BODY SCHEMA ACUITY TRAINING AND FELDENKRAIS® MOVEMENTS COMPARED TO CORE STABILIZATION BIOFEEDBACK AND MOTOR CONTROL EXERCISES: COMPARATIVE EFFECTS ON CHRONIC NON-SPECIFIC LOW BACK PAIN IN AN OUTPATIENT CLINICAL SETTING: A RANDOMIZED CONTROLLED COMPARATIVE EFFICACY STUDY A dissertation presented to the Faculty of Saybrook University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy (Ph.D.) in Psychology by Timothy J. Sobie Oakland, California November 2016 ProQuest Number: 10251703 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. ProQuest 10251703 Published by ProQuest LLC (2017 ). Copyright of the Dissertation is held by the Author. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. ProQuest LLC. 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, MI 48106 - 1346 © 2016 by Timothy J. Sobie Approval of the Dissertation BODY SCHEMA ACUITY TRAINING AND FELDENKRAIS® MOVEMENTS COMPARED TO CORE STABILIZATION BIOFEEDBACK AND MOTOR CONTROL EXERCISES: COMPARATIVE EFFECTS ON CHRONIC NON-SPECIFIC LOW BACK PAIN IN AN OUTPATIENT CLINICAL SETTING: A RANDOMIZED CONTROLLED COMPARATIVE EFFICACY STUDY This dissertation by ___Timothy J. Sobie __ has been approved by the committee members below, who recommend it be accepted by the faculty of Saybrook University in partial fulfillment of requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology Dissertation Committee: ___________________________ Richard Sherman, Ph.D., Chair ___________________ Date ___________________________ James Stephens, Ph.D., PT, GCFP ___________________ Date ___________________________ Derek S. Scott, M.D., F.A.A.P.M.R. ___________________ Date ii Abstract BODY SCHEMA ACUITY TRAINING AND FELDENKRAIS® MOVEMENTS COMPARED TO CORE STABILIZATION BIOFEEDBACK AND MOTOR CONTROL EXERCISES: COMPARATIVE EFFECTS ON CHRONIC NON-SPECIFIC LOW BACK PAIN IN AN OUTPATIENT CLINICAL SETTING: A RANDOMIZED CONTROLLED COMPARATIVE EFFICACY STUDY Timothy J. Sobie Saybrook University Back problems continue to be a leading cause for disability in all of medicine and are the number one symptom disorder for consulting integrative medicine practitioners. Feldenkrais® practitioners aim to clarify new functional interrelationships towards an improved neuroplasticity-based change in the cognitive construct of one’s own background body schema. These phenomena have been found to clinically correlate to chronic pain through concurrent distortions in the reorganization of usual sensory-motor cortical representations in the brain – being further associated with altered body perception (Wand et al., 2016). The Feldenkrais Method® (FM) is a comprehensive approach being manifested through manual sensory contact (FI®) techniques and movement experiences (ATM®) and has been anecdotally purported to improve symptoms and functions in chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP). However, there is little scientific evidence to support superior treatment efficacy. iii A randomized controlled trial (RCT) compared a novel Virtual Reality Bones™/Feldenkrais® Movement (VRB3/FM) intervention against more conventional protocols for Core Stabilization Biofeedback / Motor Control Exercises (CSB/MCE). The (VRB3)™ treatment component consisted of full-scale skeletal models, kinematic avatars, skeletal density imagery, temporal bone-vestibular system relationships, and haptic self-touch techniques being aimed to re-conceptualize participants' prior notions and beliefs regarding body schema and low back pain (LBP). Participating patients (N=30) with CNSLBP were assigned to either the experimental group (VRB3/FM @ N=15) or the control group (CSB/MCE @ N=15). Known confounding biopsychosocial variables were controlled via stratified-random assignment on the FABQ. Treatment Outcome measures included VAS-PAIN, RMDQ, PSFS, and Timed Position Endurances Tests, including Flexion/Extension Ratios at baseline, two weeks, four weeks, and eight weeks. Statistical Analysis was conducted using Wilcoxon Rank Sum and paired, twotailed t-test. Results showed that the VRB3/FM group demonstrated greater improvement in all treatment outcome measures as compared to the matched CSB/MCE control group. This is the first RCT study to demonstrate that a Feldenkrais Method® based approach being combined with Virtual Reality Bones™ can be more efficacious for the treatment of CNSLBP than the current and accepted physical medicine standard of isolated Core Stabilization Biofeedback/Training and Motor Control Exercises. Future multi-site RCT studies with larger sample sizes are therefore recommended. Dedication This body of work is dedicated to all those who seek to explore and develop an alternative set of ideas for innovative thinking and application; beyond that from which they were originally exposed, conditioned, or schooled. Acknowledgments This work would not have been possible without the help and inspiration of a great many people. From the start, I wish to commemorate the memory of my departed mother, whose encouragement and support continues with me through this day. Next, I send much appreciation and gratitude to the community of professional trainers, teachers, and colleagues for their many inspired teachings of The Feldenkrais Method® in all its bountiful applications and diverse forms, including but not limited to Ruthy Alon, Eileen Bachy-Rita, Elizabeth Beringer, Gordon and Julie Browne, Richard Corbeil, Russell Delman, Angel Di Benedetto, Staffan Elgelid, Larry Goldfarb, Jeff Haller, Todd Hargrove, Alan Questel, Mark Reese, Roger Russell, Annie Thoe, Donald van Howten, Edward Yu, David Zemach-Bersin, and most notably to my own professional trainer, Frank Wildman, for his expanding Moshe Feldenkrais’ work into the physical therapy realm, and from which otherwise, none of this life path toward a doctoral level of study and its level of required dedication would have ever happened. My advisory committee knows the struggle. Chair faculty member at Saybrook University, Dr. Richard A. Sherman, has persisted undauntingly in transitioning the degree specialization program in clinical psychophysiology into full fruition to such extent that it has attracted a large and diverse population of student expertise. I thank him for guiding me through the critically demanding and consuming process of learning how to plan for more effectively applied research designs being conducted as clinical trials, and for what he substantially shares from his vast experience in basic science and clinical areas. Physical Therapy and Feldenkrais Method® research advisor, Dr. Jim Stephens, has assisted and shared both the complexity - as well as the opportunity - for outlining the many possible paths for conducting ongoing and expansive research inquiry into the many applications of The Feldenkrais Method® through his dual role as both researcher and practitioner, but most notably as being chair of research committee during more than a decade of annual conferences for the Feldenkrais Guild® of North America. Finally, I wish to offer thanks and kind regard to medical pain management specialist, Dr. Derek S. Scott, for his interest in paying attention to alternative and multidimensional models for the collaborative management and interventional treatment of complex problems involving chronic pain, and for his making clinical recommendations that truly allowed for the success of this study to be carried out. Let it be known that none of this research could have happened without the assistance of dedicated staff and colleagues. I wish to thank fellow physical therapy clinicians Maria Bokor, MPT, Dayna Briggs, DPT, Celeste Mishko, DPT, for their role in confidently conducting the control arm of the interventional study. I would like to especially thank Yi-ran (Kenny) Li for his primary role as research coordinator for website development, patient orientation, random assignment, physical testing, and data collection for both arms of the study; to front office receptionist, Angelina Zacapu, for expanding her role in keeping smooth allocation and consistency in patient scheduling; to senior statistics tutor, Samantha Coates, of the University of Puget Sound for her role in effectively conducting the statistical analysis of the raw data, and to University of Washington - Tacoma graduate, Aeron Lloyd, for her skillful assistance in formatting graphics for the study flow diagram, and for her also depicting a complex level of visual literacy being necessary for the detailed demonstration of a novel therapeutic treatment model. Finally, much appreciation and admiration goes out to my primary practice colleague and friend, Jennifer Yagos, PTA, GCFP, for her role in co-conducting the combined physical therapy and The Feldenkrais Method® interventions that were the necessary components for completing the experimental arm of the study. Overall, she is a major reason for the success of my practice. Last, but by no measure the least, this body of work could never have happened without the understanding, support, and concurrent commitment and sacrifices being made on the part of my loving family. My loving wife and life partner, Rhonda, has sacrificed more than anyone can know in the support of her husband in challenging times. I thank her for being there in advance of my unexpectedly long quest, for her practical and skillful help in formatting tables and documents, and for just being there with me. I also give added extended thanks to her parents, Lowell and Dorothy, who were also always there for us. I give thanks for my father, Eugene, who maintains the right mix of necessary toughness and practical resolve, being responsibly combined with honest pride and a genuine concern for truly wanting what is best for others, as an emulative model for both living a life and a livelihood. Finally, from both the lessons learned and for the futures that are hoped for, I dedicate this work and continuing livelihood to the added benefit of my first-born and fraternal twin sons, Alexander Sobie and Nathanial Sobie. Though you are both now early in your personal histories, and of being only a few years old at the time of this writing, you are each rapidly and assuredly becoming your own men. And both your mom and I - and the world - look forward to the unique contributions that each of you may someday bring. Table of Contents List of Tables ................................................................................................................................... v List of Figures ..............................................................................................................................viii CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................... 1 CNSLBP in the Context of Physical Therapy .................................................................... 3 CNSLBP in the Context of Psychologically-Informed Physical Therapy ......................... 4 CNSLBP in the Context of a New Model as Proposed through the Current Study ............ 6 Traditional Frameworks, Alternative Viewpoints, and New Research Questions ......................... 8 Synopsis of the Problem ............................................................................................................... 11 Purpose of the Study, Aims, and Objectives ................................................................................. 15 Research Design and Hypotheses .................................................................................................. 15 Hypothesis 1 ...................................................................................................................... 16 Hypothesis 2 ...................................................................................................................... 16 Rationale ........................................................................................................................................ 16 Significance of the Study............................................................................................................... 18 Organization of the Dissertation .................................................................................................... 20 CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW....................................................................................... 21 Overview and Background of LBP in Outpatient Physical Therapy Settings............................... 21 Core Stabilization and Motor Control Exercise: Status and Purported Efficacy .......................... 23 Limitations of Core Stabilization and Motor Control Interventions ............................................. 31 Biopsychosocial, Cognitive-Behavioral, and Graded Activity Interventions ............................... 33 The Role of Fear-Avoidance Beliefs and Chronic Non-Specific Low Back Pain ............ 34 Cognitive Behavioral and Mindfulness Interventions ....................................................... 35 Graded Activity Functional Therapy and Therapeutic Pain Neuroscience Education ...... 35 Limitations of Combined Physical and Behavioral-Psychological Interventions ......................... 38 Applying the Culmination of Recent Literature and Combined Rx into the Design ..................... 40 Emergent Findings from the Neuroscience of Chronic Pain and Neuroplasticity ........................ 42 Some Specific Brain Regions Involved in Pain Processing .............................................. 46 Bottom-Up Influences: Their Relationship to Intervention & Purported Mechanisms .... 48 Sensory Information at the Crossroads: Differentiating the Thalamus & Rerouting the Insula ................................................................................................................................. 48 Sensory-Motor Deficiency and Excess Functional Connectivity upon fMRI Motor Imagery .............................................................................................................................. 51 The supplemental motor area ................................................................................ 54 Superior temporal gyrus/sulcus ............................................................................. 54 Event-related functional connectivity (FC) and its meaning fMRI ....................... 55 Summary of motor imagery (MI)-driven fMRI activity............................ 56 fMRI implications for clinical understanding and future treatment .......... 57 The Pre-frontal Cognitive-Attentional & Affective-Emotional Mesolimbic Domains ..... 58 Synopsis and Rx Transition: Toward a Sensory Discriminative Perceptual Model ......... 62 Distortion of Body Schema, Somatic Education Interventions, and Virtual Reality .................... 64 Distortion of Body Schema and Chronic Pain - especially Low Back Pain ..................... 68 Somatic Education Interventions and Low Back Pain ...................................................... 69 Virtual Reality, Applications to Chronic Pain, and Prospective Studies for CNSLBP ..... 71 Differences between Feldenkrais® Virtual Reality and Traditional Guided Imagery ....... 78 Gravitation as a Virtual and Invariant Constant in the Sensory-Motor World.................. 84 Hidden Senses: A Skeletal Density-Vestibular Concept for Body Schema & Pain...................... 85 Spatial Cognition as an Internal Model for Perception of Virtual Limb Segments and Bones ................................................................................................................................. 88 Vestibular-Ocular Representation: A Mediator of Body Schema Acuity & Motor Dexerity ............................................................................................................................. 90 Vestibular Contribution to Affective Limbic Processes, Body Schema, & Chronic Pain Modulation ........................................................................................................................ 92 Feldenkrais’ Postulates and Vestibular Contributions to Skeletal Organization, Movement, and Behavior .................................................................................................. 94 Current Status of the Feldenkrais Method® and the Proposed Interventions .............................. 101 Differences Exemplified through Feldenkrais Method® Features of Application .......... 104 The Feldenkrais Method® in Research ............................................................................ 109 Applying the Proposed Intervention against Core Stabilization ..................................... 111 Evolving Practice, New Visual-Haptic Techniques: The Origin of VR Bones........................... 113 Uncovering a Universal Deficiency in the Sensory-Perceptual Acuity of Background Body Schema via a Corresponding Normative Comparison to Anatomical Reference Models ............................................................................................................................. 116 "Virtual Reality Hip Replacements" via Routine Deployment of a Life-Sized Femur Model ............................................................................................................................... 121 Continuing Improvements for "Anatomical and Perceptual Reframing of Background Body Schema" through the clarification of Skeletal Support Mechanisms that occur during daily movement interactions between "Pelvis-Hips Opposite Head" .................. 129 The Lateral Chain of Distribution through Pedicle Densities and Costal-Thoracic Expansion ........................................................................................................................ 143 Location of Vestibular Apparatus Augments for a sense of Visual Spatial Alignment .. 146 "Pelvis and Hips" as a Tri-Plane Model for Center of Gravity and the Detection of Change in Center of Gravity Being Represented by "Vestibular Coordinates" as a Combined Dynamic and Unifying Movement Strategy to be used during a Simulated Martial Arts Task for achieving a Synergistic Spiral Quality of Efficient Action being applied to the function of Sit to Stand ............................................................................. 150 Discovery of "Virtual Avatars" for Anatomical Re-framing, Skeletal Transmission, and Aligning Ground Reaction Force Vectors for the Enhancement of Verticality during Gait ....................................................................................................................... 157 Epilogue: Non-Pathological Anatomical Imagery for Highlighting Areas of Highest Bone Density as a Contribution for Cognitive Reframing and Behavioral Adjustment........... 164 Consolidation and Synthesis of Initial Treatment Approach into the Acronym: (VRB3) .......... 172 Pilot Study and Continuing Observations from Practice-based Evidence .................................. 175 Summary...................................................................................................................................... 179 CHAPTER 3: METHODOLOGY ............................................................................................... 182 Overview ..................................................................................................................................... 182 Research Design, Adherence to Current Guidelines, Consistency to Prior Precedent ................ 183 Inclusion of Latest NIH Research Guidelines and Clinical Practice Guidelines for CNSLBP .................................................................................................................... 184 A Historical Precedent for Consistency of Research Design & Comparative Metrics ... 186 Participants, Sources of Recruitment, Treatment Setting & Orientation to the Study ................ 187 Outpatient Treatment Setting and Neutralizing the Environment for Matched Consistency ................................................................................................. 188 Characteristics of the Clinicians providing Interventions ............................................... 189 Orientation of Participants to and during the Study ........................................................ 191 Controlling for Fear-Avoidance and Catastrophic Pain Beliefs for Both Groups........... 193 Inclusion/Exclusion Criteria and the Stated Conditions for Continued Participation ................. 195 Inclusionary Criteria ........................................................................................................ 195 Exclusionary Criteria ....................................................................................................... 196 The Stated Conditions for Continued Participation......................................................... 197 Sample Size, FABQ Sub-Grouping, and Stratified Random Assignment into Groups .............. 197 Determining Sample Size for a Small-Scale Therapy Practice Setting – Use of Pilot Study ............................................................................................................ 198 Sub-Grouping High Fear-Avoidance Beliefs as a Known and Confounding Variable... 200 Rationale for using Fear-Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire (FABQ) as an Assessment Tool.................................................................................................................................. 200 Intended population of FABQ in reference to the current study ......................... 201 Reliability of FABQ ............................................................................................ 202 Validity FABQ .................................................................................................... 202 Implementation of FABQ .................................................................................... 204 Procedure for Consent, Gathering of Baseline Data and Stratified Randomized Assignment ...................................................................................................................... 204 Tests and Repeated Measures for Clinical Outcome ................................................................... 207 The Visual Analog Scale for Pain (VAS-PAIN) ............................................................ 207 The Roland-Morris Disability Questionnaire .................................................................. 210 The Patient-Specific Functional Scale (PSFS) ............................................................... 211 McGill’s Timed Endurance Tests (Total Endurance + Flexion / Extension Ratios) ...... 213 Flexion/Extension Endurance Ratios as an added Qualifying Measure for Trunk Control ............................................................................................................ 217 Methodological Considerations, Determining Minimally Relevant Clinical Change................. 220 Overview of Interventions and Phase Progressions during Course of Study .............................. 221 Phase Progressions for Experimental Group ................................................................... 222 Phase Progressions for Control Group ............................................................................ 222 Time Course for Progression of Interventions, Session Content, and Flow of Study ..... 223 Control Group Interventions, Sources, and Procedure ................................................................ 225 Stabilization Biofeedback Device ................................................................................... 227 Core Stabilization Biofeedback Protocols using the PBU Device .................................. 229 Phase Progression and Content Sourcing for the Control (CSB/MCE) Group ............... 231 Experimental Group Interventions, Sources, and Procedure....................................................... 237 Embodied Perceptual Assessment of Background Body Schema and its Correlation to Action .............................................................................................................................. 238 Phase Progression and Content Sourcing for the Experimental (VRB3 / FM) Group ..... 243 Data Collection Methods and Procedures ................................................................................... 253 Data Analysis using Statistical Tools and R Software ................................................................ 255 The Use of "R" Program Statistical Software ................................................................. 256 Data Backup and Record Retention ................................................................................ 257 CHAPTER 4: RESULTS ............................................................................................................ 258 Demographic and Medical History Profiles Between Groups .................................................... 258 Participant Attrition and Final Distribution for Data Collection ................................................. 260 Central Tendency (Mean) and Distribution of Data (SD) across Phases of Treatment .............. 262 Inferential Statistics Comparing Group Differences using Non-Parametric Tests...................... 270 Inferential Statistics Comparing Group Differences using Parametric Tests .............................. 273 Parametric Testing and Data Analysis for Comparing Pre-Post Flexion/Extension Ratios ............................................................................................................................... 275 Purported Research Questions, Summary and Outcome for Hypothesis .................................... 276 Supported Hypothesis 1 ................................................................................................... 277 Partially-Supported Hypothesis 2 .................................................................................... 277 CHAPTER 5: DISCUSSION ...................................................................................................... 279 Overview and Interpretation of Study Results ............................................................................ 279 Both Groups Demonstrating Improved Outcomes and their Shared Mechanisms of Influence ...................................................................................................................... 279 The control group’s intervention and corresponding mechanisms...................... 280 The experimental group’s intervention and corresponding mechanisms ............ 280 Motor control as a shared mechanism to separate pain from fear of movement? ..................................................................................................... 282 Expectation fulfillment as a confounding variable .............................................. 284 The Experimental Group Demonstrating Superior Improvement and Some Possible Rationale for Examining the Differentiated Mechanisms of Influence........................... 285 Other Qualitative Differences and Oppositional Contrasts Between Interventions ........ 292 A Critical Review of Outcome Measures and Findings from the Current Study ........................ 293 Mean VAS Pain Scales .................................................................................................... 294 Outcomes for RMDQ Disability Questionnaires ............................................................ 295 Outcomes for PSFS Functional Scale.............................................................................. 296 Outcomes for McGill’s Timed Endurance Tests ............................................................. 297 Participant Adherence, Attrition, and Contribution During Course of Study ............................. 299 Adherence to Intervention Training Intent, Home Program, and Medication List ......... 300 Participant Attrition, Priming Effects, and the Contribution of Intent to Treat .............. 301 Comparison of Study Results to Data Attained from Previous Studies ...................................... 302 Implications of Study Findings for Treatment of CNSLBP ........................................................ 305 Physical Therapy’s Regression to the Mean ................................................................... 308 The Preferred Future of PT Practice ................................................................................ 311 A New Intervention Model for the Systemic Adjustment of Working Body Schema ................ 313 Study Limitation .......................................................................................................................... 317 Deficiencies in the Monitoring of Adherence to Home Practice and Repetition ............ 317 Limitation of only an implied CBT and Pain Neuroscience Education Component ....... 318 Complexity of Experimental Group Intervention............................................................ 319 Sample Size and Generalizability .................................................................................... 320 Recommendations for Future Research and Practice .................................................................. 321 Enlisting Help from Larger Research Universities. fMRI Anyone? ............................... 321 The Advent and Recommendation of New Testing Instruments and Interventional Tools .................................................................................................. 323 The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA) ............ 324 The Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire (FreBAQ) ................................ 327 Global perceived effect (GPE) scale ................................................................... 328 Alternatives to fitness-based physical performance testing ................................ 329 Applications of Non-Muscular Paradigms in Clinical Practice ...................................... 331 Summary and Conclusions .......................................................................................................... 334 REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................ 338 APPENDICES ............................................................................................................................. 363 Appendix A: Recruitment Flyer for Pierce County Medical Society.......................................... 363 Appendix B: Pilot Study & Combined Conference Announcement Postcards ........................... 364 Appendix C: Copy of Published Pilot Study Abstract ................................................................ 365 Appendix D: Enrollment Invitation & Clinical Research Announcement Postcard ................... 366 Appendix E: Website Landing Pages for Alliant Spine Project, LTD ........................................ 368 Appendix F: Definitions of Terms and Acronyms ...................................................................... 369 Appendix G: Copy of FABQ for Stratified Randomization ........................................................ 373 Appendix H: Copy of VAS-PAIN / Numerical Rating Scale ..................................................... 375 Appendix I: Copy of RMDQ ....................................................................................................... 376 Appendix J: Copy of PSFS .......................................................................................................... 377 Appendix K: Copy of Timed Endurance Testing Assessment Form .......................................... 378 Appendix L: Photo of Stabilizer Biofeedback (PBU) Device ..................................................... 379 Appendix M: Photo of Full Scale Skeletal Models & Source References .................................. 380 Appendix N: Sources Used for Control Group Intervention (MCE) ......................................... 381 Appendix O: Sources Used for Experimental Group Intervention (FM) ................................... 382 Appendix P: Side-by-Side Listing of Treatment Interventions Between Groups ....................... 384 Appendix Q: Copies of Home Exercise Program / Graded Activity Cover Sheets .................... 391 Appendix R: Copies of Exercise / Graded Activity Adherence Diaries + Med Lists ................. 401 Appendix S: Qualitative Differences between FM & CSE / MCE ............................................. 408 Appendix T: Principles of Ideal Movement ................................................................................ 412 Appendix U: Transparent-Translucent Contrast Images for Skeletal Density ............................ 413 Appendix V: Study Flow Diagram .............................................................................................. 416 Appendix W: Sensory-Motor Learning Model for Working Body Schema ............................... 419 Appendix X: Advisory Disclaimer and Release of Responsibility ............................................. 421 v LIST OF TABLES Table 1: Major Brain Areas Where Pain is Processed .................................................................. 47 Table 2: Copy of Published Pilot Study Research Abstract ........................................................ 178 Table 3: Sources of Recruitment for Study Participants ............................................................. 188 Table 4: Average Years of Experience of Clinicians delivering the Specialty "Core" vs "Feldenkrais Method" Intervention ............................................................................................. 191 Table 5: FABQ Work Subscale (w) and the Physical Activity Subscale (pa) and their Thresholds of Criteria for designating Excessive Scores for High Fear-Avoidance Cognitions in CNSLBP .................................................................................................................................. 201 Table 6: Random Stratification Subgrouping Distribution based on FABQ ............................... 206 Table 7: Phase I CSB/MCE Treatment Progression: Core Stabilization and Motor Control Exercise Interventions at 2xs per Week for First Two Weeks .................................................... 233 Table 8: Phase II CSB/MCE Treatment Progression: Static and Dynamic Motor Control Exercise administered at 2x per Week for Second Two Weeks................................................................. 234 Table 9: Phase III CSB/MCE Treatment Progression: Dynamic and Reactive Motor Control Exercise administered at 1x per Week for Last Four Weeks ...................................................... 235 Table 10: Virtual Realty Bones (VRB3): Phase I Imagery Intervention for Body Schema Acuity TM and Skeletal Density Imagery Continuity Training (SDI) also using The Feldenkrais® Method (FM) ............................................................................................................................................ 249 Table 11: Phase II Training for Experimental (VRB3/FM) Group via 2x per Week for Second Two Weeks - Feldenkrais Method® Themes: Expanding Sense of Ground Support via Developmental Actions ............................................................................................................... 250 vi Table 12: Phase III Training for Experimental (VRB3/FM) Group via 1x per Week for Last Four Weeks - Feldenkrais Method® Themes: Reciprocating Variations of Active Movement Trajectories .................................................................................................................................. 251 Table 13: Basic Demographics of Study Participants ................................................................. 258 Table 14: Confounding Bio-Psycho-Social, Surgical, & Orthopedic Variables of Study Participants .................................................................................................................................. 259 Table 15: Drop-out Participants and Reasons for Leaving either at Start of Study/or prior to End of Phase I ..................................................................................................................................... 260 Table 16: Drop-out Participants and Reasons for Leaving at Conclusion of Phase 1 Data Collection - and without Completing Phase II or Phase III Components of Total Intervention .................................................................................................................... 261 Table 17: Calculation and Display of Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for all Primary Outcome Measures that occurred between Experimental Group and Control Group for the duration of the Current Study ...................................................................................................... 263 Table 18: Wilcoxon Rank Sum Test and Bonferroni Adjustment Method to assess NonParametric Statistical Significance of p < 0.05 for VAS, RMDQ, and PSFS Scores occurring between Groups ........................................................................................................................... 273 Table 19: Paired Two-Tailed T-Test to assess Parametric Statistical Significance of p < 0.05 for changes occurring during Timed Endurance Testing Totals over Time ..................................... 274 Table 20: Mean Average for Flexion/Extension Endurance Ratios at Pre & Post Intervention and Relevance to Clinically Meaningful Thresholds of < 1.5 being indicative of Improved Trunk function via reduced Agonist-Antagonist Disparity in Experimental Group as compared to Controls ....................................................................................................................................... 276 vii Table 21: Comparing Current Study Results with previously published 2012 Data and MICD Scores .......................................................................................................................................... 304 viii LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1: Anatomical Depictions of Transverse Abdominis (TRA) and Lumbar Mutlifidis (LM) ........................................................................................................ 25 Figure 2: A Photo Depiction of the Pressure Biofeedback Unit "PBU" Device (a.k.a. The Stabilizer™) .................................................................................................................................. 28 Figure 3: The Six Presented Video Clips for Mental Simulation of Daily Actions during fMRI Recordings ..................................................................................................................................... 53 Figure 4: Pooled Motor Imagery Tasks as a Composite of Six Video Simulated Actions as exhibited on fMRI ......................................................................................................................... 57 Figure 5: Brain Pathways for Cognitive and Emotional Influence on Pain .................................. 60 Figure 6: Differences between Traditional Guided Imagery and Feldenkrais Method Contact Imagery .......................................................................................................................................... 83 Figure 7: Anatomical Schematic and Location of Vestibular Apparatus ...................................... 86 Figure 8: Telecast Interview of Moshe Feldenkrais .................................................................... 103 Figure 9: Moshe Feldenkrais Teaching at Amherst, MA, circa 1980 ......................................... 104 Figure 10: Discovery and Implementation of Proportionate Skeletal Models ........................... 115 Figure 11: Tri-Plane Location of Hip Socket Axis ..................................................................... 116 Figure 12: Testing for Hip Socket Anatomical Axis Perceptual Acuity. ................................... 118 Figure 13: Sample Distribution of Disparities in Accurate Perceptual Localization of "Anatomical Hip Sockets" Commonly associated for Patients Presenting with Recurrent or Persistent Low Back Pain Problems ............................................................................................ 119 Figure 14: Clinical Distortion of Body Schema Acuity for Hip Sockets in Chronic Low Back Pain. ............................................................................................................................................ 120 ix Figure 15: Demonstration of Corresponding Dimensional Relationships between Width of Hip Socket Joint Axes and Width of Temporal Bones via the Visual Aid of Head of Femur Models and Eyeglass Frames ................................................................................................................... 122 Figure 16: Demonstrating Hip Axis Socket/VR Hip Replacement ............................................. 124 Figure 17: Generalizing the effects of VR Hip Replacement/Body Schema Acuity Training on Qualities of Comparative Arrangement to be Experienced and Contrasted (and therefore learned) during Daily Routine Activities................................................................................................... 125 Figure 18: Case Example: Virtual Reality Hip Replacement in Severe Chronic Low Back Pain............................................................................................... 128 Figure 19: Perceptual Discontinuity between Hips, Pelvis, and Low Back’s Spine Column ..... 131 Figure 20: The Superficial Region of the Posterior S-I Joint (SIJ) ............................................ 132 Figure 21: Components of Highest Bone Density within Pelvis and Head ................................ 134 Figure 22: Anatomical Outlines of Areas of Highest Bone Density ........................................... 135 Figure 23: Vertical Contiguity of Pelvis-Hips Opposite Head .................................................... 136 Figure 24: Tape Measure Rendering of Densest Bone Regions leading to the Operationalizing of "The Proportionality of Thirds Model™" in Feldenkrais Movements ........................................ 138 Figure 25: Photo Demonstration of the Proprietary Manual Therapy Approach ........................ 139 Figure 26: Photo-Captured Demonstration of "Self-Applied Visual-Haptic Self-Touch." ......... 140 Figure 27: Anatomical Re-framing of "Core Robustness": Pelvis-Hips opposite Head ............. 141 Figure 28: Anatomical and perceptual reframing of "Top of Leg" during Standing Trunk Rotation .................................................................................................. 142 Figure 29: Anatomic Locations and Junctions for Thoracic Pedicles and Costo-Vertebral Joints .......................................................................................................... 146 x Figure 30: Anatomical Models depicting Vestibular Apparatus ................................................. 147 Figure 31: Visual-Haptic Projection Techniques for Cardinal Axis Coordinates of Vestibular Apparatus and their Anatomic Location................................................................ 148 Figure 32: Locating the Coordinates and Angles of Orientation within RIGHT Inner Ear ........ 150 Figure 33: Visual and Dimensional Relationship Correspondences between Pelvis Diagonals, Hip Complex, and Inner Ear ........................................................................................................ 152 Figure 34: Guided Self-Exploration and Treatment involving Side-Tilting of Head ................. 153 Figure 35: Demonstration of Therapist guided Functional-Cognitive Manual Therapy (CMT) Maneuvers ................................................................................................................................... 154 Figure 36: Latearlity of Chest and Leg "Hemispheric differences" after FI® Sessions .............. 156 Figure 37: Vicon™ Kinematic Videography Image Reconstruction for Initial Stance Phase of Gait ......................................................................................................... 159 Figure 38: Vicon™ Kinematic Videography Image Reconstruction for Terminal Stance Phase of Gait .................................................................................................... 160 Figure 39: Hemi-pelvis Model of Inner Ilia depicting Pathways for Contra-lateral vs. Ipsi-lateral Skeletal Transmission .................................................................................................................. 162 Figure 40: Applications of Visual-Haptic Self-Touch Hand Placements for Simulating and Detecting the Anatomical Pathways of Skeletal Transmission during Gait................................ 163 Figure 41: Demonstration of Skeletal Transmission Contact Points for the Simulation of Gait Function from Ground-up............................................................................................................ 164 Figure 42: Centaur-Human Avatar .............................................................................................. 166 Figure 43: Use of Deer Antler for Augmented Sensory Reality ................................................. 167 Figure 44: Use of Imagery Robustness and Comparative Scaling for Cognitive Reframing ..... 169 xi Figure 45: Active Anatomical Skeletal Density Imagery through the use of Radiographic Art. ....................................................................................................... 170 Figure 46: Images Depicting Self-Simiularity of Structural Features Found to Occur within Natural Systems ........................................................................................................................... 175 Figure 47: Site Locations and Facilities used for the Current Study ........................................... 189 Figure 48: Gray-Scale Copy of VAS-PAIN / Numerical Rating Scale....................................... 210 Figure 49: Demonstration of McGill’s Timed Endurances Tests ............................................... 216 Figure 50: Demonstration of Stopwatch Instrumentation ........................................................... 217 Figure 51: Control Group Intervention Sources .......................................................................... 225 Figure 52: Trunk Range of Motion and Segmental Hypermobility Testing ............................... 226 Figure 53: The Stabilizer™ Pressure Bio-feedback Unit ............................................................. 228 Figure 54: Demonstration of PBU Biofeedback ......................................................................... 229 Figure 55: Demonstration of PBU Biofeedback Procedure in Multiple Positions ...................... 230 Figure 56: Preliminary Physical Exam for Conducting the Initial Assessment for the (VRB3/FM) Experimental Group .................................................................................................................... 239 Figure 57: Demonstration of Sensory Acuity Impedances at Dorsal Spine ................................ 240 Figure 58: Demonstration of Foot Contact & Surface Acuity Procedures .................................. 242 Figure 59: Pre- and Post-Body Scan Techniques for Self Assessment ....................................... 244 Figure 60: Outline of Principles that contribute to an effective Feldenkrais Method® Lesson ... 246 Figure 61: Resource Materials for HEP’s derived from Professional Feldenkrais® Audio Programs ...................................................................................................................................... 248 Figure 62: Excel Spreadsheet Layout for Data Collection for Control Group and Experimental Group ........................................................................................................................................... 254 xii Figure 63: Mean Pre/Post Outcome Measures for VAS PAIN Over Time ................................. 265 Figure 64: Mean Pre/Post Outcome Measures for RMDQ DISABILITY Over Time................ 266 Figure 65: Mean Pre/Post Outcome Measures for PSFS FUNCTION Over Time ..................... 267 Figure 66: Mean Pre/Post Outcome Measures for ENDURANCE SCORES Over Time .......... 268 Figure 67: Pre/Post Outcome Measures for FLEXION/EXTENSION RATIOS Over Time ..... 269 Figure 68: Bar Graph for comparing Pre- & Post-Flexion/Extension Ratios between Groups .. 275 Figure 69: Visual Schematic for Attractor States over Time ...................................................... 291 Figure 70: Conceptual Drawings: Regional Isolation vs. Regional Interdependence ................. 293 Figure 71: Expanding the Scope for a More Multi-Factorial PT Practice .................................. 311 Figure 72: Internal Model for Sensorimotor Integration and Efference Copy for Motor Control ........................................................................................................................ 313 Figure 73: Information Processing Model for working Body Schema during VRB3 FM Rx ..... 316 1 CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION Chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) continues to remain a prevalent, multifaceted and complex problem with increasing incidence, duration, costs, and escalating disability and co-morbidity (Hoy et al., 2014; Manchikanti & Hirsch, 2015). This despite an extensive array of numerous medical-surgical approaches, pharmacotherapies and procedures, physical therapy and other rehabilitative modalities and protocols, psychotherapeutic, psychophysiological, and cognitive-behavioral approaches, and different kinds of multi-varied complementary and alternative medicine practices ranging from acupuncture to chiropractic, to herbal medicine, nutrition, and naturopathy, to other massage, manual and manipulative, and movement therapies, to exercise and personal training, Pilates and yoga-based therapies. Most physical interventions demonstrate limited effectiveness (Assendelft, Morton, Yu, Suttorp, & Shekelle, 2004; Furlan et al., 2005; Hayden, van Tulder, Malmivaara, & Koes, 2005; Staal, de Bie, de Vet, Hildebrandt, & Nelemans, 2008), and different behavioral and exercise therapies appear to be equally effective (Henschke, Ostelo, & van Tulder, 2010; Ostelo et al., 2005; Henschke et al., 2010; van Tulder, Malmivaara, Esmail, & Koes, 2000). In sum, comparative meta-analyses and repeated systematic reviews - including those conducted through the trusted Cochrane Collaboration - continue to indicate that each intervention has no superiority over others, and that each category has shown limited short- and long-term effect size for impact upon disorder improvement. Therefore, effective treatments remain particularly elusive for CNSLBP. Musculoskeletal pain is the dominant type of chronic pain affecting the world population, exerting an enormous impact on individuals, societies, and health care systems (Briggs et al., 2016; World Health Organization, 2015). Among the musculoskeletal pain conditions, low back pain is the most common and often the most costly. The incidence of low back pain has reached 2 epidemic proportions, affecting up to 84% of adults at least once in their lives (Dagenais, Caro, & Haldeman, 2008). Most acute low back pain episodes are self-limited, with symptoms remitting within a few weeks and calling for little or no intervention. However, it is estimated that up to 10% of low back pain sufferers develop and transition to a chronic pain condition characterized by long-term pain and associated disability (Pengel, Herbert, Maher, & Refshauge as cited in Trost & Parsons, 2014). The Institute of Medicine has estimated that chronic pain affects approximately 100 million adults in the United States, with an estimated annual cost of up to $635 billion. In the United States, chronic low-back pain (CNSLBP) is the most common cause of job-related disability and a leading contributor to missed work. Back pain is also the second most common neurological ailment in the United States — only headache is more common. Each year, lowback pain is estimated to affect approximately 38% of people worldwide (Deyo et al., 2014a). Chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) can be defined as pain in the area on the posterior aspect of the body from the lower margin of the 12th ribs to the lower gluteal folds with or without pain referred into one or both lower limbs and with duration that lasts for at least 12 weeks to three months or more (Hoy et al., 2014). In the general practice and primary care setting, and for 85% of patients who present with low back pain, the cause cannot be definitively known - hence the term, non-specific low back pain (Waddell, 2004). Chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) also remains the number one symptom disorder for consulting complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) practitioners (Chenot et al., 2007; Kanodia, Legedza, Davis, Eisenberg, & Phillips, 2010). Low back pain has subsequently attracted a considerable amount of cross-disciplinary research. Particularly since current approaches to the management of chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) have 3 shown limited effectiveness (Wand, Parkitny, et al., 2011). Other reviews again assert the fact that CNSLBP is a complex disorder and highly resistant to change with generic approaches to management (van Middelkoop et al., 2010). Effect sizes from randomized controlled trials utilizing conservative treatment for non-specific chronic low back pain are small. Thus, chronic low back pain remains a multifaceted and complex problem with increasing prevalence and escalating disability and co-morbidity despite extensively numerous modalities of different kinds of treatments (Hoy et al., 2014; Manchikanti & Hirsch 2015). CNSLBP in the Context of Physical Therapy Physical therapy and physical medicine interventions are perhaps the most common recommendation for chronic and recurrent conditions that compromise human functioning. Filling the expansive void that exists between pharmacological and surgical interventions, physical therapists are well positioned to implement a broad spectrum of modalities, methods, and activities from which to impact the behavioral and functional aspects of chronic conditions that typically remain outside the scope of usual care. However, within the context of usual physical therapy practice, and among those that have been systematically studied, only brief courses of educational interventions, short courses of manipulation/mobilization and supervised exercise therapy are recommended as the most marginally-effective treatment approaches for addressing the persistent and recurring problem of CNSLBP (Airaksinen et al., 2006). Other traditional physical therapy electrotherapeutic and thermal modalities, such as the use of heat/cold, traction, laser, ultrasound, short wave, interferential, TENS, corsets - even repeated massage therapy - are ‘not recommended’ as has been determined through current and comparative evidence being accrued through European Union (EU) systematic reviews. 4 Overall and across the systematic compilation of international studies, the effect sizes for most therapeutic procedures are rather modest. The comprehensive review summary guidelines otherwise state that most promising approaches seem to be cognitive behavioral interventions encouraging activity and supervised exercise. However, the review panel furthermore reports that the “active ingredient” of exercise programs is largely unknown; this requires considerably more research, in order to allow the development and promotion of a wider variety of low cost, but effective exercise programs. In addition, the application of cognitive behavioral principles to the prescription of exercises also needs to be further evaluated (Airaksinen et al., 2006). Within composite reviews and guidelines for treatment of CNSLBP, Exercise Therapy is defined as Any program in which the participants are required to carry out repeated voluntary dynamic movements and /or static muscular contractions (in each case, either 'wholebody' or 'region-specific'; and either with or without external loading), and where such exercises were intended as a treatment for low back pain. As an important criterion for inclusion in a systematic review, exercise programs are required to be "supervised" and "prescribed" directly by a qualified and attending clinician (van Tulder et al., 2000). CNSLBP in the Context of Psychologically-Informed Physical Therapy Traditionally, “treatments for chronic musculoskeletal disorders (CMSDs) such as chronic low back pain (CLBP) have been anchored in a biomedical model. This model is based on a structural pathology paradigm where insult to anatomical structures is believed to be the sole driver of the condition” (Pelletier, Higgins, & Bourbonnais, 2015a, p. 1583). It is now known that multiple psychological factors have been implicated in the transition and persistence of chronic low back pain. They include anxiety and depression, catastrophizing, kinesiophobia (fear of movement) and somatization - further defined here as the expression of distress through 5 anxiety in terms of ongoing bodily attention and the corresponding persistence of physically referenced symptoms, but for which no physical cause can be found (Manchikanti & Hirsch, 2015). The practice and profession of physical therapy is only just beginning to actively incorporate the inclusion of adjunctive psychological-based approaches, such as cognitive behavioral therapy and graded exposure / graded activity methods into their usual standards for practice. Early evidence, particularly stemming from fear-avoidance models, has thus contributed toward recent trends toward the development of integrative physical therapy practice, where supervised exercise therapies are being combined with biopsychosocial and psychologicallybased graded activity strategies; the latter being informed through coping, pacing, and cognitivebehavioral principles. These hybrids of interdisciplinary influence have now emerged as among the most commonly advocated recommendations for the treatment of CNSLBP. However, as recent as 2016, a more highly specialized systematic review and meta-analysis study found no statistical difference between (a) physical therapy-based rehabilitation, (b) behavioral/psychologically informed approaches, and (c) combined interventions as a hybrid of the two (O’Keeffe et al., 2016). Another comparative study on physical therapy interventions found no difference in treatment outcomes between implementing a supervised core stabilization training and motor control exercise program (the usual conventional and biomechanically specific approach - but without necessarily invoking cognitive behavioral principles), and a matched treatment group that underwent supervised graded activity exercises. This intervention comprised a generalized program of daily and graded exposure toward performing a quota of previously avoidant activities, but being largely inclusive of cognitive behavioral therapy principles as the main 6 operative feature (Macedo et al., 2012). Since each intervention demonstrated an almost equal similarity for treatment outcomes, and by each arm of the study concurrently using the most common, usual, and customary outcome measures, these treatments thus remain concomitantly represented in predominance as the current and usual practice standard in most outpatient physical therapy and pain rehabilitation specialty centers. However, these outcomes also demonstrate only modest overall average change in reported symptom reduction and only modest gains in functional profile improvements, as compared to outcomes for other musculoskeletal pain conditions. Furthermore, the larger consortiums of systematic reviews evaluating the effectiveness of varied categories, approaches, and types of exercise therapy commonly conclude that there is little relationship between changes in clinical symptoms and changes in any “objectively measured” aspect of functional capacity (e.g., logical expectancies for corresponding changes in "strength," "flexibility," "back muscle endurance," and so on). This may explain the conclusion that “to date” there is no convincing evidence to endorse the superiority of use for one type of exercise over another type of exercise in the treatment of chronic low back pain (Hayden, van Tulder, Malmivaara, et al., 2005; Macedo et al., 2012). CNSLBP in the Context of a New Model as Proposed through the Current Study Thus, the current status of clinical evidence must reveal itself into new a whole new level of inquiry, especially when applied to the problem of CNSLBP by asking: (a) if the modest effects of exercise and/or cognitive behavioral interventions - as categories in and of themselves - are just a mere consequence of any kind of structured activity being attended to by another person, and therefore only a non-specific effect; and (b) is there a competing intervention model that looks to consider other underlying mechanisms, but from a newer neuroplasticity-based 7 perspective that is not necessarily exercise physiology based in the traditional sense, nor directly a cognitive-behavioral-based progam being primarily manifested through traditional prestructured protocols. This study - herein referred to as "the current study" – instead introduces an overview of some emergent findings being derived from the neuroscience of chronic pain and neuroplasticity. It then postulates a possibility for the training of new sensations, of forwarding a novel basis for treatment being based upon perceptually enhancing the complex interactions between sensory-acuity and movement-dexterity as underlying stimulus-association-response mechanisms that are hypothesized to be potentially effective toward creating new alterations in and throughout the somato-topic organization of the sensory-motor cortex of the human brain; thereby being also associated and correlated with the subjective experience of ‘body schema’ phenomena as well as for remedying the persistence and perseverance of chronic pain. The treatment method or approach herein referred to as ‘body schema acuity training’ is to be accomplished through implementation of lesser known somatic education and movement therapy interventions (specifically, The Feldenkrais Method®) in conjunction with additional imagery constructs being afforded through virtual reality based sensory-motor perceptual experiments with a corresponding aim to invoke awareness of vestibular – temporal bone relationships and their corresponding relationship to using skeletal density contiguities throughout the skeleton as a total perceptual framework for the re-construction of working body schema – all together being conceptually condensed for publication into the acronym VRB3 . The current status of the Feldenkrais Method® and its embellishment through the addition of the proposed VRB3 visual-tactile virtual reality interactive interventions are all comprehensively reviewed from the perspective of both historical and current published literature as well as from data attained from my original pilot study. 8 These foundations finally culminate into the implementation and performance of an actual RCT design. This design, as used in the current study, is implemented to test-compare the newly devised combined sensory-motor learning intervention, Virtual Reality Bones™ and Feldenkrais Movements (VRB3/FM) against the current standardized one, Core Stabilization Biofeedback and Motor Control Exercises (CSB/MCE), and to furthermore assess and discuss their comparative and respective effects on various metrics for discerning clinical improvement in CNSLBP that are compiled and presented in greater detail in the methods section of Chapter 3. Traditional Frameworks, Alternative Viewpoints, and New Research Questions Historically, the physical therapy exercise and manual therapy models being most utilized for treatment of back pain problems, including CNSLBP, have progressed through various stages and with trend-like fashion over the past three decades. Beginning in the late 1980s, the predominant models encompassed an isolationist, regionally specific manual therapy and specific exercise models, which were then supplemented with general exercise programs for strength, range of motion, cardiovascular (aerobic) endurance, and flexibility. Then, in the late 1990s, came the advent of the movement system coordination models and corresponding specific motor control exercises directly designed for controlling aberrant movement segments – namely inter-vertebral structures including discs and facet joints at lumbar spine. More recently, since the early-mid 2000s, there has been a growing novel consensus geared toward investigating the regional interdependence conceptual models in which the concept that “seemingly unrelated impairments in a remote anatomical region may contribute to, or be associated with, the patient’s primary presenting complaint [emphasis in original]” (Wainner, Whitman, Cleland, & Flynn, 2007, p. 658). This model is stated to become a more 9 adequate basis for the management of patients with common musculoskeletal complaints and with added implications for future research design and clinical practice; most notably, citing hip joint relationships to low back problems (Cibulka, Sinacore, Cromer, & Delitto, 1998; Porter & Wilkinson, 1997). Finally, the most recent development within the past five years has been the necessary integration of inter-disciplinary models and biopsychosocial models to which greater inclusion of cognitive-behavioral and graded-activity approaches are becoming more and more incorporated into the usual scope of physical therapy practice, particularly in conjunction with supervised exercise regimens. Prior to the emergence of traditional core stabilization and motor control exercise, usual physical therapy modality and manual therapy techniques, and their corresponding exercise interventions, had deemed the clinically observable findings of stiffness of vertebral segments in response to motion testing as the underlying mechanism and rationale for instituting a variety of manual therapy schools and certifications. These focused on spinal mobilization techniques, and where applicable, spinal manipulations being applied to vertebral subluxations, facilitated segments, Grade II hypo-mobility dysfunctions, and non-allopathic manual therapy lesions and the like, to reduce symptoms of stiffness, lack of mobility, pain, and to improve mobility function. Later, the widespread acceptance of lumbar core stabilization and motor control exercises as a more validated treatment approach was supported by a new and explanatory treatment rationale; citing low back pain as a "motor control problem" resulting in/or as a resultant of "motor control deficit," as these findings were being repeatedly demonstrated, observed, and quantified within a controlled laboratory setting. The originators and proponents of segmental stabilization and lumbar-pelvic motor control exercises (Richardson, Hodges, & 10 Hides, 2004; Richardson, Jull, Hodges, & Hides, 1999) discovered (through EMGelectromyography and ultrasonography) that a distinct neural inhibition was occurring only and specifically within deeper inter-segmental (inter-vertebral) muscle groups; namely, the Transverse Abdominus (TrA) and the Lumbar Multifidus (LM) muscles during repeated dynamic movement and perturbation tasks and only in patients with chronic or recurrent low back pain (LBP) in comparison with controls that did not have back pain. The resulting over-selection and compensatory adjustment of the more superficial, multisegmental muscle groups (i.e., Lumbar Longissimus, Quadratus Lumborum; other lumbar paraspinal groups) were demonstrated to become more reflexively activated, hypertonic, which furthermore added to aberrant motion and relative buckling at "de-stabilized" spinal segments. This is due to a localized loss or inhibition of intersegmental motor control and subsequent overreliance on passive ligamentous elements. Thus, it has been purported that these abberations and disparities can result in shear, repeated trauma, and continued over-activation of superficial multi-joint muscles that are continuously and reflexively bracing in a vicious cycle of inflammation, nociception, stiffness, and pain. Consequently, it was acquired hypermobility occurring at select vertebral segments due to trauma, injury, genotype-phenotype, or poor muscular habits over time, and that deficits of recruitment (of motor control) occurring at specific intervertebral (deep stabilizer) muscles, normally correspondent to protect them, that was implicated as an alternative cause. That is, the antithetical to prior mechanisms of rationale that had previously cited capsular stiffness and corresponding joint hypo-mobility as primary or presumed culprits. As a result, these motor control containment processes and their affected areas of deficit needed to be re-trained to 11 become more stabilized (not mobilized!) in order to reduce pain, and to improve the overall balance of control for optimal neuromuscular function at lumbar spine segments. Becoming known as The Queensland Model, via The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia, under the direction and collaboration of its founders (Richardson, Jull, Hodges, & Hides, 1999; Richardson, Hodges, & Hides, 2004), a host of repeated studies have supported this approach and its corresponding rationale internationally, and these are elaborated in Chapter 2. Subsequently, within the continued status of routine and current outpatient physical therapy practices throughout most of Europe and North America, core stability training and motor control exercise routines remain the dominant and standard prescription for chronic nonspecific LBP. This, despite emerging contrary evidence of non-dominance, is also elaborated in Chapter 2. For the current study, in lieu of investigating treatment intervention designs based on spine mobilization (with attribution to joint hypo-mobility and structural stiffness based-on presumed tissue pathology), or spine stabilization (with attribution to inter-segmental joint hyper-mobility and motor control focal deficits), my research inquiry has instead put forth an objective to test-compare a multi-modal cognitive-perceptual model for ‘spine organization’ by instituting a novel and interactive learning approach via the implementation of a body schema acuity training model. This is more specifically detailed by the acronym (VRB3) in later sections, and then being operationalized through selected Feldenkrais® movements (FM) that are clinically known to have positive influences on reducing low back pain (LBP). Synopsis of the Problem Although researchers have extensively conducted research on the sensory-nociceptive, psychological, and motor factors involved or associated with musculoskeletal disorders and 12 chronic pain, they have studied these factors quite separately from each other and in limited combinations. This has resulted in mostly separate bodies of evidence (Butera, Fox, & George, 2016). Historically, bio-medical models have had large influence over the allied-health professions and their development in both research and practice. Musculoskeletal rehabilitative care and research has therefore been traditionally guided by a structural pathology paradigm with resource emphasis being allocated towards the structural, functional, and biological abnormalities - being located locally and exclusively within the musculoskeletal system’s tissues and structures – as the sole basis to understand, implicate, and treat the continuing problem of musculoskeletal disorders and musculoskeletal pain. However, the structural pathology model does not adequately explain many of the clinical and experimental findings in subjects with chronic musculoskeletal disorders, and more importantly, treatment guided by this paradigm fails to effectively treat many of these conditions (Pelletier, Higgins, & Bourbonnais, 2015b). For example, the structural-pathology paradigm fails as a working model for resolving the continuing allusive questions, such as (a) why diagnostic findings correlate poorly with pain and dysfunction; (b) the presence of bilateral findings with unilateral injuries; (c) why a large proportion of persons with damage to musculoskeletal structures being revealed upon clinical history and/or diagnostic imaging are or remain asymptomatic; (d) why some persons heal and others go on to develop chronic musculoskeletal pain; and (e) the presence and persistence of continuing sensory-motor abnormalities that cannot be structurally explained upon regional or isolated testing – and often occurring in areas remotely distant from site of original injury or onset, and furthermore bearing no correlation to other 13 clinical or structural findings – be they orthopedic, degenerative, rheumatologic, ascribed ‘subluxation,’ muscle ‘length-tension’ imbalance, myofascial referred pain, or otherwise. More recent studies suggest that chronic musculoskeletal disorders do not simply result from ongoing structural pathology to peripheral tissues, but instead involves a complex interplay between original onset of peripheral or structural injury (versus the perception of threat without actual tissue damage), altered afferent information conveyed from peripheral receptors toward the spinal cord, brain stem, and cortical areas, compensated and protective patterns of action, emotion, and behaviors (from whatever the source) that continue long beyond the stages of tissue healing. These pathways are perhaps better explained through recognizing the highly distributive neurophysiological processes that are perpetually involved in widespread neuroplasticity, central sensitization, and change. However, in usual practice, the current conventional interventions in physical rehabilitation do not usually address the underlying neuroplastic changes in the central nervous system that are inherently, intricately, and necessarily associated with both the regulation and persistence of musculoskeletal disorders - and most particularly for chronic musculoskeletal pain (Pelletier et al., 2015a; Snodgrass et al., 2014). In an excellent Physical Therapy journal review article on neuroplastic changes and chronic musculoskeletal disorders, Pelletier et al. (2015a) state that: Failure to effectively treat conditions such as chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) may stem from the fact that the central neuroplastic changes occurring across distributed areas (that are) associated with this condition have largely been ignored and may explain why treatment effects are consistently small regardless of the type of intervention. (Pelletier et al., 2015a, p. 1585) There is additional growing evidence that pain associated with musculoskeletal disorders such as osteoarthritis and CLBP may be, at least in part, the result of the plasticity of the sensory 14 representation of the body and perceptual disturbances (McCabe, 2011; Pelletier et al., 2015b; Preston & Newport, 2011; Wand, Keeves, et al., 2013). Most recently, evidence has emerged through a prominent research team from School of Physiotherapy, The University of Notre Dame Australia, Fremantle, Western Australia, to suggest that disrupted perceptual awareness of the back is significantly and uniquely contributory to pain intensity within a sampled population (N=251) of patients with CNSLBP. Most interesting among their recent research findings in current publication is their conclusion that “disturbed body perception appears to be more strongly associated with pain intensity than psychological distress, fear avoidance beliefs, or an objective measure of lumbar spine sensitivity” (Wand et al., 2016, p. 1009). Recent findings thereby suggest that a change in both conceptual model and mode of approach is required within rehabilitation interventions such that they can begin to integrate newly emergent findings of particular neuroplastic changes that are known to occur across central, peripheral and autonomic levels of the nervous systems. Done so that they can be incorporated into actual innovative treatments for the continuing and recurrent problem of CNSLBP. As consistent with earlier research from respective fields, the interventions that target and address cortical reorganization – the sensory and motor mappings of the body within and throughout the brain’s representative cortex that alter and accrue through experience driven neuroplasticity – are among the approaches that have been hypothesized and discussed to show most promise toward producing more efficacious results in terms of decreased pain and improved function in patients with chronic low back pain (Moseley & Flor, 2012). Thus, more novel and innovative treatment approaches must be necessarily developed in order to incorporate the most recent evidence-based discoveries surrounding known neuroplastic changes that have now become known to be associated with the prevalence and persistence of 15 chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP). In addition, these developments will require further controlled research investigations to determine their efficacy upon actual clinical application in comparison to other or existing approaches currently in use. Purpose of the Study, Aims, and Objectives Based upon evolving and continuing practice based evidence becoming more substantiated through more recent peer-reviewed inter-disciplinary research and the results from my pilot study, I propose and postulate that a body schema acuity training approach and a neuroplasticity-based sensory-motor learning intervention - like The Feldenkrais Method® - is a more effective strategy to address the continuing epidemic of CNSLBP as compared to the usual, repeated conventional exercise programs that are currently in use. To test this proposal, a novel intervention was specially designed to combine Body Schema Acuity Training using a newly devised (VRB3) ™ Protocol as a pre-requisite for practicing a progression of selected Feldenkrais® movements (VRB3/FM), and to compare this with an established combined usual intervention in physical therapy for using Core Stabilization Biofeedback (via a Stabilizer™ PBU protocol) as a pre-requisite for practicing a progression of known Motor Control Exercises (CSB/MCE) for a population of patients with CNSLBP - within the same out-patient physical therapy setting - and under randomized controlled conditions for participant selection, consent, and allocation. Research Design and Hypotheses To accomplish this study's objective, a randomized controlled trial (RCT) classical experimental design was used to test the stated hypotheses, as described and outlined below. 16 Hypothesis 1 This study proposed that a population of persons with chronic, non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) who participated in a newly devised Virtual Reality Bones™ (VRB3)™ protocol using skeletal density-based anatomical models combined with motion trajectory skeletal avatars for improving body schema acuity, and undergoing further graded activity entrainment through corresponding Feldenkrais® Movements (The VRB3/FM group) would demonstrate greater symptom reduction and greater functional improvement when compared with a similar population of persons who followed a Core-Stabilization Biofeedback training protocol emphasizing specific recruitment of Transverse Abdominis (TrA) and Lumbar Multifidus (LM) muscle groups as an isolated and specified pre-text for motor learning/motor control during a Motor Control Exercise and graded activity progression series (the CSB/MCE group). Hypothesis 2 In addition, this study predicted that all comparative outcome measures being used for demonstrating greater symptom reduction and greater functional improvement in the experimental (VRB3/FM) group as compared to the control (CSB/MCE) group would all occur at a level of statistical significance being reflected at the customary p-value of less than or equal to 0.05. Rationale The Institute of Medicine identifies chronic pain as a nervous system disease and a highpriority societal health concern. However, current management of this disease and its complications, including lost quality of life, movement impairment, emotional distress, disability, relationship difficulties, and subsequent reductions in function, is inadequate. In the 17 United States (U.S.) alone, chronic pain costs the nation up to $635 billion each year in medical treatment and lost productivity (The National Academies of Science, Engineering, Medicine, Health and Medicine Division, 2011). Pain is a major driver for visits to physicians, a major reason for taking medications. Of these, CNSLBP easily constitutes a majority of cases. Of most significant and recent development, The U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) has determined that the rates of opioid use, the prevalence of use disorder and addiction, and cases for opioid overdose have reached epidemic proportions such that opioid prescriptions overall topped $259 million in 2012, "enough for every adult in the United States to have a bottle of pills," according to the CDC. Upon a systematic review of the evidence, the CDC subsequently drafted recommendations and guidelines for primary care providers around determining when to initiate or continue opioids for chronic pain as well as guidelines for drug selection and dosage, and risk assessment. Its first recommendation: "Non-pharmacologic (i.e., non-drug) therapy and non-opioid pharmacologic therapy are much preferred as first-line interventions for chronic pain" (Centers for Disease Control, 2016). As a foreshadowing trend, The Institute of Medicine specifically highlighted the need for “wider use of existing knowledge” as a main objective for transforming the understanding of pain. While there is high-quality evidence that physical therapy-based exercise interventions have the potential to improve health outcomes, reduce costs, and decrease the risks associated with opioid prescriptions – most notably for hip and knee conditions - the “effect sizes of rehabilitation approaches are otherwise consistently small regardless of intervention in many other musculoskeletal disorders, and therefore multiple and progressive interventions may be warranted (Nijs et al., 2014). Compared to an extensive body of literature and RCTs for both comparing and contrasting the efficacy of Lumbar Core Stabilization and Motor Control 18 Exercises for LBP, there are essentially no RCTs to date for assessing the comparative efficacy of other multi-modal and neuroplasticity-based alternative approaches - particularly, The Feldenkrais Method® - on determining the comparative clinical outcome effects for chronic LBP. Within the domain of classical scientific inquiry, The RCT is recognized the gold standard for determining any effects of a new or existing treatment by its effects being compared with a closely matched control group receiving an alternative form of existing treatment - and while all other corresponding variables, to the extent possible - are kept constant. As a rationale for a study method to compare for clinical efficacy outcomes between two highly contrasted and varied approaches, a randomized controlled trial, classical experimental design was chosen for conducting the current study. Significance of the Study Low back pain remains a substantial health problem and has subsequently attracted a considerable amount of research. Current approaches to the management of chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) have shown limited effectiveness (Wand, Parkitny, et al., 2011). While Core Stabilization has gained wide acceptance as a ubiquitous idea and a most frequently prescribed solution to the rehabilitation and treatment of CLBP in outpatient settings, it has come under recent criticism by a host of clinicians and reviewers (Lederman, 2010b; McGill 2007). However, to date, no study has implemented a body schema/skeletal density/vestibular imagerybased/Feldenkrais Method® intervention design that clearly seeks to ignore the isolation of specific muscle groups, thereby effectively contrasting a basic "core tenant" or baseline principle of traditional core stabilization and other traditional therapy exercises. The primary focus of many therapies on purely orthopedic aspects of structural or strength –motion functional impairments in the spine may be a factor contributing to the lack of 19 success of current treatments. Several lines of evidence suggest that structural changes by themselves (in the absence of behavioral considerations) within the back might be unimportant, and there is growing evidence of extensive cortical reorganization as well as neurochemical and structural alterations in the brains of people with CNSLBP. These changes could contribute to the persistence of the problem and might represent a legitimate dimension of approach for therapy (Flor, Braun, Elbert, & Birbaumer, 1997; Tsao, Galea, & Hodges 2008; Wand, Parkitny, et al., 2011) as well as an alternative to physical exam-based sub-groupings remaining dependent upon grading of spinal motion segments by the examiner –of which inter-rater reliability remains a question of continued bias and suggestion. The purpose of this single-blind, randomized controlled study (RCT) was to compare a Body Schema Acuity Training protocol using newly applied, newly developed low-cost technology (Virtual Reality Bones™/VRB3) with a respected complementary-alternative, movement and manual therapy, neuroplasticity-based educational intervention (The Feldenkrais Method®) against the most commonly accepted approach being utilized within current and conventional physical therapy practice settings (Core Stabilization Training and Graded Motor Control Exercises). This was conducted for improving the outcomes on usual clinical outcome measures for CNSLBP, and to determine whether there is greater clinical efficacy being demonstrated between one combined intervention or the other for treating the widespread problem of CNSLBP as an outcome of the study itself. This comparative design for the current study may be among one of the first of such comparative interventions wherein the sensory acuity training aspects of each arm of the study are able to most directly inform a consequential and qualitative platform for motor planning – motor control – and graded activity movements that follow them – all while simultaneously 20 controlling for the modulation of important biopsychosocial factors – (and especially of fearavoidance factors for CNSLBP) - through both stratified random assignment at pre-intervention and corresponding provisions for pain-science education and cognitive assurances being implemented throughout the course of all treatment sessions for both groups. In this way, both groups are better assured an inclusion of sensory, motoric, and bio-psychosocial variables coming into play, but with emphasis placed on the discerning variables between each intervention, and which outline the important and contrasting qualitative differences that were induced to occur between participants being enrolled in each arm of the study. Organization of the Dissertation This dissertation is organized in five chapters including: Chapter 1, which contains the expanded introduction and study context; Chapter 2, which contains a description and review of the literature along with my supplementing an extensive record of my historically developing a novel intervention on the basis of practice-based evidence, outlining its detail for possible replication, and its subsequent pilot study; Chapter 3, which contains a full description of the current study design and its method, Chapter 4, which contains a description of the study results and statistical analysis; and Chapter 5, which contains a discussion of the meaning of the results, their applicability, their significance, their generalizability, their limitations, and their directions implied for future research and practice. A new treatment model diagram is thereby outlined for comprehensive review. The references and appendices then formally follow from these five chapters. 21 CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW Overview and Background of LBP in Outpatient Physical Therapy Settings “Back Pain Eludes Perfect Solutions.” So states the headline from The New York Times Well Guide dated May 13, 2008. While the condition is considered epidemic in social and economic cost, the exact cause of pain is never found in 85% of the patients. Even though it is generally accepted that the natural course of acute low back pain (LBP) is self-resolving with symptom reduction and functional restoration to resume work capacity within a period of 2-4 weeks for a majority of cases, the recurrence rate under usual sampled conditions from epidemiological studies are otherwise found to be staggeringly high: ranging from 60% to 86%. This occurs particularly within the first year after the acute episode, and with a median time frame of recurrence within only two months. Many doctors now revert to exercise and counseling over drugs and surgery. Of these cases, 30% can transition into long-term cases of chronic, recurrent low back pain (Hayden, van Tulder, Malmivaara, et al., 2005). The transition to chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) remains a most persistent and disabling health problem worldwide with increasing prevalence and costs despite numerous forms of medical-surgical treatments, usual physical and rehabilitative therapies, cognitivebehavioral and psychological approaches, and a broad base of integrative or other complementary-alternative kinds of intervention. Trends toward increasing prevalence are growing. Out of all 291 conditions studied in the Global Burden of Disease 2010 Study (Vos et al., 2012), LBP ranked highest in terms of years lost to disability, and sixth in terms of overall burden for disability-adjusted years lost within varied epidemiological samplings of total lifespan. The overall global point prevalence of LBP was 9.4% with a 95% confidence interval of 9.0 to 9.8 (Hoy et al., 2014). As an ongoing problem of persistence, current approaches to the 22 management of CNSLBP have shown limited effectiveness (Wand, Parkitny, et al., 2011). Other reviews further assert the fact that CNSLBP is a complex disorder and highly resistant to change with generic approaches to management (van Middelkoop et al., 2010). Despite the purported increase in the sophistication of medical interventions, the burden of back pain continues to rise. Thus, low back pain remains a substantial health problem and has subsequently attracted a considerable amount of research. There is an abundant literature reporting that intensive exercises are more effective than usual care, physical modalities, hot packs and rest, behavioral therapy, no exercise, being put on a waiting list, or placebo treatment. But, no particular type of exercise had been shown to be superior to any other (Bogduk, 2004). The 2006 European guidelines for the management of chronic nonspecific low back pain (Airaksinen et al., 2006) cite evidence levels that purport recommendations for exercise therapies at a level A in comparison to usual general practitioner (GP) care for the reduction of pain and disability and return to work - in at least the mid-term (3-6 months); wherein interpretations of outcome data are based on the following scale: ● Level A (Strong Evidence): Generally consistent findings* provided by a systematic review of multiple high quality randomized controlled trials (RCTs); ● Level B (Moderate Evidence): Generally consistent findings provided by a systematic review of multiple low quality RCTs; ● Level C (Limited or Conflicting Evidence): One RCT (either high or low quality) or inconsistent findings from (a systematic review of) multiple RCTs; ● Level D (No Evidence): No RCTs. *The benchmark threshold for consistent findings becomes applicable when at least greater than or equal to 75% of the qualifying studies being reviewed were shown to have a similar result. 23 In the history of early exercise development, strategies commonly focused on range of motion, strength, and endurance properties of spine and trunk muscles. Historical considerations of athletic ability and other factors of personal disposition have also weighed-in as to the measures of predictive outcome, whether stemming from nature or nurture in the objective assessment of low back pain. However, a more recent work has shown that “three-dimensional low back range of motion exercises (ROM) and associated Stretching activities have no correlation to functional test scores or even the ability to perform functional work” (Parks, Crichton, Goldford, & McGill, 2003). Muscle strength, contrary to previous assumptions, does not play a strong role in the risk or perpetuation of low back pain (McGill, 2006), nor as an accustomed clinical prediction expectancy as evidenced from the contrary results of a five-year prospective study by Luoto, Heliövaara, Hurri, and Alaranta (1995). Yet, in many physical therapy clinical practices and physician referral prescriptions, "ROM exercises, flexibility, and strength measures" continue to remain as persistent and accustomed components for clinical documentation and case management. Core Stabilization and Motor Control Exercise: Status and Purported Efficacy Historically, previous physical therapy exercise standards had emphasized strength, range of motion, flexibility activities, and "proper" body mechanics programs with only marginal efficacy. During the late 1990s and 2000s, a large number of papers had been published on lumbar motor control training, led by a renowned team of researchers from the University of Queensland, Australia. Since the publication of the clinical textbook, Therapeutic Exercise for Spinal Segmental Stabilization in Low Back Pain: A Scientific Basis and Clinical Approach (Richardson et al., 1999), and its successor, Therapeutic exercise for lumbopelvic stabilization: a motor control approach for the treatment and prevention of low back pain (Richardson et al., 24 2004), many outpatient physical therapy practices and industry suppliers have embraced the specific concept of “core-stabilization” or spinal stabilization. The work was also substantiated by aspects included in the segmental instability model for disrupted motor control (movements outside the inter-segmental neutral zone) in spinal problems as was originally proposed in biomechanics research by Panjabi in 1992. This body of work thus accrued many of its foundations through biomechanics and laboratory science that eventually led to the consensus and development of spine stabilization and motor control exercises and a corresponding rationale for the preferential selections of specifically recruiting deep local vs. superficial global muscles. The authors Richardson et al. (1999) prefaced their book stating that: Spinal segmental stabilization is an innovative method of delivering therapeutic exercise to the patient. In many ways, it is the antithesis of traditional exercise methods such as strength and endurance training, which have formed the basis for the therapeutic exercise for musculoskeletal conditions for so long. Spinal segmental stabilization is designed to specifically improve the underlying joint stabilization (between vertebral segments) rather than training functional movement (in a generalized fashion) and hoping joint control improves concurrently. (p. 1) Yet, in the conclusion of their book, the last chapter entitled “Future Directions in Research and Clinical Practice,” they acknowledged that: We believe that all patients who suffer low back pain require specific exercise training and this is based on our experience of the seemingly universal reaction in the deep muscles to back injury and pain. This does not dismiss the benefits of or the need for other types of exercise. Notably, it does not deny the possibility that other methods and techniques of exercise currently in use could (also) result in successful retraining of the deep muscle supporting function. (Richardson et al., 1999, p. 170) Coming to be known as the Australian model for spinal stabilization or The Queensland Model, it classifies the focal attribution of two primary ‘deep core’ intrinsic muscle groups: (a) the Transversus Abdominis (TrA), and (b) the Lumbar Multifidus (LM) as the primary, centrally-mediated determinants for controlling spinal inter-segmental stability by exerting a 25 corset-like distributive containment around lumbar spine inter-segmental attachments. Anatomical re-sections of these muscle groups are highlighted and depicted in Figure 1. Figure 1. Anatomical Depictions of Transverse Abdominis (TrA) and Lumbar Mutlifidis (LM). Each cited as dominant inter-segmental and collective stabilizers of lumbar spine vertebrae by virute of their anatomical attachments in combination with their exerting a syergistic corseting effect around the lumbar spine when properly co-activated. The diaphragm and pelvis floor are also cited as co-accessory to maintaining postural stability. Co-modulation of relative intra-abdominal pressure and tensioning of thoraco-lumbar fascial attachments to these groups are considered to afford a corset-like dimensional mechanism of spinal stiffness directly maintained through both tonic and phasic degrees of muscular contraction of these specific and targeted selections between the coordination of four muscle groups being compositional of the core. As a composite synergy, the respiratory diaphragm and the pelvis floor, in conjunction with the TrA and LM, are co-actively conditioned to be maintained in the performance of daily activities and during leg leverage proprioceptive exercise progressions. Studies have confirmed that the activity of these muscles also occurs in direct conjunction with sudden arm movements and other postural perturbations (Hodges & Richardson, 1996). 