Uploaded by Chenille Stephens

Components of language

advertisement
Components of Language
Linguistic study involves a search for patterns in the way speakers use language;
linguists aim to describe these patterns by reducing them to a set of rules called a
grammar. Components of language include: phonology, semantics, syntax, morphology,
and pragmatics.
Phonology: As children hear and perceive oral language, they learn that language is
embedded in a sound-symbol system.
o Phonology refers to the study of the sound-symbol relations in a language.
o A phoneme is the smallest linguistic unit of sound, which is combined with other
phonemes to form words. Example: book = 3 phonemes (b-u-k)
o The International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) is used to represent the sounds of a
language which exceeds the number of letters in the alphabet. For example, bow
(bend) and bow (decorative tie using a ribbon); present vs. present
o Consonant sounds vs. vowel sounds: All vowel sounds are voiced and produced
with little or no obstruction in vocal tract; while consonant sounds are described
in terms of place, manner and voicing.
o There are phonotactic constraints of a language: For example, not every
sequence of speech sounds is a possible word in English (e.g., ng, lp, rt do not
appear at the beginning of a word in English). In English, the major constraint on
initial consonant cluster is that a word cannot begin with two stop consonants in a
row. Most initial sequences of a stop is followed by a liquid, /s/ followed by a
stop, and /s/ plus stop plus liquid are pronounceable, as in ‘true’ ‘stew’ and
‘strew’ respectively.
o Child speech: Adult-like words vs. protowords (e.g., pill for spill; tore for store;
bed for bread, lellow for yellow; wabbit for rabbit; bulu/boo for blue: na, weei
weei) There is nothing wrong unless it progresses into later years.
Make reference to the speech acquisition chart
o Children’s development of phonology is fostered by their perceptual ability
to distinguish sounds and also by the ways in which language is used
around them. Young infants around two months of age have been found to be
able to distinguish between /p/ sound and /b/ sound long before being able to
produce them. Children’s discrimination of sounds precedes their ability to
produce those same sounds, due to the complex coordination of the speech
mechanism in making those sounds.
o In every language and culture, some speech sounds are more important
than others. Gradually, young children learn to discriminate and produce the
speech sounds that are found in their home language. Phonological knowledge is
influenced by semantic knowledge (eg., differentiating between ‘car’ and ‘cat’
o Children’s phonological knowledge during infancy and toddlerhood is evident
when they produce and distinguish between the sounds used in their home
languages in communicating with those around them.
o As children move into preschool years, they may acquire a more conscious
awareness of language segments (onset and rime) = phonological awareness
and distinct speech sounds in their language (phonemic awareness) and begin
to deliberately manipulate their language. This conscious awareness of language
segments and speech sounds contribute significantly to children’s understanding
of the relation between speech and print as they begin to acquire knowledge of
written language during reading and writing interactions.
o In addition to the perception of sounds in a language, young children notice
differences in the way sounds are used in a language to add meaning to what is
said. Prosodic features (suprasegmental aspects) in a language represent the
way something is said. These include: intonation, loudness, rate, rhythm. Infants
learn to sense when caregivers or parents are happy, excited, calm, tense, or
angry based on those prosodic features of the adult’s speech. These features are
used in storybook reading.
Morphology:
o Morphology is the study of how words are put together or how words are
structured.
o A word is a building blocks of sentences
o Words are composed of one or more meaningful linguistic units.
o The smallest unit of meaning in a language is the morpheme. There are two
types of morphemes: (a) free morphemes (used alone as words e.g., dog, man,
bike), and (b) bound morphemes (must be attached to a free morpheme,; e.g.,
‘s’ in dogs, ‘ly’ in slowly, and ‘ing’ in going
o Bound morphemes are of two types: derivational and inflectional. Derivational
morphemes include prefixes, such as un (unhappy), and suffixes, such as ness
(happiness). Derivational morphemes can change the part of speech of a word
(e.g., develop = verb; development = noun). Inflectional morphemes are word
endings added to change verb tense (e.g., walking, walked), possession (Bill’s)
or plurality (cats).
Activity: How many morphemes are in the following words?
