the MPT approach of financial investment cannot be completely dismissed, despite a lack of empirical support that the geographical diversification strategy is effective in increasing returns and reducing risks, which is because financial investors widely utilized the geographical diversification strategy to manage their investments (Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013). A study conducted to assess the performance of diversified investment assets in five European Union countries, Denmark, France, Germany, Spain, and the United Kingdom, showed a decrease in investment returns of geographically diversified assets compared to domestically invested assets (Bobillo, Iturriaga, & Gaite, 2008). Another study found that when investors geographically diversified their financial assets, there was an increase in the unsystematic and total risks of the financial investments (Gocmen, 2010). Unsystematic risks are unplanned investment risks that are unique to the investment location and not usually common among all investments (Gocmen, 2010). Examples of unsystematic risks include a strike by employees of a company, drastic change in management, or political instability (Gocmen, 2010). Total risks are combination of planned and unplanned investment risks such as changes in interest rate, 2 number of stocks, and political instability (Gocmen, 2010). According to Chu-Sheng (2010), the use of geographical diversification was not effective in maximizing returns and minimizing risks in the Japanese financial market due to foreign exchange cost, risks and transaction costs. Similarly, Cai, Xu, and Zeng (2016) cautioned investors that while geographical diversification may improve investment returns, the increasing cost of operating investment across different countries should be taken into consideration. Understanding how investors make financial decisions is the main interest of financial researchers. For many decades, financial researchers have utilized conventional finance theory, which assumes that financial investors make rational investment decisions (Markowitz, 1959; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010; Resnik, 2010). However, a phenomenon has developed in which financial investors make irrational investment decisions, contrary to the conventional finance theory’s assumption that all investors make rational investing decisions based on accessible information. For example, U.S. investors continue to use geographical diversification when empirical evidence shows that the strategy is not effective (Raju & Khanapuri, 2010; Ritter, 2003; Shefrin, 2000, 2007, 2013). The conventional finance theory has not been able to fully explain why some investors make irrational investment decisions, describing this as market anomalies (Ritter, 2003; Shefrin, 2000, 2007, 2013). The behavioral finance theory assumes that people make decisions based on reallife choices rather than optimal choices and therefore takes into consideration the investors’ perceptions and emotions in making investment decisions (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). Investors’ behaviors have prompted the conventional finance theory advocates to call the behavior irrational, while advocates of behavioral finance theory 3 describe the behavior as investors utilizing real-life perceptions and emotions in decisionmaking (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Ritter, 2003; Shefrin, 2000, 2007, 2013). The behavioral finance theory has not been applied to understand human factors behind U.S. investors’ continued use of a geographical diversification strategy to maximize returns and minimize risks (Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). Background Historically, investors utilized the MPT strategy of geographical diversification to increase investment returns and decrease investment risks (Markowitz, 1959; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010; Resnik, 2010). Numerous research studies support the use of the geographical diversification strategy to improve investment outcomes and reduce risks (Hargis & Mei, 2006; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010; Meric, Jie, & Meric, 2016; Mimouni, Charfeddine, & Al-Azzam, 2016; Odier & Solnik, 1993; Saiti, Bacha, & Masih, 2014; Solink, 1974; Torres García-Heras, 2011). Studies compared the performance of domestically diversified products with geographically diversified portfolios regarding increasing investment returns and decreasing risks. For example, Masron and Fereidouni (2010) examined portfolio diversification benefits of the housing industry and the relationship between the housing performance and inflation. Masron and Fereidouni (2010) found that diversification in the housing industry provided more benefits than risks and that investment in the housing industry resulted in lowest risk-to-reward ratio. In another study, Meriç, Jie, and Meriç (2016) examined the performance of diversified portfolios of investors from the United States, Canada, Germany, the United Kingdom, and France, as related to their investments within Asian, African, and Middle Eastern markets. Meric et al. (2016) found that that the U.S. investors could increase their 4 investment returns and reduce their risks by diversifying on the Indonesian, Philippine, Malaysian, and Thai emerging stock markets. Internationally diversified investments reduced risks and increased investment outcomes better than the domestically diversified investments (Hargis & Mei, 2006; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010; Mimouni et al., 2016; Odier & Solnik, 1993; Saiti, Bacha, & Masih, 2014; Solink, 1974; Torres García-Heras, 2011). Contrary to this, not all studies found these beneficial effects of geographical diversification strategy. Other findings showed that the strategy did not increase returns and the investment risk did not reduce (Bobillo et al., 2008; Cai, Xu, & Zeng, 2016; ChuSheng, 2010; Gocmen, 2010; Maldonado & Saunders, 1981; Raju & Khanapuri, 2010; Singh, Kumar, & Pandey, 2010). The findings that the geographical diversification strategy was not effective in increasing returns and reducing risks indicates that the beneficial effects of the strategy had either significantly been reduced or totally eliminated (Raju & Khanapuri, 2010; Singh, Kumar, & Pandey, 2010). The researchers attributed the ineffectiveness of the strategy to the globalization of the financial markets over time. For many years, researchers assumed that financial investors made rational investment decisions (Markowitz, 1959; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010). However, this assumption could not explain investor behavior that appeared irrational. To better understand investors’ behavior in investment strategy, several studies that explored investors’ decisions found that their decisions were affected by their emotions and perceptions (Baker, Coval, & Stein, 2007; Duxbury, 2015; Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Lee & Andrade, 2015; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). A study conducted by Baker, Coval, and Stein (2007) found that 5 approximately 80% of investors’ decisions were influenced by their emotions and perceptions. Financial investors continue to use the geographical diversification strategy to increase returns and reduce risks (Markowitz, 1959; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010). Despite evidence of the ineffectiveness of the geographical diversification strategy, U.S. investors’ continued use of the strategy raises interest among researchers. Without changing investors’ behaviors, increased risks and decreased investment returns may result (Bobillo et al., 2008; Gocmen, 2010). Statement of the Problem Research shows that investors’ continue to use the MPT strategy of geographical diversification as an investment strategy despite evidence that it is not effective, which results in increased cost, decreased returns, and increased risks (Bobillo et al., 2008; Cai et al., 2016; Chu-Sheng, 2010; Gocmen, 2010). Development in understanding of investors’ behaviors indicates that investors do not always make rational financial decisions, contrary to conventional finance theory (Duxbury, 2015; Garcia, 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013; Reuter, 2009; Shefrin, 2007, 2013; Thaler, 2005; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). Investors’ continued use of the MPT approach suggests their investment decisions are influenced by cognitive factors such as perceptions and emotions, which may have positive or negative impacts on their investment returns, consistent with behavioral finance theory (Duxbury, 2015; Garcia, 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). The interconnection between investors’ perceptions and emotions and financial decisions-making, in order to understand or change investors’ behaviors, information is needed regarding why they continue to use geographical diversification as an investment strategy (Dow, 2011). The behavioral finance theory has not yet been specifically applied 6 to U.S. investors’ geographical diversification strategy. Since the behavioral finance theory tells us that investors are swayed by their perceptions and emotions, description of the specific emotions and perceptions that U.S. investors’ experience related to using geographical diversification strategy is needed in order to ultimately change use of this ineffective approach (Garcia, 2013; Miccolis & Goodman, 2012). Without changing the use of MPT through better understanding of investors’ perceptions, investors will continue to use a strategy that results in decreased investment returns (Bobillo, Iturriaga, & Gaite, 2008) and increased investment risks (Gocmen, 2010). Lack of this information may result in decreased returns and increased risk on geographically diversified investments (Bobillo et al., 2008; Gocmen, 2010). Purpose of the Study The purpose of this qualitative single case study was to explore U.S. financial investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding their continued use of geographical diversification as an investment strategy to increase investment returns and decrease investment risks, when empirical evidence does not support the use of the strategy. The primary data were obtained using open-ended interviews in-person or by telephone. For the purpose of data triangulation, secondary data were gathered through self-reported journaling during investment decision-making in line with qualitative case study’s best practices (Yin, 2013). Participants were at least 18 years old, diversified their financial investments geographically, and worked or resided in the Washington DC Metropolitan Area. In order to allow for attrition and data saturation, 14 participants were purposely selected but 10 participants were included in the case study (Stake, 1995). The responses obtained from participants were presented using tables and direct quote results (Yin, 7 2003, 2013). The effectiveness of geographical diversification as a strategy is not supported by evidence (Bobillo et al., 2008; Cai et al., 2016; Chu-Sheng, 2010; Gocmen, 2010), while the investors’ investing behaviors such as psychological bias as well as the influence of the investors’ perceptions and emotions on their investment decisions are well documented (Duxbury, 2015; Garcia, 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013; Reuter, 2009; Shefrin, 2007, 2013; Thaler, 2005; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). Therefore, the findings of this study contributed to research literature in the general areas of information on MPT, diversification strategy, and emotions and perceptions in investors as well as the prospects to educate investors targeting those thoughts and feelings about making smart investment choices. Theoretical/Conceptual Framework Overview Kahneman and Tversky (1979) published an article that led to behavioral finance theory, which provides a qualitative understanding of financial investment decisionmaking based on potential gains and losses rather than the actual outcome. Behavioral finance theory takes into consideration investors’ perceptions of and emotions about investment gains and losses, while the conventional finance theory does not consider these factors (Baker et al., 2007; Sewell, 2007). Behavioral finance investors make their financial investment decisions based on their emotions and perceptions rather than empirical evidence (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Ritter, 2003). Behavioral financial researchers have shown that experienced financial investors and financial managers repeatedly make financial decisions that do not conform to conventional financial investment pattern (Reuter, 2009; Shefrin, 2007, 2013). Approximately 80% of individual financial investors make financial investment decisions consistent with 8 behavioral finance theory, which is a departure from logical financial investments (Baker et al., 2007). Behavioral financial theorists believe that investors will react differently to the potential of gaining 50% on an investment and losing 25% on the same investment compared to the potential of gaining 25% on an investment without risk due to perceptions, emotions, and social biases (Kahneman, 2003; Kapor, 2014; Shefrin, 2007, 2013; Thaler, 2005). This behavior suggests that financial investors do not make perfect investment decisions due to perceptions and emotions, thus anthropology and psychology play significant roles in developing and understanding behavioral finance theory (Kahneman, 2003; Shefrin, 2007, 2013; Shiller, 2003; Thaler, 2005). Numerous studies have supported behavioral finance theory and shown that when investors made investment decisions based on perceptions and emotions, there were financial consequences such as decreased investment returns and increased investment risks (Chaarlas & Lawrence, 2012; Mitroi, 2013). One study, which applied behavior finance theory, investigated how investors made investing decisions found that when investors based their investing decisions on emotions and perceptions, their returns were lower and risks were higher (Mitroi, 2013). Other researchers used behavior finance theory to survey 519 investors in India and found that 81% of participants demonstrated anchoring biases, wherein investors refused to modify their use of ineffective investing strategies based on their emotions and perceptions (Chaarlas & Lawrence, 2012). In Croatia, Učkar and Carlin (2011) found that investors who made investing decisions based on their perceptions, consistent with behavior finance theory, incurred heavy shortterm losses. Učkar and Carlin (2011) suggested further research was needed to assess the 9 long-term impact of behavioral finance on the capital market. While training on behavioral finance theory helped increase the awareness of financial managers about biases in financial investing decision-making, training in behavior finance alone was not effective in changing their investment decision-making behaviors (Nikiforow, 2010). A recent problem has developed in that individual U.S. financial investors’ behaviors are not consistent with rational finance behaviors, as they are adhering to a strategy of geographical diversification despite evidence suggesting it is not effective, indicating irrational financial decisions (Gocmen, 2010; Latif, Arshad, Fatima, & Farooq, 2011). This situation, where U.S. financial investors’ behaviors are not consistent with increasing returns and reducing risks, calls for an extension of behavioral finance theory to the strategy of geographical diversification because whenever investors make less than optimal investment decisions, perhaps based on beliefs, perceptions, and emotions, precious resources are being wasted (Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Mitroi, 2013; Mitroi & Oproiu, 2014). While behavioral finance theory identifies the existence and importance of investor emotions and perceptions in their decision making, it does not identify specific emotions and perceptions which are influential within specific investing situations, such as geographical diversification (Geambasu, Sova, Jianu, & Geambasu, 2013). Geographical diversification has not improved actual investment outcomes and limited research exists to provide insight into the investors’ investment perceptions and emotions about this strategy (Chu-Sheng, 2010; Taffler & Tuckett, 2007). The proposed study will extend theory through application of the theory to a new investing context, use of geographical diversification strategy despite recent evidence it is not effective (Chu- 10 Sheng, 2010; Taffler & Tuckett, 2007), as well as identifying the specific emotions and perceptions that are driving investor behavior in this context. The financial crisis in 2008 shows the MPT strategy to geographical diversification was not only ineffective in maximizing return and minimizing risks, but also contributed to the financial crisis because of the impact of the U.S. financial market on other geographical financial markets (Chu-Sheng, 2010; Gocmen, 2010). While the MPT strategy still advocates for geographical diversification, the theory has not done much to understand and incorporate the perceptions and emotions of geographical diversification investors (Geambasu et al., 2013). A deeper understanding of the U.S. investors’ emotions and perceptions related to the geographical diversification strategy is needed to change the trend of this irrational and ineffective investing strategy through efforts targeted specifically at relevant emotions and perceptions (Chu-Sheng, 2010; Taffler & Tuckett, 2007). Failure to conduct this study may lead to the investors’ continuous use of geographical diversification strategy, which results in decreased returns and increased risks because the strategy is practically not effective in maximizing returns and minimizing risks of geographically diversified investments (Chu-Sheng, 2010; Geambasu et al., 2013). Therefore, this study will contribute to the behavioral finance theory by providing insight into the perceptions and emotions of investors’ who continue to use geographical diversification strategy. Research Questions This case study was designed around the central question of this study to explore why U.S. investors continue to use the geographical diversification strategy when empirical evidence indicates the strategy is not effective in maximizing returns and 11 minimizing risks. The central question was answered by using four open-ended questions to collect data from participants in persons or via phone through open-ended interviews questions as follows: Q1. How do U.S. investors describe their emotions about using geographical diversification as an investment strategy? Q2. How do U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for increasing investment returns? Q3. How do U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for reducing investment risks? Q4. How do U.S. investors explain their use of geographical diversification strategy, in the context of literature, which does not support the strategy? Nature of the Study This study was designed to explore U.S. investors continued use of the MPT, as an investment strategy, which is now considered ineffective (Bobillo et al., 2008; Cai et al., 2016; Chu-Sheng, 2010; Gocmen, 2010; Maldonado & Saunders, 1981; Raju & Khanapuri, 2010; Singh, Kumar, & Pandey, 2010), but investors continue to use the strategy due to psychological factors (Duxbury, 2015; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Paruchuri & Misangy, 2015; Ritter, 2003; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). Investors make their financial investment decisions based on their emotions and perceptions rather than empirical evidence (Duxbury, 2015; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Ritter, 2003; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015), which is problematic due to the potential that decision leading to increased investment risks and decreased investment returns may result (Bobillo et al., 2008; Gocmen, 2010). 12 To achieve the purpose of the study, a qualitative single case study design was used to obtain in depth information through interviews and participants’ journal entries on their perceptions and emotions about using the strategy at a particular point in time (Patton, 2002; Stake, 1995; Yin, 2003). To collect complex information for this study, as compared to quantitative designs, this design was most appropriate for exploring the U.S. investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding their continued use of the geographical diversification as an investment strategy (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005; Kvale & Brinkmann, 2009). Hence, this qualitative design helped to obtain the information needed to better understand the complexity of investors’ perceptions and emotions related to using the investment strategy. The study population consisted of 10 U.S. investors who were at least 18 years of age, diversified their investment across different countries, and lived or worked in the Washington DC Metropolitan Area. These participants were purposefully recruited for the study via posting recruitment notice on social networks such as Academia.edu, Facebook, LinkedIn, Meetup, and Meettheboss. Additionally, snowballing sampling was utilized to recruit some of the participants when the existing participants shared recruitment notice with new potential participants who were recruited and screened using the inclusion criteria (Draper & Swift, 2011). For the purpose of data attrition and saturation, 14 participants were recruited for the study; however, after attrition, 10 participants were included in the study. Using purposeful sampling and snowballing sampling to recruit participants ensured that appropriate data were gathered to address the problem (Draper & Swift, 2011). 13 Utilizing semi-structured interview questions and journaling, qualitative data was collected for the study. The interviews were conducted either in person or via phone depending the participant’s preference. The interview questions and journaling prompts aligned with the research questions to explore investors’ perceptions and emotions related to using geographical diversification as an investment strategy. To enhance quality of the data collection, two individuals known to me who met inclusion criteria reviewed the interview questions and journal prompts through a field-tested. This was to make sure the questions were interpreted as the researcher planned in order to obtain the appropriate information from participants and address researcher bias and interview validity (Shenton, 2004; Turner, 2010). The participants’ responses obtained through interviews and journaling were transcribed into password protected Microsoft Word documents to ensure confidentiality and security (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005; Kvale & Brinkmann, 2009). Data collection was stopped as soon as data saturation was achieved. Data saturation occurred after the tenth participant completed the interviews and journal entries and no new themes were being discovered, thus it was confirmed that data saturation was achieved with the tenth participant. Data were manually coded and analyzed using themeing. The data analysis was included in the final report after reflection of the themeing. Significance of the Study This study was significant because financial investors could apply the outcomes of the study in their investment decision making while financial researchers and students will better understand investors’ behavior through examining the study results. The investors’ investment behaviors needed to be explored due to their continued use of 14 geographical diversification strategy when evidence does not support its use. Research suggest that if investors’ perceptions and emotions can explain their use of the diversification strategy, then the investors’ decision making is consistent with behavioral finance (Duxbury, 2015; Garcia, 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013), extending application of the finance theory to this particular investment context. According to Dow (2011), information is needed to better understand investors due to the interrelationship between investors’ decision-making process and their perceptions and emotions. Specific perceptions and emotions that influence explicit investment strategies such as geographical diversification have not been identified (Dow, 2011). The findings of this study will also contribute to research literature in the general areas of information on MPT, diversification strategy, and emotions and perceptions in investors. Financial students and researchers may use the results of this study to enhance their knowledge about the investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding the investors continued use of the geographical diversification strategy. This study’s findings could contribute to improving financial investment practices. Financial investors not only do not make perfect investment decisions but also their investment decisions are influenced by psychological factors such as perceptions and emotions during investment decision making (Kahneman, 2003; Shefrin, 2007, 2013; Shiller, 2003; Thaler, 2005). Without changing investors’ behaviors increased risks and decreased investment returns may result (Bobillo et al., 2008; Gocmen, 2010). The information obtained from the description of the investors’ perceptions and emotions related to using the diversification strategy may be used to educate investors through targeting thoughts and feelings that contribute to making smart investment choices. 15 Definition of Key Terms Considering the fact that this study explores the perceptions and emotions of U.S. investors’ use of geographical diversification, as an investment strategy, it is important that some terms used in the study, which may have different meanings, as used in other research studies, were defined in this section. Behavior Finance. Behavior finance is the approach to financial investment decision where the psychological factors such as perceptions, emotions, and bias of the investors are factored into their investment decision making (Kahneman, 2003; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). Conventional Finance Theory. Conventional finance theory is the approach to financial investment where all financial investors are assumed to make rational investment decisions to maximize returns and minimize risk (Markowitz, 1959; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010). Financial Literature. Financial literature is financial information obtained by participants from formal sources such as business news, trading journals, business and articles, and books (Ateş, Coşkun, Şahin, & Demircan, 2016; Gonzalez-Perez, 2015). Geographical Diversification. Geographical diversification is an approach to investment in which investors invest their portfolios across different geographical locations, in different foreign countries, with the aim of maximizing returns and minimizing risks (Delcoure, 2010). Modern Portfolio Theory (MPT). Modern portfolio theory describes an approach to financial investment where all investors are considered to be rational 16 investors and therefore will diversify their portfolios to maximize returns and minimize risks (Markowitz, 1959). Summary This study was designed to examine the problem of investors’ continued use of the MPT strategy of geographical diversification as an investment strategy in spite of the evidence that the strategy is not effective, which results in increased cost, decreased returns, and increased risks (Bobillo et al., 2008; Chu-Sheng, 2010; Gocmen, 2010). The investor’s investment behavior of the continued use of the geographical diversification strategy, to increase returns and reduce risks decision, needs to be further explored (Garcia, 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). This behavior suggests that investors do not always make rational decisions and that their decisions are influenced by their emotions and perceptions, consistent with behavioral finance theory (Garcia, 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013; Reuter, 2009; Shefrin, 2007, 2013; Thaler, 2005). The purpose of this qualitative single case study was to understand U.S. investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding their continued use of geographical diversification as an investment strategy. The research questions addressed the purpose of this study and are related to investors’ emotions during decision-making, perceptions of the strategy to increase returns, perceptions of the strategy to decrease risk, and continued use of diversification strategy when evidence does not support its use. A sample of 14 U.S. investors, who diversify their financial investments across different countries, were purposefully selected to participate in the study. Ten participants were eventually included in the case study after attrition of participants’ responses. Qualitative data was collected via semi-structured interview questions in person or via phone and journaling. 17 The qualitative data obtained through interviews and journaling were transcribed into password protected Microsoft Word documents, manually coded, and analyzed using themeing. This study contributes to knowledge and literature by extending application of behavioral finance theory to the MPT, geographical diversification strategy. Researchers and students may use the findings of this study to enhance their knowledge on the investors’ perceptions and emotions related to continued use of an ineffective strategy. Additionally, information from this study regarding investors’ perceptions and emotions of using the diversification strategy may be used to develop teaching tools for investors, focusing on those thoughts and feelings about making prudent investment decisions. 18 Chapter 2: Literature Review The purpose of this qualitative single case study was to understand U.S. investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding their continued use of the geographical diversification as an investment strategy. The information in the literature review can be used to further understand the U.S. investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding the use of the strategy as it relates to behavioral finance. The purpose of this literature review was to provide the contextual framework, as related to the problem statement and research questions, this study seeks to explore and answer. The literature review will focus on U.S. investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding the use of geographical diversification, as an investment strategy, as it relates to behavioral finance. The subsections of the literature review will focus on the background of modern portfolio theory (MPT), MPT as an investment strategy, understanding investor behavior, influence of emotions on investors’ financial decision-making, influence of perception on investors’ financial decision-making, influence of psychological bias on investors’ financial decision-making, problems and limitations of previous studies, and summary of the literature review. In the course of reviewing the existing literature, EBSCOhost, ProQuest, and Sage journals online databases from Northcentral University (NCU) were searched. The key terms that were used during the searches included behavioral finance theory, conventional finance theory, geographical diversification strategy, investment strategy, international diversification strategy, and modern portfolio theory. Additional search phrases included effects of investors’ emotions on financial investment decisions, effects of investors’ perceptions on financial investment decisions, how emotions influence 19 investment decisions, how perceptions influence investment decisions, impacts of investors’ emotions on financial investment decisions, impacts of investors’ perceptions on financial investment decisions, and how emotions and perceptions influence investment decisions. These phrases were examined to help obtain the information needed to understand the U.S. investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding the use of the geographical diversification strategy, as it relates to behavioral finance. Scholarly and peer-reviewed articles and journals that addressed the key search terms were the central focus of the literature review. Additionally, incorporated into the review were books ranging from conventional finance through investment to behavioral finance. Articles and journals from 1948 to 2016, which were scholarly and peer-reviewed, were examined to ensure that the information has stood the test of time. Theoretical/Conceptual Framework Historically, conventional finance advocates have argued that financial investors are rational and financial information provided by these investors is adequate (Friedman & Savage, 1948). Supporters of the conventional finance theory believed that the financial market was efficient and that financial decisions accurately reflected the available information based on mathematical calculations (Friedman & Savage, 1948). However, the Great Depression’s financial downturn prompted people to think beyond the assumption that investors are rational and that their financial investment decisions are rational (Markowitz, 1952). Prior to the financial crisis during the Great Depression in the 1930s, on the basis of mathematical calculations, investors invested their financial assets in one stock with potential for high returns (Markowitz, 1959). However, after the Great Depression, a graduate student, Harry Markowitz, realized investors were not 20 diversifying their financial assets and did not consider investment risk when making investment decisions. The lack of diversity among investors’ portfolios coupled with their disregard for investment risk prompted Markowitz to publish a journal entitled “Portfolio Selection” in 1952 (Markowitz, 1959). Markowitz (1959) proposed that investors should carefully select their investment portfolios through diversification to maximize returns and reduce risk. Markowitz (1959) argued that the strategy of diversifying investment portfolios may lead to increased returns on investment and reduced risk. Markowitz’s work on portfolio diversification led to the development and popularization of modern portfolio theory (MPT). The MPT employs a diversification strategy in financial investments to increase returns on investments while reducing investment losses (Markowitz, 1959). Investors have applied the MPT strategy of financial investment over several decades. For example, Solnik (1974) conducted a seminal study that examined geographical diversification of stocks from U.S., U.K., France, Germany, Italy, Belgium, Netherlands, Switzerland markets. Solnik found that investments that were diversified internationally provided greater risk reduction and increased returns better than the domestically diversified investments. Similarly, Meriç, Jie, and Meriç (2016) examined the performance of diversified portfolios in Asia and Africa. The researchers investigated the performance of investors in the United States, Canada, Germany, the United Kingdom, and France on the Indonesian, Philippine, Malaysian, and Thai emerging stock markets, as well as the Jordanian, Moroccan, Egyptian, and Pakistani emerging stock markets. Meric et al. (2016) found that the investors from the U.S., Canada, Germany, the United 21 Kingdom, and France had the greatest portfolio diversification benefit on investments in the Indonesian, Philippine, Malaysian, and Thai emerging stock markets. Between the 1960s and 1990s, the MPT approach was the cornerstone strategy of the financial investment market (Konstantinidis, Katarachia, Borovas, & Voutsa, 2012). However, the 2008 financial crisis challenged both the MPT and conventional finance theory (Hommes & Wagener, 2009). For example, from October 2007 to May 2009, as the 2008 financial crisis heightened, the Dow Jones Industrial Average dropped by 50% (Smith & Harvey, 2011). The nature and effect of the 2008 financial and economic downturn demonstrated that financial markets’ outcomes may be influenced not only by rational financial investing behavior, as would be predicted by conventional finance theory, but also seemingly irrational financial investing behavior (Hommes & Wagener, 2009). Additionally, the impact of the 2008 financial crisis posed a challenge to the MPT’s strategy that the use of geographical diversification of financial assets might maximize profit and reduce loss (Baker, Wurgler, & Yuan, 2012). Such a drastic decline in the returns from geographical diversification in the financial stock markets and investors’ continued use of the strategy called for a comprehensive understanding of investors’ strategies and behaviors (Hommes & Wagener, 2009). MPT as an Investment Strategy Historically, research supported the MPT strategy that the use of geographical diversification as a strategy may lead to increased investment returns and decreased investment risks. Solnik (1974) conducted a seminal study that compared investments, which were diversified domestically to those investments, which were diversified across different geographical areas. Portfolios containing an increasing number of stocks from 22 U.S., U.K., France, Germany, Italy, Belgium, Netherlands, Switzerland, and international locations were collected and analyzed. Solnik found that investments that were diversified internationally provided greater risk reduction and increased returns better than the domestically diversified investments. For example, when the size of the domestic portfolio increased beyond 20 stocks, a relatively small reduction in investment risk and increase in returns occurred. On the contrary, a substantial reduction in investment risk and increase in returns occurred when the size of the international portfolios increased beyond 20 stocks. To understand the influence of international diversification on investment outcomes, Odier and Solnik (1993) conducted a study on benefits and risks of geographical diversification among British, German, Japanese, and U.S. investors. Data obtained from the international financial markets for the analysis ranged from 1970 to 1990. The findings showed low correlations between the international financial markets. The outcomes demonstrated that geographically diversified investments were more beneficial and reduced risks. Odier and Solink’s results were consistent with the findings of Solink (1974). Interestingly, the results showed that investors who diversified their portfolios on the Japanese financial markets realized the most financial benefits from international diversification. Many years later, Hargis and Mei (2006) explored whether financial assets that were diversified internationally or across different industries yielded the most benefits. The authors analyzed data from different industries and across different countries. The data consisted of discount rate, cash flow, and interest returns spanning from 1987 to 1999. Results of Hargis and Mei (2006) show that the correlations between investment 23 diversification outcomes across different countries were low while the correlations between diversification in different industries were high. Hargis and Mei’s findings indicated that financial portfolios that were diversified geographically provided more benefits than industrial diversification. The international diversification results were consistent with the findings of Odier and Solnik (1993) and Solink (1974), which asserted that geographically diversified investments provided benefits to financial investors. However, the non-beneficial diversification across different industries raised interest among researchers. In examining the benefits of global portfolio diversification, on the emerging stock markets, Meriç, Jie, and Meriç (2016) examined the performance of diversified portfolios. The researchers investigated the performance investors of the U.S., Canadian, German, U.K., and French on the Indonesian, Philippine, Malaysian, and Thai emerging stock markets and the Jordanian, Moroccan, Egyptian and Pakistani emerging stock markets. Meric et al. (2016) found that the investors of the US, Canadian, German, U.K., and French had the greatest portfolio diversification benefit on investments in the Indonesian, Philippine, Malaysian, and Thai emerging stock markets. The results showed that the U.S. investors could increase their investment returns and reduce their risks by diversifying on the Indonesian, Philippine, Malaysian, and Thai emerging stock markets. Meric et al.’s (2016) findings were consistent with findings from of Hargis and Mei (2006), Odier and Solnik (1993), and Solink (1974), which showed that geographical diversification is effective in increasing returns and reducing risks. Using the modern portfolio theory (MPT) as an investment strategy, Masron and Fereidouni (2010) examined the effectiveness of diversification on performance 24 outcomes of investment in the housing industry in Iran. The data obtained from the Central Bank of Iran, Ministry of Housing and Urban Development, and Tehran Stock Exchange from 1993 to 2008 was examined. The correlational analysis was used to examine the portfolio diversification benefits of the housing industry and the relationship between the housing performance and inflation while the risk-to-reward ratio was employed to examine the risk adjusted performance of housing and other financial assets (Masron & Fereidouni, 2010). The results showed that diversification in the housing industry provided more benefits than risks and investment in the housing industry resulted in lowest risk-to-reward ratio. The returns on housing investment exceeded rate of inflation and there was a positive and significant relationship between housing returns and rate of inflation (Masron & Fereidouni, 2010). The authors recommended future study to explore whether these results would apply to other sectors of the market (Masron & Fereidouni, 2010). Masron and Fereidouni’s findings were consistent with the findings of Hargis and Mei (2006), Odier and Solnik (1993), and Solink (1974), which demonstrated that geographically diversified portfolios improved returns on investment and reduced risks. In in the wake of 2007-2008 global financial crisis (GFC), Haran et al. (2016) sought to improve the transparency of European emerging real estate market dynamics and performance by examining the extent and nature of inter-relationships between three emerging real estate markets namely, the Czech Republic, Hungary, and Poland. In addition, the authors sought to determine the rationale and effectiveness of emerging real estate markets within a Pan-European investment portfolio. Haran et al. (2016) found that there was lack of relationship (uniformity) among the markets. The findings indicate that 25 geographical diversification on those European emerging real estate markets was effective in terms of both performance enhancement and risk diversification. Haran et al.’s findings were in alignment with the findings of Hargis and Mei (2006), Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Odier and Solnik (1993), and Solink (1974), which showed that geographically diversified portfolios improved returns on investment and reduced risks. To assess the diversification benefits from Islamic investment, during the financial turmoil, Saiti, Bacha, and Masih, (2014) examined whether the Islamic stock indices provided special avenue for the U.S.