26 Furthermore, and irrespective of specific spine pathology, Hodges and Richardson (1996) discovered and recorded the phenomenon of delayed activity / temporal inhibition of the transversus abdominis and the added finding of local multifidus atrophy to be a consistent finding and common link in subjects with low back pain. In other words, in patients with low back pain, the untimely activation of transversus abdominis (TrA) and lumbar multifidi (LM) fails to prepare the spine for the reactive forces from unanticipated--or even anticipated--limb movements. Through ultrasound imaging and surface EMG recordings, they discovered that these stabilizer muscles seem to work somewhat independently of the gross motor system. Since inter-segmental control is impaired and diminished in the presence of low back pain, with decreased sensory acuity and impaired ability of these focal muscle groups to accurately reposition unstable segments, there are added broader deficits in impairment of global factors for proprioception, inter-segmental tissue irritation, and recovery of stability in posture control; namely, observed through impairments of balance when standing on one leg, or two legs, or even sitting in patients with histories of LBP (Taimela & Luoto, 1999). Inhibition of deep proximal stabilizer muscle groups in association with local facet hypermobility leads to over-recruitment compensation by more superficial larger, longer, layers of paraspinal extensors (i.e., erector spinae group, quadratus lumborum) resulting in what is commonly known as splinting or spasm. Lack of control of the middle or deep layers becomes a neurological / motor control problem consistently confirmed through the clinically observed inability of patients or subjects with low back pain to (a) effectively recruit transversus abdominis, and/or (b) effectively recruit or select for effective use of the multifidus in everyday functional activities. Attribution of lumbar spine segmental instability, faulty position sense, and impaired 27 proprioception is given to the relative inactivity of these two deep select inter-segmental muscle groups. The prime rehabilitation prerequisite is to first stabilize and correct for activation (or reactivation) of these select stabilizer groups before restoring gross motor function activities involving the larger, more superficial muscle groups crossing multiple spine segments. These local facilitated actions of transverse abdominis and multifidi are by necessity sub-maximal contractions in order to be selected in isolation without peripheral contraction of abdominal oblique’s or flexors of the trunk. They can be selectively facilitated via palpation, sEMG, visual ultrasonography, or more affordably via pressure biofeedback device gauges under clinical supervision and observation. Thus, the use of a pressure biofeedback unit (PBU), commercially known as The Stabilizer™ are used both as a widely recognized tool for facilitating optimal selection and sub-maximal contraction of TrA and LM and as the method for “Core Stabilization Biofeedback” in the current study. The PBU’s inclusion as a cited training component had previously proved useful for significantly demonstrating that segmental stabilization is superior to superficial strengthening for all measured outcome variables for pain and disability in a previous study for chronic low back pain; and that usual superficial strengthening using a control group did not improve TrA activation capacity (França, Burke, Caffaro, Ramos, & Marques, 2012; França, Burke, Hanada, & Marques, 2010). A separate study referenced effective utilization of the pressure biofeedback unit (PBU) device during biofeedback-assisted lumbar stabilization training to inhibit and control against unwanted lateral pelvic tilt by demonstrating that gluteus medius and internal oblique activity could be significantly activated, while simultaneously differentiating a significant 28 reduction and inhibition of quadratus lumborum activity during a repeated sidelying hip abduction task (Cynn, Oh, Kwon, & Yi, 2006). The PBU testing and training instrument has also been validated by imaging and electromyography tests that are considered to be the gold-standard measurements of TrA performance. According Richardson et al. (1999) and Richardson et al. (2004), normal PBU responses range from -4 to -10 mmHg; and that clinical research applications data accrued by Hodges (2003) had indicated that composite mean normal values were around 5.82 mmHg. A photo depiction of the PBU device (The Stabilizer™) as intended for clinical and research use; and as applied to the comparison treatment group - the control arm in the current study - is shown in Figure 2. Figure 2. A Photo Depiction of the Pressure Biofeedback Unit "PBU" device (a.k.a. The Stabilizer™). This, as intended for clinical and research use; and as applied to the comparison treatment group via the control arm in my current study for the training of (a) core stabilization biofeedback, and (b) the development of improved ‘motor control’ during exercise and daily activities. Hides, Jull, and Richardson (2001) tested the Queensland Model hypothesis in clinical low back pain populations (patients with acute, first-episode low back pain). Thirty-nine participating volunteers with acute LBP were divided into a traditional medical treatment approach group and a specific exercise group. The specific exercise encompassed the Australian method of learning techniques for co-contraction of transversus abdominis and multifidus 29 muscles and challenging this learned skill with a progression of proprioceptive activities from non-weight bearing to weight bearing to balance training on unstable surfaces (gymnastic balls, etc.). Questionnaires were administered one year and three years post-intervention. Results indicated that the core stabilizer specific exercise group reported 54% fewer recurrences of low back pain than the control group (Hides et al., 2001). Another set of studies had revealed through ultrasonography imaging that the lumbar multifidus muscle remained atrophied after a 10-week period when patients with acute LBP did not exercise. But this muscle was recovered to normal size in patients who received a stabilization exercise program that stressed deep abdominal and isolated the transverse abdominis (TrA) and lumbar multifidus (LM) muscle contractions (Hides, Richardson, & Jull, 1996). In addition, some patient groups with low back pain had demonstrated hypertrophy of the lumbar multifidus (LM) muscles at post-intervention subsequent to another course of specifically directed low-load stabilization exercises (Hides et al., 2001). The efficacy of specific stabilization exercise was shown to be effective in reducing pain and disability in chronic low back pain with further expansion to other clinical application areas by suggestion that they could also be helpful in the treatment of cervicogenic headache and associative neck pain as well as for pelvic and pelvic floor pain (Ferreira, Ferreira, Maher, Herbert, & Refshauge, 2006). Thus, these evidence-based trends in rehabilitation had suggested that the problems of dysfunction in chronic recurrent low back pain were not an issue of strength vs. weakness, flexibility vs. stiffness, spinal alignment vs. subluxation, or general deconditioning vs. aerobic endurance, but one of motor control. Rather than provide inadequate general education for facilitating the control of lumbo-pelvic position to a neutral state - as with unidirectional strength training and generalized co-contraction exercises - updated strategies were then developed to 30 retrain the specific control of different components of the muscle system; namely, restoration of specific control of deep local middle layer muscles Transverse Abdominis (TrA) and Lumbar Multifidus (LM) to optimize the control of intervertebral (intersegmental) shear forces between lumbar spine segments (Richardson et al., 1999). By selecting deep muscles, the goal is to reduce over-activity of superficial muscles and then train coordinated control of deep and superficial muscles to work appropriately to meet the demands of spinal control. This is commonly instituted through a progression of exercise protocols from leg loading on spinal stability, gymnastic ball exercises, weight-bearing exercises, and with progression to dynamic balance training on unstable platforms and surfaces (O'Sullivan, Phyty, Twomey, & Allison, 1997; Richardson et al., 1999). Clinical applications textbooks and course work continues to stem from this evidencebased literature. One author cited that Success rates such as these are unheard of …and that no other method of back pain treatment has been shown to be so completely successful at correcting the persistent problems that develop once the spine becomes injured and in preventing future episodes of back pain. (Jemmett, 2003, p. 42) As such, core stabilization advocates and clinical instructors claim a validated consensus of scientific evidence in the literature from which to give acceptance to their approach (Brill & Couzens, 2001; Hanney, 2009). Correspondingly, usual clinical consensus and usual standards for common practice across the world now seems to almost universally advocate for local muscles including transversus abdominis (TrA), and lumbar multifidus (LM) - commonly called “core” muscle groups - for assessment and training in virtually any low back integrated treatment or exercise program, and for the improvement of motor control. Another updated systematic review and meta-analysis evaluating the effectiveness of motor control exercises in targeting these "stabilizer" muscles had more recently concluded that: 31 The pooled results favored motor control exercise (MCE) compared with general exercise with regard to pain in the short and intermediate term and with regard to disability during all time periods. MCE was also superior to spinal manual therapy with regard to disability during all time periods but not with regard to pain. Compared with minimal intervention, MCE was superior with regard to both pain and disability during all time periods. (Bystrom, Rasmussen-Barr, & Grooten., 2013, p. E356) In addition, the corollary of Pilates-based therapeutic exercise in subjects with nonspecific low back pain and functional disability has also revealed studies showing marked improvement in the commonly used VAS-Pain and Roland-Morris questionnaires as compared to subjects undergoing usual medical consultative treatment (p-values ranging from .002 to .023 on relevant indicators for pain intensity and disability respectively). Further, the study’s authors (Rydeard, Leger, & Smith, 2006) reported a long-term follow-up of maintaining improvements over a 12-month period. However, this was not matched to comparing against another exercisebased physical therapy intervention as an important and contingent variable of competing influence. Limitations of Core Stabilization and Motor Control Interventions Much evidence for the clinical efficacy of Core Stabilization and Motor Control Exercises had been originally championed by the method’s primary research proponents and developers (Richardson et al., 1999) as well as from evidence accrued among many other independent researchers. However, other independent studies and reviewers have detracted the efficacy of these purported claims with contrary RCT evidence (Cairns, Foster, & Wright, 2006; Ferreira et al., 2006; Koumantakis, Watson, & Oldham, 2005). Yet, core stability exercise routines remain the standard prescription for chronic non-specific LBP without radiculopathy. Moreover, other studies have shown no added specific benefit to implementing localized spinal trunk muscle stabilization exercises in populations with acute or chronic low back pain (Cairns et al., 2006; Koumantakis et al., 2005). These studies were also randomized controlled 32 trials comparing them with standard PT or general exercises and both offered a 12–month, longterm follow-up. Other researchers in rehabilitation medicine cited significant lack of uniformity regarding the meaning of core-stabilization and what therapeutic exercises may be most effective toward improving neuromuscular control, endurance and strength, and so on (Standaert & Herring, 2007). The concepts of dynamic stability, core stability, lumbar stabilization, and segmental stabilization, among other terms, have infiltrated the therapeutic arena, the medical literature, the lay press, and even late-night infomercials. Dr. Stuart McGill, a respected spine researcher from the University of Waterloo in Canada, takes issue with concepts of isolated core stability in summarizing from his textbook chapter on low back disorders / myths and realities of lumbar spine stability as follows: In summary, achieving stability is not just a matter of activating a few targeted muscles, be they the multifidus, transverse abdominis, or any other. Sufficient stability is a moving target that continually changes as a function of the three-dimensional torques needed to support postures. It involves achieving the stiffness needed to endure unexpected loads, preparing for moving quickly, and insuring sufficient stiffness in any degree of freedom of the joint that may be compromised from injury. Motor control fitness is essential for achieving the stability target under all possible conditions for performance and injury avoidance. (McGill, 2007, p. 121) McGill’s summation resonates well with key tenets of the Feldenkrais Method® in purporting that in a well-organized nervous system, uniquely selected task demands will spontaneously organize toward a quality of adaptive behavior that is most optimal and proportionate to the demands of the task (Thelen & Smith, 1994). McGill (2007) stated, Virtually all muscles play a role in insuring stability, but their importance at any point in time is determined by the unique combination of the demands involved in sustaining postures while creating movements and anticipating sudden movements or unexpected forces and challenged breathing. (p. 120) In a landmark critical literature review summary of motor control principles and treatment interventions entitled The Myth of Core Stability, author Eyal Lederman (2010b) concluded: (a) Weak trunk muscles, weak abdominals and imbalances between trunk muscles 33 groups are not a pathology just a normal variation; (b) the division of the trunk into core and global muscle system is a reductionist fantasy, which serves only to promote CS; (c) weak or dysfunctional abdominal muscles will not lead to back pain; (d) tensing the trunk muscles is unlikely to provide any protection against back pain or reduce the recurrence of back pain; (e) core stability exercises are no more effective than, and will not prevent injury more than, any other forms of exercise or physical therapy; (f) core stability exercises are no better than other forms of exercise in reducing chronic lower back pain. Any therapeutic influence is related to the exercise effects rather than stability issues; (g) there may be potential danger of damaging the spine with continuous tensing of the trunk muscles during daily and sports activities; and (h) patients who have been trained to use complex abdominal hollowing and bracing maneuvers should be discouraged from using them. Biopsychosocial, Cognitive-Behavioral, and Graded Activity Interventions The contributory interactions of physical, psychological and social influences remain significant as interdisciplinary factors involved in both the prevalence and enduring chronicity of low back pain disorders being classified under the vague, but inclusive rubric of chronic nonspecific low back pain (CNSLBP). Moreover, this has led to the development of multidisciplinary biopsychosocial rehabilitation (MBR) programs that can now be administered by a variety of healthcare professionals from different backgrounds, including from within the profession of physical therapy (Kamper et al., 2014). Within a multidimensional biopsychosocial framework, it has been proposed that CNSLBP represents a vicious cycle associated with different combinations of provocative factors. These include cognitive factors (such as negative beliefs, fear-avoidance behaviors, catastrophizing, hypervigilance, anxiety, depression, stress, poor pacing and maladaptive coping 34 (Linton, 2000; Vlaeyen & Crombez, 1999; Wertli, Rasmussen-Barr, et al., 2014). Other components of contribution have been classified as somato-physical factors, including the maintenance of pain provocative postures and movement patterns related to altered body schema, muscle guarding, pain behaviors, and general deconditioning (O’Sullivan, Mitchell, Bulich, Waller, & Holte, 2006). The Role of Fear-Avoidance Beliefs and Chronic Non-Specific Low Back Pain In addition, numerous studies have cited the role of Fear-Avoidance Beliefs as important confounding variables in the perpetuation of chronic pain states – especially CNSLBP (Linton, 2000; Vlaeyen & Crombez, 1999; Wertli, Rasmussen-Barr, et al., 2014). It is proposed that patients with CNSLBP may have altered cognition and increased fear, which impacts their ability to move, perform exercise, and partake in activities of daily living (Louw, Puentedura, & Mintken, 2012). Thus, the fear-avoidance (FA) model of low back pain represents a leading cognitive-behavioral account for the development and maintenance of pain and disability following acute back injury (Leeuw et al., 2007; Vlaeyen & Linton, 2000). According to the FA model, fear that movement or physical activity will exacerbate pain or prompt (re)injury—also known as pain-related fear or kinesiophobia—is underscored by catastrophic appraisals of pain sensations (Grotle, Vollestad, & Brox, 2006; Sieben, Vlaeyen, Tuerlinckx, & Portegijs as cited in Trost & Parsons, 2014). The main assessment tool and clinical metric for measuring fear-avoidance beliefs within the contexts of physical activity and/or work tasks due to low back pain has been the FearAvoidance Beliefs Questionnaire or "FABQ" (Waddell, Newton, Henderson, Somerville, & Main, 1993). The reliability and validity of this measure has also been repeatedly confirmed (Kovacs et al., 2006). Consequently, the use of the FABQ assessment tool for its documented 35 ability to discern excessive threshold scores was implemented into this clinical study as a method for sub-stratification of random assignment into each group of participants to control for fearavoidance as a key confounding variable – especially within a study design that compared differences between supervised physical therapy exercise and an alternative contrasting type of movement intervention for a population of patients with CNSLBP. This procedure is detailed in Chapter 3. Cognitive Behavioral and Mindfulness Interventions Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) Consists of highly specific learning experiences designed to teach patients (a) to monitor their negative automatic thoughts (cognitions); (b) to recognize the connections among cognition, affect, and behavior; (c) to examine the evidence for and against distorted automatic thoughts; (d) to substitute more reality-oriented interpretations for these biased cognitions; and (e) to learn to identify and alter the beliefs that predispose them to distort their experiences. (Wedding & Corsini, 2014, p. 251) As applied to pain management, these personal reassessments and processes aim to change pain-related thoughts and behaviors. An alternative method, Mindfulness Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) involves training in mindfulness meditation, which aims to cultivate a state of free-floating, non-judgmental attention. A local integrated health system delivered these two interventions over the course of eight weekly, two-hour programs to 294 active participants divided into two groups as compared to a control group who underwent usual care. The percentage of participants with clinically meaningful improvement (a change of five points) on the RMDQ was higher for those who received MBSR (60.5%) and CBT (57.7%) than for usual care (44.1%) with p-value = .04 and a confidence interval (CI) of 95% (Cherkin et al., 2016). Graded Activity Functional Therapy and Therapeutic Pain Neuroscience Education As a specialized variation of CBT intervention, Classification-Based Cognitive Functional Therapy (CB-CFT), is more amenable to physical therapy practice. It has four main 36 components: (a) A cognitive component, wherein each patient can outline their vicious cycle of pain in a diagram based on elucidating and reflecting upon his or her own findings from the physical examination procedures and musculoskeletal pain questionnaires; (b) specific movement exercises designed to normalize maladaptive movement behaviors as directed by the movement classification; (c) targeted functional integration of activities in their daily life, reported to be avoided or provocative by the patient; and (d) a physical activity program tailored to the particular movement classification (Vibe Fersum, O'Sullivan, Skouen, Smith, & Kvåle, 2013). Upon development and testing, the classification-based (CB-CFT) cognitive functional therapy was compared to a similar cohort of control group patients with CNSLBP who underwent only traditional manual therapy and exercise (n=59). As a total outcome, the cognitive functional therapy group (n=62) displayed significantly superior outcomes to the comparative control group, both statistically (p is less than 0.001) and clinically (Vibe Fersum et.al., 2013). Therapeutic Neuroscience Education (TNE) and/or Pain Neuroscience Education (PNE) are cognitive therapy-based approaches that teach patients about pain. It essentially conveys to the patient a more confident assurance for reducing pain amplification by explaining the underlying physiological pain mechanisms involved in the continuation of pain signaling occurring mysteriously and continuously in the absence of actual (or mis-construed) tissue damage. By re-attributions toward recognizing preeminent fear-avoidance tendencies, and of becoming aware of overly vigilant pain responses to a perceived threat, Therapeutic Neuroscience Education aims to change a patient's cognition regarding their pain state, which may result in decreased fear, ultimately resulting in confrontation of pain barriers and a resumption of normal activities (Louw et al., 2012). 37 It has been further recommended that TNE/PNE techniques aimed at decreasing fear associated with movement may be a valuable adjunct to movement-based therapy, such as exercise, especially for patients with CNSLBP (Louw et al., 2012). More recently (in PT Journal, May 2014), a perspective paper contained a recommendation to combine Pain Neuroscience Education (PNE) with Cognition-Targeted Motor Control Training. The authors’ advocated for pre-emptive exposure in therapeutic pain neuroscience education for patients presenting with chronic spinal pain and summarized a multi-phase model for helping patients change pain attribution beliefs: 1. To learn to re-conceptualize their pain signaling experience in a manner that does not automatically signify tissue damage nor correspond to or immanent or dangerous threat (i.e., “hurt does not equal harm”); 2. Reducing their level of hyper-excitability/vigilance being implemented both before and during each phase of rehabilitation exercise progression, beginning with coordinated activity of spinal muscles (i.e., citing the Core Stabilization training protocol for motor control deficits); and 3. Progressing to more complex exercises and dynamic functional tasks (i.e., citing the traditional Motor Control exercise progression). 4. Noting that “Cognition-Targeted” exercises advocate for using a time-contingent approach (i.e., “Perform the exercise for five minutes regardless of pain”) in lieu of a symptom-contingent approach (i.e., “Stop the exercise once it hurts”). In this, the authors suggest a re-appraisal of pain threshold and diminished pain expectancy to permit increasing gradations of activity tolerance, and furthermore for altering the patient’s beliefs about the interplay between pain and movement; together, with evidence that 38 novel motor skill training is associated with rapid changes in cortical excitability as well as for cortical re-organization (Nijs, et al., 2014). Limitations of Combined Physical and Behavioral-Psychological Interventions The most recent Cochrane Group systematic review assessed the global effectiveness of multidisciplinary biopsychosocial rehabilitation programs for chronic low back pain by selecting a meta-analysis of 41 RCTs with a total of 6858 participants that met their stringent criteria for inclusion. Compared to usual care, the range across all time points equated to approximately 0.5 to 1.4 units on a 0 to 10 numerical rating scale for pain and 1.4 to 2.5 points on the Roland Morris disability scale (0 to 24). There was moderate to low quality evidence of no difference on work outcomes. Across all time points, this sub-population’s scores translated to approximately 0.6 to 1.2 units on the pain scale and 1.2 to 4.0 points on the Roland Morris scale (Kamper et al., 2014). A more highly specialized systematic review and meta-analysis study was just recently published in the February 2016 Journal of Pain for comparing treatment effectiveness by instituting a new classification schema among the many published conservative interventions for Non-specific Chronic Spine Pain (NSCSP) - the Australian term for persistent pain involving cervical, thoracic, and / or lumbar-pelvic regions – but in having large correlations of contribution for shared similar variables to its research analog: Chronic Non-specific Low Back Pain (CNSLBP). By sub-classifying a comparison of outcomes between the varied nomenclatures for current conservative interventions, the study broadly divided them as (a) physical, (b) behavioral and/or psychological, and (c) interventions that combined these approaches. Physical interventions included using exercise, manual therapy and ergonomic advice. Behavioral and/or 39 psychologically informed interventions were classified as those that aim to improve behaviors, cognitions or mood by using methods such as relaxation and cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT). Combined interventions being reviewed as a hybrid, were designated as approaches which aim to improve both physical and psychological factors contributing to patients’ pain by using some combination of both approaches, up to an including the studies which implemented multidisciplinary pain management programs. Because it remains unclear whether any of these approaches are superior, and unclear as to which category of intervention has had the greatest level of supporting evidence, this review aimed to assess the comparative relative effectiveness of different conservative interventions for reducing pain and disability in people with NSCSP: physical, behavioral/psychological or combined? Nine electronic databases were searched for randomized controlled trials (RCTs). Study quality was assessed used the Cochrane Back Review Group risk of bias criteria. Criteria for inclusion included RCTs involving participants with NSCSP (neck, thoracic, low back, or pelvic) for greater than a 12-week duration. RCTs had to measure pain and/or disability and have a minimum follow-up period of 12 weeks. RCTs were only included if they had an “active” conservative treatment control group for comparison (i.e., no treatment or waiting list comparisons, as these were excluded). RCTs were also selectively excluded if the interventions were from the same domain (e.g., if the study compared two physical interventions like aerobic exercise versus strength training). At the conclusion of sampling, 24 studies were included. Eighteen RCTs investigated patients with low back pain (LBP), while only six studies investigated participants with neck pain (NP). The sample sizes of the included studies ranged from 30 to 393 participants. The average age of the participants in these studies ranged from 39 to 54 years. 40 The treatment effects of physical, behavioral/psychologically informed, and combined interventions were assessed using meta-analyses. Subsequent to meta-analyses, no clear statistically significant clinical differences were found for reducing pain and disability between physical, behavioral/psychologically informed, and the combined intervention groups. In addition, only small differences in pain or disability were observed between physical, behavioral/psychologically informed, and combined interventions. The authors concluded that current interventions for NSCSP have similarly small effectiveness on pain and disability and that there is still a lot of work to be done to find a longterm clinically effective intervention for chronic spine pain. However, “it is possible, though far from certain, that attempts to better combine different components of therapy for people with NSCSP might someday show better results [emphasis added]” (O’Keeffe et al., 2016). Applying the Culmination of Recent Literature and Combined Rx into the Design More recent developments comparing treatment parameters for combining two research questions to explore outcome differences between Core Stabilization/Motor Control Exercises and a Graded Activity/Cognitive Behavioral Therapy-based physical therapy protocol are detailed in the PT journal article entitled Effect of Motor Control Exercises Versus Graded Activity in Patients with Chronic Nonspecific Low Back Pain: A Randomized Controlled Trial (Macedo et al., 2012). By their using a selection of essentially identical (a) treatment outcome measures, and (b) the same allocation for number of treatment sessions (12), and finally (c) the same time-frames for administering the comparative interventions (8 – weeks) and as a similar framework of parameters and measurement tools used for all other prior core stabilization / motor control treatment efficacy studies that were reviewed, it will serve as a model reference for 41 comparing the outcome results of this RCT study. These are summarized in Chapter 3 of this dissertation manuscript as well as in the Results and Discussion sections. In addition, and for the homogenization of both treatment groups within the study, elements of assurance being borrowed from Therapeutic Neuroscience Education/Pain Neuroscience Education (TNE/PNE) and other CBT techniques have been detailed and discussed in advance with all of the study participants (in both arms of the study) via having a specific entry contained within the Study’s Consent Form and Participation Agreement. More specifically, the consent form content addressed and outlined an open discussion regarding the issue of pain sensitization, and furthermore disclosed the possibility of latent or spontaneous pain flares that could occurr anywhere in the body - after or even during the course of treatment. However, the form content also assured patients that these responses are normal at the initiation of sub-maximal but unfamiliar movements in persons presenting with chronic pain pathway sensitization, and that ultimately, "hurt" would in no way indicate a basis for physical harm, and perhaps could even afford a new opportunity to get better. The next section of this chapter now moves to explore the phenomenon of experience dependent neuroplasticity and its role in the development and maintenance of chronic pain sensitization and the amplification of pain signaling. The postulation of training new sensation of forwarding a novel and complex sensory-acuity and movement-dexterity perspective for reentrainment, and its postulated correspondences to new alterations in "body schema" in association with revisionist mappings known to occur within and throughout the somatotopic cortex of the human brain – is extrapolated as a competing mechanism toward the exploration, development, and comparison of a new kind of treatment intervention against a similar and closely matched control group. 42 Emergent Findings from the Neuroscience of Chronic Pain and Neuroplasticity The phenomenon of consciousness, being neither exclusively physical-structural nor exclusively psychological-behavioral, is a composite entity occurring within and among living things for perceiving their relationships and correspondences - both past and present – as well as for anticipating the future. As such, it must continually encode, conserve and renew for itself through necessary combinations of ontogenetic predisposition, psychophysiological interaction and sensory-motor representational processes; whereby each constitutes its own contribution toward the mutual formations of interdependent amalgams of both inner and outer worlds as well as for comprising and developing intrinsic models for future or anticipatory interaction. Of these, all are neurologically mediated at some level of cognitive-embodied experience. Furthermore, as a fundamental property of the human central nervous system - and as central to all learning, adaptation, and plasticity – embodied cognition processes must retain their ability to remodel in accordance with changing experience and expectation in order to adequately and flexibly function in the continuing multidimensional world. In case situations involving chronic pain, the modeling of experience occurs and recurs such that an unpleasant sensory and emotional experience becomes associated with actual or potential tissue damage, or is described in terms of such damage, and such that it develops a multifactorial self-reinforcing and self-regulatory pattern – or an anticipatory matrix of its own whether in the presence or absence of actual tissue damage. The multiple determinants and complexities of chronic pain as a constellation of factors perpetuating throughout the central nervous system – as well as through the periphery - were first summarized in the original abstract from the paper "Pain and the Neuromatrix in the Brain" by Ronald Melzack (2001): The neuromatrix theory of pain proposes that pain is a multidimensional experience produced by characteristic "neurosignature" patterns of nerve impulses generated by a 43 widely distributed neural network-the "body-self neuromatrix"-in the brain. These neurosignature patterns may be triggered by sensory inputs, but they may also be generated independently of them. Acute pains evoked by brief noxious inputs have been meticulously investigated by neuroscientists, and their sensory transmission mechanisms are generally well understood. In contrast, chronic pain syndromes, which are often characterized by severe pain associated with little or no discernable injury or pathology, remain a mystery. Furthermore, chronic psychological or physical stress is often associated with chronic pain, but the relationship is poorly understood. The neuromatrix theory of pain provides a new conceptual framework to examine these problems. It proposes that the output patterns of the body-self neuromatrix activate perceptual, homeostatic, and behavioral programs after injury, pathology, or chronic stress. Pain, then, is produced by the output of a widely distributed neural network in the brain rather than directly by sensory input evoked by injury, inflammation, or other pathology. The neuromatrix, which is genetically determined and modified by sensory experience, is the primary mechanism that generates the neural pattern that produces pain. Its output pattern is determined by multiple influences, of which the somatic sensory input is only a part, that converge on the neuromatrix. (Abstract) In situations involving the chronic pain experience and sensitization or hyperalgesia, there is ample evidence from neuroimaging and correspondent clinical presentation for demonstrating compensatory and often widespread change in the internal processing of pain signaling. Neurophysiological changes across different areas of the peripheral and central nervous systems, including peripheral receptors, dorsal horn of the spinal cord, brain stem, sensorimotor cortical areas, and the mesolimbic and prefrontal areas are associated with chronic musculoskeletal disorders, including chronic low back pain. These findings corroborate with altered representational processes, altered cognition, altered limbic association, and even morphologic structural changes revealed through attritions in cortical gray matter, specifically in bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and right thalamus (Apkarian et al., 2004), when compared to matched asymptomatic controls. Continued neuronal epi-genesis involves a necessary response to experience dependent changes exerting inhibitory/excitatory selection pressures upon pathways and connections distributive throughout the brain and spinal cord 44 (central processing), but also behaving sometimes independent of both non-nociceptive and nociceptive input from the periphery (tangible body parts/afferents & efferents). Furthermore, it is now well known from evidence in neuroscience that the human brain undergoes neuroplastic cortical reorganization –distorted representations in the mapping of body schema and the corresponding dexterity of the body—in response to sensitizing or challenging experiences, but especially with chronic pain (Flor, 2003a; Flor et al., 1997; Flor & Diers, 2009; Moseley, 2005; Moseley & Flor, 2012; Wand, Keeves, et al., 2013). Furthermore and continuously, re-constructive and de-constructive body maps/virtual homunculi are being constantly updated and dynamically sculpted by ongoing interoceptive and exteroceptive experiences--both habitual and non-habitual-- and existing at multiple levels throughout the entire CNS including cerebellar, insular, thalamic, and associated limbic regions. Aversive events and chronic pain states commonly result in and/or co-contribute to disordered functioning of working body schema (i.e., sensory-motor deficits in the clarity, resolution, and dexterity of everyday actions) correspondent with topographic disruption (smudging and dissociation) of body-related cortical representations. These unpleasant event driven, inner altercations in structure and function have been referred to in the literature as being the dark side of neuroplasticity (Doidge, 2007, 2015); and despite intact / normal ‘standard’ neurological screening (i.e., pinwheels, reflex hammers, sharp-dull tactile thresholds and the like), complex sensations (graphesia, tactile acuity, visual-spatial body awareness, cross-modal representations, discriminative proportions, etc.) remain otherwise impaired, and are co-related to deficits in motor control. Neuroplasticity is ordinarily the adaptive strategy and the ongoing interactive mediational process by which the brain encodes new experiences, learns, and develops new behaviors in 45 response to environmental perturbation and or to directed intrinsic attention. Neurophysiological changes being characteristically resultant of neuroplasticity processing can refer to Changes in structure, function, and organization within the nervous system that occur continuously throughout our lifetimes in response to internal stressors such as cognitive processes, internal changes in sensory afference, and external stressors such as motor learning and peripheral sensory stimulation. (Pelletier et al., 2015a, p. 1583) More recently, elucidating the role and processing of pre-frontal cortical and sub-cortical associative learning, and the anticipatory conditioning of directed focal attention toward interoceptive discriminative stimuli being additionally co-conditioned through mesolimbic reinforcement and memory encoding – it has been further implicated that the transition, habituation, perpetuation, and re-definition of chronic pain can now be construed in terms of constructing behavioral models for learning and neuroplasticity. Pioneering work by Apkarian from 2008, Mansour, Farmer, Baliki, and Apkarian (2014) summarized through an updated applications review the descriptive - but elusive process - as follows: Chronic pain is defined as a state of continued suffering, sustained long after the initial inciting injury has healed. In terms of learning and memory one could recast this definition as: Chronic pain is a persistence of the memory of pain and/or the inability to extinguish the memory of pain evoked by an initial inciting injury. The novel hypothesis that we advance is that chronic pain is a state of continuous learning, in which aversive emotional associations are continuously made with incidental events simply due to the persistent presence of pain. Simultaneously, continued presence of pain does not provide an opportunity for extinction because whenever the subject is reexposed to the conditioned event he/she is still in pain. Failing to extinguish, therefore, makes the event become a reinforcement of aversive association. (pp. 4-5) It may become emergent that chronic pain syndromes in general will someday become re-classified under a new rubric of neuro-ontogenetic-plasticity disorders. The uncanny aspect is that plasticity’s known mechanisms involving biased synaptic efficacy, excitatory protein synthesis, and re-routing of selected information to widespread areas of the brain-many of which are known to be selectively co- involved in pain processing-all happens at a level of involuntary 46 involvement; plastic changes co-occurring on multiple levels (firing together), and evolving into actual morphologic changes and constructs (wiring together) at a level well below the ordinary conscious awareness. People are all products of multi-systemic interactive conditioning, which ordinarily allows them to process and function better. But whether implicitly learned or inadvertently conditioned, a preferable selection toward more optimal and resilient functional adaptation is simply not so in situations that have culminated toward the development and the continuing phenomenon of chronic pain. In fact, multiple and disparate implicit and explicit associations being continually encoded and reinforced through learning, memory, and successive behavioral stimulusassociation-response chains are phenomena that are now shown to remain continually distributive and encoded throughout the central nervous system in select brain regions becoming physically representative as topographic maps of chronic pain, in addition to their implicating for the role for emotional suffering in chronic pain. Some Specific Brain Regions Involved in Pain Processing Areas of the brain involved in pain processing of sensory-motor association vs. nociceptive inputs can include: amygdala, hippocampus, anterior cingulate cortex, thalamic pathways, primary and secondary somatosensory cortex, supplementary and pre-motor cortex areas, primary motor cortex, pre-frontal cortex, posterior parietal complex, basal ganglia, cerebellum, hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal (neuroendocrine) influences (HPA-axis); furthermore involving autonomic nervous system (ANS) sympathetic arousal, in addition to spinal cord and dorsal horn gating mechanisms (Doidge, 2015; Moller, 2014). A descriptive consolidation of major brain areas and their varied corresponding functions implicated in pain processing is depicted in Table 1. 47 Table 1 Major Brain Areas Where Pain is Processed Note. Sourced from Table 1, The Brain’s Way of Healing: Remarkable Discoveries and Recoveries from the Frontiers of Neuroplasticity (Doidge, 2015, pp. 13-14). Viking Press/Penguin Random House Publishers, New York, New York. Reprinted with Permission. 48 Bottom-Up Influences: Their Relationship to Intervention & Purported Mechanisms International colleague and fellow Feldenkrais® Practitioner, Aurovici Sercomanens, D.O. poses that the gate control theory - the phenomenon of sensory gating as originally proposed by Melzack and Wall in 1962 - may be one of the most primary ways as to how both Functional Integration® (FI®), the informative conceptual teaching through hands-on contact, and slowly, gently applied movement facilitated sequences of action delivered through Awareness through Movement® (ATM®) conceptual scripts - both characteristic of The Feldenkrais Method® - might work toward the modulation of pain pathways from the outset. He described it nicely here: Sensory gating means that the processing and perception of sense information is reduced by the presence of other competing sense information. If your nervous system is busy trying to process signals resulting from (constructed, synergistic) movement (sequences) you are making, or from the sensation of (informed and communicative) touch; it will have less ability to perceive and process pain signals, and hence, your pain will reduce. Areas of sensory gating, attention -perception modulation, and movement response selection can exist at many & varied levels of the nervous system, and resultant perception is modulated by interactions between different neurons. What the brain receives are nociception (receptor) signals and it decides how to interpret these and make them result in pain or not. Pain is the brain’s output after interpreting the signals. So ultimately, Pain is an output (perception) from the brain, not an input from the body. (Sercomanens, 2012, p. 2) Sensory Information at the Crossroads: Differentiating the Thalamus & Rerouting the Insula A more central neurophysiological rationale as to how a slow, discriminatory and Feldenkrais Method®-based movement intervention might work differently for pain modulation than other, usual physical therapy exercise interventions, involves comparing and contrasting the competing levels of sensory-relevant thalamic processing: A. The ‘ventral thalamus’ is wired to process sensory information with great accuracy and refined clarity of detail and to relay extracted, continually updated information to both 49 primary sensory and associative (poly-modal) and pre-frontal (task-responsive) cortices. These systems have been classically described as the “Slow and Accurate” systems; the more discriminative ‘Where’ of it all as a “high route” of highly-processed sensory appraisal in lieu of activating lower level limbic alarm signaling to the amygdala. By mechanisms of selective signal bias and sensory clarity, this informational pathway is conducive toward cultivating correspondent and descending inhibitory processes that are believed to be involved in the containment and reduction of pain signaling. In contrast: B. The ‘dorsal--medial thalamus’ bypasses the primary cortices and instead sends axons directly to many dispersive areas of the brain, most notably secondary association cortices, limbic system nuclei, lateral nuclei of the amygdala, anterior cingulate, hippocampus, as well as PAG (periaqueductal gray) and reticular formation/activation. This part of the sensory system (non-classical or lateral, extra-lemiscal spinal-thalamic tract) is sub-cortically diffuse, less accurate, and less detailed and has been described as “Fast and Dirty,” thereby biasing toward The ‘What’ of it all as a “low route” of lesserprocessed, reactive alarm response amplification to the amygdala; triggering an endocrine, autonomic, behavioral cascade that can thus be perceived as immediate danger or threat, and co-associated with amplified pain signaling. As an alternative to lesser discriminant physical therapy activities (i.e., going through usual and repeated prescriptive exercise progressions, but also likely defaulting to usual efference copy and faulty pathway biases in the process), The Feldenkrais Method® instead approaches to construct experiences which conductively select a bias toward taking the neurological ‘high road’ through the ventral thalamic pathways via its emphasis on slow, non- 50 threatening, highly discriminative and naturalistic movement synergies that have both informed and constituted the original sensory-motor development in gaining a clear and accurate, functional familiarity with the immediate world. This serves as a competing stimulus in contrast to usual experience -- as with everyday mindless repetition of working against limitation with more effort, as is the usual mode of traditional, prescriptive or corrective, fitness-based physical therapy – including the social-culturally mediated and iatrogenic transfer to daily activities. C. The Insula cortex is involved in complex relationships between thalamic and amygdaloidal nuclei, is anatomically convergent between orbital-frontal, secondary somatosensory, temporal, and parietal lobes and mediates vast assortments of information processing between sensory perception and cognitive function, emotions and motor control, temperature regulation, vestibular and autonomic homeostasis, self-awareness, and especially, interoceptive awareness, and sense of ownership; the identification of "one’s own body," of which the phenomenology of pain experience is quite significant. It furthermore appears that the insula functions as a crossroads between the sensory discriminative and the affective dimensions of pain (Bushnell, Čeko, & Low, 2013; Pelletier et al., 2015b). Thus, There is both anatomical and some functional evidence of involvement of the insular lobe in the primary integration of multi-modal sensory input -- inclusive of (substrates for) pathological (chronic) pain, but so far, no treatment methods have been developed that target the insula. (Moller, 2014, p. 325) Though, perhaps, some undiscovered possibilities can someday emerge to integrate some uniquely novel and functionally relevant, sensory-motor experiences, being developed through cross-modal and virtual representations of enhanced body perception. These perceptual components could furthermore become integrated with empathic provisions and emotional 51 assurances for safety and security - while at the same time, introducing and experiencing something novel, but yet refreshingly different - all in conjunction with comprehensive, but background intent toward clarifying and developing an improved working body schema; being characterized here as a unifying prospect for internalizing a more effective treatment. One of the aims of the current study was to explore and devise a psychophysical intervention that can perhaps more directly involve and evoke the diversely unique propensities and convergent processing components inherent to the insular cortex itself, and to harness its uniquely respective modulatory effects with regard to controlling pain phenomena - at the crossroads. Sensory-Motor Deficiency and Excess Functional Connectivity upon fMRI Motor Imagery In a study entitled, "Differential Neural Processing during Motor Imagery of Daily Activities in Chronic Low Back Pain Patients," Vrana et al. (2015) cleverly implemented a discriminative stimulus input consisting of visually-guided motor imagery (MI) tasks via videorecordings of daily action activities in order to reveal and elucidate expected differential neural sensorimotor processing among healthy controls (HC) as compared to chronic low back pain (LBP) patients, while being assessed under functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) of brain, and while only mentally performing a simulation or rehearsal of the task activities that were concurrently being demonstrated upon the embedded video display. The motor imagery (MI) network of the brain comprises the primary motor cortex, the premotor cortex, including the supplemental motor area (SMA), the superior and inferior parietal lobe (SPL, IPL), temporal lobe, the insula, prefrontal regions as well as subcortical structures, such as the basal ganglia and the thalamus, the cerebellum, and has been studied extensively in healthy subjects, especially in motor learning and performance in sports (Vrana et al., 2015). Behavioral and neuroimaging findings have accumulated showing that imagined actions retain 52 the same spatial-temporal characteristics as the corresponding real action when it comes to execution (Jeannerod, 2001). Therefore, motor imagery (MI) of action observation and simple pre-planning estimates for corollary discharge to pre-motor and parietal areas just prior to motor execution (ME) through descending corticospinal tracts, all correspond to a subliminal activation of the sensorimotor system in such manner that it conjunctively represents a true motor format (Jeannerod, 2001). The advantage of their using imagined or simulated movement in lieu of actual movement additionally served a practical application to reduce image artifact, in that fMRI data dependent on resonance quality - are strongly sensitive to aberrant subject motion, and that fMRI scanner parameters of physical space are themselves physically constrained, and thus prohibitive to actualizing naturalistic movement. Twenty-nine subjects (15 chronic LBP patients, 14 HC) were included in their study. MI stimuli consisted of six randomly presented video clips showing every-day activities involving different whole-body movements as well as walking on even ground and walking downstairs and upstairs. Guided by the video clips, subjects had to perform simulated and imagined MI of these activities. Interestingly, pain ratings of the chronic LBP group indicated that patients experienced only the MI-performance of the “activities” as painful, while the MI of the “walking” condition was not painful (Vrana et al., 2015). The video sample clips are shown below in Figure 3. Note that they bear striking resemblance to graded activity and work hardening programs typical of many industrial and work injury rehabilitation programs. As fMRI results suggest, simulated or actual immersion into these activities directly - as an initial or continuing method of approach for the treatment of CNSLBP, and typically supported by using "exercise specificity" and "graded functional daily activities" as an accepted rationale– is a clinical paradigm that must soon be questioned. 53 Figure 3. The Six presented Video Clips for Mental Simulation of Daily Actions during fMRI Recordings. A. Activities of daily living (“Activities”), and B. Walking activities (“Walking”). *Note: Image and content courtesy of Andrea Vrana et al. (2015) and PLoS One; open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, 2016. These results from the fMRI study by Vrana et al. (2015) indicated first-time novel findings for understanding the real-time neurological events and underpinnings being developmentally constitutive for known sensorimotor reorganization processes typically found in the brains of chronic pain patients. In this, they discovered that motor imagery (MI-driven) activity yielded reduced brain activation within (a) the left supplemental motor area (SMA), and (b) the right superior temporal gyrus/sulcus (STG/STS), while fMRI connectivity analysis also indicated (c) significantly enhanced functional connectivity (FC) becoming excessively divergent and extraneous both within and outside the MI-neural network in chronic LBP patients as compared to the HC group. Implications of each primary fMRI finding and its application to typically representative clinical profiles found in patients with CNSLBP are as follows. 54 The supplemental motor area. The supplemental motor area (SMA) represents an inherent part of the MI-network and is linked to adequate motor planning and voluntary motor control. There is also considerable evidence for the involvement of SMA in postural control, and especially in anticipatory postural adjustments (APAs). Reduced activity in the SMA suggests dysfunctional mechanisms in these motor control areas in addition to disrupted feed-forward monitoring of internal efference copy to pre-motor and parietal association areas; thereby, contributory to disrupted background body balance and posture control during voluntary foreground intent and for the corresponding control of intended movement. Subsequently, these findings suggest a direct involvement of the SMA in trunk movement coordination. Therefore, the demonstrated maladaptive functioning of the SMA revealed by motor imagery perturbation in LBP might be based on progressive dysfunction of motor circuits, and thus provides a mechanism to explain the reported impairments in postural control that are a frequent finding in patients with chronic LBP or CNSLBP. Superior temporal gyrus/sulcus. When contrasted against baseline, superior temporal gyrus/sulcus (STG/STS) activity during both “activities” and “walking” was significantly reduced in chronic LBP patients compared to HC. The STG/STS responds to images of human bodies and their orientations and is known to play an important role in the understanding and interpreting of human movements as well as in matching sensory inputs with internal movement representations (Vrana et al., 2015). Correspondingly, motor imagery (MI), and motor execution (ME) require a high amount of sensory input processing in order to provide a "real-time representation of the body" (e.g., a working body-schema). In order to predict and perform everyday actions appropriately, the matching of the internal representation of a person’s body (the body schema) with an intended movement (as 55 influenced by a particular environmental affordance or task demand) is realized by comparing the predicted sensorimotor consequences of the action through existing internal action models (e.g., virtual body/efference copy), and the actual sensorimotor "afferent" feedback from the body-environment. In either case, the finding of reduced STG/STS activity indicates a deficiency in the integration of sensory inputs from whatever source, central or peripheral, internal or external, and this diminutive feature remains a developmental characteristic that is seen in many patients with long-standing CNSLBP. Event-related functional connectivity (FC) and its meaning in fMRI. Evidence continues to accumulate that connectivity measures based on the hemodynamic fluctuations and responses measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) reflect meaningful aspects of cognitive processing in terms of task, load, behavior, and pathology or psychiatric diagnosis (Rogers, Morgan, Newton, & Gore, 2007). Functional magnetic resonance imaging is widely used to detect and delineate regions of the brain that change their level of activation in response to specific stimuli and tasks. The interregional correlations between fluctuations of MRI signal potentially reveal connective relationships within and between major brain areas during task - and in real time - based on real-time rapid metabolic distributions of blood oxygenation level through brain-blood flow. The organization, inter-relationship and integrated performance of these different regions is generally described by the term “functional connectivity.” Functional connectivity (FC) has been defined as the temporal correlation of a neurophysiological index measured in different brain areas (Nallasamy & Tsao, 2011), or more specifically, through operational interactions of multiple spatially-distinct brain regions that are engaged simultaneously in a task (Rogers et al., 56 2007). These findings have been used to identify coactivating brain regions. Activity and connectivity analyses are the two main applications of fMRI (Vrana et al., 2015). In the differential processing of motor imagery study by Vrana et al. (2015), the chronic LBP patients exhibited significantly enhanced MI-driven FC compared to the HC group throughout the MI network; indicating diffuse and non-specific changes in FC, as compared to HCs. Similar findings associated with hyper-excitability in both executive attention and sensorymotor networks have also been previously demonstrated in fibromyalgia patients; a pathology similarly characterized by the perpetuation of ongoing and diffuse chronic pain states. With respect to chronic LBP, diffuse and enhanced non-specific functional connectivity across the MI network might indicate a common and widespread maladaptive neuroplasticity process that continues to occur, thereby laying the groundwork for continued morphologic distortions occurring within sensory-motor, accessory emotional-limbic, and autonomic networks within the pain neuro-matrix. Summary of motor imagery (MI)-driven fMRI activity. As calibrated for event-related activity pooled together for “activity motor imagery” and “walking motor imagery” after baseline, fMRI findings demonstrated whole-brain activations in both groups for frontal, temporal and parietal cortices as well as in the occipital lobe. However, in the chronic LBP group, the left middle frontal gyrus, which correspond to the supplemental motor area (SMA), and the right superior temporal gyrus (STG) and middle temporal gyrus (MTG) regions of sensory integration were effectively disengaged during the observation and simulation of the motor imagery videography tasks. Furthermore, and equally significant, psycho-physiologicalinteraction analysis yielded significantly enhanced functional connectivity (FC) between various MI-and non-MI-associated brain regions in chronic LBP patients; this indicated diffuse, hyper- 57 excitable, and non-specific changes in FC as compared to HCs. These findings are summarized in Figure 4. Figure 4. Pooled Motor Imagery Tasks as a Composite of Six Video Simulated Actions as exhibited on fMRI. A) Healthy controls (HC) on left, and B) chronic Low Back Pain (LBP) patients on right, demonstrated significant differences in fMRI on such features as efficiency of motor control and sensory discrimination upon mental simulation of video clip movement tasks. Compensatory and mal-adaptive selection of non-task specific neuronal circuitry is demonstrated in the LBP group on right in terms of non-specific functional connectivity being overly enhanced via a proliferation of superfluous activity – accounting in part to loss of sensory-motor integration, murky body schema, hyperexcitability, and loss of refinement for movement skill. *(Image and content courtesy of Vrana et al. (2015) and PLoS One; open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, 2016. fMRI implications for clinical understanding and future treatment. The current investigation by Vrana et al. (2015) provides first evidence for obvious differences between chronic LBP patients and HC subjects regarding MI-driven activity and FC. While healthy subjects efficiently accommodated to demanding tasks by enhancing FC within the necessary MI-network, chronic LBP patients required more extraneous cortical recruitment depicted by enhanced FC demands occurring outside of usual MI-networks in order to perform the same task. 58 The finding of non-specifically enhanced and diffuse FC within and beyond the MI network of chronic LBP patients might indicate pain-driven maladaptive alterations in the sensorimotor network in terms of compensatory hyperexcitability and/or an enlarged need for neural resources (Vrana et al., 2015). These intrinsic modifications remain unseen and "extracurricular" to the demands of the actual task. Despite retaining some ability to perform such tasks to match criteria for placating an external examiner within an industrial and/or clinical setting, these patients do so at a level that is most likely accompanied by some degree of spatialtemporal insufficiency, fatigue, internal resistance, extraneous effort, symptom reproduction, and a corresponding sense of wear and tear that can never be "stretched out" or "worked out" nor sustainably "medicated out" by conventional means. Importantly and progressively, Vrana et al. (2015) concluded that “these findings may broaden the basis for the understanding of sensorimotor reorganization processes in chronic LBP patients and might ultimately help developing novel approaches for therapeutic MI-guided interventions” (p. 8). The Pre-Frontal Cognitive-Attentional & Affective-Emotional Mesolimbic Domains Pathologic central neuropathic chronic pain becomes a different form of pain than the case that began with acute-nociceptive, episodic, and biologically protective pain; the latter being more directly associated with the original trauma or incident. Only a fraction of patients who experience an acute painful injury will develop chronic pain. How brain activity reorganizes its structure and function with the transition from acute to abnormal chronic pain continues to remain a topic of intensive scientific exploration and clinical research. These also become the subjects of "top-down" regulation of sensation and pain. The three cortical regions that consistently show decreases in grey matter within the context of neuroimaging and correspondent with the clinical presentation of chronic pain are the 59 (a) anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), (b) prefrontal cortex (PFC), and the (c) insular cortex (IC). Molecular imaging studies also show decreases in opioid receptor binding in patients with chronic pain in all three regions. Studies have additionally identified changes in white matter integrity in these regions (Bushnell et al., 2013). These brain derived biomarkers - correspondent to abnormal activity in mesolimbic and prefrontal areas - correlate strongly with clinical measures in patients with CNSLBP and correlate better with clinical findings than do structural physical exam and psychosocial findings. Increased insular activation is correlated with pain duration, while medial PFC activation is correlated with pain intensity in CNSLBP subjects (Apkarian, Hashmi, & Baliki, 2011; Pelletier et al., 2015b). These areas and regions of the brain are associated with threat, fear, aversive conditioning, attention, motivation engagement, negativity or disengagement, and executive control (Pelletier et al., 2015a, p. 1584). Thus, chronic pain might be maintained through hypervigilance toward noxious stimuli due to abnormal attentional cortical-thalamic and negatively valanced mesolimbic systems. From experimental observations and perspectives, it has been shown that attentional and emotional factors can modulate pain perception via different descending modulatory pathways. Activities involving the redirection of attention have been shown to reduce the perceived intensity of pain via the activation of circuitry involving projections from the superior parietal lobe (SPL) to the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) and the insula. The somatosensory cortices (S1 and S2) encode information about sensory features, such as the location and duration of pain. Alternatively, the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the insula, as transitional components of the somato-emotional limbic system, are more important for encoding the mood states and the motivational aspects of pain. Accordingly, psychosocial support conditions involving emotions and placebo analgesia have been shown to alter the perceived unpleasantness of pain (but 60 without significantly altering perceived intensity) primarily through circuitry co-activations transmitting and descending throughout the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), prefrontal cortex (PFC) and via inhibitory influences upon the periaqueductal grey areas (Bushnell et al., 2013). The compilation of these findings citing areas of cognitive and emotional control of pain - and its corresponding regions of disruption in chronic pain - is depicted in Figure 5. Figure 5. Brain Pathways for Cognitive and Emotional Influence on Pain. (a) Cognitive /Attentional and Emotional Determinants, and (b) their corresponding Regions of Disruption in Chronic Pain. This figure demonstrates that attentional and emotional modulation of chronic pain occurs through differentiated pathways. Of important clinical significance to the current study, it is worth re-stating that attentional pathways involving somatosensory cortices do not appear to be reliant upon mechanisms for placebo anesthesia – as this is a more predominant feature of known to occur within pre-frontal and cingulate cortices in the modulation of pain. *Image and content courtesy of Bushnell et al. (2013) and National Library of Medicine (NLM) and Pub Med Central (PMC); National Institutes of Health; Division of Intramural Research Program at NCCAM; HHS Public Access Author Manuscript: Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. Macmillan Publishers Limited © 2013. The transition from control of acute low back pain to its disruption in chronic low back pain has been demonstrated to occur within real time (Hashmi et al., 2013). By conducting a combined cross-sectional and longitudinal anatomical and functional brain imaging study in a 61 cohort of subjects with a single first-time episode of LBP (back pain persisting for at least four weeks, with no prior back pain experience for at least one year), they were followed over a period of one year as they either recovered or transitioned into chronic pain. Results of those who transitioned to CNSLBP demonstrated a spatiotemporal dynamical reorganization of brain activity, during which the representation of back pain over time had gradually shifted away from sensory and nociceptive cortical regions and instead manifested toward engaging larger scale, greater morphologic localization throughout emotional and limbic structures. What remains an inquiry from a therapeutic neuroplasticity interventionist standpoint is to determine whether this transition can be reversed-especially toward somato-sensory and sense of ownership aspects of neuronal information processing, and with less emotional-limbic divestment from body-self. Indeed, it has been shown that diminished brain regions implicated in disrupted pain modulation, including the dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex (DLPFC) and ACC, had reduced grey matter in chronic low back pain patients, but after successful treatment for resolving the pain, the grey matter reductions were subsequently reversed so that the affected brain regions were again re-normalized in size (Seminowicz et al., 2011). More specifically, DLPFC thickness correlated with the reduction of both pain and physical disability. Additionally, increased thickness in primary motor cortex was associated specifically with reduced physical disability, and right anterior insula was associated specifically with reduced pain. Left DLPFC activity during an attention-demanding cognitive task was abnormal before treatment, but normalized following treatment (Seminowicz et al., 2011). Other therapeutic approaches that have been shown to normalize representational cortical changes that are associated with chronic pain (i.e., mirror box therapy for phantom limb pain) have also been demonstrated to decrease the experience of suffering and the clinical presentation 62 of pain. Mindfulness and Meditation therapies involving free-floating vs. directed attention have also been shown to modulate and decrease pain-evoked neural activation in the dorsolateral and ventrolateral PFC. Finally, there are preliminary studies showing that people who meditate have thicker cortices in frontal regions, including the PFC, ACC, and insula (Lazar et al., 2005). While studies have not yet addressed the full impact of such mind–body therapies on the brains of patients with chronic pain, “current evidence suggests that they may have a neuroprotective effect” (Bushnell et al., 2013, p. 12). Synopsis and Rx Transition: Toward a Sensory Discriminative Perceptual Model Ultimately, all pain is the net output of constitutive interpretation and affective experience being generated from multiple influences to, from, and throughout the brain - and often in response to continued appraisal of perceived threat to life or well-being. The phenomenon itself, though explicitly consumptive of individual attention, is also implicit, often intangible, negatively valent, and only indirectly accessible by another through empirical inquiry. Yet, the aim and focus of many, if not most all, current musculoskeletal interventions remains concentrated upon the more physicalized mechanisms of the body; treatments directed almost exclusively to only the symptomatic structural-anatomic-isolated regions of dysfunction, and being guided by compartmentalized performance measures being isolated to often arbitrary and sometimes quite meaningless classification criteria with regard to overall measures for human function. Such isolationist structural determinism - quite evident in medical imaging and compartmentalized physical tests and especially embedded in medical language - can thereby lead to unnecessary pathology descriptors being ascribed to certain attributional aspects of the body parts or the perceived regions involved. These are, in all actuality, a normative, anomalous, and noncontributory natural variation, perhaps and more than likely either congenital or 63 developed over time, and cannot always be directly implicated a causal tissue aberration mechanism or otherwise as a symptom producing structural pathology in cases of chronic pain. Thus, the primary focus of many therapies on purely orthopedic aspects of structural or strength-motion functional impairments in the spine may be a factor contributing to the lack of success of current treatments. Several lines of evidence suggest that structural changes by themselves (in the absence of behavioral considerations) within the back might be unimportant, and there is growing evidence of extensive cortical reorganization as well as neurochemical and structural alterations in the brains of people with CNSLBP. These changes could contribute to the persistence of the problem and might represent a legitimate dimension of developing novel approaches for therapy (Flor et al., 1997; Wand, Parkitny, et al., 2011) as well as an alternative to physical exam-based sub-groupings remaining dependent upon grading of spinal motion segments by the examiner, of which inter-rater reliability remains a question of continued bias and suggestion. As has been cited for more than a decade, most procedures commonly used by clinicians in the physical examination of patients with back pain demonstrate low reliability (May, Littlewood, & Bishop, 2006). In general, all forms of pain are affected by many high central nervous system activities (Moller, 2014). Novel approaches to modulating neurophysiological changes occurring across distributed areas of the nervous system (including "top down" inhibitory central and biopsychosocial influences) may help to improve outcomes in patients with chronic musculoskeletal disorders, in addition to desensitizing peripheral nociceptive inputs (via usual physical medicine convention "bottom up" approaches) embedded within neural, neurovascular, and musculoskeletal tissues. 64 It thus follows that due consideration is now necessary for exploring and attending to new treatment interventions, which aim to train/entrain/re-train and to clarify complex sensations, with the underlying assumption that “accurate body perception underpins skilled movement, sensation, localization acuity, laterality discrimination, cohesive emotional states, selfawareness, etc.”; and that these competing sensations, their novel processes for re-routing and neuronal selection, and their corresponding emergent perceptions are all antithetical to the recurring and complex phenomenology of conditioned states of ongoing pain perception. Retrospectively and in sum, the same brain areas involved in the processing of pain are also involved in: (a) sensory discrimination; (b) the planning, execution, and control of movement; these are subsequently (c) correlated to the development and encoding of cortical representation and body schema; and (d) are intricately correspondent to ongoing interpretive cognitive conceptual-mediational processes and emotional affective appraisal qualities that give both relevance and tone to everyday functions that are ordinary and necessary to usual life, including those activities of doing and being that do not necessarily involve pain experience or pain processing. Perhaps these areas of co-involvement can be implicated toward the development of a new intervention that targets and reinforces toward selecting unaffected functional pathways as competing information to those pathways otherwise remaining affected or being dysfunctional. Distortion of Body Schema, Somatic Education Interventions, and Virtual Reality Body Schema is both a phenomenological construct (experientially - implicit) and a conceptual construct (explanatory – explicit). Definitions and descriptions encoded for body schema have included phenomena to account for the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space (Holmes & Spence, 2004) as well as accounting for "a nonconscious system 65 of processes that constantly regulate posture and movement" (Gallagher & Cole, 1995). Grasping a conceptual basis for alterations in body schema can help explain how felt changes in the body (the experience of somatic phenomena) can seem to correlate to corresponding and concomitant changes in cortical mapping. While, in essence, an intrinsic phenomenological event, these emergent cognitive-perceptual relationships being ever-interactive between self and environment – becoming perceptually representative as a confluence of senses and a coherence of image within working body schema – are concepts that remain yet linguistically vague and often as intangible abstractions in the use of everyday language. However, these subjective and intersubjective phenomena have been differentiated from mere artifact or normal variance through correspondent and scientific advances in neuro-imaging when correlated and matched to clinical presentation data. In addition, changes in body schema or body mapping can be most profound and disproportionate in representation when developed under the response of repeat conditions of chronic pain. Experimental psychologists have grappled with the mind-body problem of attempting to define body schema, and only recently has there been consideration for the inclusion of neurophysiological and neuroplastic correlates that are brought to light through newer advances in imaging technology, such as PET Scans and fMRI. Leading professors and founders of The Crossmodal Research Group, Charles Spence and Nicholas Holmes of Oxford University, United Kingdom, have put fourth some new thinking about bodily perception and awareness, as it relates to body schema and objects or persons in the surrounding environment becoming relational in terms of peri-personal space: Rather than invoking the abstract concept of the "body schema," we believe the focus should be on experimentally more tractable aspects of bodily experience, such as the perceived location, or the ownership of individual body-parts, the attribution of 66 sensations and movement to particular body-parts, or the extent to which external objects participate in multisensory and sensorimotor interactions.... To accomplish this, we review three broad areas of experimental research, namely: 1) The effects of the manipulation of visual information on the felt location and identity of individual body-parts, and the extent to which visual and tactile information is integrated under such conditions; 2) How artificial body-parts affect the integration of visual and tactile information, and how clothes and bodily adornments may become ‘incorporated’ into bodily representations (for example, clothes may enhance the felt dimensions of the body or body-parts); and 3) How the skilled use of a variety of tools may lead to altered multisensory or sensorimotor interactions, and the incorporation of such tools into bodily representations. (Holmes & Spence, 2006, pp. 2, 6) Classic examples of exploring these psychophysical phenomena include the "Rubber Hand Illusion" and the "Body Transfer Illusion" - both of which use vision, touch, spatial placement and context to convey a sense of ownership being attributable to an external believable object (such as a realistic-looking dummy hand) or to induce the illusion in the participating subject that even the body of another person or being is the participant's own body (as occurs with synchronized avatar movements in immersive virtual reality technologies). The Virtual Reality Bones™ component of this comparative intervention study employed the use of "Virtual Limb Segments" – namely, femur bones, pelvis models, and a vestibular system apparatus/temporal bone model – to serve as a structural corollary to "sense of ownership" being derived from the Rubber Hand Illusion. In addition, a full-scale, life-sized, upright standing, anatomical human skeleton model was employed to serve as a functional corollary for inducing a corresponding sense of ownership attribution as derived from the Body Transfer Illusion. The emergent features inherent to the model skeleton (as an avatar being of its own) were operationalized and methodically directed to facilitate an amplified sense of stand balance and central longitudinal postural axis, and for relating the anatomical transmission of ground reaction forces during gait. By outlining a deeper perspective for skeletal continuity through the densest trabecular pathways (a deeper core?) of 67 the model human skeleton, I aimed to generate and convey an added sensory dimension for internalizing a new frame of skeletal reference for updating a revitalized body schema and for experiencing enhanced proportionality in movement being both anatomically-visually reconstructed through the senses and actively operationalized through participation in specifically selected Feldenkrais Method®-based movements. Finally, through the deployment of a virtual skeletal avatar consisting of pelvis, lower limbs, and arrows, and as re-constituted through generic pre-recorded/instructional kinematic data using a Vicon™ motion capture system, I can display a transmissible three-dimensional animation using Polygon Viewer™ software to enhance visual-conceptual augmentation for entraining the vividness of skeletal experience being encountered during imagined gait cycle function. In summary, body schema ultimately and necessarily involves aspects of both central (brain processes) and peripheral (sensory, proprioceptive) neurological systems. As a collection of largely non-conscious processes, it registers the posture (and acture) of one's body parts in space and in tracking limb positions, thus playing an important role in the modeling and control of everyday action. From the essential necessity of neuroplasticity and learning, the schema is continuously updated and encoded during body movement and remains ever-active, anticipatory and adaptive for the spatial organization of effective action. It is therefore a pragmatic representation of the body’s spatial properties, which includes the length of limbs and limb segments, their arrangement, the configuration of the segments in space, their spatial-temporal sequencing and encoding, and the shape of the body surface in interactive exchange with the anticipatory and immediate affordances encountered in the environment (Holmes & Spence, 2004; Maravita, Spence, & Driver, 2003). 68 Distortion of Body Schema and Chronic Pain – especially Low Back Pain It is now known that patients with chronic back pain have reduced proprioceptive acuity and sensory discrimination at the back, and have a cortical representation of the back that is markedly different to healthy controls, and anecdotally, find subtle or differentiated movements of their pelvis and back (i.e., movement dexterity tasks) more difficult than people without back pain do (Moseley, 2008; Wand, Catley, Luomajoki, et al., 2014; Wand, Keeves, et al., 2013). Furthermore, body image/body schema is known to depend on somatic and proprioceptive input as co-determinants for cortical mapping, of which associated pain states may be inhibiting. Fundamentally, though by no means exclusively, the cerebral representation of pain can be considered to consist of two neural networks: One representing the discriminative and localization dimensions of pain and one representing the affective-emotional dimension of pain. The most studied representations of the physical body are those held in the primary (S1) and secondary (S2) somatosensory cortices and in the primary motor cortex (M1) and remains a continuing topic of study in other body schema based therapies, such as Graded Motor Imagery (Lotze & Mosely, 2007; Moseley, Butler, Beames, & Giles, 2012). All are thought to be important for the consciously-felt body – with affective dimension given to neighboring anterior cingulate cortex, insula (predominantly the anterior regions), ventral prefrontal lobe, amygdala, and adjacent hippocampus – also corresponding to known brain regions to be involved in pain signaling and processing of painful experience (Doidge, 2015; Louw & Puentedura, 2013). A descriptive consolidation of major brain areas and their varied corresponding functions implicated in pain processing is again depicted in Table 1. S1 and M1 probably hold the most precise and competing representations of the body. They are tightly connected and are functional entities for movement control and execution. This 69 somatotopic representation is thought to be maintained by lateral cortical inhibition, whereby input from a particular body part exerts an excitatory influence on its target S1 neurons and an inhibitory influence on neurons in adjacent representations. In this way, neural networks of body representation (e.g., cortical body maps) become correspondingly built into the neural architecture of the brain (Lotze & Moseley, 2007; Moseley et al., 2012). Could these networks afford a competing (functional) stimulus to the inhibition signaling of (protective) chronic pain states? It is now known that tactile input can sharpen the receptive fields of S1 neurons, especially when the individual allocates a quality of attention to the sensory input or a behavioral - learning objective that is associated with it (Moseley & Hodges, 2006). Treatment interventions can thus afford the opportunity of implementing spatial-tactile acuity and body image multi-sensory references as part of treatment for chronic back pain. As with phantom limb pain and in chronic regional pain syndrome (CRPS), graded motor imagery, spatial-temporal encoding of movement, and training tactile acuity has been shown to reduce pain and increase function (Flor & Diers, 2009), and thus a same or similar strategy might be true for modulating back pain. In fact, tactile two-point discrimination training has been compared to be more effective in decreasing movement-related pain in patients with chronic low back pain than traditional acupuncture, and is suggested as an underlying mechanism for explaining the commonly seen effects of sham acupuncture (Wand, Abbaszadeh, et al., 2013). Somatic Education Interventions and Low Back Pain More recently, "internalized" somatic education approaches which emphasize facilitation techniques and associated attentional exercises that are designed to improve toward the development of a more finely tuned quality of sensory discrimination, such as The Alexander Technique, have demonstrated significant large-scale efficacy for both symptom reduction and 70 cost containment within a population of LBP patients in the United Kingdom (U.K.; Little et al., 2008). At least one citation in Medline exists to warrant some efficacy of an approach involving the presentation of ideokinetic imagery as a movement imagery and postural awareness technique that was found effective for both improving posture development and reducing low back pain. Ideokinetic imagery is defined as “a postural development technique that involves using movement images to gain subcortical control over the spinal musculature” (Fairweather & Sidaway, 1993). As researchers in the field of exercise and sport, Fairweather and Sidaway (1993) examined the effectiveness of ideokinetic imagery and flexibility compared with abdominal strength training as methods for improving the spinal angles of lordosis and kyphosis and reducing low back pain. Findings indicated that only ideokinetic imagery - using a noninvasive video analysis technique to record changes in spinal angles - had a positive effect on the spinal column with improved spinal angles and cessation of low back pain, as compared to not using recorded movement imagery. This research concluded support for the use of ideokinetic imagery as an inexpensive and noninvasive technique to improve poor posture and reduce low back pain. As a qualitative impression, it can perhaps be surmised that the nature of imagery must also be kinesthetic and/or embodied or body-based - and not just visual, or otherwise distractive from some other pictorial image that is devoid of representing a concurrent spatial-temporal encoding context - in order to more effectively generate and modulate corticomotor excitability both within the spatial-relational and the functional aspects of cortical body maps. The Feldenkrais Method®, recognized as sharing some common principles with the Alexander Technique, ideokinetic Imagery, and Ideokinesis, seeks to link discriminative sensory–motor learning experiences (perception through action/quality of attention to novel 71 spatial-temporal configurations of embodiment) with neuro-plastic changes in the brain in conjunction with optimal use of self through sensing the core of the entire skeleton in the performance of everyday generalizable life tasks. As a corollary to sensory awareness process and multimodal integration, the work of Andre Bernard depicts a special kind of experiencial relevance toward the enhancement of skeletal – anatomical imagery references becoming more consciously internalized through his uniquely designed guided exposure processes. One captivating and seamless feature to his approach involves his combining descriptive and visual imagery within a simulated design for experiencing one's own skeleton - via both its constraints and opportunities - and for enhancing the functions of posture and movement. These inherent phenomenological features thereby become experienced through both existent biological structure and newly perceived form, and are addtionally substantive toward improving and maintaining overall function. These features are furthermore made useful toward fostering a representational mental idea or tangible cognitive image for actually experiencing and understanding the conceptual idea behind a particular functional arrangement for skeletal movement, “hence the term ideokinesis” (Bernard, Steinmuller, & Stricker, 2006). Creating a tangible reality for accessing such images and ideas, as a bridge between environment and embodiment, is the subject of the next section. Virtual Reality, Applications to Chronic Pain, and Prospective Studies for CNSLBP According to the Virtual Reality Society, virtual reality (VR) is the creation or replication of a virtual environment that is presented to our senses in such a way that we experience it as if we were really there. It uses a host of multimedia and immersive computersimulated technologies to achieve this goal, and by creating a constructive, typically threedimensional, interactive environment to target upon the senses, it can have profound effects on 72 human perception, action, and cognition. Real or imagined, it simulates a user's physical presence and environment in such a way that allows the user to interact with it, ideally through mechanisms known to be involved in multi-sensory integration. Multisensory integration, also known as multimodal integration, is the study of how information from the different sensory modalities, such as sight, sound, touch, smell, selfmotion, and taste, may be integrated by the nervous system (Stein, Stanford, & Rowland 2009). A coherent representation of objects combining modalities enables us to have meaningful perceptual experiences. Indeed, multisensory integration is central to adaptive behavior because it allows individuals to perceive a world of coherent perceptual entities (Lewkowicz & Ghazanfar, 2009). Multisensory integration also deals with how different sensory modalities interact with one another and alter each other’s processing. Most researchers distinguish three sensory systems related to sense of touch in humans: cutaneous, kinesthetic, and haptic (Lewkowicz & Ghazanfar, 2009; Stein et al., 2009). All perceptions mediated by cutaneous and/or kinesthetic sensibility are referred to as tactual or haptic perception. The term "haptic" is often associated with active touch to communicate or recognize objects (Wagemans et al., 2012). Conversely, non-contact haptic technology utilizes the sense of touch without physical contact of a device. This type of feedback involves interactions with a system that are in a 3D space around the user. Thus, the user is able to perform actions on a system in the absence of holding a physical input device – a key feature to permit freedom of movement within whole self as within the Microsoft Kinect VR interface. In the seminal work, The Merging of the Senses (Stein & Meredith, 1993), an extensive review of evidence regarding the investigations of neurophysiology functions involved in transmitting sensory information through deeper brain regions - specifically, the superior 73 colliculus in cats (Meredith, Nemitz, & Stein, 1987; Meredith & Stein, 1983, 1986b) - resulted in the distillation of three general principles by which multisensory integration may best be described: 1. The spatial rule states that multisensory integration is more likely or stronger when the constituent unisensory stimuli arise from approximately the same location. 