Antidisestablishment
Imperfection
Unhappiness
Continuously
Antiaging
o The ability to use morphemes appropriately is one of the characteristics of an
effective language user. Knowledge of morphology allows children to
comprehend others’ speech better, such as understanding plural nouns and verb
tenses.
o As children’s speech progresses beyond the one-word and two word stages,
their understanding of how words are formed is used as they attempt to
communicate. In the production of an utterance, children use their morphemic
knowledge of how words are formed to create their message. As they acquire
more knowledge of morphemes, their language becomes more precise and
meaningful.
o Young children acquire morphemic knowledge that is present in their linguistic
environments. In figuring out how language is used and words are structured,
children appear to be looking for patterns and hypothesizing. Children’s
overgeneralizations of morphological patterns are evidence of this.
o Children’s overgeneralizations occur when they assume a particular word
follows a regular pattern when, in fact, it follows an irregular pattern. For
example: verb tense, comparatives of adjectives and use of prefixes (eg., using
‘ed’ on ‘run’ = runned; funner, funnest; bestest; insist = unsist, uncomplete for
incomplete).
o We can calculate the MLU to determine a child’s syntactic and morphological
growth. MLU = # of morphemes divided by the # of utterances (no less than 50)
Refer to Brown’s stages of morphological development (google it)
Syntax: Sentence Structure
o The study of how individual words and their most basic meaningful units are
combined to create sentences is known as syntax
o To use language effectively it is necessary to know how to combine words to
create meaningful expressions.
o Each language system has rules or a grammar that prescribes how words are
combined to create sentences or meaningful phrases or utterances.
o Children learn that word order, or syntax, is important in creating meaning and in
comprehending another’s message. For example: “Did you see Mary?” asks for
different information than “Did Mary see you?”
o
Children use correct word order before they can verbalize their understanding of
that concept. For example, in their use of adjectives as noun modifiers. E.g., “The
beautiful girl kissed the boy.” instead of “The girl beautiful kissed the boy.”
o
As words are grouped together when we communicate, we must follow the rules
of grammar/syntax for our language.
o It is the knowledge of syntax that allows us to recognize that the following two
sentences, while containing different word order and levels of complexity, have
the same meaning.

The boy hit the ball.

The ball was hit by the boy.
Syntax also allows us to accept “I went to the store” as a meaningful (grammatical)
sentence while “To store went I” would not be acceptable English.
Examples of syntactic relationships include:
1. NP + V : The baby cries
2. NP + V + NP : Mary eats an apple
3. NP+ V + Adj:
Mother is ill
4. NP + V + Adv: The exam is in the gymnasium.
Can you think of other sentences that reflect the patterns/relationships cited?
Semantics: Study of meaning
Semantics refers to the ways in which a language conveys meaning. Multiple
meaning of words (run, bank, grave, fan, face)
 It is our mental dictionary, or lexicon.
 In learning that spoken words have meaning, semantic knowledge is acquired.
The development of semantic knowledge is closely related to the acquisition of
conceptual knowledge.
 Semantic knowledge refers to the word labels that specify concepts and
also to the semantic networks, or schemata, that represent the
interrelations between concepts.
 Semantic networks – schemata – are thought to be cognitive structures in our
memory that organize our conceptual knowledge. These semantic networks
facilitate new learning and recall and contribute to the reorganization and
elaboration of prior conceptual learning.
 For example, the semantic network or schemata for Ball (colours – white, red,
black, patterned; materials – rubber, plastic, leather, felt; Actions – bounce, roll,
throw, kick, hit; Purpose – baseball, football, cricket, tennis, basketball; texture –
smooth, rough, fuzzy, dimpled; shape – oblong, spherical, oval)
Dog (types, actions, physical features, etc.)
 In acquiring concepts, children learn that objects and actions with similar features
or functions can be grouped into the same category or into related categories. A
semantic network or schema develops when a child begins to see relations
between 2 concepts.
 A child’s schema is more limited than that of an adult.
 Meaning is a cognitive construct. The meaning of a word resides in the speakers
of a common language, not in the world of objects. The word is a sign that
signifies a referent, but the referent is not the meaning of the word. If, for
example, you say to a child, “Look at the kitty,” the cat is the referent, but not the
meaning of kitty --- if the cat should run away, or be run over by a truck, the word
would still have meaning.
 Vocabulary development is closely related to general linguistic competence and
to reading comprehension. Children with larger and more developed
vocabularies have more options for expressing what they want to say and,
therefore, have greater linguistic flexibility.
 One activity that contributes to vocabulary development is storybook reading.
Vocabularies used in storybooks are more descriptive and precise than
vocabulary used in daily conversations.