-based investors. Using the recentlydeveloped Dynamic Multivariate GARCH approach and Dynamic Conditional Correlation (DCC), the authors examined both the conventional and Islamic MSCI indices of Japan, GCC ex-Saudi, Indonesia, Malaysia and Taiwan provide better diversification benefits compared to Korea, Hong Kong, China, and Turkey. The findings showed that the investment in Islamic countries provided better diversification benefits of increasing returns and reducing risks compared to the Far East countries. Saiti et al.’s findings were consistent with the findings from Haran et al. (2016), Hargis and Mei (2006), Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Odier and Solnik (1993), and Solink (1974), which showed that geographically diversified portfolios improved returns on investment and reduced risks. The research published by Haran et al. (2016), Odier and Solnik (1993), and Solink (1974) has stood the test of time by demonstrating that the geographical diversification strategies help to increase returns and reduce risks. To assess evidence of the existence of diversification benefits in international stock markets, Mimouni, Charfeddine, and Al-Azzam (2016) included and examined the emerging oil producing countries in a global portfolio. Mimouni et al. (2016) explored 26 whether recent oil shocks and financial events have significant impact on the conditional correlations and diversification benefits. Results showed the correlation in Gulf Corporation Council (GCC) oil-producing countries (e.g., Saudi Arabia, Kuwait, the United Arab Emirates, Qatar, Bahrain, and Oman) stock markets remained low and constantly offered high diversification benefits. The findings show that geographical diversification on the emerging and oil-producing countries offered more potential for international diversification to increase investment returns and reduce investment risks. The authors findings are consistent with the findings of Haran et al. (2016), Hargis and Mei (2006), Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Odier and Solnik (1993), Saiti et al. (2014), and Solink (1974), which demonstrated that geographically diversified portfolios improved returns on investment and reduced risks. In another study, Torres García-Heras (2011) examined the effectiveness of using geographical diversification to reduce risk by analyzing the credit default swap (CDS) markets in US, France and Germany, PIIS (e.g., Portugal, Ireland, Italy, Greece and Spain), and Mexico and South America (e.g., Argentina, Brazil, Chile, Columbia and Peru). The data obtained compared the corporate CDS and national sovereign CDS. The results showed the South America countries experienced lower risk with diversified investments than most developed European countries, especially Spain. While highly diversified countries demonstrated lower investment risks, investments that were diversified in corporate bonds were safer than sovereign bonds in Spain (Torres GarcíaHeras, 2011). To examine return and volatility linkages among equity markets, Yavas and Dedi (2016) investigated the linkages among equity returns and transmission of volatilities in 27 the following countries: Germany, Austria, Poland, Russia, and Turkey. Multivariate Autoregressive Moving Averages (MARMA) and the Generalized Autoregressive Conditional Heteroskedasticity (GARCH) were used to analyze the data. Yavas and Dedi (2016) found that there was significant correlation of performance of investment returns among countries under consideration. Results showed that the Turkish and Russian markets were exposed to more volatility than Austria, Germany and Poland markets. The findings indicated that geographical diversification on Austria, Germany, and Poland’s markets had less risk and could be used to reduce investment risks. Yavas and Dedi’s findings were consistent with Torres García-Heras’ (2011) findings that geographical diversification was effective in reducing investment risks. In order to understand the influence of globalization of the financial markets, on geographical diversification strategy, Srivastava (2007) investigated the co-integration among seven Asian financial markets and U.S. financial markets. Data was obtained from U.S. and Japan financial markets in one category and India, Indonesia, Hong Kong, Korea, Malaysia, Singapore, and Taiwan’s financial markets in another. The data collected from the various stock markets spanned from September of 1997 to June of 2006. Srivastava found that the correlation between Asian financial markets and U.S. financial markets was low which implied that when the U.S investors diversified their investment on the Asian markets, their investment returns increased and their risks reduced. The findings showed that the growth in globalizations has both benefits and drawbacks. In short-term investments, the benefits of investment returns outweigh the investment risks. This finding shows that geographical diversification relies increasingly on investment in emerging markets. However, due to the interdependence of the global 28 financial markets, the benefit of geographical diversification strategy has decreased. This decrease is due to the financial activities of the emerging markets being dependent upon the developed countries’ financial markets. Related to this, not all research has found these benefits of the geographical diversification strategy. A study by Cheng and Roulac (2007) examined the effectiveness of geographical diversification in improving outcomes of real estate investments. Quarterly published data from NCREIF property indices was selected for the analysis. The dataset of 244 sub-indices was collected from first quarter of 1993 to fourth quarter of 2004 for the study. Cheng and Roulac (2007) found that the effectiveness of geographical diversification, in real estate investment, is limited and in order to eliminate diversifiable risk, the real estate investments should be held in a large number of properties. The results showed that the effectiveness of the geographical diversification strategy may vary depending upon the type of property under consideration. For example, Cheng and Roulac (2007) found that the effectiveness of geographical diversification in real estate investment is limited to the size of the investment. Additionally, Haran el al. (2016) found that investors who diversified in emerging real estate markets of the Czech Republic, Hungary, and Poland had potential to enhance their investment performances. Other research conducted by Maldonado and Saunders (1981) investigated the performance of portfolios, which were diversified domestically and across different geographical areas. From the point of view of United States investors over different time horizons, a United States stock index and four foreign stock market indices, Japan, Germany, Canada, and the United Kingdom, were examined. The index stock data was derived from stock indices published in the International Monetary Fund's international 29 financial statistics because the dataset has provided the foundation for most previous empirical studies of international diversification. Maldonado and Saunders found that there was no significant difference between the performance of domestically diversified investments and internationally diversified investments. This result showed that the geographical diversification strategy does not increase returns while reducing investment risk as previously thought. The findings were consistent with Bobillo et al. (2008), ChuSheng (2010), and Gocmen (2010), which showed that the geographical diversification strategy did not increase returns nor did it decrease investment risks, but contradicted findings by Solnik (1974), Srivastava (2007), and others. With the introduction of Euro currency on the European financial markets and to explore its effects on portfolio diversification, Kashefi (2006) investigated the influence of the Euro currency on financial investment diversification on the Euro countries and U.S. investors. From the European, Australia, and Far East (EAFE), U.S., and European financial markets, the data obtained from financial market indices ranging from 1993 to 2003 were analyzed. Kashefi’s findings showed that with the introduction of the Euro currency among the Euro member countries, correlations of investment outcomes and diversification among the financial markets increased. In addition to the increased correlation among the European markets, the correlations among the EAFE, U.S., and European financial markets also increased. The increase in the correlation among the financial markets showed that the benefits of international diversification have significantly reduced, which makes geographical diversification less attractive. This increase in correlation among the financial markets means that the investments diversified on the EAFE, U.S., and European financial markets depend on one another 30 and thus the financial markets are exposed to similar investment returns and risks (Kashefi 2006). This correlation among the markets suggests that when investors diversified their investments across different financial markets, their investment returns and risks did not change (Kashefi, 2006). Kashefi’s findings were consistent with the findings of Chu-Sheng (2010), Gocmen (2010), and Maldonado and Saunders (1981), but contradicted Srivastava (2007), Solnik (1974), and others. In this age of globalization, the performance of a financial market on one continent has influence on another financial market on a completely different continent. In order to assess the influence of globalization on the financial markets, Singh, Kumar, and Pandey (2010) examined how diversified investments in Asia, Europe, and North America performed with regards to interdependence and influence of one market on the other. The authors examined how price and volatility spillovers influenced diversified financial investments across North American, European, and Asian stock markets. The return spillover is modeled via a vector autoregressive (VAR 15) model in which fifteen global indices represent their respective stock market. Same day return of the spillover is also analyzed using a VAR and autoregressive (AR) model with exogenous variables. In incorporating the same day effect, the volatility spillover is modeled through autoregressive -generalized autoregressive conditional heteroskedasticity (GARCH). The AR, GARCH, and VAR models are financial and economic analytical models used to analyze data. Singh et al. (2010) found that there was a great interdependency and influences of one market on another such that there was no trend of increased returns or decreased risks of the geographically diversified investments. These findings were consistent with Maldonado 31 and Saunders (1981) but contradicted the findings of Solnik (1974) and Srivastava (2007). A similar study to Singh et al. (2010) and Maldonado and Saunders’ (1981) studies was conducted by Raju and Khanapuri (2010), which examined the performance of diversified financial investments in six Asian markets from U.S. investors’ perspectives. Between January 1, 1998 and December 31, 2008, the authors collected daily returns data of the composite stock market indices of U.S. (S&P 500), South Korea (KOSPI), China (Shanghai Composite, SSEC), Malaysia (KLSE Composite), Indonesia (QKSE Composite), Thailand (SET), and India (NSE Nifiy). The findings showed that investments that were diversified in the Asian financial markets did not increase returns nor did the diversification reduce risks. While previous high investment returns in the Asian financial markets have attracted foreign investors, the interdependence of international markets has made the geographical diversification strategy ineffective on the emerging Asian markets (Raju & Khanapuri, 2010). Raju and Khanapuri’s findings were consistent Maldonado and Saunders (1981) and Singh et al. (2010), but contradicted the findings of specifically Solnik (1974) and Srivastava (2007). In exploring the impact of geographical diversification on the Chinese banks, Cai, Xu, and Zeng (2016) examined financial corporations in the Asian market. The findings showed that geographical diversification expansion helped improved market shares and net interest shares. Cai et al. (2016) found that investors incurred higher operating costs as the level of diversification increases. Cai et al.’s (2016) results suggest a potential tradeoff between economic gains (e.g., market shares or interest margins) and operating costs due to expansion of investments across different countries. Moreover, geographical 32 diversification has a positive but insignificant impact on return on asset (ROA). Cai et al.’s findings were consistent with Maldonado and Saunders (1981), Raju and Khanapuri (2010), and Singh et al.’s (2010) findings that geographical diversification strategy was not effective in increasing returns and reducing risks. Cai et al. (2016) cautioned investors to carefully analyze and conduct cost-benefit analysis before deciding on the strategy due to the high cost of operating geographical diversification investments. The use of geographical diversification as an investment strategy has mixed outcomes from financial researchers and practitioners. While some research by Hargis and Mei (2006), Markowitz (1959), Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Odier and Solnik (1993), Solnik (1974), Srivastava (2007), and Torres García-Heras (2011) supported geographical diversification as an effective investment strategy, the majority of research by Bobillo et al. (2008), Cai et al. (2016), Chu-Sheng (2010), Gocmen (2010), Maldonado and Saunders (1981), Raju and Khanapuri (2010), and Singh, Kumar, and Pandey (2010) did not support the strategy. The differences among advocates of the strategies and challenges associated with internationalization of financial markets have prompted interest in exploring and understanding investors’ behaviors with regards to using a strategy that does not have complete empirical support. Understanding Investor Behavior For many decades, financial researchers have applied conventional finance theory to explain how investors make decisions, which assumes that financial investors make rational investment decisions (Markowitz, 1959; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010). Supporters of conventional finance theory have for many years held the view that all financial investors are rational and value investment portfolios rationally and that any deviation 33 from the rational investment decision-making corrects itself (Cotugno & Stefanelli, 2012). The globalization of financial markets has shown that the benefits of geographical diversification strategy have diminished as a result of the interdependence of global financial markets (Gocmen, 2010; Maldonado & Saunders 1981; Raju & Khanapuri, 2010; Singh et al., 2010). Investors continued use of geographical diversification strategy, without empirical support, has raised interest about thought process of the investors during investment decisions (Baker, Wurgler, & Yuan, 2012). Additionally, investors’ continued behaviors of making financial investment decisions based on perceptions and emotions influenced by psychological bias has deepened researchers’ interests in behavioral finance (Garcia, 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013; Reuter, 2009; Shefrin, 2007, 2013; Thaler, 2005). Studies have revealed that financial investors’ investment decisions do not always conform to the conventional finance theory of financial investment. Advocates of behavioral finance believe that the conventional finance theory has not adequately explored the influence of psychological factors on investors’ financial decision making processes (Fenzel & Pelzmann, 2012). Complex economic models, investors’ limited understanding of financial information, ever changing financial markets, and the influence of psychological factors on investors’ decisions make conventional finance theory more untenable (Fenzel & Pelzmann, 2012; Shiller, 2003). Researchers developed behavioral finance theory, which consider how psychological factors influence investors’ decision making process when conventional finance theories fail to address the investors’ behavior and market anomalies (Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012). Advocates of behavioral finance theory consider how various 34 psychological characteristics such as anchoring biases, emotions, and perceptions influence financial decision-making processes of investors and financial planners (Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). Researchers of behavioral finance further argued that conventional finance theory ignores the humanistic behavior of financial investors and managers as it relates to investment decision-making (Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). Investors’ humanistic behaviors are important to researchers because understanding these behaviors helps researchers explore and develop effective strategies for financial investors. Acknowledging, understanding, and modifying the behavioral biases of the financial investors and controlling such biases caused the investors to make profitable and sound investment decisions and thereby become better investors (Suresh, 2013). In other study, Nofsinger (2005) examined his students and the investment club members using mental accounting. Mental accounting or psychological accounting is when investors analyze, group, and evaluate potential financial outcomes before they make investing decisions (Nofsinger, 2005). Participants were told to estimate the risks of adding new individual stocks to the existing investment. Results showed that undergraduate students, graduate students, and members of the investment club considered the individual stock with high risks without estimating the entire portfolio’s risks. Investors should analyze the new portfolios in addition with the existing investments before making investing decisions (Nofsinger, 2005). This finding shows cognition limitations among investors when analyzing financial information. For example, due to cognitive limitations, investors were not able to accurately analyze the risk of both new and existing investment portfolios before making financial decisions. 35 The psychological aspects of investors and the complexities in fast-changing financial investment markets have made it difficult for financial investors to rely on complicated mathematical derivation to make sound investment decisions (Chandra, 2009). While the globalization of the financial markets may have made financial information readily available to investors, the psychological complexities of processing voluminous information to make sound financial decisions is an arduous task for the investors (Agrrawal & Borgman, 2010; Rosillo, De la Fuente, & Burgos, 2013). Investors’ emotions, perceptions, and biases may be intensified when they are encountered with an overwhelmingly large amount of financial information to make investment decisions. Investors make less than optimal investment decisions when their decisions are influenced by emotions, perceptions, and psychological bias (Chandra, 2009). Psychological factors such as emotions, perceptions, and bias play a significant role during financial investment decisions (Webber & Johnson, 2009). Advocates of behavioral finance theory consider investors’ investment behavior as normal behavior even when financial investors’ decisions result in decreased investment returns and increased investment risks (Taffler & Tuckett, 2010). Even neoclassical economist Keynes, in Keynes (1964), observed that investors’ investment decisions were not usually based on classical mathematical formulations. Instead, the investors’ decisions showed pattern of psychological influences that sometimes resulted in less profit and increased risks (Keynes, 1964). This is an indication that the investors’ decisions are influenced by psychological factors such as perceptions, emotions, and bias. Keynes (1964) observation that investors’ investment decisions were not usually based on classical mathematical formulations was later supported by behavioral finance economists, including Akerlof 36 and Shiller (2010) and Kahneman (2011). According to research, financial investors have a tendency to make investment decisions based on emotions, perceptions, and bias, which can result in less than optimal outcomes (Kahneman, 2011). Influence of emotions on investors’ financial decision-making. Numerous researchers have investigated how emotions influence investors’ decision-making processes (Lazarus & Lazarus, 1994). Emotions influence people in every aspect of their lives including investment decisions. Theories of emotion can help explain how emotions impact investor behavior. Lazarus and Lazarus (1994) posited that emotion and decision making based on that emotion occur simultaneously. The importance of simultaneous development of emotions and decision-making ability is routed in the human survival instinct (Lazarus & Lazarus, 1994). For example, when a human being recognizes a snake, the person may experience heart palpitations. The development of heart palpitations indicates the immediacy of danger from seeing the snake, and the resulting emotional feelings interpret the danger so that the person will run away. When stock prices significantly rise or fall, investors experience emotions (Lazarus & Lazarus, 1994). An increase in portfolio prices would result in positive emotional feelings, while a decrease in stock prices results in negative emotional feelings. Similar to the individual seeing a snake, the physiological reaction and associated emotion is interpreted and leads to action (Lazarus & Lazarus, 1994). The positive emotional feelings may motivate investors to further invest in a portfolio while negative emotional feelings may discourage investors from investing in the portfolios. Researchers have investigated how peoples’ emotions influence the way they process information. Positive emotional feelings may indicate that the information has 37 been thoroughly processed and the environment is conducive for decision-making, while negative emotional feelings may suggest that more information is needed to make sound judgment (Schwarz, 2002). Some of the emotional experiences that affect peoples’ decisions include sadness, happiness, anxiety, fear, pride, shy, joy, or sorrow, among others (Schwarz, 2002). Investors, like the rest of the population, experience emotions during decisions-making processes. Emotional feelings, negative or positive, may influence the investors’ financial decisions (Reyes, 2006). When investors’ financial decisions are negatively influenced by their emotions, they make less optimal investment decisions. On the contrary, investors make beneficial decisions when their decisions are positively influenced by their emotions (Reyes, 2006). A study conducted by Shiv, Loewenstein, Bechara, Damasio, and Damasio (2005) examined how brain activities related to emotions (e.g., sadness, anxiety, obsession, lack of self-control, and self-denial) influence the number of investment decisions that investors make. The findings showed that patients with brain lesions related to emotions made 20 more rounds of investment decisions than patients with brain lesions unrelated to emotions. The application of these findings to all financial investors implies that brain activities relating to emotions such as sadness, anxiety, obsession, lack of self-control, and self-denial, which are common among both emotional investors and patients with brain lesions, affect how investors make investment decisions. In relating the findings to financial investors’ decision making, investors who made investment decisions when they were depressed, anxious, obsessed with investment, lacked self-control, or exhibited self-denial made more irrational investment decisions, which resulted in financial losses. In addition, Shiv et al. (2005) showed that when participants with brain lesions unrelated 38 to emotions began to lose money, those investors traded less compared to participants with brain lesion related to emotions. In relating the findings to all financial investors’ decision making, and consistent with behaviors of investors who based their financial decisions on emotions, Shiv et al. showed how emotions could cause investors to make unsound investment decisions. In exploring the influence of emotions on investors’ decision-making, MyeongGu and Barrett (2007) examined emotions of 101 stock investors making investment decisions over a period of 20 consecutive days. The high and low emotional feelings corresponded to being happy and sad, respectively. Contrary to the general perception that feelings are generally bad for decision making, the authors found that stocks of financial investors who demonstrated high feelings during investment decision-making performed better than investors’ who demonstrated low emotional feelings (Myeong-Gu & Barrett, 2007). This result indicated that when investors have high feelings, they were capable of controlling their biases better than those who had low feelings and subsequently made profitable investment decisions (Myeong-Gu & Barrett, 2007). Like Myeong-Gu and Barrett, Gambetti and Giusberti (2012) examined investors’ emotions such as anger and anxiety during investment decision-making. The findings showed that investors who were emotionally angry made medium risk investment decisions while investors who demonstrated anxiety made low risk investment decisions. Gambetti and Giusberti findings were consistent with the findings of Myeong-Gu and Barrett (2007). In investigating whether mental pitfalls, due to emotions, caused financial investors to make good or bad decisions, Sullivan (2011) studied financial investors’ emotions when making investment decisions. Good investment decisions mean investors 39 made decisions that led to increased investment returns and reduced investment risks. Bad investment decisions mean investors made investment decisions that resulted in reduced profit and increased investment risks. Sullivan (2007) found that investors were distracted by their emotions, which prevented them from making good financial investment decisions. The results suggested that investors’ emotional feelings influenced their investment decisions (Sullivan, 2007). Sillivan’s (2007) findings are consistent with Gambetti and Giusberti (2012) and Myeong-Gu and Barrett’s (2007) findings that investors who had happy and excited emotions made good investment decisions. Conversely, investors who showed emotions of sadness and depression made irrational decisions that resulted in losses (Sullivan, 2011). In another study, in order to investigate the influence of emotions on investment decision-making, Chu, Im, and Jang (2012) examined the effects of investors’ emotions such as pride and shame on their investment decision-making. The findings showed that investors who demonstrated high pride traded greater volume of investments. This was because investors with pride disregarded selfcontrol since responding to losses and modifying trading behaviors created the impression of shame among the investors. Investors who showed pride made unguarded investment decisions that resulted in losses. Conversely, investors who demonstrated the emotions of shame made rational decisions through careful deliberation, which resulted in increased returns. To explore the influence of emotions on investment decision-making in London, Fenton-O'Creevy, Soane, Nicholson, and Willman (2011) investigated how regulated emotions and unregulated emotions influenced financial investors’ decisions in four investment banks in the City of London, an area within London. In regulating emotions, 40 financial investors critically analyzed their emotions and financial information available to them before making investment decisions. On the contrary, in unregulated emotions, the investments decisions were made without thoroughly evaluating their emotions. Semi-structured interviews were used to collect data from 118 investors who either engaged in financial products (e.g., bonds, stocks, or derivatives) trading, property trading, or both financial products trading and property trading in addition to 10 senior financial managers. Fenton-O'Creevy et al. (2011) found that investment returns of investors who used antecedent-focused emotional regulation performed better than those who employed response-focused strategy. Investors who used antecedent-focused regulation critically analyzed their emotions and financial information available to them before making investment decisions, thus making rational investment decisions. Conversely, the investments of investors who relied primarily on response-focused regulation without evaluating their emotions performed poorly. This showed that when investors carefully analyzed their emotions and financial information, they were able to make rational investment decisions. In assessing the influence of emotions on investors’ decisions to undertake direct foreign investment (DFI), Van de Laar and de Neubourg (2006) analyzed the influence of emotion and economic variables on Dutch investors who made DFI in Central and European markets. Emotions were included in a utility maximization model that considered not only the utility of the firm but also the utility of the individual decisionmaker. The findings showed that when the Dutch financial investors had positive emotions, investors increased volume of DFI transactions in the Central and Eastern Europe. Consistent with Gambetti and Giusberti (2012) and Myeong-Gu and Barrett 41 (2007), Van de Laar and de Neubourg’s findings showed that the variations of financial investors’ emotions influenced their investment decision-making positively or negatively depending upon their mood at the time of making investment decisions. Investors’ emotions and financial decision-making are integrated and interdependent on one another (Paulus & Yu, 2012). The investors’ emotional feelings in fast-changing financial times make investment decisions difficult for financial investors (Paulus & Yu, 2012). In order to understand how financial product price fluctuations may influence emotional changes among financial investors Lo and Repin (2002) conducted a study on financial professionals. A total of 10 financial investors were observed for emotional factors, such as sweating and palpitation during financial investment decisionmaking. Results showed that financial investors demonstrated increased sweating and palpitation when the price fluctuations were unstable. Lo and Repin’s findings showed that when investors were making difficult financial investment decisions, investors’ emotions significantly influenced their decisions. Lo and Repin’s (2002) findings were consistent with Lee and Andrade (2015) and Reye’s (2006) findings that investors’ psychological factors influenced their investment decisions. To understand the influence of emotions on decision-making, in an audit-type environment, Blay, Kadous, and Sawers (2012) examined how undergraduate students’ negative or positive emotional feelings influenced their behaviors in searching for financial information. Blay et al. (2012) investigated how participants’ emotional feelings about and levels of investment risks influenced their decision to search for more or less financial information. Blay et al.’s (2012) findings showed that when the risk was high, participants with negative emotional feelings demonstrated higher strategic search 42 behaviors compared to when the risk was lower. On the contrary, students who showed positive emotional feelings showed lower strategic search behaviors when the risk was low. The results show that people’s emotions, whether positive or negative, influenced their decision-making. Blay et al.’s (2012) findings are consistent with Reyes’ (2006) findings that investors’ emotional feelings influenced their financial investment decisions. Behavior financial researchers suggest investors’ psychological behaviors may influence their decisions (Sahi, Arora, & Dhameja, 2013). In an explanatory design, Sahi, Arora, and Dhameja (2013) investigated how investors’ psychological behaviors influenced their investment decisions. In-depth semi-structured interviews were used to collect data from 30 participants, who were residents in New Delhi, India. The qualitative data collected was analyzed using the open analysis technique. The authors found that investors’ emotions and psychological biases influenced their decision making. For example, when participants’ emotional feelings about the investments were stronger, they made increased and beneficial investment decisions. On the contrary, when the investors’ emotional feelings about the portfolios’ performances were weak, they showed disinterest in making investment decisions. Interestingly, participants relied on information they were familiar when making investment decisions instead of critically analyzing available information. Psychological motives such as fear and greed influenced investment decisions (Sahi et al., 2013). Respondents, who demonstrated fear during