2. The temporal rule states that multisensory integration is more likely or stronger when the constituent unisensory stimuli arise at approximately the same time. 3. The principle of inverse effectiveness states that multisensory integration is more likely or stronger when the constituent unisensory stimuli evoke relatively weak responses when presented in isolation. Summarized another way, the more ways that the presentation of a stimulus can co-occur and be combined through multi-modal and cross-modal representations, and the more it can occur within the same (temporal) time frame - and occurring or localizing within the same general embodied location (haptic space) in relationship to its corresponding environment (visual space), be it through peri-personal (external) or interoceptive awareness (internal) domains for sensory reference - then the better and more integrative the multi-sensory perception becomes for the accurate discernment and cognition of difference, and even perhaps for the constructed meaning of the experience. It is important to note that the term “virtual reality” does not limit the practitioner or researcher to a particular configuration of computerized hardware and software. Instead, VR may be understood as a development of simulations that make use of various combinations of interaction devices and sensory display systems. Typically, the designs for these systems are developed with consideration of balancing the level of immersiveness with the level of invasiveness. While historical uses of VR have opted 74 for highly immersive experiences in gaming by using the more invasive and expensive, and clinically cumbersome head-mounted displays (HMDs), a new generation of simulation gaming technologies are becoming available for inducing relatively lower-level immersion experiences, including the Microsoft Kinect™ for Windows. Whilst such non-invasive systems involve a lower level of immersion, the phenomenological experience of the user is one that involves a high potential for effective interaction with (the presentation of) digital (or material) content, (and by) using naturalistic body actions. (Trost & Parsons, 2014) Opinions differ on what exactly constitutes a true VR experience, but in general it should include: ● Three-dimensional images that appear to be life-sized from the perspective of the user; ● The ability to track a user's motions, particularly head and eye movements; and ● To correspondingly adjust the images on the user's display to reflect the change in perspective. Provisions for each of these features are key components built into the Virtual Reality Bones™ protocol and its corresponding selection for Feldenkrais movement applications as ® intended within the experimental arm of the current study. In which case, the user’s display is actually the user’s own body, together with its kinesthetic and haptic sensory awareness being cultivated, integrated and combined in real time, during and inclusive of the intervention. NIH Public Access has selected the publically-funded, author manuscript, "Virtual Reality and Pain Management: Current Trends and Future Directions," by Li, Montaño, Chen, and Gold (2011), to inform and guide current applications for acute pain management (i.e., VR-induced anesthesia), and also to recommend and suggest important areas for future research, namely, the more challenging problem of chronic pain. 75 In it, Li et al. (2011) cited previous investigative work, which postulates the idea that VR can act as a nonpharmacologic form of analgesia by exerting an array of emotional-affective and competing cognitive-attentional processes within the body’s intricate pain modulation system. In an earlier paper by one of its co-authors, neurobiological mechanisms were hypothesized, suggesting that VR analgesia originates from intercortical modulations among signaling pathways of the pain matrix through attention, emotion, memory and other senses being afforded through VR experiences (e.g., touch, auditory, and visual), thereby producing a competing inhibitory modulation of the conditioned pathways typically involved in the processing, production, and maintenance of chronic pain. It is further stated that an overall decrease of activities in the pain matrix may be accompanied by increases of activity in the anterior cingulate cortex and orbitofrontal regions of the brain (Gold, Belmont, & Thomas, 2007), suggesting that the cognitive demands of the task, and not simply the mechanism of attention distraction alone may be an important factor in the attenuation and modulation of pain thresholds (Seminowicz & Davis, 2007). Functional imaging studies of the human brain’s response to painful stimuli have shown increases of activities in the anterior cingulate gyrus, the insula, the thalamus, and sometimes in other regions such as the primary somatosensory cortex and the periaqueductal gray matter (Li et al., 2011) that have yet to be investigated in response to VRbased approaches. Future studies are underway to examine the complex interplay of cortical activity associated with immersive VR. Recently, new applications, including VR, have been developed to augment evidenced-based interventions, such as hypnosis and biofeedback, for the treatment of chronic pain. Interestingly, in a case study involving a 36-year-old female with a 5-year history of unretractable chronic neuropathic pain, a pilot intervention involving virtual reality 76 augmented hypnosis was found to be more effective than hypnosis alone, by reducing pain and prolonging the treatment effects (Oneal, Patterson, Soltani, Teeley, & Jensen, 2008). It is anticipated that lessons learned from these early VR investigations will lead to further applications in chronic pain management and other pain rehabilitative conditions. Other reviews of experimental evidence suggest that rapid advancement of virtual reality (VR) technologies are also applicable to the development of novel strategies for sensorimotor training in neurorehabilitation, as in cases of literature being reviewed for stroke recovery (Adamovich, August, Merians, & Tunik, 2009). The reviewer discovered through his own research that time-variant activations of the left insular cortex corresponded to a condition of “observing with the intent to imitate” the sequences of finger movement being performed by a virtual hand avatar seen in first-person perspective and then computer animated by pre-recorded kinematic data. This observation of underlying neurophysiological mechanisms revealed through real time fMRI occurred in the experimental condition of "action observation-pre-execution" (implicative of mirror neurons) but not in other conditions (Adamovich, August, et al., 2009). Moreover, imitation with veridical feedback from the virtual avatar (relative to the control condition) also recruited the angular gyrus, precuneus, and extrastriate body area, regions, which are (along with insular cortex) associated with the sense of agency. Thus, the virtual hand avatars may be useful for sensorimotor training by serving as disembodied tools when observing actions and as embodied “extensions” of the subject's own body (pseudo-tools) when practicing the actions (Adamovich, Fluet, Tunik, & Merians, 2009). In addition, the use of tangible and prehensile haptic feedback is shown to select for automatic and implicit processes that are more readily facilitative of instantaneous motor learning, which can serve to bypass the effects of biased, sometimes self-critical, and over-analytical effects of 77 explicit cognitive "proper performance" training (Boyd & Winstein, 2006). These studies have strong conceptual implications for articulating some background rationale in favor of purporting the experimental design of the current study. Again, these inferences will thus later apply to the current study wherein (a) "virtual pelvis and hip avatars" (visually re-constructed by computerized animation from pre-recorded kinematic data), and (b) the tangible haptic contact of virtual reality bones™ will be used to facilitate a basis for improving spine stability (or for at least inferring only its background involvement) in the context of gait function and generalizing the VR entrainments into repeated practice within separate components of the gait cycle. Clinical research applications of VR for the problem of CNSLBP are just now underway. A team of researchers at The University of North Texas has developed a protocol for virtual reality graded exposure therapy (VRGET) to address several central limitations of traditional graded exposure therapy and usual VR approaches to pain/disability treatment. They applied the use of a skeleton tracker being embedded within the Microsoft Kinect’s interactive technology platform to more closely match and generalize patient’s movements toward a "real world" simulation experience. An application of trials to test their therapeutic approach for a population of patients with pain-related fear and the clinical presentation of chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) is now currently at the pilot stage. By using the Microsoft skeleton tracker as a simulation tool, a calibrated skeleton model is automatically generated and proportioned through each patient’s demonstration of movement at baseline. Participants’ can use their own bodies as controls. The Kinect is one of the most widely used whole-body trackers and has the ability to integrate body-state information into various simulations. The Kinect system uses image, audio, 78 and depth sensors for movement detection, facial expression identification, and speech recognition. The Kinect’s interactive technology allows users to interact with simulations using their own bodies as controls. An important advance in the Kinect technology is that, unlike previous attempts at gesture or movement based controls, the patient is not encumbered by the need to wear accessories to enable the tracking of his or her movements (Trost & Parsons, 2014; Trost et al., 2015). Other studies have already shown improvements in common measures for LBP studies as the resultant effects of a VR-based Wii Fit exercise program involving a population of middle-aged, female patients presenting with LBP in South Korea (Kim, Min, Kim, & Lee, 2014). As a summation, adaptive and engaging virtual environments being afforded through VRbased interventions provide a unique perspective and potential to benefit patients with disordered movement. This is accomplished in ways that facilitate and promote the massive and intensive sensorimotor stimulation needed to induce change as necessary precursors known to be inherent to brain reorganization and neuroplasticity. Differences between Feldenkrais® Virtual Reality and Traditional Guided Imagery New techniques for Virtual Reality and Traditional Guided Imagery continue to be investigated as viable psychophysiological-based therapy approaches for implementation to the treatment of chronic pain. Yet, chronic pain remains a continued challenge to patients and clinicians. As was previously discussed in the Emergent Findings from the Neuroscience of Chronic Pain and Neuroplasticity section of this chapter, chronic pain seems to be maintained by diffuse neurophysiological changes across different areas of the peripheral and central nervous systems, including peripheral receptors, dorsal horn of the spinal cord, brain stem, sensorimotor cortical areas, and the mesolimbic and prefrontal areas. These have been especially studied 79 among populations of patients with chronic low back pain, among other chronic musculoskeletal disorders. Musculoskeletal rehabilitation professionals implementing psycho-physical and psychophysiological approaches have tools at their disposal to address these neuroplastic changes. These include "top-down" cognitive-based interventions (e.g., pain science education, cognitive-behavioral therapy, mindfulness meditation, biofeedback/neurofeedback and motor imagery) in addition to more traditional "bottom-up" physical interventions (e.g., peripheral sensory stimulation, including e-stim, manual therapy, motor control exercises, kinesio-taping, and motor learning) that induce neuroplastic changes across distributed areas of the nervous system and that can affect outcomes in patients with chronic musculoskeletal disorders (Pelletier et al., 2015a). Multiple lines of research continue to suggest that more comprehensive, integrative, and novel approaches to modulating neurophysiological changes occurring across distributed areas of the central nervous system may help to improve outcomes involving "self-control" in patients with chronic musculoskeletal disorders, in addition to desensitizing afferent-nociceptive peripheral inputs embedded within the more localized aspects of neural end-plate receptors and musculoskeletal tissues. In tallying a composite review of possible physiological mechanisms underlying these interventional constructs, the distinctions occurring between either "top-down" or "bottom-up" mechanisms of action can become increasingly blurred; particularly, in cases where newer, more integrative and holistic approaches become increasingly evolved toward unifying upon a common and multivariate continuum. Guided imagery is a therapeutic technique that allows a person to use his or her own imagination to connect his or her body and mind to achieve desirable outcomes, such as 80 decreased pain perception and reduced anxiety (Ackerman & Turkoski, 2000). An imagined mental image can be defined as “a thought with sensory qualities,” so as to actively conjure up or to re-invoke a memory experience of sensations for seeing, hearing, tasting, smelling, touching, or feeling and to apply them to a situation. More specifically, kinesthetic or motor imagery necessarily involves the invocation of attending to proprioception or the positional sense of body in space as well as to corresponding body regions or body parts during actual movement or in association with an imagined or intended movement. The learning of new sensory-motor skills often requires the mental rehearsal of movement being efferently fed-forward through the kinesthetic sense of the body - both it's internal/cortical representation (including sub-cerebral and cerebellar components) and through the body itself - for both intended and actual movement. These processes are usually contingent upon pre-conditioned and established patterns of action having been developed, acquired, and habituated through prior models and modes of experience, thereby serving as existing perceptual templates or internal models from which to derive, project, and direct the guided imagery phenomena. Imagery can have profound physiological consequences, and depending on how skilled a person can be in creating or re-creating a believable image or experience, his or her own physiological responses can respond to imagery as similar as they would to a genuine or actual external experience. However, when patients with chronic pain are exposed to their most powerful/distressing image (an index image; being directly associated with their diagnosis, their internally felt experience, and the perceived impact on their quality of life), the data generated through their self-report scales and structured interviews have been found to evoke significant increases in negative emotions, negative cognitive appraisals, and escalations in pain levels in 81 response to such image exposure (Phillips, 2011). Implications for image re-scripting are thus an important part of CBT and guided imagery, or other mind-body, mindfulness-based interventions. Thus, well-intended guided imagery involving some part of "the back" or "back-specific activities" are often prone to "backfire" in that they are directly reinforcing of attention to associated index imagery; be they radiographic images, MRI reports of spinal morphologic changes, routine pathologic or diagnostic descriptions, or other applicable images indicating or even remotely suggesting that "something is wrong" with the back. Even more profound, by just imagining one’s own movement through kinesthetic or motor imagery, or even the simulated actions of others through mirrored motor imagery - of observing and mentally imitating someone else’s actions - will reproduce and amplify existing pain sensory response networks and invoke a cascade of neurological pathways known to be associated with the pain experience. Thus, it can be said that even the virtual body experiences pain. Again, the recent study by Vrana et al. (2015) confirmed demonstrable findings for widespread and differential neural processing activity between chronic LBP patients as compared to healthy controls upon reviewing fMRI data during motor imagery-driven activity, wherein each group was exposed to video clips of persons performing a graded series of potentially strenuous "back-related" activities (the details from this study and the differential brain regions co-involved in both chronic pain and the simulated imagery of action were aptly discussed earlier in Chapter 2, under the Neuroscience of Chronic Pain and Neuroplasticity subsection of this dissertation). Seeing that existing imagery qualities and locus of control competencies have become disrupted within patients with chronic pain, and that internal foci remain pain-driven with a predisposition and sensitization toward provocation and re-triggering 82 of pain pathway phenomena, it seems pertinent that a discussion of difference between traditional therapist-mediated guided imagery and Feldenkrais -inspired virtual body imagery ® (via also a VRB3 model of approach) is in order. While there is much shared similarity in both approaches, traditional guided imagery exhibits a transcendent tendency to project a pre-scripted image to the patient or client, to which the patient or client may (or may not) internally re-construct an actual or accurate image as was intended by the therapist. That is to say that, internally, the image itself may (or may not) be constructed through the voluntary will, effort, or intent of either party or dyad, and without actual contact, both the therapist and the patient will thereby operate from separate inter-subjective fields. Guided imagery, by its nature of delivery and design, therefore, has to be pre-projected through the imagination and the cognitive constraints of each person in a direction that is "top down" from mind to body, and is therefore, only representative of the senses and not emergent phenomena arising or processing from direct sensation itself. Alternatively, in the Feldenkrais/VRB3 model of approach, the image itself (tangible and accurate skeletal segment models and 3-D visual-kinesthetic avatars) is already pre-constructed, and the Feldenkrais practitioner® aims to project the constructed image interactively through direct contact to afford corresponding feedback; all while operating from a shared intersubjective phenomenological field (i.e., a shared virtual environment). In all virtual reality, therefore, the image is pre-constructed and is projected directly onto the senses, and thus represents a competing stimulus strategy that is constitutively "bottom-up" from body to mind, and is automatically assimilated involuntarily. By the biological necessity of all living and conscious entities, rarely can the introduction of novel, unexpected, and direct sensations 83 (sensory commands) remain ignored or unprocessed, as a more direct experience of actual (and virtual) representations of the internal and/or external world. Figure 6 depicts differences in intersubjective phenomenological fields between top down guided imagery (as an un-shared; linguistic-abstractive domain of interaction) vs. bottom up direct contact (as a more-shared and inter-projected, concretized domain of interaction). Example of Traditional Guided Imagery ‘Top Down’ Pre-Scripted Linguistic Domain Example of Feldenkrais® virtual body imagery ‘Bottom-Up’ Physical Construct Domain Figure 6. Differences betweeen Traditional Guided Imagery and Feldenkrais Method® Contact Imagery. In traditional guided imagery, the moment by moment projected images are primarily intra-subjective, abstracted through language, and are thereby lingusitic and fleeting in nature, and not necessarily co-constructed toward accurate representation of embodied relationship; resulting essentially in a separate phenomenological field, with reduced intersubjectivity between therapist and patient. In Feldenkrais®-based contact/virtual haptic imagery (Hands-on Functional Integration®), moment in moment images are alerady pre-constructed, mutually explored, and continuously re-projected from a variety of multisensory contact points - such that they are simultaneuosly expereinced as embodied relationships with enhanced intersubjectivity betweeen practitioner and client; resulting essentially in a shared phenomenological field of interaction and with a corresponding linkage toward fostering functional adjustment to task in real time. 84 Gravitation as a Virtual and Invariant Constant in the Sensory-Motor World While phenomenological perception can also vary between one individual and another, at least one predictive area involving a mutually shared and invariant constant - being cocontributory toward the evolutionary development and strategic emergence of all living things involves their cohabitation within an incessant and persistent field of gravity that is both a universal given and an influential absolute to all known existence. Through the evolution of species – and most particularly, through evolution of uprightness and locomotion - it is only through the function of upwardly directed and dispersive movement (in directions of antigravity) that all living things are able to overcome, differentiate, and to distinguish some sense of separation (i.e., some sense of morphologic identity) from the ever-present and constraining influences of gravity. Accordingly, and adding a neurological component to the gravity of the situation, Roger Sperry, the winner of the 1981 Nobel Prize for Medicine and Physiology, reported that “Better than 90 percent of the energy output of the brain is used in relating the physical body in its gravitational field. The more mechanically distorted a person is, the less energy available for thinking, metabolism and healing [emphasis added]” (Wyszynski, 2013, p. 8, emphasis added). The development and seemingly spontaneous formations of loops, twists, and dynamic spirals at all levels of existence (both living and non-living) appears most inherent as an optimal organizational strategy for navigating a responsive and effective relationship to gravity. Thus, serving as a reflective platform for assimilating against a seamless array of un-mitigating forces while simultaneously achieving curvilinear, dynamic confluence along parallel and contiguous lines being characteristic and predictive of gravity’s direction. By being dimensionally responsive through more expansive and distributive confluences of interwoven shape and form, 85 it seems that curvilinear and semi-circular arcs - becoming intrinsically elevated and discernable within the vestibular sensory systems of all skeletal vertebrates - are well-positioned for informing a more predictive and adjustable platform for sensory navigation during locomotion; and for incremental detection of time and space variances, while occurring across changing body positions, which especially happens during the transition to uprightness of head orientation (cranial aspects) over and opposite that of a caudal base of support (i.e., lower quadrant ground reaction/support surfaces under legs and feet); and finally, most ultimately and fundamentally, while counterbalancing and rising up against gravity itself. It so happens that sustaining these primitive functions are also the same activities that are found to be the most challenging and difficult for patients afflicted with chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP). While the back itself and/or its immediately adjacent areas are most commonly cited as a source of visible rationale for producing problems and symptoms in the diagnostic foreground - there is also a hidden contribution of invisible rationale existing deeply remote in the background of everyday movement and in direct response to the push and pull of gravity - yet far away from any particular lumbar spine segment. An “above the lumbar spine” versus a “below the lumbar spine” categorical or regional perspective is virtually unknown for inclusion, and thereby remains as an undisclosed component of body schema referencing for most persons; let alone for occurring within the functional body awareness ‘diagnostic schemata’ of most all healthcare professionals and patients alike. Hidden Senses: A Skeletal Density-Vestibular Concept for Body Schema & Pain The vestibular system, as "a hidden sense" being deeply encased below the temporal lobes of the brain and within either side of the densest skull base, is very likely a key component for body schema in motor control, and quite possibly, an overlooked factor of dysregulation in 86 the maintenance of chronic pain. An anatomical schematic of a vestibular sense end organ is exposed and depicted in Figure 7. Figure 7. Anatomical Schematic and Location of Vestibular Apparatus. Encased within temporal bone to either side of skull and within each inner ear. The vestibular sensory apparatus itself, being composed of the three semicircular canals, and the utricle-saccule otoliths within the inner ear, is a multimodal sensory system that is involved in many functions including balance and equilibrium, righting reflexes, vision stabilization during body movement, spatial navigation, the ongoing perception of body configuration and proportionate length; and for maintaining the necessary consciousness alertness that is required for keeping a skeletal-vertebrate animal upright against gravity. Developmentally, it is among the first of the paired special senses to develop, becoming encased within the densest of all skeletal structures (the bony labyrinth: within the temporal bones of each side of the skull and within each inner ear), and are among the first to undergo myelination during early stage development, while still in utero (Tecklin, 2014). The vestibular system is phylogenetically the oldest part of the inner ear, yet it was only recognized as an entity distinct from the cochlea (for hearing) in the middle of the 19th century. This is because when the system is functioning normally, we are usually unaware of a distinct sensation arising from vestibular activity; it is integrated with visual, proprioceptive and other extra-vestibular information such that combined experience leads to a sense of motion. (Cullen, 2012) 87 Most vestibular research has focused on the encoding of gravitational and inertial signals emanating and reciprocating throughout each vestibular end organ (namely, through the three semicircular canals and otoliths on each contralateral side) and their dual projections to brain stem and cerebellar areas. Laboratory findings and clinical investigations have thus been applied toward understanding the involuntary neurological mechanisms involved in righting responses, including vestibulo-ocular and vestibulo-spinal reflexes, and their corresponding role in maintaining gaze stabilization during vision perturbation as well as for balance and posture control under conditions of disequilibrium or gravitational instability, and for the treatment of pathologic dizziness or vertigo. However, more recently, a more complete understanding of vestibular function is being advocated for by conducting new investigations into the role of cortical influences and the higher processing of vestibular-generated sensory phenomena. Consequently, new areas of inquiry have recently emerged to engage some other, often taken for granted psycho-physical aspects that are inherently important to human perception and effective functional actions, as they routinely recur throughout the gravitational and social world of everyday living. These include: 1. Spatial Cognition and the role of vestibular sensory impressions for spatial navigation, spatial interpretation, and the encoding of spatial memory; 2. Body Representation – including spatial proportion and representations of body part size, the perceived distances between body regions, changes in tactile sensitivity toward attenuation versus amplification of sensory phenomena, and most relational to the current investigation, having anatomical-functional correlates to sensory distortions and kinesthetic mismatches that are correspondent to known changes in somatorepresentations in association with states of chronic pain; and finally, 88 3. An associative role for disparities in vestibular signaling toward the development, high comorbidity, and/or sustenance of affective processes and mood disorders, including anxiety, panic, and depression. Knowledge gained through these research areas and other studies using the manipulation of vestibular input (e.g., body motion, imagined rotation of self and/or other objects, in vitro laboratory inductions through inner ear caloric stimulation and /or galvanic vestibular stimulations, and their concomitant neuroimaging correlates) have concluded that observed behavioral responses for each of the three research clusters are indeed co-associated, at least in part, with different neuronal core mechanisms: 1. Spatial transformations draw on parietal areas, 2. Body representation is associated with somatosensory areas, and 3. Affective processes involve insular and cingulate cortices. While all these functions are indeed conducive toward receiving (the common denominator of) vestibular input, the consensus of caveat in the literature concedes that “even though a wide range of different vestibular cortical projection areas have been ascertained, their functionality still is scarcely understood” (Mast, Preuss, Hartmann, & Grabherr, 2014, p. 1). Perhaps more complex interventions can aid to clarify inter-relationships between necessarily complex, inter-associative phenomena. Spatial Cognition as an Internal Model for Perception of Virtual Limb Segments and Bones Interestingly, the vestibular system is not only involved in the usual processing of bodyself motion in the physical world, it also seems to play an important role in building and maintaining a mental representation of the internal world. Even though people are bound to 89 physical space, they are able to represent objects and movements mentally in order to optimally predict actions, respond to events, and to solve new problems. Influences of vestibular stimulation on tactile perception and body representation in healthy subjects and patients alike indeed seem neurologically plausible given the anatomical overlap of vestibular and somatosensory networks (Lopez & Blanke, 2011; Lopez et al. as cited in Mast et al., 2014). A recent fMRI study that applied tactile and caloric vestibular stimulation in the same subjects also revealed important overlapping cortical activation in the dorsal posterior insula and the parietal areas (zu Eulenburg et al. as cited in Mast et al., 2014). It has now become evident that vestibular information is necessary for maintaining metric properties of representational space and for encoding, predicting, and mentally simulating movements in situations that do not involve displacements of the body, as in motor imagery or imagined tool use. Imaging studies involving patients with vestibular loss (as in Meniere’s Disease) have demonstrated impaired ability to access object-based mental transformations (OBMTs – defined here as imagined rotations or translations of objects relative to the environment) as compared to healthy controls. These findings indicate that vestibular signals are necessary to perform OBMTs and thereby provide a reliable demonstration of the critical role of vestibular signals in the cognitive processing of metric properties of objects and their corresponding mental representations. They suggest that “vestibular loss disorganizes brain structures commonly involved in mental imagery, and more generally in mental representation” (Péruch et al., 2011). Furthermore, studies involving induced sensory imbalance in healthy controls (via receptor activation of one [ipsilateral] vestibular organ subsequent to caloric vestibular stimulation; with resultant comparative inhibition from the contralateral receptor) have been 90 shown to induce specific "bottom-up" changes in spatial cognitive tasks in healthy controls. Conversely and potentially-therapeutically, it has also been shown that "top-down" higher cognitive processes, such as mental imagery, can be demonstrated to alter, effect, and modulate the perception of induced vestibular stimuli. The spatial-temporal sensing of virtual and actual limb segments; their corresponding mental representation, and their explorations in proportion with controlled Feldenkrais® movements involving their correspondent relationships of "pelviships opposite head" are a central feature of the experimental arm of the current study. Vestibular-Ocular Representation: A Mediator of Body Schema Acuity & Motor Dexterity? A second important area of psychophysical research demonstrates remarkable evidence that vestibular activation is compositionally involved in mediating the neuroplasticity processes that update and encode for body representation acuity in real time. Studies have now shown that the perceived size of the hand is increased during caloric vestibular stimulation in comparison to sham stimulation, implying an enlarged somato representation due to vestibular stimulation. Numerous other studies have also demonstrated that vestibular stimulation changes the representation of body parts, including altered sensitivity to tactile input - inclusive of perceived pain intensity - in relationship to perceived size (Mast et al., 2014). Interdependently, eye movements also assert and reflect an organizing modulatory effect affording a sense of stability, congruity, and contiguity of direction for all body movement and experience. There is direct neurological connection between the eye muscles, the head-neckspine, and vestibular system through the medial longitudinal fasciculus and other pathways. Within central processing between primary nuceli, the medial vestibulospinal tract is found only in the cervical spine and above. Its pathways projects bilaterally to infra-medullary regions of the 91 spinal cord and is particularly involved in controlling the neurons associated with the spinal accessory nerves (cranial nerve XI), which innervate the trapezius and sternocleidomastoid muscles. These muscles are prime movers and stabilizers of the head and neck. Additional and recursive vestibular-ocular tracts also project upward to help keep the eyes “yoked” together during rapid head movement and to maintain gaze stabilization during usual locomotion and transposition. Thus, the medial vestibular projections are of particular significance for linking (a) vestibular, (b) visual, and (c) somatosensory information through corresponding head-eye-body movements and to co-regulate whole-body orientation and posture control through the composite outcome of multi-sensory and variable input (Fitzgerald, Gruener, & Mtui, 2012). Contradirectionally, the lateral vestibulospinal tract projects ipsilaterally down the spinal cord (e.g., the right inner ear projects down the right side of the body), and thereby serves to maintain balance and posture by co-regulating para-spinal flexor-extensor muscle tone (i.e., motor control of antigravity muscles) throughout the trunk, spine and lower extremity (Fitzgerald et al., 2012). Notable researchers in the rehabilitation of vestibular disorders have stated that the human vestibular system can be said to “play a key role in the spinal movement symphony” (Herdman & Clendaniel, 2014). Thus, the vestibular and ocular systems integrate to play an important role in the multisensory coordination and detection of body representation, with corresponding informative linkages to background body schema sensory acuity and motor dexterity. Indeed, many Feldenkrais®-based movement self-explorations involve coordinating, comparing, contrasting, and understanding (through direct experience) the vital relationships between movements of the eyes (visual), movements of the head (vestibular), and the movements of other parts of the body (somato-sensory). These have been seen to have 92 observable and beneficial (yet anecdotal) effects towards the resolution of stubbornly persistent musculoskeletal pain conditions and other mobility deficit problems (Cheikin, 1986-2011). Vestibular Contribution to Affective Limbic Processes, Body Schema, & Chronic Pain Modulation Linkages between parieto-insular cortices and vestibular nuclei have been identified using fMRI (Eickhoff, Weiss, Amunts, Fink, & Zilles, 2006). Thus, the cerebral cortex processing of vestibular sensations is known to extend from cortical to cortical projections into the insula, and at least one exceptional clinical correlation case study report revealed that a small lesion in the right anterior insular cortex could be implicated as a likely cause for loss of balance and vertigo after ruling out other peripheral and brainstem causes for the patient's symptoms (Papathanasiou et al., 2006). In terms of affect regulation and sense of self, the insular cortex is increasingly recognized as an important area for assigning emotional valence to subjective feelings and a sense of self-agency and personal relevance to sensory experience. In this, it contributes a primary basis toward the formation of interoceptive awareness, with added control of autonomic homeostasis through the afferent-efferent connections between sympathetic and parasympathetic systems. Recent experiments suggest that vestibular-insular pathways may also provide a possible interaction between vestibular and nociceptive processing (zu Eulenburg as cited in Mast et al., 2014). Cross-modal assessment upon neuroimaging has revealed selectively distinct, shared activation of the anterior insula by both caloric vestibular stimulation and aversive (tactile heat) stimuli, as contrasted by more generalized activation of posterior insula by all other multisensory/multimodal signals - including vestibular, tactile, and non-nociceptive somatosensory – all converging together when demonstrated upon fMRI. It has also been 93 hypothesized that the posterior insula plays a key role in the neural underpinnings of pain alleviation induced via non-nociceptive vestibular stimulation, and change in orientation, which in turn is proposed to inhibit the continued generation of pain signaling in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). Most recently, an osteopathic neuroscience clinical specialty group and research team conducted a study to assess the incidence of vestibular dysfunction in patients receiving medication and pharmacotherapy for chronic, noncancerous pain or other underlying neurologic disorders. They found that vestibular deficits were detected in 66.9% of their patient sample. Patient ages ranged from 29 through 72 years, with a mean age of 50.7 years for women and 52.5 years for men (Gilbert et al., 2014). Upon consideration that vestibular information aids in reconstructing the global body schema, Mast et al. (2014) hypothesized that “vestibular stimulation can alleviate pain by contributing to ameliorate the impaired body schema and to help restore the “body matrix” (Moseley et al. as cited by Mast et al., 2014). “Thus, conditions like complex regional pain syndrome or chronic back pain, where disturbed body representations have been described (Moseley, 2005; Moseley, Zalucki, & Wiech, 2008), should benefit the most” (Mast et al., 2014, p. 13). Vestibular contributions toward awareness of baseline body arrangement, trans-positional body configuration and coordination within space and time, implicit/explicit attention to gravitational relationship during novel movement explorations, sensory conflict negotiation, proportionate action through reduced effort, felt-emotional associations, and the continuous readjustment to move more fluidly through everyday functional tasks are all-inclusive as key tenants of comprehensive practice applications being embodied within The Feldenkrais 94 Method®. They are conducive to both "optimal use of whole self" and as an ongoing behavioral template for the revision and enactment of continuous body schema. Feldenkrais’ Postulates and Vestibular Contributions to Skeletal Organization, Movement, and Behavior In his original book, Body and Mature Behavior, Moshe Feldenkrais (1949/2005) had accurately described much of what has only now come to be revealed through recent advances in neuroscience and the modern neuro-imaging of vestibular relationships to action, emotion, and neuroplasticity. First, from an evolutionary and developmental standpoint: Animals born with a more fully grown brain come with “ready-made” (instinctual) reactions to external stimuli, and to most stimuli they are likely to encounter in life…But in man, whose adult brain in several times its weight at birth, has fewer ready-made responses to external stimuli. His nervous system is growing while the external stimuli are reaching it…In man, there is no genetic inheritance of language, gait, or any other muscular activity (and these activities must therefore be individually learned through extended experience and apprenticeship)… Environment therefore, has a greater influence on his nervous system than on that of any other animal…Learning, in the most general sense, means acquiring new responses to stimuli…The bulk of stimuli arriving at the nervous system is from muscular activity being constantly affected by gravity. Therefore (upright) posture (and sensory-motor-coordination development) is one of the best clues not only to evolution, but also to the activity of the brain. (Feldenkrais, 1949/2005, pp. 36-37, 38-39) A predominating physical feature of the human biped is that the "center of gravity" relative to "base of support" is maintained at a higher vertical position than to that of any other animal. As such, there is more propensity to move in any direction more equally and with minimal expenditure of energy, particularly in rotation. However, the relative 360 degrees of immediate freedom, much of it imparted through automated potential energy while upright, also necessitates constraints, and these are neurologically evolved through sensory-motor processes and the regulatory achievement of motor control. 95 As a system, no segment of the body can be moved without corresponding adjustment of all the others to a new configuration particular to actual circumstance. Rather than attempt to reduce highly varied and complex acts into predictive-isolated mechanisms consisting of targeted components, or specific muscle groups, as has been the traditional mode of thinking in analytical biomechanics and for so much of conventional physical therapy, Feldenkrais instead found it more useful to describe visible features of optimally synergistic ‘motor control’ functions that could arise or emerge as explanatory outcomes for such inter-segmental co-operation within a unified whole system. First that, (a) as a matter of natural inclination, a body could be neurologically organized and responsive to initiate for selected movement dimensions in any direction with equal ease; that (b) it can start a movement without any hesitation, pre-preparation, preoccupation, or any other energy-consuming preliminary adjustment; that (c) any initiated movement can be equally and easily reversible; and that (d) all movements are performed with minimum work and maximum efficiency. In this, “the musculature shows no useless contraction in any part of the body. All the articulations participate in every act. None is held rigidly in any particular configuration not dictated by the immediate task being performed” (Feldenkrais, 1949/2005, p. 77). “This means also that no movement unnecessary for the act is done. The body moves, therefore, smoothly, and describes clear curves and lines. The aesthetic search for design and purity in movement is thus also satisfied” (p. 72). Conversely, it can be stated that dysfunction occurs when these qualities of movement become disrupted and mal-synergistic by intrinsically working against oneself, as can be seen when patients with chronic pain – including LBP and Fibromyalgia – perform usual and routine exercise movements. 96 Similarly, Feldenkrais (1949/2005), as an originating sensory integrationist, knew that multisensory influences played a key role by stating that “there is no isolated sensory impulse” (p. 79). In further discussing antigravity mechanisms involved in posture and motor control, and beyond mere "vestibular stimulation" in and of itself, he stated that "there is (continual) integrative action of the nervous system, insuring that only one final algebraic sum of all the incitations reaches the muscle at any one time” (p. 55). These sensory afferent-efferent impulses derive from multiple sources, including: (a) vestibular labyrinths composed of otoliths (utricle and saccule) and semicircular canals of the inner ear; (b) the proprioceptive sense organs richly innervating muscle fibers, tendons, and ligaments between bones; (c) exteroceptive nerve endings and receptors embedded throughout skin and connective tissue as preliminary contact surfaces preceding bone; (d) interoceptive and enteric innervations throughout viscera, smooth muscle, and vaso-motor responses being influential of affect and postural attitude, including their relationships and responses to autonomic and neuroendocrine functions; and finally, (e) teleceptors embedded within the head and directly responsive for orientation of head position, such that paired relationships between eyes, auditory ears, and olfactory nostrils can more precisely envelop their coordinates in order to apprehend a sense of direction and proximity for more distant stimuli in the outside and/or virtual-imagined world. Taken together, orientation in space and in relationship to gravity is an essential function for any living organism for its continued survival. For the animal kingdom, all perception and sensation takes place within a background of some form of muscular activity. And though people are perhaps impervious, habituated, or unaware of the background influence as humans, all muscular activity is most strictly predicated and shaped by the incessant influence of gravity. 97 While the sensory systems outlined above all co-contribute as responses to the control of movement and body orientation in space, Feldenkrais had stated that the vestibular apparatus is ‘the co-ordinating chef d’ orchestre’…it coordinates all sensory impulses that influence muscular tone and attitudes…(and while) we are not necessarily aware of the special relation of the body to space and orientation, the vestibular apparatus takes a definite part in every single perception. (Feldenkrais, 1949/2005, p. 79) Feldenkrais knew that vestibular dysfunction was contributory to sensory distortion for appreciating the visual size and weight of objects, including the image people make of themselves and their own body weight. He recognized that loss of balance occurs during episodic dizziness because one side of the body is sensed as lighter and that righting toward a false vertical axis becomes inappropriately skewed. In vestibular-based dis-coherence and disequilibrium, pallor, nausea and vomiting are common, and upon lesser threshold, alterations in breathing and cardiovascular pulse are in direct relationship to modulatory influences of the vestibular system onto autonomic and vegetative responses. Finally, he recognized that sensorymotor-vegetative perceptual sets are co-conditioned, co-associated, and inseparable as unified sensory impressions in experience. “The whole situation may therefore be reinstated by either of the three elements of a set, or as a total reaction” (Feldenkrais, 1949/2005, p. 82). Consequently, Feldenkrais postulated and proposed a linkage of vestibular function to the body pattern of anxiety. Most generally, the body pattern of fear-avoidance and pain becomes most expressed by some form of muscular co-contraction being typified in terms of postural defense, under-support and withdrawal of limbs, or by some other literal or figurative behavior of "holding back." Feldenkrais observed instinctual patterns across the animal kingdom in response to danger or perceived threat such that flexor activation first occurs via an initial protective lowering of head 98 and curling of trunk, with corresponding inhibition of spinal extensors. This action is immediately contrasted and counter-reciprocated by strong-antagonistic activation of extensors via the mechanism of tensile stretch reflexes, being most typically posterior to gravity, as a precursor to fight, or upon more effectively impacting the ground during flee or flight. Feldenkrais also recognized a remarkably similar unconditioned reaction of newborn human infants in response to withdraw of support and falling. As an astute observer of human behavior and development, he was able to distinguish innately instinctual aspects of universally biological and physiological responses as contrasted from otherwise independently learned individual life experience or early pre-conditioning. The sudden acceleration which accompanies loss of support, disequilibrium, and falling is most likely and intrinsically first detected by the inner ear vestibular-otolith receptors, with strong immediate activation of the vestibular branch of the vestibulocochlear nerve (cranial nerve VIII). Co-opted and intricately interconnected with the cochlear branch of the eighth cranial nerve and thereby diffusive of loud noises being associated, both pathways (as transmissive of unconditioned physical threats) are conducive toward inciting strong impulses diffusing into medullary nuclei involving excitation of the vagus nerve (cranial nerve X); thereby affecting a sudden reflexive diaphragmatic disturbance of halting the breath with corresponding rhythmic disturbance in the cardiac region – both contributory toward being sensed as anxiety. Early and developmentally, Feldenkrais thereby concluded that “the first experience of anxiety is therefore connected with a stimulation of the vestibular branch of the VIIIth cranial nerve” (Feldenkrais, 1949/2005, p. 85). Furthermore, “the fear of falling elicits the first inhibition of the antigravity muscles, and that anxiety is associated with this process” (p. 89). “All other fears and sensations of anxiety syndrome are therefore conditioned” (p. 87). 99 Examined further, the acquired body pattern of anxiety could also serve as a possible contributory co-determinant and explanatory factor involved in how the development and maintenance of chronic nonspecific low back pain (CNSLBP) might occur and become sustained or inadvertently learned over time: This pattern of flexor contraction is reinstated every time the individual reverts to passive protection of himself when lacking the means, or doubting his power, of active resistance. The extensors or antigravity muscles are perforce partially inhibited…(Yet), the muscular contraction being voluntarily controllable, creates a feeling of power and of control over sensations and emotions…and (a sense of) passive safety is brought about by flexor contraction and extensor inhibition….In the long run, this becomes habitual and remains unnoticed. The whole character is, however, affected. The partially inhibited extensors become (stretch) weak, the hip joint flexes and the head leans forward…The (preferred and tonically finessed) pattern of reflective erect standing is (therefore) disrupted…The antigravity mechanisms are at work without break. Like all fatigued nervous functions, they are initially overactive; hence the tonic contraction and string-like texture of the antigravity extensors. (Feldenkrais, 1949/2005, pp. 92-93) Well before fully developing and implementing his application methods, and well ahead of his time, Moshe Feldenkrais had hinted at harnessing the power of neuroplasticity long before "plasticity" itself was to become an appreciative concept emerging through modern neurosciences. Feldenkrais believed the human condition could be improved toward the systematic unlearning of faulty behavioral and postural patterns at any age by stating that the outstanding quality of the human conscious innervations seems to be a unique capacity to form new nervous paths, associations, and regroupings of interconnections. Those made while the pyramidal (motor control) tract is growing are the most stable, but even these are more labile than in other animals. (Feldenkrais, 1949/2005, p. 149) Flash forward to the 21st century, the very recent article by Mast et al. (2014) again surmises a role for spatial cognition, body representation, and affective processes as representative outcomes of vestibular information beyond that of ocular reflexes and control of posture; these statements by Mast et al. are remarkably consistent with some of the early functional prognostications between body movement, behavior, and affect that were originally made and postulated by Feldenkrais as early as 1949: 100 Disequilibrium is a stressor indicating an unintended mismatch between frames of reference. This is where the interface between balance and affective processes—both phylogenetically old mechanisms—comes into play. Disequilibrium and falls are a threat to the organism that triggers an affective response. In real life, it is possible that the body’s immediate and fast reflex loops precede the affective response such as when we miss a step on the stairs without actually falling. We start to feel the increase in heart beat just after having successfully avoided a fall. The existence of this vestibulo-affective interaction will unfold to its maximum when immediate correction of posture is impeded. Affect and body motion are interconnected, and future research will be needed to explore the underlying mechanisms. (Mast et al., 2014, p. 20) It is important to note that the experimental arm of the current intervention study (VRB3 + FM) did not perform traditional "vestibular rehabilitation" or balancing exercises, but rather highlighted the corresponding positon of the vestibular apparatus’ representative spatial location within the skull through a combination of visually projected coordinates and haptic self-touch techniques (see Figures 31 and 32). It also used a succession series of Feldenkrais®-based movements throughout the study to differentiate head and eye movement, but only in secondary response to more-proximal pelvis-hip initiation of movement. As a contrasting research design-based response to the traditional core-stabilization model of motor control (specifying mainly the TrA and LM trunk musculature), it is worth suggesting that the semi-circular canals of each inner ear - being fixated and compartmentalized within each temporal bone - are quite likely (and with good reason) to be the most fixated and stable of all structures within the entire human body. That is to say that "gyrating the position of the gyrator itself" would certainly result in less reliable and unpredictable "mixed-signals" with regard to control of dynamic posture orientation and head in space. While it’s anatomical preposition can be considered to be a highly fixed-variable in itself, awareness of its dynamic involvement in motor control, body schema, and personal affect is indeed quite multivariate and yet skeletally dependent. 101 Current Status of the Feldenkrais Method® and the Proposed Interventions “We act in accordance with our self-image.” So states Moshe Feldenkrais (1972/1990, p. 3) in his book: Awareness Through Movement. He furthermore stated: The behavior of human beings is firmly based on the self-image they have made for themselves. Accordingly, if one wishes to change one’s behavior, it will be necessary to change this image. What is self-image? I would argue that it is a body image; namely, it is the shape and relationship of the bodily parts, which means the spatial and temporal relationships, as well as the kinesthetic feelings. Included with this are feelings and emotions and one’s thoughts. All of these form an integrated whole. (Feldenkrais as cited in Beringer, 2010, p. 3) A biographical account of Moshe Feldenkrais and his life can attest to his integration of scientific disciplines, systemic inquiries, practical applications, innovations, and international teachings that would later become disseminated and known as The Feldenkrais Method ® worldwide. Moshé Pinhas Feldenkrais (May 6, 1904 – July 1, 1984) was born in Slavuta, in the present-day Ukrainian Republic. Feldenkrais received his Bar Mitzvah, completed two years of high school, and received an education in the Hebrew language and Zionist philosophy. Subsequent to World War I (WWI), in 1918, Feldenkrais left by himself on a six-month journey to Palestine where he worked as a laborer doing construction until 1923 when he returned to high school to earn a diploma. While attending school he made a living by tutoring and teaching selfdefense. After graduating in 1925, he worked for the British survey office as a cartographer. In 1930, Feldenkrais went to Paris to attend university. He graduated in 1933 with specialties in mechanical systems and electrical engineering from the École des Travaux publics de Paris. Thereafter, he worked as a research assistant under Frédéric Joliot-Curie at the Radium Institute, while studying for his Ingénieur-Docteur degree at the Sorbonne. Feldenkrais later married Yona Rubenstein, a pediatrician, in 1938 wherein he became further intrigued to observe child 102 development. From 1939-1940, he worked under Paul Langevin doing research on magnetics and ultra-sound. At the advent of World War II (WWII), Feldenkrais escaped to England in 1940, just as the Germans arrived in Paris. Becoming commissioned as a nuclear physicist and scientific officer in the British Admiralty, he conducted anti-submarine sonar research in Scotland from 1940-1945, while also teaching Judo and self-defense classes to his military ranks, which led to his 1942 publication manual of Practical Unarmed Combat, and Higher Judo in 1949. Feldenkrais began working with himself to deal with knee troubles that had recurred during his escape from France, and while walking on submarine decks. Feldenkrais gave a series of lectures about his new ideas, began to teach experimental classes, and work privately with some colleagues. In 1946 Feldenkrais left the Admiralty, and moved to London. He published his first book on his Method, Body and Mature Behavior in 1949. During his London period he also directly studied the work of other somatics and consciousness pioneers including George Gurdjieff, F. M. Alexander, and neuro-ophthalmologist, vision method trainer, William Bates... (Reese, 2015) In 1951, Feldenkrais returned to Israel to direct the Israeli Army Department of Electronics. This era marked his difficult transition from a securely-known, distinguished physicist and research scientist to pioneering his own work and discoveries about the human condition into greater practicum, and out of obscurity. “Around 1954 he moved permanently to Tel Aviv and, for the first time, made his living solely by teaching his Method” (Reese, 2015). In the late 1950s through the mid-1960s, awareness of his methods grew, and Feldenkrais presented his work through lectures, presentations, and interactive workshops throughout Europe and North America. His discoveries caught the attention of cross-cultural anthropologist, Margaret Mead, who after observing his demonstrations being congruent with cybernetics and systems theory, had declared at a televised news feature event that “The Feldenkrais Method is the most sophisticated and effective method I have seen for the prevention and reversal of 103 deterioration and function.” A photograph of her meeting Feldenkrais, together with neuroscientist Karl Pribram, is captured in Figure 8. Figure 8. Telecast Interview of Moshe Feldenkrais. (a) Karl Pribram, and (b)Margaret Mead. Moshe Feldenkrais (seated at right), with (a) Karl Pribram (left and kneeling), and (b) Margaret Mead (center and left). Images courtesy of International Feldenkrais Federation (IFF). Throughout the 1960s, 1970s, and into the 1980s, Feldenkrais presented public workshops on Awareness Through Movement® and gave public demonstrations and private sessions of Functional Integration® throughout Europe and in North America, including a program for human potential trainers conducted at the famed Esalen Institute in Big Sur, California in 1972. Within this time, he also began to enroll and train teachers in the method so they could further develop and present the work to others. He trained the first group of 13 Israeli and European teachers in the method from 1969–1971, in Tel Aviv. Over the course of four summers from 1975–1978, he trained 65 teachers in San Francisco Bay area at Lone Mountain College under the auspices of the Humanistic Psychology Institute. In 1980, 235 students began his four-year summer teacher-training course at Hampshire College in Amherst, Massachusetts. After becoming ill from onset of stroke-related illness in the fall of 1981, and after teaching only two of the planned four summers, he returned to his original studio at Alexander Yanai Street in Tel Aviv, and had continued to mentor trainees to complete and carry forward his future programs, but had otherwise stopped teaching publicly. He died on July 1, 1984. Photo-captured 104 images to both demonstrate and commemorate his last formal training program are depicted in Figure 9. Figure 9. Moshe Feldenkrais Teaching at Amherst, MA, circa 1980. Images courtesy of International Feldenkrais Federation (IFF). Toward the end of his prolific life, over 1000 of his audio sessions at roughly 45 minutes each – and each a creative and fruitful exploration on its own – had been originally recorded during live presentations in Hebrew. Much of this most creative work has only recently been transcribed into English, French, and German through the mutual cooperation and efforts of The International Feldenkrais Federation (IFF), and The Feldenkrais Guild® of North America (FGNA). Differences Exemplified through Feldenkrais Method® Features of Application A predominating feature about Feldenkrais Method® applications is that there is much more happening beneath the surface of techniques for touch and movement than simply moving stuff around. Karl Pribram, M.D., Neuroscientist, Stanford University Professor, Winner of the 2000 Havel Prize in Neuroscience, and co-developer of the Holonomic Model of Brain processing theory is quoted to have said: “Feldenkrais is not just pushing muscles around, but changing things in the brain itself.” The Feldenkrais Method® of somatic education seeks to link novel and discriminative sensory–motor—informational learning experiences (perception through action and quality of attention to novel spatial-temporal configurations of embodiment 105 supplemented by imagery) with neuro-plastic changes in the brain in conjunction with optimal use of self through sensing the core of entire skeleton in the performance of everyday generalizable life tasks in a functionally applicable and reproducible context. In referencing "the essential unity of mind and body," Feldenkrais believed them to be "one and the same," as two sides of the same coin, to which as an objective reality, they are not just somehow related, but rather, “an inseparable whole while functioning. A brain without a body could not think” (Feldenkrais as cited in Beringer, 2010, p. 28). In practicum, Feldenkrais never dealt with the affected part or articulation of the body before first bringing about an improvement in the head-neck relationship, in grounding at an expanded base of support while in supine, back-lying repose, and in service of breathing. These in turn could not be achieved without factoring involvement for spine and thorax configurations, and nearly pre-requisite to this, enabling a series of adjustive corresponding and differential explorations to discover and select for improving the relationships between pelvis and abdomen. While Feldenkrais acknowledged that “some improvement in tension can be achieved through muscular awareness alone,” he added that “beyond that, no improvement will (likely) be carried over into normal life unless people increase awareness of the (entire) skeleton and its orientation.” Elaborating much more specifically, and as a central premise re-discovered subsequent to the current study’s initial intervention design, he stated that: Here the most difficult joints are the hip joints. Awareness of the location and function of these joints is (virtually) non-existent in Western cultures, as compared with that of people who sit on the ground and not on chairs. The chair sitter is almost without exception completely out of place when locating the hip joints. Moreover, chair sitters incorrectly use their legs as if they were articulated at imaginary points in the body image and not where they actually are. (Feldenkrais as cited in Beringer, 2010, p. 36) Feldenkrais Trainer, Richard Corbeil, of Seattle-Eastside Feldenkrais Teacher ® ® Trainings, Kirkland, Washington, United States of America (USA), had stated at an in-house 106 advanced training (in 2002) that the unique intent quality of a Feldenkrais® Practitioner’s touch was “to construct and project an image.” It is this primary feature of clarity of contact and linkage to function, and in such manner that both practitioner and client "co-construct an image" of how they both correlate and contribute toward each other’s actions in the real time present moment, that differentiates Feldenkrais Functional Integration from most other manual, ® ® manipulation, and other "bodywork" therapies. Of which, isolated structural entities and disordered biomechanical tissue categories most predominate, but not their composite functions as linked to whole person in simultaneous skeletal arrangement to their presenting environment. Each Feldenkrais® lesson is structured around a particular function. Feldenkrais defined a function as movements with a definite purpose such as walking, bending, turning and so on. He did not deal with movements around a certain area or a certain part of the body, but rather with the function and its components...we are not dealing with movements, but rather with improving the organization of the function. (Shelhav & Golomb, 2003, p. I) Feldenkrais emphasized that change in behavior, as applied to function, did not simply mean substituting or replacing one mode of acting with another, which would amount to nothing more than static (1st order), or non-systemic change. Instead, he suggested a mode of approach that could develop a larger scale ‘2nd order’ systemic change; a fundamental change in the dynamic arrangement, organization, and order of process within the function. Since each function is made up of many components, and since any one component may be used in various functions, improvement in the components of one function can bring about improvement in other components that participate in other functions.... [Furthermore,] there are no Awareness Through Movement® (ATM ) lessons which do not deal, in one way or another, with these primal elements of ‘equilibrium’...having to do with the coordinated distribution of weight being coordinated throughout the skeleton. (Shelhav & Golomb, 2003, p. 45) ® Such delicacy of functional balance while moving indeed requires a listening and attentive quality of both tactile and kinesthetic awareness. 107 In this model, if a quality of touch or movement – despite its small magnitude, its attentive slowness of pacing and listening, and its inquisitively gentle nature -- should, in whole or in part, inadvertently trigger a pain signaling output from the client, then the quality of the activity itself is also concurrently modulated, altered, and adapted to differentiate the activity in some other way that re-creates a new distribution of attention and a new context for curious open-ended exploration in facilitating an alternate possibility in sense, feeling, or action. This can take the form of re-orienting the person into a constructive or novel rest position with respect to reducing the influence of gravity and of enhancing internal-external support; re-directing attentional sets by approaching and comparing the movement from a remote distal vs. local proximal perspective, providing strategic placement of external positional supports (props, pillows, foam rollers), or by offering internal images that could functionally contextualize the activity toward something purposeful or personally relevant; and even through the practitioner altering his or her own body arrangement within the subject’s peri-personal space, so as to facilitate a social-perceptual condition for enhancing a more informed quality of support and movement. Through richly interactive comparisons and contrasts, new movement variations and novel sensory explorations become much more important than the usual and customary adherences to blind repetition. Thus, non-reproduction of symptoms and a process for learning to move without pain can often be strategically selected for accomplishing at least an initial approximation for the particular pain-avoidant task that yet remains necessarily desired, but from a much more varied, newly coordinated, and internalized perspective as to how. Feldenkrais acknowledged and considered that there are two major roads for changing a person’s behavior – either through the psyche or the body. However, he believed that real change 108 required a more integrative process, such that both the body and the psyche could be changed simultaneously. Otherwise, the change will last only as long as a person can veritably maintain partial awareness in not reverting or relapsing back into spontaneous habitual patterns. In noting that thoughts and feelings are fleeting and abstractive in nature, he believed that approaching mental and physical unity through the body was more reliable and simpler because muscular expression is more concrete, more reproducibly tangible, and easier to locate. Again, in recognizing mind-body unity as an indivisible gestalt (and in contrast to mind-body separateness or dualism), Feldenkrais remarked that: the state of the cortex is directly observable on the body’s periphery by these configurations of posture and muscular tonus. A change in the central nervous system always means a change in these configurations. Each, as we have pointed out, is the other side of the same coin. (Feldenkrais as cited in Beringer, 2010, p. 24) Thus, somatic education approaches, like The Feldenkrais Method®, incorporating visualhaptic imagery with kinesthetic feedback through movement are yet another composite inroad toward the cultivation of aggregation of emergent therapeutic neuroplasticity models, which aim to improve function and develop long-term potentiation through immersive sensory-motor experience, changing dimensions and perceptions of surface contact, and a continuous platform for body learning. Feldenkrais' theory is that cognitive thinking, emotions, and feeling, sensory perception, and especially the qualitative organization of movement, are all closely interrelated and influential of each other as a unified system. In sum, “The Feldenkrais Method® aims to improve people's quality of movement, their overall physical function, and their general wellbeing by increasing students' awareness of themselves and by expanding their movement repertoire” (Claire, 2006, p. 76). Education about “the reduction of pain and the elimination of biomechanically unsound movement habits is often an important part of this process” (Knaster, 1996, p. 233). Some criticisms of Feldenkrais® 109 movements from participants interviewed through qualitative research indeed has indicated that "some ambivalence about the method was expressed, especially regarding the difficulty to continue the exercises at home" (Ohman, Aström, & Malmgren-Olsson, 2011). By nature of its extensive sequential diversity and variety, this is understandable. The Feldenkrais Method® in Research A review of case utilization and prior field studies for implementing The Feldenkrais Method® (FM) for chronic back pain - and for pain management in general - has been consolidated by James Stephens, PT, Ph.D., while he served as Research Chair for The Feldenkrais Guild® of North America's 2010 Research Meeting: Pain management: Case studies describing the resolution of chronic back pain following the failure of other methods to ameliorate the problems had been published by Lake and Panarello-Black. A retrospective study of 34 patients using FM as an adjunct to treatment in a chronic pain management clinic showed that FM helped to reduce the pain and improve function and still was used independently by patients 2 years’ post-discharge. Dennenberg showed decreased pain and increased functional mobility using FM as a component of treatment for 15 pain patients. The primary result of this study was to show that there were changes in the pattern of health locus of control in patients participating in FM. A study using a group ATM® intervention with five fibromyalgia patients showed significant decrease in pain and improved posture, gait, sleep, and body awareness. Lake showed changes in posture in patients with chronic back pain following FM. Chinn et al showed improvements in functional reach in symptomatic subjects. Ideberg showed significant change in pelvic rotation and pelvic obliquity during rapid walking in 10 patients with back pain compared to normal controls, following a series of Functional Integration lessons. Narula showed decreased pain and improved function, including improved biomechanical efficiency, measured by motion analysis, in a sit-tostand transfer from a chair, in several people with rheumatoid arthritis following 6 weeks of ATM® lessons. (Stephens, 2012) More recently, however, in a region of the world well known for prolific contributions to clinical research across many disciplines, a national review board did not bring good news. In 2015, the Australian Government's Department of Health published the results of a review of alternative therapies that sought to determine if any were suitable for being covered by health insurance. The Feldenkrais Method® (FM) was among one of 17 alternative therapies being 110 evaluated. Among 10 published systematic reviews (SR) and only three randomized controlled trials (RCT) involving only three clinical conditions (neck and shoulder pain, anxiety and low back pain, and fall risk for older adults) and a total of 178 participants, it was determined through systematic review panels that “The effectiveness of Feldenkrais® for the improvement of health outcomes in people with any clinical condition is uncertain” (Baggoley, 2015). Overall, it was concluded that available evidence remains limited by insufficient statistical power due to few numbers of qualified RCTs, and of small sample sizes. “Future research, if conducted, should focus on rigorous, well-designed RCTs that assess the effectiveness of the Feldenkrais Method® in improving health outcomes in specific patient populations” (Baggoley, 2015). A concurrent, perhaps more updated, systematic review by Hillier and Worley (2015) conversely identified 13 single, randomized-controlled studies that were able to report statistically significant, positive benefits compared to modest control interventions for areas involving neck pain vs. neck comfort after single sessions: Decreased effort of upper body torso/limb discomfort after group classes; improved balance in people with MS after eight FM sessions; improved body image parameters in people with eating disorders after a nine-hour FM course; had a reduction in nocturnal bruxism in young children after 10-week course of FM lessons; and improved dexterity in healthy young adults after a single session of FM class. Seven of the 20 studies failed to show any superior positive effects of FM compared to other comparison modalities. The authors advocated that clinicians and professionals may promote the use of FM in populations interested in more efficient and comfortable physical performance, for increasing self-efficacy, and for improving balance in older people. The study’s recommendations also concurred for implications in future research by stating that (a) best practice designs are needed 111 to further higher quality research trials, by (b) comparing FM to other modalities in the form of well-designed RCTs, and that (c) the mechanisms of effect underlying the purported benefits of the Feldenkrais Method® also need to be investigated (Hillier & Worley 2015). Applying the Proposed Intervention against Core Stabilization Feldenkrais® trainer, Frank Wildman, has been quoted from his workshop, The Brain as Core of Strength and Stability, stating that: The brain doesn’t think in terms of muscles or muscle groups (as if dissected out from one end-point to another), it rather images and selects the necessary and particular fibers from a variety of muscle groupings in relation to whole body proportion references in order to accomplish the perceived demands of a task. (Wildman, 2009) Feldenkrais® practitioner/physical therapist, intervention advisor, and colleague, Gordon Browne, of Seattle-Bellevue, Washington, USA, has authored A Manual Therapist’s Guide to Movement: Teaching Motor Skills to the Orthopedic Patient (Browne, 2006a). Although he cited the work of Hodges (University of Queensland, Australia) as “a great start in rethinking the whole approach to low back pain, we need to expand the concept of inter-segmental stabilization to include the chest and thoracic spine, the feet and knees, and most especially the hip joints” (Browne, 2006a). He further stated: It is my suspicion that this arthrokinematic system (the transversus abdominus and multifidus muscle groups selected to function in isolation as primary intersegmental spinal stabilizers) has corollaries elsewhere throughout the body. If the lumbar spine is to be stable, then the pelvis needs to be stable on the femurs, the tibia needs to be stable on the femur, the tibia needs to be stable on the talus, and the foot needs to be stable on the floor. (Browne, 2006a, p. 233) Indeed, recent trends regarding the importance of "regional interdependence" becoming applicable to everyday physical therapy practice can finally be cited to support this view (Reiman, Weisbach, & Glynn, 2009; Sueki, Cleland, & Wainner, 2013). As stated earlier, "regional interdependence" can be understood in terms of new clinical reasoning models, 112 indicating that “seemingly unrelated impairments in a remote anatomical region may contribute to, or be associated with, the patient’s primary presenting complaint” (Wainner et al., 2007, p. 658), most notably citing hip joint relationships to low back problems (Cibulka, Sinacore, Cromer, & Delitto, 1998; Porter & Wilkinson, 1997; Reiman et al., 2009). Furthermore, exercise effects and neuroscience correlational studies are now confirming a more systemic and dynamic view by citing some added importance for enriching the variability and not just the routine for neuro-musculo-skeletal functions in rehabilitation – and also for everyday fitness lifestyle habituations in stating that “it is not isolated physical activity itself that is ‘good for the brain’…but rather physically skillful activity in the context of (variant) cognitive challenges” that matters the most (Fabel & Kemperman, 2008; Kleim et al., 2007), and especially as people experience sensory-motor learning situations rich in complexity and novelty that could presumably benefit from more new neuronal and synaptic connections. The overall hint here is that there are no key stabilizer muscles per se, nor is there strong rationale for exacting a precisely repeated invariant routine for real world contingencies in selecting for optimal adaptive stability. Any set of muscles can serve a stability function to some degree of proportionate representation contingent on task. The common denominators that remain constants are the constraints of the skeleton itself, and the coordinative structures of the brain that organize them. Inherent to the design of the intact skeleton are certain densities and proportions that optimize a support into action strategy, and from which varied conditions necessary for the transmission of biomechanical stresses can be more effectively selected, constrained, and predictably controlled. In particular, when compared to the lesser predictive neuromuscular contractile or "soft tissue" muscular elements (from which increased variability necessitates a 113 greater synergistic requirement demand for resolution and selection of necessarily regionally inter-dependent relationships), there is also the corresponding added demand for necessarily invoking a much more vast array of contrasting and potentially antagonistic functions; being less efficiently contributory to the desired or predicted function. Indeed, of all the body tissues and/or body systems that can be purported to have built-in constraints against excessive motor control variances and degrees of freedom (and therefore, greater intrinsic predictability), it is perhaps the inner scaffolding and intrinsic lattice-work within the inherent design and distribution, and relative solidity of the intact human skeleton that most probably ranks # 1. Thus, in these models, the facilitation of an isolated muscle contraction to a localized region or part is of minimal importance for motor control compared to developing an optimal use for the intrinsic design of the skeleton, as guided by enhanced visual imagery, sensory dexterity, and movement patterns guided by global synergistic intent. Therefore, there are functional corollaries and considerations to cite variables that must necessarily exist well beyond the concept of "core specific" muscle entrainment of Transverse Abdominis (TrA) or Lumbar Mutifidus (LM). Perhaps deploying the use of an entire, realistic, and life-proportioned model skeleton, plus, a therapeutic construct for its functional representation as a whole system to thereby facilitate and invoke a more proportionate distribution of whole person movement using The Feldenkrais Method®, is yet another treatment and training option to consider? Evolving Practice, New Visual-Haptic Techniques: The Origin of VR Bones™ Through over 20 years of clinical applications experience, my certification in The Feldenkrais Method® has continually influenced and developed a large portion of my practice approach, my personal-professional-sensory development, and a continually evolving treatment 114 philosophy long since it was first awarded in 1996. It now continues to grow further to influence my development of conducting current research in support of somatically-inferred discoveries and to disseminate their clinical application into new areas upon validation. Even today, the method still remains in the periphery of mainstream practice, and a unique and relatively rare certification among a wide diversity of rehabilitation professionals, and from which, its applications continue to differentiate. At the start of the Millennium (2000-2002), I attempted to construct a new and novel treatment approach known as Vestibular Ergonomics™. This was accomplished using Vestibular Apparatus location, perceptual awareness, and interactive anatomical modeling as a 3-dimensinal imagery concept for employing “internal ergonomics” to navigate the seated work desk (computer-keyboard-visual display interface) environment as an intervention combined with "whole-self" Feldenkrais movements to remedy the effects of sitting posture fatigue and for the ® treatment of repetitive strain problems being attributable to work-related musculoskeletal disorders involving symptomatology throughout regions of the head, neck, upper back, arms, and hands. It was intended to be compared against a routine prescriptive approach to simple manual stretching techniques at the computer desk, as described in a popular self-help book at the time, all while controlling for psychological distress variables being accounted for via stratified random assignment of participating volunteers who scored excessively high thresholds on the Occupational Stress Inventory-Revised (OSI-R). This project was soon deemed as impractical at the time, given the constraints of restricted access to employees, worksite office environments, and corporations during an extended period of heightened national security after the events of 9/11/2001, and the project was subsequently abandoned. 115 From 2003-2005, I considered an alternative research question and preliminary design for re-training maladaptive and habituated skeletal-postural configurations of chest-thorax and intercostal diaphragm to purportedly enhance the effectiveness of respiratory biofeedback and resonant frequency training using Feldenkrais directed movements; and for examining their ® comparative effects on rapidly producing and maintaining an optimal measure of sympathetic – parasympathetic balance as measured by increasing Heart Rate Variability (HRV) for a population of subjects with generalized anxiety disorder and episodic panic attack. This too proved impractical, as both the population and the technique – though interesting – did not constitute my usual scope of everyday clinical practice. From 2005-2006, I received a set of anatomical loose bones for product demonstration and advocating for educational product re-sale at Feldenkrais Conferences via courtesy of their manufacturer: Pacific Research, Inc. (also well-known within the orthopedic internship-surgical apprenticeship trade as ‘The Sawbones Bone Factory’) of Vashon Island, WA. Nothing else was ® known about what would come of it. See Figure 10. (a) (b) Figure 10. Discovery and Implementation of Proportionate Skeletal Models. (a) Newly acquired full-scale and proportionate skeletal models placed alongside the full scale model articulated skeleton, circa 2005; and (b) a personal depiction of prelude and foreshadowing as to how Feldenkrias Method® based functional applications and body schema-based multi-sensory perceptions would later lead to the development of Virtual Reality Bones™ 116 Uncovering a Universal Deficiency in the Sensory-Perceptual Acuity of Background Body Schema via a Corresponding Normative Comparison to Anatomical Reference Models Since acquiring a set of anatomical bones in 2005-2006, the following sets of exploratory dialog for directive attention and introspective touch have now become a routine commonplace inquiry during the daily course of my clinic’s practice. For each new patient with LBP, I quote: With the precision of two finger widths at your fingertips...and somewhere between your base ribs and your knees.... can you pinpoint the exact location of where your own your legs attach to your own body - via your torso? These would be the ‘ball and sockets’ or ‘hip joints’ that support your own weight during everyday standing balance and walking. Within this usual course of practice, I discovered through "practice-based evidence" that there was almost universal misrepresentation of discernment for locating, with precision and anatomical accuracy, the exact location of an articular axis that was truly representative for depicting "actual hip joints'" articular surface among all patients, but mostly in LBP patients when queried. Figure 11 demonstrates a tri-plane axial proximation of location for true hipsocket axis, where head of femur meets concave cup of acetabulum. Figure 11. Tri-Plane Location of Hip Socket Axis. Pin-pointing a Tri-plane anatomical location of hip socket axis where femoral head meets acetabulum for the transmission of antigravity and ground reaction forces during all biped locomotion. 117 Looks of confusion and curious bewilderment would typically accompany this open inquiry as well, as they lay on back with legs supported over foam roller cylinders in a proportionate size arrangement – typically six-inch diameter behind knees and three-inch diameter behind ankles to simulate a prospective natural sway dimension for upright standing – and while relieving pressure from the all too common muscular excesses in lumbar para-spinal extensor tone that occurs when most LBP patients lay supine and with their legs fully extended without supports. Figures 12 and 13 depict a typical exploration of this all too common perceptual and/or clinical phenomenon. 118 (a) (b) (c) (d) Figure 12. Testing for Hip Socket Anatomical Axis Perceptual Acuity. (a) Opening inquiry in supine supported position; then, (b) order of sequence depicting the most common perceptual mislocalization response site (estimated to occur in at least 85% of all respondents) for the anatomical mislocalization of hip sockets; followed by (c) the initial deployment of skeletal model femur bones as an intervention /correction for improving the informed accuracy of visualtactile acuity; and finally, (d) a more anatomically accurate perception for re-localization of "true" hip sockets. An additional sample distribution of collected photos being indicative of observable disparities for what could almost be routinely considered as a universal phenomenon in association with the continuing prevalence and persistence of low back pain affecting greater than 80% of the population as well as other neurological or musculoskeletal disorders which adversely affect everyday locomotor functions. These are captured and depicted in Figure 13. Through daily practice and observational testing of patients with a variety of conditions, but 119 especially for CNSLBP, I have found that the inability to accurately discern and locate actual hip-socket axes as "true spatial coordinates" depictive of anatomically accurate representations for the gauging of perceptual acuity for one’s own body schema is almost always impaired. Again, a montage sampling of these disparities being associated for recurrent low back pain problems are depicted in Figure 13. Figure 13. Sample Distribution of Disparities in Accurate Perceptual Localization of "Anatomical Hip Sockets" Commonly associated for Patients Presenting with Recurrent or Persistent Low Back Pain Problems. Upon continuing comparative observation, I also discovered that a variant trend was emerging through a sub-population of individuals with more longstanding and/or functionally incapacitating levels of severity in chronic pain. Generally, I found that the more chronic the problem, the less accurate and more distant (the more distorted? the more asymmetric?) is the 120 perception of anatomically accurate localization for regional body parts, especially deep articular joints. Known cases are depicted in Figure 14. Figure 14. Clinical Distortion of Body Schema Acuity for Hip Sockets in Chronic Low Back Pain. Photo-captured responses indicating "clinical distortion of body schema acuity" and correspondent asymmetric disparity for accurately locating their "best perceptual estimate" for visually-tactilely "pinpointing" the exact location of anatomical hip joints (ball and socket axes) as sampled from two patients with long standing chronic nonspecific low back pain (CNSLBP) and with duration greater than five years. *I speculate that chronicity of pain conditions over time and their corresponding movement dysfunctions and aberrations of motor control (as maladaptive and developed over time) may be positively correlated to greater discrepancies of not being able to discern an appropriate level of internal body reference that is comparatively consistent with "more discernable body schema acuity" of which the latter can become more consistently predictable through the use of high-quality anatomical skeletal models serving as ideal frames of reference for re-discovering correspondent proportionally and more accurate actuality of body part representation that is closer to ‘true’- and most particularly in cases for highlighting and comparing the position(s) of lesser accessible deep articular joints. Usual treatment interventions involving the use of thermal or electrical modalities, antiinflammatory NSAIDs and/or other pain meds, relaxation or medical massage, passive spinal manipulation, traction or decompression therapy, range of motion exercises and back strengthening, targeted flexibility and core stability exercises, or any other approach based on exercise performance impairment, structural lesion or malformation, or any other ascribed medical diagnosis or tissue-based-pathology; all seemed especially irrelevant and invalid for approaching what could now be better classified as a perceptual misrepresentation phenomenon. 121 Thus, as a necessarily valid (yet elusive) prerequisite to any future treatment plan, something of a "perceptual clarification intervention" was needed to more accurately discern and locate a more refined, tangible, and experiential basis for more accurately accessing internal references for body awareness/body image/body schema, and concurrently matching each patient’s phenomenological experience and exploration toward re-referencing and clarifying a more predictable and accessible anatomical benchmark to be found in the external world. Somewhere between intuitive awareness and an unintended discovery over time, the fully intact life-sized model skeleton occupying the out of the way corner of my treatment room, together with a scattering of life-sized anatomical skeletal and vestibular models laying around the office space would readily lend themselves toward a new framework of perceptual intervention in helping to bridge the gap. “Virtual Reality Hip Replacements” via Routine Deployment of a Life-Sized Femur Model As has been originally developed and now routinely implemented in my practice, the treatment, which most often occurs the first day for a majority of patients, and without regard or relevance to their particular diagnosis (unless a clear contra-indication was evident), and usually within the latter 15 minutes of their initial intake appointment and correlative assessment, my team now routinely administers the use of "visual-tactile applications" of anatomical skeleton models. Their use is not to explain pathology (as is usually the case for display models), but rather to convey a clearer depiction for sensory awareness by converging a geometric visualspatial sense for a outlining a more accurate location of femoral head-acetabular /articular joint surfaces at the central axis point relationship for either L or R or both hips. This is accomplished by (a) having an extra, separate, full length femur bone being shown adjacent (as a replica or replication) to the one actually attached to a full-scaled, life sized 122 neighboring skeleton propped -up on its own display stand in treatment-exam room. A sense of regional inter-relationship for geometric proportion was conveyed in the sense of stating that “the length of Left to Right widths between hip socket locations roughly equates with the same width of our eyes,” by first indicating the vicinity of the pelvis hip sockets as a "local-focal picture" and then upon stating that “this is the big picture,” I would place my eyeglasses over the face landmarks of “an astute-looking professor skeleton” and then immediately fold the ear pieces and transfer the width of the frame’s corners to match the width of the spaces between L and R acetabulum, within and above the supra-pubic symphysis and anterior to the inner ring (pectineal line) of the inner ilia of the pelvis. This process is procedurally displayed in Figure 15. Figure 15. Demonstration of Corresponding Dimensional Relationships between Width of Hip Socket Joint Axes and Width of Temporal Bones via the Visual Aid of Head of Femur Models and Eyeglass Frames. 