 It is our understanding of semantics that allows us to recognize that someone
who is “green with envy” has not changed hue, or that “having cold feet” has less
to do with the appendage at the end of our legs and more to do with our anxiety
about a new experience. Because semantics moves beyond the literal meaning
of words and is culture-dependent, this is among the most difficult aspects of
language for individuals who are not native speakers and even those who speak
the same language, but come from different cultures and convey meaning using
words in unique ways. Anyone who has attempted to converse with a teenager in
his own vernacular can appreciate the importance of sharing a semantic base for
communicating clearly.
 Children’s acquisition of semantic knowledge is also influenced by their
awareness of grammatical structure in which language is used. Word order
affects the meaning of what is said.
Pragmatics: How language is used
o Pragmatics’ refers to the ways the members of the speech community
achieve their goals using language.
o Pragmatics refer to the knowledge or awareness of the overall intent of the
communication and how language is used to achieve that intent.
o Pragmatic knowledge encompasses the intent of the speaker, the specific
form of the utterance, and the anticipated effect the utterance will have on
the listener.
o Children learn that language can serve many purposes and intents. The
selection of intent or purpose in communication and the way in which
language is used contribute to a child’s level of communicative
competence in early childhood and beyond.
o Early on in the development of communicative competence, children’s
efforts appear to have purpose and intent. The 8-months old child who
looks at her mother with outstretched arms and produces strained
vocalizations [/uh/ /uh/] is assumed to be communicating that she wants to
be picked up. If not initially successful, the child may repeat her request,
vocalize louder, or gesture more emphatically.
o In addition to communicative intent, 3 other components of pragmatics are
learning conversational rules, learning to be polite, and learning to
produce connected discourse such as narratives (Nino &Snow, cited in
Gleason, 2012).
o Conversational skills are a critical part of pragmatic knowledge, because
they impact a child’s ability to engage in classroom and social interactions.
o Children who cannot differentiate how to use language in different settings
may be excluded from social interactions with peers or may be unable to
request assistance from teachers during class activities.
o The term social register refers to the specific ways of using language
differently in different social settings. Different types of conversation
include social conversations, classroom discourse, telephone
conversations, jokes, doctor-patient talk, among others. Through their
direct experiences, children become aware of the rules or expected ways
in which conversations are initiated, maintained, and discontinued. In
conversation, both people take on the roles of speaker and listener.
Conversational competence depends on the development of specific skills
of taking turns, keeping similar or related topics as the focus of the
conversation, encouraging participation from the other person/s, and
clarifying or repairing areas of confusion.
o Language use is embedded in social-cultural contexts. Different contexts
are characterized by differences in the way language is used. Through
social-cultural interactions we learn when to speak; when not to speak;
what to talk about with whom; and when, where, and in what manner to
speak. For example, the way we speak to our parents is not the same as
the way we interact with a sibling.
o The language used in a formal speech may bear little resemblance to
what we would hear at a lunch with five friends. The conversational style
of day-to-day interactions is quite different from the language used even
when reading a storybook to a toddler. Knowing the difference and when
to use which style is the essence of pragmatics.
o Pragmatic knowledge also includes the awareness of how to use
language to be polite. Children learn to distinguish different times when
quiet voices and loud voices are used and they pick up social conventions
such as saying “please” and “thank you.” Children learn how to talk most
effectively to adults and to each other. For example, language behaviours
at home may not occur at school (e.g., verbal whining).
o Children also acquire knowledge of how language is used in relation to
gender. Topics for boys differ from topics for girls.
o Children also acquire pragmatic knowledge of how language is used to tell
story narratives and communicate information through their early
experiences with storybooks and information books. Exposure to various
genre of text will provide children with a variety of ways of expressing
themselves. For example: Fairy tale creation would include: Once upon a
time, they lived happily ever after, the end, and some magical element. If
exposed to alphabet book and asked to create one such they would
incorporate that genre “ A is for apple etc.”
Facility with language is critical to social interactions. Our ability to effectively
communicate with others through spoken and written language is considered one of the
ultimate goals of our educational system.
Children can experience language deficits in any of the five components of
language. Hence, they can have phonological deficits, morphological deficits,
syntactic deficit, semantic deficits or pragmatic deficits.
Activity:
1. Visit: https://quizlet.com/...five components-of-language-learning-and-deficitsflash-cards/ (Five components of language learning and deficits flashcards)
2. Complete all seven activities to assist you in reinforcing the concepts
(Flashcards – gravity exercises)
Sources:
Gleason, J.B. (2012). The development of language (8th Ed.). Boston: Pearson.
Otto, B.W. (2017). Language development in early childhood education (3 rd Ed.).
Boston: Pearson.
Download