investing decisions, made fewer but more beneficial investment decisions. On the contrary, investors who showed greed during investment decisions traded more but made less beneficial investment decisions. This demonstrates a pattern of psychological behavior 43 with regards to feeling about the portfolios’ performances when making decisions. Sahi et al.’s (2013) findings were consistent with Blay et al.’s (2012) and Reyes’ (2006) findings that investors’ emotions influenced their financial investment decision making process. Psychological factors such as depression and paranoid may influence decision making process (Patterson & Daigler, 2014). To explore the influence of psychological factors on participants’ decisions making, Patterson and Daigler (2014) surveyed 222 finance students. Using the S&P Trading Standard, in a thirteen-week stimulation investment experiment, participants were assessed based on their risk taking behaviors. The results showed that participants demonstrated a higher degree of paranoid traits than average people. Participants diversified their portfolios in different investments but also engaged in higher than average risk investments. The findings are important for investors and financial managers to understand that self-monitoring during investment decision making may help investors reduce taking higher risk while improving their returns on investments. When investors’ decisions are predominated by paranoia characteristics, they are likely to make less than optimal investment decisions (Patterson & Daigler, 2014). Consistent with Blay et al.’s (2012) findings, Patterson and Daigler’s (2014) findings showed that financial investors’ emotions influenced their investments decisions. Investment outcomes may influence investors’ psychological emotional feelings. Haocheng, Jian, Limin, and Shuyi (2014) investigated the association between investment outcomes and investors’ positive or negative emotional feelings. From six universities, 115 undergraduate students were selected through a survey for the experimental investment. Findings showed that participants demonstrated positive 44 emotional feelings when the price of their investment portfolios went up. On the contrary, when prices of investment products fell, participants showed negative emotional feelings (Haocheng, Jian, Limin, & Shuyi, 2014). The findings reveal that when price on stocks go up, returns on investments increased and investors make profits on their invested assets. This finding subsequently results in participants showing positive emotional feelings. Conversely, investors would demonstrate negative emotional feelings when they incurred losses as a result of a falling stock price. The Haocheng et al.’s (2014) finding supports a cyclical nature of emotions, in that investor’s decisions are influenced by their emotional status and then new emotions are generated by the outcome of those decisions, influencing the new set of decisions. Decision making is influenced by many psychological factors such as fear. In a laboratory experiment, Lee and Andrade (2015) examined the effects of fears and excitement about investment value on participants’ decision making in terms of risk taking. In a stimulated investment environment, some of the participants’ experience of fear was induced while other participants’ experience of fear was not manipulated. Results showed that participants who had fear induced made less risky decisions compared to participants whose experience of fear was not induced. On the contrary, participants made risky decisions when the experimental environment was changed to a casino game, participants with induced fear states made more risky decisions while participants in the control group’s decisions remain unchanged. The findings by Lee and Andrade (2015) explained that when participants were excited about the increase in value of their investment portfolios, investors made more risky investment decisions. On the contrary, when participants were fearful about the value of their investment portfolios, 45 investors made fewer risky investment decisions (Lee & Andrade, 2015). Lee and Andrade’s findings are consistent with Reyes (2006) and Haocheng et al.’s (2014) findings, which display that investors’ emotional feelings influenced their financial investment decisions. Numerous studies suggest that emotions play a role in investor behavior (Chu et al., 2012; Myeong-Gu & Barrett, 2007; Shiv et al., 2005; Van de Laar & de Neubourg, 2006). Positive versus negative emotions appear to have various impacts on investor behavior (Myeong-Gu & Barrett, 2007). Even different specific emotions, for example, anger versus anxiety can lead to different investing behavior (Gambetti and Giusberti, 2012; Myeong-Gu & Barrett, 2007). Some research suggests that positive emotions lead to increased beneficial investing behavior (Gambetti and Giusberti, 2012; Myeong-Gu & Barrett, 2007). Other research has suggested negative emotions lead to more rational investing behavior (Chu et al., 2012; Van de Laar & de Neubourg, 2006). It is important for investors to be aware of and to factor the status of their emotional state during investing decisions. Investors’ emotions differ from their perceptions because while investors’ emotions refer to feelings which affect their behaviors during investing decisions, investors’ perceptions, on the other hand, refer to their ability to become aware of and process information through their senses during investing decisions (Gambetti & Giusberti, 2012; Wang, Keller, & Siegrist, 2011). Investors’ perceptions of benefits versus risks in the financial investment markets have impact on their investing behaviors, in addition to emotions (Učkar & Carlin, 2011). Influence of perception on investors financial decision-making. Researchers have conducted numerous investigations to understand the influence of perceptions on 46 investors’ investment decision making (Wang, Keller, & Siegrist, 2011). Adequate financial information and familiarity with financial products, as well as perception of risk, may influence investors’ decision-making. In order to understand how investors’ perception of product risk and familiarity with product influenced their decisions, Wang, Keller, and Siegrist (2011) examined investors in Switzerland. Using the psychometric paradigm, the researchers surveyed 1249 participants from German-speaking Switzerland and included 20 financial products in the study and examined how investors’ perceptions of risks influenced their investment decision-making. Psychometric paradigm is an analytical tool that is used to assess how non-experts perceive risk when making decisions (Wang, Keller, & Siegrist, 2011). While 52% of the participants did not hold portfolios, 45% held financial investments, and 3% refused to answer the question of whether or not they have financial portfolios. Results showed that financial investors’ perceptions of low risk products were financial investments that were easy to understand. On the contrary, when investors perceived information on financial products as complicated, such financial products were rated as high risk. Wang et al.’s (2011) findings showed some trend of anchoring bias, such that investors were open to only familiar information and products when making investment decisions. Investors traded more in familiar financial products that they understood better because they considered those familiar products less risky than the unfamiliar financial products (Wang, Keller, & Siegrist, 2011). Consistent with Jing, Chen, and Zhang’s (2013) findings, Wang et al.’s (2011) findings showed that investors’ perceptions of familiarity with financial products influenced their financial decisions. 47 Psychological factors and perceptions may influence decision making processes in corporations. To explore how psychological factors and perceptions affected investors’ venture capital value and their decisions, Jing, Chen, and Zhang (2013) studied venture capitalists and entrepreneurs and their investment decisions. The authors investigated the influence of moderate confidence and overconfidence on investors’ decision making. Findings showed that participants with moderate confidence made sound investment decisions. This means venture capitalist and entrepreneurs, who demonstrated moderate confidence, made decisions that resulted in increased investment value. On the contrary, participants who were overconfident made bad investment decisions, which resulted in reduced investment value. The outcomes of this study showed that when individuals making decisions have some confidence, the perception of confidence motivates people to make beneficial decisions. However, investors who demonstrate overconfidence may make more risky decisions that may subsequently result in increased investment risks and reduced portfolio returns. Jing et al.’s (2013) findings were consistent with Wang et al.’s (2011) findings that investors’ perceptions influenced their financial decision making process. Since risk is part of financial investment, investors consider several factors when making financial investment decisions (Virlics, 2013). Some researchers have argued that the more financial information that is available to investors, the better their chances of making beneficial investment decisions (Virlics, 2013). In order to explore the influence of financial information aggregation on investment decisions with regards to risk taking, Kaufmann and Webber (2013) examined financial investors’ behaviors in an experiment. Information aggregation is a phenomenon in which data are gathered and analyzed in 48 order to make informed decisions (Kaufmann & Webber, 2013). In a higher aggregation of information, a large volume of data are collected and analyzed to make decisions while in a lower aggregation of information, a small volume of information is gathered and analyzed to aid a financial decision process (Kaufmann & Webber, 2013). In a hypothetical investment situation, participants in the control group received risky and non-risky portfolios while respondents in the experimental group received information on assets with and without investment risks. Results showed that when participants received higher aggregation of information, investors made greater risk investment decisions. On the contrary, when low aggregation of financial information was made available to investors, they made less risky investment decisions. When investors’ perception of investment risk is low, investors make higher risk investment decisions (Kaufmann & Webber, 2013). The implication of the findings is that financial investors should analyze information on returns and risks in small forms rather than in aggregate. Investors have the tendency of exaggerating expected returns while underestimating risk investments when financial information is evaluated in higher aggregate. This is because aggregated financial information may lead the investors to make high-risk investment decisions. These results display how cognitive limitations in managing financial information influenced investors’ perceptions because they were not able to accurately interpret higher aggregate information during decision making. Understanding of how investors’ subjective perceptions of financial companies influence their financial decision-making is of interest to behavioral finance researchers. To examine whether investors’ perceptions of financial products and brands of companies affected their decision making, Aspara (2013) investigated investors from the 49 Helsinki Stock Exchange, Finland. The author surveyed and examined 292 investors with stocks in the automobile, gardening, and sport equipment companies. The results showed when the investors’ highly regarded a financial product, the investors developed familiarity with the product, which increased their preparedness to invest in those companies. Investors’ familiarity of companies’ brands alone did not influence their decision to invest in those companies. However, the investors’ perceptions of the companies’ products increased their expectations of higher returns and subsequently their optimism to invest in those companies. Aspara’s findings provided clarification to Jing, Chen, and Zhang’s (2013) and Wang, Keller, and Siegrist’s (2011) findings that it is not just the investors’ familiarity with company/brand that matters. It was the investors’ perception of the product that mattered (Aspara, 2013). To understand the influence on investors’ behavior based on perceptions on the stock market, Učkar and Carlin (2011) examined investors of the Croatian stock market. An equilibrium model of the stock price movements and their respective fundamental value was created to analyze the stock market data. The results showed that investors who made investing decisions based on their perceptions, consistent with behavior finance theory, incurred heavy short-term and medium-term losses. On the contrary, longterm stock prices tend to circle round the fundamental equilibrium with larger deviations from the point of equilibrium. This means investors who critically analyzed financial information and made decisions based on available financial information, improved their returns on investments and reduced investment risks. When investors make financial decisions based on their perceptions, less than optimal decisions were made, which caused reduced profit and increased risk that subsequently reduced returns on 50 investments. The findings showed investors’ understanding of macroeconomics and ambiguous financial markets and their influence not only on the financial sector but also to the real economy may help in making good investment decisions. The findings indicate that investors should analyze both the financial information and the information about real economy (e.g., actual production of goods and services) in order to make beneficial investment decisions. The authors suggested further research was needed to assess the long-term impact of behavioral finance on the capital market. Učkar and Carlin’s (2011) findings were consistent with Wang et al.’s (2011) findings, which showed investors who made investment decisions based on their perceptions, rather than empirical results, incurred heavy short-term losses. To determine how perception of weakness in material control would influence investors’ decision-making, Rose, Norman and Rose (2010) examined 97 nonprofessional investors in a first study, 53 nonprofessional investors in a second research study, and 47 investors and 28 directors of Fortune 500 firms in a third survey. Financial material control weakness arises when there are inaccuracies in the internal financial reporting that is available to investors. As a result of the inadequate information available to investors and deficiency in the internal reporting, when there are inaccuracies in the company’s financial reporting, investors cannot promptly detect the inaccuracies and the company cannot promptly prevent financial crises. The findings showed that investors increased their investment risk assessment when their perception of material weakness was more pervasive and they had distrust in management. Rose et al.’s (2010) findings were consistent with Wang, Keller, and Siegrist’s (2011) findings that investors’ perceptions of investment risks influenced their investment decision making process. 51 Financial crisis may affect investors’ decision-making based on how they perceive and tolerate investment risks. In order to understand this effect, Roszkowski and Davey (2010) examined the influence of current economic and financial crises on financial investors’ perceptions of investment risks and tolerance for investment risks and their subsequent investment decision making. Specifically, the authors studied investors’ investing behavior and if the 2008 financial crisis lowered the investors’ risk tolerance by collecting data on their risk tolerance pre-and post-crisis inception. Using monthly averages of risk tolerance scores, data from FinaMetrica between January of 2007 and June of 2009 was collected and analyzed. Roszkowski and Davey found that before the financial crisis, investors’ perceptions of risk were low and risk tolerance was high. However, after the financial crisis, investors’ perceptions of risk elevated from low to high and their risk tolerance changed from high to low. Consistent with Rose et al.’s (2010) findings, investors’ perceptions of risks influenced their tolerance for investment risk and subsequently their investment decision making. In a related study, to understand investors’ investment behavior, Hoffmann, Post, and Pennings (2013) investigated how the financial crisis from 2008-2009 influenced investors’ perceptions of trading and risk-taking behaviors, by surveying investors in the Tehran city of Iran. Between April 2008 and March 2009, the authors surveyed a panel of brokers via email to answer survey questions online. The survey elicited information on investors’ expectations of stock-market returns, risk tolerance, and risk perceptions for each upcoming month. Results showed that 1,510 clients answered at least one questionnaire, with an average of 539 clients answering each month, and a minimum of 296 in any given month. Hoffman et al. (2013) showed that during the financial crisis, 52 investors’ tolerance for risks and expectations for investment returns decreased significantly while their perceptions of investment risks rose. For example, during the worst months of the crisis, investors’ return expectations and risk tolerance decreased, while their risk perceptions increased. Hoffman et al.’s findings were in line with Roszkowski and Davey (2010) findings of investors’ reduced tolerance for investment risk during financial and economic crisis. In examining the stability of the financial markets, Harras and Sornette (2011) explored the psychological factors that influenced the market bubble. The findings showed that whenever investors have perceptions of positive feedback about investments, investors invest more of their financial portfolios. On the contrary, when the investors perceived negative feedback about investments, investors did not invest their financial assets in those investments (Harras & Somette, 2011). Consistent with Rose et al. (2010), the results demonstrated how investors’ perceptions of positive or negative feedback on investments influenced how much they invest their financial portfolios. The influence of investors’ perceptions of financial corporations’ performance or regulatory risk on investors’ decision-making is of importance to researchers and financial analysts. Chassot, Hampl, and Wustenhagen (2014) investigated how investors’ perceptions of regulatory exposure affected how investors make their financial investment decisions in the renewable energy industry. Across the U.S. and European financial markets, 29 investors who invested their portfolios in venture capital were surveyed for the study. Research participants made 1,064 investment decisions with each participant making an average of 36.7 investment decisions (Chassot, Hampl, & Wustenhagen, 2014). Findings showed that investors’ perceptions of regulatory exposure 53 risk influenced their investment decision-making. For example, results showed that when the venture capitalist investors perceived the regulatory exposure as high risk, they made less investment decisions in the renewable energy companies. On the contrary, when the venture capital investors perceived the regulatory risk as low risk, they invested more of their portfolios. These results consistent with Harras and Somette’s (2011), and Rose et al.’s (2010), and Wang, Keller, and Siegrist’s (2011) findings that investors’ perceptions of investment risk influenced their investment decisions. The results of the study by Chassot et al. (2014) underscored the importance of understanding investors’ perception of investment corporations and their subsequent investment decision-making. Investors’ perceptions of financial corporations’ strong checks and balances may also influence how they make their investment decisions. Farkas and Murthy (2014) explored how nonprofessional financial investors made investment decisions based on their perceptions of strong continuous auditing (CA) and continuous control monitoring (CCM) systems. In a two-part study, the authors first investigated 120 nonprofessional investors from a national survey company and then 84 respondents from the Amazon’s Mechanical Turk platform. The findings demonstrated nonprofessional investors did not increase their investments even though they perceived the implementation of continuous auditing would strengthen the control systems in the financial markets. Results from the second survey showed that nonprofessional investors were disinterested in increasing their portfolios investment. Investors’ perceptions of high cost CA and CCM discouraged them from increasing their investment choices in spite of investors believing that the CA and CCM controlled weaknesses in the financial system. This finding is a counterpoint to 54 Rose et al.’s (2010) findings because investors’ perceptions of strong accounting and monitoring system alone did not influence them to increase their investment portfolios. Investors’ perceptions of international investments and duration of holding portfolios remain of interest to financial investments researchers (Harvey, Bolton, WilseSamson, Li, & Samama, 2014). To help understand and explore the investors’ perception of cross-border investments and duration of holding invested assets before selling them, Harvey, Bolton, Wilse-Samson, Li, and Samama (2014) examined chief investment officers in financial corporations. Using a five-point Likert-type scale, participants were asked to rate how likely the 20 identified item-contents would decrease their willingness to invest across the border. Harvey et al.’s (2014) findings showed that perceptions of regulations, whether formal or informal, was the highest ranked item that discouraged investors from investing in cross-border countries. In addition to the cross-border investments, the authors found that experienced investors held their portfolios in longterm investments. Harvey et al.’s (2014) findings indicated that holding portfolios in long-term investments may increase returns and reduce risk. Understanding of factors that influence investors’ perceptions in making investment decisions and being satisfied with the decisions is of interest to financial researchers. Hari and Ayappan (2014) examined what influenced investors’ perceptions during investment decision-making, which subsequently led to satisfaction with their investment selections. Using risklessness, returns, reference, investment choice and analysis as variables, Hari and Ayappan tested whether analysis and reference had a positive effect on investment choices or whether investment choices have positive impact on risklessness and returns. While the return factor showed positive impact on 55 satisfaction, there was no influence of risklessness on satisfaction (Hari & Ayappan, 2014). This finding showed that the investors’ perceptions of positive returns on investments significantly influenced their investment choices. Financial researchers should attempt to understand what influences investors’ perceptions of portfolios and their subsequent investment selections. Hari and Ayappan’s findings were consistent with Rose et al. (2010), Harras and Somette (2011), and Wang, Keller, and Siegrist (2011), whose findings supported that investors’ perception of investment risks influenced their investment decisions. Investors’ perception of financial corporation performance, social responsibility, and corporate misconduct may influence the public view of the corporations in that industry (Paruchuri & Misangy, 2015). To help understand investors’ perceptions and generalization of corporate misconduct, Paruchuri and Misangy (2015) investigated how investors’ perceptions of corporate misconduct influenced general public opinion of the corporation. The authors examined 725 firms across U.S. financial markets with 84 financial misconducts. Using the cumulative abnormal return (CAR), results showed that when investors have perception of misconduct in financial corporations, such perception of misconduct is generalized to other corporations in the same financial industry creating a ripple effect. As demonstrated in Paruchuri and Misangy’s (2015) findings, investors generalized perceptions of financial corporations’ conducts influenced other corporations in the same industry. In another study, Hoffmann, Post, and Pennings (2015) investigated if financial investors’ perceptions of financial corporations indeed translated to their investment decisions. An online survey was conducted between March 2008 and April 56 2009 with participants recruited through email invitations. Research subjects were asked to provide information on their perceptions of investment returns, risk tolerance, risk perceptions for the subsequent month. Hoffmann et al.’s (2015) findings showed that financial investors who perceived investment returns as having higher and upward expectations invested more of their portfolios in such investments. The authors found that investors who demonstrated high risk tolerance held riskier investments for longer periods. Additionally, participants with higher risk perceptions had higher turnover than their counterparts with lower risk perceptions of investment. Furthermore, financial investors with higher revision of risk tolerance demonstrated higher buy-sell ratios than participants with higher revision of risk perception who showed lower buy-sell ratios. The finding is important for financial practitioners and researchers to understand how the investors’ perception influenced their investment decisions. When investors had perceptions of lower risk on investments, they made investment decisions of not only trading more but also holding their portfolios for an extended period of time. Consistent with Paruchuri and Misangy’s (2015) findings, Hoffmann et al.’s findings showed that investors’ perceptions of investment risk influenced their investment decisions. Research supports that investors’ perceptions play a significant role in investor behavior (Hoffman et al., 2013; Roszkowski & Davey, 2010; Wang et al., 2011). Perceptions, gain versus risk, seem to have different effects on investor behavior (Hoffman et al., 2013). Different specific perceptions such as high investment risk versus low risk can lead to different investing (Wang et al., 2011). Some research suggests that gain perceptions lead to more beneficial investing behavior (Roszkowski & Davey, 2010), while other research has suggested that risk perceptions lead to rational investing 57 behavior (Hoffman et al., 2013). Perceptions of financial information have an impact on financial investors’ decision making (Kaufmann & Webber, 2013). For example, cognitive limitations in managing financial information influenced investors’ perceptions because they were not able to accurately interpret higher aggregate information during decision making (Kaufmann & Webber, 2013). Other research indicated that investors’ familiarity with company/brand, perception of corporation performance, and checks and balances played significant roles in investment decisions (Aspara, 2013; Farkas & Murthy, 2014; Jing, Chen, & Zhang, 2013; Wang et al., 2011). For example, familiarity with company/brand caused investors to invest more of their portfolios (Jing, Chen, & Zhang, 2013; Wang et al., 2011), whereas other research supported that it was the investors’ perceptions of the product that mattered not just their familiarity with company/brand (Aspara, 2013). Perceptions of a strong control system can lead to checking financial weakness (Farkas & Murthy, 2014), while perception of misconduct in one financial corporation is generalized to other corporations in the same industry (Paruchuri & Misangy, 2015). Gain versus risk perceptions lead to psychological bias which subsequently impacts investing behavior. For example, research suggests anchoring bias can lead to more irrational investing behavior because the investor ignores other information in favor of familiar information (Rose et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2011). Bias occurs when investors make decisions based on familiar information, products, or preferences without carefully considering information on unfamiliar products which may lead to less than optimal outcomes (Hon-Snir, Kudryavtsev, & Cohen, 2012). It is important for investors to carefully analyze their biases before and when determining investment decisions. 58 Influence of psychological bias on investors financial decision-making. Numerous behavioral finance researchers have investigated the effects of psychological biases on investors’ decision-making. Lai, Chen, and Huang (2010) examined effects of psychological biases on technical trading signals among investors in the Taiwan Stock Exchange (TSE). Their findings showed that due to anchoring bias, investors broke-out rules regarding financial trading and thereby traded more. Anchoring bias continues to influence financial investors’ decision-making processes, regardless of the value of the financial information available to investors (Chaarlas & Lawrence, 2012). Like Chaarlas and Lawrence (2012), Gupta and Banik (2013), Lai et al. (2010), and Mitroi (2013) examined how psychological bias influenced investors’ decision-making in the Bangladesh financial market. The researchers surveyed 220 participants who were involved in financial investment. Gupta and Banik’s findings showed that 95% of respondents demonstrated anchoring bias. These findings were consistent with Chaarlas and Lawrence’s (2012) findings which may help to understand the perceptions and emotions of anchoring bias among the U.S. investors continued use of geographical diversification strategy. This means that investors’ investment decisions are influenced by their familiarity with financial products and strategies (Chaarlas & Lawrence, 2012). To understand how psychological bias influences investors’ decision-making, Hon-Snir, Kudryavtsev, and Cohen (2012) studied managers of investments and individual investors. The study surveyed 300 individual financial investors and 41 managers of financial investments to assess the influence of psychological bias on investment decisions. Utilizing a five-point Likert-scale, the authors examined how investors’ psychological bias influenced their financial investment decisions in the Israeli 59 financial market. In recruiting individual investor participants, the researchers invited qualified and interested participants by posting the online survey to one of the major Israeli financial websites. Participants who were managers of financial investments were contacted directly for participation. Results showed that both investment managers’ and individual investors’ decisions-making were influenced by psychological bias. Interestingly, investment decisions of individual financial investors with the most experience were less influenced by psychological bias (Hon-Snir, Kudryavtsev, & Cohen, 2012). Hin-Snir et al.’s (2012) results suggest that less experienced investors’ decisions are most at risk for psychological bias, as the impact of bias goes down as experience increases. Hin-Snir et al.’s (2012) findings were consistent with Gupta and Banik’s (2013) findings that investor’s psychological bias influenced their investment decisions. To understand the effect of financial literacy and behavioral biases of financial investment decisions, Ateş, Coşkun Şahin, and Demircan (2016) investigated the influence of financial literacy on the behavioral biases of individual stock investors from Borsa Istanbul market. Ates et al. (2016) examined the behavior biases of 596 individual stock investors and the relationship between financial literacy and behavioral biases. The results showed that there was a significant relationship between a number of other biases and the level of financial literacy. Specifically, the results showed that approximately 50% of the investors with low financial literacy level demonstrated a high level of behavioral biases in their decision-making compared with investors with high financial literacy level. Ates et al.’s (2016) findings were consistent with Gupta and Banik’s (2013) and Hin-Snir et al.’s (2012) findings that investors psychological bias influenced their investment decisions. The implication of this result is that behavioral biases 60 influenced investors’ investment decisions. Similarly, the result indicates that depending on informal financial information may result in making non-beneficial financial decision. In an attempt to suggest strategies to overcome biases among behavioral finance investors, Chaarlas and Lawrence (2012) conducted a survey among equity investors in India. Chaarlas and Lawrence (2012) found that 81% of participants made investment decisions based on anchoring bias of their familiarity with financial information. Results showed that when investors are more familiar with specific financial information and anchored to that information, the investors were more likely to increase their worth if a rise in the prices of such investments occurred. On the contrary, if the prices in such investments fell, the investors were more likely to incur losses because they underestimated the fall in the prices due to anchoring bias (Chaarlas & Lawrence, 2012). In order to overcome biases in investment, financial investors should critically analyze information and figures in the financial markets, as opposed to basing their decisions on little familiar financial information (Chaarlas & Lawrence, 2012). To explore factors that influence behavioral bias, Tekçe, Yılmaz, and Bildik (2016) investigated behavioral biases among Turkish individual stock investors during 2011. Tekce et al. (2016) examined how common disposition effect, familiarity bias, representativeness heuristic, and status quo bias affected overconfidence and return performance. The authors found that biases were common among investors (Tekce et al., 2016). Results showed that familiarity bias had a nonmonotonic effect on investment returns. This means that lower levels of familiarity bias resulted in lower investment return, while higher levels of familiarity bias produced higher investment returns (Tekce et al., 2016). The findings showed that overconfidence was positively correlated with 61 familiarity bias (Tekce et al., 2016). This shows that investors with higher levels of familiarity bias were more confident about their investment decisions than investors who demonstrated lower levels of familiarity bias. Tekce et al.’s (2016) findings were consistent with findings from Chaarlas and Lawrence (2012), which showed that investors familiarity bias did not only influence their investment decision but also investors with higher levels of familiarity bias were more likely to increase their investment worth compared to those with lower familiarity bias. Different forms of psychological tendencies may influence how people make decisions. Pandit and Ken (2014) investigated how participants’ psychological tendencies influenced their investment decision making with regards to postponement of buying shares. Psychological tendencies are cognitive biases which influence the way people think and demonstrate behavioral bias in one way or the other during decision making (Pandit & Ken, 2014). For example, when investors have more knowledge of a particular financial product, they are more likely to have psychological tendencies towards that particular product and purchase more shares of the product. From the Indian stock exchange, 250 investors were surveyed and analyzed using regression analysis. An illusion of knowledge occurs when the financial investors believe they know more about a particular financial product than they actually do know about the product. For example, the investors’ illusion of knowledge influenced their decision on postponing buying of shares. When the degree of illusion of knowledge was large, investors bought fewer shares. Conversely, investors purchased more shares when the extent of illusion of knowledge was small. Risk propensity is the extent to which investors are willing to take chances regarding risk of losing investment worth during investment decisions (Pandit & 62 Ken, 2014). Therefore, higher risk propensity means the investors are willing to take higher chances of risk of loss when making investment decisions. Higher risk propensity influenced investors to buy fewer stocks and postpone purchasing more shares. However, in diversified portfolios, higher risk propensity did not influence participants from buying more shares. The implication of findings is that when investors believe they have adequate knowledge and information on the shares, they would make more purchases of shares and less postponement of buying shares. On the contrary, investors would make fewer purchases of shares and postpone more purchasing of shares when their knowledge on the shares is less. Research suggests psychological bias influenced investors’ decision making processes (Ates et al., 2016; Chaarlas & Lawrence, 2012; Gupta & Banik, 2013; Lai et al., 2010; Mitroi, 2013; Tekce et al., 2016). For example, anchoring bias led investors to go against investment rules and regulations (Chaarlas & Lawrence, 2012). As many as 95% of investors demonstrated anchoring biases while making investment decisions (Gupta & Banik, 2013). Other research indicated that while psychological bias influenced investors decisions, the more experienced the investors are, the less likely they were at risk for psychological bias (Hon-Snir, Kudryavtsev, & Cohen, 2012). It is essential for investors to critically analyze their emotions, perceptions, and psychological bias when making investing decisions. While the behavioral finance studies provided insight into how investors’ emotions, perceptions, and psychological biases have influenced their decision-making, some limitations of these studies need to be addressed. Problems and Limitations of the Previous Studies 63 The major problem confronting MPT investment strategy is the overinternationalization of financial markets that makes it difficult to effectively differentiate domestically and internationally diversified financial