123 I would then (b) ask the patient to place the width of their index and middle fingers (both between and inclusive of each hand) to match the precise width of their own imagined eyeglasses or sunglasses and to direct this "converged fingertips" arrangement to a space overlying the anterior creases slightly above the groin to either side of the ilia to furthermore imagine an overlay of where the new location for a precisely located actual hip socket might be. I would then (c) implement use of the anatomical femur model as a "virtual limb segment" to offer the patient a “virtual reality hip replacement” procedure, but that I would also need their help. There would be no need for anesthesia and no loss of blood. I would ask them to "imagine that they are a comic book super hero with laser beam finger tips that can aptly and precisely direct a vivid and clear beam virtually anywhere to illuminate and detect a location." I would then proceed to place the model femur to overlay the femoral head directly over the anterior hip crease with the distal end roughly corresponding caudally in the direction of the knee. The patient was then asked to conform their hand around the spherical shape of the bone model’s femoral head while I simultaneously apprehended the roundness of the patient’s heel at the calcaneus with a similar conforming hand shape hold and stating that “the size of your heel is likely roughly the size of your hip socket location”…to which I would then direct them to converge all five finger-tips together to form a convergent laser beam to direct it in an A-P (anterior-posterior/front to back) line of direction focalized over the hip crease while I covertly slipped out the femur bone to set it aside for the moment...I would then state that “I too am a comic book super hero with laser beam fingertips and that I was going to assist them in solidifying their new hip replacement." Most patients would invariably reveal a limitation in the direction of internal rotation and a corresponding tendency to hold both the pelvis and the trochanter muscle insertions (i.e., piriformis, obturator externus) in a retracted, extensor biased direction, which I would demonstrate by holding the model femur in a similar orientation to mimic a tactilely observed external rotation, holding pattern tonal bias. Attempts to "directly stretch" a shortened muscle were deemed futile, whereas instead a known Feldenkrais® principle of "going with and supporting the direction that is already happening" and connecting a vector line in the direction of head and spine in an upwardly projected manual diagonal was performed to convey a sense of 124 relationship that can be felt to informationally occur in more rapid and conducive fashion, with a corresponding shift of head position also typically happening upon the application and completion of this manually applied maneuver. I would then apply a vector line of manual support from a lateral to medial direction to proximate the joint surfaces further – while at the same time re-directing the patient to attend to a precise vertical A-P direction of their (imagined) laser beam in a direction perpendicular to my (imagined) laser beam and stating that... “where our two beams meet inside you – the point of reference for coordinates crossing over each other, as a space between four street corners, a cross-point; this is the new location for your new hip socket!?! ......note that it is not behind you …it is not in front of you…it is actually within you at this level” …as I rotate the patient’s actual femur inward and outward along it’s longitudinal axis…before moving to their foot and transmitting a vector line through the "core of their hip socket" as informed from yet another sensory directional perspective; but this time from a caudal, weight –bear direction -- upward. Not to be construed with energetic mechanisms for "laying on of hands" these novel haptic sense augmentation techniques for "laying on of bones" are roughly demonstrated in Figure 16. ________________________________________________________________________ Figure 16. Demonstrating Hip Axis Socket/VR Hip Replacement. Note width of eyeglasses’ temple frames roughly corresponds to width between L and R hip socket axes (i.e., between L and R acetabular cup and L and R femoral head interface). Note also that though these newly created VR Hip Replacement techniques require some prior element of manipulation and manual therapy skill being developed on the part of the therapist, these novel techniques are also otherwise quite differentiated from usual high-velocity, low- 125 amplitude thrust techniques being more commonly instituted through chiropractic and osteopathy. This is due to the fact that the patient is actively involved in producing his or her own outcome through co-opting his or her own application of haptic self-touch/self-contact. And by his or her directing his or her own visual-tactile imagery toward manifesting a task-oriented quality of intrinsic attention, he or she can thereby continue to participate in continued ownership of his or her own experience. In other words, patients are not just being passively manipulated and/or objectively corrected. I then took the experience into sitting, standing, and walking to explore and generalize the experience into other contexts of postural orientation. These features, and their correspondence toward improving the quality of posture and movement during sitting and gait, are demonstrated in Figure 17. (a) (b) Figure 17. Generalizing the effects of VR Hip Replacement/Body Schema Acuity Training on Qualities of Comparative Arrangement to be Experienced and Contrasted (and therefore learned) during Daily Routine Activities. (a) sitting, and (b) for standing and walking. In both situations, the first picture represents the mis-localization of proximal hip axis condition (perception of false axis), whereas the second vs. third pictures represent the re-localization of hip socket ("trueaxis") as being correspondent to the improved sensory acuity perceptual condition that was instituted post-treatment. 126 On continued observation and implementation, I discovered that just by clarifying a patient’s visual-tactile mental representation (body schemata) for improving accuracy for "pinpointing" the exact and central location of the articular joint surfaces representing the mislocated verbal schemata for "hip socket," that qualitative movement improvements also occurred for: ● A-P pelvic tilts/pelvic rocks becoming more accessible and symmetrical; ● Pelvis Floor Kegel’s Exercises demonstrating more immediate and effective calibration of control for various magnitudes of contraction gradient with greater precision, dexterity, and reversibility; ● Markedly reduced para-spinal muscle guarding/unnecessary parasitic/dysponetic muscle tone during rest in both supine lying and in upright sitting/standing positions; ● Spontaneously maintained improvements in frontal and sagittal plane postural symmetries in standing alignment for head, neck, shoulders, and inferior costal margin landmarks, as well for lower quadrant landmarks at pelvis iliac crest, trochanters of hips, and fibular heads at both knees bilaterally; ● Improved extensibility of active/passive motions for deep lateral hip rotators (i.e., piriformis) to permit legs/feet to cross midline upon internal rotation as well as to reextend laterally; and ● Improvements in walking revealed through spontaneous and un-prompted demonstrations of more harmonious, deviation-diminished qualities of gait, concurrent with more efficient and sustained ground reaction support becoming more evenly reciprocated by a corresponding smoothness of contralateral swing phase. This quality became more pronounced while having patients co-conduct their own enhanced sensory referencing 127 from the new perspective of their new hip socket as a new anatomical reference via their own application of visual-haptic self-touch techniques while walking. Furthermore, when patients were asked to revert back their attention to imagine walking from the previous erroneous location of hip axis schemata and re-placing their hands there, (usually at least three inches superior and lateral from actual hip axis location, thereby proximating a vicinity just below or laterally behind the prominent ASIS bony landmark; being the most and tangible and superficial aspect of pelvis’ usual surface anatomy), it was rediscovered that the faulty gait pattern returned, as did an almost immediate corresponding report of return of familiar pain symptom recurrence throughout the low back and buttock regions that were originally assessed at intake for at least one-half of the patients encountered. They were then asked to re-reference the new location again with ever-increasing sense of acuity-accuracy, which again corresponded with freer movement dexterity and restoration of more fluid gait quality as well as symptom improvement with regard to decreased stiffness, improved steadiness, or decreased pain. These initial sessions were most commonly supplemented with both in-clinic and athome Feldenkrais®-based Awareness Through Movement® audio recorded programs being specifically chosen and selected from my audio file CD library to best simulate and to reinforce learning conditions for re-discovering a truer hip axis within the context of developmental patterns and/or within functional movement sequence progressions, and as tailored for each individual patient. As applied to the current low back pain study, Figure 18 further demonstrates effects of interactive "virtual reality hip replacements" as a method for body schema acuity training in a patient with long standing chronic nonspecific low back pain (CNSLBP) and with a severity of duration at greater than five years. While she had been originally referred for 128 participation in the current study, her initial appointment was not able to be scheduled until two weeks after the IRB-approved enrollment period and subsequent study closure. She nonetheless received the same benefits as study participants in terms of content and delivery of treatment involvement as a regular patient. a (a) (b) (c) Figure 18. Case Example: Virtual Reality Hip Replacement in Severe Chronic Low Back Pain. Effects of "virtual reality hip replacements," as a method for more accurate body schema acuity training. Beginning with the usual baseline inquiry condition (a) the patient perceives a marked disparity for accurately localizing the anatomical location their own hip socket axes; and (b) continuing with the patient undergoing a therapist facilitated transition for re-localization of "more accurate" Hip Socket Acuity location as a novel treatment and training intervention using "life-sized femur bone models," along with facilitating awareness for improving a closer proximation of "perceived space-distance" in support of longitudinally arranged lumbar vertebrae anatomical segment models placed above waistline - as a corresponding connection of virtual props. At re-test, (c) the result is a more accurate anatomical hip axis location from the patient’s newfound perspective. *Accurate representation for hip socket axes and improved body schema acuity (upon post-treatment) most commonly results in (a) improved symptom modulation for decreased awareness of usual pain; (b) improved dexterity for posture control and symmetry of sitting and standing; and (c) improved quality of gait being exhibited through harmonious, more proportionate and reciprocating qualities of support and movement occurring between "stance" absorption and pre-propulsion "swing" phases during post-intervention observation of their repeated gait cycle. 129 Finally, it should be disclosed that the discovery of this phenomenon and the applied use of anatomical femur models onto patients with a variety of conditions was a process that had first occurred over many years in my clinic (since at least 2006), and within the usual context of my daily clinical practice, and much in advance of designing conditions for any formalized type of applied clinical research program. Like the Feldenkrais Method® itself, there was really no accurate, peer-reviewed consensus of verbal nomenclature to accurately describe what I was doing at the time. Only later was it discovered from the literature review in preparation for publication of my original pilot study (2012-2013), and for the internal review approval process in preparation for the current dissertation study (2015) that a conceptual language and verbal construct for "mis-localization of tactile acuity for patients with chronic LBP" was to be found in the literature via the associated and published works of Benedict Martin Wand et al. of The School of Physiotherapy, The University of Notre Dame Australia, Fremantle, Western Australia, Australia (Wand, Catley, Luomajoki, et al., 2014, Wand, Di Pietro, George, & O'Connell 2010; Wand, Keeves, et al., 2013). Continuing Improvements for "Anatomical and Perceptual Reframing of Background Body Schema" through the clarification of Skeletal Support Mechanisms that occur during daily movement interactions between "Pelvis-Hips Opposite Head" While the visible results achieved through the previously described "virtual reality hip replacements" interventions have been seen to be most often reliable, predictable, and reproducible among nearly all cases treated, I nonetheless commonly advise patients not to become over-invested in "hip joint localization" as an end-all or be-all panacea. There are still hidden insights throughout the rest of the body that have yet to be uncovered. I, in fact, refer to any new-found shift of body schema or change of body awareness become self-directed to such 130 an extent that it can maintain itself "as only a loose assembly" and not as something to be rigidly held or overly rehearsed, so as to become over-habituated as a narrowly applied prescriptive dictate. Instead, a looser directive is facilitated toward adopting an otherwise unaccustomed mindset for inherent flexibility. This quality is especially important to better permit the generalization and integration of new skills into the naturalistic environment of ever-changing systemic conditions, and to more fluidly provide a background context from which adapt to possible unexpected perturbations or exposures that could likely require an alternative version of the originally adopted response. Consequently, as a second follow-up to evolving the treatment progression, and as most commonly applied for patients with low back or knee problems, patients are next told that: The hip replacement you received last time is not the whole story...we didn’t reveal the whole truth last time...and perhaps the anatomical femur itself is not really the "top of leg"... In fact, your "top of leg" may actually be somewhere else...Look here, if I placed my model skeleton on all-fours and place a saddle on his back - like a riding horse - then his pelvis...this area here just behind the saddle - would actually become the true "top of his legs" and he could gallop away! But now look, stand him up here on his vertical post and put some dress slacks and a nice belt on him, make him feel human again...then this same area suddenly gets transformed to becoming his waistline again...where the top of his pelvis and sacrum meet-up upon his low back vertebrae... That’s the area of what doctors and nearly everyone else calls out as being "the small of the back" - but maybe that’s not altogether true...maybe...like a drumstick on a chicken...by its original true support function and primal form...it’s really the "top of the leg." We just don’t know it yet! Figure 19 demonstrates the new question for phenomenological and somatic inquiry to enable patients - and nearly everyone - to re-consider their accustomed unawareness and high likelihood of maintaining their vaguely under-explored sense of ongoing perceptual discontinuity occurring between hips, pelvis, and low back: 131 Figure 19. Perceptual Discontinuity between Hips, Pelvis, and Low Back’s Spine Column. Ordinarily conceptualized as separate anatomical regions is remedied to question via the aid of using comparative developmental anatomy imagery, plus the handy prop of a horse saddle. Common low back pain (LBP) mostly occurs in regions largely associated to the areas of the body that are most often linguistically referred to as "the small of the back" from a common or colloquial perspective. Physical exams will frequently elicit point tenderness throughout L5S1 paraspinal regions upon palpation/inspection, and with particular targeting of pain or provocation becoming more directly reproduced when examiners more directly pinpoint the posterior sacral sulcus anatomical landmark of concavity overlying the posterior S-I joint; otherwise being characteristically referred to as "the divot portion" of the sacral-iliac joint. This region is pictured in Figure 20. 132 Figure 20. The Superficial Region of the Posterior S-I Joint (SIJ). Frequently indexed as a source of pain emanating from what is also colloquially described as the "small of the back" in many common outpatient clinical settings. Many clinicians – perhaps mistakenly - will immediately equate this finding as a positive diagnostic feature for classifying the many varieties of "sacral-iliac dysfunction" and will then cite this region as a primary cause and progenitor of low back pain. However, such pre-directed and overly-determinant or conclusive qualities of inspection/palpation may also inadvertently and erroneously re-sensitize the supposed region at fault, and thereby reinforce a localized somatic marker or body reference neurotag that furthermore implies and sensitizes a continued fragile or dysfunctional state (e.g., think "subluxation") becoming co-conditioned, co-associated and thereby ever-perceptually linked to the "S-I joint posterior" as a pervasive and selfreinforcing topic of conversation and self-report upon all future chronic pain treatment consults with all future clinicians. 133 As an antidote to such erroneous - but not uncommon - construct development; and as a further application for the anatomical and perceptual reframing of background body schema as a novel and educational treatment intervention, I was able to devise an "inner-ilia hidden bridge" session that could convert the perceived notion of "L4, L5, S-I joint" as "small of back" into a more robust perception of instead re-depicting it as "top of leg" by creatively using the visualtactile frameworks of anatomical models being representative of "skeletal density imagery" in conjunction with added demonstrative implications for linking the entire region to a new and more supportive notion for function being further re-constructed and outlined in terms of "skeletal density proportionality." These inherent and perceptually tangible characteristics of the human skeleton can be consolidated and described in terms of bearing a "skeletal contiguity model." A primary and most interesting feature of a skeletal contiguity model - being based on antigravity support mechaisms and skeletal density trabecular pathways becoming biologically emergent through interaction in the real physical and gravitational world - is its correspondingly vivid capacity to translate itself into human conscious awareness; more specificially, through a tangibley apprehensible visual-tactile, spatial-conceptual, and multi-sensory array of dimensions for accessing an experiencial inroad into the inner representative and movement-based perceptual worlds. The component regions of highest bone density, considered as robust skeletal properties within pelvis and head are vividly displayed in Figures 21 and 22. These features, in turn, become part and parcel of every succeeding anatomical imagery interactive intervention program having to do with the conceptual visualization and the experiential transmission of skeletal-gravitational forces, while being concurrently guided through Feldenkrais Method ® 134 based Awareness Through Movement lessons, and most particularly within the design ® framework of the prospective and current low back pain study. (a) (b) Figure 21. Components of Highest Bone Density within Pelvis and Head. Regions of highest bone density within (a) pelvis, and (b) head as revealed by highlighted anatomical illustration and whiteness contrast being classically indicative of greater structural density upon radiographic viewing of x-ray films. As can be seen, femoral cortical bone leading into inner concavity of acetabulum (ball and socket) and inner ilia pectineal line/ring to anterior ilio-sacral (I-S) joints (as "inner bridges") and lateral pedicles of spine vertebrae (corresponding columns in parallel) all ascend to upwardly converge through the temporal bones of the skull as the corresponding structures of highest radiographic bone density. 135 (a) (b) (c) Figure 22. Anatomical Outlines of Areas of Highest Bone Density. These areas (darkened and embellished by clay overlay being placed over them) represent the densest anatomical and structural landmarks of the humnan skeleton. More specifically, they are visually highlighted as areas that are known to represeent the regions of greatest concentration for skeletal vs. trabecular "highest bone desnisity" on both sides of the body, and for the transmission vs. absorbtion of gravitational forces between "hips-pelvis opposite head" as a partial model for skeletal continuity: (a) Right inner-ilia/pectineal ridge line aspect of R pelvis, (b) Right temporal bone ridge within skull (and exisitng just inferior to R temporal lobe of brain), and (c) an exposed depiction of Right vestibular apparatus with three semicircular canals, as would otherwise be completely encased within the boney labyrynth of the Right temporal bone. Another discovered feature is that the temporal bone-vestibular complex and the innerilia pelvis ridge and hip axis, and even the ischial tuberosity or sit bone happen to line-up vertically and directly over each other when the skeleton is observed in neutral upright placement. This schematic is revealed in Figure 23. 136 The Temporal Bone and the Inner-ilia Hip Axis Happen to Overlie Each Other... (b) (a) (c) Figure 23. Vertical Contiguity of Pelvis-Hips Opposite Head. The depiction outlines vertical contiguity and proportionate lateral dimensions of (a) Pelvis-Hips opposite to (b) corresponding Dimensions of Head, and (c) revelation of the only recently discovered ‘Proportionality of Thirds™’ model. © 2006 by Tim Sobie. 137 Furthermore, and with regard to the Feldenkrais Method® principle of emphasizing for proportionate skeletal movement, I had discovered through laboratory measurement upon my skeletal models that the bone density length dimensions of the inner-ilia bridge within the pelvis are roughly 2/3 larger in size than the temporal bone’s corresponding width dimension encasing each inner ear at 1/3 the size. By having measured these dimensions in the life-sized model skeleton, it was found that each inner ilia ridge contour curve measured out at 9 cm as was compared to the length of each temporal bone becoming measured out at 3 cm width. I then proposed and postulated a new "Proportionality of Thirds Model™" to predict and guide the outcome of contextualizing and improving upon a whole range of existing Feldenkrais® movements, and most particularly, to permit an algorithmic basis for recall and more tangible guidance during their actual instruction and performance. These scaled features are seen in both Figures 23 and 24. 138 (a) (b) (c) Figure 24. Tape Measure Rendering of Densest Bone Regions leading to the Operationalizing of "The Proportionality of Thirds Model™" in Feldenkrais Movements. Given that (1) proximal iniation of movement, and (2) proportionality of action in synergisitc distribution from "larger denser powerful areas" to "smaller effector distal zones" are cited as key features for the efficiency of movement in usual Feldenkrais Method® Practice, I discovered anatomical geometric corelates to action function in (a) mesureing the distance from hip-socket acetabular central axis to inner-illiosacral joint interface to measure 9 cm, and (b) the dimensions of termporal bone’s analogous ridge containing the vestibular apparatus sensory end-organ to measre one-third of that at 3 cm. Therefore, all Feldenkrais sessions delivered and directed toward the current study manifested a directive for (c) 2/3 initiation of range for movement occuring first from pelvis-hips complex then contrasted by 1/3 counter-balancing actions and dimensions occuring at head and neck. A compilation of interventional steps involved in clarifying and converting a more robust functional relationship between corresponding hip axes and ilia-sacral joints as "tops of legs" through "skeletal density imagery" and through "skeletal density proportionality" procedures (in lieu of emphasizing focal muscle attachments and other soft tissue or joint compression "language reference abstractions" being erroneously allocated to "small of back") is demonstrated in Figures 25, 26, 27, and 28; and more extensively through some other image 139 compilations being routinely shown, exhibited, and demonstrated to patients in my clinic in Appendix U. Figure 25. Photo Demonstration of the Proprietary Manual Therapy Approach. Relectively outlining and anatomically re-framing a constructed image for depicting the "core robustness" of the Right anterior ilio-sacral joint via congruently leveraging hip joint up through pelvis by way of contra-diagonal directions from right foot placement press-loading and directing a contralateal vector line of movement being projected toward the direction of left temporal bone at skull base. Life-sized skeletal models and shortened ski poles (e.g., children’s trek poles) are again used to faciltate visual-haptic imagination and active intention into constructed action. The procedure is typically initiated first to the side of diminished suppport so as to create a larger sense of difference upon recomparison within the "legs extended" rest position, and as a manner of constructive self-assessment for comparative body schema and body scan prior to working with the contralateral side. 140 Figure 26. Photo-Captured Demonstration of "Self-Applied Visual-Haptic Self-Touch." Indexing "right inner-ilia-as-new top of leg" for the projection of skeletal contiguity to route itself diagonally upward and leftward through skeletal trabecular pathways to left-side of top of head (i.e., through direction of contralateral left temporal bone as a corresponding structure of highest bone density). A life-sized hemi-pelvis skeletal model is also deployed for retaining and generalizing the new-found sense of pelvis-spine continuity, but this time being applied within the actual functional context of standing fully upright, and again re-applying hand placements for "visual-haptic self-touch" as a continuity of treatment session to extend from clinic environment to daily life. 141 (a) (b) Figure 27. Anatomical Re-framing of "Core Robustness": Pelvis-Hips opposite Head. (a) Photo profiles again demonstrating the proprietary manual therapy approach for relectively outlining and anatomically re-framing a constructed image for depicting the "core robustness" of the Left anterior ilio-sacral joint within the conext of leveraging hip through pelvis through contradiagonal directions from Left foot placment, and directing a contralateal vector line of movement being re-directed this time toward the direction of Right temporal bone at skull base in conjunction with self-rehearsal of repeated acuity actions via the instructive and concurrent deployment of visual-haptic self-touch techniques to refine the alignment vector for greater discernrment of path line for skeletal transmission; (b) Repeated actions are then re-explored via the assitance of self-applied ‘haptic self touch’ along with other visual imagery (e.g., the post-it note with letter "X" marks the spot of an imaginary laser beam being directed upward) conducting in straight line fashion from base of Left knee - via tibial plateau at distal femur through "top of leg" at I-S joint, then onward and upward through contralateral chain of pedicles at Right side of thoracic spine, and eventualizting through Right skull base, through temporal bone/vestibular channels, and ultimately emitting through an imaginary cattle horn spike at Right side of top of skull/top of head. Conversely, the imaginary laser beam can also be imagined and directed in reverse direction – from cranial top to caudal knee base – as an alternative exploration of novelly perceived alignment relationships through "core regions" of highest bone density within the full continuity of the skelteal chain. * This image set additionally reverals the 2/3:1/3 spatial-temporal relationship of pelvis/hip position rotation to Right (at 2/3 of motion capacity) being opposite to that of adding-in the auxiliary movement of counter-rotating the head position to Left ( i.e., being counterbalanced at roughly 1/3 of motion capacity). 142 (a) (b) Figure 28. Anatomical and perceptual reframing of "Top of Leg" during Standing Trunk Rotation. (a) Photo capture rendering of "anatomical and perceptual reframing" for backgroundforeground body schema acuity training being applied to inner-ilia pectineal line ("inner ridge" as "inner bridge") and linking detectable spatial-temporal relationships through self-contact to functions of standing, standing orientation variations, standing balance, and elemental qualities for pre-gait during movement, as corroborated via visual assistance of full-scale anatomical skeleton; (b) demonstration of correlational proportions for top of inner ilia ridge corresponding 143 to outer dimensions of right foot during initial stance phase of gait; and base of inner ilia/pelvis floor corresponding to dimensions for instep of ball of foot (which also happens to match for width dimensions of intracranial lateral temporal bone) during terminal stance/pre-swing phase of gait. Envisioning for a new top of leg thereby occurs via a similar detailed process of anatomical sample modeling and using "visual-haptic self-touch" interchangeably between the deployment of external anatomical models and comparing and contrasting them against inner anatomical references within the perceptual acuity aspects of envisioned whole self. However, at this juncture – and in anticipation of developing a contrasting and evolving treatment model against "core muscular stabilization" – the "core of the entire skeleton" is now instead referenced through outlining a pathway of boney trabeculae ascending to "link-up" from core of hip axes (e.g., from the initial previous session), continuing onward through the anterior ilio-sacral "inner ridge/inner bridge" of pelvis, and then onward and upward (both laterally and contralaterally) through thoracic pedicles, rapidly culminating toward contra vs. ipsilateral temporal bones in skull (e.g., vestibular component); and finally, dissipating through top of head as "a comprehensive frame of continuing body reference" for all succeeding sessions, and as a total model for linking a broader perspective toward the enhancement and perception of total skeletal contiguity, while also being inclusive of all its primary related and regulatory components. The Lateral Chain of Distribution through Pedicle Densities and Costal-Thoracic Expansion Bone tissue is generally laid out between two types of concentrations for the distribution of mechanical forces vs. the fluidity of self-maintenance for retaining its regenerative properties. The two structural-functional divisions are described as cortical vs. cancellous. Cortical bone is the outer cortex or thick outer shell aspect of compact bone and enables the primary skeletal functions of shielding for protection to the softer internal structures of the body, and for 144 longitudinal and mechanical support throughout the entire body as well as for affording mechanical levers for movement. At the other continuum, cancellous bone, is also referred to as trabecular bone or spongy bone because its continuity is more variable and its structural compactness is overall less dense. The inner distribution of "structural latticework" known as the trabeculae within cancellous bone are arranged, aligned, and concentrated according to the mechanical and gravitational load distribution that a boney tensegrity continually experiences. This has been most studied within long bones, such as the human femur en route to the femoral head, with added consideration that total hip replacement surgical arthroplasties are among the most common types of orthopedic procedures performed. By most accounts, the femur is the strongest bone in the body as well as being the longest bone in the body. As far as short bones are concerned, trabecular alignment has been most studied in the vertebral pedicle. For all spine vertebrae, the vertebral arch is formed by pedicles and laminae. The pedicles connect and "bridge-together" the anterior vertebral bodies (predominantly cancellous or spongey bone) to the posterior-lateral boney projection processes existing for muscle attachments (primarily cortical or compact bone). The two pedicles firmly extend and anchor from the sides each vertebral body. For purposes of an anatomical-perceptual reframing/cognitive educational intervention, they are best described as paired cylinder-like structures designed for bridging gaps and for fostering a design for structural continuity between front and back sides of body along a longitudinal arrangement of intrinsically robust stacked vertebrae. Most importantly, the inferior vertebral notches are of large size, and deeper than in any other region of the vertebral column. Finally, their medial cortical layers are thicker and denser than that of their lateral aspects; rendering them suitable for the insertion of "pedicle screws" for repair procedures in cases of 145 orthopedic spinal trauma. They can therefore be cognitively re-constructed, conceptualized, and reconsidered as a continuity of intrinsically robust (yet again, "hidden") structures manifesting in ascending and descending fashion. Finally, they are furthermore distinguished adjacently by the presence of superior facets on the posterior sides of corresponding vertebral bodies for providing a tangible landmark for adjacent articulation and foundational support for the heads of each rib and "for distinguishing each rib as a force-dissipating yard arm" at each corresponding thoracic vertebral level during reaching. By harnessing thoracic rib-ring and costal-vertebral mobilizations with expansive breathing patterns, and as an added theme for anatomical-perceptual reframing, thoracic pedicles are then used as the next linkage for bone density pathways en-route toward bridging the perceptual gaps between pelvis-hips opposite head. A directed implementation - again using anatomical skeletal models and corresponding imagery - is demonstrated in Figure 29. 146 (a) (b) (c) Figure 29. Anatomic Locations and Junctions for Thoracic Pedicles and Costo-Vertebral Joints. Anatomic locations and junctions for (a) imaging thoracic pedicles and costo-vertebral joints; (b) applying triplicates of anatomical rib models, informing about changing planes of facet motion orientaion in anatomical transition zones between L1 to T12; the corresponding chain of pedicles at each vertebral level, and harnessing the volumetric and laterality aspects of the lungs, and finally; (c) informing awareness from hand-placements and pnuematic costal expansion that the densest line of internal reference to transmit skeletal stresses through core of vertebrae is not anterior-front nor posterior-dorsal, but along the lateral-medial aspects of the spine coulumn. Location of Vestibular Apparatus Augments for a Sense of Visual Spatial Alignment Here, I also demonstrate and correlate anatomical imagery sensory re-referencing by exposing the tri-plane structure of the three loops that comprise the semi-circular canals (sccs) of the vestibular system’s apparatus by again employing the full scale skeleton model and an inner ear anatomical model together – and dimensionalizing an exploration of head position into three comparative directions in space – using the temporal bone’s (and the corresponding vestibular 147 organ’s) exacted location as a new point of internal reference for motor control. The anatomical model references for these explorations are depicted in Figure 30. Figure 30. Anatomical Models depicting Vestibular Apparatus. Particularly the semi-circular canals (sccs) within each inner ear, and as encased within each temporal bone deep in the skull. Each vestibular end-organ apparatus comprised of the three semi-circular canals (sccs), the utricle, and the saccule - though deeply encased within the skull - can nonetheless be perceptually accessed by implementing particular visual-haptic spatial coordinate locator techniques that lend themselves well to the treatment concept of anatomical and perceptual reframing for clarifying a background body schema, and for post-session improvements in cervical active range of motion. These techniques are profiled and described in Figure 31. 148 (a) (b) Figure 31. Visual-Haptic Projection Techniques for Cardinal Axis Coordinates of Vestibular Apparatus and their Anatomic Location. Visual-haptic projection is self-applied for locating both the central convergence of cardinal axis coordinates for movement and for encapsulating the positional-structural orientation of the vestibular apparatus end organ being deeply encased within each temporal bone on each side of the skull. (a) Locating and pinpointing the location position for right vestibular appratus inside skull by arranging perpendicularly placed ‘imagined laser beam’ coordinates between 2-index pointer fingers: [(1) R hand referencing finger beam is latearlly crossing midline from right ear-hole entrance, projecting horizontally and leftward in mid-frontal plane, and exiting through L ear hole; (2) L hand referencing finger beam is posteriorly projecting in anterior-posterior (A-P) direction from front of pupil at right eye and through to exit along mid-lateral saggital plane at "back of skull," but also behind the eye (i.e., from front to back); (3) The cross-point inside the skull of where the two beams meet together via their perpendicularly directed coordinates is the proximate location of the right inner ear’s vestibular end organ.] Here within this space the patient is manually and visually guided to imagine (1) a miniture ferris wheel, (2) a miniture merry-go-round carousel, and (3) a miniature tilted diagonal ferris wheel – all side by side and adjacent to each other. Each of these corresponds to one of the three semi-circular canals and all three are roughly correlated to correspond with the three primary cardinal anatomical planes commonly used for referncing the whole body; (b) Locating the angle of orientation in right vestibular apparatus for its facing diagonally outward by retaining the R index finger placement in R ear and connecting a single line of reference to match the direction of L index finger beam being placed to the inside corner of L eye – just adjacent to nose bridge. Co-incidentally the vestibular apparatus’ surrounding temporal bone density lies directly and vertically above the lines of trabecular density that connect vertically between corresponding 149 hip axis and inner ilia-sacral joint, as has been both previously described and pictured (see Figures 15 and 23), and conceptually re-framed via anatomical imagery techniques as combined with deep, para-visceral manual therapy for outlining along pectineal line/inner ring of pelvis and through to anterior Y-ligaments overlying ilia-sacral joint line (see Figures 25 and 27) - to be intrinsically referred to again as "new top of leg," and no longer truthfully depicted as "small of back," as remains the common colloquialism in traditional clinical exams and every-day thought. An added photo demonstration for locating the tri-plane positional coordinates and the three angles of orientation for the right vestibular apparatus, as dually shown in combination with the full-scale model skeleton and an anatomical diagram for outlining the directions of "spatial arrangement" for each semi-circular canal’s directional reference is again depicted in Figure 32. 150 Figure 32. Locating the Coordinates and Angles of Orientation within RIGHT Inner Ear. A demonstration of locating the coordinates and angles of orientation. Here, for right semi-circular canals in right inner ear/vestibular apparatus via the use of visual-haptic self-touch clarification techniques and the deployment of a full-scale anatomical skeletal model. Note again the corresponding spatial dimensions between pelvis-hips opposite head as both an internal and external frame of reference. "Pelvis and Hips" as a Tri-Plane Model for Center of Gravity and the Detection of Change in Center of Gravity Being Represented by "Vestibular Coordinates" as a Combined Dynamic and Unifying Movement Strategy to be used during a Simulated Martial Arts Task for achieving a Synergistic Spiral Quality of Efficient Action being applied to the function of Sit to Stand In general, the habituated and usual function of seated sitting exhibits a more selective tendency toward the convergence of the senses, especially for the combined visual-spatial and manual prehension and/or precision tasks of modern life, and in synergistic association with an activation pattern of muscular tone to concurrently select for a global internal rotation and flexor bias activity occurring at torso and limbs. 151 In contrast, moving from seated sitting to fully erect standing exhibits a greater tendency in selecting toward divergent expansion of the senses, and with greater concurrent rotational affordances for the selection of both global external rotation and for global extension synergies at body and limbs; and thereby, also opening greater opportunities in the directional schemata for re-referencing the global senses in a greater multitude of affordances for directional acuity. In chronic low back pain, a miscalculation of spatial-temporal relationships for the optimal coordination of smooth and efficient motor control between these two primary synergistic global patterns is seen to most often occur in the transition from sitting to standing, especially if either of the two end-state posture orientations has been sustained over a prolonged period of time. For which, the availability of synergistic control for inhibition of co-contracted side-bending movements, as accessory dimensions for translational movements during rotation, is also typically lacking. Interestingly, most traditional physical therapy clinics and fitness center-based exercise programs continue to utilize equipment designs and "core exercises," which emphasize cardinal movements being mostly limited, isolated, and constrained to occur only within a central midline sagittal plane of action. Yet, the curvilinear design features inherent to the densest frameworks of the human skeleton, occurring in and around a dimensional center of gravity, are anything but linear or singular plane. The primary components depictive of curvilinear skeletal action and for the control of movement plus adaptive recovery in a gravitational field are revealed in Figure 33. 152 Figure 33. Visual and Dimensional Relationship Correspondences between Pelvis Diagonals, Hip Complex, and Inner Ear. The diagonal pelvis orientation and posterior-lateral hip relationship to access anterior medial opening for full hip external rotation permits a ‘stacked quality’ of femoral extension and concurrent pelvis counter-rotation in conjunction with curvilinear semi-circular canals also being conducive for achieving the eventual function of maintaining fully upright and erect standing. These systems are also imperative for the continuous detection of change in center of gravity upon the thresholds of deviation for loss of caudal support and to permit the rapid selection of counter-actions and righting responses to prevent falling. Were these structures to be re-designed in linear fashion, dynamic recovery would not be possible upon crossing the threshold of a squared-off or right-angled hard surface boundary; being the point of no return. As an alternative, by re-linking the head through pelvis and by magnifying the effects of their respective curvilinear features over a correspondingly round sitting surface, the relationships between variable support and dynamic action for balance recovery can thereby be applied and explored into the rotation transitions of sitting into standing as is demonstrated in Figure 34 and Figure 35. 153 (a) (b) (c) Figure 34. Guided Self-Exploration and Treatment involving Side-Tilting of Head. This photocapture of guided self-exploration and treatment effectively demonstrates (a) Side-tilting of head and torso in frontal plane from left to right to determine anterior-lateral scc vestibular influences on counterbalancing a freedom for effective displacement of contralateral pelvis-hip; then (b) verifying the R hip as the restricted and less yielding side; the index fingers highlight for both the position coordinates and angle of orientation for the right ear’s scc’s and corroborte the same dimensions for R hip’s angle of inclination; before (c) self-organizing an interplay for pelvis-hip relationships in both preparation and simulated execution for perfrorming a "martial arts pointy star throw" via the transition from "saddle-straddle" sitting over bolster to spiraling up into standing. 154 (a) (b) (c) (d) Figure 35. Demonstration of Therapist guided Functional-Cognitive Manual Therapy (CMT) Maneuvers. (a) Lateral ear tilt and detecting for pelvis drop counter-response while sitting saddle-straddled over rolled cylinder pad to discern for unyielding side of limitation; (b) provision of tele-props visual tubes applied to corresponding side of hip limitation for R inner ear, noting especially the posterior-lateral to anterior-medial vector line for the R vestibular apparatus' angle of orientation also corresponds to the same angle of vector line for applying manual direction of (1) central pelvis rotation L, against (2) distal lever of femoral head displacing anteriorly to R; while again using (c) virtual imagery anatomical bones in conjunction contact; for (d) performing the simulated martial arts action of throwing an imagined "5-point Ninja Star Dagger" directly and horizontally behind oneself via the actions of torso rotation L from the vantage point of R axis hip. 155 In sum, and as a common outcome for nearly all ipsi-laterally and effectively applied Feldenkrais sessions, there occurs a unique and observable phenomenon indicative of "a ® hemispheric laterality bias" for improving the dimensional resting qualities of embodiment, as being revealed through lateralized variations in the volumetric distribution of resting muscle tone and/or corresponding length-width tension relationships. For example, most patients can immediately detect the sensation of their attended leg feeling much longer, and/or the extensor side of their back feeling much flatter on the corresponding, ipsilateral side of the applied therapy session by self-observing through their own "body contact scan" while in resting repose. As a frequent yet transient feature at post-session, these differences can also be seen by an external observer as is captured in Figure 36. 156 (a) (b) (c) Figure 36. Latearlity of Chest and Leg "Hemispheric differences" after FI® Sessions. These visual and somatically embodied phenomena (being likley representative of hemispheric differnces in motor control being revealed through differentiated laterality in resting postural tone states) are a common observational outcome of Feldenkrais sessions. Here demonstrated and photocaptured subsequent to (a) left hip session, (b) left diaphragm session, and (c) right ilia session. ® Yet, applying these changes and generalizing them into other functional contexts was not always easy for patients to reproduce when transitioning up from floor pad or off from treatment table from either a supine, side-lying or prone rest position, and then to sitting up, then to 157 standing upright, and finally, but most especially, for generalizing them into all of walking. In most cases, they would always notice some form of qualitative difference, but beyond the simple "awareness of difference," there were no real tangible tools from which to exemplify a continuing qualitative transfer for wholly representing the complete gait cycle in its entirety. Despite its wonderful visual affordances, it was rather challenging to get a life-sized skeleton on a display stand to effectively demonstrate a graceful and easy pattern for walking or sauntering across a treatment room. Something of more visual-spatial importance was still missing for visually conveying a more lasting impression of the profound lateral and contralateral (and supposedly hemispheric) differences that were occurring on the treatment table and somehow transferring them - with equal perceptual vividness - into the more varied dynamics and reciprocating complexities of human gait. Discovery of "Virtual Avatars" for Anatomical Re-framing, Skeletal Transmission, and Aligning Ground Reaction Force Vectors for the Enhancement of Verticality during Gait I had the good fortune of attending a technology and clinical update presentation conducted by Dr. Christopher M. Powers, PT, Ph.D. (USC.edu), entitled “Lower Quarter Biomechanics: New Research,” as part of the 2009 PTWA Fall Conference in Tacoma, Washington, in October, 2009. In it, he shared a video kinematic biomechanical analysis sampling of gait patterns being demonstrated by a student research subject using a Vicon™ Motion Capture System - side by side with a corresponding computer-animated reconstruction of her skeletal frame being outlined from foot to pelvis over a force plate using a Polygon Viewer™ program from which to determine and visually map-out the dimensions of her ground reaction forces during an entire cycle of gait. 158 While the emphasis of his demonstration display was to codify how the knee joint would decelerate upon the subject’s arrival toward the mid-stance phase of gait, my attention was otherwise immediately drawn to the dynamic summation of force vectors that were moving upward. These were all distinctly and consistently targeting most specifically through the inner ilia aspect of the pelvis into the "top of leg" anterior ilia-sacral robustness surface that I had been continually attempting to convey through the previously presented deep manual therapy tissue manipulation access points. Now it had become more visually depicted through vertical arrows and diagonal yellow lines that were effectively outlining a believable trajectory that could more easily "connect the dots," but only to convey the idea of a correspondent functional relationship between hip-pelvis opposite head (and/or vice-versa) during the human gait cycle in walking. However, also necessarily occurring through a visually-inferred, trabecular highway by having line vectors summate and visually depict a motion pathway occurring throughout the densest longitudinal and surface aspects of deep articular bone structures - inclusive of inferring ipsilateral and contralateral longitudinal pathways passing directly into the reference frames for each temporal bone – of which each paired vestibular apparatus functioning as a "hidden sense reference" is deeply encased and contained. He already had a few copies of a replicated compact disc (CD) file on hand, and I procured one for only $20 to put to immediate use upon return to clinic practice the next day. Visual depiction and description of Vicon™-inspired motion-capture virtual avatars, anatomical re-framings of body schema, and their functional relevance toward simulating a sense for the transmission of forces through skeletal density imagery pathways as occurs during the gait cycle are referenced in Figure 37 and in Figure 38. 159 (a) (b) (c) ________________________________________________________________________ Figure 37. Vicon™ Kinematic Videography Image Reconstruction for Initial Stance Phase of Gait. Vicon™ Kinematic Videography image reconstruction of (a) Right initial contact for stance phase during gait; (b) lateral, oblique and posterior views as "high point of the hip"; and (c) real time anterior-frontal view depicting ground-reaction force plate summation of vectors culminating contralaterally inward and upward to Left in the direction of Left inner ear temporal bone. These "avatar images" are presented and explained to patients in slow motion with instructions to ‘feel and imagine this happening within yourself’ prior to therapist-assisted "simulation of action" maneuver via haptic self-touch and corresponding Feldenkrais® movements. 160 (a) (b) (c) ________________________________________________________________________ Figure 38. Vicon™ Kinematic Videography Image Reconstruction for Terminal Stance Phase of Gait. Vicon™ Kinematic Videography image reconstruction of (a) Right instep terminal stance phase of gait as a phase transition into medial forefoot propulsion/pre-swing; (b) lateral, posterior, and anterior views of force plate antigravity summation vectors depicting a lesser magnitude due to forward momentum; and (c) real time anterior-frontal plane views depicting ground reaction vector summations culminating this time ipsilaterally and upwardly in the corresponding direction of Right temporal bone for alignment, stability, and control of Right mid-terminal stance as a platform from which to enable a successful contralateral Left swing phase. 161 Upon reviewing the three-dimensional Vicon™ Polygon Viewer™ assembled images on video displays for simulating the predictive summation of ground-reaction forces emanating upward through skeletal architecture during the transitional stance components of the human gait subject, it was found that I could also revert back to explore the same set of conditions again through corresponding life-sized skeletal models, thereby affording a sense of haptic touch experience to complement the visual simulation of kinematic pathways. Most interestingly, the vector summation magnitudes for ground reaction forces - being greater at initial contact stance and lesser at terminal stance phase of gait - were seen to correspondingly match both the direction of distribution and the thickness of articular surface directionality upon outlining a structural palpation/inspection survey of the ilio-sacral joint surface becoming revealed on the hemi-pelvis skeletal model. These anatomical skeletal framework-based visual-haptic awareness techniques, as accessories for more tangible patient-avatar interaction, are demonstrated and displayed here in Figure 39. 162 (a) (b) ________________________________________________________________________ Figure 39. Hemi-pelvis Model of Inner Ilia depicting Pathways for Contra-lateral vs. Ipsi-lateral Skeletal Transmission. These images convey corresponding displays for the transmission of ground reaction forces through skeletal density transmission pathways in reverse funnel direction from inner ilia-sacral joint upward-this time using right hemi-pelvis skeletal model via index finger and clay overlay to depict: (a) initial stance phase conducting contralaterally upward, and (b) terminal stance conducting ipsilaterally. Corresponding vertical alignment experiences for the instruction of skeletal transmission vectors becoming targeted longitudinally through the anatomical and regional pathways of highest bone density; for the overall enhancement of verticality and reciprocation of segmental counterbalance during gait was further achieved by co-opting varied combinations of skeletal simulators being applied through visual-haptic self-touch; and by imagining anticipatory responses being predictive of proportionate, anatomical trajectories within patients and colleagues who consented to be actual live action photography models during the coordinated demonstration of repeated gait cycles. These demonstrations are referenced in various ways – from both top-down and from bottom-up in Figure 40 and Figure 41. 163 (a) (b) Figure 40. Applications of Visual-Haptic Self-Touch Hand Placements for Simulating and Detecting the Anatomical Pathways of Skeletal Transmission during Gait. (a) Right initial stance phase of gait corresponds to imaginary longitudinal coordinates overlying the L temporal bone; and (b) right terminal stance corresponds to self-touch linkage from right I-S joint to R ipsilateral temporal bone. The middle pictures depict the use of ski poles to augment and highlight motion trajectories. 164 (a) (b) Figure 41. Demonstration of Skeletal Transmission Contact Points for the Simulation of Gait Function Form Ground-up. (a) The L initial stance phase transmitting toward R contralateral temporal bone, and (b) the L terminal stance phase of gait (the pre-propulsion phase) transmitting ipsilaterally toward the direction of L temporal bone. Again, the visual-haptic selftouch techniques as internal reference cues are self-applied to the service of anatomical awareness and perceptual re-framing. Epilogue: Non-Pathological Anatomical Imagery for Highlighting Areas of Highest Bone Density as a Contribution for Cognitive Reframing and Behavioral Adjustment As a composite development throughout, the re-depiction of inner anatomical imagery is also aggregated and operationalized into the common Feldenkrais® movement themes that involve or begin with supine knee bending for foot placement as a platform precursor for semibridging and leveraging of pelvis to roll diagonally opposite and superior laterally. In situations of CNSLBP wherein the lumbar-pelvic junction was deemed too heavy or too stiff or too painfully vulnerable to move, it was succeedingly found that the inclusion of cognitive-perceptual reframing for "new" intra-pelvic ilia-sacral joint being depicted as "top of leg" - in lieu of the more compartmentalized and diminishing nomenclature for "small of back" 165 that more spontaneous and productive actions could emerge in concurrence with decompressive tractioning of adjacent lumbar and thoracic spine segments – all further embellished by counterbalancing head position to rotate 1/3 of its distance – usually in opposite direction as if to fine-tune or reflexively mediate a more effective synergy for motor control. I have observed that this anatomical/categorical re-framing seems efficacious to create a more robust cognition that seems to correspond with a more confident quality of emergent sensory-motor coordination for improving standing alignment, pre-gait, and gait for dynamic stability in the relationship of depicting “an inner bridge in support of a more solid ‘top of leg’ connection to the spine" in lieu of referring to the region as “a small of back” or “S-I Joint Dysfunction” context, and this again seems to diminish the region of the sacral sulcus as a palpable sore spot, and furthermore serves to nullify any previous cognitive-emotional pre-occupation to "L4, L5, S1 as a problem area." In this, I found that indeed, from an anatomical cognitive reframing perspective, “there is no ‘low back’ anymore” nor is there a "central core" to invoke or protect only a "top of leg" and that after these maneuvers I would convey the impression that: “like the horned centaur –you are ½ human – ½ horse…with new robust top of legs.” Images used to convey and animate this impression are seen in Figure 42, and also for use of antlers in gait, in Figure 43. 166 Figure 42. Centaur-Human Avatar. This is modified to invoke greater robustness for "new" top of leg. 167 Figure 43. Use of Deer Antler for Augmented Sensory Reality. The deployment of an authentically weighted deer antler horn is used for augmenting a sense of core skeletal transmission throughout the body during gait - with projection emphasis of selecting contact placement over L versus R temporal bone correspondingly during initial and terminal stance phases of gait through right lower limb. In this, participants can likely detect the inclusion of antlers into their avatar identity for the anatomical reframing of usual background body schema. 168 These maneuvers have also been termed in my practice as “cognitive-imaginative lumbar-bypass surgery,” a virtual reality/cognitive re-attribution technique to de-emphasize the mistaken contribution of implicating unchangeable structural findings (i.e., naturalistic “degenerative discs” and ruminative normal variation as “subluxations”); factors known to be involved in maintaining and perpetuating fear-avoidance beliefs, and by maintaining an "overly localized" quality of attention as a pain focus becoming perceptually amplified toward an emergently constructed neurotag by continually referencing neuroplastic qualities of ruminative attention to areas between and around lumbar spine segments L1–L5. Components of the varied anatomical and perceptual reframing maneuvers, and how their imagery after-effects can translate and generalize into the contexts of refining for improved standing balance and gait have been well-demonstrated in previous figures. Other strategies for the cognitive reattribution of imagery robustness have included the use of comparative scaling. In this application, the distorted versus actual features for differentiating ‘articular surfaces’ at "small of back" can also provide a basis of "contrasting evidence." By clarifying through demonstrative and dimensional models that the ilio-sacral joint itself is the longest and largest singular continuity for a joint surface area to be found anywhere in the body, but that it is not ordinarily seen nor verified from an anterior or front-sided perspective is a real eye-opener. While many patients with low back pain can typically and easily pinpoint a "nickel or dime -sized region" at the sensitized SI joint posterior with amplified representation, a competing situation for a higher return on accurate perceptual investment is newly rendered to in Figure 44. An additional visual strategy makes use of radiographic artworks by Nick Veasey, wherein the lines and pathways of highest skeletal density are robustly depicted in skeletal 169 persons engaging a host of lively and vibrant life affirming activities. These are seen in Figure 45. Figure 44. Use of Imagery Robustness and Comparative Scaling for Cognitive Reframing. Rather than the usual and typical clinical practice of depicting size the posterior S-I joint surface erroneously and implicitly as the mere and fragile diameter of a dime or a quarter (i.e., small of back); the joint surface can instead be depicted more accurately - and robustly as expanding its landscape and contours threefold – to at least the size of well packed change purse or billfold. From the perspective of biological reserve, this is a new cognition they can truly bank on. 170 Figure 45. Active Anatomical Skeletal Density Imagery through the use of Radiographic Art. *Note: Radiographic photo art shared via coutesey of the public domain by its creator, technical artist Nick Veasey. Again, these novel schematic acuity relationships are generalized into broader functional contexts by highlighting their anatomical presence and their perceptual involvement for referencing space and supporting equilibrium during variations and/or perturbations encountered while sitting, standing, walking, as they apply to the general and reinforcing activities of everyday life. It is again important to again note that (a) the "manual therapy" manipulations using skeletal density imagery for anatomical and perceptual reframing, and (b) the Feldenkrais-based movement patterns that follow them are intended to facilitate and develop a concept for possibility – and not to treat or correct (and to possibly reinforce) a pathology or problem area – such as a "subluxation" or "unstable" segment, both of which may be nothing more than a 171 pedagogically misinformed, continuing social consensus, and a profession perpetuated, delusional iatrogenic construct that continues to reinforce notions for illness and dysfunction. In contrast, I aim to construct and implement carefully selected Feldenkrais® Movement experiments with particular action themes that are designed to directly target a fuller utilization of novel skeletal density properties, pathways, and congruent trajectories via the inclusion of interregional areas that have not yet likely been multi-modally explored and brought into novel awareness. This, in turn, can continually modify ongoing attentional and behavioral processes, again in a new way or new manner of being, and in ways that can also have the cognitivebehavioral effect of continuously challenging some deeply rooted and pre-existing beliefs about the quality of possibility for embodiment and diminishing the corresponding vulnerabilities to pain. When practiced with continued novelty and variation, these processes can likely result in the self-reinforcement and re-conditioning of newly compatible neural pathways, and new cognitive-perceptual dendritic assemblies that perhaps cultivate toward the physical re-allocation of newly proportioned cortical body maps; these become less compatible with selecting for previously conditioned central sensitization pathways that had otherwise become overly efficient for concretizing and amplifying the transmission of pain signaling. Thus, by re-allocating attention to and affording a continuing competing stimulus (Neural Darwinism) through skeletal density imagery and improved visual-tactile acuity for body schema via (a) the use of virtual reality bones™ as a perceptual concept, and (b) the use of Feldenkrais® Movements to operationalize them, then perhaps a selection against the usual conditioned "protective – avoidant" pain processing pathways (that seem to typically dominate the spectrum of usual cognition and constricted action in patients with longstanding chronic pain) may occur. 172 Consolidation and Synthesis of Initial Treatment Approach into The Acronym: (VRB3)™ As a compilation of broad and systemically related concepts seeking order, these intuitive, epistemological, somatic-based-phenomenological explorations derived from immersive inquiry and contemplative reflection throughout many years of clinical practice only later became conceptualized, revised, and consolidated as “Virtual Reality Bones™” upon the writing of this dissertation. Through this, they now have a more concrete context and branded identity from which to become more reproducible and communicable. The acronym symbol “(VRB3)™ ” further consolidates a more compact description of the methods used and coalesces the entirety of background activities that were actually implemented during Phase I in the experimental arm of the current study. It furthermore serves to operationalize and concretize a more reproducible method for facilitating an approach to “Body Schema Acuity Training” - the original, but conceptually vague and incomplete description for Phase I - as it was previously submitted prior to and during the review process. Prior incarnations of the Phase I component had included such broad and varied descriptions and titles as (a) Ideokinetic imagery, (b) Skeletal Density Imagery (SDI), (c) VisualTactile Acuity of Deep Articular Joints, and (d) a Skeletal Density-Vestibular Concept for Body Schema; then, somehow relating all these components to tie into the accomplished performance of five. “Proportioned Feldenkrais® Movements.” Given the space constraints of future publication, title searches, editing, and especially the word limit requirements for most abstracts, something inspirational and creative - yet also practical - had to be done to unify toward a more concise description. Virtual Reality Bones™ and its corresponding acronym (VRB3)™ also afforded an added, but unexpected visual literacy symbolic component in that "VRB" phonetically reads as 173 "VERB," as an implication for action and movement; and then, culminating into the three-part final acronym “(VRB3)” to integrate, consolidate, and summarize the systematic and broad-based conceptual ideas inherent to Phase I of the experimental intervention, outlining them into three basic simpler ideas. 1. (VRB1) = Virtual Reality Bones: indicates the primary use of "true to scale" anatomical models superimposed & immersed as both augmented sensation and sense of ownership so as to enhance visual, tactile-haptic, and proprioceptive acuity. 2. (VRB2) = Vital Relationships Between: permits the delineation of inherent "skeletal transmission" features (of seeing and sensing for trabeculae density; occurring mostly through longitudinal shafts of bones), and the "skeletal transition" features (structural convergences and expansions of trabeculae occurring between deep articular joint surfaces throughout the body - and further revealed through corresponding motion trajectories required for proportionate dissipation and re-distribution of biomechanical stresses during functional activities. It is again important to reiterate that the cognitiveembodied internalization of these concepts is again accomplished through modeling the entire skeleton. Again, using the complete and articulated full-scale (5-foot tall vertical stand) anatomical model and/or kinesthetic images from both radiology and motion capture kinematic software to convey areas of highest bone density (areas of inner strength, lowest structural variation, and hence, highest predictability); and by more specifically identifying the “Proportionality of Thirds” guideline for efficient movement; scaled as a measurable relationship between “pelvis-hips (2/3-larger - as initiator of movement) opposite the head (1/3-smaller - as fine-motor control/modulator of 174 movement)," as these features are both congruently operationalized and internally consistent with basic Feldenkrais Method® movement principles. 3. (VRB3) = Vestibular Representation of Body in Brain: As a major regulatory system for sensory-motor integration and control of movement, the vestibular -visual system reveals itself - and its influences throughout the body; namely, through visual-ocular reflexes (VOR), vestibulocollicular reflexes (VCR), asymmetrical tonic neck reflexes (ATNs) and other developmental reflexes for posture control, and the co-regulation of spinal muscle tone as a background pre-requisite for everyday functional movement. This concept is again both experienced and operationalized by implementing the "proportionality of thirds" demonstration on the skeletal model, and during actual performance of Feldenkrais® movements throughout all phases of the entire experimental intervention. As a corollary, an added display arrangement of images depicting fractal geometry, inspired self-similarity features in distant anatomical structures relevant to the current study can also be demonstrated throughout the design of the skeleton; namely, through structures of the pelvis-hips opposite head, yet all made congruent through a three-dimensional vestibular representation. Figure 46 depicts these images below and Table 10 demonstrates how their conceptual implementation can be implemented into Feldenkrais® movements, as are presented later in Chapter 3. 175 Figure 46. Images Depicting Self-Simiularity of Structural Features Found to Occur within Natural Systems. Fractal geometry and self-similarity features of repeating analogous and homologous patterns occuring within natural systems, especially for skeletal forms in relationship to spatial orientation and survival. All in all, it can be stated that the skeleton itself – its cognizant image construction and its projected possible actions - affords everything that is necessary to construct a virtual and interactive environment, including the immersive entrainment of novel movement configurations when modeled, revealed; and thus, ultimately experienced through a cross-modal and multisensory context of application, as is reconstituted and accomplished through the composite application of the VRB3 ™ method as a behavioral training sequence for the enhancement of body schema acuity and movement dexterity prior to the actual administration of traditional Feldenkrais sessions or lessons. ® Pilot Study and Continuing Observations from Practice-based Evidence In my earlier retrospective pilot study (Sobie, 2013) conducted at my facility (details to follow), using an accrued "practice-based evidence" demonstration technique to supplement a need for understanding and explaining some ideas behind The Feldenkrais Method® to the 176 regular caseload of spine patients, I inadvertently discovered practical utility in using life-scaled skeletal models and implementing their characteristics as a basis for multimodal/anatomy/skeletal density imagery explorations for each patient. Plus, I found that perceptual accuracy experiments could be augmented for actualizing deep articular joint surface with greater and greater acuity - as a demonstration cognition for re-conceptualizing typically held and clinically familiar notions (muscular deficiency and disc damage) that are likely contributory to a distorted structureal perception of ‘diagnostically-based’ body schema. These models - particularly the femur bone - were then more routinely used as a basis for clarifying and conducting their role in corresponding Feldenkrais® movements for patients presenting with LBP. In the summer of 2012, a retrospective chart review was conducted through a student assisted data compilation of LBP patients being derived from my usual clinical caseload from the preceding eight months, (N = 40). It was discovered that there were significant changes in reported pain level and gait quality after only three sessions of this kind of approach. Results indicated a marked reduction of pain intensity upon repeated before and after VAS scales after the initial three treatment sessions that were for clarifying perceptual acuity for these actual joint articulations - in conjunction with visual-tactile tracings of adjacent skeletal density imagery correspondences - from an average VAS rating of 6/10 at baseline to a new and significantly reduced average of 2/10 after session three. It should be clarified that not all of these patients were being sub-classified as acute, sub-acute, post-surgical, or chronic at the time of this study and data sampling. Additionally and anecdotally, it had also been consistently observed from the outcome of all initial sessions (for clarifying hip axes through pelvis to head), that there was almost a 177 universal co-observation consensus for indicating changes of awareness in gait pattern in terms of proportionate support distribution alignment and fluidity. These observations were qualitatively indicative of performance magnitudes that would have been likely deemed as significant had there been a prior administration of a Dynamic Gait Index test profile and/or other technical-based assessment tool for changes in gait cadence and/or inequalities in force distribution (this of course becomes a prospect for conducting yet an entirely different kind of research project at a later date). It was thereby concluded that body schema based somatic education interventions, including the Feldenkrais Method®, appear efficacious, and deserve further investigation. Furthermore, the interventions do not appear to rely on treating particular anatomical regions that are specific to a diagnostic category or perceived area of involvement (i.e. specific lumbar spine segments) directly. This information was shared in poster presentations and approved abstracts at various national/international scientific meetings including The Feldenkrais® Research Symposium in San Francisco, California, in 2012, The 44th Annual Scientific Meeting of The Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback (AAPB) in Portland, Oregon, during March 2013, and finally to The International Research Congress for Clinicians in Complementary and Alternative Medicine (ICCCIM) in Chicago, Illinois, during October 2013, from which my poster abstract became published in the Global Advances in Health and Medicine Journal, which can also be accessed via the National Library of Medicine Internet link (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3875009/; see Sobie, 2013). The outline and content of the abstract is further summarized below in Table 2, and a hard copy publication reprint is also appended in Appendix C. 178 Table 2 Copy of Published Pilot Study Research Abstract Abstract P03.03. Title: Body Schema and Feldenkrais®: Effects Upon Subjects with Low Back Pain Timothy Sobie (1) Scientific abstracts presented at the International Congress for Clinicians in Complementary & Integrative Medicine 2013 Focus Areas: Integrative Approaches to Care, Alleviating Pain Background and Purpose: Back problems continue to be the number one symptom disorder for consulting complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) practitioners. Neuroscience continues to indicate that the human brain undergoes a process of somato-topic cortical reorganization in association with sustained states of chronic pain. Feldenkrais® practitioners aim to create individualized multimodal learning experiences that are believed to clarify an improved neuroplasticity-based change in the cognitive construct of one's own body schema. A specific protocol is applied to observe some responses in subjects with mechanical, non-specific low back pain (LBP) in a clinical practice setting. Methods: Forty subjects (30 females, 10 males) diagnosed with persistent LBP attended a Feldenkrais®-based physical therapy intervention series of sessions while assessing usual baseline measures—including Pain Intensity on VAS and observations of Gait Quality. Using anatomical skeleton models and proprioceptive touch, 3 inquiries for primary learning conditions were made for clarifying anatomical imagery including (1) The Hip Socket axis of rotation, (2) Inner ilia pelvis as an inner bridge of leg support, and (3) correlating the vestibular apparatus in combination with global Feldenkrais® movements. No attention was given to treating isolated lumbar segments directly. Results: All conditions were novel interpretations of body awareness for subject's previous notions of body schema. Pain on VAS reduced from 6/10 average to 2/10. All subjects had a more balanced gait. Conclusions: Body schema–based somatic education interventions, like the Feldenkrais Method®, deserve further investigation and do not appear to rely on treating the anatomical regions of perceived involvement directly. Note. Reprinted from Scientific Abstracts Global Adv Health Med, 20132(Suppl), p. 3. doi:10.7453/gahmj.2013.097CP. P03.03. Reprinted with permission via Pub Med Central. 179 Summary The accurate examination and effective treatment of CNSLBP remains a continuing and perplexing problem for many scientific researchers and clinicians as well as for conducting health care policy guidance and administration. While research continues to indicate that no one type of exercise intervention or program is superior in efficacy to any other, Core Stabilization Exercise (CSE) approaches have emerged in ubiquitous popularity, and to this day, still remain as one of the most prominent trends for implementation and utilization in physical therapy clinics and fitness centers throughout North America, Europe, and Australia, since original research was published in the late 1990s. The hallmark of CSEs is to entrain patients to perform a "draw –in" maneuver via isolated sub-maximal recruitment of Transverse Abdominus (TrA) and Lumbar Multifidus (LM) muscle groups, most specifically with the aid of either palpation instruction and/or a pneumatic pressure recording biofeedback device known as The Stabilizer®. Exercise progression then implements functional and callisthenic types of activities to challenge the patient to maintain stability (consistent core control) across a range of static positions and dynamic perturbations – being consolidated as a progression as motor control exercises. Conversely, The FeldenkraisMethod® of somatic education seeks to cultivate and link discriminative sensory–motor—informational learning experiences (i.e., perception modified through action and intrinsic qualities of interoceptive attention toward novel spatial-temporal configurations via a range of exploratory embodiment variations that can often be supplemented by action-oriented imagery) in conjunction with fostering new, more efficient, soon to be habituated, neuro-plastic changes in the brain that concur with "optimal use of self," through sensing and leveraging the core of entire skeleton in the performance of everyday generalizable 180 life tasks in a functionally applicable and reproducible context; and ideally, with a proportionate distribution of biomechanical forces such that there is no additional requirement for extraneous or unnecessary muscular effort. Thus, said differently, Feldenkrais® practitioners aim to create individualized multi-modal learning experiences that are believed to clarify new functional interrelationships in both perception and action; and to effectively make better use of an improved neuroplasticity-based change in the cognitive construct of one’s own body schema. Again, the purpose of this single-blind, randomized controlled study (RCT) is to compare a Body Schema Acuity Training protocol using newly applied, newly developed low-cost technology (Virtual Reality Bones™/VRB3) with a respected complementary-alternative, movement and manual therapy, neuroplasticity-based educational intervention (The Feldenkrais Method®) against the most commonly accepted approach being utilized within current and conventional physical therapy practice settings (Core Stabilization Training and Graded Motor Control Exercises). This was conducted for improving the outcomes on usual clinical outcome measures for CNSLBP, and to determine whether there is greater clinical efficacy being demonstrated between one combined intervention or the other for treating the widespread problem of CNSLBP, as an outcome of the study itself. Finally and furthermore, consistent with entire synopsis for literature review, and in corroboration with my attending the proceedings from the 7th Interdisciplinary World Congress on Low Back and Pelvic Pain in Los Angeles during November 2010, it remains conclusive that little has changed over the past five years, and that what is known about the status of expert recommendations for the conservative management and treatment of CNSLBP in terms of movement re-education, cognitive exposure and graded activity and/or supervised exercise programs under the rubric of motor control can be summed-up in the following five points: 181 ● Movement Variation seems more important than Movement Repetitions; ● Sensory Components (the quality of discriminative selection) seems a greater variable of importance than traditional notions of ROM, Strength, & Flexibility; ● In all movements, stability & mobility elements interact; ● Exercise programs that include imagery seem to have better outcomes than those without (Franklin 2010); and ● Bio-Psycho-Social Factors continue to play an important role. 182 CHAPTER 3: METHODOLOGY Overview In this chapter, the research design and its support through current NIH and APTA guidelines and from previous empirical studies, along with its format and historical timeline, are stated, outlined, and described. Details regarding the recruitment of patients to qualify as volunteer human participants, assuring their inclusion and exclusion criteria, providing for their written informed consent for voluntary participation in the study, controlling for fear-avoidance catastrophic pain beliefs in both groups and implementing sequential procedures used for stratified random assignment into two single-blinded treatment groups are also all described. In conjunction with study orientation and consent, I furthermore describe how all study participants were blinded during the course of the study while also being controlled to allow for their individualized treatment progression and to permit their self-application toward resuming graded activity. Provisions for reliable data gathering methods at baseline and for repeated outcome measures throughout the course of the study are described, and the respective tools and test instruments, as metric indicators suitable for assessing clinical difference of change in CNSLBP, are validated. The opposing strategic foundations and qualitative differences occurring between control group and the experimental group’s methodological interventions are identified and described. Finally, the determinants for sample size and the statistical analysis methods selected for procedural comparison of different data sets are introduced and described for purposes of discerning and quantifiably demonstrating the scale and degree of measured differences occurring between two groups over time, and in comparative response to the two interventions. 183 The entire study design was approved to meet IRB guidelines for the protection of human subjects by Saybrook University’s IRB in April 2015. The study began on November 23, 2015, and concluded on April 24, 2016. Again, the purpose of this single-blind, randomized controlled study (RCT) was to compare a Body Schema Acuity Training protocol using newly applied, newly developed lowcost technology (Virtual Reality Bones™/VRB3) with a respected complementary-alternative, movement and manual therapy, neuroplasticity-based educational intervention (The Feldenkrais® Method); and against the most commonly accepted approach being utilized within current and conventional physical therapy practice settings (Core Stabilization Training and Graded Motor Control Exercises). This was conducted for improving the outcomes on usual clinical outcome measures for CNSLBP, and to determine whether there is greater clinical efficacy being demonstrated between one combined intervention or the other for treating the widespread problem of CNSLBP, as an outcome of the study itself. Research Design, Adherence to Current Guidelines, Consistency to Prior Precedent A randomized controlled trial (RCT) using matched control conditions to the closest extent possible was deemed the best design method for outlining and answering the essential research question of evaluating and comparing the respective outcome measures between two interventions, and for the determination of superior treatment efficacy between one intervention versus the other. While the current study makes fastidious attempt to control all variables with exception of the outcome measures as the dependent ones, elements of a pragmatic RCT were also included in the research design. This was to permit some flexibility of delivery within each treatment intervention as they are rendered within the context of actual day-to-day practice, and as adjusted to meet the individual qualities of each participant in each group. For example, if a 184 participant was unable to tolerate a particular position involved in the delivery of a treatment, then modifications were made to assure that that patient could receive the same content of the session’s intended benefit, but from an alternative or imagined position. These adjustments were in part co-determined by the scope, experience, and professional judgement of the treating therapist and these situations were known to likely occur within both arms of the study. Inclusion of Latest NIH Research Guidelines and Clinical Practice Guidelines for CNSLBP Citing a lack of investigator consensus and inconsistency in investigations into the growing worldwide problem of CLBP or CNSLBP, The National Institutes of Health (NIH) released a task force report establishing new standards for research in June 2014. These new standards are additionally and uniformly recommended to be included as necessary requirements for all future NIH grant proposals. The recommendations happen to include: ● Definition of CLBP as "a back pain problem that has persisted at least 3 months and has resulted in pain on at least half the days in the past 6 months." ● Stratification of CLBP impact by "personal impact" considerations including pain intensity, pain interference with normal activities, and functional status. ● Establishment of a minimum data set for describing individuals participating in all research studies on CLBP that captures demographics, medical history, and self-report of symptoms and function including pain intensity, physical interference, depression, and sleep disturbance. ● Affirmation of earlier consensus documents on outcome measures for chronic pain. (The RMDQ and the VAS – PAIN scales qualify for this domain). (De Litto et al., 2012) In addition, the Clinical Practice Guidelines linked to the International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health from the Orthopedic Section of the American Physical Therapy Association outline the following clinical practice guidelines concurrent with the current research design: ● Place patients with LBP in subgroups based on items from the examination, and provide subgroup-specific treatment [moderate evidence]; 185 ● Consider cognitive affective factors when placing patients in subgroups [moderate evidence]; ● The Oswestry Disability Index and the Roland-Morris Disability Questionnaire should be used as outcome measures for patients with LBP [strong evidence]; ● Thrust manipulative procedures can be used as a component of a comprehensive treatment plan to reduce pain and disability in patients with patients with mobility deficits, acute LBP, and/or back-related buttock or thigh pain [strong evidence]; ● Progressive endurance exercise and fitness activities should be encouraged for patients with chronic LBP [strong evidence]; ● Patient education and counseling should focus on the inherent strength of the spine, how pain is processed by the nervous system, the importance of returning to activities, positive coping strategies for pain, and the overall favorable prognosis for LBP. In-depth discussion on pathoanatomical sources of LBP should be avoided, as this strategy may increase a perceived threat or fear associated with LBP [moderate evidence]. (De Litto et al., 2012) The design for the current research study/RCT reflects consistent adherence to both the current NIH research guideline recommendations and the most current APTA clinical practice guidelines being applied to both treatment groups as cited above. Beginning November 2015, I thereby conducted the current randomized controlled trial. Patients received either an original application of a novel intervention program (Body Schema Acuity Training) using the VRB3 approach, plus Feldenkrais® Movements (VRB3/FM; herein specified as an experimental group), or a standard and customary intervention program (Core Stabilization Biofeedback), plus Motor Control Exercises (CSB/MCE; herein specified as a control group). This was conducted over the course of two months, with repeated measures after baseline data gathering occurring at two weeks (Phase I), and again at four weeks (the end of Phase II), and at the end of eight weeks (the conclusion of Phase III) being depicted here in this study as post-intervention. A projected long-term follow-up is intended at six months (post-date of last day for formal intervention at conclusion of Phase III) to assess the effects of long-term potentiation and learning. This detail was fully disclosed in each participant’s consent agreement at start of study 186 to be revisited again between September and October 2016. This trial was prospectively designed with pre-IRB approvals of the signing dissertation committee members as of April 2015, and infollow-up to the results of a previous pilot study published in November 2013 before being finally conducted as a full-scaled RCT from November 2015 – April 2016. A Historical Precedent for Consistency of Research Design & Comparative Metrics A previous comparable study conducted by a respectable team of internationally cited investigators (Macedo et al., 2012), though not previously known during the implementation phase of this study, and was newly referenced in the more recently updated Literature Review section of this document being published as: “Effect of Motor Control Exercises Versus Graded Activity in Patients with Chronic Nonspecific Low Back Pain: A Randomized Controlled Trial.” Again, it is important to disclose that my review for this study was actually discovered after the original formulation of my design parameters for the current study, and within only two weeks of finalizing the data collection and just prior to conducting the statistical analysis. Yet, by having a comparable study design as a similar kind of benchmark or yardstick for exploring the outcomes of treatments for CNSLBP in a comparative study, especially one that also applied the same Core Stabilization/Motor Control Exercise model as a corresponding control group to an alternate treatment intervention (to Graded Activity), I can perhaps at least compare some of their outcome results with my own in terms of: 1. Their using essentially identical outcome measurement tools: ● Visual Analog Scale for Pain (VAS vis a vis NRS), ● Roland Morris Disability Questionnaire (RMDQ), and ● Patient Specific Functional Scale (PSFS); 2. By their using a same number of intended treatment visits (12 sessions), and 187 3. By their using the same distribution schedule for administering the interventions: ● 2 xs per week for first four weeks, ● 1 x per week for the following four weeks. Otherwise, the major difference in this study is that they had a much larger and experienced research team, more multi-site locations for the treatment setting, more significant and varied funding resources, and hence a much larger sample size (N=172). Participants, Sources of Recruitment, Treatment Setting & Orientation to the Study Patients as "Participants" presenting with clinical diagnosis of Chronic, Non-Specific Low Back Pain (CNSLBP), and without significant medical complications that would prevent their attendance in an outpatient setting, were recruited to the clinical intervention study. This was provided through area physicians via special referral to "Alliant Physical Therapy/Alliant Spine Project, LTD," specifying "Chronic Pain Research & Neuroplasticity for Low Back Pain" check box under "Tim Sobie, PT, Ph.D. Candidate." Generation of study referrals via primary care (family practice and internal medicine) physicians occurred mostly through prior professional networking, and less through research announcement flyers and mass mailings of direct mail, as co-facilitated through Pierce County Medical Society. A significant source came through direct networking with Dr. Derek S. Scott, as medical director of CHI/Franciscan Pain Management Center, Tacoma, Washington, who also served on the dissertation committee as the medical advisor for the current study. Other referrals came in support from University of Washington (UW) Pain Center, Veterans Administration/Veteran’s Choice Community Access programs, suitable referrals from other allied health professionals, and by public newspaper announcement. All participants, whether initially referred by physician or self-referred, underwent physical therapy screening and corroboration with their primary care physician via 188 documentation of evaluation and plan of care being inclusive of "participation in low back pain clinical study involving the use of manual therapy, graded exposure, exercise, and movement." Table 3 summarizes sources of recruitment for study participants. Table 3 Sources of Recruitment for Study Participants Control Group Primary Care Experimental Group 3 Pain Specialist (CHI /Franciscan) 3 4 Other Pain Specialist (UW) 1 0 Veterans Administration 2 1 Licensed Mental Health Counselor 2 0 Occupational Therapist 1 0 Self-Referred 3 6 4 Outpatient Treatment Setting and Neutralizing the Environment for Matched Consistency All participants underwent orientation and consent, initial data collection, stratified random assignment into groups, therapist screening assessment, continued intervention procedures, and continued data collection within the same facility at Alliant Physical Therapy and Integral Medicine, PLLC, care of The Alliant Building, 201 N. I Street, Tacoma, WA, 98403. Three experimental group participants opted a request to have at least ½ of their visits allocated to the Gig Harbor satellite branch location of Alliant Physical Therapy and Integral Medicine, PLLC, 7195 Wagner Way, Suite 105, Gig Harbor, WA, 98335, to reduce their cost of highway 16 bridge tolls. These sessions took place on Thursdays only. Reassessment and data 189 collection, however, could only take place with designated research coordinator Kenny Li and reception staff at the Alliant Building main practice location in Tacoma. Both facilities share common color themes and interiors, depict a warm and holistic atmosphere, and are devoid of usual sports medicine or gymnasium fitness equipment products that are typical of most outpatient physical therapy facilities. Figure 47 displays the session areas for each location with the former being shared by the majority of participants between each group. (a) (b) Figure 47. Site Locations and Facilities used for the Current Study. (a) Tacoma, WA, USA; (b) Gig Harbor, WA, USA. Characteristics of the Clinicians providing the Interventions The legislative scope of practice for licensed physical therapists in many areas – including in Washington State, USA, the jurisdiction from which this study was conducted, specifically includes “direct access to the evaluation and treatment of persons afflicted with any mental or physical impairment that impairs function [emphasis added]" (Washington Administrative Code, RCW 18.74.010). All physical therapists (PTs) and one physical therapy 190 assistant (PTA) maintained qualified Washington licensure before, during, and months after the study. The control group PTs (N= 3) were contracted to be available for treating patients with CNSLBP from November 23, 2015 – April 22, 2016. One control group PT had specific dedicated experience as the lead PT for a comprehensive multi-disciplinary, CARF certified pain rehabilitation center for approximately 11 years. Another was currently working for an injured worker’s specialty facility full time, but with prior certifications in holistic-oriented CAM therapies, including Qi Gong and Biodynamic Cranial Sacral Therapy. Both of these therapists had accrued 25 and 17.5 years of total physical therapy experience respectively, with 20 and 15 years being respectively familiar and experienced for the delivery and training of core stabilization and motor control exercises. The third therapist had three years of licensed experience, but is also a daughter of physical therapy clinician and practice-owner from another mid-western state for over 25 years. Alliant Physical Therapy’s office and support staff deemed all to be personable and professional. The experimental group PTs included the principal investigator having 30 years’ experience in licensed physical therapy practice, and with 20 years’ experience as a Guild Certified Feldenkrais Method® Practitioner. The added clinician for the experimental group intervention had 2 years’ experience as a licensed physical therapy assistant, but she has maintained active separate certification as a Guild Certified Feldenkrais Method® Practitioner for almost 16 years. The average years of licensed physical therapy experience compared to specialty experience of clinicians delivering the specialty “Core Stabilization/Motor Control” vs. the “Feldenkrais Method®-based” intervention approaches are depicted in Table 4. 