investments. Unlike Solnik (1974) and Srivastava (2007), who found the strategy to be effective in increasing returns and reducing risks, other research observed the effects of globalization have diminished the benefits of geographical diversification (Bobillo et al., 2008; Chu-Sheng, 2010; Gocmen, 2010; Raju & Khanapuri, 2010; Singh et al., 2010). The introduction of the EURO currency, into the European financial market, has further diminished the effectiveness of geographical diversification by increasing the interdependence among the European and U.S. financial markets (Kashefi, 2006). Interestingly, some supporters of geographical diversification strategy, such as Maldonado and Saunders (1981), Raju and Khanapuri (2010), and Singh et al. (2010) found that the interdependence of global financial markets, where a financial crisis or boost in one continent influences other continent(s), is making geographical diversification strategy less attractive to investors. However, the limitation is that the globalization of financial markets that has rendered the strategy ineffective has not been critically examined and explained to investors to discourage them from using the strategy (Maldonado & Saunders, 1981; Raju & Khanapuri, 2010; Singh et al., 2010). Investors may find it difficult to recall their emotions while making investment decisions when later questioned (Fenton-O'Creevy et al., 2011). This action is common among studies investigating the influence of emotions and perceptions on investment decisions. For example, Fenton-O'Creevy et al. showed that investors’ ability to accurately recall their emotions during investment decisions posed threats to the overall 64 outcome of the research. Therefore, utilizing numerous data collection techniques such as open-ended interviews and self-reporting journals may help address the investors’ difficulty in recalling their emotions when making investment decisions. Open-ended interviews may allow participants enough time and space to remember and record their emotional feelings during investment decisions, which may not be available when using closed-structured interviews (Sherry, 2008; Stake, 1995; Yin, 2003, 2014). Additionally, keeping self-reporting journals, at the time of decision-making, may help participants remember their emotions during investment decisions. A self-reporting journal is a valuable technique for collecting reliable information using a qualitative design, including when conducting case study (O’Connor, 2013; Yin, 2003, 2014). Therefore, using both interviews and journaling to collect data may help the researcher obtain information that participants might have difficulty recalling if interviews alone had been utilized (O’Connor, 2013). The studies that have examined how investors’ emotions, perceptions, and psychological biases influenced their investment decisions have not always investigated U.S. investors or investors directly. For example, Fenton-O'Creevy et al. (2011) examined investors in London and Van de Laar and de Neubourg (2006) analyzed the Dutch investors. Furthermore, Wang et al. (2011) investigated how perceptions of investment risks affect investors’ investment decision-making in the German speaking area of Switzerland and Hoffman et al. (2013) studied how investors’ perceptions of trading and risk-taking influenced their investment behaviors in Iran. In addition, some previous studies that examined the influence of investors’ emotions, perceptions, and anchoring biases on investment decisions focused on financial planners and financial 65 corporations instead of the individual investors (Harvey et al., 2014). In order to effectively assess how investors’ emotions, perceptions, and anchoring biases influence their decision making, most of the participants should be individuals who conduct the investment activities (Chassot et al., 2014; Hin-Snir et al., 2012; Hoffman et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2011). The major problem with the MPT investment strategy is that due to the globalization of the financial markets, the benefits of the strategy have drastically diminished, or erased, which makes the strategy less attractive (Maldonado & Saunders, 1981; Singh et al., 2010). One of the limitations of behavior finance studies is that investors have difficulty recalling their emotions during investment decisions (FentonO'Creevy et al., 2011). The difficulty in recalling emotions during investment decisions makes it difficult to obtain accurate information (Fenton-O'Creevy et al., 2011). Another limitation is that the previous research conducted by Harvey et al. (2014) focused heavily on financial planners and corporations instead of the individual investors. Emotional feelings of fear and excitement about investment value influenced investors’ decisions differently (Lee & Andrade, 2015). Investors who demonstrated fear made less risky decisions, while those who were emotionally excited about portfolio value made more risky investment decisions (Lee & Andrade, 2015). Summary While some research has supported the diversification strategy for increasing returns and reducing risks, other research did not support the diversification strategy. For example, Solnik (1994) and Srivastava (2007) found the strategy of geographical diversification effective in increasing returns and reducing risks, while other research did 66 not (Bobillo et al., 2008; Chu-Sheng, 2010; Gocmen, 2010; Raju & Khanapuri, 2010; Singh et al., 2010). The differences in outcomes of geographical diversification strategy among the advocates of conventional finance theory have prompted interest in understanding the investors’ behaviors with regards to using a strategy. Understanding investors’ behaviors is important because psychological factors such as perceptions, emotions, and psychological biases influence their investment decision-making processes, which subsequently influences the health of the financial markets. For example, Gambetti and Giusberti (2012) found that investors who demonstrated anger emotions made more risky and less profitable investment decisions, while investors with anxious emotions made less risky and more profitable investment decisions. Investors’ perceptions of investment risks influenced their willingness to invest or not to invest in a particular financial product (Gambetti & Giusberti, 2012). In times of financial crises, investors’ perceptions of risks increased which decreased their willingness to invest in such financial markets (Hoffman et al., 2013). Investors’ perceptions of corporate strong checks and balances (Jing, Chen, & Zhang, 2013; Wang, Keller, & Siegrist, 2011), familiarity with company/brand (Aspara, 2013), and product quality influenced their investment decisions (Aspara, 2013). Anchoring biases influenced investors’ decisions during investment decision-making when they invested only in familiar products (Gupta & Banik, 2013; Lai, Chen, & Huang, 2010). The benefits of MPT strategy have diminished due to globalization of the financial markets, which should make the strategy less attractive to investors; yet, investors continue to use this strategy (Raju & Khanapuri, 2010; Singh et al., 2010). Investors continue to use the geographical diversification strategy to increase investment 67 returns and decrease investment returns in spite of empirical evidence. Positive versus negative emotions, perceptions, and biases play a role in investor behavior, which can lead to different investing behavior. Since investing decisions based on psychological factors lead to different investing behavior, it is important for investors to become of aware of and be cognizant of their perceptions, emotional state, and biases during investing decisions (Hoffman, 2013; Lai et al., 2010). 68 Chapter 3: Research Method The 2008 financial crisis showed that investors’ continued use of the modern portfolio theory (MPT) strategy of geographical diversification, as an investment strategy, may not be supported by evidence (Smith & Harvey, 2011). The 2008 financial crisis that resulted in increased cost, decreased returns, and increased risks (Bobillo, Iturriaga & Gaite, 2008; Chu-Sheng, 2010; Gocmen, 2010) calls for expanded understanding of investors’ behaviors. The purpose of this qualitative case study was to explore U.S. financial investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding their continued use of geographical diversification, as an investment strategy, to increase investment returns and decrease investment risks, when empirical evidence (Bobillo et al., 2008; Chu-Sheng, 2010; Gocmen, 2010) does not support the use of the strategy. The following research questions provided direction for the case study: Q1. How do U.S. investors describe their emotions about using geographical diversification as an investment strategy? Q2. How do U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for increasing investment returns? Q3. How do U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for reducing investment risks? Q4. How do U.S. investors explain their use of geographical diversification strategy, in the context of literature, which does not support the strategy? This qualitative single case study was outlined in the following sections. The sections included detailed information on the study population, sample, and instruments in the study. In addition, the data collection process and analysis, assumptions, 69 limitations, delimitations, and ethical assurances for the study are also discussed in this chapter. Research Design A qualitative case study was appropriate to elicit detailed responses on investors’ perceptions and emotions about geographical diversification as an investment strategy (Ahrens & Chapman, 2006; Patton, 2002). The single case study design is appropriate for this research because this design was used to obtain in depth information on investors at a particular point in time (Patton, 2002; Stake, 1995; Yin, 2003). The case that was investigated is the investors’ emotions and perceptions and each investor was a unit of analysis. This design enabled the researcher to explore investors’ perceptions and emotions of the investment strategy through using semi-structured interviews. For example, the qualitative case study allowed research participants to describe, in detail, their investing experience in their own words (Cozby & Bates, 2012; Creswell, 2009). Other research designs were considered but not selected. The ethnography design studies an entire group with shared common interest over several months or several years (Zenker & Kumoll, 2010). For example, an ethnographical design explores people and their cultures from the participant’s point of view (Zenker & Kumoll, 2010). The phenomenological study explores the meaning of an event requiring the researcher to be immersed in the study settings (Giorgi, 2009; Yin, 2003). The researcher must then directly observe the participants to obtain data and translate into research in a phenomenological design (Giorgi, 2009). In a case study, the researcher does not have to be immersed in the daily life of the participants in order to obtain the data (Leedy & Ormrod, 2010). Through using case study design, utilizing interviews and journaling, the 70 researcher obtained detailed information at one time point. Therefore, the case study design was appropriate for this study because it helped the researcher explore the research participants’ behaviors, perceptions, and emotions related to the investment strategy (Giorgi, 2009; Yin, 2003, 2014). Quantitative methodology uses close-ended questions and depends on measures to elicit and quantify variables (Hunter & Leahey, 2008). Using a quantitative method, which collects numeric data, would not have allowed participants to provide detailed descriptions of their emotions and perceptions of the investment strategy (Hunter & Leahey, 2008). The quantitative methodology was not pursued because it was not considered the best approach for the study’s purpose. The qualitative method, however, offers the opportunity for the researcher to deduce meaning from the descriptive data through building themes and drawing conclusions from the findings (Flick, 2007). To obtain information on the investors’ perceptions and emotions about an investment strategy, which was the purpose of this study, a qualitative approach was deemed as more appropriate. Triangulation in a qualitative study is a process of using multiple approaches in data collection or analysis to improve the research outcomes (Patton, 2002). The various types of triangulation include data triangulation, theoretical triangulation, method triangulation, and investigator triangulation (Patton, 2002). Data triangulation obtains multiple sources of data within the same method for the purpose of comparison for similarities or differences (Patton, 2002). Method triangulation uses multiple methods, usually both quantitative and qualitative to collect the data (Patton, 2002). Method triangulation was not appropriate for the study because rich data on participants’ perceptions and emotions requires a qualitative approach. Both theoretical and 71 investigator triangulations were not appropriate because they require a third party to interpret and analyze the data which may be a violation of the independent work of the dissertator. While triangulation may be used in qualitative study, simultaneous use of multiple types of triangulation is not a requirement in qualitative designs (Golafshani, 2003). Triangulation does not suggest a specific method for qualitative study (Golafshani, 2003; Guion, Diehl & McDonald, 2011). Obtaining data from participants who are not investors may result in data that are not related to the purpose of the study, threatening the validity of the study results (Baxter & Jack, 2008). According to Seidman (2005), when data are obtained by employing multiple techniques from different sources, a study produces richer data and results. This study used a data triangulation approach to gather multiple sources of information from the participants. In addition to using semi-structured, open-ended interviews, via in-person or by telephone, journaling was used to obtain additional data from the investors. Journaling being a form of documentation is a valuable source of collecting reliable information in a qualitative design, including a case study (O’Connor, 2013; Yin, 2003). Using interviews and journaling to obtain data allows the researcher to obtain information that participants may have difficulty recalling if an interview alone had been used to gather data (O’Connor, 2013). In order to explore the perspectives of retired or near-retired U.S. investors seeking information and use behaviors, O’Connor (2013) utilized interviews and journaling to obtain subjective data from the participants. Obtaining data using multiple techniques ensures that the credibility, transferability, and trustworthiness requirements are improved (Shenton, 2004). 72 Population/Sample Financial investors over the age of 18 years in the Washington DC Metropolitan Area who geographically diversified their financial investments were the targeted population for the study. The Washington DC Metropolitan Area included Washington DC, Northern Virginia, some counties in Maryland, and Jefferson county in West Virginia. The Washington DC Metropolitan Area is an appropriate context for this study because investors in this area are likely to geographically invest their financial assets. Professionals who currently worked or resided in the Washington DC Metropolitan Area may have come from many different states and therefore were more likely to be a good representation of all U.S. investors. In addition, the proximity of the location in relation to the researcher’s residence made the Washington DC Metropolitan Area readily accessible for the researcher to conduct in-person interviews with participants. Financial investors who were members of Academia.edu, Facebook, LinkedIn, Meetup, and Meettheboss social networks were initially targeted as the study population. As of 2015, these five social networks have a combined estimated 650,000 members in the study area. Snowball sampling was also used to recruit as qualified interested participants were asked to inform anyone who meets the selection criteria about the study, so that might have extended the population beyond these social networks. Fourteen participants from the population in the Washington DC Metropolitan Area were purposely recruited for the case study. Over-recruitment was used in order to allow for attrition and data saturation (Stake, 1995), even though the planned sample size was 10 to 12 participants. Ten participants completed the interviews and journal entries and were finally included in the study. Two participants completed the interviews but did 73 not return any of the three journal entries, one participant completed one-half of the interview questions but decided to quit and one participant dropped out without taking part in the interview or journal entry. Purposeful sampling ensures that a focused sample is represented in the study and also ensures that the researcher collects appropriate data for the study (Robinson, 2014; Yilmaz, 2013). Participants who were 18 years or older, diversified their financial investments across different geographical areas, and resided or worked in the Washington DC Metropolitan Area were purposely selected for the study. The data that were gathered from these individuals was appropriate to answer the research questions because it provides the researcher insight into how participants’ perceptions and emotions of geographical diversification strategy influence their investment decision process. A case study design requires that in-depth information is obtained, which makes the process extremely difficult to include a large sample (Patton, 2002). According to Patton (2002) and Stake (1995), the recommended sample size for a case study is between eight and 12 participants (Patton, 2002; Stake, 1995), thus the sample size of 10 was well within the recommended sample size of a case study to ensure credibility and transferability of a case study results. Data saturation is achieved when the researcher does not obtain new themes or discovers new information (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005; Kvale & Brinkmann, 2009). Data saturation has the potential to increase the transferability of the study findings (Shenton, 2004). After the 10th participants who completed the interviews and journal entries, no new themes were being discovered, so data saturation was achieved with the 10th participant. 74 Materials/Instrumentation This study used various instruments to gather data for analysis. Open-ended semistructured interviews, in person or via telephone, and journaling prompts were the instruments used to collect participants’ responses. Another important instrument in the study was the researcher. The researcher’s impact on the study outcomes was immense because in developing and applying the instruments, the researcher collects, analyzes, and interprets the collected information. It was therefore important to make sure that the instruments utilized are appropriate to collect rich data and subsequently ensure credibility, confirmability, dependability, and transferability of the research outcomes (Robinson, 2014; Yilmaz, 2013). The researcher engaged two individuals known to him, who meet inclusion criteria, to review the interview questions and journaling prompts as part of enhancing the quality of the research outcomes through the use of field-testing. The rationale for conducting this field test was to make sure the questions were interpreted how the questions were planned, which would assist in obtaining the appropriate information from participants (Shenton, 2004). The interview questions and journaling instructions were modified based on the responses from the field test. Shenton (2004) recommends the use of field testing to ensure that interview questions were interpreted how they were intended in order to obtain the appropriate information from participants to help improve the quality of research outcomes. Each instrument is described in the following subsections. Interview questions. This case study required participants to complete a set interview questions either in person or via phone. Research questions were used as guidelines for the development of the interview questions (Sherry, 2008; Stake, 1995; 75 Yin, 2003, 2014; see Appendix H). The nature of qualitative design requires that the interviews be conducted with outmost care to preserve the quality and accuracy of responses (Patton, 2002). This means that the interview questions were clear, concise, and open-ended but semi-structured to allow participants to express themselves in their own words. Each question included one idea to ensure enhance easy of analysis (Patton, 2002). The open-ended semi-structured interview questions ensured that comparable responses were elicited from participants for the purpose of standardization (Patton, 2002). At the same time, these open-ended interview questions were meant to obtain responses that could create more questions depending upon the participant’s response and subsequently bring themes into this analysis. The first set of questions explored how the U.S. investors describe their emotions about using geographical diversification as an investment strategy. The second set of questions sought to obtain data on the description of U.S. investors’ perceptions of geographical diversification, as a strategy for increasing investment returns. The third set of questions explored how the U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for reducing investment risks. The fourth set of questions asked participants to explain their use of geographical diversification strategy, in the context of literature that does not support the strategy. Journals. For the purpose of triangulation, journaling was also be used to collect data. This study required participants to complete three journal entries of their feelings during decision making, mood at the time of decision making, and thoughts during decision making (see Appendix I) on a Microsoft word journal template provided to them (see Appendix J). Journaling has been used to collect data in qualitative designs (Yin, 76 2014). To be able to effectively gather data on participants’ thoughts and experiences, Kağnici (2014) used journaling to collect data. Researcher. The researcher plays an important role in qualitative studies. The researcher acts as an instrument in guiding the participants to provide their descriptive response, without unduly influencing the participants. The researcher provided clarification when necessary to help participants provide true reflection of their experiences. The researcher may be considered to be in the powerful position when interviewing participants (Glesne, 2011), which can lead to the participants providing inaccurate information, which may affect the data and subsequently negatively influence the study results (Glesne, 2011). It is important that the researcher ensures integrity at all times to enhance the dependability and trustworthiness of the study outcomes (Brinkman & Kyale, 2015). The researcher’s background may introduce biases, which are acknowledged below. While the researcher has not held a position as an economist, his undergraduate degree in agricultural economics led him to believe that investors invest their assets to optimize returns and reduce risks. As a Medical Director, who has managed a medical practice for over five years, the researcher believes that risk reduction and profit maximization are important in all for-profit investments. The paramount objective of investment of any kind is to put resources to good use so as to increase outcomes (Masron & Fereidouni, 2010; Resnik, 2010). The researcher’s belief that investors will make decisions that they believe will result in increasing returns and decreasing risks might influence how he perceives the participants’ description of their investment decision making. In addition, the researcher has a belief that when investors are under 77 excessive stress they are more likely to make less than optimal decisions than when the investors are calm and relaxed. The researcher’s background, experience, and beliefs shape biases, which might potentially affect how he developed and conducted the interview and how he analyzed the data collected. To help reduce the influence of researcher position and biases, the researcher ensured that participants’ confidentiality was maintained at all times (Glesne, 2011). In addition, the researcher demonstrated reflexivity by keeping weekly entries of his values, interests, beliefs, and preconceptions of the geographical diversification strategy in order to remind himself of possible biases or preconceived positions (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). Furthermore, being conscious of the researcher’ experiences, background, beliefs, and biases throughout his interactions with the participants helped the researcher to obtain the true reflection of participants’ responses (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). Utilizing thorough documentation of the participants’ responses, such as using participants’ quotations and presenting detailed description of responses, reduced the researcher’s biases, which helped enhance the dependability of the research outcomes (Shenton, 2004; Williams & Marrow, 2009). Experience with and good skills in interviewing participants and gathering data for analysis are important to enhance the credibility, dependability, transferability, and confirmability of results (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005; Kvale & Brinkmann, 2009). A researcher’s knowledge of ethical guidelines ensures the accuracy of the data and subsequently the dependability of the research findings (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005). The researcher maintained equal partners’ environment between himself and the participant, during the interview, because such an environment enables the interviewee to feel comfortable, which helps to increase the accuracy of data (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005). Creating an equal partners environment 78 means the researcher and participant see each other as equal in the interview process (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005). Creating and maintain an equal partners environment were achieved by recognizing the researcher’s own biases and respecting the participant’s views and offering the participant enough room and time to think and answer questions. To recognize the researcher’s own biases, he demonstrated reflexivity by keeping weekly entries of his values, interests, beliefs, and preconceptions of the geographical diversification strategy in order to remind himself of possible bias or preconceived position (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). The researcher respected participants’ views by maintaining personal space and asking appropriate questions, as approved by NCU’s IRB, about participant perceptions and emotions regarding the use of the geographical diversification as an investment strategy (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005). In addition, participants were given enough time (e.g., up to 90 minutes) and space (e.g., open ended interview questions and journal entry template), to freely describe their opinions about the strategy without unnecessary interruptions (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005). Knowledge of ethical guidelines requires that the researcher maintains integrity through constantly recognizing his/her biases and be open about biases and measures correcting biases (Kvale & Brinkmann, 2009). The semi-structured open-ended interview questions were used to collect data where participants were allowed up to 90 minutes to answer the interview questions. Participants were encouraged to ask for clarification when they needed explanation(s) regarding the interview questions. The semi-structured open-ended interview questions and up to 90 minutes to answer the interview questions provided the environment for participants to provide narrative and true meaning of their responses, which could enhance the accuracy of information (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005). 79 Incorporating feedback from the field test will help ensure that participants interpret research questions how they were intended which will result in obtaining rich data. Planning for a sample size on the large side based on recommendations for case study will help in strengthening my interview skills and experience (Patton, 2002; Stake, 1995). Study Procedures Financial investors, who were members of the Academia.edu, Facebook, LinkedIn, Meetup, and Meettheboss social networks, were the targeted groups for participation. The researcher posted a recruitment notice (see Appendix A) on his webpage, in the five identified social networks, which informed potential participants about the study being conducted. Qualified interested individuals were asked to email the researcher. After the interested individuals had contacted the researcher, the researcher provided more study details and confirmed the inclusion criteria using the recruitment and screening email response (see Appendix B), including attaching the informed consent document (see Appendix C). Individuals who met inclusion criteria, as based upon participants who were 18 years or older, diversified their financial investments across different geographical areas, and resided or worked in the Washington DC Metropolitan Area, received a participation confirmation email (Appendix D), with directions for completion of the informed consent document. The researcher asked the individual participants who requested to complete the interview by phone to print, sign, scan, and email the informed consent back to the researcher or request his address to mail back a hard copy of the informed consent to the researcher. The signed informed consent was obtained prior to conducting the interview by phone. Individual participants who requested to participate in the interview in person were asked to choose a public place for 80 the in person interview. This was done due to availability of convenient interview space and ensured that participants were comfortable for the interviews. The in person interviews were conducted once the researcher had obtained a signed informed consent document from the individual. The participation confirmation email was re-sent one day prior to the scheduled interview to remind the interviewee about the interview appointment, thus decreasing the likelihood of participants forgetting about the interviews. Interested individuals who did not meet the selection criteria received an email (Appendix E) thanking them for their interest in the study. In all, thirty potential participants initially expressed interest in participating in the study. After the 14th qualified participants confirmed his/her eligibility, returned the signed informed consent, and scheduled his/her interview date, the remainder 16 potential participants were informed via email (Appendix F) that they had been placed on a waiting list. After the tenth participant completed the interviews, along with the 3 journal entries, as required for complete study participation, the researcher sent a thank you email notifying participants of data saturation, as there were additional16 potential participants on the waiting list (Appendix G). Each of the 16 potential participants, on the waiting list, were offered the opportunity to receive a copy of the study findings, pending committee approval. In addition to purposeful sampling, snowball sampling, through which participants helped to recruit new participants, was employed in the participants’ recruitment process (Salganik & Heckathorn, 2004). To utilize the snowballing recruitment approach, participants were asked to notify anyone they knew who might fit 81 the inclusion criteria for the study. Snowball sampling has been effectively utilized in previous studies in which the researcher encounters difficulty in recruiting qualified participants (Draper & Swift, 2011). The screening process, informed consent requirements, and wait list notification processes utilized for participants recruited from the five social networks was the same for participants recruited through snowball sampling. Data Collection and Analysis In this case study, data collection was conducted over the duration of two months. Participants responded to interview questions and completed three journal entries examining perceptions and emotions regarding their continued use of geographical diversification as an investment strategy to increase investment returns and decrease investment risks, when empirical evidence does not support the use of the strategy. The data were manually coded and thematized for analysis. The follow sections elaborated how the data were collected, coded, and analyzed. Data Collection. Interviews were used to collect data from participants, after they signed the informed consent. After the interviews, each participant completed three journal entries on the Microsoft word format provided and emailed their responses to me as participants complete them each week. In order to ensure that participants complete the journal entries, the researcher sent out reminders to participants on a weekly basis. Semi-structured interviews using open-ended interview questions were conducted either in person or via telephone to collect data from each participant and responses recorded on a handheld recorder. The researcher personally transcribed the recorded data to ensure accuracy of information. Kvale (2012) and Saldaña (2013) recommend 82 transcribing data in small amounts, preferably with a pen/pencil and a piece of paper, to get used to the work before transferring onto a computer. The transcribed responses were emailed to participants for member validation. Each participant verified their transcribed responses and no changes were requested by any of the participants. To ensure the accuracy of data, the member validation is a useful technique (Kvale, 2012). The transcribed interview responses were added to participant journal entries onto a password protected Microsoft Word format. The researcher kept the recorded data onto a password protected Microsoft Word format to ensure the safety of the data and the protection of participants’ confidentiality (Kvale & Brinkmann, 2009). Data Analysis. In this case study, I personally manually coded the data to discover themes for the analysis. Themeing is appropriate for processing and analyzing descriptive data, especially interviews and journals (Saldaña, 2013). In themeing, phrases and sentences that provide meaning are identified and grouped in order to provide the true meaning of the participants’ responses (Saldaña, 2013). Themes were identified and written next to the data and the themes were then categorized after the similarities, differences, or relationships within the data were considered. The categorization of the themes helped the researcher appropriately process and analyze the data (Saldaña, 2013). In analyzing the data, themes that emerged from the data were organized to answer the four research questions (Saldaña, 2013). The themes were identified at the manifest level (Saldaña, 2013). The identification of themes at the manifest level means the themes were formed directly from observable data in the study (Saldaña, 2013). While using multiple raters, such as different investigators, to analyze the data set may improve the confirmability of the study outcomes, such approach is not 83 feasible for the proposed study as the researcher does not have access to other investigators qualified to conduct qualitative analyses and that technique could violate the expectation that the dissertation is the original and independent work of the dissertator. Stages 6 and 7. Multiple techniques were used to verify the quality of the study results. Field-testing ensured the interview questions were designed to obtain the appropriate data the researcher planned to collect. Recruiting participants from the five social networks helped increased the transferability of the research outcomes. The researcher obtained signed consent from participants before conducting the interviews. In the informed consent, the researcher explained the details of the study including the participants’ ability to quit the study without retaliation and how the participants’ confidentiality would be protected. According to Brinkmann and Kvale (2009) obtaining informed consent before interviews encouraged participants to provide honest responses when they knew that their safety and confidentiality are protected. Each potential data collection method has potential pros and cons. In person interviews may impact participant feelings of comfort in either direction, while phone interviews may present visual barriers (Shenton, 2004). Using data triangulation, the researcher obtained data using in person or on phone interviews and journaling, which helped strengthened the outcomes of this qualitative study through data triangulation (Shenton, 2004). Member checking enabled the participants to verify and validate his or her responses to ensure data accuracy (Kvale, 2012). After themeing the data, the researcher reflected on the results and considered rearrangement of themeing the data when necessary for accuracy. Direct quote responses demonstrating participants’ own words related to their emotions of geographical 84 diversification strategy, perceptions of geographical diversification strategy, and perceived risk of geographical diversification strategy were reported in the results of this study. Recruiting from the large population, from the five identified social networks and, besides the interviews, using journaling to gather data from participants provided rich data, which is key to the study outcomes’ transferability and dependability. Assumptions It was assumed that participants provided accurate information reflecting their emotions and perceptions regarding their continued use of geographical diversification as an investment strategy. The open-ended semi-structured interviews allowed participants more time and free space to speak openly and