191 Table 4 Average Years of Experience of Clinicians delivering the Specialty “Core” vs. “Feldenkrais Method®” Intervention Physical Therapy (Years) Specialty Area (Years) Control Group 15.1 12.3 16.0 18.0 Core Ex. & Motor Control Experimental Group Feldenkrais Method® Note. The first column quantifies average years of ‘standard PT clinical practice’ overall inclusive of specialty area for specified training. Orientation of Participants Prior to and during the Study Prospective participants for each group were uniformly blinded, both prior to study entry and throughout the course of the study, by depicting a consistent language within the informed consent form. The form adequately communicated an overall impression for study intent, but without specifically disclosing the particular characteristics of difference distinctly inherent as important and independent distinguishing variables uniquely attributable to either the control group or the experimental group’s style of difference for actually depicting them as a competing intervention. Thus, the language of the consent form was designed with mutual intent to both inform and to necessarily blind the participating subjects between each arm of the RCT study. At no time were study participants in either group ever referred to as being in an experimental group or a comparison or control group. 192 Vital points of consideration upon patient consent included titling the clinical research and study description in adequate and believable terms. The consent form title read: A Comparison of Two Body Awareness Training Methods for Maintaining Precisely Coordinated Movements and Optimal Control of Spinal Stability for the Improvement of both Symptoms and Activity Capacities in Daily Life: Comparative Effects on Subjects with Chronic Non-Specific Low Back Pain in an Out-patient Clinical Setting. Furthermore, the purpose of the study was stated in descriptive terms “to find out whether there are significant differences between two existing types of body awareness training methods for improved stability and control of the spine column and surrounding relationships through precisely coordinated muscle and movement activity.” It was also clarified that “...both treatment interventions are specifically designed for persons who commonly relapse into episodes of chronic recurrent low back pain, have persistent pain; also known and diagnosed as Chronic, Non-Specific Low Back Pain (CNSLBP).” The consent from additionally depicted the background nature of the current clinical study by stating to participants that: You are being asked to participate in a clinical investigation trying to find out about comparative relationships between two types of patient education and training methods involving the awareness and experience of muscle control activities and coordinative movements designed to improve back and spine function and to decrease the debilitating effects of CNSLBP, including the attenuation (or reduction) of pain intensity itself. We have discovered that for some people who have Chronic (CNSLBP) Low Back Pain, that there is a corresponding disruption of clarity for both the sensation of where your body is in space and how it moves, and in how muscles can both under-contract and over-contract as if they sometimes have a mind of their own. Also, there is a corresponding disparity of how to coordinate daily or novel movement (motor) activities with clear dexterity-- especially for larger movements involving a balance throughout the whole body. We believe that these ordinary activates have become hampered or blocked over time by the predominance of pain pathways. Yet, we also believe that by training precisely coordinated movements with these newer methods, you can invoke ‘competing pathways’ in your brain and nervous system that are not compatible with usual, often misunderstood, pain-invoking pathways. ....after your consent and the initial intakes, you will be informed about your status as a continued participant in the study and be assigned to one of two groups wherein a 193 licensed physical therapist - specifically trained in one of the two methods - will assess and carry-out the respective treatment progression. Another, most important feature embedded within the consent form was the assurance of participant safety. Here, it seemed fitting to introduce elements and advance directives and principles being derived through Therapeutic and Pain Neuroscience Education. This was to control for fear-avoidance, to describe symptom relapse (of usual symptoms) as a normative initial phenomenon in chronic pain, and to challenge the common cognitive-behavioral distortion of "hurt equating to harm or actual tissue damage occurring under otherwise innocuous conditions," as being an unsubstantiated automatic thought capable of reinforcing and amplifying the pain experience. Controlling for Fear-Avoidance and Catastrophic Pain Beliefs for Both Groups As further referenced from the literature review, and by inferring the status of current treatment recommendations derived through practice and research, there was added consideration for controlling confounding bio-psychosocial variables; namely, fear-avoidance factors. In consequence, the practice literature over past five years has documented the emergence of newer therapy approaches that aim to combine (a) cognitive-behavioral and exposure therapy approaches with (b) supervised exercise programs in order to entrain improved motor control, while simultaneously seeking to alter the patient’s beliefs about the interplay between pain and movement, in other words, that “hurt does not equal harm” (Nijs et al., 2014). As these have become the new basis of standard for the therapeutic treatment of chronic pain – particularly, low back pain, and in consultation with other colleagues, I have sought to include them in the study design. To control for this important variable, this study highlighted a brief mention of attribution of expectancy for the possible emergence of pain output phenomena (unexpected spontaneous 194 and/or latent flare-ups of escalated pain intensity being experienced) that could occur at any time during the course of the study for either group. This offered a corresponding, but contrasting reattribution using basic principles of therapeutic neuroscience education (i.e., by stating that "imagined movements and gentle sub-maximal forces to be employed within both groups are in no way causal of producing actual tissue damage,"; e.g., hurt does not equal harm) by way of: A. Embedding a description of such within the subject’s consent form/agreement to participate in the study as a category of pre-informed risks for participation in the study; B. Of orienting each subject about the nature and essential themes to be encountered during each treatment session, exercise program and/or movement progression, and reminding them that such activities will be mildly challenging to attend to (in order to progress forward), but are not likely to be physically harmful in any direct or indirect way such that they would cause tissue damage or injury; and C. Of reviewing for non-correspondences in subject’s adherence/activity/home exercise log, and/or continuously monitoring their subjective reporting during the course of the study. The occasional phenomenon of latent or spontaneous pain flares occurring anywhere in the body after or during the course of treatment, or at any time was explained to each subject as a normative and common finding in activity situations involving sensitization to chronic pain. These phenomena were re-framed as constituting “an output from the hyper-sensitized brain in guarded response to essentially innocuous, but albeit unfamiliar events,”; and not as a consequence of new injury/traumatic strain or tissue damage, nor likely due to any serious “medical condition,” as was further evidenced by complete medical work-up and/or physical therapy screening exam prior to entering the study. These pre-treatment and during-treatment elements are essentially borrowed from Therapeutic Neuroscience Education/Pain Neuroscience 195 Education and Cognitive Behavioral Therapy re-attribution principles, and were again explained, detailed, and discussed in advance via a specific entry within the Consent Form and Participation Agreement for the prospective volunteer participants, and for enrolled participants for both arms of the study. Inclusion/Exclusion Criteria and the Stated Conditions for Continued Participation The primary criteria for participation in my current study included any patient presenting with clinical presentation, diagnosis, and/or history of chronic, non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP), being recurrent and/or persistent between posterior costal margins and base of buttocks / gluteal folds, and lasting greater than three months without significant change. Age Range 18-80, and open to all genders, classes, ethnicities, orientations, and race. Inclusionary Criteria More specifically, patients were eligible for inclusion if they met all of the following inclusion criteria: ● Chronic nonspecific low back pain (greater than three month's duration) with or without leg pain, but not distal to knees; ● Currently seeking care for low back pain; ● Between 18 and 80 years of age; ● English speaker (to allow response to the questionnaires, being amenable to therapy instruction, and for communication with the physical therapist); ● Clinical assessment indicated that the patient was suitable for active exercises. 196 Exclusionary Criteria Patients were excluded from the comparative clinical research study for chronic nonspecific low back pain if they presented with: ● Peripheral radicular symptoms distal to knee; ● Previous spinal surgery within past year or scheduled for surgery during study period; ● History of multiple surgical lumbar spine fusions and/or resultant "failed spine syndrome"; ● Known or suspected serious lumbar pathology including changes in bowel or bladder function, severe weakness, other neurovascular changes, or complete loss of sensation; ● Comorbid health conditions (cardiac, respiratory, malignant or neurological) that would contraindicate participation in moderate to potentially strenuous exercise activity; ● Confirmed or expectant pregnancy, or less than six months, post-partum status for LBP; ● Recent history of epidural procedure and /or pain device implants within prior three months; ● Current documented risk and/or clinical presentation of severe opioid addiction or abuse; ● Pending litigation/attorney representation for injury claim having to do with LBP; The co-presentation of significant orthopedic hip, knee, or podiatric foot problems, fibromyalgia syndrome as well as rheumatoid arthritis or other autoimmune conditions, was to be considered on a case-to-case basis prior to entry into study in discussion collaboration with the attending or primary care physician. Fortunately, there were no severe co-morbidities in participating volunteers that precluded their participation in the current study. 197 The Stated Conditions for Continued Participation Consenting participants were permitted to maintain their current medication regimen throughout the course of the study, but were not permitted to abruptly increase or abruptly discontinue their dosage. Instead, they could opt to gradually titrate down their average dosing amounts gradually over time, week by week, during the eight-week course of the study. Where applicable, they were asked to limit their alcohol intake to two drinks per day. Addendum entry fields were added to home program adherence diaries to permit, track, and attain records of difference for medication intake during the course of treatment, after the eight weeks, and before the six-month follow-up. As an added consideration for the control of potentially confounding variables, patients were asked not to seek any other form of physical, behavioral, and/or medical/surgical, over-thecounter interventions during the eight-week course of the study in order to remain enrolled as a continued participant in the study. These competing intervention restrictions would have included other physical therapy or physical or occupational therapists for back or spine, massage and bodywork, chiropractic, osteopathy, acupuncture, interventional pain medical specialists, naturopathic interventions, counselors, biofeedback practitioners, yoga, pilates, personal trainers, energy healers, medical cannabis (unless already in use as a medical regimen), and so on. Other onset of any severe medical or psychological conditions that would impair a participant’s ability to attend or actively respond to any usual rehabilitative form of care, or that would have endangered their safety, would also exclude them form continued participation in the study. Sample Size, FABQ Sub-Grouping, and Stratified Random Assignment into Groups Determining how to achieve an adequate sample size for statistical power, how to arrange for an independent coordinator for assessment and sub-grouping, random assignment and data 198 collection, and for managing the projection of added cost expenditures being anticipated to fulfill the necessary human resource requirements, but without significant funding and stringent budget constraints in the era of continuing managed care, began as a daunting and worrisome task. Outside resources and other self-education venues were a necessary component toward resolving an unrelenting and uncertain dilemma. Determining Sample Size for a Small-Scale Therapy Practice Setting – Use of Pilot Study One method of projecting an estimated sample size during clinical research, particularly in new intervention situations without a prior precedent for assessing the comparative effects of a novel approach against a standardized or current one can be achieved by conducting an ongoing comparative statistical analysis in real time, while continuously randomizing participants into two groups, and continuously tracking for trends of change in their data. For economy of scale, this study could have begun with ten per group (N=20) and then followed by power analysis to determine how many more subjects were needed for comparative statistical significance via verification through t-test distribution. However, it was well beyond my scope to hire on an onstaff statistician at start of study. Having the good fortune to attend the 2012 International Research Congress for Integrative Medicine and Health (IRCIMH), being co-sponsored by Consortium of Academic Health Centers for Integrative Medicine in Portland, Oregon, I enrolled myself in the NIH/NCCAM-sponsored pre-conference workshop “Advancing Research Literacy,” as taught by Claudia Witt, MD, MBA on May 15, 2012. While larger sample size numbers of 100 or more are most desired for larger statistical power in clinical studies, she quoted the following support guideline in her slide presentation by stating that “...however, given stability of estimates in the 199 literature, an N=15 per arm of study is considered a good rule of thumb for early studies, along with consideration of 10 participants per measured variable” (Witt, 2012). Another more specific method for determining sample size - and in more direct relationship to the clinical research question and current study design - is to garner data sampling and statistical relevance of expected change as derived from prior pilot studies involving the same or similar patient populations and the same or similar attribution characteristics as the intervention to be prospectively studied. As was described in Chapter 2 of this dissertation manuscript, at the conclusion of the literature review section, an in-house clinical pilot study and retrospective review of LBP patients receiving SDI/Feldenkrais interventions (during the usual course of treatment – no matched controls) was conducted from 2011-2012 and published in 2013. In this study, N=40 had rendered an average change in reported pain levels on VAS from 6/10 to 2/10; an interval difference of four points. Applying a formula for sample size estimate (16 s²/d² + 1) using the pilot data of mean difference (d) between average change (4 points) and the average standard deviation (s) from total average of the individual measurements of variability for all individuals within the group (given +/- 2 points; an approximate per group sample size is calculated as: 16 s²/d² + 1 > > 16 2X2 / 4X4 +1 > > 64/16 +1 >> 4+ 1 = 5/group [N=10]) However, given the potentially equalizing factor of diminished difference using a control group, the requirement for screening eligibility for appropriate inclusion, and for and the likelihood of attrition and drop-out rate, I could anticipate recruiting at least 40 subjects to attain the recommended guideline for N=30. The formula for sample size estimate was sourced through accessing a public domain statistician’s website link: http://www.jerrydallal.com/LHSP/SIZE.HTM 200 Sub-Grouping High Fear-Avoidance Beliefs as a Known and Confounding Variable Historically, the physical therapy profession has attempted to sub-group patients with CNSLBP into diagnostic sub-groups based on "objective" classification physical exam findings, but with little success. Cross-sectional data has revealed significant contribution from biopsychosocial influences; namely, fear-avoidance and catastrophic beliefs upon the exacerbation and magnification of chronic pain states. As a further control of potentially confounding psychosocial variables, qualified and consenting participants (i.e., who presented with scores exceeding preliminary pre-qualifying scores on the Fear Avoidance Belief Questionnaire [FABQ] greater than 34 for work sub-scales and greater than 15 for physical activity sub-scales) underwent random stratified assignments into each group at baseline/preintervention to better assure comparable representation of bio-psychosocial outlier tendencies and greater homogeneity in each group. Rationale for using Fear-Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire (FABQ) as an Assessment Tool The Fear-Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire (FABQ) is a questionnaire based on the FearAvoidance Model of Exaggerated Pain Perception, a model created in attempts to explain why some patients with acute painful conditions can recover while other patients develop chronic pain from such conditions. The FABQ measures patients’ fear of pain and consequent avoidance of physical activity because of their fear. This questionnaire consists of 16 items, with each item scored from 0-6. Higher scores on the FABQ are indicative of greater fear and avoidance beliefs (Waddell, Newton, et al., 1993). Within the FABQ, two subscales exist, the, which facilitate the identification of the patient’s beliefs about how work and physical activity affect their current low back pain (LBP). The numbers in parentheses below designate which items from the FABQ are included in each 201 subscale, along with total possible points for each subscale (Fritz & George, 2002). These are summarized in Table 5: Table 5 FABQ Work Subscale (w) and the Physical Activity Subscale (pa) and their Thresholds of Criteria for designating Excessive Scores for High Fear-Avoidance Cognitions in CNSLBP Subscale Questions Included Total Possible Points High Score FABQ w (items 6,7,9-12, 15) 42 >34 24 >15 FABQ pa Items 2-5 A strong relationship exists between elevated fear avoidance beliefs and chronic disability secondary to LBP. “Avoidance may lead to reduced activity levels, an exacerbation of the fear and avoidance behaviors, prolonged disability, and adverse physical and psychological effects” (Vlaeyen, Kole-Snijders, Boeren, & van Eck, 1995). Thus, the FABQ is an outcome measure that serves as a clinically useful screening tool in identifying patients with high fear avoidance beliefs who are at risk for prolonged disability. Management of patients with elevated FABQ scores thus requires clinicians to tailor interventions to meet those needs. Research reviews again suggest multi-disciplinary approaches, including cognitive behavioral therapy and graded exposure to physical activity. Intended population of FABQ in reference to the current study. The FABQ has been proven to be a reliable and valid assessment tool based on patients with chronic low back pain. In recent research, the FABQ is also being used preventatively in populations with acute low back pain to identify the risk of long-term disability (Fritz & George, 2002). 202 Reliability of FABQ. Test-retest reliability of the FABQ has been classified and rated as good to excellent through an in-depth clinical review of statistical research to rate the agreement between repeated measures: ● Total FABQ test-retest reliability (ICC=0.97); ● FABQ Physical Activity subscale test-retest reliability (ICC=0.72-0.90); and ● FABQ Work subscale test-retest reliability (ICC=0.80-0.91). (Williamson, 2006) Cicchetti (1994) published guidelines for interpretation for kappa inter-rater or ICC inter-class agreement measures: ● Less than 0.40—Poor. ● Between 0.40 and 0.59—Fair. ● Between 0.60 and 0.74—Good. ● Between 0.75 and 1.00—Excellent. Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC) is an inferential statistical appraisal for the assessment of consistency or reproducibility of quantitative measurements made by different observers measuring the same given quantity of phenomena. In clinical research, it is most typically applied to what is being referred to as inter-rater reliability (Cohen's kappa coefficient), when conducted through inter-professional assessment by separate examiners; or in terms of test-retest reliability, as when conducted through individual self-assessment in the absence of therapeutic intervention and during a prospective period of repeat sampling. Validity of FABQ. The validity of a measurement tool is considered to be the degree to which the tool or testing instrument measures what it purports to measure, particularly in relationship to current evidence in association with known theoretical constructs and observable or measurable phenomena. Statistical conclusion validity is the degree to which conclusions 203 about the relationship among variables accrued through the sampling of a data set are determined as likely correct or reasonable (Cozby, 2009). As this type of validity is concerned solely with the relationship that is found among discretely occurring variables, the relationship must be qualifiedly expressed in terms of numerical correlation. Correlation coefficients measure the strength of association between two variables. The most common correlation coefficient, called the Pearson correlation coefficient, measures the strength of the linear association between variables. The strongest linear relationship is indicated by a correlation coefficient of -1 or 1. Most related phenomena correlate toward a modest correlation coefficient value of 0.40 or more, with stronger correlations rating at 0.80 or more. The weakest linear relationship is indicated by a correlation coefficient equal to 0, expressed in terms as "no direct linear correlation," but only within the designated constraints of the current sampling frame. A positive correlation means that if one variable gets bigger, the other variable tends to get bigger too (toward +1). In contrast, a negative correlation means that if one variable gets bigger, the other variable tends to get smaller (toward -1). Evidence shows that the FABQ is well correlated with the Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire (RMDQ). By comparison, the correlation coefficients for the FABQ in total, the FABQ W (work subscale) and the FABQ PA (physical activity subscale) are 0.52, 0.63, and 0.51, respectively. The FABQ was also shown to be correlated with the Tampa Scale of Kinesiophobia, another measure of fear avoidance. Within its own internal construct validity for like components, the correlation coefficients for the FABQw and the FABQpa subscales are 0.53 and 0.76, respectively (Williamson, 2006). The more recently developed STarT Back Questionnaire is also undergoing concurrent review to assess reliability and validity as well as its correlational relationships to other instruments. 204 Implementation of FABQ. Accordingly, the current comparative study implemented the FABQ based on criteria outlined above as a basis for stratified randomization of sampling between each group to account for the confounding variables of fear of movement, fear of reinjury, and perceived work demand incapacity, as especially relevant to persons with chronic low back pain disorders (CNSLBP), and to better assure a more equal representation in each arm of the study. A copy of the Fear-Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire (FABQ) and Score Sheet are found in Appendix G. Procedure for Consent, Gathering of Baseline Data and Stratified Randomized Assignment Patients being referred as prospective participants underwent usual physical therapy office admission and intake procedures, orientation to HIPPA privacy and protection policy, study orientation, and signed consent to participate in the study, as was described earlier in this chapter. Subsequent to administration of baseline tests and questionnaires by front desk receptionist and physical performance measures by on-site research coordinator (details to be described later), they underwent blinded procedures for stratified random assignment to one of two groups. Two sets of randomization rosters were created in excel: ● One for patients (as consenting participants) to be scheduled into the control group therapy block (up to 15 spaces); and ● One for therapists delivering the experimental group intervention (up to 15 spaces) being alternated as a concurrent, but separate scheduling block from which to place the qualified entry of prospective participants within the Practice Perfect EMR dedicated appointment slots for each available clinician representing the control or experimental group interventions. 205 Allocation of initial group assignment corresponded with real world points of entry via alternating the order sequence of referrals that arrived randomly either via fax or phone call to the front office staff in real time. Once signing the informed consent, the patient was assigned to participate in the intended group as determined by random but alternating-sequential order in the scheduling. All intake data was kept separate from view by principal investigator and treating clinicians in an excel spreadsheet ledger being organized and stored in separate cabinet and data file by an employed therapy intern and research coordinator working on-site at the therapy setting’s back office. If it was found by the attending research coordinator that a patient as participant either met or exceeded the indicated threshold scores in the FABQ (greater than 34 for FABQ W and/or greater than or equal to 15 for FABQ PA) upon scoring at intake, and there was a pending imbalance in number for these participants being represented or allocated to one group, then the research coordinator, in conjunction with front desk scheduling, would then transfer the outlying participant into the opposite, alternate treatment group. Participating subjects who presented with scores exceeding preliminary pre-qualifying threshold scores on the Fear Avoidance Belief Questionnaire (FABQ), defined as exceeding greater than or equal to 34 for work sub-scales (W) and/or greater than or equal to 15 for physical activity (PA) sub-scales, thus underwent and adhered to provisional procedures for random stratified assignment into each group. This alternating sequence of sub-strata assignment cyclically continued until both groups were more or less equally matched for comprising a like sample of high FABQ threshold scores, and this process continued as both groups approached the necessary participation levels for sample size power at n=15. 206 For this study, 10 of n=15 qualified in the control group and 10 of n=15 qualified in the experimental group. Of this, 17 were identified to have high scores for Physical Activity (PA) avoidance sub-scale, whereas only three were identified as qualifying for the Work (W) taskavoidance sub-stratum. Fortunately for control features of the research design, but unfortunate as a population characteristic, it was found that high FABQ scores were a dominant feature in both groups for participating patients having been diagnosed with CNSLBP. All in all, 10 of 15 subjects in both groups (n=30) were accounted for in this sub-stratum classification, with n=8 registering high in the PA sub-scale in the experimental group compared to n=7 in the control group. For work task avoidance, n=3 participants scored high in the W subscale for the control group, as contrasted by n=2 included participants achieving concomitantly high scores in both PA and W sub-scales for the experimental group. The final allocation of distribution achieved for study participant’s high FABQ threshold values, being allocated for random stratification assignment into each group, is depicted and summarized in Table 6: Table 6 Random Stratification Subgrouping Distribution based on FABQ Experiment al Group Control Group W-subscale: Hi Work Task Avoidance Only (> 34) 0 3 PA-subscale: Hi Physical Activity Avoidance Only (> 15) 8 7 2 0 10 10 W&PA-subscales combined: Both Hi Work Task Avoidance (>34) Hi Physical Activity Avoidance (>15) Total Subjects qualifying for Stratified Random Assignment 207 From this metric, it can perhaps be inferred and confirmed that high scores on FABQ may be a consistently high metric for populations of patients with CNSLBP; becoming co-morbid with a presenting diagnosis of chronic low back pain lasting greater than three months or more. Tests and Repeated Measures for Clinical Outcome Tools, scales, questionnaires, physical performance tests, and other instruments that were found to be amenable for sensitivity to change and for quantification of detecting clinically relevant change over time for conditions involving chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) were additionally selected on the basis on prior literature reviews; these also ran comparative randomized controlled trials as well as other preliminary studies. Further selection was based on known practice guidelines and substantiated by the concurrent research recommendations from the 2014 NIH Guidelines Report. All in all, the tools that have been in longest practice application were also the tools that were most abundantly cited. Thus, the following instruments were chosen as a reliable benchmark for my research, especially in light of my introducing an original, newly constructed and combined intervention. The Visual Analog Scale for Pain (VAS-PAIN) Routinely used for rating the intensity of Low Back Pain, the Visual Analog Scale (VAS) also combined with implementing its relationship to a numerical rating scale (NRS), asks patients to rate their pain intensity on an 11-point, horizontal continuum scale where “0” indicates “no pain” and “10” indicates their “worst imaginable pain.” The patient marks on the line the point number that they feel represents their perception of their current state. At each administration, this study sought to contextually qualify "worst imaginable pain" (Level 10) by adding criteria to state that "Level Ten is super-severe, such that with or without a dose of morphine anesthesia taking hold, you would likely pass-out just in order to cope." 208 Studies have been conducted with intent to discern the cut-off points on the visual analogue scale (VAS) to distinguish among mild, moderate, and severe categories of pain level. Boonstra, Schiphorst Preuper, Balk, and Stewart (2014) sampled the VAS scores of 456 patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain and cross-correlated them with other health assessment functional scales, including the commonly used Short Form-36 Health Survey (SF-36). The study results showed that VAS scores less than 3.4 corresponded to mild interference with functioning, whereas 3.5 to 6.4 implied moderate interference, and is greater than 6.5 implied severe interference. Correspondingly, they interpreted VAS scores for patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain at less than 3.4 to be descriptive as a "cut-off" point for mild pain, 3.5 to 7.4 as a demarcation for moderate pain, and is greater than 7.5 as severe pain. However, they added the caveat that As there appear to be no universally accepted cut-off points, and in view of the low-tomoderate associations between VAS scores and functioning and between VAS and verbal rating scale scores, the correct classification of VAS scores as mild, moderate, or severe in clinical practice seems doubtful. (Boonsta et al., 2014) Stated more commonly, the patient’s pain severity "is what they say it is." In the absence of a gold standard for measures of pain, criterion validity can only be evaluated at face value for its purported construct. For construct validity, the NRS for pain was shown to be highly correlated with the VAS in patients with rheumatic and other chronic pain conditions (pain is greater than six months) with correlations ranging from 0.86 to 0.95 (Ferraz et al., 1990). High test–retest reliability has likewise been observed in both literate and illiterate patients with rheumatoid arthritis and other chronic pain conditions (r = 0.96 and 0.95, respectively) before and after medical consultation (Ferraz et al., 1990). For optimal use across multiple interpretations of ratings for the quantification of pain intensity as a cognitive-phenomenological construct that is unique to each individual, the current 209 study implemented a combined visual analog – numerical rating scale. This relied on combined "visual" use of numerical ratings being equally ascribed to equally-spaced intervals, with corresponding verbal quantification descriptors (cut-off points) in addition to the inclusion of emotionally valanced verbal qualifiers (e.g., moods); all correspondent with visual shading of color intensities along a horizontal continuum inversing from cool blue to hot red. For sake of simplicity, its documented cross-validation with the traditional NRS, and with reference to abundance of visual information, I have adopted to retain the name of this instrument as simply VAS-PAIN. A shaded gray scale example of the form used is shown in Figure 48. A copy of the VAS-PAIN reference scale as viewed in "true color" during repeat administration throughout the course of my study is found in Appendix H. 210 (a) (b) ______________________________________________________________________ Figure 48. Gray-Scale Copy of VAS-PAIN / Numerical Rating Scale. (a) VAS-PAIN numerical rating scale being further embellished from (b) traditional uni-linear scales with added multimodal qualifiers from which to have participants visually re-quantify their pain intensity during repeat administration throughout the course of the study. The Roland-Morris Disability Questionnaire (RMDQ) The Roland-Morris Disability Questionnaire (RMDQ) is one of the most commonly used tools for measuring self-rated disability due to low back pain (Roland & Fairbank, 2000). The RMDQ consists of 24 questions about activity limitations due to back pain (e.g., walking, lying, and self-care), and is relatively easy to use in a clinical setting. Participating CNSLBP patients simply provided yes or no answers to each statement. Each affirmation answer is worth one point with scores ranging from “0” (no disability) to “24” (severely disabled). Upon re-administration re-test, a change in one to two points is considered a significant change if the initial score 211 showed little disability, whereas a change in seven to eight points is indicative as significant if the original RMDQ showed high levels of disability at the initial administration. Test-retest reliability 24-item: intraclass correlation (ICC) ranges from 0.42 – 0.91. Construct validity for the RMDQ correlates well with other tests, which purport to measure physical disability, including the physical subscales of SF-36, physical subscales of Sickness Impact Profile, the Quebec Low Back Scale, the Oswestry Disability Questionnaire, and usual scales for pain ratings (Roland & Fairbank, 2000). One study (Hall, Maher, Latimer, Ferreira, & Costa, 2011) concluded that the RMDQ and PSFS both demonstrate good responsiveness to assess activity level changes according to chronic LBP guidelines. However, the PSFS is more responsive than the RMDQ for patients with low levels of activity limitation, but not for patients with high levels of activity limitation. A copy of this scale is also provided for review in Appendix I. The Patient-Specific Functional Scale (PSFS) The Patient-Specific Functional Scale (PSFS) is used to tally a patient’s self-assessed ability and/or experienced margin of difficulty to perform or participate in the daily specific activities in life that are deemed to be most personally relevant to them. Patients rate their current ability to complete an activity on an 11-point scale as compared to rating the level they experienced prior to their injury or change in functional status: ● "0" represents “unable to perform”; ● "10" represents “able to perform at prior level. (Stratford, Gill, Westaway & Binkley, 1995) Upon administration and orientation to the instrument, patients are asked to identify up to three or more activities that they had difficulty with or were unable to perform as a result of their 212 recurring low back pain and to rate these activities on an 11-point scale from “0” as "unable to perform activity" to “10” as being "able to fully perform the activity at same level before back pain." Patients as research participants select a value that best describes their current level of ability on each activity assessed. At follow-up, and as per design of the scale, patients as subjects are allowed to access their original scores, and are invited to rescore each activity according to their current perception of their performance. The scale is appropriately used for populations of subjects with chronic LBP based on its prior history, research development, and areas of application, including previous Lumbar Core Stabilization efficacy studies providing evidence that “patient-generated measures of disability are more responsive than condition-specific measures” (Maher et al., 2005). The scale has also been shown to be a sensitive measure for changes encountered before and after Feldenkrais Sessions (Connors, Pile, & Nichols, 2011). The PSFS’s criterion validity had been originally researched by comparing concurrent validity with the Roland-Morris scale (RMDQ); outlining predictive correlational averages occurring across five commonly listed PSFS functional abilities and activities as compared with that of RMDQ (disability) scores; inversely correlated as excellent at r = -0.67 (Stratford et al., 1995). Another series of comparisons of correlation coefficients had determined good convergent validity for the Patient Specific Functional Scale (PSFS) when compared with the self-identified Global Rating of Change Scale (GRC) as well as for the more generic pre-design format contained within the 36-item Short Form Health Survey (36-SF). As applied to measuring the instrument’s reliability, when the PSFS was applied for clinical case populations involving chronic LBP, it was found that inter-rater reliability was determined as excellent, with an 213 Intraclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC) = 0.92 (Maughan & Lewis, 2010) and with excellent test-retest reliability of ICC = 0.97 (Stratford et al., 1995). In the comparative instrumentation study conducted by my Australian Physiotherapist/Feldenkrais colleague, Karol Connors, a pre/post-test cohort design was used to investigate the use of PSFS as an outcome measurement for clients experiencing problems performing everyday functional tasks who attended Feldenkrais sessions. Eleven Feldenkrais practitioners submitted data on 48 clients. Changes were detected in the clients' ability to perform everyday tasks (PSFS improved 3.8 points, p is less than 0.001), and thus this tool was designated, among others, to be suitable for detecting changes in client function before and after a series of Feldenkrais sessions (Connors et al., 2011). Appendix J contains the format copy of the Patient-Specific Functional Scale (PSFS), complete with self-contained guidelines for implementation instruction and scoring. McGill’s Timed Endurance Tests (Total Endurance + Flexion/Extension Ratios) As cited in the literature review, strength deficits (as measured by the capacity to generate high forces) and range of motion limitations with corresponding flexibility deficits do not appear contributory. The frequent impairment finding of loss of spine range of motion being commonly cited and believed as being a primary factor in continuing low back pain has been shown to have little to do with restoring the capacity for resuming usual functions at work (Parks et al., 2003). Furthermore, other studies have shown that static stretching of spine ligaments was highly correlated toward causing a higher incidence of muscle spasms and diminished protective stretch reflex responsivity (Solomonow, Zhou, Bratta, & Burger, 2003). These biological mechanisms are known to be e physiologically protective (McGill, 2006). Now becoming evident through broader inquiry and further epidemiological investigation, some other common 214 and perhaps more likely determinants have been found by identifying some re-appearing observational factors being recurrent in large populations (N=480) of both men and women where increased occurrence of first-time back troubles become correlated to recurrent episodes. They are listed as follows: 1. Larger amounts of spine mobility with aberrant motor patterns, and 2. Less lumbar extensor muscle endurance. Controlling for other factors, these findings were cited as significant independent factors (Biering-Sorensen, 1984; Luoto, Heliövaara, et al., 1995), particularly with regard to endurance factors about the back. McGill (2007) therefore surmises that “muscular endurance appears to be more protective,” but adds the caveat that “these may be only randomly associated (co-present) in people with poor motor control systems” (p. 13). Since trunk muscular endurance has been timed, measured, and quantified by seconds as the primary unit of measure in original studies, it can thus be measured and assessed in repeat studies via the use of a simple timer or stopwatch. While original work by Biering-Sorensen (1984) showed that decreased trunk extensor endurance was most predictive of who would be at greater risk of developing future back troubles, “more recent work has suggested that the balance of endurance among the torso flexors, extensors, and lateral musculature better discriminates those who have back troubles from those who do not” (McGill, 2006, p. 230). McGill (2006) further stated that “because these three muscle groups are involved in spine stability during virtually any task, the endurance should be measured in all three” (p. 230). McGill et al. (2007) went on to develop a series of timed endurance tests for Flexion, Extension, and Lateral musculature about the trunk and torso. Each component for torso orientation was proven to have a high reliability coefficient of at least .98 or higher when repeated over five consecutive days and across eight weeks when re-administered to same 215 sample participants and between separate test operators. Examples of each test position used in the current study are depicted in Figure 49 and the format procedure form that was used for repeat data collection during the current study are found in Appendix K. The timing from start to finish for holding each sustained position time during each endurance test was measured in a 00:0.00 seconds level of accuracy using a single user’s stopwatch feature on the iPhone Model 6s (Apple Corporation, USA) for each study participant and for each repeated test. This instrumentation is shown in Figure 50. 216 (a) (b) (c) Figure 49. Demonstration of McGill’s Timed Endurances Tests. Examples of each test position used in the current study for conducting McGill’s Timed Endurance Tests for quantifying spine function muscle endurance for both groups: (a) Trunk Flexor endurance for maintaining 45° to 50 ° trunk angle to hips while starting from wedge backrest supported to backrest unsupported; and then, (b) Lateral Trunk muscle endurance (modified planking); first in side-lying Right, then side-lying Left with hips/ knees in 90° semi-flexed position, and raising lateral side of pelvis off table surface by rolling hips forward onto knees, and with uninvolved contralateral hand placed across chest to opposite shoulder; and finally to (c) Trunk Extensor endurance testing from prone on elbows in contact with table surface as start position (over standard-sized OPTP Pro Balance Pad™ as soft, but supportive fulcrum placed from infra-costal / abdominal margin superiorly to anterior pelvis/supra-pubic margin inferiorly), and then to maintain sustained full extension with elbows off table as the timed position. The firm and predictive table surface used during all baseline measures and repeated testing (as pictured), and during the course of most treatment and 217 training interventions for both groups is shown here as The Astra-Lite™ Mat Table, Watsonville, CA, USA. The Airex®/OPTP Pro Balance Pad™ is directly sourced from http://www.OPTP.com, Minneapolis, MN, USA. Figure 50. Demonstration of Stopwatch Instrumentation. McGill’s Timed Endurance Tests were conducted via stopwatch application measured in hundredth seconds’ intervals from continuously updated iOS software via iPhone Model 6s (Apple Corporation, USA). Flexion/Extension Endurance Ratios as an added Qualifying Measure for Trunk Control Through laboratory implementation and further intention for the formulation of clinical applications targets, McGill et al. (2007) derived normative data for absolute endurance times from a collection sampling of young, healthy, college-aged individuals (mean age of 21 years old, N=92 men, N=137 women). Subsequently, they noted that women have greater endurance than men in sustaining extensor activation. Later, they found that men with onset and history of back troubles had demonstrated a lingering upset in muscle imbalance wherein extensor endurance is diminished in comparison with flexors and lateral trunk musculature of the same cohort without back problems. Using this total sample cohort of workers from the same 218 workplace (N=24 who never had back troubles compared to N=26 who had lost work due to LBP), they differentiated flexion/extension ratios of .71 for the asymptomatic group as compared to 1.15 for the LBP (off work) group. Through this, they concluded that “interpreting absolute endurance is probably secondary to interpreting the balance among the three muscle groups” (McGill et al., 2003, p. 233), and that a discrepancy of ratio between flexion and extension endurance at greater than one (greater than 1.0) would suggest an unbalanced endurance in an individual. A systematic review by May et al. (2006) revealed that there is moderate evidence in favor of high reliability of timed muscle endurance tests as compared to most other physical and manual testing procedures being routinely used in the physical examination of non-specific low back pain. However, considering that the "normative" sample data values attained from McGill et al. (McGill et al., 2003; McGill & Karpowicz, 2009) were derived from "young healthy adults" and also from "a cohort of workers with and without LBP, but from an industrial facility," and that the population sample of study participants with CNSLBP would likely be much more variant in usual activity levels (with predicted long histories of sedentary lifestyle and pain activity-avoidant behavior), my research team opted accordingly to adjust the achievable flexion/extension endurance ratio from less than 1.0 to less than 1.5 to more closely validate and represent a clinical population of persons perhaps more chronically afflicted with back pains; and associated longer-term declinations in both motor control and their capacity for performing usual physical endurance functions. Likewise, the procedures used in my current study for application of McGill’s Timed Tests for Assessing Muscular Endurance for Low-Back Health, which outlines the parameters used for testing unsupported, static hold time of endurance tolerance for four sustained positions 219 (Flexion, Right lateral side-planking, Left lateral side-planking, and Extension), as found in Appendix K, were also adjusted and modified in consideration to begin at "beginner’s level" as is actually demonstrated in Figure 49 by: 1. Allowing extension to occur from a neutrally-placed (e.g., non-hyper flexed) baseline position; and with 2. Side-lying lateral trunk endurance testing being initiated from a modified bent knee position of 45 to 90 degrees’ hip-knee flexion, and rolling forward into side bridging (in lieu of exerting a direct dead lift of side torso with hips and knees being fully extended, as in advanced side-bridging or planking). In his reference textbook and training manual, Ultimate Back Fitness and Performance (2nd edition), McGill (2006) demonstrates support for valid accommodation of modified testing and training positions in reporting that “pushing hips forward, as in beginner’s side bridge (figure 10.52) elevates all muscle activation levels (about the trunk)” (p. 299). As well, that the same neuromuscular activity patterns demonstrated to occur under the hips extended condition had also occurred in similar proportion of distribution under the modified hips flexed testing conditions being co-represented synergistically for: internal/external oblique muscle groups; ipsilateral rectus abdominis, corresponding gluteus medius; and ipsilateral (ground-sided) latissimus dorsi when examined concurrently for quantifying the relative distribution of comparative microvoltage intensities. This, while under the surveillance of task-specific electromyography and via their use of multiple channel surface electromyography (S-EMG) instrumentation sensors being standardly placed at site-specific areas being considered as "prime movers" for the lateral trunk side-lift task, whether at beginner’s adjusted level or at advanced traditional (cardinal frontal plane) level. 220 I then, by implementing trunk position endurance test hold-times as a physical performance objective measure, modified the parameters of both the task performance itself and for the designation of optimal performance ratios, so as to more validly meet the projected constraints occurring within a clinical sub-population of patients who would most likely present with longer-term episodes of chronic low back pain (CNSLBP) and co-morbid states of deconditioning; and by further stating that these adjusted and modified standards at baseline had also stayed consistent throughout all levels of continued data sampling and throughout the entire course of the study. These are discussed and displayed in the results and discussion sections of this dissertation. Methodological Considerations, Determining Minimally Relevant Clinical Change Consideration was made to determine how expected improvements for both groups could be interpreted or considered as clinically relevant from the outcome measures that have been already outlined, validated, and selected. A collaborative study resulting from the proceedings of The VIII International Forum on Primary Care Research on Low Back Pain (Amsterdam, Denmark, June 2006) provided a systematic literature review and the formation of an expert panel to put forward an international consensus for interpreting a basis for minimal important change scores (MIC), and for demonstrating a threshold that would identify clinically meaningful improvement on each of the most commonly cited measures for pain and functional status in low back pain. This study covered the European version of the Visual Analogue Scale (0-100) and the Numerical Rating Scale (0-10) for pain, and the Roland Disability Questionnaire (0-24) for assessment of self-reported disability. Proposed outcome results recommended MIC values as a change in 15 points for the Visual Analogue Scale (0-100), as contrasted by two for the Numerical Rating Scale (0-10), and 221 five points for the Roland Disability Questionnaire. The general consensus was that for the range of most commonly used back pain outcome measures, a 30% change from baseline may be considered clinically meaningful improvement when comparing before and after measures for individual patients (Ostelo et al., 2008). Since then, a revised construct for minimal clinically important difference (MCID) has been defined as “the smallest difference that patients and clinicians perceive to be worthwhile” (Maughan & Lewis, 2010). For the scales and questionnaires most commonly used for Chronic Nonspecific Low Back Pain (CNSLBP), Maughan and Lewis (2010) had listed the relevant criteria of change for citing the benchmarks of discerning “minimal clinically important difference” for each of the test instruments being used in the current study as: ● A point score change of 2.4 on the VAS (NRS), ● A point score change of 5.0 on the RMDQ, and ● A point score change of 1.4 on the PSFS corresponded to the MCID. Applications of minimal clinically important difference (MCID) are cross evaluated with the results attained from the current study, and with that of data attained from another previously published and preceding study, in the discussion section of this dissertation. Overview of Interventions and Phase Progressions during Course of Study Patients as consenting participants in clinical research for chronic nonspecific low back pain (CNSLBP) were randomly assigned to receive either Core Stabilization Biofeedback plus Motor Control Exercises (CSB/MCE) as the more conventionally accepted intervention, or an original, newly devised, and decisively non-conventional intervention, Body Schema Acuity Training (using the VRB3 method™) plus Feldenkrais® Movements (VRB3/ FM). Both the control group and the experimental group interventions were supplemented with inclusions and 222 provisions for participants to practice their newly learned skills within the cognitive-behavioral and pain neuroscience education contexts of a graded activity paradigm as could be made applicable within their daily and/or previously un-avowed life activities. Phase Progressions for Experimental Group The experimental group (VRB3/FM) participated in a newly devised Virtual Reality Bones™ (VRB3)™ protocol using (a) skeletal density-based, full scale anatomical models, combined with (b) visual motion trajectory skeletal avatars, and (c) haptic self-touch as methods intended for body schema and joint acuity training for the first four preliminary sessions as Phase I of the total intervention. They then underwent further awareness entrainment through eight sessions of a corresponding developmental progression of Feldenkrais® Movements, with four sessions for Phase II (emphasizing foundation of ground support) and four sessions for Phase III (emphasizing reciprocating variations of motion trajectories) as themes being delivered through both Functional Integration® (FI®) and Awareness Through Movement® (ATM®) components of the Feldenkrais Method®; however, specifically emphasizing pelvis-hip relationships opposite righting (vestibular) responses of head and purposely ignoring any specific directed attention being isolated to the lumbar spine itself. Phase Progressions for Control Group The matched control comparison group (CSB/MCE) followed a core-stabilization training protocol emphasizing biofeedback assisted specific recruitment of Transverse Abdominis (TrA) and Lumbar Multifidus (LM) muscle groups as an isolated differentiation from global, multi-joint movement using a Stabilizer™ Pressure Biofeedback Device (PBU) positioned under support surfaces in (a) supine, (b) prone, (c) sitting/quadruped, and (d) sidelying/standing positons for the first four preliminary sessions as Phase I of the total intervention. 223 These prerequisite skills were then carried over into maintaining a differentiated quality of core muscular contraction becoming generalized into Static (Phase II) and Dynamic/Rhythmic (Phase III) motor control activities for the remaining 8 sessions and were deemed as necessary pre-sets for properly performing motor control exercises as per The Queensland Model of Therapeutic Exercise for Lumbopelvic Stabilization in Low Back Pain. Control group participants were explained the anatomical rationale for specific activation of TrA and LM muscles as a mechanism of control and containment for unstable and/or hypermobile spine segments in the lumbar-pelvic region being suspected as the underlying cause of low back pain. Time Course for Progression of Interventions, Session Content, and Flow of Study Interventions for both groups were delivered and progressed at two times per week for the first month (Phase I and Phase II), and one time per week for the second month (Phase III) for a total of 12 sessions over eight weeks. As an essential qualitative control for both comparative conditions, each intervention’s background theme, goal, or intent remained internally consistent and/or otherwise objectively constant throughout all phases of the study; thereby, preserving a methodological basis for maintaining a significant theoretical and operational (if not also a correspondingly antithetical) difference between treatment groups. Appendix P contains a three-part "side by side" set of detailed description tables for each phase of each intervention program for each group, in addition to detailing the content of each session for each group. The source references for replicating the control group’s intervention progression are cited and embedded in each table for each session. Embedded within the very next sub-section of this methodology chapter follows an additional set of tables with visual depictions for each phase level of progression and with treatment intentions being outlined for each succession of training and exercise program contents 224 that are being delivered to the control group. After first describing the procedure for core stabilization biofeedback via the use of a pressure biofeedback unit (PBU), these combined lumbar core stabilization/motor control exercise progressions are shown in Table 7, and continuing through Tables 8 and 9. Next, follows some image references and review sources for depicting the experimental group’s intervention progression as they appear in Table 10, Tables 11 and 12 as well as in Appendices P and Q. It is certainly worth mentioning in this methodology chapter’s sub-section that the overall complexity and originality of the experimental group’s intervention design remains and necessitates a situation such that a specialized brand of skill development and demonstrated return competence is required for this study to ever be re-implemented or re-replicated. These have been amply described in the evolution of visual-haptic techniques sub-section of the literature review (Chapter 2) of this dissertation. However, as Principal Investigator and Originator, I would anticipate a four- to six-day training program for Phase I skill-set clinician development and another two weekends of two days each for Phase II and Phase III being taught by either myself or another qualified Guild Certified Feldenkrais® Method Practitioner (GCFP®), in combination with being a dually credentialed Licensed Physical Therapist. Justifications of further need and necessity for the development, inclusion, and implementation of more complex physical therapy and rehabilitation interventions, particularly for applications to complex multi-factorial and multi-regional conditions like chronic musculoskeletal pain syndromes - including CNSLBP - are emphatically and adamantly put fourth within the discussion and conclusion sections of this dissertation. A highly detailed and comprehensive Study Flow Diagram for outlining the sequence of progression for the current study is otherwise appended for visual reference in Appendix V. 225 Control Group Intervention, Sources, and Procedure In designing the study intervention for the control group, Phase I addressed the question and intent for devising an intervention to correct for lumbar spine segmental instability as a primary and suspected contributor to CNSLBP. The control group (CSB/MCE @ N=15) underwent initial PT assessment by a qualifiedly trained team of physical therapists - not otherwise employed nor directly affiliated with usual therapy operations - to determine active motion and passive stability tests as delivered through a provided source manual and CD program authored by a national instructor for CEUs/CMEs, and entitled as Facilitation and Training Techniques for Core Stability (Hanney, 2009). These initial assessment techniques and the entire control group’s treatment progression were largely derived from this source manual, and were conducted in a manner congruent with The University of Queensland Australian Model for lumbo-pelvic stabilization and motor control exercises. These source manuals plus CD are shown in Figure 51. Figure 51. Control Group Intervention Sources. Course Flyer, Audio Program, and CD Instruction Course Manual by Hanney (2009) for use by all physical therapists who were contracted to conduct and deliver the control group’s (CSB/MCE) intervention. Also used as a supplementary resource was a popular book and exercise progression manual for Spinal Stabilization and Back Pain by Jemmett (2003). 226 However, the (CSB/MCE) group also received added instruction in specialized training for ‘core activation’ techniques using a specific biofeedback device - in addition to implementing the manual hands-on facilitation techniques being taught through the course manual. Initial therapy assessment techniques included the intake of a standard medical history and a generalized physical therapy screening. More specific to the intervention, control group physical therapists were required to perform an AROM spine motion screen to detect for spinal motion instabilities. This procedure – prior to implementing the added testing and training procedures being used with the biofeedback device - is demonstrated and depicted below in Figure 52. (a) (c) (b) (d) Figure 52. Trunk Range of Motion and Segmental Hypermobility Testing. (a) right sacral-iliac joint, (b) spine flexion and extension, (c) side bending right and left, and (d) trunk rotation left and right. The clinician observes for excessive mobility and/or areas of abrupt focal change in continuity of spine angle (i.e., apex angles); being indicative in the core stabilization model as areas of segmental hypermobility due to ligamentous instability vs. deficits in neuromuscular motor control. These areas are demonstrated for each picture via the addition of blue arrows. 227 Stabilization Biofeedback Device The Core Stabilization Biofeedback Protocol, as it is used in this study, is consistent with previous studies using a pressure biofeedback unit (PBU) in both device design parameters and in its clinical application. In particular, the Stabilizer™ Pressure Bio-feedback Unit (by Chattanooga Group, Hixon, Tennessee [TN], USA) is most commonly used in North America and internationally for purposes of providing feedback to ensure quality and precision in exercise performance and movement training and testing and by better assuring the proper selection of TrA and LM "stabilizer muscles" during motor control exercises. Conceptually originating out of Australia and further embellished by design teams through physical therapy clinical application and research applications, it is described as a simple device, which registers changing pressure gradients in a closed cell flexible chamber, much like an inflated sphygmomanometer or blood pressure cuff. Parameters can be set such that more excessive aberrations of segmental mobility occurring across lumbar spine segments (i.e., excessive movements that de-stabilize and deviate from the preferred actions of proximal stabilizer muscle groups around the spine column) can become literally inflated and readily amplified across a contained surface area. As a result, they are detected, monitored, and acquiesced from oscillatory deviations of a sensitive needle indicator being correspondingly represented on a pressure gauge, and thus inhibited, quieted, contained and corrected. A sample picture of the device is shown below in Figure 53 and again in Appendix L. 228 Figure 53. The Stabilizer™ Pressure Bio-feedback Unit. The Stabilizer™ Pressure Bio-feedback Unit (by Chattanooga Group, Hixon, TN, USA) is used as a surface contact pressure transducer that conforms between hard surfaces and adjacent body regions at mid-trunk - but especially at lumbar spine curvature - for learning to limit and prevent excessive movement deviations between spine segments that are produced by over-active superficial multi-joint muscles - of which also correspondingly exert higher pressures upon the pressure biofeedback unit itself. By maintaining a baseline pressure setting at a constant level during limb movements and activity perturbations (typically cited at 40 mm Hg as goal), the user is receiving positive feedback on how to invoke and select a bias for recruiting and developing activations for the deeper stabilizer muscle groups that are deemed responsible for stabilizing (i.e., not moving) the position of vertebral segments – namely by biasing a selective contraction of Transverse Abdominis and Lumbar Multifidus (TrA and LM) intersegmental muscle groups. The PBU’s inclusion as a cited training component had previously proved useful for significantly demonstrating that segmental stabilization is superior to superficial muscle activation and global strengthening for all measured outcome variables for pain and disability in a previous study for chronic low back pain; and that usual superficial strengthening using a control group did not improve their TrA activation capacity (França et al., 2010; França et al., 2012). The PBU testing and training instrument has also been validated by ultrasonography imaging and electromyography tests that are considered to be the gold-standard measurements of TrA performance. A separate study referenced effective utilization of the pressure biofeedback device (PBU) during biofeedback-assisted lumbar stabilization training to inhibit and control against unwanted lateral pelvic tilt by demonstrating that gluteus medius and internal oblique activity could be significantly activated, while simultaneously differentiating a significant reduction and 229 inhibition of quadratus lumborum activity for effective "motor control" during a repeated side lying hip abduction task (Cynn et. al., 2006). A demonstration of this technique is depicted in Figure 54. Figure 54. Demonstration of PBU Biofeedback. The intent is to inhibit unwanted quadratus lumborum over activity on either side during repeated side-lying leg lift activities while lying on right side. Thus, the use of a pressure biofeedback unit (PBU), commercially known as The Stabilizer™ are used as a widely recognized tool for facilitating optimal selection and sub-maximal contraction of TrA and LM and as the method for “Core Stabilization Biofeedback” in the current study. Core Stabilization Biofeedback Protocols using the PBU Device The PBU Stabilizer™ was instituted along with tactile palpation to train subjects to develop proper contraction of TrA and LM beginning the first day during session one, and its continued training and use during the control group's entire Phase I intervention is summarized in Table 2. According to its protocol originators, Richardson et al. (1999), the normal PBU response deviations at start of treatment range from -4 to -10 mmHg; and through later research measures conducted by Hodges et al. (2004), the composite mean normal values at baseline were around -5.82 mmHg. The positions of body orientation and placement for the device for each of 230 the first four sessions is shown in Figure 55. These have also correlated to 30% - 40% of perceived maximal voluntary contraction. (a) (b) (c) (d) (e) Figure 55. Demonstration of PBU Biofeedback Procedure in Multiple Positions. The positions of body orientation and placement for the Stabilizer™ PBU/biofeedback device for each of the first four treatment sessions are shown in consecutive order for sessions 1 through 4 in conjunction with Phase I of the control group intervention: (a) placement of unit is horizontally across lumbar vertebrae levels between T 12 and L5-S1, such that the pressure gauge settings and changes are available for visual feedback monitoring in dominant hand; (b) supine placement is set at baseline to 40 mm Hg prior to activating "abdominal wall draw-in maneuver" 231 to co-activate pelvis floor and TrA stabilization contractions without added motion pressure being imparted to the device. Also, adding-in leg lift loading maneuver while maintaining the 40 mmHg threshold setting – both with and without external feedback from device gage; (c). Prone placement under abdomen is inflated to 70 mm Hg adjusted baseline, wherein appropriate abdominal draw-in maneuver should decrease pressure reading by only 6-10 mm Hg; (d) and (e) Sidelying and Sitting positions are again adjusted to the 40 mmHg baseline mark prior to directing its maintenance during de-stabilizing leg-lift movement perturbations. All contractile movements and stabilization progressions are held for 10-15 seconds each while breathing normally. They are ordinarily performed for 10 sets of repeated activation for 10 seconds each (Stabilizer™ Pressure Bio-Feedback Operating Instructions booklet, Chattanooga Group of Encore Medical, 2005). Phase Progression and Content Sourcing for the Control (CSB/MCE) Group The remaining motor control exercise and conditioning interventions then proceeded with each control group participant moving through the course delineated stages for achieving a phase level of progression as cited in the manual and CD program by Hanney (2009): ● Phase I: Core Initiation ● Phase II: Static Core Stability ● Phase III: Dynamic Core Stability ● Phase IV: Reactive Core Stability Added recommendations were made to encourage hands-on manual therapy contact and muscle palpation skills for purposes of teaching appropriate selections of touch contact for facilitating the TrA and LM "core muscle group activations," inclusive to training and feedback on the Stabilizer™ PBU device as applied during Phase I. The second Phase of the control group intervention (Phase II) involved the transferability of core-stabilization skills into more functional and gym-based motor control activities into static postural, then dynamic mobility tasks, before progressing toward Phase III, wherein more dynamic activities (use of physioballs and rollers, etc.) were expanded to higher level "dynamic reaction" and "rhythmic recovery" activities against unexpected perturbations and in response to 232 greater, more diverse forms of external resistance. Individually contracted physical therapy clinicians were advised to progress each patient- participant according to individual patient tolerance and in accord to their own clinical and professional judgement. The next three visual demonstration tables consolidate the contents and activity themes for Phase I, II, and III of the treatment progressions delivered to the CSB/MCE control group. 233 Table 7 Phase I CSB/MCE Treatment Progression: Core Stabilization and Motor Control Exercise Interventions at 2xs per Week for First Two Weeks Visit 1: TrA and Pelvis Floor Draw-in Maneuvers Supine Abdominal Draw-in Manuever to Activate TrA and Pelvis Floor without de-stabilizing spine chain through overactive superficial muscles Heel slides and Arm Raises Also performed in sitting and standing positions Visit 2: Activating Lumbar Multifidus (LM) The therapist palpates to facilitate the multifidus. Intent is to teach patients to learn to use the multifidus muscles at will and separately from other extensor muscles. Visit 3: Corset Action of TrA and LM Combined Common “Dead bug” and “Bridging” Exercises Visit 4: Lateral Trunk Stabilization Goal or intent is to control the quadratus lumborum and lateral fibers of the oblique abdominals. 234 Table 8 Phase II CSB/MCE Treatment Progression: Static and Dynamic Motor Control Exercise administered at 2x per Week for Second Two Weeks Visit 5: Prone over physioball and ground Arm Bridge static holding over physioball . ...and hip extension holding over platform Visit 6: Supine Static Challenge Single Leg Lift Visit 7: Seated and Standing Static Challenge Visit 8: Quadruped Static Challenges Supine Static Challenge Double Leg Lift Side-lying Static-Dynamic Position Challenges Quadruped to Kneeling Dynamic Challenges (Classic “Bird Dog” Position) 235 Table 9 Phase III CSB/MCE Treatment Progression: Dynamic and Reactive Motor Control Exercise administered at 1x per Week for Last Four Weeks Visit 9: Supine Reactive Challenges on Physioball Visit 10: Kneel and Quadruped Resistance Drills Visit 11: Sitting and Side-Bridging Resistance Visit 12: Squatting and Deep Sitting Drills Dynamic Bridging in Extended Leg Lift Prone and TrA / LM Extension Drills Side-planking over Physioball Extended Flexor Extensor Bridge Stability + Review of Home Exercise and Principles + Review of Home Exercise and Principles 236 At the conclusion of each treatment visit, each participant from the CSB/MCE group received printed copies of home exercises selected by their individual therapist. In addition, each participant received a "Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet" to delineate specific adherence requests and to convey some imagery specific "core principles" for both attitudinal mindset and physical practice during each corresponding phase of the treatment progression. Accordingly, and by verbatim, patients as participants were asked to adhere to the following behavioral orientations on the home exercise program (HEP) cover sheets: 1. “At least once daily, practice those exercises that you feel gave you the best sense that you were strengthening your core- and for which you were able to maintain stability and control of your mid-section at all times.” 2. “Hold for 10 Repetitions at 10 seconds each- without losing proper form - at least once daily.” 3. “Throughout the day, practice your draw-in maneuver the same way you felt it happen when using the biofeedback device.” 4. “Maintain your draw-in maneuver before each progressive fitness exercise and whenever you think something might be strenuous –so as to maintain stability and control.” 5. “As much as possible, get up and go somewhere, and try to spend a little bit more time enduring the activities of daily life.” Copies of each "Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet" being allocated for each primary phase of treatment progression for each group can be found in Appendix R. Furthermore, at each return visit for each phase of treatment, patients as participants were again asked to track their adherences to home exercise and graded activity progressions as well as for charting any significant changes in reducing the dosage and/or frequency of their current pain 237 medication regimen. Taking the format of a "Graded Activity Diary," and being specifically scaled to each phase of treatment for each group, these form samples are sequentially outlined in Appendix R. Experimental Group Intervention, Sources, and Procedure Again, as with the control group, the initial therapy assessment included the intake of a standard medical history and a generalized physical therapy screening interview and cursory exam. However, in designing the comparative study intervention for the experimental group, Phase I addressed the question of devising a novel alternative to traditional "core stabilization training" and "motor control exercise" programs, and to function as a viable and competing approach in such manner that it was decisively antithetical to usual and customary "gym-based" physical therapy exercise and standardized performance prescriptions in its overall design, purpose, and intent. By having no indication for pre-designating a specific pathology, nor by identifying a focal neuromuscular deficit or muscle weakness as cause, nor by otherwise implicating some other particular structural-functional mechanism or compartmentalized movement aberration at fault, the newly devised VRB3 approach at Phase I instead outlined some components and parameters for reconstituting an improved perceptual awareness for "coming to know" one’s own existing background body schema - as a whole - and improving upon its functional expression in terms of sensory acuity and motor dexterity. In lieu of practicing unidimensional standards for repeated performance, and with little to no flexibility for essential variation or constructive deviation being permitted for undoing a singular plane-based prescriptive movement pattern (being usually isolated to cardinal sagittal plane), the Feldenkrais®-based movement model instead seeks to involve all curvilinear 238 possibilities inherent to the design of the human skeleton and their concurrent relationship toward easily accessing multiple directions in space. All along with a proportionate quality of skeletal continuity such that a more effective use of whole self could substitute as a primary functional alternative to the usual guarded and antalgic compensatory movement patterns that are typically associated with an individual’s body presentation occurring between baseline rest postures and movement in CNSLBP. Embodied Perceptual Assessment of Background Body Schema and its Correlation to Action The experimental (VRB3/FM) group (N=15) thus underwent a different kind of preliminary "physical exam" in order to simply discern the capacity to shift weight in multiple planes and to discover a preference bias for leg support from head to foot and vice-versa in both standing and supine positions as well as assessing for weight bear biases in sitting. In other words, the preliminary pre-treatment assessment was designed for involving both practitioner and participant to collaborate upon a mutual interactive inquiry to ask as to How do individuals [in the VRB3/FM group] habitually support themselves through their skeleton, and how and where do they deviate or gravitate their changing center of mass in relation to changing base of support, and how can this "dynamic stability quality" vary as an acquired or preferred pattern within an incessant and constant gravitational field? Upon determining a bias for accessing some areas of space in favor to avoidance to others, the use of an imaginary "angels halo" and a series of imaginary "hula hoops" were used as imagery constructs to enable an internal/external shared reference of three-dimensional discernment for circumferential access to space; increasingly delineated by way of an imaginary "clock dial" to demarcate areas of accessibility vs. areas of avoidance in weight shift. Beginning at "top of head" and ending at "base of feet" these preliminary assessment procedures are demonstrated in Figures 56 and 57. Though not pictured, these same "top-down/hands-on" 239 facilitated explorations also occur in the functional contexts of sitting upon either a flat chair or a Feldenkrais Method® table. (a) (b) Figure 56. Preliminary Physical Exam for Conducting the Initial Assessment for the (VRB3/FM) Experimental Group. (a) Standing Alignment from a hands-on top down direction with guided imagery being converted from a usual spine curves and joint angles perspective to an angel’s halo, circumferential rim of a clock dial and hula hoop perspective for assessing an individual’s ability to access three dimensional space; (b) Comparative Rotation around three cardinal axial- 240 planes in conjunction with ground reaction support - as analagous to three semi-circular canals of inner ear. Concurrent with assessment findings from the preliminary top-down circumferential perspective, both a skilled practitioner and an attentive patient/client can typically co-discover a side of avoidance as compared to a side of preference for leg support. Even more typical for CNSLBP or any back-related condition, a corresponding dimension of reduced tactile contact acuity can also be unveiled and co-detected by the use of a mesh screen seated back rest/support surface device to reveal a uniquely palpable surface contact impedance vs. vagueness phenomenon being largely correspondent to the same side of the stance avoidance leg. This simple contact technique being vertically applied across back of the torso in a cephalic-caudal descending direction para-spinally, and by comparing for friction-like impedances vs. foggy-like amorphous qualities at left vs. right sides, is demonstrated in Figure 57. Figure 57. Demonstration of Sensory Acuity Impedances at Dorsal Spine. A mesh-screen back rest product was used as a contact surface amplifier for the detection of inter-subjective sensory acuity differences and impedences occurring between left and right sides of para-spinal musculature in patients with CNSLBP while standing. 241 While seemingly a subjectively implied phenomenological event, the timing and localization features of the maneuver seem inter-subjectively reproducible between both patient and examiner, and this tool remains the topic of reference for a future inter-rater reliability study between varied samplings of participants and examiners as well as for testing for crosscorrelations between different kinds of modalities (e.g., S-EMG recordings and force plate data). It may also serve as a possible marker for smudging phenomena being seen in the cortical brain representation studies being disruptive of body representation in chronic pain during fMRI. Next followed a "ground-up" correlative assessment maneuver via the application of a foot board contact surface being compared to test against the plantar surface receptivity of each foot – again comparing left and right - and within the context of a simulated standing positon (i.e., laying supine on back in a "gravity attenuated" rest position). Commonly known in Feldenkrais Method® -based colloquialism circles as “artificial floor” or "board on foot" lessons, these comparative sensory-motor perceptual investigations again reveal a detectable difference in quality of foot contact impedance vs. surface contact avoidance; most typically, with diminished acuity and dexterity correspondingly occurring on same side of involvement as the CNSLBP’s side of primary symptoms. This contact maneuver is visually demonstrated from various orientations of interactive inquiry and through demonstrating a longitudinal “bottom-up” skeletal perspective in Figure 58. 242 Figure 58. Demonstration of Foot Contact & Surface Acuity Procedures. The “artificial floor” or “board on foot” assessment platforms are used by Feldenkrais® Practitioners to reveal previously under-appreciated qualities of over-aversive vs. under-responsive foot and leg alignment synergies during the actual "contact simulation" of standing and pre-gait functions in a relatively non-weight bearing longitudinal orientation with respect to gravity. Very often, a non- or lesserresponsive foot contact corresponds to the same side of involvement in cases of CNSLBP for which the focus of symptoms remains more predominant to one side. My clinical practice has referred to this commonly observed contact responsiveness deficiency as a “suspended leg phenomenon” being perhaps representative of a global deficiency in body schema acuity overall. Its correlates can often also be seen upon upright clinical gait assessment via correspondingly observable deficiencies between timing and support of the more involved side. Specialized Feldenkrais Functional Integration (FI ) sessions have been known to demonstrate an overall improved response quality in both contact surface responding and in stance-propulsion during gait. ® ® By becoming emergently aware of corresponding impedances and vagueness being distributed throughout the mostly inattentive regions of the body, and at multiple levels as a continuum, the patient (as participant) develops some new appreciation for "the concept of disrupted working body schema" to serve as a basis during all future sessions in the current study. Finally, for the VRB3/FM group, no references are otherwise made during the initial assessment to evaluate or further assess for inter-segmental AROM or PROM differences at the lumbar spine level; nor for otherwise rating component motion stability vs. restriction at each spinal level; nor of muscle weakness or other "dysfunctions" being isolated or directly attributed to the lumbar spine or SI joint; nor were any of these anatomical “lumbar” or “SI Joint” regions either directly referenced as a cause, nor were they directly treated at any time for the VRB3/FM experimental group during or throughout the entire course of the current study. 243 Phase Progression and Content Sourcing for the Experimental (VRB3 / FM) Group Immediately following the preliminary physical exam and perceptual self-assessment of background body schema from a mostly vertical top-down vs. bottom-up perspective, as was just described, each succeeding session for the experimental group also involved participants engaging in a Feldenkrais Method® -based body scan - being mostly conducted while in backlying - at both the beginning and at the end for each session in order to compare for differences of session-directed thematic effects. In most situations involving chronic low back pain and other back-spine related conditions, it is not uncommon to find that persons will remain totally aversive to the prospect of extending hips and legs into a fully extended or elongated position while laying supine on back, as this is most typically accompanied by an exaggerated arching of lumbar spine away from floor and into a direction of extensor rigidity or lumbar lordosis (i.e., toward a direction of reverse curvature against the direction of gravity that is almost immediately associated with provocation low back pain and the exacerbation of bow-string-like qualities of muscle tension throughout lumbar extensors and hip flexors). This, all in a manner not unlike the body pattern of anxiety that was outlined and described earlier through referencing Feldenkrais’ (1949/2005) postulates within the "hidden senses-vestibular concept" sub-section of the "literature review chapter" embedded within this dissertation. However, subsequent to an effective Feldenkrais®-based Functional Integration® session or Awareness Through Movement® class, it is also not uncommon for such persons to implicitly re-discover a new manner for spontaneous extensor inhibition, and for truly "giving their weight to the ground" to thereby lie more fully extended, and without concurrent exacerbation of pain symptoms or exaggeration of spine curves. Figure 59 demonstrates guided arrangements for self-referencing of resting body schema during: (a) "pre-intervention body scans" requiring added accommodation and support 244 though bent knees and/or leg pillows or other external supports, vs. (b) "post-intervention body scans" requiring no added accommodation or needs for added support – in that the tonic state of the musculature is now more easily conforming to the continuous state of matching the support affordances being directly derived through the horizontal predictability of the extended floor or table firm surfaces upon which the person is resting or lying. (a) (b) Figure 59. Pre- and Post-Body Scan Techniques for Self Assessment. Pre- and post-body scan techniques for self assessment at (a) pre-intervention, and (b) post-intervention, as contrasted body scans for comparing differences. A common script for facilitating an individual’s participation toward taking a personal inventory of his or her own body awareness acuity during a Feldenkrais class, or for invoking an ® awareness dimension for locating internal references being constitutive of one’s own background body schema within the current study would be as follows: Please lay on your back with either your knees bent or your legs fully extended straight down below you. Choose whichever is most comfortable for you. Take a moment to notice where your attention automatically goes. Do you attend to areas of discomfort or to areas of ease? What is the landscape of your body to each situation? Notice the points and parts of yourself that make firm contact on the floor. Perhaps your heels, your elbows, the back of your head...the boney parts of yourself. How about the 245 pelvis? Is there more sense of weight or pressure contact behind your right buttock or back-pocket compared to your left side? Do you feel lifted or rolled to one side? Do some parts of your feel overtly held back? In ways that would seem more than necessary? Now notice areas of space behind you where your body surface is not supported. Are these areas more-clear of less-clear compared to areas that do make contact? What is the elevation and length- span of curves behind your knees? Behind your ankles? The tops of your thighs where they meet the buttocks. The curves that bridge upward and arch backward to connect between the top of your waistline to the base of your ribs - and perhaps even to the back of your shoulder girdle? How about the length of the spine between your shoulders? At what point to these vertebrae make contact? And how do these compare to the curves behind your neck? Does one side feel higher off the ground overall as compared to your other side? Again, compare left and right. Notice the length of each leg from hip to heel. Does one heel feel heavier than the other...with more pressure behind it...and what part of the heel? From this, can you sense what direction each foot points in the room? And without looking at it with your eyes! Does one leg feel further from the center of your head? Where is the center of your head? – the exact geographic center? Does one leg feel more continuous than the other? Again compare left and right. If you suddenly had to respond to get up...then from which direction would you most likely roll toward? Where and how would you initiate the movement...imagine...which areas have most affinity to respond first? Which other areas of yourself drag behind? And if you were laying on a balance beam – would you automatically teeter off to one side? Left or Right? Or would you congruently counterbalance? How do you know this? How do you sense this? Does your breathing change to either side? Sense the difference between the front half of your right lung to the back half of your right lung – and compare this to your left side...Do the same thing from the center of each knee...Is one side of your entire body beginning to reveal a consistent difference? ...Most interesting is the fact that this may be the first time that you have ever encountered such noticing... and to note that the things that you are currently noticing do not show up - and will not show up - in routine X-Rays, CT-Scans, or MRI exams – but only through this kind of quality of attention. Within the actual intervention framework of the current study for the VRB3/FM experimental group, phase I - session 1, then continues forward to conduct a "virtual reality hip replacement" procedure as was aptly and most thoroughly described in the “Evolving Practice, New Visual-Haptic Techniques: The Origin of VR Bones™” sub-section of this dissertation as is extensively embedded within Chapter 2. The continuation of Phase I treatment interventions 246 also follows the same descriptive progression form the same sub-section of the same chapter, and its chief components are also again summarized and visually rendered below in Table 10. The remaining interventions then proceeded with each experimental group participant moving through Phase II and Phase III of the total intervention; this consisted mostly of Feldenkrais Method®-based movement themes and sessions that are anecdotally known to have positive effects on patients (as participants) or for clients (as students) who have presented with long histories of CNSLBP. These session content themes are consolidated and outlined below in Tables 11 and 12. Sessions were also chosen based upon their ability to adhere to some authoritative principals and stated design criteria for carrying-out and conducting an effective Feldenkrais® (2010) lesson as had been outlined in an advanced training workshop by Santa Febased Feldenkrais® Trainer, Alan Questel, in Figure 60. Figure 60. Outline of Principles that contribute to an effective Feldenkrais Method® Lesson. In addition, the Feldenkrais®-based Awareness Through Movement® and Functional Integration® movement theme interventions being delivered during Phase II and Phase III of the total intervention had also been informed and supplemented by direct personal and clinical immersive experiences with broad assortments. Assortments of commercially available 247 professional and public audio programs from Feldenkrais Method® Teachers and Trainers designed to address the problem of low back pain and its related dysfunctions. Some specific tracks and lesson themes from each audio program are more thoroughly referenced and enlarged for viewing in Appendix O. They were individually selected and disseminated to experimental group participants on a case by case basis as per therapist judgment to serve as respective "sample" components from which to further enrich their home exercise programs (HEPs) - and as a corresponding complement to the particular session theme that was clinically administered through the research protocol for that day. A photo-capture of program titles and resource materials used during the course of intervention – again as a reinforcement of their particular session theme - is otherwise pictured in Figure 61. 248 Figure 61. Resource Materials for HEP’s derived from Professional Feldenkrais® Audio Programs. 249 Table 10 Virtual Realty Bones (VRB3): Phase I Imagery Intervention for Body Schema Acuity and Skeletal Density Imagery Continuity Training (SDI) also using The Feldenkrais® Method (FM) TM Phase I (VRB3) Protocol Session Number: Visit 1 (Hip Sockets & Stilts) Visit 2 (Inner Ilia & Centaur) Visit 3 (Costo-vertebral Pedicles) Visit 4 (Temporal Bone & Ninja) Life-Sized Skeletal Models Life-sized and/or life-scaled bones as anatomical constructs for Re-constructing an alternative notion for body schema Avatar Identity Action Scenarios Feldenkrais Method® of Application Visual action theme scenarios as Corresponding theme functional-conceptual for Feldenkrais® contexts for Hands-on Movement performing an cues to optimize: imagery-inspired task 1. Haptic Self-touch or movement 2. Directed Attention 3. Connected Action 250 Table 11 Phase II Training for Experimental (VRB3/FM) Group via 2x per Week for Second Two Weeks Feldenkrais Method® Themes: Expanding Sense of Ground Support via Developmental Actions Visit 5: Prone Frog Leg Pre-Crawl Development Prone Lumbar-Pelvis-Hip Integration in 3-D Visit 6: Pelvis Wishbone Coupling and Diagonals Diagonal Rib Flexion and External Spine Visit 7:Side-Bending and Lengthening Movement Visit 8: Inverted Sitting and Kneel over Table Side-lying Leg Reaching and Wt. Shift Sitting Transition to ½ Kneeling and Contralateral Crawl 251 Table 12 Phase III Training for Experimental (VRB3/FM) Group via 1x per Week for Last Four Week Feldenkrais Method® Themes: Reciprocating Variations of Active Movement Trajectories Visit 9: Balance Beam Rotation Flexors-Extensors Prone Leg Tilt opposite Head and Eyes Visit 10: Diagonal and Ipsilateral Trunk Flexion Mid-Thoracic Extension & Contralateral Limbs Visit 11: Side-lying Leg Lift & Torso Side Bend Lateral Chair Movement & Side-Reach Synergies Visit 12: ‘Head Through Gate’ and Proportionate Arching Skeletal Transmission Resistance Contiguity + Review of Movement Strategies & Themes 252 At the conclusion of each treatment visit, each participant from the VRB3/FM group received recorded samples of "matched theme" Feldenkrais Method Awareness through ® Movement audio lessons on either CD or MP3 media as selected by their individual therapist. In ® addition, each participant received a "Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet" to delineate specific adherence requests and to convey some specific "joint acuity/skeletal density imagery" and "Feldenkrais Movement" principles for both attitudinal mindset and physical ® practice during each corresponding phase of the treatment progression. Accordingly, and by verbatim, patients as participants were asked to adhere to the following behavioral orientations on the home exercise program (HEP) cover sheets: 1. From your program today, practice the movement sequence and skeletal alignment(s) that helped you to detect or visualize the inner location of your newly discovered weight bear joints and how they dissipated stress. Include "laser beam" soft touch. 2. Can you sense it from a variety of ways in manners that "connects the dots" from bottomup and through top down? Navigate and Sense it from both directions? 3. At least once daily, follow the movement sequence that works best for one side. Find a connection that highlights your sense of constructing an inner line through your skeleton from that "standpoint." Explore it again with slight variation for 5 times. 4. Rest before repeating same strategy on opposite side, but begin first with only imagined movement. Then do. 5. Throughout each day, discover and attend to how awareness of these areas can be included in daily activities as a background support for sitting, standing, walking, etc. Be sure to maintain a flexibly aligned, softly assembled quality of being --and not a hard focus. 6. As much as possible, get up and go somewhere, and try to spend a little bit more time enduring the activities of daily life. If you feel you used less medication than usual, feel free to medicate a bit less and meditate on life a bit more. Copies of each "Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet" being allocated for each primary phase of treatment progression for each group can again be found in Appendix Q. Furthermore, at each return visit and for each phase of treatment, patients as participants 253 were again asked to track their adherences to home exercise and graded activity progressions as well as for charting any significant changes in reducing the dosage and/or frequency of their current pain medication regimen. Taking the format of a Graded Activity Diary and being specifically scaled to each phase of treatment for each group, these form samples are sequentially outlined in Appendix R. Data Collection Methods and Procedures All self-reported repeated measures from all scales and questionnaires were collected by an independent research coordinator and/or from front desk office reception staff. Each of these employment positions was occurring on a temporary basis only, and as administrative personnel, they had no particular stake or personal investment in the outcome of the research project. The research coordinator, having a BSc. in Kinesiology and with intentions to become a physical therapist, was also primarily responsible for properly conducting and overseeing the timed position endurance test protocols by McGill, and was able to demonstrate competence in providing instructions and supervision for proper performance, and for appropriately marking "start" and "finish" times with use of the stopwatch. Baseline measures of pain level, disability, functional ability, and timed-endurance tolerance were collected upon the initial administration of (a) VAS/pain scale, (b) RMDQ, (c) the PSFS, and (d) McGill’s times endurance tests, and again at intervals of two weeks, four weeks, and at eight weeks, immediately at post-conclusion of each phase of the intervention for both groups. All succeeding data collection measures of "raw data" for both groups were entered into a Microsoft® Excel spreadsheet, and these files were maintained in strict confidence to the blinded exclusion of treating therapists and principal investigator throughout the entire course of the 254 study. Only at the conclusion of data collection and at notice of study closure were files then released to principal investigator for their transmission - in their entirety- to a hired statistician for purposes of data analysis. The names of all study participants remained confidentially encrypted for each group. Photo-samples of Excel spreadsheets depicting the data collection procedures for each group, and for each phase of the study, are shown in Figure 62. Figure 62. Excel Spreadsheet Layout for Data Collection for Control Group and Experimental Group. Again, a highly detailed and comprehensive Study Flow Diagram for outlining the sequence of progression for the current study, including the time-frame margins used for all phases of data collection, is otherwise appended for visual reference in Appendix V. 255 Data Analysis using Statistical Tools and R Software Statistical analysis for this comparative study employed the use of inferential statistics between and within both groups. Specifically, the use of Wilcoxon’s Rank Sum (paired sample) test was employed to assess for statistically relevant and significant degrees of change in the ordinal, non-parametric variables occurring between study participants for repeated measures over time with regard to the VAS pain scale, the RMDQ, and the PSFS. The two-tailed t-test was paired for repeated measures to assess for comparative statistical significance of representational change over time, as reflected from the successive re-administration of the timed physical endurance (sustained position tolerance) tests by McGill as the continuing interval-based parametric variable. At approximately 1-2 months prior to end of study closure date, I attended clinical research mentorships at the 47th Annual Meeting of the Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback (AAPB) in March 9-12, 2016, in Seattle, Washington. Under the discussion and corroboration of Saybrook faculty member and chair of the school’s Mind Body Medicine Program, Donald Moss, Ph.D., it was determined a standard practice for doctoral dissertation students at Saybrook University to conduct the statistical analysis of their research data via the contracting of resource assistance and software programs at other local colleges or universities’ math and social sciences departments that were most local or accessible to the doctoral student. I was thereby able to contract the services of senior statistics tutor Samantha Coates of The University of Puget Sound, Tacoma, Washington, USA, who in consultation with my research design and her faculty, was able to make the appropriate corroboration in my original selection of statistical tools upon pre-submission to the Saybrook University IRB (SIRB) on April 22, 2015. 256 The two primary statistical analysis tools, the Wilcoxon’s Rank Sum and the two-tailed ttest, and their appropriate use for application in the current study were also approved as viable and “robust” instruments via e-mail consultations on April 26, 2016, with Saybrook University consulting statistics faculty member, Howard Barken Ph.D., and in concurrent collaboration with Samantha Coates consulting her own two faculty advisors, University of Puget Sound Mathematics Department faculty, Assistant Professor Wendy Dove, and their leading Associate Professor, James Bernhard, Ph.D. The Use of "R" Program Statistical Software The program of choice for statistical computing being used by The University of Puget Sound Mathematics Department was cited by Ms. Coates as being the open source “R” Program for its flexibility to scale to a more diverse array of individualized and systemic applications. While a majority of social science applications have traditionally implemented SPSS as the program of choice, a more recent post by data science analyst, Robert A. Muenchen, titled as "R Passes SPSS in Scholarly Use, Stata Growing Rapidly" had indicated that the open source "R" package is destined to become more and more dominant as a tool being increasingly referenced in scholarly publications by stating in a previous statistical trend study that the use of R is experiencing very rapid growth and is pulling away from the pack, solidifying its position in third place. In fact, extending the downward trend of SPSS and the upward trend of R make it likely that sometime during the summer of 2014, R (likely) became the most dominant package for analytics used in scholarly publications. (Muenchen, 2014) In an updated post, Muenchen (2015) stated that SPSS is (still) by far the most dominant package, as it has been for over 15 years. This may be due to its balance between power and ease-of-use. For the first time ever, R is in second place with around half as many articles...R has not only caught up with SPSS, but surpassed it with around 50% more job postings. 257 Seeing these trends, and verifying that R is a free, open source, software program for data science that is similar to the “big three” commercial packages: SAS, SPSS, and Stata, I approved my contractual arrangement to move forward on implementing this product for conducting the statistical analysis of study results. Data Backup and Record Retention Consistent with informed consent and the study design, a substantial amount of important data was gathered for this randomized controlled study. All data were stored electronically on an external drive, plus a cloud-secured service in a secure location for a period of seven years. After this time, the data hard drive will be destroyed and the cloud-server storage portion deleted. 258 CHAPTER 4: RESULTS Results from this RCT/comparative efficacy study demonstrated that the VRB3/FM experimental group demonstrated greater improvement in all treatment outcome measures as compared to the matched CSB/MCE control group. However, for all measures, only the Patient Specific Functional Scale (PSFS) demonstrated the most definitive statistical significance for greatest improvement at p is less than or equal to 0.05 upon more stringent non-parametric testing between groups. A more detailed synopsis of descriptive statistics between sample groups and the use of inferential statistics to extrapolate the results for application to the general population thus follows for the entire remainder of presentation for this chapter. Demographic and Medical History Profiles Between Groups Through reviewing the demographic and medical history profiles between sampled groups, it was determined that demographic and potentially confounding variables had an essentially matched distribution of allocation; and was deemed to be likely representative of the general population. Basic demographics of study participants for each group are displayed in Table 13. Table 13 Basic Demographics of Study Participants Age Gender Marital Status Work Status Bariatric Status Experimen tal Group 47.2 9 Female 6 Male 10 Single 5 Married 6 Employed 6 Unemployed 3 Retired 8 Normal Range 4 Overweight 3 with Obesity Control Group 58.4 6 Female 9 Male 7 Single 8 Married 3 Employed 8 Unemployed 4 Retired 7 Normal Range 5 Overweight 3 with Obesity 259 As could be expected, a heterogeneous sampling of patients with CNSLBP being randomly assigned to each group had problem severities, physical impairments, and functional capacities ranging from significantly disabling (i.e., limping with stooped posture and requiring a cane to stabilize gait) to frustratingly fatiguing by characterizing their symptoms as being a continuing interference in limiting their full access to their preferred quality of life, but still able to work. Fortunately, these kinds of non-predictable ranges for confounding variables were reasonably homogenous upon randomization after stratified assignment and allocation of participants into both groups. Table 14 outlines some potentially confounding variables that have been controlled through fortuitous random assignment for particular Bio-Psycho-Social, Surgical and Orthopedic situations that were found to occur as common comorbidities between study participants for both groups. Table 14 Confounding Bio-Psycho-Social, Surgical, & Orthopedic Variables of Study Participants History of Back Surgery Experimental Group 2 Yes 13 No Control Group 3 Yes 12 No Medication List for Pain 12 Yes 3 No 13 Yes 2 No 10 No 4 Actively Disabled 1 Pending 13 No 1 Actively Disabled 1 Pending 11 Yes 4 No 8 Yes 7 No 9 Yes 6 No 4 Yes 11 No 14 Yes 1 No 9 Yes 6 No Disability Status Anxiety/Depression Upper Quadrant Regional Symptoms or Diagnoses Lower Quadrant Regional Symptoms or Diagnoses 260 It is worth stating that although the mean chronological age difference between the experimental group (47.2 years) and the control group (58.4 years) reflects a differential age gap for the experimental group being essentially 10 years younger. The results of my literature review nonetheless support a more valid assertion for implicating the role of fear-avoidance beliefs as a more primary confounding and determinant variable from which to control the distribution of participants through stratified random assignment into each arm of the study. Participant Attrition and Final Distribution for Data Collection During the course of this study, and for a sub-population of consenting enrolled participants, there were various lifestyle and choice interruptions resulting in incomplete attendance to the number of intended sessions, and dropouts from the study before phase one data could even have had a chance to be accrued. These situations occurred for a variety of disclosed versus undisclosed reasons. Table 15 displays the distribution of allocation for participants who withdrew – or were withdrawn - before Phase I data collection, along with some reported reasons for doing so. Table 15 Drop-out Participants and Reasons for Leaving either at Start of Study/or prior to End of Phase I Experimental Group Control Group 4 Males 2 Males 1 Female 1. Opioid addiction relapse under medical supervision 2. “Felt uncomfortable” 3. Preferred traditional PT despite prior failures 4. Prior exposure to another Feldenkrais® practitioner; (he presented with low symptoms & high functioning) 1. No insurance 2. Boredom, treatment not helping 3. Family & work demands 261 Another minority sub-set of study participants had continued through only Phase I of the study and adhered to its corresponding provisions for data collection, but had otherwise confronted a similar set of lifestyle and choice vs. circumstantial issues that had caused them to opt out of continued study participation; thus, their being voluntarily exempted from completing Phase II and Phase III of the total intervention. Table 16 displays the distribution of allocation for participants who withdrew after Phase I data collection, but who yet completed the first introductory component for each intervention and data collection for each group. Table 16 Drop-out Participants and Reasons for Leaving at Conclusion of Phase 1 Data Collection - and without Completing Phase II or Phase III Components of Total Intervention Experimental Group Control Group 3 Females 1 Male 2 Females 3 Males 1. Family matters 2. Performance Anxiety to Adherence Diary 3. Dissonance and overwhelmed 4. Schedule conflict that was unexpected 1. Family matters 2. Transportation issues 3. Schedule conflict that was unexpected 4. Onset of other more urgent health problem 5. Too busy In sum and for both groups, approximately 2/3 of participants (N=10 for the control group and N=11 for the experimental group) completed the entire intervention (Phase I – III). However, the remaining 1/3 of participants (N=5 for the control group and N= 4 for the experimental group) completed only the Phase I preliminary component of the study before dropping out. Participant attrition was thus essentially similar for both groups at a reasonable margin of plus or minus one. Again, a highly detailed and comprehensive Study Flow Diagram 262 for outlining the sequence of progression for the current study, including an outline for participant attrition at each phase, is appended for visual reference in Appendix V. Central Tendency (Mean) and Distribution of Data (SD) across Phases of Treatment The statistical software ‘R’ was used to conduct all statistical analysis and to create all tables and graphical displays (R Core Team, 2015). These were tabulated and compiled by senior statistics tutor, Samantha Coates, of The University of Puget Sound, Tacoma, WA, USA, and were submitted for assessment and review for the dissertation write-up on April 30, 2016. Over the course of eight weeks and for all phases of treatment intervention, the experimental group showed improvements in VAS-Pain (from 6.07 plus or minus 1.58 points to 2.00 plus or minus 1.55 points), RMDQ-Disability (from 11.40 plus or minus 6.17 points to 3.91 plus or minus 4.01 points), and PSFS-Function scores (from 3.33 plus or minus 1.51 points to 7.08 plus or minus 1.56 points). With much wider variation, the Timed Endurance Test totals also improved (from 160.32 plus or minus 146.55 seconds to 198.58 plus or minus148.30 seconds). The control group showed improvements in VAS-Pain (from 5.60 plus or minus 2.53 points to 3.00 plus or minus 2.96 points), RMDQ-Disability (from 12.40 plus or minus 5.80 points to 8.56 plus or minus 6.73 points), and PSFS-Function scores (from 3.93 plus or minus 1.73 points to 5.26 plus or minus1.77 points). With again similarly wide variation, the Timed Endurance Test totals also improved for the control group (from 85.01 plus or minus 84.91 seconds to 106.69 plus or minus 48.03 seconds). These extrapolated findings for the tabulation and calculation of Mean and Standard Deviation occurring between all measures as they were conducted between the raw data sampling of responses from the both the experimental group and the control group over time - from baseline to eight weeks - are further displayed for all phases of treatment in Table 17. 263 Table 17 Calculation and Display of Mean Scores and Standard Deviations for all Primary Outcome Measures that occurred between Experimental Group and Control Group for the duration of the Current Study Measurement Scale Pain (VAS) Baseline 2 weeks (Phase 1) 4 weeks (Phase 2) 8 weeks (Phase 3) Disability (RMDQ) Baseline 2 weeks (Phase 1) 4 weeks (Phase 2) 8 weeks (Phase 3) Function (PSFS) Baseline 2 weeks (Phase 1) 4 weeks (Phase 2) 8 weeks (Phase 3) Endurance Test Total Baseline 2 weeks (Phase 1) 4 weeks (Phase 2) 8 weeks (Phase 3) Experiment Group SD Mean Control Group Mean SD 6.07 3.93 3.45 2 1.58 2.25 2.02 1.55 5.60 3.93 3.80 3.00 2.53 2.66 2.86 2.96 11.40 8 6.18 3.91 6.17 5.69 4.38 4.01 12.40 11.13 10.50 8.56 5.80 6.03 6.82 6.73 3.33 5.29 5.80 7.08 1.51 1.95 1.78 1.56 3.93 4.83 4.66 5.26 1.73 1.81 2.25 1.77 160.32 184.75 190.42 198.58 146.55 174.85 156.04 148.30 85.01 91.02 100.00 106.69 84.91 78.91 72.14 48.03 Graphs of the mean scores for the Visual Analog Scale (VAS), Roland Morris Disability Questionnaire (RMDQ), Patient-Specific Functionality Scale (PSFS), endurance test totals, and the corresponding flexion/extension ratios (McGill) that occurred between the control group and the experimental group over time were all created to reveal their respective and appropriate visual scales; and especially from which to also compare and display the proportionate and comparative changes that occurred between the two groups. All graphs were embellished and 264 created using the Lattice™ package (Sarkar, 2008). They are sequentially revealed as figure insets below – in sequence of order from Figures 63, 64, 65, 66, and Figure 67. 265 (a) (b) Figure 63. Mean Pre/Post Outcome Measures for VAS PAIN Over Time. (a) Bar graph, and (b) Line dot plot for Mean VAS Scores between groups over time. 266 (a) (b) Figure 64. Mean Pre/Post Outcome Measures for RMDQ DISABILITY Over Time. (a) Bar graph, and (b) Line dot plot for Mean RMDQ Scores between groups over time. 267 (a) (b) Figure 65. Mean Pre/Post Outcome Measures for PSFS FUNCTION Over Time. (a) Bar graph, and (b) Line dot plot for Mean PSFS Scores between groups over time. 268 (a) (b) Figure 66. Mean Pre/Post Outcome Measures for ENDURANCE SCORES Over Time. (a) Bar graph, and (b) Line dot plot for Mean Timed Endurance Test Totals over time. 269 (a) (b) Figure 67. Pre/Post Outcome Measures for FLEXION/EXTENSION RATIOS Over Time. (a) Bar graph, and (b) Line dot plot for viewing Flexion/Extension Ratios over time. 270 Inferential Statistics Comparing Group Differences using Non-Parametric Tests The Wilcoxon Rank Sum test, also known as a Mann-Whitney test, was deemed the most suitable test for comparing (a) Visual Analog Scale (VAS), (b) Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire (RMDQ), and (c) Patient-Specific Functional Scale (PSFS) as the efficacy-based outcome scores that had been collected between the experimental group and control group. The decision to use the Wilcoxon Rank Sum test was made because the outcome scores that were collected in the current study were derived from ordinally scaled formats of reference for measured improvement by each patient’s individual responses over time, which classifies it as nonparametric. The Wilcoxon rank sum test, a classic nonparametric test, was also selected because of the two-sample aspect of the study, ultimately seeking a valid measure for statistical significant difference between the control group and the experimental group. The Wilcoxon Rank Sum test, again being applied as a repeating statistical formula, was implemented to compare the differences between baseline scores and three phase levels of scores that had occurred between the experimental group and control group within the three different outcome data sets as mentioned above. As the preferred benchmark stated in most clinical research literature, testing at a significance level of .05 was the designated threshold that was set for all applications upon merging the three outcome measure raw-mean data sets into the Wilcoxon Rank Sum test. A Basic Statistics and Data Analysis (BSDA) software package component embedded within the R package was used as the recommended feature to conduct the test (Arnholt, 2012). In accordance with comparative statistical applications and operating procedures, a projected null hypothesis that the group labels (experimental and control) were assigned at random, but that the two groups had the same distribution of scores for each individual test. In addition, there were no differences to be found in the analysis of results was compared against 271 the alternative hypothesis, that group labels were also assigned at random, but with projected differences to be found in the analysis of results, implying that the two groups did not have the same distribution of scores for each test, and that there were compelling differences that likely exceeded usual expectation or random chance. After conducting the Wilcoxon Rank Sum test for comparing the difference of baseline scores to various phase scores between the experimental group and the control group for different measurement scales, I received only two statistically significant p-values at p < 0.05. The two significant p-values came from the comparison of "baseline to phase 2" and "baseline to phase 3" of the Patient-Specific Functional Scale (PSFS) between the two groups. The resulting p-values were about 0.038 and 0087, respectively. These p-values suggest that the control and experimental groups had significantly different distributions of the change between baseline PSFS scores and both phase two (Phase II) and phase three (Phase III) PSFS scores. In modern statistics, the Bonferroni correction is one of several methods that are used to include a provision for the problem of multiple comparisons. For example, a given study may be well powered to detect a certain effect size when only one test is to be made, but the same effect size may have much lower power if several tests are to be performed. These are correspondingly reflected as an adjusted p-value. Using the Bonferroni adjustment/Bonferroni correction method as a more stringent statistic for the prospect of multi-variate and repeating situations, I made the corresponding adjustments for multiple comparisons and repeated measures. This adjustment made the previously statistically significant p-values become not statistically significant. Under these test conditions, it infers that I can neither say that there is or is not a significant difference between 272 the experimental and control group’s change in VAS, RMDQ, or PSFS scores from the baseline to phase one (I), phase two (II), or phase three (III). However, an important criticism regarding the Bonferroni correction is that its intent to control against inflation of the Type I error rate (rejecting a null hypothesis by attributing an important significant difference – when there really is not one; i.e., a false positive) comes at the cost of increasing the probability of producing false negatives (as in Type II error: erroneously accepting the null hypothesis premise of no significant difference, when there really is one), and consequently reducing statistical power. A display of non-parametric statistical comparisons to discern p-value using Wilcoxon and Bonferroni inferential statistics is itemized for each measurement scale and across each phase of treatment as compared between both groups in Table 18. 273 Table 18 Wilcoxon Rank Sum Test and Bonferroni Adjustment Method to assess Non-Parametric Statistical Significance of p < 0.05 for VAS, RMDQ, and PSFS Scores occurring between Groups Measurement Scale Comparison Unadjusted P-Value 0.4699 0.72 0.1093 Adjusted P-Value 1 1 0.9837 Visual Analog Scale (VAS) Baseline-Phase1 Visual Analog Scale (VAS) Baseline-Phase2 Visual Analog Scale (VAS) Baseline-Phase3 Roland Morris Disability Questionnaire (RMDQ) Baseline-Phase1 0.05859 0.52731 Roland Morris Disability Questionnaire (RMDQ) Baseline-Phase2 0.08025 0.72225 Roland Morris Disability Questionnaire (RMDQ) Baseline-Phase3 0.572 1 Patient-Specific Functionality Scale (PSFS) Baseline-Phase1 0.05565 0.50085 Patient-Specific Functionality Scale (PSFS) Baseline-Phase2 0.03777 0.33993 Patient-Specific Functionality Scale (PSFS) Baseline-Phase3 0.008714 0.078426 Note. Bolded p-values are indicative of meeting or exceeding the likelihood of random chance – thus increasing the likelihood of change being directly attributable to the effects of the experimental intervention. Inferential Statistics Comparing Group Differences using Parametric Tests A paired, two-tailed t-test was used to assess relative significance of changes of the endurance test totals within the control group and the experimental group, individually. The setpoint for p-value statistical significance (depicted as alpha value: α) was once again tested at the .05 level. The null hypothesis was again stated to assume the likelihood of no difference between the baseline and phase three endurance test totals within each group, and the alternative hypothesis was there is a significant difference between the baseline and phase three endurance test totals. After conducting a matched t-test for each group, I received p-values of about 0.001 for the control group and 0.019 for the experimental group, both of which fall below .05 making 274 them both statistically significant. Therefore, I have evidence to believe that there was a significant difference between the baseline endurance test total, and phase three endurance totals that had occurred both within the control group and within the experimental group over time. A two-sample t-test was then used to compare the average change between baseline and phase three endurance test totals between groups. Again, my alpha value (α) was set at the threshold of .05. The null hypothesis test is that there is no difference in the means between the control and experimental group. After conducting this two-sample t-test, I found the p-value to be 0.7323. Therefore, I did not find statistically significant evidence against the null hypothesis, that there is no difference in the difference in baseline and phase three means between the control and experimental group. I can neither say there is or is not a difference in average change between baseline and phase three endurance test totals between the experimental group and control group. Table 19 presents p-values within and between groups using t-test parametric testing in order to discern the significance of effect change of repeated timed intervals for endurance testing over time. Table 19 Paired Two-Tailed T-Test to assess Parametric Statistical Significance of p < 0.05 for changes occurring during Timed Endurance Testing Totals over Time Measurement Scale Endurance Test Total (Control Group) Endurance Test Total (Experimental Group) Endurance Test Total (Between Groups) Comparison Baseline - Phase 3 Baseline - Phase 3 Baseline - Phase 3 P-Value 0.0011 0.019 0.7323 Note. Bolded p-values again indicate significant changes – but due in part to the extreme variability between participants, there was no essential difference between both groups to be directly attributable to the effects of either intervention. 275 Parametric Testing and Data Analysis for Comparing Pre-Post Flexion/Extension Ratios Table 20 and Figure 68 demonstrate a total decrease in Flexion/Extension (F/E) ratio of about seven proportions for the experimental group and a decrease of about 4.5 in the control group. However, upon again implementing the parametric two-sample t-test, the mean difference between Pre-intervention (Baseline) F/E Ratio and Post-intervention (Phase 3) F/E Ratios for the two groups, the result was found to be a p-value of .5902. Thus, despite the apparent surface magnitude of larger-scale change at face value, it was again found that that the statistical comparison result of difference was nonetheless still registering an insignificant p-value (greater than .05). Yet, upon comparing the previously cited predictive threshold for F/E ratios themselves being below 1.5 for being clinically indexed as a measure of more sufficiently balanced trunk control, it was found that 11/15 experimental group participants met this threshold index of less than 1.5 as compared to only 1/15 of control group participants, indicating greater episodic phenomena of dysfunctional trunk imbalance in the control group at post-intervention, which is itself significant. Figure 68. Bar Graph for comparing Pre- & Post-Flexion/Extension Ratios between Groups. 276 Table 20 Mean Average for Flexion/Extension Endurance Ratios at Pre & Post Intervention and Relevance to Clinically Meaningful Thresholds of < 1.5 being indicative of Improved Trunk function via reduced Agonist-Antagonist Disparity in Experimental Group as compared to Controls Control Group Experiment al Group Average Flexion / Extension Endurance Ratio at Baseline for all study participants (Baseline) 21.865 8.545 Average Flexion / Extension Endurance Ratio at Conclusion for all study participants (s/p Phase III) 17.197 1.398 Percentage of Change in Flexion / Extension Ratio from Baseline to Conclusion of Study 21.35% 83.64% 1 11 Number of Participants Concluding the Study with Clinically Relevant Flexion / Extension Endurance Ratios < 1.5* *Note. Threshold of clinical relevance for improved "trunk flexion/extension ratio" is modified and adjusted to < 1.5 for a CNSLBP population as compared to a < 1.0 threshold for collegeaged healthy controls and a less-sedentary industrial worker population (McGill et al., 2003) Purported Research Questions, Summary and Outcome for Hypothesis Again, the purpose of this single-blind, randomized controlled study (RCT) was to compare a Body Schema Acuity Training protocol using newly applied, newly developed lowcost technology (Virtual Reality Bones™/VRB3) with a respected complementary-alternative, movement and manual therapy, neuroplasticity-based educational intervention (The Feldenkrais® Method) against the most commonly accepted approach being utilized within current and conventional physical therapy practice settings (Core Stabilization Training and Graded Motor 277 Control Exercises). This was conducted for improving the outcomes on usual clinical outcome measures for CNSLBP, and to determine whether there is greater clinical efficacy being demonstrated between one combined intervention or the other for treating the widespread problem of CNSLBP as an outcome of the study itself. In accordance with traditional precepts of the scientific method, my RCT design was necessarily set-up to permit the full probability and/or possibility for demonstrating and confirming the outcome of a null hypothesis (i.e., no difference). However, the actual accrued results of the current study have instead tended more toward supporting and at least partially confirming my intuitive hunches via the projected scientific hypotheses as they were originally stated within the introduction section of this dissertation. Supported Hypothesis 1 The results support the hypothesis that a population of persons with chronic, non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) who participated in a combined Virtual Reality Bones™/Feldenkrais® Movements (the VRB3/FM group) protocol for improving body schema acuity demonstrated greater symptom reduction and greater functional improvement in all outcome measures, as compared to a similar population of persons with CNSLBP who followed a Core-Stabilization Biofeedback training/Motor Control Exercise protocol for improving motor control (the CSB/MCE group). Partially-Supported Hypothesis 2 The results did not fully support the hypothesis stating that all comparative outcome measures being used for demonstrating greater symptom reduction and greater functional improvement would all occur at a level of statistical significance being reflected at the customary 278 p-value of less than 0.05 between the experimental VRB3/FM group and the CSB/MCE control group. 279 CHAPTER 5: DISCUSSION Overview and Interpretation of Study Results As just reported, the Virtual Reality Bones™/Feldenkrais® Movement (the VRB3/FM group) protocol for improving body schema perceptual acuity and movement dexterity function demonstrated greater symptom reduction and greater functional improvement in all outcome measures, as compared to a similar population of persons with chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) who followed a Core-Stabilization Biofeedback training/Motor Control Exercise protocol. This was conducted for improving motor control (the CSB/MCE group), and with both groups being encouraged to progress their graded activity levels while simultaneously controlling for fear-avoidance via cognitive-behavioral therapy-informed qualities of therapistpatient interaction throughout the course of the study. However, only the final outcome measures for the Patient Specific Functional Scale (PSFS) were able to demonstrate superior efficacy at a p-value of 0.008714 for the VRB3/FM experimental group as compared to the CSB/MCE control group. Both Groups Demonstrating Improved Outcomes and their Shared Mechanisms of Influence Beyond the usually given explanatory rationale for positive treatment expectancy and attributions to the Hawthorne effect or observer effect (to which individuals modify or improve some aspect of their behavior just merely in response to their awareness of being attended to or observed) there are added components and variables in both groups that can perhaps account for both groups improving between their respective interventions. Both groups received preliminary "awareness affordances" that necessarily required them to pay attention to certain regions of their body in new and specified ways, and mostly (or at least more tangibly) during Phase I of the intervention. 280 The control group’s intervention and corresponding mechanisms. For the CSB/MCE control group, this "awareness affordance" was informed through specific attention to precise recruitment and activation of specific muscle groups; namely, Transverse Abdominis (TrA) and Lumbar Multifidus (LM) made tangible through visual depictions of anatomical location, using medical-anatomical nomenclature, and describing their rationale for “stabilizing and protecting the spine.” These mechanisms for experiencing a modulated quality for muscle activation more proactively, in contrast to reactive muscle spasm, guarding, or bracing, were further facilitated through implementation and use of the Stabilizer™ Bio-Feedback (PBU) device. This was conducted to accomplish a degree of trunk muscle activation and contractility specific to deep muscle groups and at a sub-maximal threshold quality that was "not too much" and "not too little," but "just right," furthermore affording an active experience through feedback that would err toward generating a cognitive orientation deemed more conducive toward conditioning an internal locus of control. The experimental group’s intervention and corresponding mechanisms. Similarly, for the VRB3/FM experimental group, relevant bodily attention to specific regional areas was again informed (however, this time through non-muscular elements by giving specific attention [or by more precisely via the acuity of attention]) to more accurately discover and locate the more exacting self-referenced locations for deep articular joints; namely, (a) deep "hip joint" femoral-acetabular axes, (b) inner ilia bridges, (c) costo-vertebral joint/thoracic pedicles, and (d) paired temporal bone/vestibular apparatus locations. This was made tangible through visual-tactile explorations and direct interactions with full-scale, life-sized, anatomical skeletal models (i.e., virtual reality bones™ applications), again using medical-anatomical nomenclature, and by furthermore describing their assembled and 281 combined rationale for “containing and aligning an improved proportion for distribution of labor throughout the entire skeleton” from a “hips-pelvis-legs/opposite head” whole body continuity of perspective, and through cultivating an awareness and use of more robust and supportive "pathways of highest bone density" throughout the body as a way to distribute everyday ground forces involved in sitting, standing, and walking, and to dissipate their tension during everyday movement; but without involving any particular attention to the lumbar spine itself; nor to involving any attribution or target contact to "muscles themselves" or to "muscle recruitment issues" as the source of the problem. These mechanisms for experiencing a modulated quality of being for ‘internal selfreferencing of whole self through both experiencing and embodying the inherent properties of skeletal contiguity’ had also demonstrated an inhibitory and proactive effect in contrast to clinical presentations of reactive muscle spasm, guarding, or bracing – as well as being antithetical to the training isolated muscle activation as the purported mechanism. Precision of synergistic actions were otherwise facilitated through implementation and use of Vicon™ Kinematic Avatar images during standing and gait, and matched-theme Feldenkrais® Movements (FM) to accomplish an enhanced degree of spatial-temporal dexterity during functional movements, and at sub-maximal thresholds of motor control such that they were "not too much" and "not too little," but "just right." Likewise, these sense and movement qualities furthermore afforded an active experience, through combined mechanisms of feedforward being altered by self-referenced visual-tactile-kinesthetic feedback (i.e., haptic self-touch) that would again err toward generating a new cognitive orientation deemed more conducive toward conditioning an internal locus of control during everyday action and living. 282 Motor control as a shared mechanism to separate pain from fear of movement? As was put fourth in the literature review earlier, chronic pain being exacerbated by movement has been proposed be the result of associative learning resulting from the concurrent pairing between movement and pain, thereby imprinting an implicitly and/or explicitly learned response that forms into a maladaptive memory for sustaining the persistence of chronic pain. In particular, during the presentation of discriminative neurotags or other aversive stimuli, fear of provocative or awkward movements being one of them. Thus, a continuing aversive association is reflected and maintained by plastic changes in the meso-limbic, sensory-motor, and prefrontal areas (Pelletier et al., 2015a). In a series of imaging experiments cross-referenced with clinical findings, Tsao et al. investigated the organization of the representation of muscles in the lumbar spine within the primary motor areas (M1) in subjects with chronic low back pain and demonstrated that concurrent corticospinal recruitment deficiencies were correspondent to smudging of cortical representations at M1 in a sample of patients with chronic or recurrent LBP (Tsao, Danneels, & Hodges, 2011; Tsao, Druitt, Schollum, & Hodges, 2010). Motor Control Exercises involving the learning of new coordination skills, inclusive of exercises to specifically recruit the Transverse Abdominus (TrA) muscle, but not a generalized walking exercise, were shown to restore the representation of lumbar cortico-spinal projections within M1 and that EMG activation patterns in CLBP subjects were also more normalized as compared to a non-treatment group of healthy controls. Thus, interventions targeted toward the improvement of coordination of movement and correspondent cortical representations would seem to be equally efficacious toward improving function and decreasing pain. 283 However, seeing that previous clinical trials have found no difference in specific motor control exercises in comparison to generalized supervised exercise and graded activity programs, an alternative effect mechanism may not be as easily discernable nor reducible to implicating a specific mechanism for ‘core musculature’ specifically. Other studies have shown that spine stabilization exercises in the treatment of chronic low back pain do not require the demonstration of improved abdominal and trunk muscle function as important factors being associated with a good clinical outcome (Mannion, Caporaso, Pulkovski, & Sprott, 2012). It has otherwise been suggested that simply moving differently from that which was previously associated with the original injury state, or as has been otherwise continuously conditioned through repeated cycles of pain and avoidance, is a simple and viable change agent on its own for breaking the habits of protective bracing, inducing exercise analgesia, changing fear, improving confidence, building self-efficacy, learning to move without pain, and to transfer that confidence over to other activities (Lehman, 2013). Indeed, how people compensate their movement is highly individualized, and it is likely true that movement changes that persist beyond their original protective stage may lead to changes in function and further affect recovery (Butera, Fox, & George 2016). Therefore, the therapists who are most able to attend to the individual nuances as to how a person can move differently, stand the best chances for successful outcomes in lieu of one-sizefits-all rigidly designed protocols, and these affordances - of being able to adjust treatment and movement re-education parameters to meet the needs for individual patients enrolled in the study - were a stated recommendation for treating clinicians in both arms of the study. In addition, clinicians in each arm in the study were encouraged to use hands-on attention and manual 284 contact as necessary in order to satisfy treatment and learning objectives for both VRB3/FM and CSB/MCE arms of the study. Expectation fulfillment as a confounding variable. The Expectation Fulfillment Confounder (EFC), which, with close association to other confounding variables, including placebo response, regression to the mean over time, and the subtleties implied through confirmation bias and patient-practitioner belief all remain a challenge as to actually and conclusively determine whether a particular therapy, and its purported biological mechanisms, are actually effective or in some way causative. However, the role of expectation fulfillment being more confirmatory to the perspective of the control group is a factor that cannot be ignored. Certainly the ubiquitous nature of common and publicized vernaculars, such as "core strengthening" and "core control" or "of having a weak core," have become somewhat of a culturally-societally reinforced meme that is much more known and culturally substantiated on the basis of face validity and social proof; this, in comparison with more diffuse models emphasizing "correlative relationships between skeletal density pathways and corresponding movement trajectories," as was the more arcane purview of the experimental group. Accordingly, there is high likelihood that CNSLBP patients in both arms in the study were told at one or more times by their primary care provider or specialist that they needed to "work on their core." Subsequently, when they were given a specific form of "core-stabilization muscle recruitment" trainings and a corresponding motor control exercise program that would "improve the stability of their spine," it would "not be unusual that this subset would respond better because they had been primed to respond better" (Lehman, 2016). In the current study, the control group therapists were indeed talented, personable, professionally believable, and confident in both their personal disposition and treatment delivery. 285 One of them even received Valentine’s Day candy from one of more of the control group participants. The experimental group therapists received no such offering or appreciative gift during this time frame, but one did receive a handmade doily after patient decided to continue with therapy for her shoulder diagnosis at post-study intervention. The Experimental Group Demonstrating Superior Improvement and Some Possible Rationale for Examining the Differentiated Mechanisms of Influence As has been previously reported, there is growing evidence from neuro-imaging and concurrent clinical presentation that the ongoing persistence of chronic body pain syndromes results in mal-adaptive and reflective distortions in the sensory cortical representation (S1 mappings) of the body, and co-appearing in clinical concordance with various types of other perceptual disturbances. These include nociceptive amplification, loss of sense of limb proportion, changes in sensory and motor representations of head position in space, of laterality and spatial dimension between torso and limbs, changes in the overall quality of movement and posture control; thus, further disrupting the brain’s sense of movement, and ultimately resulting in lasting perceptual changes in the body image or body schema. These have also become especially evident in situations involving phantom limb pain and complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS), and have become known as "the dark side of neuroplasticity" (Doidge, 2007, 2015). Researchers in body schema phenomena and rehabilitation medicine have known that planning and coordination of movement requires an intact perception of the body and its position in space, and movement quality may be compromised if body perception is disrupted (Wand et al., 2016). Suboptimal movement patterns might abnormally load the back and contribute to 286 nociceptive input and movement-related pain in those with CNSLBP (Hodges & Smeets, 2015; O’Sullivan, 2005). Furthermore, studies in both animals and humans demonstrate that altered sensory transmission may result in changes in neuronal properties and organization within different subcortical and cortical areas including the thalamus, primary somatosensory cortex (S1) and the primary motor cortex (M1) all being co-implicated in sensory transmission, perception, and motor control (Kambi et al., 2014; Jones et al. as cited in Pelletier et al., 2015a). Neurophysiological studies have also revealed that increased activation of insular cortex is correlated with pain duration, while medial pre-frontal cortex activation is correlated with continuing pain intensity in patients with CNSLBP (Apkarian, Hashmi, & Baliki, 2011). Abnormal increased connectivity between the medial pre-frontal cortex and the nucleus accumbens is also noted to be highly predictive of who will go on to develop chronic LBP suggesting that there may be pre-disposing biomarkers for the development of chronicity (Baliki et al., 2006; Mansour et al., 2013; Mansour et al., 2014). One result of the abnormal activity in these areas is increased limbic, autonomic, and somatic vigilance, and a decreased ability to cognitively over-ride or disengage from pain (Davis & Moayedi, 2013). The experimental arm of the current study considered that these areas of co-involvement could be all be comprehensively more addressed through the development of a new intervention that targets and reinforces toward selecting unaffected functional pathways, as competing information to those pathways otherwise remaining affected or being dysfunctional. That is to say that “rather than attempting to target a presumed cause of the pain, and then treat it, an alternative approach is to help a person to recover a sense of missing or lost perceptual parts of themselves – including the asymptomatic ones – and to develop a more clear and effective 287 working body schema overall; and one that is free of neuromuscular and perceptual-cognitive belief habits that have otherwise become co-conditioned through chronic pain states.” In this way, I more likely avoid manually contacting or attempting to directly change or treat guarded and hyper-sensitized areas corresponding to both somatic and cortical mal-adaptive representations and their direct corresponding association for inadvertently inducing a perceived threat. In addition, by avoiding the topic of “low back pain” altogether during the course of treatment, I also avoid reinforcing the cognitive-iatrogenic belief attribution that there is something faulty or dangerously wrong in or with “the back.” Instead, a person has a unique chance to explore regions of support, robustness, and adaptability that anatomically and somatically exist adjacent to the back (i.e., above and below the back) but are not the back and within the context of whole body/whole self being experienced through synergistic and attentive supported action using Feldenkrais Method® -based movements. By instituting a novel and interactive learning approach via the implementation of a body schema acuity training model, more specifically, implemented by the acronym (VRB3) from earlier sections, and then being operationalized through selected Feldenkrais® movements (FM) that have been clinically known to have positive influences on reducing low back pain (LBP), the experimental intervention went on to both include and be informed by some additional inquiries and features not particularly found within the control group’s (CSB/MCE) intervention; nor for most other common approaches to chronic low back pain that are currently in use: • Three general principles of multisensory and multimodal integration, including the spatial rule, the temporal rule, and the principle of inverse effectiveness as put forth by Stein and Meredith (1993). 288 • Three components conducible toward being recognized as a combined virtual realitybased kinesthetic and visual-haptic sensory experience including the use of (a) threedimensional images that appear to be life-sized (i.e., virtual reality bones™) from the perspective of the user (b) the ability of a user to track their own motions, particularly through variations of head and eye movements, and (c), to correspondingly adjust the images on the user's display (i.e., their own body) to reflect the change in multi-sensory perspective. • Three vestibular-based and temporal bone contributions being manifested toward multimodal awareness and combined neurophysiological utilizations of (a) spatial cognitions drawing upon on parietal areas, (b) body representation being associated with somatosensory areas, and (c) affective processes modulating insular and cingulate cortices as cited by Mast et al. (2014). Though not cited in the literature review, another key theoretical and practice perspective becoming increasingly adopted through the Feldenkrais Method® practitioner community has been the emergence of Dynamic Systems Theory. Since the publication of A Dynamic Systems Approach to the Development of Cognition and Action by Thelen and Smith (1994), its key tenants have served to help explain the phenomena of coordination, cognition, and learning, and control of action in the human movement system; most especially, how interventions being informed through applications of systems-based thinking, like the Feldenkrais Method®, can seem to somehow rapidly and sustainably create an automatic quality of constitutive and lasting change without having to be subservient or dependent upon an explicitly dictated performance override for conducting "proper performance" objectives, nor for adhering to a rote protocol, repeated regimen, or structured routine. 289 The basic concept is that the organism, the task, and the context "self-organize" behavior into a preferred form, or attractor. A dynamical approach states that during development, new forms of behaviors (attractors) emerge as old forms (habits) lose their stability through wideranging phase shifts in behavioral organization. However, the nature of all systems - including living systems - is such that they tend to conserve for the self-maintenance of perpetuating their own existing and accustomed conditions until effectively destabilized, re-adapted, or phase-shifted into a newly existing order through appropriated interactions. Review sources from cognitive science and rehabilitative fields have distilled this landscape into two contexts from which to interpret this further: A dynamic system is created when conflicting forces of various kinds interact, then resolve into some kind of partly stable, partly unstable, equilibrium. The relationships between these forces and substances create a range of possible states that the system can be in. This set of possibilities is called the state space of the system. The dimensions of the state space are the variables of the system. However, although these variables define the range of possibilities for the system, only a few of these possibilities actually occur. (These become embedded as attractor states). Port and van Gelder define "attractor" as " the regions of the state space of a dynamical system toward which trajectories tend as time passes. As long as the parameters are unchanged, if the system passes close enough to the attractor, then it will never leave that region. (Rockwell, 2005, p. 573) And from a therapeutic intervention standpoint: If poor patterns are already in a deep attractor well, interventions are required that disrupt this current stability, if possible. Patients who have experienced the bias of one organization longer will have developed a deeper well of this preferred organization, pulling further subsystems into a less flexible pattern. Once the system has alternative patterns available, the therapist can assist the patient in discovering, through natural movements, the range of possible new solutions. Thus, the goal of treatment, according to a dynamical view, is to work on the system when it is in transition. Treatment is change that involves seeking new, and more adaptive, movement configurations. Therapeutic handling should allow as many degrees of freedom to vary as the patient can flexibly explore. Control around these transition points is one key to adaptive behavior. (Kamm, Thelen, & Jensen, 1990) 290 When an attractor well is very deep, the behavior of the system becomes limited to this area and is often described as hard-wired, stereotyped, or obligatory (Kamm et al., 1990). By constituting chronic pain as a stuck place, or as an accrued culmination of habituated and regressive states bearing much concurrent relationship to a limited or restrictive movement repertoire, a "deep well attractor" image of the nervous system can serve a useful metaphor for understanding both the transition to chronic pain and disability over time; and for outlining some transition paths afforded through a Feldenkrais style of intervention that can "expand the ® territory" in favor of more dynamic functions and restorative influences, and thereby contribute to its resolution and reversal. An accrued attractor landscape is depicted below in Figure 69. 291 (a) (b) (c) (d) ________________________________________________________________________ Figure 69. Visual Schematic for Attractor States over Time. (a) Cumulative effects of deep attractor well representing the downward spiral of developmental pain and compensated movement over time and persisting into old age with increasingly restrictive movement repertoire; (b) The effects of usual and repeated therapeutic exercise and most personal fitness routines are only a mere rehearsal of preferred and habituated states and thus do not effectively alter the topography landscape of the state space; (c) The institution of novel inroads afforded through Feldenkrais® movements expands the behavioral repertoire into a preferred, but more flexible configuration; permitting (d) a larger space for play. *Diagram and concept courtesy of Feldenkrais® Trainer, Roger Russell, PT, FGNA 2004. 292 Other Qualitative Differences and Oppositional Contrasts Between Interventions In sum, the design of this study was set up to test-compare two types of treatment interventions such that they could be comparatively scrutinized on the basis of their being diametrically opposed and antithetical to each other in terms of their historical developments, their purported mechanisms of effect, and their respective regional areas of application. The Core Stabilization Biofeedback plus Motor Control Exercises (CSB/MCE) treatment control group aimed to entrain the following foundational precept: In how many ways can a patient best experience a progression of challenges for maintaining their core stability for control of deep stabilizers about the lumbar spine via selected sub-maximal activation and specific recruitment patterns for transverse abdominis (TrA) and lumbar multifidus (LM) muscle groups via the aid of a regionally applied pressure biofeedback unit (PBU) device and through therapist-directed facilitation in supine, prone, side-lying, and sitting positions so as to more consistently stabilize and control the position of lumbar spine segments T12-L1 through L5-S1 during motor control exercise and for future conditioning progressions? In comparative contrast, the Body Schema Acuity Training (using the VRB3 method™) plus Feldenkrais® Movements (VRB3/FM) treatment experimental group concerned itself with a different question for exploration: In how many ways can a person best experience varied dimensions of pelvis-hips-legs opposite that of head & vestibular orientation with emphasis on developing a sense for skeletal continuity in three-dimensional space by perceiving their densest trabecular skeletal pathways through global and proportionate movement experiments; mostly through sensory interactions with life-scaled skeletal models and avatar animations, and by aid of visual-haptic self-touch with the assistance of therapist...and essentially ignoring any importance for particular attention to isolated spine segments being regionally localized to lumbar vertebrae; or even to any specific muscular recruitment of any particular name; nor of any particular kind? Figure 70 depicts an outline of differentiated schematic intent between the two groups: 293 (a) (b) Figure 70. Conceptual Drawings: Regional Isolation vs. Regional Interdependence. Early preliminary conceptual drawings from the reference point of three cardinal anatomical planes being contrasted against regional isolation vs. regional interdependence: (a) The CSB/MCE group’s area of entrainment focus remains confined to TrA and LM muscle groups and gives little affordance toward three-dimensional space. In contrast, (b) The VRB3/FM ignores any reference to isolating abdominal (core) aspects of trunk and instead gives primary reference emphasis toward experiencing dimensional relationships between pelvis-hips-legs opposite thorax and head, and also particularly for vestibular end-organ relationships within multiple planes of three-dimensional space. In addition, Appendix S depicts an extremely extensive outline of the differentiated schematic intent between the two groups. The epistemological differences, being compared and contrasted side by side, should hopefully invoke some added understanding of each approach’s foundational paradigms as well as providing for implications for real world applications in the improvement of human function overall, and to hopefully influence practice and policy development in all of health care practice, human development, and clinical research. A Critical Review of Outcome Measures and Findings from the Current Study Chapter 3 (Methodology section) again reveals and substantiates that all outcome measures and tools being used for the current study had met baseline requirements for validity and reliability, and that they had furthermore revealed long track records of superior selection 294 precedent through extensive literature reviews of clinical research trials involving comparative treatments and determinants of problems involving low back pain, especially CNSLBP. However, added considerations are worthy of discussion in terms of how these various tools were exemplified within the actual context of the current study. Mean VAS Pain Scales Post-study review reveals that a Visual Analogue Scale (VAS) is a measurement instrument that tries to measure a characteristic or attitude (i.e., pain intensity) that is believed to range across a continuum of values and cannot easily be directly measured, furthermore displayed without actual segmented demarcation during assessment, and is only later quantified at post assessment via the applied use of a millimeter ruler being placed as a pre-calibrated overlay in correspondent alignment with the horizontal line in which the patient had ticked the line. As a matter of correct nomenclature, it can be argued that the correct nomenclature should actually have been more specifically classified as the Numerical Rating Scale (NRS); being correctly described as a segmented numeric version of the visual analog scale (VAS) to which a respondent selects a whole number (0–10 integers) that best reflects the intensity of his/her pain. However, the important feature on application is that the 1-10 qualitative scaling criterion was consistently used, in addition to its comprising a visual analog being repeatedly reflective of pain experience, for all participants, and for in all phases, and for both groups in the entire study. Furthermore, multi-modal versions of display, including the addition of language qualifies and 295 colorized references to pain intensity being corroboratively scaled with the numerical ratings, were an essential visual feature that was used in the current study, and these are displayed together in Appendix H and in terms of their being described as Copy of VAS-PAIN/Numerical Rating Scale. So, while the “VAS-PAIN” scale used for the current study is arguably "more visual," it will be more likely better for future literature publications to have it be re-depicted as a “1-10 Numerical Rating Scale for Pain (NRS-PAIN) being embellished by matching color scales and language descriptors” for purposes of method and citation. Outcomes for RMDQ Disability Questionnaires RMDQ had scored as consistent at baseline with closely similar initial mean averages of 11.40 plus or minus 6.17 for the experimental group and 12.40 plus or minus 5.80 for the control group. Subsequent to Phase I of the study, repeat administration of the RMDQ demonstrated a change in mean scores to 8 plus or minus 5.69 for the experimental group as compared to 11.13 plus or minus 6.03 for the controls. When statistical comparisons were made using the Wilcoxon rank sum test, the p-value of change distribution for the RMDQ between group means subsequent to Phase I was calculated at p=0.058. Upon inclusive consideration for high variability being reflected in corresponding standard deviations between both groups, I maintain that this outcome signifies a positive congruence for actually supporting the outcome for the secondary hypothesis being reasonably proximal to the pre-stated p-value of p less than or equal to 0.050. While the remaining phases of concurrent treatment did not demonstrate closer approximation toward reaching statistical significance, The VRB3/FM group’s results nonetheless consistently demonstrated greater magnitude of improvement as compared to that of the CSB/MCE control group. 296 Outcomes for PSFS Functional Scale Of all the methodological tools and instruments used in the current study, only the final outcome measures for the Patient Specific Functional Scale (PSFS) were able to most definitively demonstrate superior efficacy at a p-value of 0.008714 for the VRB3/FM experimental group as compared to the CSB/MCE control group. An important factor to consider for this outcome measure is that the Patient-Specific Functional Scale (PSFS) makes broader consideration as to "what’s important to the patient?" Within the context of a profession, so much of clinical practice can become a reflection of technical features that are aligned according to the particular training bias, the professional jargon, and the particular competence expectancies that arise for the individual clinician, but of which have little perspective or relevance of value that is specific for the individual patient as they attempt to adjust to varied compensatory challenges in their daily manner of living. For example, many physical therapy- and personal trainer-based programs emphasize an external referent for "proper performance." Patients and clients are "told what to do" and to "do it correctly" in terms of imitative demonstration from someone with a completely different level of personal fitness and/or proportion of body-build, and/or by saying “I want you to do this…” and by further demonstrating some exemplary form of advanced fitness that is typically beyond the current capacity and perception of the injured or recovering patient, or the deconditioned person. In contrast, the Feldenkrais Method first creates conditions of internal reference and ® outlines sequences for independent and varied exploration of what is possible, thereby generating a sense of free-reign and inner attention toward improving a quality of movement such that a response characteristic of enhanced self-efficacy can permit greater generalization into the usual or preferred activities of daily life, as opposed to adhering to someone else’s pre-conditioned 297 exercise program or exacting routine. From this perspective of explanation of difference between usual fitness-based and core exercises as compared with a more patient-centered/person-centered Feldenkrais orientation of perspective, it is somewhat easier to understand why the PatientSpecific Functional Scale (PSFS) had demonstrated more exceptional outcomes at a p-value of p less than or equal to 0.05. Outcomes for McGill’s Timed Endurance Tests I have no explanation for how or why the aggregate of baseline and continuing total endurance of mean scores for holding all four positions in the sustained position endurance tests began substantially higher (at 160.32 seconds for the experimental group), as compared for roughly half-that (at only 85.01 seconds) for controls. Likewise, how the experimental group continued to remain substantially higher for post-intervention improvements (at 198.58 seconds) in comparison to that of the control group (at only 106.69 seconds), other than random outlier and clustering effects being inadvertently derived through random sampling. Despite the large variation of magnitude between groups, the proportion of scaled difference when compared using parametric paired t-tests revealed a similar p-value of 0.019 for the experimental group as compared to 0.0011 for the control group - demonstrating comparable statistical significance between groups and therefore indicating no statistically significant difference being reflective of greater clinical efficacy between groups on the basis of changes for total endurance times alone. Yet, upon comparing the previously cited predictive threshold for Flexion/Extension timed-endurance ratios themselves being below 1.5 for being clinically indexed as a measure of more sufficiently balanced trunk control, it was found that 11/15 experimental group participants met this threshold index of less than 1.5 at post-intervention as compared to only 1/15 of control group participants; thus, indicating greater episodic phenomena 298 of dysfunctional trunk imbalance in the control group at post-intervention, which is itself clinically significant. So, while similar outlier effects were shared between both groups at the start of the study, with correspondingly high average flexion/extension (F/E) ratios at the start for both groups at baselines of greater than 1.5 (via 8.45 total average for the experimental group as compared to higher ratio of average proportion for the control group at 21.86), the results at eight weeks postintervention had indicated that only the experimental group had demonstrated more proportionate success at attaining an average flexion/extension ratio at below the cited 1.5 level threshold (via 1.39 for the experimental group as compared to continued higher perseveration average ratios of 17.19 for the control group).These results reflect greater clinically meaningful thresholds of less than 1.5 being indicative of improved overall resiliency in trunk function via reduced agonist-antagonist disparity in the experimental group as compared to controls. A possible explanation for disparity between groups is to suggest that core stabilization training/motor control exercises (CSB/MBE), being abdominal-centric in their intended "coresetting" (vs. their un-intended excessive corseting) effects, may be inadvertently vs. overtly conditioning a flexor bias at the expense of extensor recruitment. Thus, raising the risk factor specter for imbalanced trunk control and resulting in decreased extensor endurance, a factor being previously cited by researchers as a future predictor of recurrent LBP episodes (McGill, 2005). Conversely, in lieu of conditioning and reinforcing a set of co-contraction inspired muscular habits that more than likely already exist as a predominant behavioral tendency in patients with CNSLBP, The VRB3/FM group instead became more well-rounded at postintervention to access multiple directions in space with equal distribution of effort, and improved 299 endurance synergy between flexors and extensors, so as to maintain better balance of contralateral vs. A-P trunk tone, on demand. This outcome ties well with a known and frequently cited quotation from Moshe Feldenkrais in his stating that: The aim of the work is a body that is organized to move in any direction with equal ease, and without hesitation or pre-preparation – not from muscular strength - but from an increased consciousness of how it works. (Haller, 1997) Finally, it is rather paradoxical to observe that the repeated exercises of the control group intervention would - on the surface - appear more conducive toward adhering to "exercise specificity principles" via the simulated conditioning of both strength and endurance factors, and their corresponding positions for entrainment (planking and so forth) actually replicating the same exacting conditions as were slated for repeated endurance testing in the McGill timed endurance protocols. So, despite the theory of "exercise specificity" being predicative of improving performance, the CSB/MCE group’s intervention perhaps inadvertently conditioned the control group to co-contract and to work against themselves and to thereby fatigue earlier, despite any surface appeal for their exercises being selected for purposes of coordinated efficiency and improvements in "motor control." The VRB3/FM group, conversely, did nothing to focus on specific skills being aimed or rehearsed to improve strength, "proper form" nor the endurance simulation of specific postures being specifically replicated for the timed endurance test protocols by McGill, and yet, they consistently performed better. Participant Adherence, Attrition, and Contribution During Course of Study In most cases, patients as participants had demonstrated and reported positive treatment effects in response to each intervention immediately subsequent to its delivery and for both arms of the study. However, the carry-over of treatment effects into retaining an adherence feature for maintaining an independently and regularly performed or unsupervised home exercise program 300 (HEP) remains a classic problem in rehabilitation medicine. While therapeutic exercise has been shown to reduce pain and increase function in patients with chronic low back pain, it has also been shown that up to 70% of patients do not engage in prescribed home exercise (Beinart, Goodchild, Weinman, Ayis, & Godfrey, 2013). Additional barriers to adherence to home exercise programs have included boredom, questionable effectiveness, complexity and the overall burden of exercises in terms of time, effort and expectation (Palazzo et al., 2016). Furthermore, physiotherapists/physical therapists need to understand more about the complex factors influencing patients' adherence to prescribed home exercise to tailor their exercise interventions more effectively and to support patients to better self-manage. Sub-factors cited for improved adherence have included greater health locus of control, supervision, participation in an exercise program, and participation in a general behavior change program incorporating motivational strategies (Beinart et al., 2013). Adherence to Intervention Training Intent, Home Program, and Medication List In this study, these adherence and support factors were made aware to the patients, but only monitored to the best of my ability in terms of time and resources. It was generally beyond the scope of the research design and administrative resource to monitor and confirm these variables regarding specific parameters for adherence, other than through return visit "verbal self-reports," and through the anticipated written completion of graded activity and medication logs, both of which were within themselves also an adherence issue, with only minimal completion between participants and clinicians. However, based on clinician discussions and interviews, it can be estimated that 30% of the control group (CSB/MCE group) remained likely adherent to actual performance of core stabilization/motor control home exercise program, and 70% likely adherent to simply 301 understanding and applying the principles. For the experimental group, it can likewise be estimated that 70% of participants were attentive to actively incorporating VRB3/Feldenkrais Movement principles into their daily activities, and with both groups advancing the extent and duration of their graded activity concomitantly. While some patients enrolled in the study had reported either decreasing their pain medication or abstaining from continued medication use in both arms of the study, the tracking of such data was unfortunately inconsistent, with more time required and invested in actually being devoted to delivering the interventions themselves. Participant Attrition, Priming Effects, and the Contribution of Intent to Treat Patients in both groups were originally intended to receive a total of 12 sessions to include (a) provisions for two highly differentiated methods for qualitative sensory-motor and activity entrainment (CSB vs VRB3) in Phase I, and being subsequently applied in combination with either (b) a supervised exercise intervention (MCE) or a movement therapy intervention (FM) as components for both Phase II and Phase III, thus, all compositional as a combined aggregate of continuity within each total intervention for each group. For both groups, roughly 2/3 of participants (N=10 for the control group and N=11 for the experimental group) completed the entire intervention (Phase I – III). However, the remaining 1/3 of participants (N=5) for the control group and (N=4) for the experimental group completed only the Phase I preliminary component of the study before dropping out. Participant attrition was thus essentially similar for both groups. In this study, all results were included for data analysis and review based on accrued principle and precedence in clinical research trials to accommodate the intention-to-treat effect. “Intention to treat” is a strategy for the analysis of randomized controlled trials that compares patients in the groups to which they were originally randomly assigned. This is generally 302 interpreted as including all patients, regardless of whether they actually satisfied the treatment goals and/or subsequent withdrawal or deviation from the protocol (Hollis & Campbell, 1999). In other words, in an intention to treat population, none of the patient’s data are excluded, and everyone who is randomized in the trial is part of the trial regardless of whether he or she completes the trial. Randomized clinical trials analyzed by the intention-to-treat approach are purported to provide for more unbiased comparisons among the treatment groups. In addition, the inclusion of an intention to treat sampling methodology for data analysis also provides for information about the potential effects of a treatment category as a whole, rather than on attempting to isolate the potential effects of specific treatment. All participants (n=15) for each group thus made proportionate contributions to the total data set (n=30) with respect to each intervention’s qualitative type and for statistical analysis of total outcome as an aggregate of the combined, integrative approaches for each group’s comparative outcome. Comparison of Study Results to Data Attained from Previous Studies A previous comparable study conducted by a respectable team of internationally cited investigators, though not previously known during the implementation phase of this study, was newly referenced in the more recently updated Literature Review chapter of this document being published as: “Effect of Motor Control Exercises Versus Graded Activity in Patients with Chronic Nonspecific Low Back Pain: A Randomized Controlled Trial” by Macedo et al. in 2012. Again, it is important to disclose that my review for this study was actually discovered after the original formulation of my design parameters for the current study, and within only two weeks of finalizing the data collection and just prior to conducting the statistical analysis. By having a comparable study design as a similar kind of benchmark or yardstick for cross-comparing the results of the current study, especially one that applied the same Core Stabilization/Motor 303 Control Exercise model as a corresponding control group to an alternate treatment intervention (to Graded Activity), with their latter approach comparison also being a sub-component of mutual treatment orientation for both groups in the current study, I are thereby able to now compare the outcomes from the current study retrospectively to the data sets and outcome results that were attained from their previously published study. Again, by their using a selection of essentially identical (a) treatment outcome measures, (b) the same allocation for number of treatment sessions (12), and finally (c) the same timeframes for administering the comparative interventions (eight weeks) and as a similar framework of parameters and measurement tools used for all other prior core stabilization/motor control treatment efficacy studies that were reviewed, it serves as an appropriate model reference for comparing the outcome results of this RCT study. As previously cited, the results accrued from the larger-scale, multi-site study (N=172; Macedo et al., 2012) had demonstrated "no difference" between Graded Activity (GA) and Motor Control Exercises (MCE) on symptoms, perceived disability, and function outcomes for CNSLBP. The results from their study are cross-compared with results from this study in Table 21. 304 Table 21 Comparing Current Study Results with previously published 2012 Data and MICD Scores A. Measurement Scales for Comparative Efficacy in CNSLBP Pre/Post Time Frames for Treatment Outcomes and the mean average difference between groups VAS Baseline (0-10) 8 weeks (VRB3 & FM) Mean Scores (2016) Control Group (CSB/MCE) Mean Score (2016) Graded Activity + Motor Control 5.60 6.10 2.00 3.00 4.10 4.7 2.6 2.0 Baseline 11.40 12.40 11.30 8 weeks 3.91 8.56 7.75 Maughan & Lewis 2010 Mean Difference 7.49 3.84 3.55 Baseline 3.33 3.93 3.65 8 weeks 7.08 5.26 5.70 Mean Difference 3.75 1.33 2.05 Baseline 160.00 85.01 N/A 8 weeks 198.58 106.69 N/A Mean Percent Difference (%) 80% 0.79% N/A Baseline 8.545 21.865 N/A Interval of 2.4 5 points PSFS Timed Total for average Absolute Endurance (in seconds) B. MCID (GA + MCE) Combined Mean (2012) 6.07 Mean Difference RMBQ (0-24) Experimental Group 2.0 points 30% change Flexion/Extension Endurance Ratios for Improving the Balance of Trunk Control 8 weeks Magnitude of Difference * 1.398 7.147 * 17.197 4.668 N/A NA *A ratio of < 1.5 is indicative of more balanced control between flexors & extensors (McGill,200 3) (Sobie, 2016) 305 *Note. (a) Pre-Post Mean Intervention scores and differences from experimental (VRB3 & FM) and control (CSB/MCE) groups at eight weeks as compared to post-intervention graded activity and motor control exercises (GA & MCE) combined average outcomes and differences from the 2012 PT Journal CNSLBP study by Macedo et al. (2012) after eight weeks; and (b) All differences between mean scores compared to list of minimal clinically important differences (MCID) in scores for clinical outcomes for CNSLBP as cited by Maughan and Lewis (2010).*The ratio scores for unbalanced flexion/extension endurance from McGill (2003) are adjusted and modified from threshold of <1.0 to < 1.5 to more closely represent a CNSLBP population. From this compilation of multiple data sets and minimally important clinical difference (MICD) guideline sets, it can be seen that the experimental (VRB3 & FM) group outperformed both the current control (CSB/MCE) group and the aggregate data from the combined Graded Activity (GA) and Motor Control Exercise (MCE) quasi-control, historical groups by average means of difference across all comparative measures - constituting a factor of almost two-fold. Furthermore, all experimental (VRB3 & FM) group measures of mean difference for improvement from baseline (start of study) to eight weeks (at post-treatment) had exceeded the benchmarks for minimal clinically important difference (MCID); whereas all respective control group measures of mean difference for improvement (whether current control group or historical larger-scale group) either "just met" or "fell short" of achieving a clinically significant effect size for response to treatment. This is again by itself, significant. Implications of Study Findings for Treatment of CNSLBP Perhaps the most broadly intriguing and far-reaching implication that can be stated from the total outcome of this original and current comparative efficacy study is the fact that not only did the experimental group demonstrate consistently superior results as compared to the control group and its related historical comparative subset, but that it did so through the acquisition of largely non-conventional observational considerations and through implementation and discovery of entirely new categories for treatment intervention. These stand in stark contrast to 306 the usual biomedical, bio-psychosocial, and other physical rehabilitation approaches commonly in use as well as remaining quite variant from other usual complementary, alternative, and integrative models of approach that are already stylistic in current everyday practice and in common prescriptive use for applications intended to resolve the continuing, perplexing, and troublesome problem of chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP) and its associated continued movement impairments and disabilities. Most notably, the experimental group’s intervention is uniquely distinguished by the fact that there was never any overt attention nor treatment modality intention being directed or applied to: Muscles, muscle groups, the naming of muscles, nor of treating, strengthening, stretching, nor of training of muscles of any kind; nor for implicating any of its associations to pathologically deficient structures including ligaments, tendons, tendinosis, discs, cartilage, bursae, plica, nerve roots, fascial restriction, myofascial trigger points, soft tissue inflammation, deep tissue scaring, capsular impingement, neurovascular entrapment, arthritic degeneration, stenosis, subluxation; and without none of these ever being characterized as being either symptomatic, nor for being symptom causing nor symptom provoking. All of which is an ironic departure from usual culprits in "musculoskeletal and structural medicine." Thus, the key and important feature about the experimental group intervention was that its superior outcome did not seem to depend upon nor rely upon the localized treatment of particular anatomical regions that are specific to the diagnostic category of "low back pain" itself, nor for being directly applied to perceived areas of involvement (i.e., of specific lumbar spine segments) directly. In other words, within the experimental group, the symptomatic parts and regions were decisively and distinctively avoided and not treated directly, and yet, this group improved more and performed better for all outcome measures in and throughout the current study. 