honestly about their emotions and perceptions. To obtain accurate data on subjective topics, an interview is most appropriate (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005; Kvale & Brinkmann, 2009) and using openended semi-structured interviews facilitated obtaining accurate information. Data triangulation obtains multiple sources of data within the same method for the purpose of comparison for similarities or differences (Patton, 2002). Data triangulation was used in order to obtain accurate data, which represents a true reflection of the participants’ emotions and perceptions regarding the use of the strategy (Patton, 2002). This means in addition to using semi-structured, open-ended interviews via in-person or by telephone, journaling was used to obtain additional data from the investors. According to Yin (2014), journaling is effective tool to corroborate data collected from other sources. Informed consent was obtained from participants before conducting interviews, which encouraged the participants to provide honest responses when they knew that their safety 85 and confidentiality was protected (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005; Kvale & Brinkmann, 2009). Secondly, it was assumed that the researcher’s experience and biases might impact data collection and analysis of participants’ responses (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). This is because the researcher’s own experience in economics and bias about investors making rations decisions to increase returns and reduce risks might influence how he collected and analyzed participants’ data (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005, 2015; Lincoln & Guba, 1985). The researcher kept notes about his values, interests, beliefs, and preconceptions of the geographical diversification strategy (Lincoln & Guba, 1985) to consistently reminded himself about his bias and thus, allowed participants to do all of the talking (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). The researcher audio-recorded participants responses, asked approved interview questions, and did not interrupt while participants were talking (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). Similarly, member checking was used where participants verified and validated their responses to ensure data accuracy. Finally, direct quotes of participants’ responses were reported in the results, which helped to reduce or eliminate the researcher’s potential influence and bias on the research outcomes. Limitations The first limitation of the study was the ability to satisfy the transferability and dependability requirements due to small sample size. According to Shenton (2004), a case study, like any other qualitative design, faces constraints of meeting transferability, confirmability, credibility, and trustworthiness issues (Shenton, 2004). Including 10 participants in this study to enhance the dependability and transferability of the study outcomes minimized the sample size limitation. The recommended sample size of 86 qualitative case study ranges from 8 to 12 (Patton, 2002; Stake, 1995). Additionally, using member checking and direct quotes of responses, obtaining informed consent before conducting interviews, and providing detailed description of participants’ responses helped enriched the study results to support transferability and dependability (Kvale, 2012). Another limitation of this study was the possible use of interviews via phone, which may be a barrier to understanding nonverbal cues or result in missed signals. The researcher asked participants to explain such as hand gestures, change in tones, and facial expressions during the in person interviews. This potential missed signal limitation was reduced when seven of the 10 participants, representing 70% of the respondents, selected the in person interviews. Mealer and Jones (2014) and Siedman (2005) suggested that interviews, via phone, can create a visual barrier and cause the interviewer to miss nonverbal information. Delimitations The scope of the study was limited to U.S. investors who were at least 18 years old, diversify their financial investments across different geographical areas, and reside or work in the Washington DC Metropolitan Area. The findings of this study may not be applicable or transferable to other investors in different countries due to dissimilarity of emotions and perceptions of using geographical diversification as an investment strategy. Ethical Assurances Before recruiting and collecting data from participants, I obtained an IRB approval from Northcentral University as required. IRB approval was important to ensure the protection of participants that includes but is not limited to their confidentiality and 87 safety (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005; Kvale, 2012). Obtaining informed consent from participants before the interviews were conducted and thus, assured participants of protection of their safety and confidentiality (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2005; Kvale, 2012). The identity of participants and the information they provided remained confidential through de-identification and presentation of data by identifying participants only as Participant A, B, C, etc. Before and after data were de-identified, the data, both transcribed interviews and journal entries, were kept in a Microsoft Word format with a password-protected on the documents. While qualitative designs provide enough room for participants to speak freely, the researcher did not ask manipulative (e.g., leading) questions, and thus only focused upon the questions approved by NCU IRB (Brinkmann & Kvale, 2000). Additionally, the researcher avoided asking personal questions, which would provide personal information, thus, prevented crossing participant’s personal space (Turner, 2010). Since no research design is without ethical concerns, field-testing was conducted to identify and correct questions that might have presented ethical problems, including but not limited to asking leading questions and crossing personal boundaries (Turner, 2010). Questions that sought only to provide answers to the research questions were asked. Generally, interview questions take more time to respond to than other sources of data collection like surveys (Patton, 2002; Yin, 2003, 2014). Additionally, participants were asked to keep journal entries of their emotional feelings, when they were making investment decisions, and submit their journal entries to the researcher. Participants were made aware of the amount of time they were expected to spend for participation in the interviews and self-reporting journal aspect of the study. 88 Summary This qualitative case study was conducted to explore U.S. financial investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding their continued use of geographical diversification as an investment strategy. A qualitative case study was most suitable to explore the perceptions and emotions of U.S. investors using the geographical diversification strategy. The rationale for selection of the case study design was that this design helped explored the meaning of the investors’ perceptions and behaviors. Additionally, case study design utilized in-depth semi-structured open-ended interviews to ensure that research participants provided the necessary data when limited information exists (Yin, 2003, 2014). In addition to the interview, participants were asked to provide a journal, three times in four weeks, during investment decision-making (O’Connor, 2013; Patton, 2002). The transferability, confirmability, credibility, and trustworthiness of the study outcomes were strengthened by field testing, data triangulation, and a utilizing recommended sample size as per qualitative research. These measures helped reduced researcher bias and influence and improve interview techniques (Shenton, 2004). The population of the study included financial investors in the Washington D.C. metropolitan area, who geographically diversified their financial investments as an investment strategy. Financial investors who were members of Academia.edu, Facebook, LinkedIn, Meetup, and Meettheboss social networks were initially targeted as the study population. Snowball sampling was also used to recruit, as qualified interested participants were asked to inform anyone who meets the selection criteria about the study. Purposeful sampling and snowballing sampling were used to recruit ten investors who satisfied the research requirements. According to Patton (2002) and Stake (1995), 89 the recommended sample size for a case study is between 8 and 12 participants (Patton, 2002; Stake, 1995), thus the sample size of ten was well within the recommended sample size of a case study required to ensure credibility and transferability of case study results. The assumptions made included that participants would provide accurate information reflecting their emotions during investment decisions and perceptions of using geographical diversification strategy, as well as that researcher influence and bias can be minimized. Since open-ended semi-structured interviews and self-reporting journaling were used responses were confidential, participants should feel comfortable and free to provide a true reflection of their emotions and perceptions. Missing nonverbal signals through obtaining data via phone interview and the study outcomes meeting transferability and dependability requirements were the limitations of the study. To help obtain quality data and produce rich research outcomes, participants had the option of selecting in person interviews. IRB approval was sought and obtained from Northcentral University prior to collecting data from participants. An informed consent was obtained from each participant before conducting data collection. Obtaining informed consent before collecting data encouraged participants to provide quality responses since the participants knew their information and confidentiality were protected. Data for this study was collected over the duration of two months during which participants were interviewed and kept three journal entries of their emotions and perceptions when making decisions. The data was manually coded and analyzed, and then categorized into themes. Direct quotes of participants’ responses were used to present the results. Member checking enabled the participants to verify and validate their responses to ensure data 90 accuracy. Themeing has been used to process and analyze qualitative data with descriptive responses (Kayle, 2012; Saldana, 2013). 91 Chapter 4: Findings The purpose of this qualitative case study was to explore U.S. financial investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding their continued use of geographical diversification as an investment strategy. Chapter 4 comprises a reporting of the study’s results with the themes that emerged from the data. This chapter is subdivided into four main sections. The first section comprises trustworthiness of data where population, sample size, procedure, and study design are briefly introduced. Organized by research questions, the second section consists of the results from the study. Evaluation of findings forms the third section, where findings from this study are explained and described in comparison with existing literature. The fourth and last section of this chapter consists of the summary of the discussion of the study’s results. Trustworthiness of Data Using purposive and snowballing sampling, the researcher collected data through interviews in person or by phone and journaling from 10 investors, who lived or worked in Washington D.C. Metropolitan Area and diversified their investments across different countries. All participants submitted at least one journal entry. A total of 18 journal entries were received from 10 participants documenting their perceptions and emotions regarding geographical diversification as an investment strategy. This design allowed the researcher to collect and analyze data from each participant in order to better understand the investor perceptions and emotions of using geographical diversification as an investment strategy. 92 Results The purpose of this study was achieved through examination of four research questions: Q1. How do U.S. investors describe their emotions about using geographical diversification as an investment strategy? Q2. How do U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for increasing investment returns? Q3. How do U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for reducing investment risks? Q4. How do U.S. investors explain their use of geographical diversification strategy, in the context of literature, which does not support the strategy? Research Question 1: How do U.S. investors describe their emotions about using geographical diversification as an investment strategy? Based on the participants’ responses to interviews and journal entries, two themes emerged: (a) participants feel positive about geographical diversification and (b) participant think positive emotions lead to positive investment decisions/behavior (Table 1). 93 Table 1 Theme Surmised from Processing and Analysis of the Data for Research Question 1 Themes Participants felt positive about Number of participants Percent of participants who affirmed who affirmed 9 90% 8 80% geographical diversification strategy Participants think positive emotions lead to positive investment decisions/behavior Participants described emotions when deciding to use the geographical diversification strategy. According to participants, they had positive emotional feelings during decision making about the geographical diversification strategy. Nine of the 10 participants described their feelings when deciding to use geographical diversification as a strategy as good, passionate, happy, excited, forward looking, optimistic, and positive. Participant A reported, “My feelings are actually good when I’m making decisions to diverse my portfolios geographically.” Similarly, Participant C stated, “I always feel positive and excited when I’m making decisions to diversify across different countries.” Participants H, I, and J reported feeling happy when they were making investment decisions. Participant J stated, “I feel happy when I’m making investment decisions.” Participants’ responses showed that they felt optimistic about the use of the strategy, because they perceived that use of the geographical diversification strategy would help to increase their investment returns and reduce their investment risks. 94 Participants D, E, F, and G reported they feel optimistic about using the geographical diversification strategy. Participant D reported, “I feel happy and optimistic anytime I make decisions to invest my money across different countries.” Additionally, Participant F stated, “I feel positive and optimistic about geographical diversification, because the Asian financial markets are robust and recession free.” Participants seemed to relate this optimism to pessimism about the U.S. financial market, specifically related to the 2007-2008 financial crises. Participant E stated, “I have negative feelings and negative outlook (perspective) about the domestic investment. My feelings are positive toward geographical diversification investment.” As emphasized by participants’ responses, participants felt positive about using the geographical diversification strategy when making decisions to invest their money. Participants thought having positive emotional feelings such as feeling happy or excited, during investment decision-making, led to making positive investment decisions such as increasing returns and decreasing risks. Similarly, participants described perceiving that negative emotions would lead to negative investment decisions. Eight of the 10 participants stated that their feelings influenced their decisions when diversifying their financial assets geographically. For example, Participant A reported, “I know that my feelings affect my investment decisions in a positive way.” He/she proceeded to state, “I only make decisions when I’m happy and excited. I deferred investment decisions when I’m angry, down or sad to avoid making risky decisions.” He/she continued to say, “If I’m not passionate, excited or don’t feel good about a foreign investment, I don’t invest in it.” Additionally, Participant F stated, “There were a number of days that I did not make investment decisions because I was emotionally down. My investment is my 95 money; therefore, I don’t want my sad feelings to negatively influence my investment decisions.” Similar to the other respondents, Participant D, E, G, H, I, and J stated their feelings influence the investment decisions they make. The participants stated that they did not want negative and angry feelings to influence their investment decisions. Participants noted that negative feelings may result in non-beneficial investment decisions. Participant D reported, “Well, investment comes with benefits and risks. I don’t think about risks but I concentrate on the benefits because I’m optimistic when making decision.” Participant J went on to state, “I refuse to make decisions when I’m emotionally sad or angry because negative feelings can result in poor decisions.” Two of 10 participants stated their emotional feelings did not influence their investment decisions. Participant C stated, “I always feel positive and excited when I’m making decisions to diversify across different countries but those feelings do not influence my decisions. I don’t think my feelings influence my decisions.” The only thing that influences my decision is “my yearly returns, and they keep going up.” And only one of the 10 participants (Participant B) described his/her feelings during investment decision as “usual.” The usual feelings mean Participant B felt neutral, thus, he/she had no feelings of happiness, sadness, excitement, or depression when he/she was making investment decision. Participant B reported: I make decision to diversify my investment across different countries when I’m not happy or sad, excited or depressed. I always look forward to investing when conditions are right for decision making. I don’t want my investment decisions to be unduly influenced by my emotional feelings. My investment decisions are free from emotions. 96 In summary, participant responses indicated that they felt positive emotional feelings about using geographical diversification as a strategy to invest. Additionally, the majority of participants thought having positive emotional feelings during investment decision-making would lead to positive investment behavior, thus, they made investment decisions when they experienced positive emotions and avoided decisions when they felt negative emotions. Research Question 2: How do U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for increasing investment returns? Two themes emerged from participants’ responses to research question two. The first theme showed that participants perceived geographical diversification as an effective investment strategy in increasing investment returns. The second theme indicated participants thought having positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign markets influenced their perceptions of using geographical diversification as an effective strategy in increasing investment returns (Table 2). 97 Table 1 Theme Surmised from Processing and Analysis of the Data for Research Question 2 Themes Participants perceived geographical Number of participants Percent of participants who affirmed who affirmed 10 100% 10 100% diversification as effective investment strategy in increasing investment returns Participants’ positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign markets influence their perception of diversification as a good strategy for increasing returns The first theme affirmed by all participants was that they perceived geographical diversification as an effective strategy to increase investment returns. While participants diversified their investments on different financial markets, participants unanimously agreed with the perception that diversifying their financial assets across different countries helps to increase investment returns. For instance, Participant A reported, “Putting all your eggs in one basket is not a good investment strategy. My perception is that investing my money in different countries helps to increase my investment profits.” Likewise, Participant B stated, “I think holding investment across different countries in different portfolios help increase investment returns. So my thought is that the 98 geographical diversification is a prudent investment strategy.” Correspondingly, Participant C explained: The geographical diversification helps to maximize investment at a faster rate. I feel that wherever your money is, that’s where your heart is (the investment strategy that you are familiar with that it works, that is where you invest your money). My geographical investment returns in land and housing keep going up (increasing). So I know definitely that my investment returns going to increase more. Participant D stated: My thought is that the geographical diversification is going to help me. I think I can get more returns from my geographical diversification investments. The idea that I’m going to make more profit when I invest across different countries appeals to me. As emphasized by participants, diversifying financial assets across different countries was perceived as an effective strategy to increase investment returns. As Participant A reflected, “Putting all your eggs in one basket is not a good investment strategy.” Participants reported they felt optimistic about using the geographical diversification strategy to increase their investment returns. Participants cited the 20072008 financial crises as the causes of their pessimism about the U.S. financial market and cited these crises as a reason to spread their investments across different countries. Specifically, participants described that the emerging or growing financial markets are ideal markets for geographical diversification to increase investment returns. According to participants, spreading their investment portfolios across developed and 99 emerging/developing markets is a prudent investment strategy. Participants’ positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign markets influenced their perception of diversification as a good strategy for increasing returns. Participants unanimously concurred that their perceptions of emerging and rapidly growing markets in Europe, Asia, Africa, and South America shaped their opinions and influenced their investment decisions to use geographical diversification. For example, Participant B reported, “Because of the introduction of the EU markets, the Eastern European countries’ economies are expanding in size and growing in strength. As an investor, diversifying in growing financial markets is appealing to me.” Participant A stated, “The real estate market in some parts of Africa, Asia, and South America are booming. The booming and growing real estate markets are appealing and influence my investment decisions.” Factors relating to time and stability were noted as benefits of emerging and growing foreign markets. Participants likened fast growing markets not only to increasing investment returns, but also to accruing profits at a faster rate. In explaining how his/her perception of emerging and growing foreign markets influence his/her perception of the strategy, Participant C said “Investing your financial assets in the financial markets of the emerging and developing countries is a no brainer for increasing investment returns.” This participant expanded, saying: If I invest outside the U.S., the rate at which it increases is great. The rate at which investment returns from geographical diversification increases appeals to me. My investment returns keep growing year after year, so apparently, the strategy helps to increase my profits. It takes too long to get profits in U.S. investment. 100 Time was also factor for participants who needed short-term investments. Participants J reported: I have only 5 years to retire. The security of my retirement influenced my decision to invest across different countries. The Asian financial market is strong and does not see many fluctuations. This stability appeals to me and influences my investment decisions. Stability was an appealing feature of these markets. Participant J stated, “The Middle-Eastern financial market is strong and does not see many fluctuations.” Participant E shared: My feeling of strong economic stability and the potential for increasing returns of fast growing and expanding Asian, African, and South American financial markets influenced my decision to geographically diversify my financial assets. The emerging and growing nature of these markets appeals to me and shapes my decision to diversify geographically. In summary of participants’ responses from research question 2, the results showed that participants perceived geographical diversification strategy increased their investment returns. Specifically, participants’ positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign markets, due to stability and rapid returns, influenced their preference for the geographical diversification strategy. Research Question 3: How do U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for reducing investment risks? Two themes emerged from participants’ responses to interviews and journal entries: (a) participants perceived geographical diversification as an effective investment strategy in reducing 101 investment risks and (b) participants’ positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign markets influence their perception of diversification as a good strategy for reducing risks (Table 3). Table 3 Theme Surmised from Processing and Analysis of the data for Research Question 3 Themes Participants perceived geographical Number of participants Percent of participants who affirmed who affirmed 10 100% 10 100% diversification as effective investment strategy in reducing investment risks Participants’ positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign markets influence their perception of diversification as a good strategy for reducing risks All participants perceived geographical diversification as an effective strategy to reduce investment risks. Specifically, participants perceived the distribution of their investments across different countries as an effective method for reducing investment risks. Participants noted that some markets are strong and exposed to minimal risks compared to other markets. Participant A noted: 102 If you put all your eggs in one basket and you drop that basket, you will lose all your eggs. In the same way, if I invest all my money in one market and something happens, I will lose all my investment. However, if I spread my investment across different countries, I will significantly reduce my investment risks. In short, the geographical diversification helps me reduce investment risks. In explaining his/her perception about geographical diversification, as a strategy to reduce investment risk, Participant B stated, “We saw what happened to the U.S. and European financial markets in 2007-2008. Not every country has the same investment risks. My investments in Asian and African markets were subjected to relatively lesser investment risks.” Similarly, Participant C stated: I think my investments have low risks. If I have investments invested in different countries, I reduce my risks because some of the countries’ financial markets are expanding. I think the geographical diversification strategy is helping to reduce my investment risks. Diversifying my investment across different countries is a good strategy to reduce my investment risks. The rest of the participants’ responses about their perceptions of the geographical diversification strategy in reducing investment risks are similar to the responses from other participants in this study. Similarly, participants perceived geographical diversification strategy as an upside risks because of their perceptions that the returns on investments across different countries will exceed the expected returns on their investments. Participants reported that investing all their financial portfolios in the U.S. may expose their investments to high risks if the U.S. market faces financial problems. 103 To reduce such investments risks, participants argued, they perceived the diversification strategy as effective in achieving their goal of reducing investment risks. The second theme of research question 3 shows that the participants’ positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign markets influence their perception of diversification as a good strategy for reducing risks. The results show that participants responses of positive perceptions of the African, Asian, South America emerging and growing markets influenced their perception of reducing investment risks by diversifying their across those markets. Participant D stated: Geographical diversification strategy helps to reduce investment risks because reduced risks translate into increased investment returns. The growing markets in Africa, Asia, and South America are perfect markets to distribute investment risks and virtually reduce investment risks. Such markets shape and influence my decision about geographical diversification to reduce investment risks. Additionally, Participant I explained: The strategy of distributing investments across different markets to reduce risks appeals to me and shapes investment decisions. The geographical diversification strategy helps reduce investment risks because investment risks are different across different countries. Investments in real estate in Asia and Africa are growing and less risky. These less risky markets appeal to me and shape my geographical diversification decisions. Specifically, participants reported their positive perceptions of some industries in foreign markets, as influencing their investment decisions to use geographical diversification to reduce risks. According to participants, spreading investments on the 104 Asian, European, South American, and African markets helps reduce investment risks. Specifically, participants noted their perceptions of diversifying on the emerging and growing real estate in African, Asian, and South American markets to reduce risks as influencing their perceptions of the strategy. Participant F reported: The strategy of casting your net wide, as in geographical diversification strategy, appeal to me and shape my investment decision to reduce risk. The real estate investments in Asia and Africa have are emerging and growing. These markets have nearly zero risk. Similarly, Participant G noted: I have been investing in Asian and African real estate for the past 15 years. My investments have not been exposed to many risks. This strategy of spreading investment across different countries to reduce risks is what shapes my opinion and appeal to me and influenced my investment decisions. Participant J reported, “Diversifying in these oil producing and emerging markets helps to reduce investment risks due to the market stability.” In summary of participants’ responses from research question 3, participant responses showed that participants perceived the geographical diversification strategy as effective in reducing investment risks. Specifically, participants’ positive perceptions of the emerging and growing foreign markets were based on the rapid returns on market investments. Participants perceived the real estate and oil markets as providing investment stability, hence the participants’ preference of using the geographical diversification strategy to reduce investment risks. 105 Research Question 4: How do U.S. investors explain their use of geographical diversification strategy, in the context of literature, which does not support the strategy? Two themes emerged from participants’ responses to interviews and journal entries: (a) participants believe the strategy is working for them and (b) participants value their own experience over the financial literature (Table 4). Table 4 Theme Surmised from Processing and Analysis of the Data for Research Question 4 Themes Participants believe the Number of participants who Percent of participants who affirmed affirmed 10 100% 10 100% strategy is working for them Participants value their own experience over the financial literature Only three of 10 participants reported being familiar with the financial literature, which says geographical diversification was not effective in increasing returns and reducing risks (Bobullo, Iturriaga, & Gaite, 2008; Cai, Xu, & Zeng, 2016; Chu-Sheng, 2010; Gocmen, 2010; Maldonado & Saunders 1981; Raju & Khanapuri, 2010; Singh, Kumar, & Pandey, 2010). Regardless of participants’ knowledge of this specific literature, all participants described negative feelings and thoughts about the financial literature based on their own positive perceptions and experiences related to the use of geographical diversification. Participants believed the geographical diversification 106 strategy is working for them, specifically in terms of increasing returns and reducing returns. For example, Participant A reported: I’m not familiar with the financial literature but I can say I totally disagree. Even though I’m a beginner investor with only 5 years investment experience, I’ve seen profit. I have negative feelings about the literature because I think the geographical diversification strategy is an effective investment strategy. My thoughts are that spreading my investments across different countries does not only help increase my returns but also help reduce investment risks. Similarly, Participant B stated, I’m not aware of any literature that says geographical diversification is not effective. There are a number of good resources for investment, when you go international, you’re extending the market. My feelings about the literature are negative, because I know the strategy is effective. I can also direct you to some of the resources that indicate the strategy is effective in increasing returns and reducing investment risks. Additionally, Participant E noted: I don’t know about the literature. My investment in shipping companies and real estate developments in Europe and Africa are doing very well. I want to get copies of the literature you’re alluding to. I will like to know the basis of that literature. Participants seemed to support the contradiction of the financial literature based on specific factors, such as which countries and what industries investors use to diversify their financial portfolios. Participant F noted, “Yes, I know of different financial 107 literature that says geographical diversification is not an effective strategy. I have negative feelings about that literature. My returns on diversified investments in real estate on African and Asian markets are doing well.” Participant J stated: Yes, I’m familiar with the financial literature that says geographical diversification strategy is not effective. I have negative feelings about the financial literature. I think it depends on the industry that is invested in. The real estate markets in the emerging markets are perfect markets to diversify your investments. I’m aware of what happened on the U.S. financial market in 2007 to 2008. Despite the financial literature, the results showed that participants believed that the geographical diversification strategy is working for them to increase returns and reduce risks. As a result of those beliefs, as well as that experience, participant reported having negative feelings about the financial literature that says geographical diversification strategy is not effective. Specifically, the data also suggested that participants valued their own experience over the financial literature. Even though seven participants were not familiar with the financial literature, which says geographical diversification strategy is not effective, they were dismissive of the literature and did not plan to change their use of the strategy. For example, Participant A reported, “Yes, I will continue to use geographical diversification strategy because my investment returns keep growing and increasing.” He/she went on to say, “I see that Dow Jones fluctuating. I will definitely continue to diversify geographically.” Similarly, Participant B stated: Yes, my diversified investments in Europe, Africa and South America are performing better than those in the U.S. markets. There are a good number of 108 international investments out there with great investment potential. I will question the notion that international investments are not effective. In the same way, Participant E reported: Yes, I will continue to invest across different countries. I do not understand why I should change my strategy if the current one is working well. You don’t change or modify your strategy on the battlefield if your troops are winning. That is exactly how I see my investment abroad, winning. Participant H noted, “I have been investing in government bonds in Eastern Europe for over 10 years. My investment returns keep growing. I will not change my investment decision when everything is going well.” Participant J reported, “Yes, I will continue diversifying my investment across different countries, my investments are held in construction and real estate. My investment returns increase every year. I’m not going to change my position.” In summary of participants’ responses from research question 4, results showed that participants believed geographical diversification strategy is working for them by increasing their investment returns and reducing their investment risks. As a result of those beliefs, they had negative feelings about the financial literature, which contradicted their own experiences. The results showed that participants valued their experiences over the financial literature and did not plan to change their use of the strategy. Evaluation of Findings In order to understand the results from this case study, it is imperative to evaluate the findings from the perspectives of the existing literature. The purpose of this qualitative case study was to explore U.S. financial investors’ perceptions and emotions 109 regarding their continued use of geographical diversification as an investment strategy to increase investment returns and decrease investment risks, specifically when empirical evidence does not support the use of the strategy. Research Question 1: How do U.S. investors describe their emotions about using geographical diversification as an investment strategy? Research question one allowed the researcher to understand the investors’ emotional states during investment decisions and the influence of those emotions on their investment decision-making. The results from research question one highlighted a number of commonalities that were reduced to two themes. Specifically, participants had positive emotional feelings about using geographical diversification as a strategy. Moreover, the majority of participants thought having positive emotional feelings during investment decision-making would lead to positive investment behavior. The findings of this study are consistent with the outcomes of previous studies from Gambetti and Giusberti (2012), Myeong-Gu and Barrett (2007), and Sullivan (2011), who noted that investors had happy and excited emotions at the time of making investment decisions made more investment decisions. Similarly, the findings by Sahi, Bacha, and Masih (2013) demonstrated that when participants felt positive emotional feelings about investments, they invested more in those investments. Lee and Andrade (2015) found that when people were excited about the increase in value of their investment portfolios, they made more decisions. The findings of this study showed that the majority of participants thought positive emotional feelings during investment decision-making would lead to positive investment behavior. On the contrary, participants avoided making investment decisions 110 when they felt negatively because they believed negative emotions would lead to negative investment decisions. The findings of this study are consistent with findings from Blay, Kadous, and Sawers (2012), Gambetti and Giusberti (2012), Haocheng, Jian, Limin, and Shuyi (2014), Lee and Andrade (2015), Myeong-Gu and Barrett (2007), Patterson and Daigler (2014), Reyes (2006), and Sahi et al. (2013), which explain that investors’ emotional feelings, negative or positive, influenced investors’ investment decisions. Specifically, the results of this study are consistent with Myeong-Gu and Barrett’s (2007) findings that investors who demonstrated positive feelings during investment decision-making performed better than investors who demonstrated negative emotional feelings. Gambetti and Giusberti (2012) also found that the type of emotions matters in the decision-making; these authors found that investors who were angry at the time of investment decision-making made higher risk decisions than those were anxious. Contrary to this finding, other research has suggested negative emotions lead to more rational investing behavior (Chu, Im, & Jang, 2012; Van de Laar & de Neubourg, 2006). Additionally, the findings of this study are supported by Sahi et al.’s (2013) findings that when participants’ emotional feelings about the investments were positive and stronger they made more and beneficial investment decisions. Consistent with the findings of this study, when the investors’ emotional feelings about portfolio performance were negative, they demonstrated reluctance in investing is such markets (Sahi et al., 2013). Research Question 2: How do U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for increasing investment returns? The significance of research question two is that it allowed the researcher to understand the investors’ perception of geographical diversification as a strategy for increasing 111 investment returns. The results from research question two highlighted a number of commonalities that were reduced to two themes. Specifically, participants perceived geographical diversification as effective investment strategy for increasing investment returns. Additionally, participants’ positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign markets influenced their perception of diversification as a good strategy for increasing investment returns. The findings in this study demonstrated that participants perceived geographical diversification as effective investment strategy for increasing investment returns. The findings in this study are consistent with findings of Hargis and Mei (2006), Markowitz (1959), Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Odier and Solnik (1993), Solnik (1974), Srivastava (2007), Torres García-Heras (2011) findings that the strategy helped to increase investment returns. Other research by Bobullo, Iturriaga, and Gaite (2008), Cai, Xu, and Zeng (2016), Chu-Sheng (2010), Gocmen (2010), Maldonado and Saunders (1981), Raju and Khanapuri (2010), and Singh, Kumar, and Pandey (2010) found that the interdependence of the global investments have diminished the benefits of using the geographical diversification as a strategy to increase investment returns and reduce investment risks. The results of this study are in alignment with studies conducted by Harras and Somette (2011) and Rose, Norman, and Rose (2010). Consistent with the findings of this study, Harras and Somette (2011) and Rose et al. (2010) found that whenever investors have perceptions of positive perceptions about investment outcomes, investors invested more of their financial portfolios in that investment. On the contrary, when the investors perceived negative feedback about investments, investors did not invest their financial assets in those investments (Harras & Somette, 2011; Rose et al., 112 2010). The responses from participants in this study showed that participants’ confidence in the geographical diversification strategy influenced their decisions to use the strategy to increase investment returns. Consistent with the findings of this study, investors who demonstrated confidence in the markets made increasingly beneficial investment decisions (Jing, Chen, & Zhang, 2013; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). Additionally, Hoffmann, Post, and Pennings (2015) found that investors who perceived investment returns as having higher and upward expectations invested more of their portfolios in such investments. Similar to the findings in this study, investors’ perceptions of positive returns on investments significantly influenced their investment choices (Hari & Ayappan, 2014). The findings of this study showed participants’ positive perceptions of the emerging and growing foreign markets were due to stability, rapid returns, and specific industries such as real estate or oil. Several studies have shown positive investment returns from these emerging markets (Haran el al., 2016; Hari & Ayappan, 2014; Jing, Chen, & Zhang, 2013; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). The results of this study are consistent with Srivastava (2007) findings that increases in investment returns of geographically diversified investments depended on the emerging Asian markets. Correspondingly, Saiti et al. (2014) found that investors diversified in the growing foreign markets of Japan, GCC ex-Saudi, Indonesia, Malaysia and Taiwan due to increased investment returns. Similarly, Meriç, Jie, and Meriç (2016) found that US, Canadian, German, U.K., and French investors, who diversified their financial portfolios in the Indonesian, Philippine, Malaysian, Thai, Jordanian, Moroccan, Egyptian, and Pakistani emerging stock markets, experienced beneficial investment returns. Additional, 113 prior literature has connected investment returns to specific industries, as noted by the participants. The results of this study are supported by Masron and Fereidouni’s (2010) findings that the returns on housing investments exceeded the rate of inflation on the emerging real estate market of Iran. In another study, Haran el al. (2016) found that investors who diversified in the emerging real estate markets of the Czech Republic, Hungary, and Poland had potential to enhance their investment performances. Participants’ perceptions of diversifying in oil-producing countries to increase investment returns are in line with a study conducted by Mimouni, Charfeddine, and Al-Azzam (2016). Mimouni et al. (2016) found that an increase in investment returns related to international diversification in foreign markets of Gulf Cooperation Council (GCC) countries (e.g., Saudi Arabia, Kuwait, the United Arab Emirates, Qatar, Bahrain, and Oman) further develop. In an unrelated study, Cheng and Roulac (2007) found that the effectiveness of geographical diversification in real estate investment is limited to largesized investment. Research Question 3: How do U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for reducing investment risks? The importance of research question three is that it allowed the researcher understand the investors’ perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for reducing investment risks. The results from research question three highlighted a number of commonalities that were reduced to two themes. Specifically, participants perceived geographical diversification as an effective investment strategy in reducing investment risks. Moreover, participants’ positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign markets influenced their perception of diversification as a good strategy for reducing 114 risks. The findings of this study showed that participants perceived the geographical diversification strategy as effective in reducing investment risks. The findings in this study are consistent with Hargis and Mei (2006), Markowitz (1959), Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Odier and Solnik (1993), Solnik (1974), Srivastava (2007), and Torres García-Heras’ (2011) findings that the geographical diversification strategy is effective in reducing investment risks. The findings in this study however, contradict Chu-Sheng (2010), Gocmen (2010) Maldonado and Saunders (1981), Raju and Khanapuri (2010), and Singh et al.’s (2010) findings, which explain that the geographical diversification strategy is not effective in reducing risks due to the globalization of the markets. Cheng and Roulac (2007) found that the effectiveness of geographical diversification in real estate investment is limited to the size of the investment. Učkar and Carlin’s (2011) and Wang, Keller, and Siegrist (2011) findings showed investors who made investment decisions based on their perceptions, rather than empirical results, incurred heavy shortterm losses and risks. The findings of this study are aligned with the findings from previous studies. The findings are consistent with the findings by Roszkowski and Davey (2010), who found that investors’ perceptions of risks influenced their decisions because when investors’ perceptions of risks were low, their risks tolerance level was high. The results of this study indicated that participants perceived the strategy as helping to reduce investment risks and thus continued the use of the strategy. Similar to the findings of this study, a study conducted by Chassot, Hampl, and Wustenhagen (2014) found that when venture capitalist investors perceived the regulatory exposure as low risk, they made more investment decisions. On the contrary, when investors perceived regulatory exposure as 115 high risk, investors made less investment decisions in those industries. Additionally, the findings of this study are in line with findings by Paruchuri and Misangy (2015). The findings from Paruchuri and Misangy showed that investors not only traded more when they perceived the investments as low risk but also that they held their portfolios for a long period of time. The findings showed participants’ positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign markets and specific industries in foreign markets contributed to their positive perception of the strategy to reduce risks. As expressed by participants of this study, Srivastava (2007) found that geographical diversification strategies to reduce investment risks relied primarily on the emerging Asian markets. Additionally, Masron and Fereidouni (2010) found that diversification in the housing industry resulted in lowest risk-to-reward ratio in the Iran market. Consistent with the findings of this study the results from Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Srivastava (2007), and Torres García-Heras (2011) found that diversified investments on the U.S. and the European countries’ credit default swap markets, particularly, the Spanish market, performed worst in reducing investment risks compared to the diversified investments on the South American countries’ markets. The findings of this study are in also in alignment with Haran el al.’s (2016) findings that diversifying in real estate markets on the emerging markets of Czech Republic, Hungary, and Poland helped to reduce investment risks. Diversification on the Austria, Germany, and Poland stock markets experienced low volatility compared to the Russian and Turkey markets (Yavas & Dedi, 2016). Consistent with the results of this study, Yavas and Dedi’s (2016) findings showed that those markets had less risk and could be used to reduce investment risks. Additionally, Saiti et al. (2014) found that 116 international diversification was effective in reducing unforeseen investment risks on the emerging and growing foreign markets of Japan, GCC (e.g., Saudi, Indonesia, Malaysia, and Taiwan). Other research Učkar and Carlin (2011) and Wang et al.’s (2011) found that investors, who made investment decisions based on their perceptions, rather than empirical results, incurred heavy short-term losses and risks. Research Question 4: How do U.S. investors explain their use of geographical diversification strategy, in the context of literature, which does not support the strategy? The significance of research question four is that it allowed the researcher to understand the investors’ thoughts on continuing use of geographical diversification as a strategy when literature does not support the strategy. The results from research question four highlighted a number of commonalities that were reduced to two themes. Specifically, participants believed the strategy is working for them. Additionally, participants valued their own experience over the financial literature. As a result of those beliefs, they had negative feelings about the financial literature, which contradicted their own experiences. The results of this study are in line with the findings from previous studies. The findings of this study showed that participants believed geographical diversification strategies were working for them by increasing their investment returns and reducing their investment risks. Results showed that participants did not plan to change their investing behavior, even though the majority of participants were not familiar with the financial literature suggesting that the geographical diversification strategy is not effective. The findings of this study are consistent with previous studies on the psychological bias characteristics of investors (Akerlof & Shiller, 2010; Duxbury, 2015; 117 Gupta & Banik 2013; Kahneman, 2011; Mitroi & Oproiu, 2014; Tekçe, Yılmaz, & Bildik, 2016). The findings of this study showed that participants believed the strategy is working for them and they valued their experience over financial literature. As a result of those beliefs, they described negative feelings about the financial literature that contradicted their own experiences. Chaarlas and Lawrence (2012) found that regardless of the available financial information, investors’ decisions were influenced by their anchoring bias. Similarly, a study conducted by Kapor (2014) found that financial investors made investment decisions based on their own experience without using all financial information available to them. In other research, Yu and Xiaosong (2015) found that investors made decisions based on individual cognitive prejudices rather than rational financial evidence. Tekçe, Yılmaz, and Bildik (2016) found that investors demonstrated feelings of overconfidence. Investors’ reliance on their own experiences and judgments was positively correlated with familiarity bias, which may affect sound decision making (Tekçe, Yılmaz, & Bildik, 2016). In another study, Ateş, Coşkun, Şahin, and Demircan (2016) found that the investment decisions of investors who relied on informal sources of financial information were more likely to be influenced by behavioral biases, rather than those investors who based their decisions on formal financial information. Duxbury (2015) and Mitroi and Oproiu (2014) found that investors’ perceptions of familiarity with financial products influenced their investment decisions. Gupta and Banik (2013), Lai, Chen, and Huang (2010), and Wang et al. (2011) found that investors traded more due to anchoring bias. Research conducted by Hon-Snir, Kudryavtsev, and Cohen (2012) showed that while the most experienced investors’ decisions were less likely influenced by their psychological biases, compared to the least 118 experienced investors, generally, all investors’ investment decisions were influenced by psychological biases. Gupta and Banik’s (2013) findings indicated that 95% of investors demonstrated anchoring bias during investment decisions. Summary A qualitative case study that utilized interview questions and journal entries to collect data from participants was conducted to explore U.S. financial investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding their continued use of geographical diversification as an investment strategy. Themes that emerged from research question 1 were that (a) participants felt positive about using geographical diversification and (b) participants think positive emotions lead to positive investment decisions/behavior. Themes that emerged from research question 2 were that (a) participants perceived geographical diversification as an effective investment strategy for increasing investment returns and (b) participants’ positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign markets influenced their perception of diversification as a good strategy for increasing returns. Themes that emerged from research questions 3 were that (a) participants perceived geographical diversification as an effective investment strategy for reducing investment risks and (b) participants’ positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign markets influenced their perception of diversification as a good strategy for reducing risks. Lastly, themes that emerged from research question 4 were that (a) participants believe the strategy is working for them and (b) participants value their own experience over the financial literature. The evaluation of this study’s findings can be concluded as follows: (a) the findings in this study showed that the majority (90%) of participants felt positive 119 emotions when making decisions. The findings are consistent with Blay et al. (2012), Gambetti and Giusberti (2012), Haocheng et al.(2014), Lee and Andrade (2015), Myeong-Gu and Barrett (2007), Patterson and Daigler (2014), Reyes (2006), Sahi et al.’s (2013), and Sullivan (2011) findings that that investors’ emotional feelings, negative or positive, influenced their investment decisions and (b) the results of this study showed that the majority of participants thought having positive emotional feelings during investment decision-making would lead to positive investment behavior. The findings of this study regarding the benefits that positive emotional feelings that participants have during investment decision-making are consistent with the findings by Gambetti and Giusberti (2012), Myeong-Gu and Barrett (2007), and Sullivan (2007). Contrary to this finding, other research has suggested negative emotions lead to more rational investing behavior (Chu et al., 2012; Van de Laar & de Neubourg, 2006). The findings showed that participants in this study believed that the geographical diversification strategy was effective in increasing investment returns and reducing investment risks. Consistent with the findings in this study, Haran el al. (2016), Hargis and Mei (2006), Markowitz (1959), Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Odier and Solnik (1993), Saiti et al. (2014), Solnik (1974), Srivastava (2007), Torres García-Heras (2011), and Yavas and Dedi’s (2016) findings indicated that the strategy is effective in increasing returns and reducing risks. Contrary to the findings in this study, Bobillo et al. (2008), Cai et al. (2016), Chu-Sheng (2010), Gocmen (2010), Maldonado and Saunders (1981), Raju and Khanapuri (2010), and Singh et al. (2010) found that the effects of globalization had diminished the general benefits of geographical diversification in increasing returns and reducing risks, (d) the findings showed that participants’ positive perceptions of emerging and growing foreign 120 markets due to stability and rapid returns of real estate and oil markets influenced their preference for the geographical diversification strategy. The results in this study are consistent with Meriç et al. (2016), Saiti et al. (2014), and Srivastava (2007) findings that diversification on the emerging and growing markets resulted in increased investment returns and reduced risks. Contrary to the findings of this study, Raju and Khanapuri (2010) and Singh et al. (2010) found that the interdependence of international markets has made the emerging Asian markets non-beneficial for geographical diversification. Similarly, Cai et al. (2016) cautioned that investors should carefully consider the high cost of operating geographical diversification investments before making decisions. Participants’ perceptions and emotions, as well as their own experience of the geographical diversification strategy, influenced their investment decisions in increasing returns and reducing risks. Consistent with the findings of this study, Gupta and Banik (2013), Lai et al. (2010), and Wang et al. (2011) found that investors traded more due to anchoring bias. The findings of this study are consistent with literature on behavioral finance theory because the investors’ perceptions and emotions of the geographical diversification strategy influenced their investment decisions to use the strategy (Akerlof & Shiller, 2010; Duxbury, 2015; Gupta & Banik, 2013; Kahneman, 2011). The findings are consistent with Akerlof and Shiller (2010), Duxbury (2015), Gupta and Banik (2013), Kahneman (2011), Mitroi and Oproiu (2014), and Tekçe, Yılmaz, and Bildik’s (2016) findings that financial investors have the tendency to make investment decisions based on emotions, perceptions, and biases. 121 Chapter 5: Implications, Recommendations, and Conclusions The 2008 financial crisis showed that investors’ continued use of the modern portfolio theory (MPT) strategy of geographical diversification, as an investment strategy, may not be effective in increasing returns and reducing risk (Smith & Harvey, 2011). The 2008 financial crisis that resulted in increased cost, decreased returns, and increased risks (Bobillo, Iturriaga, & Gaite, 2008; Chu-Sheng, 2010; Gocmen, 2010) calls for an expanded understanding of investors’ behaviors. The purpose of this qualitative case study was to explore U.S. financial investors’ perceptions and emotions regarding their continued use of geographical diversification as an investment strategy. In order to achieve the purpose of the study, a case study design was used to collect data from participants through interviews in person or by phone and journaling to answer the research questions. This qualitative case study design allowed the researcher to collect and analyze data from each participant in order to better understand the investor perceptions and emotions of using geographical diversification as an investment strategy. The data were reflected upon and deduced into themes for analysis. One of the delimitations of the study was the location of Washington D.C. metropolitan area. Regarding the location, the study recruited U.S. investors who live or work in Washington DC Metropolitan Area. Due to the limitation of the location, the findings of this study may not be applicable to investors in other countries. Chapter 5 consists of implications, recommendations, and conclusions. The implication section provides a discussion of the present study. Additionally, recommendations will be made for practical application of the findings while providing 122 recommendations for future research. Finally, a summary of this chapter will be provided in the conclusions section. Implications From the results obtained from participants on their perceptions and emotions about using geographical diversification as an investment strategy, implications for financial investors, practitioners, and researchers were drawn. The findings of this study may contribute to research literature in the general areas of information on MPT, diversification strategy, and emotions and perceptions in investors. The conclusions made from this study have practical and theoretical implications because the findings may help investors and financial practitioners make beneficial investment decisions. Additionally, researchers and students may use the findings of this study to enhance their knowledge of the investors’ perceptions and emotions related to continued use of an ineffective strategy. Research Question 1: How do U.S. investors describe their emotions about using geographical diversification as an investment strategy? Two outliners that could not be supported by literature were observed. In the first outliner, one of ten participants stated “his/her decisions were emotion-free.” In regards the second outliner, two of 10 participants reported their “decisions were not influenced by emotions.” The two outliers are not supported by literature. The findings from Blay, Kadous, and Sawers (2012), Gambetti and Giusberti (2012), Haocheng, Jian, Limin, and Shuyi (2014), Lee and Andrade (2015), Myeong-Gu and Barrett (2007), Patterson and Daigler (2014), Reyes (2006), and Sahi et al. (2013) indicate that investors’ emotional feelings, negative or positive, influenced investment decisions. Specifically, the results of this study are 123 consistent with Myeong-Gu and Barrett’s (2007) findings that investors who demonstrated positive feelings, during investment decision-making, performed better than investors who demonstrated negative emotional feelings. Gambetti and Giusberti (2012) also found that the type of emotions matters in the decision-making; these authors found that investors who were angry at the time of investment decision-making made higher risk decisions than those who were anxious. Similarly, Lee and Andrade (2015) found that when people were excited about the increase in value of their investment portfolios they made more decisions that were effective in increasing investment returns and reducing investment risks. Participants had positive emotions when deciding to use the strategy and they felt positive about geographical diversification strategy. Participants described their emotions as good, passionate, happy, excited, forward looking, optimistic, and positive, which influenced them when making decisions to use geographical diversification. Numerous studies suggest that emotions play a role in investor behavior (Chu et al., 2012; MyeongGu & Barrett, 2007; Shiv et al., 2005; Van de Laar & de Neubourg, 2006). The results of this study are consistent with studies by Baker, Coval, and Stein (2007), Duxbury (2015), Hyoyoun and Wook (2013), Muradoglu and Harvey (2012), and Peteros and Maleyeff’s (2013) findings that investors demonstrated emotions when making investment decisions. Moreover, participants of this study thought positive emotions lead to positive investment decisions/behavior. For example, as Participant A reported, “I know that my feelings affect my investment decisions in a positive way.” He/she proceeded to state, “I only make decisions when I’m happy and excited. I deferred investment decisions when I’m angry, down or sad to avoid making risky decisions.” He/she continued to say, “If I’m not 124 passionate, excited or don’t feel good about foreign investments, I don’t invest in it.” In line with the conclusions of this study, investors who had happy and excited emotions at the time of making investment decisions made rational investment decisions, which resulted in increased returns and decreased investment risks (Gambetti, Giusberti, 2012; Myeong-Gu & Barrett, 2007; Sullivan, 2011). The findings of this study showed that positive emotional feelings led to investors making positive investment decisions. Consistent with behavioral finance theory, participants in this study made investment decisions about using geographical diversification when they felt positive emotions and avoided decisions when they felt negative emotions (Duxbury, 2015; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). The findings of this study have practical implications for investors to make profitable investment decisions to increase returns and reduce risks. For example, when participants’ emotional feelings about the investments were stronger, they made increased and beneficial investment decisions (Duxbury, 2015; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). On the contrary, when the investors’ emotional feelings about the portfolios’ performances were weak, they showed disinterest in making investment decisions (Duxbury, 2015; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). Investors and financial planners should recognize and acknowledge that those emotional feelings could influence them to make beneficial or non-beneficial investment decisions. O'Creevy et al. (2011) found that the investments of investors who critically analyzed their emotions and regulated those emotions during investment decisions performed better than the investments of investors who made investment decisions without analyzing and regulating their emotions. Analyzing and regulating emotional feelings may help investors make profitable decisions leading to 125 increased investment returns and reducing investment risks (Duxbury, 2015; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). Understanding of investor’s behaviors may help financial planners provide advice to investors, which may result in increasing returns and decreasing risks (Duxbury, 2015; Jing et al., 2013; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). The findings from this study regarding investors’ perceptions and emotions of using the diversification strategy may be used to develop teaching tools for investors, focusing on those thoughts and feelings about making prudent investment decisions (Duxbury, 2015; Jing et al., 2013; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). Participants’ investments behaviors, where their emotional feelings influenced their investment decisions, are consistent with behavioral finance theory (Chu et al., 2012; Duxbury, 2015; Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013; Van de Laar & de Neubourg, 2006). The participants’ investment behaviors cannot be explained by the conventional finance theory, which assumes that all investors make rational investment decisions (Chu et al., 2012; Duxbury, 2015; Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013; Van de Laar & de Neubourg, 2006). Positive versus negative emotions appear to have different impacts on investor behavior (Duxbury, 2015; Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Myeong-Gu & Barrett, 2007). Emotional feelings, negative or positive, when making investment decisions influenced the investment decision (Myeong-Gu & Barrett, 2007). The implication is that participants’ positive emotional feelings influenced their investment decisions to increase returns and reduce risks. This finding means that participants who demonstrate emotional feelings, when making decision, is a normal process as supported in previous studies’ findings (Duxbury, 2015; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). 126 However, other research has suggested negative emotions lead to more rational investing behavior (Chu et al., 2012; Van de Laar & de Neubourg, 2006). The authors’ findings contradict the results in this study. Financial researchers should acknowledge that consistent with behavioral finance theory, investors demonstrate emotional feelings during investment decisions (Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). Moreover, financial planners and financial researchers should recognize that investors’ perceptions of the strategy influenced their investment decisions, which could lead to making profitable or risky investment decisions (Duxbury, 2015; Jing et al., 2013; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). Financial researchers should know that different emotional feelings positive or negative, influence investors differently to make beneficial or less than optimal investment decisions (Duxbury, 2015; Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013). Acknowledging and understanding that investors’ investment behaviors are consistent with behavioral finance theory may help researchers to better understand the investors’ investment behaviors, thus incorporating investors’ investment behaviors in the analysis of investment outcomes to be able to develop effective investment tools, which may help investors make profitable investment decisions. This study contributes to knowledge and literature by extending application of behavioral finance theory to the MPT, geographical diversification strategy. Research Question 2: How do U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for increasing investment returns? The findings of this study showed that participants perceived geographical diversification as an effective strategy, which increased their investment returns, which are consistent with findings of Haran et al. (2016), Hargis and Mei (2006), Markowitz (1959), Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Meriç, Jie, and Meriç (2016), Odier and Solnik (1993), Saiti, Bacha, 127 and Masih (2014), Solnik (1974), Srivastava (2007), and Torres García-Heras (2011) that the geographical diversification strategy helped to increase investment returns. Participants’ responses to their perceptions of continued use of the geographical diversification strategy to increase investment returns demonstrated the application of the behavioral finance theory to the geographical diversification strategy (Fenzel & Pelzmann, 2012; Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Jing et al., 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013; Shiller, 2003; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). Specifically, participants’ positive perceptions of the emerging and growing foreign markets were based on the rapid returns on investments in those markets. Participants perceived the real estate and oil markets as providing investment stability, hence the participants’ preference for using the geographical diversification strategy to increase investment returns. A study conducted by Srivastava (2007) indicated that investors, who diversified their investments on the emerging markets, increased their investment returns. Specifically, Saiti, Bacha, and Masih (2014) examined whether the Islamic stock indices provided a special avenue for the US-based investors’ investments in Islamic countries. Saiti et al. (2014) found that the investment in Islamic countries provided better diversification benefits of increasing returns compared to the far East countries (e.g., China, Hong Kong, Indonesia, Japan, Malaysia, North Korea, South Korea, and Taiwan). Additionally, results from Meric et al. (2016) showed that the investors from the U.S., Canada, Germany, the U.K., and France had the greatest portfolio diversification benefits on investments in the Indonesian, Philippine, Malaysian, and Thai emerging stock markets. Meric et al.’s results indicate that the U.S. investors could increase their investment returns by diversifying on the Indonesian, Philippine, Malaysian, and Thai 128 emerging stock markets. Moreover, research conducted by Haran et al. (2016) examined the extent and nature of inter-relationships between three emerging real estate markets namely, the Czech Republic, Hungary, and Poland. Haran et al. (2016) found that there was lack of relationship (e.g., uniformity) among the markets. Haran et al.’s (2016) findings indicate that geographical diversification on those European emerging real estate markets was effective in terms of performance enhancement, thus, increasing investment returns. Furthermore, Mimouni et al. (2016) explored whether recent oil stocks and financial events have significant impact on the conditional correlations and diversification benefits of investments. Mimouni et al.’s (2016) findings showed that the correlation in Gulf Corporation Council (GCC) oil-producing countries (e.g., Saudi Arabia, Kuwait, the United Arab Emirates, Qatar, Bahrain, and Oman) stock markets remained low and constantly offered high diversification benefits. The findings indicate that geographical diversification on the emerging and oil-producing countries offered more potential for international diversification to increase investment returns (Haran et al., 2016). However, other research findings are contrary to the findings of this study. Bobillo et al. (2008), Cai et al. (2016), Chu-Sheng (2010), Gocmen (2010), Maldonado and Saunders (1981), and Raju and Khanapuri (2010), and Singh, Kumar, and Pandey (2010) found that due to the globalization of markets among countries, the benefits of geographical diversification in increasing investment returns have diminished. Specifically, Singh et al. (2010) examined how diversified investments in Asia, Europe, and North America performed with regards to interdependence and influence of one market on the other. Singh et al.’s (2010) findings showed that there was a greater 129 interdependency and influences of one market on another such that there was no trend of increased returns of the geographically diversified investments. Similarly, Raju and Khanapuri (2010) examined the performance of U.S. investors’ diversified financial investments in six Asian markets (e.g., KOSPI of South Korea, Shanghai Composite, SSEC of China, KLSE Composite of Malaysia, QKSE Composite of Indonesia, SET of Thailand, and NSE Nifiy of India). The findings from Raju and Khanapuri (2010) showed that investments that were diversified in the Asian