307 One possible rationale for this difference is to consider that “when patients and clinicians pay attention to parts, the parts themselves get reinforced and become further and automatically conditioned into mutually consensual and attentional dominance” (i.e., the painful parts or regional areas persist in the attentional field). Whereas When patients and clinicians pay attention to parts as relational components within a functional context, then the parts self-organize into global functions to the extent that the usual isolated attention - being usually given to symptomatic parts (i.e., to painful parts) - will dominate less. In my opinion, this intended design feature for the experimental group, of designing and applying an experience-based "treatment condition" that emphasized and targeted only the asymptomatic and normative areas of highest bone density that are involved in proportionate action with each other, and for purposes of improving a more supportive framework and organization for the integration of disrupted body schema acuity, and in conjunction with developing an inter-systemic quality of movement distribution, and finally, being also proportionate for improvement of movement dexterity in cases of CNSLBP, has strong implications (if not also some necessarily strong provocations for producing cognitive dissonance among clinicians). Implications for questioning the existing and dominant structural determinism models that use structurally-specific diagnostic criteria to guide their treatment methods, and to have them reconsider their thinking - and actions - when it comes to treating chronic musculoskeletal conditions wherein the originating structural rationale or pathologic validation for continuing such treatments have long since expired. These more "tangible" structural pathology assumptions, being more known or more favored in lieu of recognizing less tangible, but nonetheless clinically significant central neuroplasticity changes that have transpired as implicit patterns over time, have permeated medical, physical therapy, chiropractic, and massage therapy and bodywork practices for quite 308 some time. Modern neuroscience and known changes in the cortical representation of the body now imply that the "parts themselves may not the continuing cause," but that instead perhaps a more valid contributor to chronic pain is the "misrepresentation of parts being in dysfunctional inter-regional relationship - relative to their desired enlistment for movement tasks in an incessantly persistent gravitational environment," and in relationship to "disrupted perceptual sustenance of ‘whole self’ in everyday action" – as an inter-systemic and dynamic relationship factor that matters so much more than the mere "strengthening, stretching, or training, or injecting of muscle tissue." Quite remarkably different, the VRB3/FM approach fosters a direction, which supports and educates healthy cognitions that err more toward adopting the anatomy of possibility (i.e., reinforcing conditions for what’s potentially right with the person). This, in lieu of the usual biomedical bias of diagnosing and explaining a disconcerting framework for automatically reinforcing the anatomy of pathology (i.e., what’s wrong with the person), with resultant, but unintended, dissociative and protective cognitions for mediating perceived threats being both implicitly and limbic-ally associated toward perseverating the continued state of chronic, nonresolving musculoskeletal pain responses under continued conditions, wherein such structural determinants can no longer provide a basis for reasonable or supported causation. Physical Therapy’s Regression to the Mean The real and observational conceptual phenomenon of "arrival and regression toward a commonly shared central tendency" can trace its modern roots for linguistic classification to the late 19th century since the publication of "Regression towards Mediocrity in Hereditary Stature" (Galton, 1886). The geneticist, Sir Francis Galton, had observed that extreme characteristics (e.g., height) in parents are not passed on completely to their offspring. Rather, the 309 characteristics in the offspring regress towards a mediocre point (a point which has since been identified as the mean). In much the same manner, the depth and breadth of intensive educational content from physical therapy academic curricula has become subjected to similar phenomena. Despite extensive training in applied neuroscience, the necessarily complex activities involving recovery from musculoskeletal injury have remained almost exclusively to the domain of "tissuespecific exercise physiology principles" – with greater primacy for generalized strengthening, range of motion, and endurance training, creating little applied practicum difference between medically-trained physical therapists and fitness industry-trained personal trainers. Virtually anyone in the United States could conduct an informal survey of outpatient physical therapy practices in their local community, and limit their sample to orthopedics, and "sports and spine" sports medicine type specialties as the most frequent and dominant type. Next they could sample and view the offerings and equipment and exercise techniques at specialty fitness centers emphasizing personalized attention from certified trainers, and again, find little to no discernible difference. In many ways, the usual routines being primarily derived through the fitness industry have stifled and corrupted the full potential for greater variation and individuation within the most common of physical therapy outpatient practice settings. Humans are not necessarily exercise machines, and nor should physical therapy outpatient clinics become reduced to mostly exercise machines as a usual high volume, one-size-fits-all, customary practice standard. It is not that physical fitness is not important. It is just that it should not be used as a firstline defense intervention for persons debilitated with chronic pain who will likely remain fearful and/or rightfully suspicious that callisthenic-like activities from gym class (which can be done anywhere) and/or through their already existing associated memories from prior experiences 310 with usual and ubiquitous fitness activities would cause them to arrive at some personal conclusion to determine that "these have not really helped me in the past…I didn’t enjoy them …so why pursue them?" Notwithstanding, more recent trends for commonly prescribed and commonly endorsed "Rehabilitative Yoga" and "Rehabilitative Pilates" routines still also fall under the usual fitness paradigm by virtue of their prescribing pre-conceived ideals for "proper movement" and regimens for explicitly "moving correctly" in accordance with an externally authoritative standard - to the extent that they negate the opportunities for more varied developmental and organic explorations being permitted for implicitly affirming the uniquely individualized internal variations that are more the properties and domains of body schema-based awareness and cognitive practices – like the Feldenkrais Method®. Historically, and for too long, outpatient orthopedic-based physical therapy has embraced the structural biomechanical model to the exclusion of central representational processes more akin toward hemispheric entrainment within the virtual body; essentially ignoring any specific viable role for the brain or central nervous system in usual movement training – with exception of cases involving a structural and pathological lesion being found within the brain itself, such as a CVA malformation, tumor history, or an infarct, being co-morbid to a presenting orthopedic condition. Yet, seeing that the effectiveness of traditional physical therapy exercise programs being combined with the recent addition/inclusion of psychological and cognitive behavioral therapies (CBT) has been found to be superior to outcomes obtained from usual general practitioner care. These nonetheless also occur with only marginal and modest outcomes at best. This shortfall leads generation to consider a role and a place for the investigation of new treatment options that permit the sensory inclusion for novel and interactive competing stimuli, a competing cognition- 311 attention frame, and a further frame of reference for endorsing a competing behavioral set that involves the concurrent entrainment of movement and enhanced perception as necessary functions for human improvement, but not necessarily within the usual and repeated context of exercise per se. A more ideal perception frame for usual physical therapy practice, particularly for therapeutic reframing and having multi-modal opportunities for the brain to re-sample itself, is depicted in Figure 71. Figure 71. Expanding the Scope for a More Multi-Factorial PT Practice. A more ideal frame of perceptual reference for interactive physical therapy practice as originally presented by Lois Gifford and updated by Zachery Cupples. Public use of image and concept is courtesy of https://zaccupples.com/tag/louis-gifford/. Used with Permission. The Preferred Future of PT Practice Metaphorically, it can be said that traditional outpatient physical therapy, having a dominant orientation in catering toward orthopedics and sports medicine populations, has for too long become a "performance-based gymnasium" being heavily influenced by the fitness industry, body conformity, and an expectant familiarity to exercise specificity with stricter adherence 312 toward pre-conceived routines. In contrast, very little exists in current practice that accounts for multi-modal development of neuroplasticity-based, cortical representation, and body-schemabased movement models, which instead point toward creating and constructing a "processoriented sensorium" being informed through human developmental learning models and embodied experiences, variations of sensory acuity and movement dexterity themes (as with fugues occurring in musical compositions), and a re-conceptualization of attention toward novelty and novel configurations for movement sequences serving as an opportunity to safely flee from ordinary accustomed exploration and in to extraordinary insightful play. These manners of activities, and the comparisons between them, thereby become "phenomenologically reperceived" from an internal constitutive reference being extremely unique to each individual in lieu of being "prescriptively pre-conceived" from a universally prescribed or externalized or authoritative paradigm; otherwise more conventionally known and more routinely experienced as "one size fit all." Patients could be better oriented toward greater functional improvement by having their prescribing practitioners in fact state that There’s a big difference in being explicitly told or shown what to do toward achieving ‘proper performance’ (i.e., from an external standard), and implicitly discovering and experiencing an attenuation of multiple versions of yourself, in terms of who you are and how you move, through exploration, attention, and perceptual experiments being wholly inclusive of navigating your whole self; versus the non-nuanced un-accommodation: “Here’s the corrective exercise sheet. Do these three times per day, and make sure you do them properly.” As the current study has now made significant contribution toward developing a reproducible protocol for revamping a multi-modal, body schema-based orientation and approaching internalizing the patient’s experience, and for demonstrating a more rapid capacity for change in working body schema becoming evident 313 through concurrent improvements in both sensory acuity accuracy and corresponding motor dexterity execution, a working model for sensory-motor learning has been developed to speculate about some possible underlying mechanisms that likely occur during actual implementation of the current study’s VRB3/FM treatment protocol. A New Intervention Model for the Systemic Adjustment of Working Body Schema Based on theoretical and computational studies, it has been suggested that the central nervous system (CNS) internally simulates the behavior of the motor system in planning, control, and learning. “Such an internal "forward" model is a representation of the motor system that uses the current state of the motor system and motor command to predict the next state” (Wolpert & Miall, 1996). The internal model for sensorimotor integration/efference copy schematic (as originally formatted by Wolpert, Ghahramani, and Jordan (1995), as depicted in Figure 72, describes the cognitive pretext of a "feed-forward internal model" for simultaneously conducting and monitoring the accomplishment of a routine motor task, but only within the established feedforward context as it is already happening in real time; and being modified only by sensory discrepancies and re-afferent feedback to in order to adjust the motor processes accordingly. Figure 72. Internal Model for Sensorimotor Integration and Efference Copy for Motor Control. *Though conceptually developed by Wolpert et al. (1995) and Wolpert and Miall (1996), original work on applied schematic refers back to Michelle Costanzo, as seen in Theory of Motor Control UMD Spring 2008. Licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-Share Alike 3.0 Unported. Used with Permission. 314 However, as a pre-habituated and pre-established forward model, it stereotypically "does what it most predictably does" reflecting a continuous cycle of 1st order change. There is no actual lasting change to the intrinsic or pre-programmed aspects of the forward model itself. It just does what it does. In the larger Sensory-Motor Learning and "Working Body Schema/MultiModal Information Processing Model" (as schematically depicted in Figure 73 and again in larger scale in Appendix W), this forward ‘intrinsic’ model becomes a contingency sub-set of a larger dynamic process occurring both within and between an organism and its environment (whether actual environment or an imagined/virtual one) and is herein embedded within the schematic diagram being depicted as "an inner loop." The inner loop, as an existing model, depicts only 1st order change as an individual’s primary model of habitual approach to everyday action. It is largely constitutive of an individual’s historical development, and pertains to the redundant processing within already existing feed-forward models of working body schema. It includes familiar actions of sensory anticipation and expectant routines, being constituted by habituated spatial-temporal pathways of existing cortical and sensory-motor representation; being only exhibitive and/or behaviorally demonstrative of merely "going through the motions" with ample repetition, but little in the way of variation. Thereby, essentially reinforcing the current conditions in terms of quality and distribution of movement, even if “performed correctly” according to the supervising therapists’ external (and therefore explicit) standard. This model, in fact, parallels the modus operandi of many, if not most, traditional physical therapy "therapeutic exercise" prescriptions and routines being carried-out and billed under the American Medical Association’s classified Current Procedural Terminology (CPT) codes for Therapeutic Exercise (97110) and Kinetic Activity (97530). 315 In corollary contrast, the "outer loop" (in direct parallel for routing and representing the VRB3/FM experimental intervention) depicts 2nd order change as a secondary competing model toward non-habitual and implicit learning. This loop presents a divergent developmental model by introducing and invoking parallel, but re-entrant qualities for novel processing of sensory information; a compelling and competing stimulus that is essentially disruptive of the established processing that is occurring within existing feed-forward models of working body schema inductive and facilitative of exploratory actions and sensory re-adjustment. All this toward cognitive re-appraisal becoming generative of deconstructing and reconstructing spatial-temporal pathways that are involved in cortical and sensory-motor reorganization toward greater efficiency through neuroplasticity-based mechanisms. The co-conditioning and invocation of these processes are purported to result in the emergence of completely new actions (i.e., 2nd order change) with much variation and differentiation toward newly-constructive cognitions for working body schema and new habituations for new movement qualities that ultimately seek toward integration into the existing "feed-forward models" of action, thereby, leading to selection plasticity and change within the primary model; thus, altering the original movement qualities of the pre-existing internal feed-forward model itself. 316 Figure 73. Information Processing Model for working Body Schema during VRB3 FM Rx. 317 Through this schematic, I can perhaps conclude that unless there is significant change within a person’s internal model for the sensory representation of effective action becoming expressed and confirmed through new attention to new movement, then there is really no change at all. Study Limitation It was rather daunting to be a first-time principal investigator, combined with being a clinical practice owner of one main practice location and two side-practice locations, during a time of tumultuous change in the administration of health care services becoming rampant with added time-consuming pre-authorization requirements and multiple additional documentation steps becoming required for hopeful assurance of third party coverages for usual and customary services and reimbursement. Also factored into my overwhelm was having to contract an independent study research coordinator, numerous physical therapy interns, and additional office staff as well as qualified physical therapists for administering the control group arm intervention. Yet, by having a neutral staff member hired-on to collect and aggregate data, safeguards were thereby in place to minimize the registration effect of confirmation bias by keeping data bases separate from all clinical staff and for both arms of the study. Deficiencies in the Monitoring of Adherence to Home Practice and Repetition As was previously stated, it was generally beyond the scope of the research design and administrative resources to monitor and confirm for adherences to graded activity and home exercise programs via the ongoing verification of written completion of graded activity and medication logs - both of which were within themselves also an adherence issue – occurring at only very minimal completion between both participants and clinicians. This proves problematic in making attempts toward generalizing the total effects of any behavioral intervention and 318 contributes toward weakening the influences of the independent variables being cited as the predominant causative attributions for inducing change in either or both groups. Moreover, while mere exposure to differentiating and prominent or conspicuous conditions can be of sufficient perturbation to elicit new selections for the stimulus-response driven processing of neuroplasticity pathways, and to alter their corresponding qualities for engaging and sustaining more prolonged and heightened states of attention, and also for attentional association to memory, it also appears that adequate repetition for the internalization of such processing is necessary for creating more lasting neuroplastic changes (i.e., sustained learning, new habits, and long-term potentiation). Thus, this is an area where again, more careful and intensive monitoring of participants’ abilities to integrate and generalize their new skills into the contexts of daily life and/or to adhere to practicing the principals involved in their rehearsal of new sensory-motor learning skills, would have been an improved contribution for solidifying an assertion for implicating the results of the study. Nonetheless, the study results indicate that there is attribution of difference coming from somewhere, and I assert that mere exposure to discerning and qualitative mechanisms of difference can be enough of an impetus to create widespread difference through systemic and neuroplastic change, and that these appear to be of greater magnitude in the experimental group in comparison to the control group under similarly controlled conditions. In particular, if they are contextually meaningful and personally relevant to either the history and development of the disorder, or to the current state of the organism. Limitation of only an implied CBT and Pain Neuroscience Education Component A second area of limitation in the current study concerned a lack of formal integration of a duly and appropriately administered cognitive behavioral and pain neuroscience education 319 program as a corresponding co-intervention during and throughout the course of the study. Despite good intentions to include a psychological component through the administration of the FABQ as a stratified random assignment strategy, and to make initial mention of safety assurances and behavioral re-attributions being embedded within the consent form, this study could have integrated a more formalized co-intervention beyond just outlining suggestion expectancy references in the consent form. This, through otherwise only implementing ongoing situational-mediational adjustments for re-directing some mistaken cognitions that occurred, but only in real time, and during the usual course of movement re-training and for either arm of the study. As is a good point brought-up by O’Sullivan, Dankaerts, O’Sullivan, and O’Sullivan (2015) in their cognitive functional therapy and chronic low back pain article published in Physical Therapy, “simply combining conservative interventions (physical and psychosocial) in a nonintegrated manner may be no more effective than either intervention provided in isolation” (p. 1485). My research team is now aware of emerging "out of the box" integrative programs to formally teach pain neuroscience education principles to physical therapy patients, and to implement actual cognitive behavioral exercises as part of a more integrative program. These can be considered for successive trials. Complexity of Experimental Group Intervention It is only fair to state that the VRB3/FM is a comparatively complex intervention, likely requiring an advanced apprenticeship in order to effectively learn its nuances and applications for this study to become replicated. Notwithstanding, there is likely probable and presuppositional concern about the cognitive capacity of patients to actually learn and integrate an understanding and application of these principles and activities into daily life. A common 320 refrain or rebuttal from Feldenkrais® trainers is to state that "the design and the functional organization of the nervous system can be quite a bit smarter than any pre-conceived ideas or prescriptive dictates that we may have about them" as another way of saying "trust the process." Correspondingly, within the current study and clinical experience, it has been observed and found that these processes can automatically and implicitly work, and perhaps at a more substantial sub-cortical level of interlinked neurological and reflexive processing; regardless of the superficial or interpretive attempts for understanding or logically explaining them within the constraints of conceptual, yet comparatively limited language. As a matter of difference with many other interventions that continue to attempt "simple solutions," it otherwise seems that systemic complexity is a realistically important feature to include toward the development of newer treatment interventions, and especially for situations pertaining to chronic pain, since simple solutions for complex problems rarely seem to sustainably work over time. As Albert Einstein, had said: “Make everything as simple as possible, but not simpler.” Sample Size and Generalizability Finally, a relatively small sample size for statistical power (N=30) and the confident generalizability of results to be ascribed to larger populations of patients with CNSLBP is one added limitation of inference from this study. It is particularly unknown to really know if a larger sample size would result in greater effect size to better substantiate the outcomes of the experimental group; or if it would show a regressive comparative improvement by larger statistical comparison. Provisions for these are to be discussed in the next section. 321 Recommendations for Future Research and Practice Numerous citations and reviews within this dissertation have called for more innovative treatment approaches based on motor imagery, neuroplasticity, sense of movement, virtual reality applications, and even for the inclusion of vestibular sensory mechanisms (Mast et al., 2014), in addition to the revamping of usual and current physical and psychological approaches. Some final thoughts and recommendations in consideration for future studies and for re-thinking usual clinical practice are discussed below. Enlisting Help from Larger Research Universities. fMRI Anyone? There is much to be gained from greater collaboration between literature- and publication-based research institutions and various independent clinicians working in the trenches of actual and daily clinical practice. Each domain has its own knowledge base, and its ranges of unique – yet segregated - experience are such that neither can be independently nor wholly known without real-time meetings and discussions for future collaboration. Of utmost interest to the current study is to consider that if the changes being revealed through the VRB3/FM intervention protocol for CNSLBP can be readily seen in the body and its response behaviors, can they also be correspondingly verified in the mind-brain via the implementation of fMRI. Furthermore, could pathways and mechanisms being constitutive of verifiable neuroplasticity-based changes also be elucidated and discerned? Indeed, single session pain neuroscience education/CBT has demonstrated decreased coactivation of pain and fear related cortical pathways encountered during the abdominal draw-in maneuver (Moseley, 2005). In the differential processing of motor imagery fMRI study by Vrana et al. (2015), the chronic LBP patients exhibited significantly excessive motor imagery-driven functional connectivity indicating diffuse and non-specific changes becoming maladaptive to the 322 usual processing of sensory-motor and kinesthetic information. It is therefore inferred that excessive functional connectivity on fMRI between brain areas has strong correlation toward revealing excesses in non-essential and parafunctional movement patterns when usual tasks are imagined under originating pre-intervention conditions. A corollary hypothesis originating out of the current study, thereby suggests that a mental simulation of action using a post VRB3/FM intervention movement strategy (of hips-pelvis-legs opposite head, while negating specific attention to lumbar areas) would demonstrate more effective and efficient use of primary S1 and M1 areas upon their simulation of the same motor-imagery driven functional tasks that were used in their 2015 study. In the other fMRI study by Hashmi et al. (2013), results indicated that those who transitioned to CNSLBP demonstrated a spatiotemporal dynamical reorganization of brain activity, during which the representation of back pain over time had gradually shifted away from sensory and nociceptive cortical regions and instead manifested toward engaging larger scale, greater morphologic localization throughout emotional and limbic structures. What remains an inquiry from a therapeutic neuroplasticity interventionist standpoint is to determine whether this transition can be reversed-especially toward soamto-sensory and sense of ownership aspects of neuronal information processing, and with less emotional-limbic divestment from body-self. Again, I believe that the unique entrainment affordances being made available through a VRB3/FM intervention and its correspondences for the perceptual enhancement of body schema acuity (i.e., of localization dimensions) and of movement strategy (i.e., of dexterity relationships for functional interaction between self and environment) would demonstrate a shift of activity predominance back toward more normative levels of S1 and M1. 323 Indeed, it has been shown through other fMRI studies in patients with CNSLBP that diminished brain regions implicated in disrupted pain modulation, including the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), had reduced grey matter, but after successful treatment for resolving the pain, the grey matter reductions were subsequently reversed so that the affected brain regions were again re-normalized in size (Seminowicz et al., 2011). In more specific regard to somatotopic representation in the primary somatosensory cortex (S1), it has been found that patients with CNSLBP demonstrate a 2.5 cm shift of the grey matter volume, and that these changes correlate with chronicity of symptoms (Apkarian et al., 2004; Flor et al., 1997; Schmidt-Wilcke et al., 2006). In similar fMRI study conditions where S1 somatotopic reorganization has been the target of sensory discriminative training and visual distortion interventions (as with graded motor imagery and mirror box therapies), there is purported evidence for the renormalization of S1 representation occurring in association with the attenuation of pain; and that some, but not all, of the morphological changes in brain grey matter volume and changes in cortical somatotopic distribution return to those seen in normal healthy subjects when pain is eliminated (Pelletier et al., 2015a). These studies support the idea that a multi-modal approach that is coherent and consistent with neuroplasticity-based learning principles, and with regard toward improving a body-schema basis of entrainment for the enhancement of both functional sensory acuity and proportionate motor dexterity – as are seen within the VRB3/FM protocol - are worthy of continued and collaborative investigation. The Advent and Recommendation of New Testing Instruments and Interventional Tools The tools and instruments used in the current study have attained long-standing and historical precedence for demonstrating their cross-comparative value upon successive re- 324 implementation; in terms of their valid and reliable acceptance and correlations to a broad variety of clinical research trials involving the ongoing assessment and treatment of outcomes for the recurring problem of low back pain, especially CNSLBP. However, as new fMRI study findings and their corresponding neuroplasticity mechanisms begin to unfold, and as new clinical observations begin to implicate the role of disrupted sensation-perception in movement, some new and innovative assessment scales and physical performance testing considerations are now discussed for future or pending applications being re-applied to the design of the current study. The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA). The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA) is a qualitative and quantitative scale that brings due consideration to the idea that mind-body interactions can invariably produce signaling mechanisms that can emanate from anywhere in the body in accordance with varied events, circumstances, constitutions, and coping styles, but are much more varied in terms of their arising into detectable consciousness awareness for different populations of individuals. The idea that awareness of interoceptive responses can prospectively play a major role in the treatment and prognosis of chronic pain and other kinds of problems was based upon purported benefits of somatic therapies such as Tai Chi, Mindfulness Meditation, Yogic practice, and the Feldenkrais Method® being used to self-manage them. The instrument was developed under the collaborative leadership of Dr. Wolf Mehling of The Osher Center for Integrative Medicine, Institute for Health and Aging, University of California, San Francisco, in conjunction with area practitioners in various relevant fields of mindfulness meditation, martial arts, yoga, mind-body medicine, and somatic arts and movement practices. The resulting 32-item multidimensional instrument was designed to measure key aspects of mind-body interaction with eight key feature concepts for categorical assessment being 325 comprised, developed and cross-validated with area individuals who were practicing mind-body therapies. The key areas include: ● Noticing – awareness of uncomfortable, comfortable, and neutral body sensations; ● Not Distracting – tendency not to ignore or distract oneself from sensations of pain or discomfort; ● Not Worrying – tendency not to worry or feel emotional distress with sensations of pain or discomfort; ● Attention Regulation – ability to sustain and control attention to body sensation; ● Emotional Awareness – awareness of the connection between body sensations and emotional states; ● Self-Regulation – ability to regulate psychological distress by attention to body sensations; ● Body Listening – active listening to the body for insight; and ● Trusting – experiences of one’s body as safe and trustworthy. The instrument was next applied and cross-validated to assess self-reported interoceptive awareness in primary care patients with past or current low back pain. It was then concluded that The MAIA may be useful in assessing changes in aspects of interoceptive awareness and in exploring the mechanism of action in trials of mind-body interventions in pain patients. The MAIA may therefore help in answering some important qualitative questions for the quantitative assessment of dominant states and learned skills that individuals may be actually experiencing and applying, while undergoing mind–body therapies (Mehling et al., 2013). However, a more recent study for comparative interventions for chronic nonspecific low back pain again found no difference between using a Feldnkrais Method trained physiotherapist ® 326 supervising a Feldenkrais-based Awareness through Movement class program and a customary ® course of treatment using an educational back school. The method also included the VAS-PAIN scales, the McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ), the Waddel Disability Index, and the Short Form36 Health Survey (SF-36), but this time also using the MAIA instrument, and found no difference between using a Feldnkrais Method trained physiotherapist supervising a ® Feldenkrais-based Awareness through Movement class program and a customary course of ® treatment using an educational back school. Its authors furthermore state that “the efficacy of the two approaches are the same” and that "a physician can recommend a body-mind rehabilitation approach, such as the Feldenkrais Method®, or an educational and rehabilitation program, such as a Back School to the patient, based on individual needs." In addition, "the two rehabilitation approaches are equally as effective in improving interoceptive awareness" (Paolucci et al., 2016). The content and design of this study, and how it was performed, has yet to be reviewed. However, it can be stated with confidence that prior trial failures arising from many of the other studies that have been conducted and reviewed and claiming to use “The Feldenkrais Method®,” as an intervention from the literature review section of this document, have in retrospective review been determined to not be truly representative of delivering the full method of the work in terms of selection, content allocation, accuracy, presence or depth. By comparative contrast, and within the context of the current dissertation study, there is much broader and intensive methodological detail being more accurately correspondent to actual Feldenkrais Method® applications, including the provisions for including at least some form of hands-on Functional Integration sessions. By these tokens, the research design is in direct contrast to much of what ® has been historically presented by research teams who have purportedly not had adequate apprenticeship nor full developmental exposure to the field. 327 The Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire (FreBAQ). Seeing that several new dimensions of evidence are now suggesting that body perception is altered in people with chronic back pain, and that disturbed body perception appears to be more strongly associated with pain intensity than psychological distress, fear avoidance beliefs, or pressure sensitivity-based thresholds as objective measures for lumbar spine sensitivity, Wand et al. of the School of Physiotherapy, The University of Notre Dame Australia, Fremantle, Western Australia, Australia, sought to develop and test new and intriguing questionnaire. In particular, a questionnaire for assessing the implications of how maladaptive perceptual awareness, specifically related to the back, might contribute to the pain experience as well as to serve as a guide for the clinical targeting of treatment. Now cited as the Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire (FreBAQ), it is a simple questionnaire comprising of nine items listed below: 1. My back feels as though it is not part of the rest of my body; 2. I need to focus all my attention on my back to make it move the way I want it to; 3. I feel as if my back sometimes moves involuntarily, without my control; 4. When performing everyday tasks, I don’t know how much my back is moving; 5. When performing everyday tasks, I am not sure exactly what position my back is in; 6. I can’t perceive the exact outline of my back; 7. My back feels like it is enlarged (swollen); 8. My back feels like it has shrunk; and 9. My back feels lopsided (asymmetrical). In their pilot sample of patients with chronic low back pain, N=251 respondents completed the questionnaire and were cross-compared with their taking a battery of clinical tests to discover that their level of altered self-perception was positively correlated with pain intensity 328 and disability as well as showing associations with psychological distress, pain catastrophizing, fear avoidance beliefs, and lumbar pressure pain threshold (Wand et al., 2016). Knowing that phenomenological perception is a central and appreciative feature of the Feldenkrais Method®, and now having correlates to other more common clinical determinants that theoretically account for the persistence and recurrence of chronic low back pain, this questionnaire needs and deserves further collaboration and integration into a future replication of the current study. Global perceived effect (GPE) scale. The Global Perceived Effect (GPE) scale is a single item scale that simply reflects the patient’s global impression of change by asking them to rate how much their overall condition has improved or deteriorated since some predefined time point. It has been recommended for use as a simple outcome measure for chronic pain trials more specifically asking: “How do you describe your situation today compared to when you first entered the study?” As a numerical 11-point scale with scores ranging from –5 (“much worse”) to +5 (“completely recovered”) through zero (“no change”), the patient simply rates where they are now in comparison to where they were before. Higher scores indicate higher clinical recovery. Intra-class correlation coefficient values of 0.90-0.99 indicate excellent test-retest reliability, and construct validity of the Global Perceived Effect (GPE) scale is also an excellent consideration for patients with musculoskeletal disorders (Kamper et al., 2010). The systemic and global effects emerging from Feldenkrais Method® sessions, while most often diffusely pleasurable and energetically revitalizing in terms of sensory experience, are also quite often uniquely intangible for placing into exact words or existing language constructs. So one benefit of administering a GPE scale is its ability to accept such generalized outcomes in more global terms, and yet convert and quantify their composite effects into a more tangible subset for measurable difference. 329 Alternatives to fitness-based physical performance testing. While the use of the timed endurance tests by McGill did reveal some interesting discoveries about imbalanced ratio comparisons emerging between the control group and the experimental group at postintervention, there were also added concerns and reservations about using this set of performance measures from a naturalistic perspective. For one, the required body planking and trunk postures involved for sustained position holding appear contextually dependent, para-functional, and overly rigidified to the meet the expectant biomechanical appeal of a North American fitnessstylized paradigm in lieu of achieving a more naturalistic, curvilinear, or organic one that is more true to everyday living in an average, but not necessarily athletic population. Another problem encountered, but equally distributed between groups, was my having to accommodate and adjust for unexpected orthopedic co-morbidities (such as shoulder impingement and rotator cuff problems that essentially precluded certain participants from having easy access for attaining a propped-arm side-planking test on the involved side) causing me to either abandon the test for that side, or to make hesitant modifications. Knowing that patients with CNSLBP can exhibit much uniform difficulty with everyday transitional movements, such as simply rising up from a chair in the waiting room, or in having to walk rapidly at a moment’s notice, or in having to change directions suddenly, it is therefore recommended that future studies begin with test comparing more functionally relevant ADL metrics, including the "Timed Up and Go" (TUG) test. The TUG simply involves having the patient begin in their accustomed seated positon in a standardized sized chair. When the therapist or examiner says, “Go” they stand up, walk exactly three meters, turn around and return to chair – to which the therapist or examiner stops the timer. While this test was originally developed to assess senior citizens and persons with 330 neurological disorders to assess dynamic balance and fall risk, it should also equally apply as a valid measure for motor control and trunk control for populations of patients with CNSLBP. From the perspective of the current study, it is hypothesized that a CSE/MCE group (cocontracting their core) would perform slower and more laboriously (by inadvertently working against themselves) whereas the VRB3/FM group would more likely perform faster and better given their corresponding awareness of body schema and movement trajectories becoming more elaborated between "pelvis-hips-legs" (as most proximal to center of gravity) opposite that of "head" (detection of change in center of gravity), giving primary functional emphasis for transferring up and forward over base of support in lieu of a decontextualized agenda for "contracting core muscles to protect the back" while getting-up. Similarly, implementing the use of either a Dynamic Gait Index (DGI) or the Functional Gait Assessment (FGA) would additionally provide a comparative basis for demonstrating both qualitative and quantitative functional changes in coordination and trunk control within the context of gait; though, originally intended for patients and populations with neurological balance problems and/or vestibular disorders to assess for posture stability, unsteadiness, and fall risks during dynamic and varied walking tasks. Again, it is hypothesized that a VRB3/FM group would more likely perform better, given that it has been clinically observed that the most stereotypically antalgic gait patterns that are commonly observed in patients with CNSLBP (retracted holding of pelvis backward on corresponding side of symptoms, non-reciprocating contra-lateral torso movements, top-down compressive momentum substitutive strategies, limping and lurching, decreased caudal propulsion, etc.) have all demonstrated clinical and transferrable improvement subsequent to Feldenkrais-based interventions. Again, the range of variations of head positional movements (e.g., vertical nodding contrasted by horizontal 331 rotation), speed, direction, level versus unlevel surfaces, pivoting and turning, stepping over an obstacle, eyes closed versus eyes open, narrow versus broad base of support, ambulating (walking) backwards – being cited as primary components of the DGI/FGA testing battery – are certainly more dependent on body schema acuity and motion trajectory than on the pre-activation of core muscles. With the development of ADPM gait and balance tracker sensor technology being worn on the body in conjunction with Mobility Lab software (Horak & Mancini, 2013), and then possibly combining them with additional metrics that can be generated through the Microsoft® Kinect skeletal avatar tracking system (as cited by Trost et al., 2015), there is no limit as to how far the linkages between body schema acuity, movement trajectories, and changes in movement compensation being dictated by pain-avoidance experiences can be further explored toward the continued development of new and novel validations for treatment intervention. Applications of Non-Muscular Paradigms in Clinical Practice Since the original and seminal works of Sir Charles Scott Sherrington (1857-1952) and his discovery of the synaptic reflex arc in the regulation of agonist-antagonist relationships in muscle, he furthermore expressed his theory that the nervous system acts as the coordinator of various parts of the body and that the reflexes are the simplest expressions of the interactive action of the nervous system; thus, enabling the entire body to function toward one definite end at a time, and by most typically being goal-directive and purposive via the establishment of postural reflexes and their dependence on the anti-gravity stretch reflexes being traced the afferent stimulus of the proprioceptive end organs (Pearce, 2004). Seeing the broader picture, the overall function as it exists beyond its implied causative or constitutive mechanisms, it seems that the prior emphasis of clinicians and therapists has been 332 to treat the muscle whether through strengthening or stretching, massaging or injecting, or through more discrete training via the mechanisms of SEMG and/or through multi-channel electromyography biofeedback, or through pressure unit biofeedback (PBU). But suppose the motor end-plate or individual constellation of motor units embedded within muscle tissue is only a microcosm, an isolated mechanism being only a mere constituent to more global or meaningful movement function, and therefore being the wrong effector or wrong end-organ from which to focus clinical attention? The perspective of Feldenkrais® practitioners is such that perception and action are cooptive and always on, and that the environmental task co-configurations are selective of proactive internal adjustments that impart the corresponding selection of an action pattern and movement trajectory, as a different kind of externalized reflex arc, thereby contributing to an expansive and radical conclusion that "total skeletal organization" (being a more direct and interactively operating interface in the immediate task environment) is more truly the correct effector or end-organ; thus, from which to better focus clinical attention, and in correspondence with the much important context of being more effectively and personally relevant toward better engaging the macrocosm-specific tasks of everyday and distributive life. So while muscles themselves must truly remain as the prime movers of individual bones, they should not to be erroneously construed as the prime organizers for the whole body being engaged in constructive and purposeful action, nor for the complete coordination of movement trajectories by virtue of simple assumption or additive expansion becoming isolated to either compartmentalized "origin -insertion" muscle labeling, nor through the focal activity of its motor end-plates. Therefore, in the application of movement improvement for humans, it is perhaps now time to newly emphasize more current and novel intervention approaches being geared toward 333 developing a constructive coordination of bones in lieu of traditional over-focusing upon the coordination of muscles, and perhaps most importantly, have these also become meaningful to the person. From his book, Neuromuscular Rehabilitation in Manual and Physical Therapies: Principles to Practice (2010), noted instructor, author, and reviewer Dr. Eyal Lederman, himself a practicing osteopath and research professor, brings forth a more expansive alternative to the usual and accustomed structural or pathologic model by instead proposing a co-created process model - an inclusive approach that encompasses the cognitive, behavioral, and neurophysiological dimensions of the individual. These features are summarized in his book via a compilation of five review recommendations that had corresponded well with the experimental intervention process that was already conducted within the scope of the current study: 1. Co-create an environment in which an individual’s recovery (including underlying biological processes of healing, neural regulation and repair) can be optimized. 2. Intervention should be all-inclusive…a combination of cognitive, psychosocial, behavioral, organizational, and neuromuscular approaches. 3. In the neurological dimension, there is no injury specific rehabilitation. A body area is rehabilitated according to its function rather than to the underlying pathology. Think movement --not muscles. 4. Neuromuscular Rehabilitation is a creative process --it is not protocol based. It is more about facilitating cognitive-sensory-motor processes by providing a stimulating and variations-rich environment. Recovery of motor control is an intrinsic person / nervoussystem process. It is not just exercising. 334 5. Specific exercises or techniques that aim to target, strengthen or "correct" a specific region, a muscle group, or other structural deviation are unlikely to contribute to recovery, nor serve as a means to improve daily function. This approach does not work. Forget about it (Lederman, 2010a, p. 171). Summary and Conclusions This single-blind, randomized controlled efficacy trial (RCT) successfully compared a Body Schema Acuity Training protocol using a newly applied, newly developed low-cost technology (Virtual Reality Bones™/VRB3) with a respected complementary-alternative, movement and manual therapy, neuroplasticity-based educational intervention (The Feldenkrais Method® as FM) against the most commonly accepted approach being utilized within current and conventional physical therapy practice settings (Core Stabilization Biofeedback Training and Graded Motor Control Exercises - as CSB/MCE). This was conducted for treating the widespread problem of chronic non-specific low back pain (CNSLBP), and determined that there is greater clinical efficacy being demonstrated by the new and novel VRB3/FM experimental intervention across all measures. Both the control group and the experimental group clearly benefitted from concurrent awareness of using non-habitual body sensations and respective movement experiences. In particular, becoming self-perpetuated as an implicit vs. explicit strategy that permitted a change in behavior such that they could begin to learn to "move differently" and with less pain, reduced disability, greater function, and longer sustained endurance activity times during all phases of treatment, as they were encountered throughout the study, but albeit much more so for the experimental group than the control group. 335 While current and existing standards in usual outpatient physical therapy and rehabilitation practice continues to implicate the entrainment of core musculature, more specifically the Transverse Abdominis (TrA) and the Lumbar Multifidus (LM), as a primary method for "control of stability" and "balance of action" around the spine, other implications inherent to low back function have also been stated. Specifically, to imply that “the back is a major highway of function in the body,” and that “when the spine loses its intrinsic support, problems ensue” and furthermore, that “being ‘upright’ involves a delicate balance in achieving effective control around the ‘line’ of gravitational force, and that one of the many challenges in the developmental progression is to strike the balance between too little and too much control” (Key, 2010). As a way to achieve and maintain such optimal control for dynamic balance between spinal stability and spinal mobility, and as a stated outcome from the current study, I maintain that a VRB3/FM-based approach emphasizing proportionate skeletal density and the awareness of vestibular pathways for "hips-pelvis-legs" opposite "head" (as both a corresponding experiential and cognitive framework for the enhancement and development of sensory acuity and working body schema) is more thoroughly effective than a CSB/MCE approach emphasizing an isolated muscle control strategy for the optimal coordination of trunk tone occurring within the daily activities of patients presenting and recovering from CNSLBP. A total synopsis of body schema acuity and its role for functionally referencing a continuous impression for navigating the world of perception and action through both a combination of multi-modal and multi-sensory awareness (i.e., sensory acuity) and its concurrent cognitive cultivation for the coordination of effective and proportionate action (i.e., motor learning and motor dexterity) is summarized by virtual reality applied researcher Giuseppe Riva (1998) below: 336 The physical world, including the body, is not given directly in our experience but is inferred through observation and critical reasoning. This means that, in everyday life, the representation of the body plays an important and often under-rated role. It is interesting to note that this representation is not limited to visual "images", i.e. pictures in one's head of one's body, but comprises the schema of all sensory input internally and externally derived—lived experiences processed and represented within a maturing psychic apparatus. In fact, this "virtual body" is not static but changes as a part of the dynamic process by which we try to organize and understand our experiences. The body schema is a model/representation of one's own body that constitutes a standard against which postures and body movements are (continuously) judged. This representation can be considered the result of comparisons and integrations at the cortical (and sub-cortical) level of past sensory experiences (postural, tactile, visual, kinesthetic and vestibular) with current sensations. This gives rise to an almost completely unconscious "plastic" reference model that makes it possible to move easily in space and to recognize the parts of one's own body in all situations. (p. 2) In conclusion, body schema based somatic education interventions, like the (VRB3)™/FeldenkraisMethod® protocol, appear more efficacious, and deserve further investigation. Furthermore, I propose and postulate the ‘prospective axiom’ that the cultivation of widespread functional improvements in mobility and function in terms of sensory acuity and motor dexterity are the "inverse relationship" and antithesis of perceptual distortions and dysfunctions involved in maintaining the conditions of continuation for producing widespread and continuing perception of chronic pain. I also propose that some form of introductory "Body Schema Acuity Training" should be instituted as a vital pre-requisite pre-emptive to all usual care and exercise prescription in outpatient physical therapy and rehabilitation settings, even to the extent that it deserves its own CPT procedural code as an entirely new and separate classification for treatment. I also commend that such skill levels have potentially high effect size and portable value and should therefore be compensated at much higher levels of fair reimbursement. This is therefore a broadsweeping and far-reaching premise to be tested and expanded through multiple future studies and 337 designs. Future multi-site RCT studies with larger sample sizes for statistical power are therefore both recommended and warranted. 338 REFERENCES Ackerman, C. J., & Turkoski, B. (2000). Using guided imagery to reduce pain and anxiety. Home Healthc Nurse, 18(8), 524-30. Adamovich, S. V., August, K., Merians, A., & Tunik, E. (2009). A virtual reality-based system integrated with fmri to study neural mechanisms of action observation-execution: a proof of concept study. Restor Neurol Neurosci, 27(3), 209-223. doi:10.3233/RNN-2009-0471 Adamovich, S. V., Fluet, G. G., Merians, A. S., Mathai, A., & Qiu, Q. (2009). Incorporating haptic effects into three dimensional virtual environments to train the hemiparetic upper extremity. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng, 17, 512–520. Adamovich, S. V., Fluet, G. G., Tunik, E., & Merians, A. S. (2009). Sensorimotor training in virtual reality: A review. NeuroRehabilitation, 25(1), 29-44. doi:10.3233/NRE-20090497. Airaksinen, O. (2006). Chapter 4: European guidelines for the management of chronic nonspecific low back pain. Eur Spine J, 15(Suppl. 2), S192-S300. doi:10.1007/s00586006-1072-1 Airaksinen, O., Brox, J. I., Cedraschi, C., Hildebrandt, J., Klaber-Moffett, J., Kovacs, F., Mannion, A. F., Reis, S., Staal, J. B., Ursin, H., & Zanoli, G. (2006). Cost B13 working group on guidelines for chronic low back pain. Eur Spine J, 15(Suppl 2), S192-300. doi:10.1007/s00586-006-1072-1 Akuthota, V., Ferreiro, A., Moore, T., & Fredericson, M. (2008). Core stability exercise principles. Curr Sports Med Rep, 7(1), 39-44. doi:10.1097/01.CSMR.0000308663.13278.69 Allison, G. T. (2012). Abdominal muscle feedforward activation in patients with chronic low back pain is largely unaffected by 8 weeks of core stability training. J Physiother, 58(3), 200. doi:10.1016/S1836-9553(12)70114-5 Alon, R. (1993a). Lesson 3: Long side, short side - relief through enhancing personal tendency. On Free your back [CD, Disc Two]. Portland, OR: The Feldenkrais Guild. Alon, R. (1993b). Lesson 5: Healing a hip through support reflex. On Free your back [CD, Disc Two]. Portland, OR: The Feldenkrais Guild. Angelaki, D. E., & Cullen, K, E. (2008). Vestibular system: The many facets of a multimodal sense. Annu Rev Neurosci, 31, 125-150. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125555 Apkarian, A. V., Hashmi, J. A., & Baliki, M. N. (2011). Pain and the brain: Specificity and plasticity of the brain in clinical chronic pain. Pain, 152(3 Suppl), S49–S64. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2010.11.010 Apkarian, A. V., Sosa, Y., Sonty, S., Levy, R. M., Harden, R. N., Parrish, T. B., & Gitelman, D. R. (2004). Chronic back pain is associated with decreased prefrontal and thalamic gray matter density. J Neurosci, 24(46), 10410-10415. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.254104.2004. Arnholt, A. T. (2012). BSDA: Basic Statistics and Data Analysis. R package version 1.01. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=BSDA Assendelft, W. J., Morton, S. C., Yu, E. I., Suttorp. M. J., & Shekelle, P. G. (2004). Spinal manipulative therapy for low back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, 2004(1), CD000447. Baggoley, C. (2015). Review of the Australian Government Rebate on Natural Therapies for Private Health Insurance. Australian Government – Department of Health. Retrieved from 339 http://health.gov.au/internet/main/publishing.nsf/content/0E9129B3574FCA53CA257BF 0001ACD11/$File/Natural%20Therapies%20Overview%20Report%20Final%20with%2 0copyright%2011%20March.pdf Baliki, M. N., Chialvo, D. R., Geha, P. Y., Levy, R. M., Harden, R. N., Parrish, T. B., & Apkarian, A. V. (2006). Chronic pain and the emotional brain: Specific brain activity associated with spontaneous fluctuations of intensity of chronic back pain. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 26(47), 12165– 12173. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3576-06.2006 Baliki, M. N., Schnitzer, T. J., Bauer, W. R., & Apkarian, A. V. (2011). Brain morphological signatures for chronic pain. PLoS One, 6(10), e26010. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0026010 Beinart, N. A., Goodchild, C. E., Weinman, J. A., Ayis, S., & Godfrey, E. L. (2013). Individual and intervention-related factors associated with adherence to home exercise in chronic low back pain: A systematic review. Spine J, 13(12), 1940-50. doi: 10.1016/j.spinee.2013.08.027 Bekrater-Bodmann, R., Foell, J., & Flor, H. (2011). Relationship between bodily illusions and pain syndromes. Pain Manag, 1(3), 217-28. doi:10.2217/pmt.11.20 Berger, L. (2008). Back pain eludes perfect solutions. New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2008/05/13/health/13back.html?_r=0 Beringer, E. (1999). The back and lungs support each other. On Embodied learning: Focus on the hips and low back with Elizabeth Beringer [CD, Disc Two]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Resources. Beringer, E. (Ed.). (2010). Embodied wisdom: The collected papers of Moshe Feldenkrais. Berkeley, CA: Somatic Resources. Bernard, A., Steinmuller, W., & Stricker, U. (2006). Ideokinesis: A creative approach to human movement & body alignment. Berkeley, CA: North Atlantic Books. Biering-Sorensen, F. (1984). Physical measurements as risk indicators for low-back trouble over a one-year period. Spine, 9, 106-119. Blakeslee S., & Blakeslee, M. (2008). The body has a mind of its own: How body maps in your brain help you do (almost) everything better. New York, NY: Random House Publishers. Boendermaker, B., Meier, M. L., Luechinger, R., Humphreys, B. K., & Hotz-Boendermaker, S. (2014). The cortical and cerebellar representation of the lumbar spine. Hum Brain Mapp. doi:10.1002/hbm.22451 Bogduk, N. (2004). Management of chronic low back pain. Med J Aust, 180(2), 79-83. Boonstra, A. M., Schiphorst Preuper, H. R., Balk, G. A., & Stewart, R. E. (2014). Cut-off points for mild, moderate, and severe pain on the visual analogue scale for pain in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain. Pain, 155(12), 2545-50. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2014.09.014 Boonstra, A. M., Schiphorst Preuper, H. R., Reneman, M. F., Posthumus, J. B., & Stewart, R. E. (2008). Reliability and validity of the visual analogue scale for disability in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain. Int J Rehabil Res, 31(2), 165-9. doi:10.1097/MRR.0b013e3282fc0f93 Borel, L., Lopez, C., Péruch, P., & Lacour, M. (2008). Vestibular syndrome: A change in internal spatial representation. Neurophysiol Clin, 38(6), 375-389. doi:10.1016/j.neucli.2008.09.002 Bowering, K. J., Butler, D. S., Fulton, I. J, & Moseley, G. L. (2014). Motor imagery in people with a history of back pain, current back pain, both, or neither. Clin J Pain, 30(12), 10701075. doi:10.1097/AJP.0000000000000066 340 Bowes, D. (2006). Lesson 5: Dynamic sitting with the right and left sides of the pelvic floor. On Pelvic health and awareness with Deborah Bowes PT CFT [CD, Disc Three]. San Francisco, CA: Learning for Health. Boyd, L., & Winstein, C. (2006). Explicit information interferes with implicit motor learning of both continuous and discrete movement tasks after stroke. J Neurol Phys Ther 30, 46–57, discussion 58–49. Bras, S. (2015). Core Stability. In Physiopedia. Retrieved from http://www.physiopedia.com/Core_stability Bray, H., & Moseley, G. L. (2011). Disrupted working body schema of the trunk in people with back pain. Br J Sports Med, 45(3), 168-73. doi:10.1136/bjsm.2009.061978 Briggs, A. M., Cross, M. J., Hoy, D. G., Sànchez-Riera, L., Blyth, F. M., Woolf, A. D., & March, L. (2016). Musculoskeletal health conditions represent a global threat to healthy aging: A report for the 2015 World Health Organization world report on ageing and health. Gerontologist, 56(Suppl 2), S243-55. doi:10.1093/geront/gnw002 Brill, M. W., & Couzens, G. S. (2001). The core program: Fifteen minutes a day that can change your life. New York, NY: Bantam. Browne, G. (2005). Lesson #7: Bending from standing. On Outsmarting low back pain [DVD, Disc Two]. Bellevue, WA: Movement Matters, Inc. Browne, G. (2006a). A manual therapist’s guide to movement: Teaching motor skills to the orthopedic patient. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier. Browne, G. (2006b). Lesson #15: Wishbone. On A manual therapist’s guide to movement CD Collection [CD, Disc Six]. Bellevue, WA: Movement Matters, Inc. Browne, G., & Browne, J. (2005). Outsmarting low back pain. Bellevue, WA: Movement Matters, Inc. Buchanan, P. A., & Ulrich, B. D. (2001). The Feldenkrais Method: A dynamic approach to changing motor behavior. Res Q Exerc Sport, 72(4), 315-23. Buchholz, V. N., Goonetilleke, S. C., Medendorp, W. P., & Corneil, B. D. (2012). Greater benefits of multisensory integration during complex sensorimotor transformations. J Neurophysiol, 107(11), 3135-43. doi:10.1152/jn.01188.2011 Burns, S. A., Mintken, P. E., Austin, G. P., & Cleland, J. (2011). Short-term response of hip mobilizations and exercise in individuals with chronic low back pain: A case series. J Man Manip Ther, 19(2), 100-7. doi:10.1179/2042618610Y.0000000007 Bushnell, M. C., Čeko, M., & Low, L. A. (2013). Cognitive and emotional control of pain and its disruption in chronic pain. Nat Rev Neurosci, 14(7), 502–511. doi:10.1038/nrn3516 Butera, K. A., Fox, E. J., & George, S. Z. (2016). Toward a transformed understanding: From pain and movement to pain with movement. Phys Ther, 96(10), 1503-1507. doi:10.2522/ptj.20160211 Butler, D. S., & Moseley, G. L. (2003). Explain pain. Adelaide, Australia: Noigroup Publications. Butz, M. V., Kutter, E. F., & Lorenz, C. (2014). Rubber hand illusion affects joint angle perception. PLoS One, 9(3), e92854. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0092854 Bystrom, M. G., Rasmussen-Barr, E., Grooten, W. J. (2013). Motor control exercises reduces pain and disability in chronic and recurrent low back pain: A meta-analysis. Spine 38(6), E350-8. 341 Cairns, M. C., Foster, N. E., & Wright, C. (2006). Randomized controlled trial of specific spinal stabilization exercises and conventional physiotherapy for recurrent low back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976), 31(19), E670-81. Čeko, M., Shir, Y., Ouellet, J. A., Ware, M. A., Stone, L. S., & Seminowicz, D. A. (2015). Partial recovery of abnormal insula and dorsolateral prefrontal connectivity to cognitive networks in chronic low back pain after treatment. Hum Brain Mapp, 36(6), 2075-92. doi:10.1002/hbm.22757 Centers for Disease Control. (2016). Draft CDC guideline for prescribing opioids for chronic pain — United States. (2016). Retrieved from https://www.regulations.gov/document?D=CDC-2015-0112-0002 Chang, Y. (2014). Reorganization and plastic changes of the human brain associated with skill learning and expertise. Front Hum Neurosci, 8, 35. Chapman, J. R., Norvell, D. C., Hermsmeyer, J. T., Bransford, R. J., DeVine, J., McGirt, M. J., & Lee, M. J. (2011). Evaluating common outcomes for measuring treatment success for chronic low back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976), 36(21 Suppl), S54-68. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e31822ef74d Cheikin, M. I. (1986-2011). The Feldenkrais method: Scientific basis for a new contribution to rehabilitation medicine from the world of art. St. Vincent's Hospital and Medical Center of New York, Department of Rehabilitation Medicine (212-790-8505,6,7). Draft C2. Retrieved from http://www.c4oh.org/papers/feld.pdf Chenot, J., Becker, A., Leonhardt, C., Keller, S., Donner-Banzhoff, N., Baum, E., . . . Kochen, M. M. (2007). Use of complementary alternative medicine for low back pain consulting in general practice: A cohort study. BMC Complementary and Alternative Medicine, 7, 42. doi:10.1186/1472-6882-7-42 Cherkin, D. C., Sherman, K. J., Balderson, B. H., Cook, A. J., Anderson, M. L., Hawkes, R. J., . . . Turner, J. A. (2016). Effect of mindfulness-based stress reduction vs cognitive behavioral therapy or usual care on back pain and functional limitations in adults with chronic low back pain: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA, 315(12), 1240-9. doi:10.1001/jama.2016.2323 Cibulka, M. T., Sinacore, D. R., Cromer, G. S., & Delitto, A. (1998). Unilateral hip rotation range of motion asymmetry in patients with sacroiliac joint regional pain. Spine, 23, 1009-1015. Cicchetti, D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychological Assessment, 6(4), 284–290. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.6.4.284 Claire, T. (2006). Bodywork: What type of massage to get and how to make the most of it. Laguna Beach, CA: Basic Health Publications, Inc. Cobb, E. (2015). Why your client’s back pain keeps returning: The vestibular system. Retrieved from http://www.ptonthenet.com/articles/why-your-clients-back-pain-keeps-returningthe-vestibular-system-part-one-3930#sthash.gkzhQOVZ.dpuf Coello, Y., & Delevoye-Turrell, Y. (2007). Embodiment, spatial categorisation and action. Conscious Cogn, 16(3), 667-83. Connors, K. A., Pile, C., & Nichols, M. E. (2011). Does the Feldenkrais method make a difference? An investigation into the use of outcome measurement tools for evaluating changes in clients. J Bodyw Mov Ther, 15(4), 446-52. doi:10.1016/j.jbmt.2010.09.001 342 Costa, L. O., Maher, C. G., Latimer, J., Hodges, P. W., Herbert, R. D., Refshauge, K. M., . . . Jennings, M. D. (2009). Motor control exercise for chronic low back pain: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Phys Ther, 89(12), 1275-86. doi:10.2522/ptj.20090218 Cozby, P. C. (2009). Methods in behavioral research (10th ed). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill Higher Education. Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52(4), 281–302. doi:10.1037/h0040957 Cullen, K. E. (2012). The vestibular system: Multimodal integration and encoding of self-motion for motor control. Trends Neurosci, 35(3), 185-196. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2011.12.001 Cupples, Z. (2015). Favorable inputs: A model for achieving outcomes. Retrieved from https://zaccupples.com/tag/louis-gifford/ Cynn, H. S., Oh, J. S., Kwon, O. Y., & Yi, C. H. (2006). Effects of lumbar stabilization using a pressure biofeedback unit on muscle activity and lateral pelvic tilt during hip abduction in sidelying. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, 87(11), 1454-8. Dagenais, S., Caro, J., & Haldeman, S. (2008). A systematic review of low back pain cost of illness studies in the United States and internationally. Spine J, 8(1), 8-20. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2007.10.005 Dallal, G. E. (1997). Sample size calculations simplified. Retrieved from http://www.jerrydallal.com/LHSP/SIZE.HTM Davis, K. D., & Moayedi, M. (2013). Central mechanisms of pain revealed through functional and structural MRI. J Neuroimmune Pharmacol, 8, 518–34. de Lange, F. P., Helmich, R. C., & Toni, I. (2006). Posture influences motor imagery: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 33(2), 609-17. de Rugy, A., Loeb, G. E., & Carroll, T. J. (2012). Muscle coordination is habitual rather than optimal. J Neurosci, 32(21), 7384-91. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5792-11.2012 de Vignemont, F. (2014). Shared body representations and the 'Whose' system. Neuropsychologia, 55, 128-36. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.08.013 Delitto A., George, S. Z., Van Dillen, L. R., Whitman, J. M., Sowa, G., Shekelle, P., . . . Godges, J. J. (2012). Low back pain: Clinical practice guidelines linked to the international classification of functioning, disability, and health from the orthopaedic section of the American Physical Therapy Association. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther, 42(4), A1-A57. doi:10.2519/jospt.2012.0301 Demoulin, C., Huijnen, I. P., Somville, P. R., Grosdent, S., Salamun, I., Crielaard, J. M., . . . Volders, S. (2013). Relationship between different measures of pain-related fear and physical capacity of the spine in patients with chronic low back pain. Spine J, 13(9), 1039-47. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2013.02.037 Demoulin, C., Vanderthommen, M., Duysens, C., & Crielaard, J. M. (2006). Spinal muscle evaluation using the Sorensen test: A critical appraisal of the literature. Joint Bone Spine, 73(1), 43-50. DePoy, E., & Gitlin, L. N. (1998). Introduction to research, understanding and applying multiple strategies. St. Louis, MO: Mosby. Deyo, R. A., Dworkin, S, F., Amtmann, D., Andersson, G., Borenstein, D., Carragee, E., . . . Weiner, D. K. (2014a). Report of the NIH task force on research standards for chronic low back pain. Spine, 39(14), 1128–1143. doi:10.1097/BRS.0000000000000434 Deyo, R. A., Dworkin, S. F., & Amtmann D., Andersson, G., Borenstein, D., Carragee, E., . . . Weiner, D. K. (2014b). Focus article report of the NIH Task Force on Research Standards 343 for Chronic Low Back Pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, 30(8), 701-712. Diers, M., Zieglgänsberger, W., Trojan, J., Drevensek, A. M., Erhardt-Raum, G., & Flor, H. (2013). Site-specific visual feedback reduces pain perception. Pain, 154(6), 890-6. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2013.02.022 Doidge, N. (2007). The brain that changes itself: Stories of personal triumph from the frontiers of brain science. New York, NY: Penguin Books Publishers. Doidge, N. (2015). The brain’s way of healing: Remarkable discoveries and recoveries from the frontiers of neuroplasticity. New York, NY: Penguin Books Publishers. Dolphens, M., Nijs, J., Cagnie, B., Meeus, M., Roussel, N., Kregel, J., . . . Danneels, L. (2014). Efficacy of a modern neuroscience approach versus usual care evidence-based physiotherapy on pain, disability and brain characteristics in chronic spinal pain patients: Protocol of a randomized clinical trial. BMC Musculoskelet Disord, 15, 149. doi:10.1186/1471-2474-15-149 Dr. Moshe Feldenkrais - His Life and Work, Karl Pribram and Moshe Feldenkrais. (2015a). Retrieved from http://www.feldenkraismethod.com/his-life/karl-pribram-moshefeldenkrais Dr. Moshe Feldenkrais - His Life and Work, Margaret Mead and Moshe Feldenkrais. (2015b). Retrieved from http://www.feldenkraismethod.com/his-life/margaret-mead-moshefeldenkrais Dreisinger, T. E. (2014). Exercise in the management of chronic back pain. Ochsner J, 14(1), 101-107. Eickhoff, S. B., Weiss, P. H., Amunts, K., Fink, G. R., & Zilles, K. (2006). Identifying human parieto-insular vestibular cortex using fMRI and cytoarchitectonic mapping. Hum Brain Mapp, 27(7), 611-621. Einstein, A. (n.d.) In AZQuotes.com. Retrieved on October 20, 2016, from http://www.azquotes.com/author/4399-Albert_Einstein/tag/science Fabel, K., & Kempermann, G. (2008). Physical activity and the regulation of neurogenesis in the adult and aging brain. Neuromolecular Med, 10(2), 59-66. doi:10.1007/s12017-0088031-4. Fairweather, M. M., & Sidaway, B. (1993). Ideokinetic imagery as a postural development technique. Res Q Exerc Sport, 64(4), 385-92. The Feldenkrais Institute, Acclaim & Praise. (2016). Retrieved from http://www.feldenkraisinstitute.com/about_feldenkrais/acclaim_praise?lid=ec_fptp0509 Feldenkrais, M. (1964). Mind and body. Systematics: The Journal of the Institute for the Comparative Study of History, Philosophy and the Sciences, 2(1). Feldenkrais, M. (1980). Mind and body. In G. Kogan (Ed.), Your body works. Berkeley, CA: Transformations. Feldenkrais, M. (1988). Bodily expression (T. Hanna, Trans). Somatics, 6(4). Feldenkrais, M. (1990). Awareness through movement: Health exercises for personal growth. San Francisco, CA: Harper Collins. (Original work published 1972) Feldenkrais, M. (2005). Body and mature behavior: A study of anxiety, sex, gravitation and learning. Berkeley, CA: Frog Books. (Original work published 1949) Feldenkrais, M. (2009). Awareness through movement: Easy-to-do health exercises to improve your posture, vision, imagination, and personal awareness (reprint ed.). San Francisco, CA: HarperOne. Feldenkrais, M. (2010). Mind and body. In E. Beringer (Ed.), Embodied wisdom: The collected 344 papers of Moshe Feldenkrais (pp. 27-44). Berkeley, CA: North Atlantic Books. Ferraz, M. B., Quaresma, M. R., Aquino, L. R., Atra, E., Tugwell, P., & Goldsmith, C. H. (1990). Reliability of pain scales in the assessment of literate and illiterate patients with rheumatoid arthritis. J Rheumatol, 17, 1022–4. Ferreira, P. H. (1), Ferreira, M. L., Maher, C. G., Herbert, R. D., & Refshauge, K. (2006). Specific stabilisation exercise for spinal and pelvic pain: A systematic review. Aust J Physiother, 52(2), 79-88. Fitzgerald, M. J., Gruener, G., & Mtui, E. (2012). Clinical neuroanatomy and neuroscience (6th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier Limited. Flor, H. (2002). The modification of cortical reorganization and chronic pain by sensory feedback. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback, 27(3), 215-27. Flor, H. (2003a). Cortical reorganisation and chronic pain: implications for rehabilitation. J Rehabil Med, (41 Suppl), 66-72. Flor, H. (2003b). Remapping somatosensory cortex after injury. Adv Neurol, 93, 195-204. Flor, H. (2012). New developments in the understanding and management of persistent pain. Curr Opin Psychiatry, 25(2), 109-13. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283503510 Flor, H., Braun, C., Elbert, T., & Birbaumer, N. (1997). Extensive reorganization of primary somatosensory cortex in chronic back pain patients. Neurosci Lett, 224(1), 5-8. Flor, H., & Diers, M. (2009). Sensorimotor training and cortical reorganization. NeuroRehabilitation, 25(1), 19-27. doi:10.3233/NRE-2009-0496 França, F. R., Burke, T. N., Caffaro, R. R., Ramos, L. A., & Marques, A. P. (2012). Effects of muscular stretching and segmental stabilization on functional disability and pain in patients with chronic low back pain: A randomized, controlled trial. J Manipulative Physiol Ther, 35(4), 279-85. doi:10.1016/j.jmpt.2012.04.012 França, F. R., Burke, T. N., Hanada, E. S., & Marques, A. P. (2010). Segmental stabilization and muscular strengthening in chronic low back pain: A comparative study. Clinics (Sao Paulo), 65(10), 1013-7. Franklin, E. (2010, November). Imagery Interventions: To improve and maintain practical human movement on a daily basis: The 7th Interdisciplinary World Congress on Low Back and Pelvic Pain, Post-Conference Workshop (Experiential Learning Modules), Los Angeles, CA. Fritz, J. M., & George, S. Z. (2002). Identifying psychosocial variables in patients with acute work-related low back pain: The importance of fear-avoidance beliefs. Physical Therapy, 82(10), 973-983. Retrieved from http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/82/10/973 Furlan, A. D., van Tulder, M., & Cherkin, D., Tsukayama H., Lao L., Koes B., & Berman, B. (2005). Acupuncture and dry-needling for low back pain: An updated systematic review within the framework of the Cochrane collaboration. Spine, 30, 944–963. Gallace, A., & Spence, C. (2008). The cognitive and neural correlates of "tactile consciousness": A multisensory perspective. Conscious Cogn, 17(1), 370-407. Gallagher, L., McAuley, J., & Moseley, G. L. (2013). A randomized-controlled trial of using a book of metaphors to reconceptualize pain and decrease catastrophizing in people with chronic pain. Clin J Pain, 29(1), 20-5. doi:10.1097/AJP.0b013e3182465cf7 Gallagher, S., & Cole, J. (1995). Body schema and body image in a deafferented subject. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 16(4), 369–390. 345 Galton, F. (1886). Regression towards mediocrity in hereditary stature. The Journal of the Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland, 15, 246–263. doi:10.2307/2841583 George, S. Z., Wittmer, V. T., Fillingim, R. B., & Robinson, M. E. (2010). Comparison of graded exercise and graded exposure clinical outcomes for patients with chronic low back pain. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther, 40(11), 694-704. doi:10.2519/jospt.2010.3396 Gibson, G., & Werrick, K. (Eds.). (2015). Fear-Avoidance Belief Questionnaire. In Physiopedia. Retrieved from http://www.physiopedia.com/Fear%E2%80%90Avoidance_Belief_Questionnaire Gilbert, J. W., Vogt, M., Windsor, R. E., Mick, G. E., Richardson, G. B., Storey, B. B., Herder, S. L., . . . Maddox, M. L. (2014). Vestibular dysfunction in patients with chronic pain or underlying neurologic disorders. J Am Osteopath Assoc, 114(3), 172-178. doi:10.7556/jaoa.2014.034 Gold, J. I., Belmont, K. A., & Thomas, D. A. (2007). The neurobiology of virtual reality pain attenuation. Cyberpsychol Behav, 10(4), 536–544. Gordon, J. (2006, January-February). Lectures conducted from Mind Body Medicine Professional Training, Berkeley, CA. Grotle, M., Vøllestad, N. K., & Brox, J. I. (2006). Clinical course and impact of fear-avoidance beliefs in low back pain: prospective cohort study of acute and chronic low back pain: II. Spine, 31(9), 1038-46. Gruther, W., Wick, F., Paul, B., Leitner, C., Posch, M., Matzner, M., . . . Ebenbichler, G. (2009). Diagnostic accuracy and reliability of muscle strength and endurance measurements in patients with chronic low back pain. J Rehabil Med, 41(8), 613-9. doi:10.2340/165019770391 Hall, A. M., Maher, C. G., Latimer, J., Ferreira, M. L., & Costa, L. O. (2011). The patientspecific functional scale is more responsive than the Roland Morris disability questionnaire when activity limitation is low. Eur Spine J, 20(1), 79-86. doi:10.1007/s00586-010-1521-8 Haller, J. (1997, January-April), Exploring the senses/creating new movements. Experiential learning modules conducted from Jeff Haller Public Workshops, Seattle, WA. Hanney, W. (2009). Interactive CD-ROM. On Testing, facilitation training for core stability [CD]. Rockledge, FL: Theralinx. Hardwick, R. M., Rottschy, C., Miall, R. C., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2013). A quantitative metaanalysis and review of motor learning in the human brain. Neuroimage, 67, 283-97. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.020 Hargrove, T. (2010). Back pain myths: Posture, core strength, bulging discs. Retrieved from http://www.bettermovement.org/blog/2010/back-pain-myths-posture-core-strengthbulging-discs Hargrove, T. (2012). Review of Paul Hodges talk on motor control, part one. Retrieved from http://www.bettermovement.org/blog/2012/review-of-paul-hodges-talk-on-motor-controlpart-one Harrar, V., Spence, C., & Makin, T. R. (2014). Topographic generalization of tactile perceptual learning. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 40(1), 15-23. doi:10.1037/a0033200 Hartcher-O'Brien, J., Levitan, C., & Spence, C. (2010). Extending visual dominance over touch for input off the body. Brain Res, 1362, 48-55. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.09.036 Hashmi, J. A., Baliki, M. N., Huang, L., Baria, A. T., Torbey, S., Hermann, K. M., . . . Apkarian, 346 A. V. (2013). Shape shifting pain: Chronification of back pain shifts brain representation from nociceptive to emotional circuits. Brain, 136(9), 2751–2768. http://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt211 Hayden, J. A., van Tulder, M. W., Malmivaara, A., & Koes, B. W. (2005). Exercise therapy for treatment of non-specific low back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, (3), CD000335. Hayden, J. A., van Tulder. M. W., & Tomlinson. G. (2005). Systematic review: Strategies for using exercise therapy to improve outcomes in chronic low back pain. Ann Intern Med, 142(9), 776–85. Henry, D. E., Chiodo, A. E., & Yang, W. (2011). Central nervous system reorganization in a variety of chronic pain states: A review. PM R, 3(12), 1116-25. doi:10.1016/j.pmrj.2011.05.018 Henschke, N. R., Ostelo, R. W., & van Tulder, M. W., Vlaeyen, J. W., Morley S., Assendelft, W. J., & Main, C. J. (2010). Behavioural treatment for chronic low-back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, 2010(7), CD002014. Herdman, S. J., & Clendaniel, R. (2014). Vestibular rehabilitation (4th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: F.A. Davis Company. Hides, J. A., Jull, G. A., & Richardson, C. A. (2001). Long-term effects of specific stabilizing exercises for first-episode low back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976), 26(11), E243-8. Hides, J. A., Richardson, C. A., Jull, G. A. (1996). Multifidus muscle recovery is not automatic after resolution of acute, first-episode low back pain. Spine, 21, 2763–2769. Hillier, S., & Worley, A. (2015). The effectiveness of the Feldenkrais Method: A systematic review of the evidence. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine: eCAM, 2015, 752160. doi:10.1155/2015/752160 Hodges, P. W. (2003). Core stability exercise in chronic low back pain. Orthop Clin North Am, 34(2), 245-54. Hodges, P. W., & Moseley, G. L. (2003). Pain and motor control of the lumbopelvic region: Effect and possible mechanisms. J Electromyogr Kinesiol, 13(4), 361-70. Hodges, P. W., & Richardson, C. A. (1996). Inefficient muscular stabilization of the lumbar spine associated with low back pain: A motor control evaluation of transversus abdominis. Spine, 21, 2640–50. doi:10.1097/00007632-199611150-00014 Hodges, P. W., & Smeets, R. J. (2015). Interaction between pain, movement, and physical activity: short-term benefits, long-term consequences, and targets for treatment. Clin J Pain, 31, 97-107. Hodges, P. W., & Tucker, K. (2011). Moving differently in pain: A new theory to explain the adaptation to pain. Pain, 152, S90–8. Hollis, S., & Campbell, F. (1999). What is meant by intention to treat analysis? Survey of published randomised controlled trials. BMJ: British Medical Journal, 319(7211), 670– 674. Holmes, N. P., & Spence, C. (2004). The body schema and the multisensory representation(s) of peripersonal space. Cogn Process, 5(2), 94-105. Holmes, N. P., & Spence, C. (2006). Beyond the body schema: Visual, prosthetic, and technological contributions to bodily perception and awareness. Human Body Perception from the Inside Out, 15-64. Horak, F. B., & Mancini, M. (2013). Objective biomarkers of balance and gait for Parkinson’s Disease using body-worn sensors. Movement Disorders: Official Journal of the Movement Disorder Society, 28(11), 1544–1551. doi:http://doi.org/10.1002/mds.25684 347 Hoy, D. (1), Bain, C., Williams, G., March, L., Brooks, P., Blyth, F., . . . Buchbinder, R. (2012). A systematic review of the global prevalence of low back pain. Arthritis Rheum, 64(6), 2028-37. doi:10.1002/art.34347 Hoy, D. (1), March, L., Brooks, P., Blyth, F., Woolf, A., Bain, C., . . . Buchbinder, R. (2014). The global burden of low back pain: Estimates from the global burden of disease 2010 study. Ann Rheum Dis, 73(6), 968-74. doi:10.1136/annrheumdis-2013-204428 Hasenbring, M. I. (1), Chehadi, O., Titze, C., & Kreddig, N. (2014). Fear and anxiety in the transition from acute to chronic pain: there is evidence for endurance besides avoidance. Pain Manag, 4(5), 363-74. doi:10.2217/pmt.14.36 Ingraham, P. (2016). Save yourself from low back pain! Retrieved from https://www.painscience.com/tutorials/low-back-pain.php Jack, K., McLean, S. M., Moffett, J. K., & Gardiner, E. (2010) Barriers to treatment adherence in physiotherapy outpatient clinics: a systematic review. Man Ther, 15(3), 220-8. doi:10.1016/j.math.2009.12.004 Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage, 14(1 Pt 2), S103-9. Jemmett, R. (2003). Spinal stabilization: The new science of back pain (2nd ed.). Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada: Libris Hubris Publishing. Jensen, M. C., Brant-Zawadzki, M. N., Obuchowski, N., Modic, M. T., Malkasian, D., & Ross, J. S. (1994). Magnetic resonance imaging of the lumbar spine in people without back pain. N Engl J Med, 331(2), 69-73. Jones, E. G., & Pons, T. P. (1998). Thalamic and brainstem contributions to large-scale plasticity of primate somatosensory cortex. Science, 282, 1121–5. Kalckert, A., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2014). The moving rubber hand illusion revisited: Comparing movements and visuotactile stimulation to induce illusory ownership. Conscious Cogn, 26, 117-32. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2014.02.003 Kambi, N., Halder, P., Rajan, R., Arora, V., Chand, P., Arora, M., & Jain, N. (2014). Large-scale reorganization of the somatosensory cortex following spinal cord injuries is due to brainstem plasticity. Nat Commun, 5, 1–10. Kamm, K., Thelen, E., & Jensen, J. L. (1990). A dynamical systems approach to motor development. Physical Therapy, 70(12), 763-775. Retrieved from http://ptjournal.apta.org/content/70/12/763 Kamper, S. J., Apeldoorn, A. T., Chiarotto, A., Smeets, R. J., Ostelo, R. W., Guzman, J., & van Tulder, M. W. (2014). Multidisciplinary biopsychosocial rehabilitation for chronic low back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, 9, CD000963. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD000963.pub3 Kamper, S. J., Ostelo, R. W., Knol, D. L., Maher, C. G., de Vet, H. C., & Hancock, M. J. (2010). Global Perceived Effect scales provided reliable assessments of health transition in people with musculoskeletal disorders, but ratings are strongly influenced by current status. J Clin Epidemiol, 63(7), 760-766.e1. doi:10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.09.009 Kane, M. (2004). Research made easy in complementary & alternative medicine. Edinburgh, Scotland, United Kingdom: Elsevier. Kanodia, A. K., Legedza, A. T., Davis, R. B., Eisenberg, D. M., & Phillips, R. S. (2010). Perceived benefit of complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) for back pain: A national survey. The Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine, 23(3), 354-362. 348 Karayannis, N. V., Smeets, R. J., van den Hoorn, W., & Hodges, P. W. (2013). Fear of movement is related to trunk stiffness in low back pain. PLoS One, 8(6), e67779. Key, J. (2010). Back pain: A movement problem. Edinburgh, Scotland, United Kingdom: Elsevier. Kim, S. S., Min, W. K., Kim, J. H., & Lee, B. H. (2014). The effects of VR-based Wii fit yoga on physical function in middle-aged female LBP patients. J Phys Ther Sci, 26(4), 549-52. doi:10.1589/jpts.26.549 Kleim, J. A., Barbay, S., Cooper, N. R., Hogg, T. M., Reidel, C. N., Remple, M. S., & Nudo, R. J. (2002). Motor learning dependent synaptogenesis is localized to functionally reorganized motor cortex. Neurobiol Learn Mem, 77, 63–77. Kleim, J. A., Markham, J. A., Vij, K., Freese, J. L., Ballard, D. H., & Greenough, W. T. (2007). Motor learning induces astrocytic hypertrophy in the cerebellar cortex. Behav Brain Res, 178(2), 244-9. Knaster, M. (1996). Discovering the body's wisdom. New York, NY: Bantam. Koumantakis, G. A., Watson, P. J., & Oldham, J. A. (2005). Trunk muscle stabilization training plus general exercise versus general exercise only: randomized controlled trial of patients with recurrent low back pain. Phys Ther, 85(3), 209-25. Kovacs, F. M., Muriel, A., Medina, J. M., Abraira, V., Sánchez, M. D., & Jaúregui, J. O. (2006). Psychometric characteristics of the Spanish version of the FAB questionnaire. Spine (Phila Pa 1976), 31(1), 104-10. Lafond, D., Dimmock, M., Champagne, A., & Descarreaux, M. (2009). Intrasession reliability and influence of breathing during clinical assessment of lumbar spine postural control. Physiother Theory Pract, 25(3), 218-27. doi:10.1080/09593980902776589. Laird, R. A., Kent, P., & Keating, J. L. (2012). Modifying patterns of movement in people with low back pain -does it help? A systematic review. BMC Musculoskeletal Disorders, 13, 169. doi:http://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2474-13-169 Lake, B. (1985). Acute back pain. Treatment by the application of Feldenkrais principles. Aust Fam Physician, 14(11), 1175-8. Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Wasserman, R. H., Gray, J. R., Greve, D. N., Treadway, M. T., . . . Fischl, B. (2005). Meditation experience is associated with increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport, 16(17), 1893–1897. Lederman, E. (2010a). Neuromuscular rehabilitation in manual and physical therapies: Principles to practice. London, England, United Kingdom: Churchill Livingstone. Lederman, E. (2010b). The myth of core stability. J Bodyw Mov Ther, 14(1), 84-98. doi:10.1016/j.jbmt.2009.08.001 Lederman, E. (2011). The fall of the postural-structural-biomechanical model in manual and physical therapies: exemplified by lower back pain. J Bodyw Mov Ther, 15(2), 131-8. doi:10.1016/j.jbmt.2011.01.011 Leeuw, M., Goossens, M. E., Linton, S. J., Crombez, G., Boersma, K., & Vlaeyen, J. W. (2007). The fear-avoidance model of musculoskeletal pain: Current state of scientific evidence. J Behav Med, 30(1), 77-94. Legrand, D., Brozzoli, C., Rossetti, Y., & Farnè, A. (2007). Close to me: Multisensory space representations for action and pre-reflexive consciousness of oneself-in-the-world. Conscious Cogn, 16(3), 687-99. Lehman, G. (2012). Core stability and pain: Is it time to stop using the word stability to explain pain? Retrieved from http://www.greglehman.ca/blog/2012/12/26/core-stability-and- 349 pain-time-to-stop-using-this-word-to-explain-pain Lehman, G. (2013). Core stability and low back pain: How stability exercises might help. Part two. Retrieved from http://www.greglehman.ca/blog/2013/01/01/core-stability-and-lowback-pain-how-stability-exercises-might-help-part-two Lehman, G. (2016). Exercise for pain – More about what you don’t do? Retrieved from http://www.greglehman.ca/blog/2016/5/31/exercise-for-pain-more-about-what-you-dontdo Lejkowski, P. M., & Poulsen, E. (2013). Elimination of intermittent chronic low back pain in a recreational golfer following improvement of hip range of motion impairments. J Bodyw Mov Ther, 17(4), 448-52. doi:10.1016/j.jbmt.2013.01.004 Leland, T. (2011). Trunk muscular endurance testing. Retrieved from http://www.sfu.ca/~leyland/Kin143%20Files/TrunkEnduranceTesting.pdfLenggenhager, B., Smith, S. T., & Blanke, O. (2006). Functional and neural mechanisms of embodiment: importance of the vestibular system and the temporal parietal junction. Rev Neurosci, 17(6), 643-57. Lewkowicz, D. J., & Ghazanfar, A. A. (2009). The emergence of multisensory systems through perceptual narrowing. Trends Cogn. Sci (Regul. Ed.), 13(11), 470–8. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.004 Li, A., Montaño, Z., Chen, V. J., & Gold, J. I. (2011). Virtual reality and pain management: current trends and future directions. Pain Manag, 1(2), 147-157. doi:10.2217/pmt.10.15 Linton, S. J. (2000). A review of psychological risk factors in back and neck pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976), 25(9), 1148-56. Little, P., Lewith, G., Webley, F., Evans, M., Beattie, A., Middleton, K., . . . Sharp, D. (2008). Randomised controlled trial of Alexander technique lessons, exercise, and massage (ATEAM) for chronic and recurrent back pain. Br J Sports Med, 42(12), 965-8. Lopez, C. (2013). A neuroscientific account of how vestibular disorders impair bodily selfconsciousness. Front Integr Neurosci, 7, 91. doi:10.3389/fnint.2013.00091 Lopez, C., & Blanke O. (2011). The thalamocortical vestibular system in animals and humans. Brain Res Rev, 67(1-2), 119–146. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.12.002 Lopez, C., Halje, P., & Blanke, O. (2008). Body ownership and embodiment: Vestibular and multisensory mechanisms. Neurophysiol Clin, 38(3), 149-61. doi:10.1016/j.neucli.2007.12.006 Lopez, C., Lenggenhager, B., & Blanke, O. (2010). How vestibular stimulation interacts with illusory hand ownership. Conscious Cogn, 19(1), 33-47. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.12.003 Lopez, C., Schreyer, H. M., Preuss, N., & Mast, F. W. (2012). Vestibular stimulation modifies the body schema. Neuropsychologia, 50(8), 1830-7. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.04.008 López-de-Uralde-Villanueva, I., Muñoz-García, D., Gil-Martínez, A., Pardo-Montero, J., Muñoz-Plata, R., Angulo-Díaz-Parreño, S., . . . La Touche, R. (2015). A systematic review and meta-analysis on the effectiveness of graded activity and graded exposure for chronic nonspecific low back pain. Pain Med. doi:10.1111/pme.12882 Lotze, M., & Moseley, G. L. (2007). Role of distorted body image in pain. Curr Rheumatol Rep, 9(6), 488-96. 350 Lotze, M., & Moseley, G. L. (2015). Theoretical considerations for chronic pain rehabilitation. Phys Ther, 95(9), 1316-20. doi:10.2522/ptj.20140581 Louw, A., Diener, I., Butler, D. S., & Puentedura, E. J. (2011). The effect of neuroscience education on pain, disability, anxiety, and stress in chronic musculoskeletal pain. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, 92(12), 2041-56. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2011.07.198 Louw, A., & Puentedura, E. (2013). Therapeutic neuroscience education, teaching patients about pain. Minneapolis, MN: OPTP. Louw, A., Puentedura, E. L, & Mintken, P. (2012). Use of an abbreviated neuroscience education approach in the treatment of chronic low back pain: A case report. Physiother Theory Pract, 28(1), 50-62. doi:10.3109/09593985.2011.562602 Louw, A,, Schmidt, S. G., Louw, C., & Puentedura, E. J. (2015). Moving without moving: Immediate management following lumbar spine surgery using a graded motor imagery approach: a case report. Physiother Theory Pract, 31(7), 509-17. doi:10.3109/09593985.2015.1060656 Luomajoki, H., & Moseley, G. L. (2011). Tactile acuity and lumbopelvic motor control in patients with back pain and healthy controls. Br J Sports Med, 45(5), 437-40. doi:10.1136/bjsm.2009.060731 Luoto, S., Heliovaara, M., Hurri, H., & Alaranta, H. (1995). Static back endurance and the risk of low back pain. Clin Biomech, 10, 323-324. Macedo, L. G., Latimer, J., Maher, C. G., Hodges, P. W., McAuley, J. H., Nicholas, M. K., . . . Stafford, R. (2012). Effect of motor control exercises versus graded activity in patients with chronic nonspecific low back pain: A randomized controlled trial. Phys Ther, 92(3), 363-77. doi:10.2522/ptj.20110290 Maher, C. G., Latimer. J., Hodges, P. W., Refshauge, K. M., Moseley, G. L., Herbert, R.D., . . . McAuley, J. (2005). The effect of motor control exercise versus placebo in patients with chronic low back pain [ACTRN012605000262606]. BMC Musculoskelet Disord, 6, 54. Malmgren-Olsson, E. B., & Bränholm, I. B. (2002). A comparison between three physiotherapy approaches with regard to health-related factors in patients with non-specific musculoskeletal disorders. Disabil Rehabil, 24(6), 308-17. Manchikanti, L., & Hirsch, J. A. (2015). What can be done about the increasing prevalence of low back pain and associated comorbid factors? Pain Management, 5(3), 149-152. doi:10.2217/pmt.15.10 Mannion, A. F., Caporaso, F., Pulkovski, N., & Sprott, H. (2012). Spine stabilisation exercises in the treatment of chronic low back pain: A good clinical outcome is not associated with improved abdominal muscle function. European Spine Journal, 21(7), 1301–1310. http://doi.org/10.1007/s00586-012-2155-9 Mannion, A. F., O'Riordan, D., Dvorak, J., & Masharawi, Y. (2011). The relationship between psychological factors and performance on the Biering-Sørensen back muscle endurance test. Spine J, 11(9), 849-57. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2011.08.004 Mansour, A., Baliki, M. N., Huang, L., Torbey, S., Herrmann, K., Schnitzer, T. J., & Apkarian, A. V. (2013). Brain white matter structural properties predict transition to chronic pain. Pain, 154(10), 2160–2168. doi:http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2013.06.044 Mansour, A. R., Farmer, M. A., Baliki, M. N., & Apkarian, A. V. (2014). Chronic pain: The role of learning and brain plasticity. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 32(1), 129– 139. doi:http://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-139003 351 Maravita, A., Spence, C., & Driver, J. (2003). Multisensory integration and the body schema: Close to hand and within reach. Curr Biol, 13(13), R531-9. Mast, F. W., Preuss, N., Hartmann, M., & Grabherr, L. (2014). Spatial cognition, body representation and affective processes: The role of vestibular information beyond ocular reflexes and control of posture. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 8, 44. doi:http://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2014.00044 Maughan, E. F., & Lewis, J. S. (2010). Outcome measures in chronic low back pain. Eur Spine J, 19(9), 1484-94. doi:10.1007/s00586-010-1353-6 May, S., Littlewood, C., & Bishop, A. (2006). Reliability of procedures used in the physical examination of non-specific low back pain: A systematic review. Aust J Physiother, 52(2), 91-102. McCabe, C. S. (2011). When illusion becomes reality. Rheumatology, 50, 2151–2. McGill, S. M. (2006). Ultimate back fitness and performance (2nd ed.). Waterloo, Ontario, Canada: Backfitpro Inc. McGill, S. M. (2007). Low back disorders: Evidence-based prevention and rehabilitation (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. McGill, S. M., Grenier, S., Bluhm, M., Preuss, R., Brown, S., & Russell, C. (2003). Previous history of LBP with work loss is related to lingering effects in biomechanical physiological, personal, and psychosocial characteristics. Ergonomics, 46(7), 731-746. McGill, S. M., & Karpowicz, A. (2009). Exercises for spine stabilization: Motion/motor patterns, stability progressions, and clinical technique. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, 90(1), 118-26. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2008.06.026 Medina, J., & Coslett, H. B. (2010). From maps to form to space: Touch and the body schema. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 645-54. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.017 Mehling, W. E. (2012, February). Non-surgical Approaches to Orthopaedic Conditions. Lectures conducted from American Academy of Orthopaedic Surgeons (AAOS) Annual Meeting, San Francisco, CA. Mehling, W. E., Daubenmier, J., Price, C. J., Acree, M., Bartmess, E., & Stewart, A. L. (2013). Self-reported interoceptive awareness in primary care patients with past or current low back pain. J Pain Res, 6, 403-18. doi:10.2147/JPR.S42418 Mehling, W. E., Gopisetty, V., Daubenmier, J., Price, C. J., Hecht, F. M., & Stewart, A. (2009). Body awareness: construct and self-report measures. PLoS One, 4(5), e5614. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005614 Mehling, W. E., Price, C., Daubenmier, J. J., Acree, M., Bartmess, E., & Stewart, A. (2012). The Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA). PLoS One, 7(11), e48230. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0048230. PMID: 23133619 Mehling, W. E., Wrubel, J., Daubenmier, J. J., Price, C. J., Kerr, C. E., Silow, T., . . . Stewart, A. L. (2011). Body awareness: A phenomenological inquiry into the common ground of mind-body therapies. Philos Ethics Humanit Med, 6, 6. doi:10.1186/1747-5341-6-6. Mellor, F. E., Thomas, P. W., Thompson, P., & Breen, A. C. (2014). Proportional lumbar spine inter-vertebral motion patterns: A comparison of patients with chronic, non-specific low back pain and healthy controls. Eur Spine J, 23(10), 2059-2067. Melzack, R. (2001). Pain and the neuromatrix in the brain. J Dent Educ, 65(12),1378-82. Melzack, R. (2005). Evolution of the neuromatrix theory of pain. The Prithvi Raj Lecture: presented at the third World Congress of World Institute of Pain, Barcelona 2004. Pain Pract, 5(2), 85-94. 352 Meredith, M. A., Nemitz, J. W., & Stein, B. E. (1987). Determinants of multisensory integration in superior colliculus neurons. I. Temporal factors. J Neurosci, 7(10), 3215–29. Meredith, M. A., & Stein, B. E. (1983) Interactions among converging sensory inputs in the superior colliculus. Science, 221(4608), 389–91. doi:10.1126/science.6867718 Meredith, M. A., & Stein, B. E. (1986a). Spatial factors determine the activity of multisensory neurons in cat superior colliculus. Brain Res, 365(2), 350–4. doi:10.1016/00068993(86)91648-3 Meredith, M. A., & Stein, B. E. (1986b). Visual, auditory, and somatosensory convergence on cells in superior colliculus results in multisensory integration. J Neurophysiol, 56(3), 640–62. Moller, A. (2014). Pain: Its anatomy, physiology, and treatment (2nd ed.). Dallas, TX: The University of Texas at Dallas, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences. Moseley, G. L. (2005). Widespread brain activity during an abdominal task markedly reduced after pain physiology education: fMRI evaluation of a single patient with chronic low back pain. Aust J Physiother, 51(1), 49-52. Moseley, G. L. (2008). I can't find it! Distorted body image and tactile dysfunction in patients with chronic back pain. Pain, 140(1), 239-43. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2008.08.001 Moseley, G. L., Butler, D. S., Beames, T. B., & Giles, T. J. (2012). The graded motor imagery handbook. Adelaide, Australia: Noigroup Publications. Moseley, G. L., & Flor, H. (2012). Targeting cortical representations in the treatment of chronic pain: A review. Neurorehabil Neural Repair, 26(6), 646-52. doi:10.1177/1545968311433209 Moseley, G. L., Gallace, A., & Spence, C. (2012). Bodily illusions in health and disease: Physiological and clinical perspectives and the concept of a cortical 'body matrix'. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 36(1), 34-46. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.013 Moseley, G. L., Gallagher, L., & Gallace, A. (2012). Neglect-like tactile dysfunction in chronic back pain. Neurology, 79(4), 327-32. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e318260cba2 Moseley, G. L., & Hodges, P. W. (2006). Reduced variability of postural strategy prevents normalization of motor changes induced by back pain: A risk factor for chronic trouble? Behav Neurosci, 120(2), 474-6. Moseley, G. L., Nicholas, M. K., & Hodges, P. W. (2004). Randomized controlled trial of intensive neurophysiology education in chronic low back pain. Clin J Pain, 20(5), 32430. Moseley, G. L., Zalucki, N. M., & Wiech, K. (2008). Tactile discrimination, but not tactile stimulation alone, reduces chronic limb pain. Pain, 137(3), 600-8. Moskowitz, M., Heit, H., McCarberg, W., Thernstrom, M., & Chestnut, R. (2012, March). Lectures conducted from 3rd Annual Spine Symposium, Pain Society of Oregon, Vancouver, WA. Muenchen, R. (2014). R passes SPSS in scholarly use, stata growing rapidly. Retrieved from http://r4stats.com/2014/08/20/r-passes-spss-in-scholarly-use-stata-growing-rapidly/ Muenchen, R. (2016). The popularity of data analysis software. Retrieved from http://r4stats.com/articles/popularity/ Nallasamy, N., & Tsao, D. Y. (2011). Functional connectivity in the brain: Effects of anesthesia. Neuroscientist, 17(1), 94-106. doi:10.1177/1073858410374126 The National Academies of Science, Engineering, Medicine, Health and Medicine Division. (2011). Relieving pain in America: A blueprint for transforming prevention, care, 353 education, and research (Report brief). Retrieved from http://www.nationalacademies.org/hmd/Reports/2011/Relieving-Pain-in-America-ABlueprint-for-Transforming-Prevention-Care-Education-Research/Report-Brief.aspx Nicholas, P., Hefford, C., & Tumilty, S. (2012). The use of the Patient-Specific Functional Scale to measure rehabilitative progress in a physiotherapy setting. J Man Manip Ther, 20(3), 147-52. doi:10.1179/2042618612Y.0000000006 Nijs, J., Meeus, M., Cagnie, B., Roussel, N. A., Dolphens, M., Van Oosterwijck, J., & Danneels, L. (2014). A modern neuroscience approach to chronic spinal pain: Combining pain neuroscience education with cognition-targeted motor control training. Phys Ther, 94(5), 730-8. doi:10.2522/ptj.20130258 Nijs, J., van Wilgen, C. P., Van Oosterwijck, J., van Ittersum, M., & Meeus, M. (2011). How to explain central sensitization to patients with 'unexplained' chronic musculoskeletal pain: Practice guidelines. Man Ther, 16(5), 413-8. doi:10.1016/j.math.2011.04.005 Norris, C., & Matthews, M. (2008). The role of an integrated back stability program in patients with chronic low back pain. Complement Ther Clin Pract, 14(4), 255-63. doi:10.1016/j.ctcp.2008.06.001 O'Keeffe, M., Purtill, H., Kennedy, N., Conneely, M., Hurley, J., O'Sullivan, P., . . . O'Sullivan, K. (2016). Comparative effectiveness of conservative interventions for nonspecific chronic spinal pain: Physical, behavioral/psychologically informed, or combined? A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Pain, pii, S1526-5900(16)00504-6. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2016.01.473 O'Sullivan, K., Dankaerts, W., O'Sullivan, L., & O'Sullivan, P. B. (2015). Cognitive functional therapy for disabling nonspecific chronic low back pain: Multiple case-cohort study. Phys Ther, 95(11), 1478-88. doi:10.2522/ptj.20140406 O'Sullivan, P. (2005). Diagnosis and classification of chronic low back pain disorders: Maladaptive movement and motor control impairments as underlying mechanism. Man Ther, 10(4), 242-55. O'Sullivan, P. (2012). It's time for change with the management of non-specific chronic low back pain. Br J Sports Med, 46(4), 224-7. doi:10.1136/bjsm.2010.081638 O'Sullivan, P. B., Mitchell, T., Bulich, P., Waller, R., & Holte, J. (2006). The relationship between posture and back muscle endurance in industrial workers with flexion-related low back pain. Man Ther, 11(4), 264-71. O'Sullivan, P. B., Phyty, G. D., Twomey, L. T., & Allison, G. T. (1997). Evaluation of specific stabilizing exercise in the treatment of chronic low back pain with radiologic diagnosis of spondylolysis or spondylolisthesis. Spine (Phila Pa 1976), 22(24), 2959-67. Ohman, A., Aström, L., & Malmgren-Olsson, E. B. (2011). Feldenkrais® therapy as group treatment for chronic pain--a qualitative evaluation. J Bodyw Mov Ther, 15(2), 153-61. doi:10.1016/j.jbmt.2010.03.003 Oneal, B. J., Patterson, D. R., Soltani, M., Teeley, A., & Jensen, M. P. (2008). Virtual reality hypnosis in the treatment of chronic neuropathic pain: A case report. Int J Clin Exp Hypn, 56(4), 451–462. Osborn, M., & Smith, J. A. (2006). Living with a body separate from the self. The experience of the body in chronic benign low back pain: An interpretative phenomenological analysis. Scand J Caring Sci, 20(2), 216-22. Ostelo, R. W., Deyo, R. A., Stratford, P., Waddell, G., Croft, P., Von Korff, M., . . . de Vet, H. C. (2008). Interpreting change scores for pain and functional status in low back pain: 354 Towards international consensus regarding minimal important change. Spine (Phila Pa 1976), 33(1), 90-4. doi:10.1097/BRS.0b013e31815e3a10. Ostelo, R. W., van Tulder, M. W., Vlaeyen, J. W., Linton, S. J., Morley, S. J., & Assendelft, W. J. (2005). Behavioural treatment for chronic low-back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, 2005(1), CD002014. Palazzo, C., Klinger, E., Dorner, V., Kadri, A., Thierry, O., Boumenir, Y., . . . Ville, I. (2016). Barriers to home-based exercise program adherence with chronic low back pain: Patient expectations regarding new technologies. Ann Phys Rehabil Med, 59(2), 107-13. doi:10.1016/j.rehab.2016.01.009 Panjabi, M. M. (2003). Clinical spinal instability and low back pain. J Electromyogr Kinesiol, 13(4), 371-9. Paolucci, T., Zangrando, F., Iosa, M., De Angelis, S., Marzoli, C., Piccinini, G., & Saraceni, V. M. (2016). Improved interoceptive awareness in chronic low back pain: A comparison of back school versus Feldenkrais Method. Disabil Rehabil, May 23, 1-8. doi:10.1080/09638288.2016.1175035 Papathanasiou, E. S., Papacostas, S. S., Charalambous, M., Eracleous, E., Thodi, C., & Pantzaris, M. (2006). Vertigo and imbalance caused by a small lesion in the anterior insula. Electromyogr Clin Neurophysiol, 46(3), 185-192. Parks, K. A., Crichton, K. S., Goldford, R. J., & McGill, S. M. (2003). On the validity of ratings of impairment for low back disorders. Spine, 28(4), 380-384. Pearce, J. (2004). Sir Charles Scott Sherrington (1857–1952) and the synapse. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 75(4), 544. Pelletier, R., Higgins, J., & Bourbonnais, D. (2015a). Addressing neuroplastic changes in distributed areas of the nervous system associated with chronic musculoskeletal disorders. Phys Ther, 95(11),1582-91. doi:10.2522/ptj.20140575 Pelletier, R., Higgins, J., & Bourbonnais, D. (2015b). Is neuroplasticity in the central nervous system the missing link to our understanding of chronic musculoskeletal disorders? BMC Musculoskelet Disord, 16, 25. doi:10.1186/s12891-015-0480-y Pengel, L. H., Refshauge, K. M., & Maher, C. G. (2004). Responsiveness of pain, disability, and physical impairment outcomes in patients with low back pain. Spine, 29(8), 879-83. Péruch, P., Lopez, C., Redon-Zouiteni, C., Escoffier, G., Zeitoun, A., Sanjuan, M., . . . Borel, L. (2011). Vestibular information is necessary for maintaining metric properties of representational space: Evidence from mental imagery. Neuropsychologia, 49(11), 313644. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.026 Petkova, V. I., Björnsdotter, M., Gentile, G., Jonsson, T., Li, T. Q., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2011). From part- to whole-body ownership in the multisensory brain. Curr Biol, 21(13), 111822. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.05.022 Pezzulo, G., Iodice, P., Ferraina, S., & Kessler, K. (2013). Shared action spaces: A basis function framework for social re-calibration of sensorimotor representations supporting joint action. Front Hum Neurosci, 7, 800. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00800 Philips, H. C. (2011). Imagery and pain: The prevalence, characteristics, and potency of imagery associated with pain. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 39, 1–18. doi:10.1017/S1352465811000282 Pisters, M. F., Veenhof, C., de Bakker, D. H., Schellevis, F. G., & Dekker, J. (2010). Behavioural graded activity results in better exercise adherence and more physical 355 activity than usual care in people with osteoarthritis: A cluster-randomised trial. J Physiother, 56(1), 41-7. Porter, J. L., & Wilkinson, A. (1997). Lumbar-hip flexion motion. A comparative study between asymptomatic and chronic low back pain in 18- to 36-year-old men. Spine, 22, 15081513. Postal, N. (Ed.). (2015). Roland-Morris Disability Questionnaire. In Physiopedia. Retrieved from http://www.physiopedia.com/Roland%E2%80%90Morris_Disability_Questionnaire Powers, C. M. (2009, October). Lower Quarter Biomechanics: New Research. Powerpoint slides presented at 2009 PTWA Fall Conference, Tacoma, WA. Preston, C., & Newport, R. (2011). Analgesic effects of multisensory illusions in osteoarthritis. Rheumatology, 50, 2314–5. Pritchett, D., Gallace, A., & Spence, C. (2011). Implicit processing of tactile information: Evidence from the tactile change detection paradigm. Conscious Cogn, 20(3), 534-46. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2011.02.006 Pugh, J. D., & Williams, A. M. (2014). Feldenkrais method empowers adults with chronic back pain. Holist Nurs Pract, 28(3), 171-83. doi:10.1097/HNP.0000000000000026 Questel, A. (2010, March). Skeletal Power, Optimizing Function through Muscle Inhibition and Skeletal Continuity. Experiential learning modules conducted from Alan Questel, Feldenkrais® Advanced Training, Kirkland, WA. Quinn, B. T., Carlson, C., Doyle, W., Cash, S. S., Devinsky, O., Spence, C., . . . Thesen, T. (2014). Intracranial cortical responses during visual-tactile integration in humans. J Neurosci, 34(1), 171-81. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0532-13.2014 R Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Retrieved from https://www.Rproject.org/ Ramakonar, H., Franz, E. A., & Lind, C. R. (2011). The rubber hand illusion and its application to clinical neuroscience. J Clin Neurosci, 18(12), 1596-601. doi:10.1016/j.jocn.2011.05.008 Ramond-Roquin, A., Bouton, C., Gobin-Tempereau, A. S., Airagnes, G., Richard, I., Roquelaure, Y., & Huez, J. F. (2014). Interventions focusing on psychosocial risk factors for patients with non-chronic low back pain in primary care: A systematic review. Fam Pract, 31(4), 379-388. Reiman, M. P., Weisbach, P. C., & Glynn, P. E. (2009). The hips influence on low back pain: A distal link to a proximal problem. J Sport Rehabil, 18(1), 24-32. Reese, M. (1995, 2005a). Lesson 1: Tilting the legs/rolling the feet. On The Feldenkrais Method: Moving out of pain with Mark Reese [CD, Disc One]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Resources. Reese, M. (1995, 2005b). Lesson 2: Rolling the head/bending the legs. On The Feldenkrais Method: Moving out of pain with Mark Reese [CD, Disc One]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Resources. Reese, M. (1995, 2005c). Lesson 7: Breathing movements. On The Feldenkrais Method: Moving out of pain with Mark Reese [CD, Disc Four]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Resources. Reese, M. (1995, 2005d). Lesson 8: Pelvis clock. On The Feldenkrais Method: Moving out of pain with Mark Reese [CD, Disc Five]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Resources. 356 Reese, M. (2015). Moshe Feldenkrais: A life in movement (Vol. 1). San Rafael, CA: ReeseKress Somatics Press. Richardson, C., Hodges, P., & Hides, J. (2004). Therapeutic exercise for lumbopelvic stabilization: A motor control approach for the treatment and prevention of low back pain (2nd ed.). London, England, United Kingdom: Churchill Livingstone. Richardson, C., Jull, G., Hodges, P. W., & Hides, J. A. (1999). Therapeutic exercise for spinal segmental stabilization in low back pain: Scientific basis and clinical approach (1st ed.). Sydney, Australia: Churchill Livingstone. Riva, G. (1998). Modifications in body-image induced by virtual-reality. Percept Mot Skills, 86(1), 163-70. Rockwell, T. (2005). From neither brain nor ghost. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Retrieved from http://schwitzsplintersunderblog.blogspot.com/2008/07/brief-introduction-to-dynamicsystems.html?m=1#!/2008/07/brief-introduction-to-dynamic-systems.html Rogers, B. P., Morgan, V. L., Newton, A. T., & Gore, J. C. (2007). Assessing functional connectivity in the human brain by fMRI. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 25(10), 1347– 1357. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.mri.2007.03.007 Roland, M., & Fairbank, J. (2000). The Roland-Morris Disability Questionnaire and the Oswestry Disability Questionnaire. Spine, 25(24), 3115-24. Ryan, C., Harland, N., Drew, B. T., & Martin, D. (2014). Tactile acuity training for patients with chronic low back pain: A pilot randomised controlled trial. BMC Musculoskelet Disord, 15, 59. doi:10.1186/1471-2474-15-59 Rydeard, R., Leger, A., & Smith, D. (2006). Pilates-based therapeutic exercise: effect on subjects with nonspecific chronic low back pain and functional disability: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Orthopaedic & Sports Physical Therapy, 36(7), 472-484. Rywerant, Y. (2000). Acquiring the Feldenkrais profession. Tel Aviv: El-Or Ltd. Sacks, O. (n.d.) In AZQuotes.com. Retrieved July 18, 2016, from http://www.azquotes.com/quote/1358739 Saner, J., Kool, J., de Bie, R. A., Sieben, J. M., & Luomajoki, H. (2011). Movement control exercise versus general exercise to reduce disability in patients with low back pain and movement control impairment: A randomised controlled trial. BMC Musculoskelet Disord, 12, 207. doi:10.1186/1471-2474-12-207 Sarkar, D. (2008) Lattice: Multivariate data visualization with R. New York, NY: Springer. Schaefer, M., Flor, H., Heinze, H. J., & Rotte, M. (2007). Morphing the body: Illusory feeling of an elongated arm affects somatosensory homunculus. Neuroimage, 36(3), 700-5. Schaefer, M., Heinze, H. J., & Rotte, M. (2005). Viewing touch improves tactile sensory threshold. Neuroreport, 16(4), 367-70. Schicke, T., Bauer, F., & Röder, B. (2009). Interactions of different body parts in peripersonal space: how vision of the foot influences tactile perception at the hand. Exp Brain Res, 192(4), 703-15. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1587-2 Schmidt, S. (2014, June). The dark side of neuroplasticity: Clinical test and treatment. Powerpoint slides presented at International Spine & Pain Institute Clinical Conference, Minneapolis, MN. Schmidt-Wilcke, T., Leinisch, E., Ganssbauer, S., Draganski, B., Bogdahn, U., Altmeppen, J., & May, A. (2006). Affective components and intensity of pain correlate with structural differences in gray matter in chronic back pain patients. Pain, 125, 89–97. 357 Schubiner, H., & Betzold, M. (2012). Unlearn your pain: A 28-day process to reprogram your brain (2nd ed.). Detroit, MI: Perfect Paperback Pub. Schultz, T. (Ed.). (2015). Regional interdependence. In Physiopedia. Retrieved from http://www.physio-pedia.com/Regional_Interdependence#cite_noteManipulations.2C_Inc-0 Schwoebel, J., Coslett, H. B., Bradt, J., Friedman, R., & Dileo, C. (2002). Pain and the body schema: effects of pain severity on mental representations of movement. Neurology, 59(5), 775-7. Schwoebel, J., Friedman, R., Duda, N., & Coslett, H. B. (2001). Pain and the body schema: evidence for peripheral effects on mental representations of movement. Brain, 124(Pt 10), 2098-104. Seminowicz, D. A., & Davis, K. D. (2007). Interactions of pain intensity and cognitive load: The brain stays on task. Cereb Cortex, 17(6), 1412–1422. Seminowicz, D. A., Wideman, T. H., Naso, L., Hatami-Khoroushahi, Z., Fallatah, S., Ware, M. A., . . . Stone, L. S. (2011). Effective treatment of chronic low back pain in humans reverses abnormal brain anatomy and function. J Neurosci, 31(20), 7540-50. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5280-10.2011 Sercomanens, A. (2012). Pain and the Feldenkrais Method some thoughts. Retrieved from http://whatisfeldenkrais.com/feldenkrais-method/feldenkrais-method-of-movementeducation Shaikh, A. G., Meng, H., & Angelaki, D. E. (2004). Multiple reference frames for motion in the primate cerebellum. J Neurosci, 24(19), 4491-7. Shelhav, C., & Golomb, D. (2003). A guide to awareness through movement 18 lessons based on the Feldenkrais Method. Israel: Shelhav Institute. Sherman, R. A. (2011). Pain: Assessment & intervention from a psychophysiological perspective (2nd ed.). Wheatridge, CO: AAPB Publications. Sibbald, B., & Roland, M. (1998). Understanding controlled trials: Why are randomised controlled trials important? BMJ, 316, 201. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.316.7126.201 Slade, S. C., Patel, S., Underwood, M., & Keating, J. L. (2013). What are patient beliefs and perceptions about exercise for non-specific chronic low back pain? A systematic teview of qualitative studies. Clin J Pain, 30(11), 995-1005. Smith, A., Kolt, G., & McConville, J. (2001). The effect of the Feldenkrais method on pain and anxiety in people experiencing chronic low back pain. New Zealand Journal Of Physiotherapy, 29(1), 6-14. Snodgrass, S. J., Heneghan, N. R., Tsao, H., Stanwell, P. T., Rivett, D. A., & Van Vliet, P. M. (2014). Recognising neuroplasticity in musculoskeletal rehabilitation: A basis for greater collaboration between musculoskeletal and neurological physiotherapists. Man Ther, 19(6), 614-7. doi:10.1016/j.math.2014.01.006 Sobie, T. (2013). P03.03. Body schema and Feldenkrais®: Effects upon subjects with low back pain. Global Advances in Health and Medicine, 2(Suppl), S136. doi:10.7453/gahmj.2013.097CP.P03.03 Solomonow, M., Zhou, B-H., Bratta, R. V., & Burger, E. (2003). Biomechanics and electromyography of a cumulative lumbar disorder: Response to static stretching. Clinical Biomechanics, 18, 890-898. 358 Soto-Faraco, S., Ronald, A., & Spence, C. (2004). Tactile selective attention and body posture: Assessing the multisensory contributions of vision and proprioception. Percept Psychophys, 66(7), 1077-94. Spence, C. (2010). Crossmodal spatial attention. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 1191, 182-200. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05440.x Spence, C., & Deroy, O. (2013). How automatic are crossmodal correspondences? Conscious Cogn, 22(1), 245-60. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2012.12.006 Spence, C., & Parise, C. V. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of crossmodal correspondences. Iperception, 3(7), 410-2. doi: 10.1068/i0540ic Spence, C., & Santangelo, V. (2009). Capturing spatial attention with multisensory cues: A review. Hear Res, 258(1-2), 134-42. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2009.04.015 Sperry, R. W. (n.d.). In AZQuotes.com. Retrieved July 18, 2016, from http://www.azquotes.com/author/26595-Roger_Wolcott_Sperry Staal, J. B., de Bie, R., de Vet, H. C., Hildebrandt, J., & Nelemans, P. (2008). Injection therapy for subacute and chronic low-back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, 2008(3), CD001824. Standaert, C. J., & Herring, S. A. (2007). Expert opinion and controversies in musculoskeletal and sports medicine: core stabilization as a treatment for low back pain. Arch Phys Med Rehabil, 88(12), 1734-6. Stat Trek Teach Yourself Statistics. (2016). Retrieved from http://stattrek.com/statistics/correlation.aspx Stein, B., & Meredith, M. A. (1993). The merging of the senses. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Stein, B. E., Stanford, T. R., & Rowland, B. A. (2009). The neural basis of multisensory integration in the midbrain: Its organization and maturation. Hear Res, 258(1-2), 4–15. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2009.03.012 Stephens, J. (2012, September). Research update. Powerpoint slides presented at Embodying Neuroscience, 2012 Feldenkrais Method® Conference & Esther Thelen Research Symposium, San Mateo, CA. Stinear, C. M., Byblow, W. D., Steyvers, M., Levin, O., & Swinnen, S. P. (2006). Kinesthetic, but not visual, motor imagery modulates corticomotor excitability. Exp Brain Res, 168(12), 157-64. Stratford, P., Gill, C., Westaway, M., & Binkley, J. (1995). Assessing disability and change on individual patients: A report of a patient specific measure. Physiotherapy Canada, 47(4), 258-263. Strickland, J. (2007). How virtual reality works. Retrieved from http://electronics.howstuffworks.com/gadgets/other-gadgets/virtual-reality.htm Sueki, D. G., Cleland, J. A., & Wainner, R. S. (2013). A regional interdependence model of musculoskeletal dysfunction: Research, mechanisms, and clinical implications. J Man Manip Ther, 21(2), 90-102. Taimela, S., & Luoto, S. (1999). Does disturbed movement regulation cause chronic back trouble? Duodecim, 115(16), 1669-76. Tecklin, J. S. (2014). Pediatric physical therapy (5th ed.). Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Thelen, E., Fogel, A., Ulrich, B., Rochat, P., Heriza, C., & Sheets-Johnstone, M., (2004, August). Movement and the development of sense of self. In R. Russell & E. Thelen (Co-chairs), A Mult-Disciplinary Symposium on Research and The Feldenkrais Method®. Symposium 359 conducted at the North American Feldenkrais Method® Annual Conference, Seattle, WA. Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Trost, Z. L., & Parsons, T. D. (2014). Beyond distraction: Virtual reality graded exposure therapy as treatment for pain-related fear and disability in chronic pain. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research, 19(2), 106–126. Trost, Z. L., Zielke, M., Guck, A., Nowlin, L., Zakhidov, D., France, C. R., & Keefe, F. (2015). The promise and challenge of virtual gaming technologies for chronic pain: The case of graded exposure for low back pain. Pain Manag, 5(3), 197-206. doi:10.2217/pmt.15.6 Tsao, H., Danneels, L. A., & Hodges, P. W. (2011). ISSLS prize winner: Smudging the motor brain in young adults with recurrent low back pain. Spine, 36, 1721–7. Tsao, H., Druitt, T. R., Schollum, T. M., & Hodges, P. W. (2010). Motor training of the lumbar paraspinal muscles induces immediate changes in motor coordination in patients with recurrent low back pain. J Pain, 11, 1120–8. Tsao, H., Galea, M., & Hodges, P. (2008). Reorganization of the motor cortex is associated with postural control deficits in recurrent low back pain. Brain, 131, 2161–71. Tsao, H. (1), & Hodges, P. W. (2008). Persistence of improvements in postural strategies following motor control training in people with recurrent low back pain. J Electromyogr Kinesiol, 18(4), 559-67. Tsakiris, M., Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Gallagher, S. (2007). On agency and body-ownership: Phenomenological and neurocognitive reflections. Conscious Cogn, 16(3), 645-60. Vahdat, S., Darainy, M., Milner, T. E., & Ostry, D. J. (2011). Functionally specific changes in resting-state sensorimotor networks after motor learning. J Neurosci, 31(47), 16907-15. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2737-11.2011 van den Hoorn, W., Bruijn, S. M., Meijer, O. G., Hodges, P. W., & van Dieën, J. H. (2012). Mechanical coupling between transverse plane pelvis and thorax rotations during gait is higher in people with low back pain. J Biomech, 45(2), 342-7. doi:10.1016/j.jbiomech.2011.10.024 van Gelder, T. J., & Port, R. (1995). It's about time: An overview of the dynamical approach to cognition. In R. Port & T. van Gelder (Eds.), Mind as motion: Explorations in the dynamics of cognition (pp. 1-43). Cambridge MA: MIT Press. Van Howten, D. (1997). Clarify your heart and lungs in your self image. On seeking our healing memories [CD, Disc Two]. Santa Fe, NM: Life Impressions Institute. Van Hulle, L., Juravle, G., Spence, C., Crombez, G., & Van Damme, S. (2013). Attention modulates sensory suppression during back movements. Conscious Cogn, 22(2), 420-9. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.01.011 van Middelkoop, M., Rubinstein, S. M., Verhagen, A. P., Ostelo, R. W., Koes, B. W., & van Tulder, M. W. (2010). Exercise therapy for chronic nonspecific low-back pain. Best Pract Res Clin Rheumatol, 24(2), 193-204. doi:10.1016/j.berh.2010.01.002. van Tulder, M., Malmivaara, A., Esmail, R., & Koes, B. (2000). Exercise therapy for low back pain: a systematic review within the framework of the Cochrane collaboration back review group. Spine, 25, 2784–2796. Ventre-Dominey, J., & Vallee, B. (2007). Vestibular integration in human cerebral cortex contributes to spatial remapping. Neuropsychologia, 45(2), 435-9. 360 Vera-Garcia, F. J., Moreside, J. M., & McGill, S. M. (2011). Abdominal muscle activation changes if the purpose is to control pelvis motion or thorax motion. J Electromyogr Kinesiol, 21(6), 893-903. doi:10.1016/j.jelekin.2011.08.003 Vibe Fersum, K., O'Sullivan, P., Skouen, J. S., Smith, A., & Kvåle, A. (2013). Efficacy of classification-based cognitive functional therapy in patients with non-specific chronic low back pain: A randomized controlled trial. Eur J Pain, 17(6), 916-28. doi:10.1002/j.1532-2149.2012.00252.x Villemure, C., & Bushnell. M. C. (2009). Mood influences supraspinal pain processing separately from attention. J Neurosci, 29(3), 705–715. Virtual Reality Society. (2016). What is virtual reality? Retrieved from http://www.vrs.org.uk/virtual-reality/what-is-virtual-reality.html Vlaeyen, J. W., & Crombez, G. (1999). Fear of movement/(re)injury, avoidance and pain disability in chronic low back pain patients. Man Ther, 4(4), 187-95. Vlaeyen, J. W. S., Kole-Snijders, A. M. J., Boeren, R. G. B., & van Eck, H. (1995). Fear of movement/(re)injury in chronic low back pain and its relation to behavioral performance. Pain, 62, 36, 272. Vleeming, A., & Fitzgerald, C. (Chairs). (2010, November). Lectures conducted from The 7th Interdisciplinary World Congress on Low Back and Pelvic Pain, Los Angeles, CA. Vos, T., Flaxman, A. D., & Naghavi, M., Lozano, R., Michaud, C., Ezzati, M., . . . Memish, Z. A. (2012). Years lived with disability (YLDs) for 1160 sequelae of 289 diseases and injuries 1990–2010: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet, 380, 2163–2196. Vrana, A., Hotz-Boendermaker, S., Stämpfli, P., Hänggi, J., Seifritz, E., Humphreys, B. K., & Meier, M. L. (2015). Differential neural processing during motor imagery of daily activities in chronic low back pain patients. PLoS ONE, 10(11), e0142391. doi:http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142391 Waddell, C., Newton, M., & Henderson, I., Somerville, D., & Main, C. J. (1993). A FearAvoidance Beliefs Questionnaire (FABQ) and the role of fear-avoidance beliefs in chronic low back pain and disability. Pain, 52, 157-168. Waddell, G. (2004). The back pain revolution (2nd ed). Edinburgh, Scotland, United Kingdom: Churchill Livingstone. Wagemans, J., Elder, J. H., Kubovy, M., Palmer, S. E., Peterson, M. A. Singh, M., & von der Heydt, R. (2012). A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: I. Perceptual grouping and figure-ground organization. Psychol Bull, 138(6), 1172–217. doi:10.1037/a0029333 Wainner, R. S., Whitman, J. M., Cleland, J. A., & Flynn, T. W. (2007). Regional interdependence: A musculoskeletal examination model whose time has come. J Orthop Sports Phys Ther, 37(11), 658-60. Wand, B. M., Abbaszadeh, S., Smith, A. J., Catley, M. J., & Moseley, G. L. (2013). Acupuncture applied as a sensory discrimination training tool decreases movement-related pain in patients with chronic low back pain more than acupuncture alone: A randomised crossover experiment. Br J Sports Med, 47(17), 1085-9. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2013-092949 Wand, B. M., Catley, M. J., Luomajoki, H. A., O'Sullivan, K. J., Di Pietro, F., O'Connell, N. E., & Moseley, G. L. (2014). Lumbar tactile acuity is near identical between sides in healthy pain-free participants. Man Ther, pii, S1356-689X(14)00003-4. doi:10.1016/j.math.2014.01.002 361 Wand, B. M., Catley, M. J., Rabey, M. I., O'Sullivan, P. B., O'Connell, N. E., & Smith, A. J. (2016). Disrupted self-perception in people with chronic low back pain. Further evaluation of the Fremantle Back Awareness Questionnaire. J Pain, 17(9), 1001-12. doi:10.1016/j.jpain.2016.06.003 Wand, B. M., Di Pietro, F., George, P., & O'Connell, N. E. (2010). Tactile thresholds are preserved yet complex sensory function is impaired over the lumbar spine of chronic non-specific low back pain patients: a preliminary investigation. Physiotherapy, 96(4), 317-23. doi:10.1016/j.physio.2010.02.005 Wand, B. M., James, M., Abbaszadeh, S., George, P. J., Formby, P. M., Smith, A. J., & O'Connell, N. E. (2014). Assessing self-perception in patients with chronic low back pain: Development of a back-specific body-perception questionnaire. J Back Musculoskelet Rehabil, 27(4), 463-473. Wand, B. M., Keeves, J., Bourgoin, C., George, P. J., Smith, A. J., O'Connell, N. E., & Moseley, G. L. (2013). Mislocalization of sensory information in people with chronic low back pain: a preliminary investigation. Clin J Pain, 29(8), 737-43. doi:10.1097/AJP.0b013e318274b320 Wand, B. M., O'Connell, N. E., Di Pietro, F., & Bulsara, M. (2011). Managing chronic nonspecific low back pain with a sensorimotor retraining approach: Exploratory multiplebaseline study of 3 participants. Phys Ther, 91(4), 535-46. Wand, B. M., Parkitny, L., O'Connell, N. E., Luomajoki, H., McAuley, J. H., Thacker, M., & Moseley, G. L. (2011). Cortical changes in chronic low back pain: Current state of the art and implications for clinical practice. Man Ther, 16(1), 15-20. doi:10.1016/j.math.2010.06.008 Wand, B. M., Tulloch, V. M., George, P. J., Smith, A. J., Goucke, R., O'Connell, N. E., & Moseley, G. L. (2012). Seeing it helps: movement-related back pain is reduced by visualization of the back during movement. Clin J Pain, 28(7), 602-8. Wang, X. Q., Zheng, J. J., Yu, Z. W., Bi, X., Lou, S. J., Liu, J., . . . Chen, P. J. (2012). A metaanalysis of core stability exercise versus general exercise for chronic low back pain. PLoS One, 7(12), e52082. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0052082. Wainner, R. S., Whitman, J. M., Cleland, J. A., & Flynn, T. W. (2007). Regional interdependence: A musculoskeletal examination model whose time has come. Journal of Orthopaedic & Sports Physical Therapy, 37(11), 658-660. Washington State Legislature. RCW 18.74.010, Physical Therapy Definitions. Retrieved from http://app.leg.wa.gov/RCW/default.aspx?cite=18.74.010 Wedding, D., & Corsini, R. (2014). Current psychotherapies (10th ed.). Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole Cengage Lea. Wertli, M. M., Burgstaller, J. M., Weiser, S., Steurer, J., Kofmehl, R., & Held, U. (2014). Influence of catastrophizing on treatment outcome in patients with nonspecific low back pain: A systematic review. Spine (Phila Pa 1976), 39(3), 263-73. doi:10.1097/BRS.0000000000000110 Wertli, M. M., Rasmussen-Barr, E., Held, U., Weiser, S., Bachmann, L. M., & Brunner, F. (2014). Fear-avoidance beliefs-a moderator of treatment efficacy in patients with low back pain: A systematic review. Spine J, 14(11), 2658-78. doi:10.1016/j.spinee.2014.02.033 Wildman, F. (2006a). Lesson 1: Folding your body with ease - Activating the abdominal flexors. On The intelligent body Feldenkrais program [CD, Volume I]. Berkeley, CA: 362 Feldenkrais Movement Institute. Wildman, F. (2006b). Lesson 2: Bending backwards - Activating the back extensors. On The intelligent body Feldenkrais program [CD, Volume I]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Movement Institute. Wildman, F. (2006c). Lesson 2: The magic spinal twist. On The intelligent body Feldenkrais program [CD, Volume II]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Movement Institute. Wildman, F. (2006d). Lesson 4: Lengthening the sides - Experiencing yourself in 3D. On The intelligent body Feldenkrais program [CD, Volume II]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Movement Institute. Wildman, F. (2006e). Lesson 5: Baby alligator. On The intelligent body Feldenkrais program [CD, Volume I]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Movement Institute. Wildman, F. (2009, February). Intelligent posture. Experiential learning modules conducted from Feldenkrais Movement Institute, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR. Williamson, E. (2006). Fear Avoidance Behavior Questionnaire. Austrailian Journal of Physiotherapy, 52, 149. Winance, M. (2006). Pain, disability and rehabilitation practices. A phenomenological perspective. Disabil Rehabil, 28(18), 1109-18. Witt, C. (2012, May). Advancing research literacy. Powerpoint slides presented at PreConference for International Research Congress for Integrative Medicine and Health, Portland, OR. Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269(5232), 1880-1882. doi:10.1126/science.7569931 Wolpert, D. M., & Miall, R. C. (1996). Forward models for physiological motor control. Neural Netw, 9(8), 1265-1279. World Health Organization. (2015). World report on ageing and health. Retrieved from http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/10665/186463/1/9789240694811_eng.pdf?ua=1 Wyszynski, M. (2013). Unity of mind and body in scientific research and the Feldenkrais Method: The example of gait. The Feldenkrais Journal, 26, 3-9. Yamato, T. P., Maher, C. G., Saragiotto, B. T., Hancock, M. J., Ostelo, R. W., Cabral, C. M., . . . Costa, L. O. (2015). Pilates for low back pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev, Jul 2(7), CD010265. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD010265.pub2 You, J. H., Kim, S. Y., Oh, D. W., & Chon, S. C. (2014). The effect of a novel core stabilization technique on managing patients with chronic low back pain: A randomized, controlled, experimenter-blinded study. Clin Rehabil, 28(5), 460-9. doi:10.1177/0269215513506231 Zemach-Bersin, D., & Reese, M. (1999). Track 2: Lengthening the spine. On relaxercise [CD, Disc Two]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Resources. Zhang, H., Xu, L., Wang, S., Xie, B., Guo, J., Long, Z., & Yao, L. (2011). Behavioral improvements and brain functional alterations by motor imagery training. Brain Res, 1407, 38-46. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.038 Zhang, H., Xu, L., Zhang, R., Hui, M., Long, Z., Zhao, X., & Yao, L. (2012). Parallel alterations of functional connectivity during execution and imagination after motor imagery learning. PLoS One, 7(5), e36052. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036052 zu Eulenburg, P., Baumgärtner, U., Treede, R. D., & Dieterich, M. (2013). Interoceptive and multimodal functions of the operculo-insular cortex: tactile, nociceptive and vestibular representations. Neuroimage, 83, 75-86. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.057 363 APPENDICES Appendix A: Recruitment Flyer for Pierce County Medical Society RECRUITMENT FLYER FOR PIERCE COUNTY MEDICAL SOCIETY 364 Appendix B: Pilot Study & Combined Conference Announcement Postcards PILOT STUDY & COMBINED CONFERENCE ANNOUNCEMENT POSTCARDS 365 Appendix C: Copy of Published Pilot Study Abstract COPY OF PUBLISED PILOT STUDY ABSTRACT Note: Reprinted from Scientific Abstracts Global Adv Health Med. 2013;2 (Suppl): P03.03. DOI: 10.7453/gahmj.2013.097CP. P03.03. Reprinted with permission via Pub Med Central. 366 Appendix D: Enrollment Invitation & Clinical Research Announcement Postcard ENROLLMENT INVITATION & CLINICAL RESEARCH ANNOUNCEMENT POSTCARD 367 368 Appendix E: Website Landing Pages for Alliant Spine Project, LTD WEBSITE LANDING PAGES FOR ALLIANT SPINE PROJECT, LTD. 369 Appendix F: Definitions of Terms and Acronyms DEFINITIONS OF TERMS AND ACRONYMS AAPB – Association for Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback ACC – Anterior Cingulate Cortex of brain ADL – Activities of Daily Living AROM – Active Range of Motion of limbs, torso and body segments ASIS – Anterior Superior Iliac Spine – a bony landmark of superficial protuberance ATM – Awareness Through Movement® – teaching through sequential movement progressions using The Feldenkrais Method®, and usually conducted in group classes A-P – Anterior Posterior direction, i.e. from front to back BSDA – Basic Statistics and Data Analysis CAM – Complementary and Alternative Medicine or Therapies CARF – Commission on Accreditation of Rehabilitation Facilities CBT – Cognitive Behavioral Therapy CEUs – Continuing Education Units for Allied Health Professionals CLBP – Chronic Low Back Pain CNSLBP – Chronic Non-Specific Low Back Pain CME – Continuing Medical Education CMT – Cognitive Manual Therapy – Hands-on manipulation and manual therapy techniques with intent to alter body perception away from ‘correction of illness pathology’ and forward toward ‘the development of functional possibility’ with particular emphasis upon exploiting the inherent robustness of skeletal density contiguities being distributed throughout the entire body as a whole. 370 CSB – Core Stabilization Biofeedback to train specificity for TrA and LM muscle recruitment CSB/MCE – Core Stabilization Biofeedback being combined with Motor Control Exercises DGI – Dynamic Gait Index assessment procedure DLPFC – Dorsal Lateral Pre-Frontal Cortex of brain EMG – Electromyography FABQ – Fear Avoidance Belief Questionnaire FABQw – Fear Avoidance Belief Questionnaire work sub-scale FABQpa – Fear Avoidance Belief Questionnaire physical activity sub-scale FC – Functional Connectivity on fMRI of brain FGA – Functional Gait Assessment FGNA – Feldenkrais Guild of North America FI – Functional Integration® – Hands-on individualized teaching using The Feldenkrais Method® FM – The Feldenkrais Method® and/or Feldenkrais-based movements F/E Ratios – Flexion/Extension Ratios of Trunk Endurance Testing (McGill, 2006) HEP – Home Exercise Program as prescribed by Physical Therapists HIPPA – Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act – protection of privacy IC – Insular Cortex of brain (Insula) ICC – Intraclass Correlation Coefficient – statistical comparison for test-retest reliability IFF – International Feldenkrais Federation I-S Joint – Ilia-Sacral Joint – as referenced from anterior to posterior aspect of pelvis LBP – Low Back Pain LM – Lumbar Multifidus – a primary lumbar intersegmental stabilizer muscle group M 1 – Primary Motor Cortex of brain 371 MCE – Motor Control Exercises MI – Motor Imagery MIC – Minimally Important Change MCID – Minimal Clinically Important Difference (Maughan & Lewis, 2010) NCCAM – National Centers for Complementary and Alternative Medicine NIH – National Institutes of Health NRS – Numerical Rating Scale NSAID’s – Non-Steroidal Anti-Inflammatory Drugs NSCSP – Non-specific Chronic Spine Pain; being essentially synonymous with CNSLBP OBMT – Object Based Mental Transformations OSI-R – Occupational Stress Inventory – Revised PBU – Pressure Biofeedback Unit – such as The Stabilizer TM PNE – Pain Neuroscience Education PSFS – Patient Specific Functional Scale PFC – Pre-Frontal Cortex of brain PT – Physical Therapy and/or Physical Therapist PTA – Physical Therapy Assistant R – R Statistical Software Package RCT – Randomized Controlled Trial RMDQ – Roland-Morris Disability Questionnaire S 1 – Primary Sensory Cortex of brain SCC’s – Semi-Circular Canals – as a major component of vestibular apparatus in inner ear S-EMG – Surface Electromyography 372 S-I Joint – Sacral-Iliac Joint SIJ – also referencing the Sacral-Iliac Joint SMA – Supplemental Motor Areas of brain STG – Superior Temporal Gyrus of brain STS – Superior Temporal Sulcus of brain TNSE – Therapeutic Neuroscience Education – as a cognitive behavioral approach to pain treatment TUG – Timed “Up and Go” Test – for sit, stand and walking balance TrA – Transverse Abdominis – as a primary muscle for core stabilization activation in LBP VAS – Visual Analog Scale VAS-PAIN – Visual Analog Scale for the Measurement of Pain perception VR – Virtual Reality VRB1 – Virtual Reality Bones via use of life-scaled skeletal anatomical models VRB2 – ‘Vital Relationships Between’ via the conduction of force transmissions through skeletal density pathways and deep articular joints using the proportionality of thirds method VRB3 – Vestibular Representation of the Body via using the three-dimensional coordinates of the semi-circular canals/vestibular apparatus as a metaphor and physiological mechanism for the representation of space. (VRB3) – Virtual Reality Bones as the trademark entity compositional of VRB1, VRB2, and TM TM VRB3. VRB3/FM – Combined Virtual Reality Bones and Feldenkrais-based movements for the entrainment of body schema acuity and effective action. 373 Appendix G: Copy of FABQ for Stratified Randomization COPY OF FABQ FOR STRATIFIED RANDOMIZATION 374 375 Appendix H: Copy of VAS-PAIN / Numerical Rating Scale COPY OF VAS-PAIN / NUMERICAL RATING SCALE a). b). ________________________________________________________________________ Appendix H: Gray-scale copy of a). VAS-PAIN numerical rating scale...being further embellished via b). with added multimodal qualifiers from which to have participants visually requantify their pain intensity during repeat administration throughout the course of the study. 376 Appendix I: Copy of RMDQ COPY OF RMDQ 377 Appendix J: Copy of PSFS COPY OF PSFS 378 Appendix K: Copy of Timed Endurance Testing Assessment Form COPY OF TIMED ENDURANCE TESTING ASSESSMENT FORM Procedure and Assessment Form used for McGill’s Timed Endurance Tests 379 Appendix L: Photo of Stabilizer Biofeedback (PBU) Device PHOTO OF STABILIZER BIO-FEEDBACK (PBU) DEVICE TM The Stabilizer™ Pressure Bio-feedback Unit (by Chattanooga Group, Hixon, TN, USA 380 Appendix M: Photo of Full Scale Skeletal Models & Source References PHOTO OF FULL SCALE SKELETAL MODELS & SOURCE REFERENCES Inner ear vestibular model from Anatomical Chart Company, Chicago, IL Full-scaled life-sized intact skeleton model from Anatomical Chart Company, Chicago, IL. Dis-articulated segmental models from Sawbones® Pacific Research Labs, Vashon Island, WA. 381 Appendix N: Sources Used for Control Group Intervention (MCE) SOURCES USED FOR CONTROL GROUP INTERVENTION (MCE) Hanney, W. (2009). Interactive CD-ROM. On Testing, facilitation training for core stability [CD]. Rockledge: Theralinx. Jemmett, R. (2003). Spinal Stabilization: The New Science of Back Pain (2nd ed.). Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada: Libris Hubris Publishing 382 Appendix O: Sources Used for Experimental Group Intervention (FM) SOURCES USED FOR EXPERIMENTAL GROUP INTERVENTION (FM) Individual selections derived from Feldenkrais ATM® movement sequences & programs: Alon, R. (1993a). Lesson 3: Long Side, Short Side - Relief Through Enhancing Personal Tendency. On Free Your Back, Disc Two [CD]. Portland, OR: The Feldenkrais Guild. Alon, R. (1993b). Lesson 5: Healing a Hip Through Support Reflex. On Free Your Back, Disc Two [CD]. Portland, OR: The Feldenkrais Guild. Beringer, Elizabeth (1999). The Back and Lungs Support Each Other 1. On Embodied Learning: Focus on the Hips and Low Back with Elizabeth Beringer, Disc Two [CD]. Berkley, CA: Feldenkrais Resources. 383 Bowes, Deborah (2006). Lesson 5: Dynamic sitting with the right and left sides of the pelvic floor. On Pelvic Health and Awareness with Deborah Bowes PT CFT, Disc Three. San Francisco, CA: Learning for Health. Browne, G. (2006b). Lesson #15: Wishbone. On A Manual Therapist’s Guide to Movement CD Collection, Disc 6 [CD]. Bellevue, WA: Movement Matters, Inc. Reese, M. (1995, 2005). Lesson 7: Breathing Movements. On The Feldenkrais Method: Moving Out of Pain with Mark Reese, Disc Four [CD]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Resources. Reese, M. (1995, 2005). Lesson 8: Pelvis Clock. On The Feldenkrais Method: Moving Out of Pain with Mark Reese, Disc Five [CD]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Resources. VanHowten, D. (1997). Clarify Your Heart and Lungs in Your Self Image. On Seeking Our Healing Memories, Disc 2 [CD]. Santa Fe, NM: Life Impressions Institute. Wildman, F. (2006). Lesson 1: Folding Your Body With Ease - Activating the Abdominal Flexors. On The Intelligent Body Feldenkrais Program, Volume I [CD]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Movement Institute. Wildman, F. (2006). Lesson 2: Bending Backwards - Activating the Back Extensors. On The Intelligent Body Feldenkrais Program, Volume I [CD]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Movement Institute. Wildman, F. (2006). Lesson 2: The Magic Spinal Twist. On The Intelligent Body Feldenkrais Program, Volume II [CD]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Movement Institute. Wildman, F. (2006). Lesson 4: Lengthening the Sides - Experiencing Yourself in 3D. On The Intelligent Body Feldenkrais Program, Volume II [CD]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Movement Institute. Wildman, F. (2006). Lesson 5: Baby Alligator. On The Intelligent Body Feldenkrais Program, Volume I [CD]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Movement Institute. Zemach-Bersin, D & Reese, M. (1999). Track 2: Lengthening the Spine. On Relaxercise, Disc Two [CD]. Berkeley, CA: Feldenkrais Resources. 384 Appendix P: Side-by-Side Listing of Treatment Interventions Between Groups SIDE-BY-SIDE LISTING OF TREATMENT INTERVENTIONS BETWEEN GROUPS (Appendix P is one continuum and continues the next six pages) 385 TABLE 2.1 Treatment Interventions administered 2x’s/week for 1st 2 Weeks PHASE I: Core Initiation Stabilization Training Visit# 1 vs. Rx Session Theme for VRB3/FM Group Perform Axial Loading from head to foot to discern Stance Leg (also include classic ‘Lay on Back’ Body Scan) Test ‘Hip Axis’ Awareness & use Femur Model as Visual-Tactile overlay to manually perform ‘Virtual Hip Replacement’ Rx Rx Session Detail for CSB/MCE Group Perform AROM Spine Screen, Detect Motion Instabilities Test & Manually Instruct Pelvis Floor + TrA isolated contraction 1st wk 2 1st wk 3 2nd wk SUPINE Position – Stabilizer Biofeedback Device to continue practicing “Draw-In” Maneuver 10 sets x10 s. Repeat in varied positions: Sitting Standing, Q-Ped, Kneel. Maintain Lumbar at N with Heel Slides, Clam Shells, Arm Raises. (Hanney, 2009, pp. 28-36; Jemmett, 2003, pp. 43-48). Test & manually Instruct Lumbar Multifidus (LM) activation from back side to match anterior Draw-In Maneuver from previous session. PRONE POSITION – Stabilizer Biofeedback Device for combined Pelvis Floor, TrA, and Multifidus Activation. Use combined ‘Draw-in Manuever’ to decrease to 9 mmHg Hold for 10 sx10 repetitions. Repeat in varied position: Supine, Sitting, Standing, Q-Ped. Continue to hold with heel slides and arm raises. (Hanney, 2009, pp. 42-44; Jemmett, 2003, p. 49) Joint Acuity/Skeletal Density Imagery SUPINE POSITION – Multi-sensory integration of Hip Axis in A-P pelvic rock (12-6) from feet, eyes, see-saw breathing, bell hands. UE RadialStyloid/Spine of Scapulae to spiral reach across midline to enable Lateral Pelvis Clock (9-3). Compare Perpendicular Hip Axis Schema in Sit, Stand, & Gait. Reframing ‘small of back’ SI Joint as new ‘top of leg’ from a deep anterior ilia-sacral perspective via outlining the inner ring-ridge of the pectineal line – as area of highest bone density. SUPINE POSITION (but also referencing from Lumbar posterior back side) ‘connecting the dots’ via a hand placement for leveraging ‘core density’ of deep anterior iliasacral joint as a base reference for distending pelvis and hips in a caudal rotation extension direction (to a 2/3 range) via semi-bridged leg and foot placement, countered by head rotation opposite to 1/3 range. Proportioned Movement via Head Lift (HODORF) Early Static Core Control Stabilization Progression with Activities involving ‘Keeping One Foot On the Ground.’ SUPINE POSITION – Stabilizer Biofeedback Device for Training the Corset Action of TrA and LM with Leg Loading/Alternating Leg Lifts(i.e. Dying Bug Exercise) Maintain 40 mmHg. 10 Secondsx10 reps for each leg. Repeat progression in Standing, Bridging, Sit, Q-ped, and Prone (Hanney, 2009, pp. 45-48; Jemmett, 2003, pp. 64, 74-75, 77-78) Anatomical Imagery for outlining the interface of a chain of pedicles juxtaposed with costovertebral/transverse-costal joints as an interplay between bone density stability along the thoracic spine, and distributive selections for ribcage mobility. SUPINE into SIDE-LYING transition via semibridge into contralateral elevation of costal arm reaching via support prop reference under posteriorlateral ilia onto trochanter. Compare Contralateral Weight Shift Alignments in Sit & Stand. 386 4 2nd wk Early Dynamic Core Stabilization Progression for Balance & Stability on Unstable Surfaces (Balls and Rollers) Model emphasis of semi-circular canal orientations of inner ear) vestibular apparatus) is demonstrated as a reference for 3 dimensions of balance in space and is encases within each Temporal Bone – the densest bone in the body. Side-Lying/Sitting Position – Stabilizer Biofeedback Device for training stability of position and preventing lateral pelvic tilt (maintaining 40 mmHg.) during side-lying Hip abduction-adduction. Progress to repeat in Sit Balance activities on standard-sized Gymnastic Physio Ball for Side Shift to Side, A-P; Lateral and Supine Bridging activities. (Hanney, 2009, pp. 49-52; Jemmett, 2003, pp. 51-53, 65-72) SIDE TILTS of Head position are correlated to side-shifts in pelvis laterality and weight distribution into sit bones while straddled over contoured bolster roll with leg positions lateralized. Posterior-lateral hip axis is correlated with trunk rotation and the plane of orientation for each vestibular organ. 387 TABLE 2.2 Treatment Interventions administered 2x’s/week for 2nd 2 Weeks Phase II: Visit# 5 rd 3 wk 6 rd 3 wk 7 th 4 wk Static into Dynamic Core Stability Rx Session Detail for Lumbar MCE Group Integrative Training - Static Middle Layer Exercises Exercises with Floor and Physioball vs. Expanding Sense of Ground Support Rx Session Theme for FM Group Feldenkrais® Functional Integration® Session: PRONE POSITION STABILIZATION Front bridging and planking activities on floor over forearms & elbows and over Physio ball and Foam Rollers. Maintain TrA & LM Draw-in Maneuver at all times. Hold for 10 Seconds X 10 Reps for each exercise. (Jemmett, 2003, pp. 8083). Pre-Crawl Multi-plane Developmental Variations emphasizing 3 spatial directions for Pelvis-hip and Head: Emphasis on translational ground support as a constant PRONE-SEMIPRONE POSTION on Firm FI Table or Floor: 1) Lumbar-Pelvic-Hip - Frog Leg Integration in 3-D 2) Fixed Arm Distal selecting Contralateral Frog Leg with MidThoracic Flexion-Rotation Synergy Integrative Training - Static Middle Layer Exercises Exercises with Floor and Physio ball Feldenkrais® Awareness Through Movement® Session: SUPINE POSITION STABILIZATION Back bridging and planking activities on floor and over Physio-ball – but this time permitting both feet to rise up and leave ground support ( i.e. Leg Lift). Maintain Pre-set of TrA & LM Draw-in Maneuver and Hold for 10 Seconds X 10 Reps for each exercise. (Jemmett, 2003, pp. 75-76, 78, 79). SUPINE Learning position generalized into Standing diagonal flexion, walking, and Stepup activities: ·Emphasis on Diagonal LQ Rotation opposite Head < Pelvis Wishbone Coupled with Diagonal Leg lift < Stand-Diagonal Flexion with external broom stick / dowel rod serving as ‘External Spine Column’ Integrative Training - Static Middle Layer Exercises Exercises with Floor and Physio ball Feldenkrais® Awareness Through Movement® Session: SIDE-LYING, STANDING & SITTING POSITION STABILIZATION Sidebridging from knees and planking activities, Single Leg Standing on Floor, Sit and Side-lay on Physio-ball – this time with 5 lb wt on lifted ankle.(Maintain Preset Draw-in maneuver at all times . Hold for 10 Seconds X 10 Reps for each exercise. (Jemmett, 2003, pp. 83, 64-69). SIDE-LYING transitions into SIDE-SITTING on either FI Table or Floor. LOP-SIDED SITTING on Firm Chair. Emphasis on Side-lengthening LQ Torso to Head: ·Elevated Hip Hiking into Leg Reach to Stand / Walk · Contralateral Weight Shift in Unbalanced Sit Stand via varied ground support of chair, table or floor 388 8 4th wk Integrative Training - Static Middle Layer Exercises Exercises with Floor and Physio ball QUADRUPED & KNEELING POSITION STABILIZATION and planking stability activities on Floor and over Physio ball. Maintain Pre-set TrA and LM Draw-in maneuver at all times . Hold for 10 Seconds X 10 Reps for each exercise. (Hanney, 2009, pp. 46-50; Jemmett, 2003, pp. 70, 86-87) Feldenkrais® Functional Integration® Session: QUADRUPED 4-POINTs POSITION: Kneeling over FI Table (KOT) FI ® Session differentiating into lateral weight shift & translational elevation of leg climbing, crossing midline, and proportionate nested contour of limb segment self-referencing. Transition ½ Kneel horizon scouting, arm spiraling, ½ Squat, and Re-visiting Chair Sitting. 389 TABLE 2.3 Treatment Interventions administered 1x/week for Last 4 Weeks Phase III: Dynamic into Reactive-Rhythmic Stability vs Reciprocating Variations of Movement Trajectories Visit# 9 5th wk 10 6th wk 11 7th wk Rx Session Detail for Lumbar MCE Group Dynamic-Reactive Core Motor Control Exercise SUPINE POSITION for Integrative and Advanced Training with diminished support of over Physio balls and Foam Rollers for leg lifts and back bridging while maintaining NEUTRALCORE STABILITY. Progress to applied manual resistance and perturbation via Rhythmic Stabilization and Clam Shell activities, (Hanney, 2009, pp. 49-53; Jemmett, 2003, pp. 74-79) then add 5 lb. free weight and elastic band resistance. (p. Hanney, 2009, pp. 45-46; Jemmett, 2003, p. 109) Rx Session Themes for FM Group Feldenkrais® Awareness Though Movement® Session Counter-Reciprocation of Support and Movement involves comparative relationships of trajectories at L vs R , Front & Back, and Upper vs Lower Body: ( Rotation / Counter-Rotation Motion Themes) ·Magic Spine Twist / Imagined Balance Beam ( Coordination of Flexors & Extensors in SUPINE) AND Carriage of Head affects state of the Musculature (PRONE LEG TILT swivel Side to Side mobilizes the Torso) Dynamic-Reactive Core Motor Control Exercise PRONE and QUADRUPED POSITION for Integrative and Advanced Training with diminished support of over Physio balls and Foam Rollers with applied counterresistance via elastic bands and motion perturbations and emphasizing Stability of Flexion vs. Extension. (Hanney, 2009, pp. 46-47, 49-51; Jemmett, 2003, pp. 80-83, 86-89) Feldenkrais® Awareness Though Movement® Session Distribution of Movement through active pressing and lifting , head-eye orientation, and balancing the selection between intention, attention & activation ( Diagonal Flexion / Extension Motion Themes ) Active Mid-Thoracic Extension into Big X Diagonals AND Activation of Flexors into Diagonal Rib Flexion ( First Prone, then Supine before Integration to Q-ped ) Dynamic-Reactive Core Motor Control Exercise SIDE BRIDGE / PLANKING and SITTING POSITION for Integrative and Advanced Training with diminished support of over Physio balls and applied counterresistance via elastic bands and motion perturbations for Lateral Trunk Stability. (Hanney, 2009, pp. 43-44, 48, 50-53; Jemmett, 2003, pp. 84, 87, 66-69) Feldenkrais® Awareness Though Movement® Session Distribution of Movement through Spatial-Temporal Coherence and Synergy of Proportionate Control from Isolated Part to Integrated Pattern: (Side-bend and Side-Lift Motion Themes) Side-lying Leg Rolling in Leg Tilting Side-Lying Leg Lift with Arm over Head Bracketing 390 12 8th wk Dynamic-Reactive Core Motor Control Exercise BRIDGING, SQUATTING, and STANDING POSITIONS for Integrative and Advanced Training with diminished support of Physio balls Elastic bands, Foam Rollers with emphasis on REVIEW OF HOME PROGRAM and Primary Core Stability Principles. (Hanney, 2009, pp. 51-55; Jemmett, 2003, pp. 85, 13-48 + Education Posters) Feldenkrais® Awareness Though Movement® Session Distribution of Resistance through Core Skeletal Density Pathways via Sensory - Motor Selections of Optimal Skeletal alignments in Proportion to Tosk: (Tri-Plane Summation of Motion Themes as HEP) Distal Extremity Platforms for Head Through Gate AND Tactile Acuity Surface Play for Distributive Resistance and Selecting Full Transmission of Skeletal Contiguity 391 Appendix Q: Copies of Home Exercise Program / Graded Activity Cover Sheets COPIES OF HOME EXERCISE PROGRAM / GRADED ACTIVITY COVER SHEEETS (Appendix Q is one continuum and continues for the next nine pages) 392 Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet Phase I: Joint Acuity/Skeletal Density Imagery Visit 1 1. “From your program today, practice the movement sequence and skeletal alignment(s) that helped you to detect or visualize the inner location of your newly discovered weight bear joints and how they dissipated stress. Include ‘laser beam’ soft touch”. 2. “Can you sense it from a variety of ways in manners that ‘connects the dots’ from bottom-up and through top down? Navigate and Sense it from both directions?” 3. At least once daily, follow the movement sequence that works best for one side. Find a connection that highlights your sense of constructing an inner line through your skeleton from that ‘standpoint’. Explore it again with slight variation for 5 times. 4. Rest before repeating same strategy on opposite side --but begin first with only imagined movement. Then do. 5. Throughout each day, discover and attend to how awareness of these areas can be included in daily activities as a background support for sitting, standing, walking, etc. Be sure to maintain a flexibly aligned, softly assembled quality of being --and not a hard focus. 6. As much as possible, get up and go somewhere, and try to spend a little bit more time enduring the activities of daily life. If you feel like you need less medication than normal, feel free to medicate less and experience life a bit more. 393 Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet Phase I: Joint Acuity/Skeletal Density Imagery Visit 2 1. “From your program today, practice the movement sequence and skeletal alignment(s) that helped you to detect or visualize the inner location of your newly discovered weight bear joints and how they dissipated stress. Include ‘laser beam’ soft touch”. 2. “Can you sense it from a variety of ways in manners that ‘connects the dots’ from bottom-up and through top down? Navigate and Sense it from both directions?” 3. At least once daily, follow the movement sequence that works best for one side. Find a connection that highlights your sense of constructing an inner line through your skeleton from that ‘standpoint’. Explore it again with slight variation for 5 times. 4. Rest before repeating same strategy on opposite side --but begin first with only imagined movement. Then do. 5. Throughout each day, discover and attend to how awareness of these areas can be included in daily activities as a background support for sitting, standing, walking, etc. Be sure to maintain a flexibly aligned, softly assembled quality of being --and not a hard focus. 6. As much as possible, get up and go somewhere, and try to spend a little bit more time enduring the activities of daily life. If you feel like you need less medication than normal, feel free to medicate less and experience life a bit more. 394 Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet Phase I: Joint Acuity/Skeletal Density Imagery Visit 3 1. “From your program today, practice the movement sequence and skeletal alignment(s) that helped you to detect or visualize the inner location of your newly discovered weight bear joints and how they dissipated stress. Include ‘laser beam’ soft touch”. 2. “Can you sense it from a variety of ways in manners that ‘connects the dots’ from bottom-up and through top down? Navigate and Sense it from both directions?” 3. At least once daily, follow the movement sequence that works best for one side. Find a connection that highlights your sense of constructing an inner line through your skeleton from that ‘standpoint’. Explore it again with slight variation for 5 times. 4. Rest before repeating same strategy on opposite side --but begin first with only imagined movement. Then do. 5. Throughout each day, discover and attend to how awareness of these areas can be included in daily activities as a background support for sitting, standing, walking, etc. Be sure to maintain a flexibly aligned, softly assembled quality of being --and not a hard focus. 6. As much as possible, get up and go somewhere, and try to spend a little bit more time enduring the activities of daily life. If you feel like you need less medication than normal, feel free to medicate less and experience life a bit more. 395 Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet Phase I: Joint Acuity/Skeletal Density Imagery Visit 4 1. “From your program today, practice the movement sequence and skeletal alignment(s) that helped you to detect or visualize the inner location of your newly discovered weight bear joints and how they dissipated stress. Include ‘laser beam’ soft touch”. 2. “Can you sense it from a variety of ways in manners that ‘connects the dots’ from bottom-up and through top down? Navigate and Sense it from both directions?” 3. At least once daily, follow the movement sequence that works best for one side. Find a connection that highlights your sense of constructing an inner line through your skeleton from that ‘standpoint’. Explore it again with slight variation for 5 times. 4. Rest before repeating same strategy on opposite side --but begin first with only imagined movement. Then do. 5. Throughout each day, discover and attend to how awareness of these areas can be included in daily activities as a background support for sitting, standing, walking, etc. Be sure to maintain a flexibly aligned, softly assembled quality of being --and not a hard focus. 6. As much as possible, get up and go somewhere, and try to spend a little bit more time enduring the activities of daily life. If you feel like you need less medication than normal, feel free to medicate less and experience life a bit more. 396 Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet Phase II: Expanding Sense of Ground Support Visits 5, 6, 7, & 8 1. “From your program today, practice the movement sequence and skeletal alignment(s) that helped you to detect or visualize the inner location of your newly discovered weight bear joints and how they dissipated stress. Include ‘laser beam’ soft touch.” 2. “Can you sense it from a variety of ways in manners that ‘connects the dots’ from bottom-up and through top down? Navigate and Sense it from both directions?” 3. At least once daily, follow the movement sequence that works best for one side. Find a connection that highlights your sense of constructing an inner line through your skeleton from that ‘standpoint’. Explore it again with slight variation for 5 times. 4. Rest before repeating same strategy on opposite side --but begin first with only imagined movement. Then do. 5. Throughout each day, discover and attend to how awareness of these areas can be included in daily activities as a background support for sitting, standing, walking, etc. Be sure to maintain a flexibly aligned, softly assembled quality of being --and not a hard focus. 6. As much as possible, get up and go somewhere, and try to spend a little bit more time enduring the activities of daily life. If you feel like you need less medication than normal, feel free to medicate less and experience life a bit more. 397 Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet Phase III: Reciprocating Variations of Movement Trajectories Visits 9, 10, 11, & 12 1. “From your program today, practice the movement sequence and skeletal alignment(s) that helped you to detect or visualize the inner location of your newly discovered weight bear joints and how they dissipated stress. Include ‘laser beam’ soft touch.” 2. “Can you sense it from a variety of ways in manners that ‘connects the dots’ from bottom-up and through top down? Navigate and Sense it from both directions?” 3. At least once daily, follow the movement sequence that works best for one side. Find a connection that highlights your sense of constructing an inner line through your skeleton from that ‘standpoint’. Explore it again with slight variation for 5 times. 4. Rest before repeating same strategy on opposite side --but begin first with only imagined movement. Then do. 5. Throughout each day, discover and attend to how awareness of these areas can be included in daily activities as a background support for sitting, standing, walking, etc. Be sure to maintain a flexibly aligned, softly assembled quality of being --and not a hard focus. 6. As much as possible, get up and go somewhere, and try to spend a little bit more time enduring the activities of daily life. If you feel like you need less medication than normal, feel free to medicate less and experience life a bit more. 398 Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet Phase I: Core Initiation Stabilization Training Visits 1, 2, 3, & 4 1. “At least once daily, practice those exercises that you feel gave you the best sense that you were strengthening your core - and for which you were able to maintain stability and control of your mid-section at all times”. 2. “Hold for 10 Repetitions at 10 seconds each- without losing proper form - at least once daily. 3. Throughout the day, practice your draw-in maneuver the same way you felt it happen when using the biofeedback device. 4. Maintain your draw-in maneuver before each progressive fitness exercise and whenever you think something might be strenuous –so as to maintain stability and control. 5. As much as possible, get up and go somewhere, and try to spend a little bit more time enduring the activities of daily life. If you feel like you need less medication than normal, feel free to medicate less and experience life a bit more. 399 Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet Phase II: Static into Dynamic Core Stability Visits 5, 6, 7, & 8 1. “At least once daily, practice those exercises that you feel gave you the best sense that you were strengthening your core - and for which you were able to maintain stability and control of your mid-section at all times”. 2. “Hold for 10 Repetitions at 10 seconds each- without losing proper form - at least once daily. 3. Throughout the day, practice your draw-in maneuver the same way you felt it happen when using the biofeedback device. 4. Maintain your draw-in maneuver before each progressive fitness exercise and whenever you think something might be strenuous –so as to maintain stability and control. 5. As much as possible, get up and go somewhere, and try to spend a little bit more time enduring the activities of daily life. If you feel like you need less medication than normal, feel free to medicate less and experience life a bit more. 400 Home Exercise Program/Graded Activity Cover Sheet Phase III: Dynamic into Reactive-Rhythmic Stability Visits 9, 10, 11, & 12 1. “At least once daily, practice those exercises that you feel gave you the best sense that you were strengthening your core- and for which you were able to maintain stability and control of your mid-section at all times”. 2. “Hold for 10 Repetitions at 10 seconds each- without losing proper form - at least once daily. 3. Throughout the day, practice your draw-in maneuver the same way you felt it happen when using the biofeedback device. 4. Maintain your draw-in maneuver before each progressive fitness exercise and whenever you think something might be strenuous –so as to maintain stability and control. 5. As much as possible, get up and go somewhere, and try to spend a little bit more time enduring the activities of daily life. If you feel like you need less medication than normal, feel free to medicate less and experience life a bit more. 401 Appendix R: Copies of Exercise / Graded Activity Adherence Diaries + Med Lists COPIES OF EXERCISE / GRADED ACTIVITY ADHERENCE DIARIES + MED LISTS (Appendix R is one continuum and continues for the next six pages) 402 Graded Activity Diary – Phase I: Joint Acuity/Skeletal Density Imagery Answer each of the following on a six-point scale: Please circle the number that best describes your ability All The Time = 5 Most of the Time = 4 Some of the Time = 3 Rarely or Seldom = 2 Never or Not at All = 1 Don’t really Know = 0 1. I was able to re-construct the basic movement sequences and matching skeletal alignment(s) that helped me to detect or visualize the inner location(s) and successive connections of newly discovered weight bear joints and how they could pass through or include the entirety of my skeleton from one end to the other during movement. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 2. I found that during my daily activities, I could locate my sense of joint positions at any moment, and could adjust my balance and alignment through them for greater comfort and support. I could also engage a broader sense of curious attention throughout the lines of distribution within my skeleton during unfamiliar or more demanding activities. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 3. I find I can spend a little bit more time engaging the things that I used to do without early onset of my fatigue and / or my pain worsening and stopping me from doing so. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 4. I am noticing I can get by with less medication and am reducing my frequency of use dosage accordingly. Medication Dose/ frequency of intake in a day. 1 2 3 Mo Tu Wed Th Fr Sa Su Dose Dos e Dos e Dose Dose Dose Dose 403 Graded Activity Diary – Phase II: Expanding Sense of Ground Support Answer each of the following on a six-point scale: Please circle the number that best describes your ability All The Time = 5 Most of the Time = 4 Some of the Time = 3 Rarely or Seldom = 2 Never or Not at All = 1 Don’t really Know = 0 1. I was able to re-construct the basic movement sequences and matching skeletal alignment(s) that helped me to detect or visualize the inner location(s) and successive connections of newly discovered weight bear joints and how they could pass through or include the entirety of my skeleton from one end to the other during movement. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 2. I found that during my daily activities, I could locate my sense of joint positions at any moment, and could adjust my balance and alignment through them for greater comfort and support. I could also engage a broader sense of curious attention throughout the lines of distribution within my skeleton during unfamiliar or more demanding activities. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 3. I find I can spend a little bit more time engaging the things that I used to do without early onset of my fatigue and / or my pain worsening and stopping me from doing so. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 4. 1 2 3 I am noticing I can get by with less medication and am reducing my frequency of use dosage accordingly. Medication Mo Tu Wed Th Fr Sa Su Dose/ frequency of intake in a day. Dose Dose Dose Dose Dose Dose Dose 404 Graded Activity Diary – Phase III: Reciprocating Variations of Movement Trajectories Answer each of the following on a six-point scale: Please circle the number that best describes your ability All The Time = 5 Most of the Time = 4 Some of the Time = 3 Rarely or Seldom = 2 Never or Not at All = 1 Don’t really Know = 0 1. I was able to re-construct the basic movement sequences and matching skeletal alignment(s) that helped me to detect or visualize the inner location(s) and successive connections of newly discovered weight bear joints and how they could pass through or include the entirety of my skeleton from one end to the other during movement. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 2. I found that during my daily activities, I could locate my sense of joint positions at any moment, and could adjust my balance and alignment through them for greater comfort and support. I could also engage a broader sense of curious attention throughout the lines of distribution within my skeleton during unfamiliar or more demanding activities. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 3. I find I can spend a little bit more time engaging the things that I used to do without early onset of my fatigue and / or my pain worsening and stopping me from doing so. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 I am noticing I can get by with less medication and am reducing my frequency of use dosage accordingly Medication Mo Tu Wed Th Fr Sa Dose/ frequency of intake in a day. 1 2 3 Dose Dose Dos e Dose Dose Dose Su Dose 405 Graded Activity Diary – Phase I: Core Initiation Stabilization Training Answer each of the following on a six-point scale: Please circle the number that best describes your ability All The Time = 5 Most of the Time = 4 Some of the Time = 3 Rarely or Seldom = 2 Never or Not at All = 1 Don’t really Know = 0 1. I chose mostly exercise numbers ____, ____, ____, ____, and did them mostly at Morning___, at Mid-day____, before bed____ for 10 times each. I was able to practice refining my Abdominal Draw-in Maneuver before each and every new exercise movement, and was able to hold for 10 seconds. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 2. I found that I was able to able to maintain stability and control of my mid-section at all times during my daily activities - and by especially practicing my draw-in maneuver- if the activity involved seemed more strenuous than usual. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 3. I find I can spend a little bit more time engaging the things that I used to do without early onset of my fatigue and / or my pain worsening and stopping me from doing so. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 4. I am noticing I can get by with less medication and am reducing my frequency of use dosage accordingly. Medication Dose/ frequency of intake in a day. 1 2 3 Mo Tu Wed Th Fr Sa Su Dose Dose Dos e Dose Dose Dose Dose 406 Graded Activity Diary – Phase II: Static into Dynamic Core Stability Answer each of the following on a six-point scale: Please circle the number that best describes your ability All The Time = 5 Most of the Time = 4 Some of the Time = 3 Rarely or Seldom = 2 Never or Not at All = 1 Don’t really Know = 0 1. I chose mostly exercise numbers ____, ____, ____, ____, and did them mostly at Morning___, at Mid-day____, before bed____ for 10 times each. I was able to practice refining my Abdominal Draw-in Maneuver—before each and every new exercise movement, and was able to hold for 10 seconds. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 2. I found that I was able to able to maintain stability and control of my mid-section at all times during my daily activities - and by especially practicing my draw-in maneuver- if the activity involved seemed more strenuous than usual. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 3. I find I can spend a little bit more time engaging the things that I used to do without early onset of my fatigue and / or my pain worsening and stopping me from doing so. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 4. I am noticing I can get by with less medication and am reducing my frequency of use dosage accordingly. Medication Dose/ frequency of intake in a day. 1 2 3 Mo Tu Wed Th Fr Sa Su Dose Dose Dos e Dos e Dose Dose Dose 407 Graded Activity Diary – Phase III: Dynamic-Reactive Rhythmic Stability Answer each of the following on a six-point scale: Please circle the number that best describes your ability All The Time = 5 Most of the Time = 4 Some of the Time = 3 Rarely or Seldom = 2 Never or Not at All = 1 Don’t really Know = 0 1. I chose mostly exercise numbers ____, ____, ____, ____, and did them mostly at Morning___, at Mid-day____, before bed____ for 10 times each. I was able to practice refining my Abdominal Draw-in Maneuver—before each and every new exercise movement, and was able to hold for 10 seconds. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 2. I found that I was able to able to maintain stability and control of my mid-section at all times during my daily activities - and by especially practicing my draw-in maneuver- if the activity involved seemed more strenuous than usual. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 3. I find I can spend a little bit more time engaging the things that I used to do without early onset of my fatigue and / or my pain worsening and stopping me from doing so. ≥_______________≤ 0 1 2 3 4 5 4. I am noticing I can get by with less medication and am reducing my frequency of use dosage accordingly. Medication Mo Tu Wed Th Fr Sa Su Dose/ frequency of intake in a day. Dos e Dos e Dos e Dos e Dose Dose Dose 1 2 3 408 Appendix S: Qualitative Differences between FM & CSE / MCE QUALITATIVE DIFFERENCES BETWEEN FM & CSE / MCE (Appendix S is one continuum and continues the next three pages) Appendix S outlines qualitative differences, principles, and philosophical foundations occurring between traditional Core Stabilization Training and conventional Motor Control Exercise programs...versus...Feldenkrais® Somatic Education approaches to Skeletal Density & Vestibular Imagery (VRB3) being inclusive of other movement themes, explorations and inquiries. 409 Core Stabilization Exercise protocol for Facilitating TrA and Multifidus Feldenkrais® Somatic Education & VRB3 Skeletal Density Imagery DERIVATIONS DERIVATIONS Concept applied is/was achieved by Deductive Reasoning Concept(s) applied are achieved by Inductive Reasoning Identification of specific cause (1 or 2 variables) to account for controlled reproducible effects Observing, invoking, and identifying many variables in interaction and giving them equal weight of opportunity to influence a system Explicit demonstration for proper Rx performance Implicit Exploration for successive proximation Integrating variables / Naturalistic research Isolating a variable / Empirical research BEHAVIORAL REFERENCE BEHAVIORAL REFERENCE Specificity of muscle contraction Specificity of sensory discrimination Muscle Selection precedes the Functional Task Task Demands Organize the Skeletal Behavior MECHANISMS MECHANISMS Hidden Muscles ● Hidden Senses Activated explicitly in a routine fashion as a feature or precursor to exercise and daily activities. DIRECTIVES Concentration of effort as a reference: • “Keep constant core to stabilize your back” • Isolated sub-maximal effort at 10% = law of protective corset surrounding spinal column between pelvis and ribs. • Concept for ‘regional interdependence’ remains mostly compartmentalized to diaphragm and pelvis floor as governed by TrA and LM motor control training and exercise. • Operationalized through the four-phased progression Queensland Australia model. ● Activated implicitly as a manner of noticing and being in the functions of everyday life DIRECTIVES Distribution of effort as a reference • i.e. “One area lifts from gravity/another area yields to gravity” • Task selected allocations of action with proportional distribution of effort throughout the entire skeleton. • Concept for ‘regional interdependence’ extends throughout the entire continuity of the skeleton and whole-self; from base of feet to top of head and vice-versa. • Operationalized through “The Law of Proportionate Thirds” 410 Core Stabilization Exercise protocol for Facilitating TrA and Multifidus (Appendix S Continued) Feldenkrais® Somatic Education & VRB3 Skeletal Density Imagery (Appendix S Continued) PROCEDURES / EFFECTS PROCEDURES / EFFECTS ‘Proximal fixation of movement’ ‘Proximal initiation of movement’ • Corseting Strategy = stability through learning isolated muscle contraction at Transversus Abdominus (TrA) and Lumbar Multifidus (LM). • Hip-Pelvis Strategy, Skeletal Imagery and Vestibular Strategy • Serves as background foundation for all progressive activities • Area of attention to control is directly associated with lumbar spine segments and is framed above the waistline… ending at diaphragm; with some reference to pelvis floor ● ● Use of sEMG or pressure unit biofeedback device to inhibit selection of superficial global trunk-flexor torque producing muscles (rectus abdominus and obliques) during initial training in “The Abdominal Drawing-In Maneuver” in order to more specifically select the deep stabilizer TrA and LM groups. This procedural efficacy has been confirmed in concurrent administration of ultrasonic imaging to co-observe learned motor skill in actually isolating the Transverse Abdominus. A Pressure biofeedback device is also used in training and selection and repeated conformational testing of patient’s ability to perform an isolated contraction of the TrA. (See Figure X re. “Prone Test”) Foreground muscle contractions are causal and overtly conditioned as underlying mechanisms to decrease segmental instability and associated low back pain. • Foundation for stability begins in region of larger, denser skeletal segments and muscle groups (i.e. pelvis and hips) • Also serves as a background foundation (by virtue of anatomical location of center of gravity) for virtually any gross motor activity. • Area(s) of attention to control is a task-based neural assembly of various elements of whole self: For this study attention is given to: 1) Outlining, mobilizing and initiating activity of pelvis and hips (the pelvis force couple…wherein attention is framed to areas below the waistline i.e. indirect and adjacent to lumbar spine segments as a systemic basis for segmental control – as per Gordon Browne, PT, GCFP) 2) Counterbalancing of head – with specific attention to the anatomical region of the vestibular system encased in L vs. R temporal bones. 3) Interactions between head and pelvis through using “The Law of Proportionality of Thirds” as a guideline for generalizing a distributed quality of movement in both ATM® lessons, FI® sessions, and daily activities Background muscle activation patterns are merely incidental and a covert indirect consequence of multisensory-mediated improvements in the efficient organization of function to match the demands of a task – with awareness. Improved quality of movement function = decreased segmental instability and decreased low back pain. 411 As with most conventional exercises, proprioceptive challenges tend to occur in a limited context of a pre-set position and most actions are limited to being within controlled, cardinal saggital plane. Clinical Observations & Discovery As with real life (and that of our ancestors), proprioceptive challenges will occur in the expanded context of multiple positions, the possible variations within them, and gradations of action within at least three planes of movement. Clinical Observations & Discovery Decreased TrA activation and LM atrophy is a consistent finding in LBP (as documented through laboratory assessment and literature review). The observation of inaccurate perceptual knowledge of hip axes is also a consistent finding in LBP (as accrued through practice-based evidence via repeat clinical observation). EVIDENCE Purports to have the most evidence on efficacy AND a specified functional mechanism for LBP and instability EVIDENCE • Lacks conclusive hard evidence or controlled studies. • Does have an abundance of experiential frameworks and anecdotal evidence. CRITICISMS Selected evidence to implicate an isolated component or part is perhaps too deterministic and out of context of everyday function to be reliable. • Principles are just beginning to be articulated but no clear or proven mechanism to currently address the problem of chronic LBP. OPPORTUNITIES Variable evidence existing throughout a system is emergent, contingent on task or otherwise incessantly present…but contextually active at different times AND in different ways. Learning Goal Learning Goal Train a Specific Skill…and have it generalize to multiple tasks. Train Constellations of Skills (i.e. Awareness) and have them integrate (multi-systemically) toward meeting specific task affordances (i.e. adaptive function) selectively in real time. One skill set …continually conditioned. Primacy of precision for contraction of TrA and LM muscle groups as both preliminary and primary sources of stability and protection for all of life’s challenges and daily activities (i.e. “Core Strength”). Primacy of orchestrating alignment and proportionate action through the densest aspects of the skeleton through highlighting multi-sensory joint acuity and body schema clarity and for learning experiences designed for enhancing the optimal use of whole self. Internal feed-forward model becomes continually re-rehearsed as a pre-condition for learning a new motor skill (i.e., the internal model ‘stabilizes’ it’s anticipatory expectation and motor responses, but does not necessarily amend itself to other changing background or nuanced conditions). It thereby reinforces its own unidimensional quality. Limited scope of adaptability Internal feed-forward model becomes continually updated through greater sensory acuity and greater motor dexterity via movement variation and attention-based challenges to both existing and dynamically changing body schema in direct relationship to changing environment. Greater awareness of 3-dimensional space Greater resilience and adaptability 412 Appendix T: Principles of Ideal Movement PRINCIPLES OF IDEAL MOVEMENT FYI #3 – Principles of Ideal Movement ● Economy of effort. Don’t put any more effort into a movement than you really have to. Don’t strain to move farther, but figure out what you have to let go of to allow your bones to move more freely. ● Even distribution of movement. Don’t continue to force movement in places that already move too much, but figure out what else you can invite to participate in any given movement. ● Proportional use of synergistic muscles. Use your larger muscles more intensely to move your larger bones for larger movements. Use your small muscles less intensely to move your smaller bones in more delicate movements. ● Use your legs to control the position and movement of your pelvis. Corollary to proportional use of synergists. Use your much larger hip muscles to move and position your pelvis and use your smaller torso muscles to control the relationship between your pelvis and your chest. ● Bearing weight skeletally. Arrange your bones “vertically” over each other so your bones support the weight of the bones above... not through muscle effort, joint compression or by overstretching connective tissue. From: Outsmarting Low Back Pain: “A revolutionary new approach to solving the low back pain puzzle” - Book and DVD program by Gordon Browne, PT, GCFP and Julie Browne, PTA, GCFP, (© 2005, Reprinted with permission). Browne, G., & Browne, J. (2005). Outsmarting Low Back Pain. Bellevue, WA: Movement Matters, Inc. 413 Appendix U: Transparent-Translucent Contrast Images for Skeletal Density TRANSPARENT-TRANSLUCENT CONTRAST IMAGES FOR SKELETAL DENSITY Appendix U. (Caption for images 1of 2). Comparison of transparence vs. translucence as an image demonstration to highlight the lesser skeletal density of the ilia shell concavity below iliac crest – but more especially to clinically contrast an image and outline for ‘greater skeletal density’ at inner ilia and pectineal line as a depiction for the primary transmission of skeletal weight bear forces through the inner skeletal architecture being largely constituted by boney trabeculae and transmissive through anterior-medial aspects of ilio-sacral (I-S) joints in through anterior-lateral aspects of corresponding sacro-iliac (S-I) joints. 414 (a) Posterior S-I joint usually indexed as ‘small of back’ (b) Uncovering the visual-spatial obstruction of simulated intra-pelvic viscera (c) Anterior I-S joint becomes reframed as new ‘top of leg’ ______________________________________________________________________________ Appendix U. Demonstration of “inner ilia VR” visual-tactile imagery session for the anatomical reframing and conceptual transition of usual body schema. (a) The usual focal indexing of sacroiliac (S-I) joint’s posterior ‘sacral sulci’ region(s) being cited as a common pain associated neurotag in LBP being outlined by only one or two finger’s width, and verbally depicted in a vulnerable defeatist fashion as the colloquial ‘small of back’...to...; (b) uncovering and revealing the hidden deeper and larger anterior ilio-sacral (IS) joints (ordinarily hidden from usual perceptual awareness as it lies deeply embedded behind intra-pelvic organs - and yet it remains as the longest single joint in the entire body); and to finally, (c) a re-conceptualization of the uncovered region as the new ‘top of leg’ – further re-framing them as ‘horse-hips’ or ‘drumsticks’ - and thereby linking a corresponding non-fragile association toward greater 415 solidity and structural support– as an “inner bridge” for perceptual robustness (Note: The posterior S-I ‘sulci’ aspect scarcely bears any weight!). 416 Appendix V: Study Flow Diagram STUDY FLOW DIAGRAM (Study Flow Diagram is first appended to next page for larger scale viewing) 417 Appendix V. Study Flow Diagram in Larger Scale 418 Appendix V. Study Flow Diagram and time line for the current RCT clinical research study comparing control group and experimental group. The WHITE boxes depict steps and procedures conducted by administrative research staff. The SHADED boxes depict interventions and steps conducted through single-blinded clinical staff, but being necessarily inclusive of having a non-blinded Principal Investigator. However, all Data Collection steps were otherwise double-blinded. 419 Appendix W: Sensory-Motor Learning Model for Working Body Schema SENSORY-MOTOR LEARNING MODEL FOR WORKING BODY SCHEMA (Diagram of model is appended to next page for larger scale viewing) 420 421 Appendix X: Advisory Disclaimer and Release of Responsibility ADVISORY DISCLAIMER AND RELEASE OF RESPONSIBILITY Advisory Disclaimer The Author, his professional practice, his advisory committee, faculty, staff, and the educational institution do not assume any responsibility for any loss or injury and/or damage to persons or property arising out of or related to any use of the material contained in this dissertation. It is the responsibility of the treating practitioner, relying on independent expertise and knowledge of the patient, and their appropriate scope of practice to determine the best treatment and method of application for the patient. Nor should these contents ever be misconstrued as a substitute for individual medical consultation or treatment.