financial markets did not increase returns nor did the diversification reduce risks. Moreover a study conducted by Cai et al. (2016) found that investors incurred higher operating costs as the level of diversification increased. Cai et al.’s (2016) results suggest a potential tradeoff between economic gains (e.g., market shares or interest margins) and operating costs due to expansion of investments across different countries. Results indicated that the geographical diversification has a positive but insignificant impact on return on asset (ROA) (Cai et al., 2016). The differences among advocates of the strategies and challenges associated with internationalization of financial markets have prompted interest in exploring and understanding investors’ behaviors with regards to using a strategy that might not increase investment returns (Fenzel & Pelzmann, 2012; Shiller, 2003). The findings of this study showed that participants’ positive perceptions of the emerging and growing foreign markets were due to stability, rapid returns, and specific industries (e.g., real estate or oil). The findings of this study showed that participants’ investment behaviors of using the geographical diversification strategy were consistent with behavioral finance theory. The findings of this study showed that participants’ perception of geographical 130 diversification strategy to increase investment returns influenced their investment decisions. Studies conducted by Harras and Somette (2011) and Rose et al. (2010) found that investors’ positive perceptions of investment outcomes influenced their investment decisions to invest more. Similarly, findings from Jing et al. (2013) and Yu and Xiaosong (2015) showed that investors’ confidence in the diversification strategy also influenced investment decisions. Moreover, Hoffmann, Post, and Pennings (2015) found when investors perceived investment returns as having higher and upward expectations; they invested more of their portfolios in such investments. The findings of this study have practical implications for investors, financial planners, and researchers because the participants’ use of geographical diversification strategy in real estate and oil industries on emerging and growing markets helped increased their investment returns (Haran et al., 2016; Meriç et al., 2016). The practical implications for the findings of this study are that when the geographical diversification strategy is appropriately applied, the use of the strategy might result in increasing investment returns (Haran et al., 2016; Meriç et al., 2016). The findings of this study explain that when the geographical diversification strategy is appropriately applied, in diversified investment in the real estate and oil industries on emerging and growing markets, the strategy might help investors increase investment returns. The implications of the findings are based on Haran et al. (2016), Hargis and Mei (2006), Markowitz (1959), Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Meriç et al. (2016), Odier and Solnik (1993), Saiti, Bacha, and Masih (2014), Solnik (1974), Srivastava (2007), and Torres GarcíaHeras’ (2011) findings that the geographical diversification strategy helped to increase investment returns. Therefore, investors should carefully analyze their emotions and 131 financial information while making investment decisions, which could lead to increasing investment returns (Fenzel & Pelzmann, 2012; Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Jing et al., 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013; Shiller, 2013; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). The findings of this study are consistent with behavioral finance theory because investors’ perceptions of the geographical diversification strategy influenced their decision-making process, and thus, their investment behaviors cannot be explained by the conventional finance theory (Fenzel & Pelzmann, 2012; Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013; Shiller, 2003; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). For example, Harras and Somette (2011) and Rose et al. (2010) found that investors’ positive perceptions of investment outcomes influenced their investment decisions to invest more. Similarly, investors’ confidence in the diversification strategy also influenced investment decisions (Jing et al., 2013; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). The findings of this study showed that participants’ perception of geographical diversification strategy to increase investment returns influenced their investment decisions. The findings of this study are consistent with some previous studies that found that perception of confidence in the markets motivated investors to make investment decisions (Hoffmann et al., 2015; Jing et al., 2013). In line with the conclusions of this study, when investors perceived investment returns as having higher and upward expectations, they invested more of their portfolios in such investments (Hoffmann et al., 2015). Therefore, in order to understand the investors investment behaviors, financial researchers should recognize that investors’ investment behaviors are consistent with behavioral finance theory (Fenzel & Pelzmann, 132 2012; Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Jing et al., 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013; Shiller, 2003; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). Research Question 3: How do U.S. investors describe their perceptions of geographical diversification as a strategy for reducing investment risks? Participants in this study perceived the geographical diversification is an effective investment strategy for reducing investment risks. The findings in this study are consistent with findings of Haran et al. (2016), Hargis and Mei (2006), Markowitz (1959), Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Meriç, Jie, and Meriç (2016), Odier and Solnik (1993), Saiti, Bacha, and Masih (2014), Solnik (1974), Srivastava (2007), and Torres García-Heras (2011), because the investors perceived the geographical diversification strategy as effective in reducing their investment risks. Participants’ responses to their perceptions of continued use of the geographical diversification strategy to reduce investment risks demonstrated their application of behavioral finance to the geographical diversification strategy. Specifically, participants’ positive perceptions of the emerging and growing foreign markets were based on the ability to diversify the investment on the markets to reduce investment risks. Participants perceived the real estate and oil markets as providing investment stability hence the participants’ preference for using the geographical diversification strategy to reduce investment risks. A study conducted by Masron and Fereidouni (2010) examined the portfolio diversification benefits of the housing industry and the relationship between housing performance and inflation in Iran. Using the riskto-reward ratio, Masron and Fereidouni (2010) examined the risk-adjusted performance of housing and other financial assets. Masron and Fereidouni (2010) found that diversification in the housing industry provided more benefits than risks and investments 133 in the housing industry, which resulted in lowest risk-to-reward ratio. In another study, Torres García-Heras (2011) examined the effectiveness of using geographical diversification to reduce risk by analyzing the credit default swap (CDS) markets in the U.S., France, Germany, PIIS (e.g., Portugal, Ireland, Italy, Greece and Spain), and Mexico and South America (e.g., Argentina, Brazil, Chile, Columbia and Peru). Torres García-Heras (2011) found that the South American countries experienced lower risk with diversified investments than most developed European countries, especially Spain. Similarly, a study conducted by Mimouni et al. (2016) found that the correlation in Gulf Corporation Council (GCC) oil-producing countries (e.g., Saudi Arabia, Kuwait, the United Arab Emirates, Qatar, Bahrain, and Oman) stock markets remained low and constantly offered high diversification benefits for reducing investment risks. The findings from Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Mimouni et al. (2016), and Torres GarcíaHeras (2011) indicated that the geographical diversification on the emerging and oilproducing countries offered more potential for international diversification to reduce investment risks. Additionally, the findings from the Yavas and Dedi (2016) showed that the geographical diversification investments on Turkish and Russian markets were exposed to more volatility than Austria, Germany and Poland markets. Yavas and Dedi (2016) findings indicated that geographical diversification on Austria, Germany, and Poland’s markets had less risk and could be used to reduce investment risks. However other research findings are contrary to the findings of this study. Cai et al. (2016), Chu-Sheng (2010), Gocmen (2010), Maldonado and Saunders (1981), Singh et al. (2010), and Raju and Khanapuri (2010) found that due to the globalization of markets among countries, the benefits of geographical diversification in reducing investment risks 134 might not be effective. Specifically, Singh et al. (2010) examined how diversified investments in Asia, Europe, and North America performed with regards to interdependence and influence of one market on the other. Singh et al.’s (2010) findings showed that there was a greater interdependency and influences of one market on another such that there was no trend of decreased risks of the geographically diversified investments. Similarly, Raju and Khanapuri (2010) examined the performance of U.S. investors’ diversified financial investments in six Asian markets. The findings from Raju and Khanapuri (2010) showed that investments that were diversified in the Asian financial markets did not result in reducing investment risks. Similarly, Cai et al. (2016) found that a potential tradeoff between economic gains (e.g., market shares or interest margins) and operating costs from the expansion of investments across different countries makes the geographical diversification strategy ineffective for increasing returns and reducing risks. Findings from Cai et al. (2016) showed that the geographical diversification had a positive but insignificant impact on return on asset (ROA). The differences among advocates of the strategies and challenges associated with the globalization of the financial markets have prompted interest in exploring and understanding investors’ behaviors with regards to using the geographical diversification strategy to reduce investment risks (Fenzel & Pelzmann, 2012; Shiller, 2003). The findings of this study showed that participants’ positive perceptions of the emerging and growing foreign markets were due to stability, rapid returns, and specific industries (e.g., real estate or oil). Results of this study showed that participants perceived the real estate and oil markets as providing investment stability, hence the participants’ preference for using the geographical diversification strategy to reduce investment risks. The 135 implications of the findings of this study indicate that participants’ investment behaviors are consistent with the application of behavioral finance theory to the geographical diversification strategy for reducing investment risks. The findings of this study showed that participants’ perception of geographical diversification strategy to reduce investment risks influenced their investment decisions. The findings of this study are supported by some studies conducted by Chassot, Hampl, and Wustenhagen, (2014), Hoffmann et al.’s (2015), Paruchuri and Misangy (2015), Roszkowski and Davey (2010), and Srivastava (2007), which found that investors’ perceptions of investment portfolios, as low risks, influenced their decisions to invest more of their portfolios. A study conducted by Roszkowski and Davey (2010) found that before the financial crisis, investors’ perceptions of risk were low and risk tolerance was high. However, after the financial crisis, investors’ perceptions of risk elevated from low to high and their risk tolerance changed from high to low (Roszkowski & Davey, 2010). Roszkowski and Davey’s findings indicate that investors’ financial decisions were influenced by their behavior. The findings of this study have practical implications for investors, financial planners, and researchers because the participants’ use of geographical diversification strategy, in real estate and oil industries, on emerging and growing markets helped reduced their investment risks (Chassot et al., 2014; Haran et al., 2016; Meriç et al., 2016; Paruchuri & Misangy, 2015). The practical implications of the findings of this study are that the appropriate application of the geographical diversification strategy may result in reducing investment risks (Haran et al., 2016; Hoffmann et al., 2015; Meriç et al., 2016; Paruchuri & Misangy, 2015). The findings of this study are supported by the results from Chassot et al. (2014), Hoffmann et al. (2015), Paruchuri and Misangy 136 (2015), and Roszkowski and Davey (2010), which are consistent with the application of behavioral finance theory to the geographical diversification strategy. Findings showed that investors’ perceptions of regulatory exposure risk influenced their investment decision-making (Chassot et al., 2014). For example, results showed that when the venture capitalist investors perceived the regulatory exposure as high risk, they made less investment decisions in the renewable energy companies (Chassot et al., 2014). On the contrary, when the venture capital investors perceived the regulatory risk as low risk, they invested more of their portfolios (Chassot et al., 2014). Similarly, Paruchuri and Misangy (2015) investigated how investors’ perceptions of corporate misconduct influenced general public opinion of the corporation. The authors examined 725 firms across U.S. financial markets by assessing 84 financial misconducts. Using the cumulative abnormal return (CAR), Paruchuri and Misangy (2015) found that when investors have perceptions of misconduct in financial corporations, such perceptions of misconduct are generalized to other corporations in the same financial industry creating a ripple effect. Moreover, Hoffmann et al. (2015) found that financial investors who perceived investment returns as having higher and upward expectations invested more of their portfolios in such investments. Hoffmann et al. (2015) found that investors who demonstrated high-risk tolerance held riskier investments for longer periods. Additionally, participants with higher risk perceptions had higher turnover than their counterparts with lower risk perceptions of investment. Furthermore, financial investors with higher revision of risk tolerance demonstrated higher buy-sell ratios than participants with higher revision of risk perception, who showed lower buy-sell ratios (Hoffmann et al., 2015). When investors had perceptions of lower risk on investments, 137 they made investment decisions of not only trading more but also holding their portfolios for an extended period of time (Hoffmann et al., 2015). The findings of this study contribute to behavioral finance that it is important for financial practitioners and researchers to understand how the investors’ perception influenced their investment decisions (Hoffmann et al., 2015). The findings of this study explain the practical implications that when the geographical diversification strategy is appropriately applied in diversified investments, specifically in real estate and oil industries on emerging and growing markets, the strategy may help investors reduce investment risks. These implications of this study’s findings are based on Haran et al. (2016), Hargis and Mei (2006), Markowitz (1959), Masron and Fereidouni (2010), Meriç, Jie, and Meriç (2016), Odier and Solnik (1993), Saiti, Bacha, and Masih (2014), Solnik (1974), Srivastava (2007), and Torres GarcíaHeras’ (2011) findings that the geographical diversification strategy helped to reduce investment risks. The implications of the findings are also premised on investors’ application of geographical diversification strategy in real estate and oil industries on emerging and growing markets to reduce investment risks (Haran et al., 2016; Meriç et al., 2016; Saiti et al., 2014; Yavas & Dedi, 2016). Finally, the findings of this study indicate that participants’ investment decisions are consistent with behavioral finance (Chassot et al., 2014; Duxbury, 2015; Hoffmann et al., 2015; Paruchuri & Misangy, 2015; Roszkowski & Davey, 2010). The findings of this study indicate that the participants’ use of geographical diversification in real estate and oil industries on emerging and growing markets helped reduced their investment risks (Haran et al., 2016; Meriç et al., 2016; Saiti et al., 2014; Yavas & Dedi, 2016). The implications of the 138 findings are that when participants had prudent perceptions of investment risks, their geographical diversification strategy might help reduce their investment risks (Haran et al., 2016; Meriç et al., 2016; Saiti et al., 2014; Yavas & Dedi, 2016). On the contrary, when the investors’ perceive potentially high risk investments as low risk, their investment strategy may result in high investment risks (Haran et al., 2016; Meriç et al., 2016; Saiti et al., 2014; Yavas & Dedi, 2016). Therefore, it is incumbent on investors to recognize the impact of their perceptions of the geographical diversification strategy and carefully analyze their perceptions of the strategy to be able to make investment decisions, which could lead to reducing investment risks. Moreover, financial planners and financial researchers should recognize that investors’ perceptions of the strategy influenced their investment decisions, which could lead to making profitable or risky investment decisions (Jing et al., 2013; Yu & Xiaosong, 2015). Research Question 4: How do U.S. investors explain their use of geographical diversification strategy, in the context of literature, which does not support the strategy? The findings of this study showed that participants believed geographical diversification strategies were working for them by increasing their investment returns and reducing their investment risks. Research suggests anchoring bias can lead to more irrational investing behavior because the investor ignores other information in favor of familiar information (Rose et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2011). While anchoring bias of the participants in this study led to increased returns and reduced risks, investment decisions based on anchoring bias could result in reduced returns and increased risks. The findings of this study showed that participants of this study believed they had adequate information on the geographical diversification strategy to make profitable investment 139 decisions. According to Pandit and Ken (2014) when investors believed that they have adequate information on financial products, they made more investment on such products. On the contrary, investors would make fewer purchases of shares and postpone more purchasing of shares when their knowledge on the shares is less (Pandit & Ken, 2014). The results of this study showed that participants did not plan to change their investing behavior, even though the majority of participants were not familiar with the financial literature suggesting that the geographical diversification strategy is ineffective. The findings showed that participants in this study valued their own experience over the financial literature. As a result of those beliefs, they had negative feelings about the financial literature, which contradicted their own experiences. The implications of the findings in this study are that participants’ investment decisions were influenced by their psychological bias (Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). The influence of investors’ decisions by their psychological biases as shown in this study cannot be explained by conventional finance theory approach that assumes investors who use geographical diversification strategy are rational investors (Friedman & Savage, 1948; Markowitz, 1959; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010). The participants’ investment behaviors are consistent with behavioral finance theory (Fenzel & Pelzmann, 2012; Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013; Shiller, 200; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). Advocates of behavioral finance theory consider how various psychological characteristics, such as anchoring biases, emotions, and perceptions influence financial decision-making processes of investors and financial planners (Hyoyoun & Wook, 2013; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). Financial investors’ decisions are 140 bias and in favor of familiar financial information over unfamiliar information (Lai et al., 2010). A study conducted by Chaarlas and Lawrence (2012) demonstrated that regardless of the available financial information, investors’ decisions were influenced by their anchoring bias. Similarly, as indicated by Gupta and Banik (2013), 95% of financial investors demonstrated psychological bias when making investment decisions. Participants in this study believed in the strategy, relied on their own experience, and avoided the financial literature, which contradicted their own experiences. The findings of this study are consistent with Yu and Xiaosong’s (2015) findings that investors’ decisions are based on individual cognitive prejudices rather than rational financial evidence. Ateş et al.’s (2016) findings showed that investors based their decisions on informal sources of financial information. Investors’ humanistic behaviors are important to researchers because understanding these behaviors helps researchers explore and develop effective strategies for financial investors. Financial investors should control psychological biases in order to make profitable and sound investment decisions and thereby become better investors (Suresh, 2013). The findings explain that investors need to recognize that their anchoring bias, during decision making, has the potential to influence them to make prudent or less than optimal investment decisions. Acknowledging, understanding, and modifying the behavioral biases of the financial investors and controlling such biases caused the investors to make profitable and sound investment decisions and thereby become better investors (Suresh, 2013). Recommendations for application The first recommendation for application of this study pertains to the influence of emotions on investment decisions. Positive or negative emotional feelings, during 141 investment decision-making, have different influences on investors’ decisions (Duxbury, 2015; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). The findings explain that participants in this study demonstrated positive emotional feelings when making decision that are consistent with previous studies’ findings (Duxbury, 2015; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). The findings show that participants in this study avoided making decision when their emotions were negative. However, other research has suggested negative emotions could lead to more rational investing behavior (Chu et al., 2012; Van de Laar & de Neubourg, 2006). It is recommended that investors critically assess their emotional feelings and regulate those emotions during decision-making in order to make beneficial investment decisions. This recommendation is based on the finding in this study and consistent with Fenton-O'Creevy et al.’s (2011) findings. Financial investors who critically analyzed their emotions and financial information available before making investment decisions were more likely to make profitable decisions (Fenton-O'Creevy et al., 2011). The responses from participants demonstrated the applicability of the behavioral finance theory to the geographical diversification strategy (Duxbury, 2015; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). The findings of this study contribute to literature by extending behavioral finance theory to the geographical diversification strategy (Duxbury, 2015; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). When investors analyzed their emotions before making investment decisions, the investors’ decisions might lead to increasing investment returns and reducing investment risks (Duxbury, 2015; Muradoglu & Harvey, 2012; Peteros & Maleyeff, 2013). Increased investment returns and reducing investment risks may help reduce or prevent financial crises that occurred in 2007-2008 (Chu-Sheng, 2010; 142 Geambasu et al., 2013). Investors’ failure to critically analyzed their emotions and financial information available before making investment decisions may result in decreased investment returns and increased investment risks (Chu-Sheng, 2010; Geambasu et al., 2013). The second recommendation for application of this study is regarding the effectiveness of geographical diversification and investors continued use of the strategy as a way of increasing returns and reducing risks. The findings of this study showed that participants’ positive perceptions of the emerging and growing foreign markets were due to stability, rapid returns, and specific industries such as real estate or oil markets (Haran el al., 2016; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010; Mimouni et al., 2016; Roszkowski & Davey, 2010; Saiti et al., 2014; Srivastava, 2007; Torres García-Heras, 2011). Specifically, Haran et al. (2016) and Masron and Fereidouni (2010) found that geographical diversification in real estate industry in emerging and growing markets increased returns and reduce risks. Similarly, Mimouni et al.’s (2016) findings indicated geographical diversification in oil-producing emerging and growing markets had less risk and could be used to increase returns and reduce investment risks. Other research by Bobillo et al. (2008), Cai et al. (2016), Chu-Sheng (2010), Gocmen (2010), Maldonado and Saunders (1981), Singh et al. (2010), and Raju and Khanapuri (2010) found that the geographical diversification strategy was ineffective in increasing returns and reducing risk due to the interdependence of the global markets. The third recommendation for application in this study is regarding the investors’ investment decisions and their anchoring bias behaviors. The findings in this study showed that participants valued their own experience over the financial literature. As a 143 result of those beliefs, they had negative feelings about the financial literature, which contradicted their own experiences. This indicates that participants continued use of geographical diversification was influenced by their anchoring bias of the strategy, which is consistent with results from Chaarlas and Lawrence (2012), Gupta and Banik (2013), and Kapor (2014). On the contrary, Rose et al. (2010) and Wang et al. (2011) found that anchoring bias could lead to more irrational investing behaviors. Although investment decisions of participants in this study led to increased returns and reduced risks, investment decisions based on anchoring bias could result in reduced returns and more risks. Anchoring biases continues to influence financial investors’ decision-making processes, regardless of the value of the financial information available to investors (Chaarlas & Lawrence, 2012). The investors’ investment behaviors, where their decisions are influenced by their own experience over financial literature are consistent with behavioral finance (Chaarlas & Lawrence, 2012; Chu-Sheng, 2010; Geambasu et al., 2013). The researcher recommends that investors should recognize, analyze, and control their emotional feelings, perceptions, and psychological biases in order to make increasingly beneficial investment decisions. Investors’ failure to critically analyze their psychological biases before making investment decisions could result in decreasing investment returns and increasing investment risks (Chu-Sheng, 2010; Geambasu et al., 2013). Recommendation for future research The recommendation for future research of this study is related to the participants’ responses that the geographical diversification strategy, in real estate and oil industries, on emerging and growing markets helped increased their investment returns and reduced 144 their investment risks (Haran el al., 2016; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010; Mimouni et al., 2016; Roszkowski & Davey, 2010; Saiti et al., 2014). The researcher recommends that future quantitative studies should be conducted to evaluate the effectiveness of geographical diversification on the real estate and oil markets of the emerging and growing countries in Africa, Asia, Eastern Europe, and South America. Specifically, a quantitative study that provides an estimation of time period that investors take to increase their returns and reduce their risk and by how much as a results of their investments in real estate and oil industries, on emerging and growing African, Asian, Eastern European, and South American markets. The outcomes of the quantitative research study may contribute to financial literature by providing quantitative information on the investors’ perceptions of increased returns and reduced risks in addition to helping to strengthen the geographical diversification strategy (Haran el al., 2016; Masron & Fereidouni, 2010; Mimouni et al., 2016). Conclusions Investors continued to use geographical diversification as an investment strategy to increase investment returns and reduce investment risks, in spite of financial literature that says the strategy is not effective. The participants continued use of the strategy is because investors perceived geographical diversification as an effective strategy to increase investment returns and reduce investment risks. Moreover, investors make investment decisions based on their emotional feelings and psychological biases. The investors’ perceptions of the strategy, their emotional feeling when making investment decisions and their psychological bias in favor of the strategy, influenced their investment decisions. A qualitative case study was conducted to understand the U.S. investors’ 145 perceptions and emotions of using geographical diversification, as an investment strategy, when evidenced does not support it use. Based on the findings of this study, the researcher made three major conclusions, which incorporated practical and researchbased implications. The majority of participants in this study thought having positive emotional feelings during investment decision-making would lead to positive investment behavior, thus, they made investment decisions when they felt positive emotions and avoided decisions when they felt negative. The researcher was able to draw conclusion with implications for investors, financial practitioners, and future research. First, investors should recognize that their emotional feelings influenced them during investment decisions. Secondly, financial practitioners and researchers should acknowledge that different emotional feelings positive or negative, influence investors differently to make beneficial or less than optimal investment decisions. Thirdly, investors, financial practitioners, and researchers should understand that geographical diversification in real estate and oil industries, on emerging and growing markets, may help investors increase investment returns and reduce investment risks. The final implication is that anchoring bias of the participants in this study led to increase returns and reduced risks. According to research conducted by Akerlof and Shiller (2010), Duxbury (2015), Gupta and Banik (2013), Kahneman (2011), Mitroi and Oproiu (2014), and Tekçe et al. (2016), financial investors have a tendency to make investment decisions based on psychological biases. Understanding and regulating investors’ psychological biases may help investors make profitable investment decisions (Duxbury 2015; Mitroi & Oproiu, 2014; Tekçe et al., 2016). 146 Four recommendations were made for investors, financial practitioners, and researchers. First, it is recommended that investors critically assess their emotional feelings and regulate those emotions during decision-making in order to make beneficial investment decisions. Secondly, the researcher recommends that investors assess the emerging and growing real estate and oil markets before diversifying their portfolios on such markets in order to increase returns and reduce risks. Thirdly, the researcher suggests future quantitative studies should be conducted to evaluate the effectiveness of geographical diversification on the real estate and oil markets of the emerging and growing countries in Africa, Asia, Eastern Europe, and South America. Finally, investors should recognize, analyze, and control their emotional feelings, perceptions, and psychological biases in order to make more beneficial investment decisions. 147 References Agrrawal, P., & Borgman, R. (2010). What is wrong with this picture? A problem with comparative return plots on finance websites and a bias against income generating assets. The Journal of Behavioral Finance, 11(4), 195-210. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15427560.2010.526260 Ahrens, T., & Chapman, C. S. (2006). Doing qualitative field research in management accounting: Positioning data to contribute to theory. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 31(8), 819-841. doi:10.1016/j.aos.2006.03.007 Akerlof, G., & Shiller, R. (2010). Animal spirits: How human psychology drives the economy, and why it matters for global capitalism. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Aspara, J. (2013). The role of product and brand perceptions in stock investing: Effects on investment considerations, optimism and confidence. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 14(3), 195-212. doi:10.1080/15427560.2013.819803 Ateş, S., Coşkun, A., Şahin, M. A., & Demircan, M. L. (2016). Impact of financial literacy on the behavioral biases of individual stock investors: Evidence from Borsa Istanbul. Business & Economics Research Journal, 7(3), 1-19. doi:10.20409/berj.2016321805 Baker, M., Coval, J., & Stein, J. C. (2007). Corporate financing decisions when investors take the path of least resistance. Journal of Financial Economics, 84, 266-298. doi:10.1016/j.jfineco.2006.03.005 Baker, M., Wurgler, J., & Yuan, Y. (2012). Global, local, and contagious investor sentiment. Journal of Financial Economics, 104(2), 272-287. doi:10.1016j.jfineco.2011.11.002 Baxter, P., & Jack, S. (2008). Qualitative case study methodology: Study design and implementation for novice researchers. The Qualitative Report, 13(4), 544-559. Retrieved from http://nsuworks.nova.edu/tqr/vol13/iss4/2 Blay, A. D., Kadous, K, & Sawers, K. (2012). The impact of risk and effect on information search efficiency. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 117(1), 80-87. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.09.003 Bobillo, A. M., Iturriaga, F. L., & Gaite, F. T. (2008). Firm performance and international diversification: An international analysis. The International Business Review, 9(2), 84-91. doi:10.1016/j.ibusrev.2010.03.006 148 Brinkmann, S., & Kvale, S. (2005). Confronting the ethics of qualitative research. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 18(2), 157-181. doi:10.1080/10720530590914789 Cai, W., Xu, F., & Zeng, C. (2016). Geographical diversification and bank performance: Evidence from China. Economics Letters, 147, 96-98. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2016.08.022 Chaarlas, L. J., & Lawrence, A. (2012). Behavioral finance a boon to investors. Journal of Finance, Accounting & Management, 3(1), 32-44. Retrieved from http://eds.a.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer Chandra, A. (2009). Individual investors' trading behavior and the competence effect. IUP Journal of Behavioral Finance, 6, 56-71. Retrieved from http://www.iupindia.in/1208/Ijbf.asp Chassot, S., Hampl, N., & Wüstenhagen, R. (2014). Original research article: When energy policy meets free-market capitalists: The moderating influence of worldviews on risk perception and renewable energy investment decisions. Energy Research & Social Science, 3, 143-151. doi:10.1016/j.erss.2014.07.013 Cheng, P., & Roulac, S. E. (2007). Measuring the effectiveness of geographical diversification. Journal of Real Estate Portfolio Management, 13(1), 29-44. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/docview/197769527?accountid=28180 Chu-Sheng, T. (2010). Foreign exchange risk and risk exposure in the Japanese stock market. Managerial Finance, 36(6), 511-524. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/proxya.ncu.edu/docview/312275970?accountid=28180 Chu, W., Im, M., & Jang, H. (2012). Overconfidence and emotion regulation failure: How overconfidence leads to the disposition effect in consumer investment behavior. Journal of Financial Services Marketing, 17(1), 96-116. doi:10.1057/fsm.2012.7 Cotugno, M., & Stefanelli, V. (2012). Geographical and product diversification during instability financial period: Good or bad for banks? International Research Journal of Finance and Economics, 85, 87-100. Retrieved from http://www.internationalresearchjournaloffinanceandeconomics.com/ Creswell, J. W. (2009). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods approaches (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Cozby, P. C., & Bates, S. C. (2012). Methods in behavior research. Boston, MA: McCaw Higher Education. 149 Delcoure, N. (2010). Diversification benefits: Correlation and return gaps. International Review of Applied Financial Issues & Economics, 2(3), 597-607. Retrieved from http://www.irafie.com/content4.php Dow, S. C. (2011). Cognition, market sentiment and financial instability. Cambridge Journal of Economics, 35(2), 233-249. doi:10.1093/cje/beq029 Draper, A., & Swift, J. (2011). Qualitative research in nutrition and dietetics: Data collection issues. Journal of Human Nutrition & Dietetics, 24(1), 3-12. doi:10.1111/j.1365-277X.2010.01117.x Duxbury, D. (2015). Behavioral finance: Insights from experiments II: Biases, moods and emotions. Review of Behavioral Finance, 7(2), 151-175. doi:10.1108/RBF09-2015-0037 Farkas, M., & Murthy, U. S. (2014). Nonprofessional investors' perceptions of the incremental value of continuous auditing and continuous controls monitoring: An experimental investigation. International Journal of Accounting Information Systems, 15, 102-121. doi:10.1016/j.accinf.2013.11.002 Fenton-O'Creevy, M., Soane, E., Nicholson, N., & Willman, P. (2011). Thinking, feeling and deciding: The influence of emotions on the decision making and performance of traders. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32(8), 1044-1061. doi:10.1002/job.720 Fenzel, T. & Pelzmann, L. (2012). Psychological and social forces behind aggregate financial market behavior. The Journal of Behavioral Finance, 13, 56-65. doi:10.1080/15427560.2012.655383 Flick. U. (2007). Designing qualitative research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Friedman, M., & Savage, L. J. (1948). The utility analysis of choices involving risk. Journal of Political Economy, 56, 279-304. doi:10.1016/j.accinf.2013.11.002 Gambetti, E., & Giusberti, F. (2012). The effect of anger and anxiety traits on investment decisions. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(6), 1059-1069. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2012.07.001 Garcia, M. (2013). Financial education and behavioral finance: New insights into the role of information in financial decisions. Journal of Economic Surveys, 27(2), 297-315. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6419.2011.00705.x 150 Geambasu, C., Sova, R., Jianu, I., & Geambasu, L. (2013). Risk measurement in post modern portfolio theory: Differences from modern portfolio theory. Economic Computation & Economic Cybernetics Studies & Research, 47(1), 113-132. Retrieved from http://eds.a.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer? Giorgi, A. (2009). The descriptive phenomenological method in psychology. Pittsburgh, PA: Duquesne University Press. Glesne, C. (2011). Becoming qualitative researchers: An introduction (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson. Gocmen, T. (2010). Re-examining the diversification and welfare effects of joint ventures: New empirical evidence. The Journal of Applied Business and Economics, 11(2), 65-81. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/815978211?accountid=28180 Golafshani, N. (2003). Understanding reliability and validity in qualitative research. The Qualitative Report, 8(4), 597-607. Retrieved from http://www.nova.edu/ssss/QR/QR8-4/golafshani.pdf Gonzalez-Perez, M. T. (2015). Model-free volatility indexes in the financial literature: A review. International Review of Economics and Finance, 40, 141-159. doi:10.1016/j.iref.2015.02.018 Guion, L. A., Diehl, D. C., & McDonald, D. (2011). Triangulation: Establishing the validity of qualitative studies (Publication No. FCS6014). Retrieved from University of Florida, Institute of Food and Agricultural Sciences (IFAS) website: http://www.edis.ifas.ufl.edu/fy394 Gupta, A., & Banik, S. (2013). Investors' psychological biases toward stock market investment with special reference to Bangladesh. International Journal of Business Insights & Transformation, 6(2), 38-43. Retrieved from http://www.ijbit.org/v6i2.php Haocheng, W., Jian, Z., Limin, W., & Shuyi, L. (2014). Emotion and investment returns: Situation and personality as moderatos in stock market. Social Behavior & Personality: An International Journal, 42(4), 561-569. doi:10.2224/sbp.2014.42.4.561 Haran, M., McCord, M., Davis, P., McCord, J., Lauder, C., & Newell, G. (2016). European emerging real estate markets. Journal of Property Investment & Finance, 34(1), 27-50. doi:10.1108/JPIF-04-2015-0024 151 Hargis, K., & Mei, J. (2006). Is country diversification better than industry diversification? European Financial Management, 12, 319-340. doi:10.1111/j.1354-7798.2006.00323.x Hari, K., & Ayappan, K. (2014). Choice and satisfaction: Mutual fund investment. SCMS Journal of Indian Management, 11(4), 18-28. Retrieved from http://eds.a.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer Harras, G., & Sornette, D. (2011). How to grow a bubble: A model of myopic adapting agents. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 80(1), 137-152. doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011.03.003 Harvey, R., Bolton, P., Wilse-Samson, L., An, L., & Samama, F. (2014). Barriers to longterm cross-border investing: A survey of institutional investor perceptions. Rotman International Journal of Pension Management, 7(2), 50-59. doi:10.3138/ripjm.7.2.50 Hoffmann, A. I., Post, T., & Pennings, J. E. (2013). Individual investor perceptions and behavior during the financial crisis. Journal of Banking & Finance, 37(1), 60-74. doi:10.1016/j.jbankfin.2012.08.007 Hoffmann, A. I., Post, T., & Pennings, J. E. (2015). How investor perceptions drive actual trading and risk-taking behavior. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 16(1), 94104.doi:10.1080/15427560.2015.1000332 Hommes, C., & Wagener, F. (2009). Complex evolutionary systems in behavioral finance. In T. Hens & K. Schenk-Hopp (Eds.), Handbook of financial markets: Dynamics and evolution (pp. 218-264). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier. Hon-Snir, S., Kudryavstev, A., & Cohen, G. (2012). Stock market investors: Who is more rational, and who relies on intuition? International Journal of Economics and Finance, 4(5), 56-72. Retrieved from http://www.ccsenet.org/journal/index.php/ijef/issue/view/470 Hunter, L., & Leahey, E. (2008). Collaborative research in sociology: Trends and contributing factors. The American Sociologist, 39(4), 290-306. doi:10.1007/s12108-008-9042-1 Hyoyoun, P., & Wook, S. (2013). Behavioral finance: A survey of the literature and recent development. Seoul Journal of Business, 19(1), 3-42. Retrieved from http://eds.a.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer Jing, G. U., Chen, H., & Zhang, X. (2013). Influence of psychological and emotional factors on the venture enterprise value and the investment decision making. Procedia Computer Science, 17, 919-929. doi:10.1016/j.procs.2013.05.117 152 Kağnici, D. (2014). Reflections of multicultural counseling course: A qualitative study with counseling students and counselors. Education Science: Theory & Practice, 14(1), 53-62. doi:10.12738/estp.2014.1.1965 Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus & Giroux. Kahneman, D. (2003). Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. The American Economic Review, 93(5), 1449-1475. Retrieved from http://eds.a.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer? Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society, 47(2), 263-291. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/docview/214665840?accountid=28180 Kapor, P. (2014). Behavioral finance. Megatrend Review, 11(2), 73-94. Retrieved from http://eds.b.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer Kashefi, J. (2006). The effect of the euro on European equity markets and international diversification. Journal of International Business Research, 5(2), 1-21. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/docview/215462379?accountid=28180 Kaufmann, C., & Weber, M. (2013). Sometimes less is more: The influence of information aggregation on investment decisions. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 95, 20-33. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2013.08.005 Keynes, J. (1964). The general theory of employment, interest, and money. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace. Konstantinidis, A., Katarachia, A., Borovas, G., & Voutsa, E. (2012). From efficient market hypothesis to behavioral finance: Can behavioral finance be the new dominant model for investing. Scientific Bulletin-Economic Science, 11(2), 16-26. Retrieved from http://economic.upit.ro/buletin_cd/Buletin_2012_2.pdf Kvale, S. (2012). Doing interviews [Kindle version]. Retrieved from http://www.amazon.com/Kindle-eBooks/b?ie=UTF8&node=154606011 Kvale, S., & Brinkmann, S. (2009). Interviews: Learning the craft of qualitative research interviewing. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Lai, H., Chen, C., & Huang, C. (2010). Technical analysis, investment psychology and liquidity pension: Evidence from Taiwan stock exchange. Emerging Markets Finance & Trade, 46(5), 18-38. doi:10.2753/REE1540-496X460502 153 Latif, M., Arshad, S., Fatima, M., & Farooq, S. (2011). Market efficiency, market anomalies, causes, evidences, and some behavioral aspects of market anomalies. Research Journal of Finance and Accounting, 2(9), 1-13. Retrieved from http://www.iiste.org/Journals/index.php/RJFA/index Lazarus, R, & Lazarus, B. N. (1994). Passion and reason: Making sense of our emotions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Lee, C. J., & Andrade, E. B. (2015). Fear, excitement, and financial risk-taking. Cognition & Emotion, 29(1), 178-187. doi:10.1080/02699931.2014.898611 Leedy, P. D. & Ormrod, J. E. (2010). Practical research: Planning and design. New York, NY: Pearson Publications. Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications. Lo, A, & Repin, D. (2002). The psychophysiology of real-time financial risk processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 323-339. doi:10.1162/089892902317361877 Maldonado, R., & Saunders, A. (1981). International portfolio diversification and the inter-temporal stability of international stock market relationships, 1957-78. Financial Management, 10(4), 54-63. doi:10.2307/3665219 Markowitz, H. M. (1952). Portfolio selection. The Journal of Finance, 7(1), 77-91. doi:10.2307/2975974 Markowitz, H. M. (1959). Portfolio selection: Efficient diversification of investments. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. Masron, T. A., & Fereidouni, H. G. (2010). Performance and diversification benefits of housing investment in Iran. International Journal of Business and Economics, 2(4), 7-11. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/820912432?accountid=28180 Mealer, M., & Jones, J. (2014). Methodological and ethical issues related to qualitative telephone interviews on sensitive topics. Nurse Researcher, 21(4), 32-37. doi:10.7748/nr2014.03.21.4.32.e1229 Meriç, İ., Jie, D., & Meriç, G. (2016). Global portfolio diversification with emerging stock markets. EMAJ: Emerging Markets Journal, 6(1), 58-62. doi:10.5195/emaj.2016.88 154 Miccolis, J. A., & Goodman, M. (2012). Next generation investment risk management: Putting the modern back in modern portfolio theory. Journal of Financial Planning, 25(1), 44-51. Retrieved from http://www.fpanet.org/journal/ Mimouni, K., Charfeddine, L., & Al-Azzam, M. (2016). Do oil producing countries offer international diversification benefits? Evidence from GCC countries. Economic Modeling, 57, 263-280. doi:10.1016/j.econmod.2016.05.001 Mitroi, A. (2013). Behavioral finance: Biased individual investment decision making; like the company but dislike the investment. Theoretical & Applied Economics, 21(1), 63-74. Retrieved from http://eds.b.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer Mitroi, A., & Oproiu, A. (2014). Behavioral finance: new research trends, socionomics and investor emotions. Theoretical & Applied Economics, 21(4), 153-166. Retrieved from http://eds.a.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer Muradoglu, G., & Harvey, N. (2012). Behavioral finance: the role of psychological factors in financial decisions. Review of Behavioral Finance, 4(2) 68-80. doi:10.1106/19405971211284862 Myeong-Gu, S., & Barrett, L. (2007). Being emotional during decision-making-good or bad? An empirical investigation Academy of Management Journal, 50(4), 923940. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2007.26279217 Nikiforow, M. (2010). Does training on behavioral finance influence fund managers' perception and behavior?. Applied Financial Economics, 20(7), 515-528. doi:10.1080/0960310090345983 Nofsinger, J. R. (2005). The psychology of investing (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc. O’Connor, L. G. (2013). The information seeking and use behaviors of retired investors. Journal of Librarianship and Information Science, 45(1), 3-22. doi:10.1177/0961000611434761 Odier, P., & Solnik, B. (1993). Lessons for international asset allocation. Financial Analysts Journal, 49(2), 63-77. doi:10.2469/faj.v49.n2.63 Pandit, A., & Ken, Y. (2014). Psychological tendencies in an emerging market: A study of individual investors in India . Journal of Developing Areas, 48(3), 129-148. doi:10.1353/jda.2014.0049 155 Paruchuri, S., & Misangy, V. F. (2015) Investor perceptions of financial misconduct: The heterogeneous contamination of bystander firms. Academy of Management Journal, 58(1), 169-194. doi:10.5465/amj.2012.0704 Patterson, F. M., & Daigler, R. T. (2014). The abnormal psychology of investment performance. Review Of Financial Economics, 23(2), 55-63. doi:10.1016/j.rfe.2013.08.004 Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative research and evaluation methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. Paulus, M., & Yu, A. (2012). Emotion and decision-making: Affect-driven belief systems in anxiety and depression. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(9), 476-483. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.07.009 Peteros, R., & Maleyeff, J. (2013). Application of behavioral finance concepts to investment decision-making: Suggestions for improving investment education courses. International Journal of Management, 30(2), 249-261. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/docview/1315212889?accountid=281 80 Rahim, A. M., & Masih, M. (2016). Portfolio diversification benefits of Islamic investors with their major trading partners: Evidence from Malaysia based on MGARCH DCC and wavelet approaches. Economic Modeling, 54, 425-438. doi:10.1016/j.econmod.2015.12.033 Raju, G. A., & Khanapuri, H. R. (2010). Is the west’s money really diversified in the east? A closer look at portfolio diversification benefits in Asia. Journal of Wealth Management, 13(3), 25-37. doi:10.3905/jwm.2010.13.3.025 Resnik, B. L. (2010). Did modern portfolio theory fail investors in the credit crisis? CPA Journal, 80(10), 10-12. Retrieved from http://www.nysscpa.org/publicationsa.htm Reuter, C.H. (2009). A survey of culture and finance. Retrieved from http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=1317324 Reyes, R. L. (2007). The psychological meanings of money (Order No. 3247237). Available from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global. (304701988). Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/docview/304701988?accountid=28180 Ritter, J. (2003). Behavioral finance. Pacific-Basin Finance Journal, 11(4), 429-437. Retrieved from http://bear.cba.ufl.edu/ritter 156 Robinson, O. C. (2014). Sampling in interview-based qualitative research: A theoretical and practical guide. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 11(1), 25-41. doi:10.1080/14780887.2013.801543 Rose, J. M., Norman, C., & Rose, A. M. (2010). Perceptions of investment risk associated with material control weakness pervasiveness and disclosure detail. Accounting Review, 85(5), 1787-1807. doi:10.2308/accr.2010.85.5.1787 Rosillo, R., De la Fuente, D., & Brugos, J. (2013). Technical analysis and the Spanish stock exchange: Testing the RSI, MACD, momentum and stochastic rules using Spanish market companies. Applied Financial Economics, 45, 1541-1550. Retrieved from http://www.tandfonline.com/toc/rafe20/current Roszkowski, M. J., & Davey, G. (2010). Risk perception and risk tolerance changes attributable to the 2008 economic crisis: A subtle but critical difference. Journal of Financial Service Professionals, 64(4), 42-53. Retrieved from http://eds.a.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer? Sahi, S. K., Arora, A. P., & Dhameja, N. (2013). An exploratory inquiry into the psychological biases in financial investment behavior. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 14(2), 94-103. doi:10.1080/15427560.2013.790387 Saiti, B., Bacha, O. I., & Masih, M. (2014). The diversification benefits from Islamic investment during the financial turmoil: The case for the US-based equity investors. Borsa Istanbul Review, 14, 196-211. doi:10.1016/j.bir.2014.08.002 Saldaña, J. (2009). The coding manual for qualitative researchers. London, England: Sage Publications. Saldaña, J. (2013). The coding manual for qualitative researchers. Thousands Oak, CA: Sage Publications. Salganik, M. J., & Heckathorn, D. D. (2004). Sampling and estimation in hidden populations using respondent-driven sampling. Sociological Methodology, 34(1), 193-239. doi:10.1111/j.0081-1750.2004.00152.x Schwarz, N. (2002). Feelings as information: Moods influence judgments and processing strategies. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, and D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 534-547). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Seidman, I. (2005). Interviewing as qualitative research: A guide for researchers in education and the social sciences (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Teachers College Press. 157 Seidman, I. (2013). Interviewing as qualitative research: A guide for researchers in education and the social sciences (4th ed.). New York, NY: Teachers College Press. Sewell, M. (2007). Behavioral finance. Retrieved from http://www.behaviouralfinance.net/behavioural-finance.pdf Shefrin, H. (2000). Beyond greed and fear. New York, NY: Oxford University. Shefrin, H. (2007). Behavioral corporate finance. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Shefrin, H. (2013). Building on Kahneman's insights in the development of behavioral finance. Loyola University Chicago Law Journal, 44(5), 1401-1421. Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/docview/1535065908?accountid=2810 Shenton, A. K. (2004). Strategies for ensuring trustworthiness in qualitative research projects. Education for Information, 22(2), 63-75. Retrieved from http://eds.b.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer? Sherry, M. (2008). Identity. In L. M. Given (Ed.), The sage encyclopedia of qualitative research methods (pp. 86-89). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Shiller, R. (2003). From efficient markets theory to behavioral finance. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 17(1), 83-104. Retrieved from http://eds.a.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer Shiv, B., Loewenstein, G., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). Investment behavior and the negative side of emotion. Psychological Science (Wiley-Blackwell), 16(6), 435-439. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01553.x Singh, P., Kumar, B., & Pandey, A. (2010). Price and volatility spillover across North American, European and Asian stock markets. International Review of Financial Analysis, 19, 55-64. doi:10.1016/j.irfa.2009.11.001 Smith, A., & Harvey, T. (2011). Test of a theory: An empirical examination of the changing nature of investor behavior. Journal of Management Policy and Practice, 12(3), 49-68. Retrieved from http://www.nabusinesspress.com/jmppopen.html Solnik, B. (1974). Why not diversify internationally rather than domestically? Financial Analysts Journal, 30(4), 48-54. doi:10.2469.faj.v30.n4.48 Srivastava, A. (2007). Cointegration of Asian markets with US markets: International diversification perspectives. Global Business Review, 8, 251-265. doi:10.1177/097215090700800205 158 Stake, R. E. (1995). The art of case study research. Thousand Oak, CA: Sage Publications. Sullivan, R. N. (2011). Deploying financial emotional intelligence. Financial Analysts Journal, 67(6), 4-10. Retrieved from www.cfapubs.org Suresh, A. (2013). Understanding behavioral finance through biases and traits oftrader vis-a-vis investor. Journal of Finance, Accounting & Management, 4(2), 11-25. Retrieved from http://eds.b.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer Taffler, R., & Tuckett, D. (2007). Emotional finance: Understanding what drives investors. Retrieved from http://www.ucl.ac.uk/psychoanalysis/unitstaff/piemotional_financearticle%2009 07.pdf Taffler, R. J., & Tuckett, D. A. (2010). Emotional finance: The role of the unconscious in financial decisions. In H. Baker & J. Nofsinger (Eds.), Behavioral finance: Investors, corporations, and markets (pp. 95-112). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Tekçe, B., Yılmaz, N., & Bildik, R. (2016). What factors affect behavioral biases? Evidence from Turkish individual stock investors. Research in International Business and Finance, 37, 515-526. doi:10.1016/j.ribaf.2015.11.017 Thaler, R. H. (2005). Advances in behavioral finance (Vol. II). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University. Tillman, L. C. (2003). Mentoring, reflection, and reciprocal journaling. Theory into Practice, 42(3), 226-233. Retrieved from http://eds.b.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer Torres García-Heras, A. (2011). Geographical diversification and risk throughout the credit default swap market. GCG: Revista De Globalización, Competitividad, and Gobernabilidad, 5(2), 52-61. doi:10.3232/GCG.2011.V5.N2.03 Turner, D. W. (2010). Qualitative interview design: A practical guide for novice investigators. The Qualitative Report, 15(3), 754-760. Retrieved from http://nsuworks.nova.edu/tqr/vol15/iss3/19 Učkar, D., & Carlin, S. (2011). Impact of behavioral finance on the Croatian capital market. Proceedings of the International Scientific Conference, Pula, Croatia, 13, 570-582. Retrieved from http://eds.b.ebscohost.com.proxy1.ncu.edu/eds/pdfviewer/pdfviewer? 159 Van de Laar, M., & de Neubourg, C. (2006). Emotions and foreign direct investment: A theoretical and empirical exploration. Management International Review (MIR), 46(2), 207-233. Wang, M., Keller, C., & Siegrist, M. (2011). The less you know, the more you are afraid of: A survey on risk perceptions of investment products. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 12(1), 9-19. doi:10.1080/15427560.2011.548760 Weber, E., & Johnson, E. J. (2009). Mindful judgment and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 53-85. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163633 Williams, E. N., & Morrow, S. L. (2009). Achieving trustworthiness in qualitative research: A pan-paradigmatic perspective. Psychotherapy Research, 19(4-5), 576-582. doi:10.1080/10503300802702113 Virlics, A. (2013). Investment decision making and risk. Procedia Economics and Finance, 6, 169-177. doi:10.1016/S2212-5671(13)00129-9 Yavas, B. F., & Dedi, L. (2016). An investigation of return and volatility linkages among equity markets: A study of selected European and emerging countries. Research In International Business And Finance, 37, 583-596. doi:10.1016/j.ribaf.2016.01.025 Yilmaz, K. (2013). Comparison of quantitative and qualitative research traditions: epistemological, theoretical, and methodological differences. European Journal of Education, 48(2), 311-325. doi:10.1111/ejed.12014 Yin, R. K. (2003). Case study research: Design and methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oak, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Yin, R. K. (2014). Case study research design and methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Yu, Z., & Xiaosong, Z. (2015). A study of the investment behavior based on behavioral finance. European Journal of Business & Economics, 10(1), 1-5. doi:10.12955/ejbe.v10i1.556 Zenker O., & Kumoll, K. (2010). Beyond writing culture: Current intersections of epistemologies and representational practices. New York, NY: Berghahn Books. 160 161 Appendixes 162 Appendix A: Recruitment Notice on Social Network Websites RESEARCH PARTICIPANTS NEEDED My name is Samuel Antwi. I am a doctoral student at Northcentral University. I am conducting a research study on U.S. financial investors’ thoughts and feelings about investing their money across different countries as way to increase profits and decrease risks. You are eligible to participate in this research if you are at least 18 years old, live or work in the Washington DC Metropolitan Area, and invest your money across different countries. As a participant in my study, you will do one interview (up to 90 minutes) and also you will complete 3 journal entries about your feelings and thoughts when making financial investment decisions (up to 1 hour total). You will also be asked to review your interview responses for accuracy (up to 30 minutes). If you are interested in participating in this study or have questions, please email me at s.antwi2313@email.ncu.edu. 163 Appendix B: Recruitment and Screening Email Response Dear Name of Participant, Thank you for your quick response to take part in my study. This study will explore U.S. investors’ thoughts and feelings about investing their money across different countries as an investment strategy. As a participant in my study, you will be asked to complete an interview to collect your thoughts about diversifying your investments across different countries. The interview will last up to 90 minutes. After the interview, you will be asked to review your interview transcript for accuracy and email any changes to me within one week. The review may take up to 30 minutes. In addition to the interview, you will be asked to complete 3 journal entries over 4 weeks about your feelings and thoughts when making an investment decision. The journal entries may take up to 60 minutes. The informed consent form is attached to this email and will provide more details on participation. Any information you share about yourself and your identity will be kept confidential. To make sure that you meet the requirements for the study, I need you to answer three brief questions. These questions require yes/no answer questions. No elaboration is required. The answers to these questions do not obligate you to participate in the study. Even if you are selected to participate, you can opt out at any time. 1. Are you at least 18 years old? 2. Do you currently work or live in the Washington DC Metropolitan Area? 3. Do you invest your money in different countries? If you answered “yes” to all the three questions above, the next step is to set a time for our interview. We can do the interview over the phone or in person. If you prefer an in person interview, please suggest a convenient public place (with private space, such as a public library) where we could meet. For your convenience, I will let you choose the date and time. I would suggest choosing a day and time when interruptions can be held to a minimum. When you reply to this email, please include your preference for the date, time, and location of the interview. In my reply email, I will confirm our interview appointment. If I have already scheduled the planned number of interviews, you will be asked to be on a waiting list in case additional participants are need. Thank you, Samuel Antwi Northcentral University Doctoral Student s.antwi2313@email.ncu.edu 164 Appendix C: Informed Consent Introduction My name is Samuel Antwi. I am a doctoral student at Northcentral University. I am conducting a research study on U.S. investors’ thoughts and feelings about investing their money across different countries as a way to increase profits and decrease risks. I am completing this research as part of my doctoral degree. I invite you to participate. Activities: If you participate in this research, you will be asked to: 1. Answer interview questions. You can choose to answer the interview questions in person or by phone. The interview may take up to one hour and 30 minutes. 2. Complete three journal entries within four weeks in the template provided. This activity will take you up to 1 hour total. 3. Review your interview responses for accuracy. Email any changes to me within seven days. This activity may take you up to 30 minutes. Eligibility: You are eligible to participate in this research if you: 1. Are at least 18 years of age 2. Work or live in the Washington DC Metropolitan Area 3. Invest your money across different countries You are not eligible to participate in this research if you: 1. Are under 18 years of age 2. Do not work or live in Washington DC Metropolitan Area 3. Do not invest your money across different countries I hope to include 10 to 12 people in this research. 165 Risks: There are minimal risks in this study. Some possible risks include: A total of up to three hours of your time will be used to answer interview questions, complete three journal entries, and review your interview responses for accuracy. To decrease the impact of these risks, you can: skip interview questions, skip journal entries, skip review of interview transcript, or stop participation at any time. Benefits: If you decide to participate, there are no direct benefits to you. The potential benefits to others are: Researchers and financial practitioners may use the results to help change investors’ behavior in order to increase profits and decrease risks. Confidentiality: The information you provide will be kept confidential to the extent allowable by law. Some steps I will take to keep your identity confidential are: When I am reporting the information, all participants will be identified only as participant 1, 2, 3, or a, b, c, etc. to maintain your confidentiality. The people who will have access to your information are: Me, my committee chair, and committee members. The Institutional Review Board may also review my research and view your information. I will secure your information with these steps: I will securely keep the information in the Microsoft Word document. I will be the only person who knows the password on the document. The computer and other information will be securely locked in a cabinet in my home office. I will keep your data for 7 years. Then, I will delete electronic data and destroy paper data. Contact Information: If you have questions for me, you can contact me at: s.antwi2313@email.ncu.edu. Phone: 1-888-327-2877 My dissertation chair’s name is Dr. Stephanie Wallio. She works at Northcentral University and is supervising me on the research. You can contact her at: SWallio@ncu.edu. Phone: 1-888-327-2877. 166 If you have questions about your rights in the research, or if a problem has occurred, or if you are injured during your participation, please contact the Institutional Review Board at: irb@ncu.edu or 1-888-327-2877 ext 8014. Voluntary Participation: Your participation is voluntary. If you decide not to participate, or if you stop participation after you start, there will be no penalty to you. You will not lose any benefit to which you are otherwise entitled. Audiotaping: I would like to use a voice recorder to record your responses. You cannot participate if you do not wish to be recorded. Please sign here if I can record you: Signature: A signature indicates your understanding of this consent form. You will be given a copy of the form for your information. Participant Signature Printed Name Date Researcher Signature Printed Name Date 167 Appendix D: Participation Confirmation Email Dear Potential Participant, Thank you for your interest to take part in my research. This study will explore U.S. investors’ thoughts and feelings regarding investing their money across different countries as an investment strategy. I wanted to confirm our interview appointment at DAY, TIME, and PLACE. I am re-sending the informed consent document with this email. If your interview will occur by phone, please print and sign the informed consent document after reading it, scan it, and email to me before your interview appointment. If you prefer to mail back a hard copy of the signed document, please reply to this email asking for my mailing address. If your interview will occur in person, please print and sign the informed consent document after reading it and bring a signed copy with you to the interview. I will also have a copy at the interview if you forget. I will like to remind you that the interview will last for up to 90 minutes. You will not be able to take part in the interview until I have a signed copy of the informed consent document from you. If you have any questions or concerns after reading it, please contact me. Thank you, Samuel Antwi Northcentral University Doctoral Student s.antwi2313@email.ncu.edu 168 Appendix E: Email Response to Unqualified Applicant Dear Participant Name, Thank you for your interest in participating in my research to explore U.S. financial investors’ thoughts and feelings regarding their use of investing their money across different countries as an investment strategy. Based on your responses, you do not meet the requirements for participating in the study. Thank you once again for your interest to take part in the study. If you would like to receive a summary of the study’s results when the research is complete, please reply to this email. Samuel Antwi Northcentral University Doctoral Student s.antwi2313@email.ncu.edu 169 Appendix F: Participant Waiting List Email Dear (Name): Thank you for your interest in participating in my research to investigate U.S. financial investors’ thoughts and feelings about investing their money across different countries as an investment strategy. Sometimes more people express interest than a study was planned to include, therefore your participation in the study is not needed right now. But your name will be placed on the waiting list and I will contact you as soon as possible if I need additional participants to take part in the research. Please feel free to contact me if you have any questions. Thank you, Samuel Antwi Northcentral University Doctoral Student S.Antwi2313@email.ncu.edu 170 Appendix G: Participation Not Needed Due to Data Saturation Email Dear (Name): Thank you for your continued interest to take part in my research and your patience to stay on the waiting list. All the interview spaces were filled and the study has ended. I am happy to give you a summary of the study results as soon as the research is complete. Please let me know if you are interested in receiving a copy of the research results. Feel free to contact if you have any questions. Thanks, Samuel Antwi Northcentral University Doctoral Student S.Antwi2313@email.ncu.edu 171 Appendix H: Interview Questions Demographic Data 1. Describe your investment experience. 2. What is your area of work? 3. What is your highest educational level? What was your degree in? 4. In what city and state do you work and live? 5. What is your age? 6. Describe investing your money across different countries as an investment strategy. As I ask more questions, think about times you have made a decision to use geographical diversification. Emotions and Geographical Diversification Strategy 7. Describe your feelings when you are deciding to use geographical diversification. 8. How do your feelings influence your decision to diversify your money across different countries? Perception, Geographical Diversification Strategy, and Investment Returns 9. Tell me your thoughts about using geographical diversification as a strategy to increase investment returns. 10. What has shaped your opinions about using geographical diversification as a strategy to increase investment returns? 11. What about geographical diversification makes it an appealing strategy to increase investment returns? Perception, Geographical Diversification Strategy, and Investment Risks 12. Tell me your thoughts about using geographical diversification as a strategy to reduce investment risks. 13. What has shaped your opinions about using geographical diversification as a strategy to reduce investment risks? 14. What about geographical diversification makes it an appealing strategy to reduce investment risks? Strategy of Geographical Diversification and Literature 15. Are you aware of financial literature that says the geographical diversification strategy is not effective? 16. What are your feelings and thoughts about financial literature that says the geographical diversification strategy is not effective? 17. What are your thoughts about continuing to use geographical diversification given that literature? If you will continue to use it, why? 172 This activity of answering the interview question will take you up to 1 hour and 30 minutes 173 Appendix I: Journaling Instructions Dear (Name): Thank you for taking part in my research study to explore U.S. financial investors’ thoughts and feelings regarding investing their money across different countries as an investment strategy. Using the attached journal template in the form of MS Word document, please complete three journal entries in four weeks. Open and use “Save As” to save the journal template with your first initial and last name and journal number. For example, if I were to journal three entries, they would be saved as santwi1, santwi2, and santwi3, respectfully. Using this instruction, please email your journal entries to me as you complete them. This activity will take you up to 60 minutes. If you have any questions, please contact me directly at the email below. Thank you for your participation. Samuel Antwi Northcentral University Doctoral Student S.Antwi2313@email.ncu.edu 174 Appendix J: Journal Template Date: Participant Name: Participant Email: Please describe what happens as you make investment decisions related to investing your money across different countries as an investment strategy. Write on the following in the boxes below: • Your feelings when you are making decision on investment; • Your mood at time of making decision on investment; • Your thoughts when you are making decision on investment 175 Appendix K: Member Validation of Recorded Responses Email Dear (Name): Thank you for taking part in my research study to explore U.S. financial investors’ thoughts and feelings regarding investing your money across different countries as an investment strategy. The transcript of your interview is attached. Please go through your answers to the questions to make sure what you said during the interview accurately reflects your perceptions. You may wish to request changes to remove or clarify something you said. Please email your response to me within one week to enable me to continue with the research. You may make changes directly in the transcript or let me know that no changes are needed. This activity will take you up to 30 minutes. If I do not hear from you within one week, I will assume your transcript is accurate. Feel free to contact if you have any questions. Thanks, Samuel Antwi Northcentral University Doctoral Student S.Antwi2313@email.ncu.edu