REPORT DOCUMENTATION PAGE Form Approved OMB NO. 0704-0188 The public reporting burden for this collection of information is estimated to average 1 hour per response, including the time for reviewing instructions, searching existing data sources, gathering and maintaining the data needed, and completing and reviewing the collection of information. Send comments regarding this burden estimate or any other aspect of this collection of information, including suggesstions for reducing this burden, to Washington Headquarters Services, Directorate for Information Operations and Reports, 1215 Jefferson Davis Highway, Suite 1204, Arlington VA, 22202-4302. Respondents should be aware that notwithstanding any other provision of law, no person shall be subject to any oenalty for failing to comply with a collection of information if it does not display a currently valid OMB control number. PLEASE DO NOT RETURN YOUR FORM TO THE ABOVE ADDRESS. 1. REPORT DATE (DD-MM-YYYY) 2. REPORT TYPE 3. DATES COVERED (From - To) - New Reprint 4. TITLE AND SUBTITLE 5a. CONTRACT NUMBER Neurotechnology to Accelerate Learning: During Marksmanship Training W911NF-12-1-0039 5b. GRANT NUMBER 5c. PROGRAM ELEMENT NUMBER 6. AUTHORS 5d. PROJECT NUMBER Adrienne Behneman, Chris Berka, Ronald Stevens, Bryan Vila, Veasna Tan, Trysha Galloway, Robin Johnson, Giby Raphael 5e. TASK NUMBER 5f. WORK UNIT NUMBER 7. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION NAMES AND ADDRESSES Washington State University Office of Grants and Research 423 Neill Hall Pullman, WA 8. PERFORMING ORGANIZATION REPORT NUMBER 99164 -3140 9. SPONSORING/MONITORING AGENCY NAME(S) AND ADDRESS (ES) 10. SPONSOR/MONITOR'S ACRONYM(S) ARO 11. SPONSOR/MONITOR'S REPORT NUMBER(S) U.S. Army Research Office P.O. Box 12211 Research Triangle Park, NC 27709-2211 61844-NS-DRP.2 12. DISTRIBUTION AVAILIBILITY STATEMENT Approved for public release; distribution is unlimited. 13. SUPPLEMENTARY NOTES The views, opinions and/or findings contained in this report are those of the author(s) and should not contrued as an official Department of the Army position, policy or decision, unless so designated by other documentation. 14. ABSTRACT This article explores the psychophysiological metrics during expert and novice performances in marksmanship, combat deadly force judgment and decision making (DFJDM), and interactions of teams. Electroencephalography (EEG) and electrocardiography (ECG) are used to characterize the psychophysiological profiles within all categories. Closed-loop biofeedback was administered to accelerate learning during marksmanship training in which the results show a difference in groups that received feedback compared with the control. During known distance marksmanship and DFJDM scenarios, experts show superior ability to control physiology to meet the 15. SUBJECT TERMS training, neuroscience, EEG, ECG, marksmanship 16. SECURITY CLASSIFICATION OF: a. REPORT b. ABSTRACT c. THIS PAGE UU UU UU 17. LIMITATION OF ABSTRACT UU 15. NUMBER 19a. NAME OF RESPONSIBLE PERSON OF PAGES Bryan Vila 19b. TELEPHONE NUMBER 509-358-7711 Standard Form 298 (Rev 8/98) Prescribed by ANSI Std. Z39.18 Report Title Neurotechnology to Accelerate Learning: During Marksmanship Training ABSTRACT This article explores the psychophysiological metrics during expert and novice performances in marksmanship, combat deadly force judgment and decision making (DFJDM), and interactions of teams. Electroencephalography (EEG) and electrocardiography (ECG) are used to characterize the psychophysiological profiles within all categories. Closed-loop biofeedback was administered to accelerate learning during marksmanship training in which the results show a difference in groups that received feedback compared with the control. During known distance marksmanship and DFJDM scenarios, experts show superior ability to control physiology to meet the demands of the task. Expertise in teaming scenarios is characterized by higher levels of cohesiveness than those seen in novices. Learning a novel task generally relies heavily on the conventional classroom instruction with qualitative assessment and observation. Integration of neuroscience-based evaluation and training techniques could significantly accelerate skill acquisition and provide quantitative and objective evidences of successful training. In a project supported by Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency’s (DARPA) Accelerated Learning program, EEG (brain’s electrical activity) and ECG (heart’s electrical activity) techniques were used to assess expertise in marksmanship, combat DFJDM, and team neurodynamics. REPORT DOCUMENTATION PAGE (SF298) (Continuation Sheet) Continuation for Block 13 ARO Report Number 61844.2-NS-DRP Neurotechnology to Accelerate Learning: During...Marksmanship Training Block 13: Supplementary Note © 2012 . Published in IEEE Pulse, Vol. Ed. 0 (2012), (Ed. ). DoD Components reserve a royalty-free, nonexclusive and irrevocable right to reproduce, publish, or otherwise use the work for Federal purposes, and to authroize others to do so (DODGARS §32.36). The views, opinions and/or findings contained in this report are those of the author(s) and should not be construed as an official Department of the Army position, policy or decision, unless so designated by other documentation. Approved for public release; distribution is unlimited. By Adrienne Behneman, Chris Berka, Ronald Stevens, Bryan Vila, Veasna Tan, Trysha Galloway, Robin R. Johnson, and Giby Raphael During Marksmanship Training T his article explores the psychophysiological metrics during expert and novice performances in marksmanship, combat deadly force judgment and decision making (DFJDM), and interactions of teams. Electroencephalography (EEG) and electrocardiography (ECG) are used to characterize the psychophysiological profiles within all categories. Closed-loop biofeedback was administered to accelerate learning during marksmanship training in which the results show a difference in groups that received feedback compared with the control. During known distance marksmanship and DFJDM scenarios, experts show superior ability to control physiology to meet the demands of the task. Expertise in teaming scenarios is characterized by higher levels of cohesiveness than those seen in novices. Learning a novel task generally relies heavily on the conventional classroom instruction with qualitative assessment and observation. Integration of neuroscience-based evaluation and training techniques could significantly accelerate skill acquisition and provide quantitative and objective evidences of successful training. In a project supported by Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency’s (DARPA) Accelerated Learning program, EEG (brain’s electrical activity) and ECG (heart’s electrical activity) techniques were used to assess expertise in marksmanship, combat DFJDM, and team neurodynamics. Digital Object Identifier 10.1109/MPUL.2011.2175641 Date of publication: 6 February 2012 60 IEEE PULSE ▼ JANUARY/FEBRUARY 2012 2154-2287/12/$31.00©2012 IEEE © BRAND X PICTURES Learning a novel task generally relies heavily on conventional classroom instruction with qualitative assessment and observation. Marksmanship skill acquisition was accelerated by first modeling the psychophysiological characteristics of expertise and then developing sensor-based feedback to accelerate novice-to-expert transition. A preshot peak performance profile was identified in expert marksmen, characterized by an increase in theta and alpha power (derived from EEG) and a decrease in heart rate in the seconds leading up to a shot [1]. A recent study incorporated a novel device called the adaptive peak performance trainer (APPT) into a marksmanship training protocol with novice participants. The APPT provided real-time feedback of heart rate and EEG alpha power to the trainee. A haptic motor clipped to the collar vibrated in sync with the trainee’s heart beat and stopped vibrating when alpha power was sufficiently elevated. The goal of the APPT was to indicate to the trainee when he is in the ideal state to fire the weapon. All participants completed eight trials of five shots each in a simulated indoor shooting range. The training protocol incorporated videobased coaching on the fundamentals of marksmanship by a qualified marksmanship coach. An instrumented weapon was developed using off-the-shelf sensing components and a demilitarized M16/A2 housing a pneumatic recoil system designed to approximate the weight, noise, and action of a real live fire weapon. Shots were directed against a scaled projection of a circular target simulating a 20-in diameter target at 200 yd. Marksmanship performance was measured as the average dispersion of five shots from the shot group center (inches), where lower dispersion represents a tighter shot grouping and better performance. Performance trajectories of novices trained with the APPT (n = 37) were 2.3 times greater than those of novices trained with an identical protocol without the APPT (n = 17) [1] (Figure 1). A one-way ANOVA revealed a control versus APPT difference in both final trials, F (1,51) = 6.65, P < 0.05, and percent improvement, F (1,52) = 5.34, P < 0.05. Combat DFJDM The decision of when to use deadly force is a complex skill to assess and train. Split-second life-or-death decisions require a delicate balance between restraint and aggression. The psychophysiology of DFJDM was evaluated using realistic video-based simulations to fully engage participants. Twelve experts (infantry with urban combat experience and police with $5 years patrol in active areas) and 12 novices (civilians with no military or police experience) participated in the study. All participants engaged in 24 DFJDM scenarios (approximately 70% justifying the use of deadly force). Scenarios were filmed using professional actors and represented characteristics of the most common deadly force situations encountered by law enforcement. When participants perceived a threat requiring deadly force, they responded by firing a simulated Glock 17 with a laser barrel insert. Between scenarios, participants were given a short rest (+2 min) to recover from the previous scenario and prepare for the next scenario. Participants were given a long rest (+40 min) after every three scenarios. EEG and ECG were measured throughout the day-long session (Figure 2). Experts had significantly lower low-frequency heart rate variability (LF HRV) than novices during the scenarios [t (22) = –2.79, P < 0.05]. Lower LF HRV indicates less influence by Mean Shot Group Dispersion (in) Accelerated Marksmanship Skill Learning 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 Control (n = 17) APPT (n = 37) Baseline Trials Final Trials FIGURE 1 The performance trajectory for the APPT group is 2.3 times greater than the control group. JANUARY/FEBRUARY 2012 ▼ IEEE PULSE 61 The psychophysiology of DFJDM was evaluated using realistic video-based simulations to fully engage participants. example, entropy, a meathe sympathetic nervous syssure of randomness or untem, suggesting a lower state certainty of NSs expressed of stress [2]. Using a twoover a specified time period, way ANOVA (group # task was significantly higher for demands), experts showed expert submarine piloting greater suppression of right and navigation teams comparietal EEG alpha power (8– pared to less-experienced 12 Hz) during scenarios relateams. Entropy increased tive to rest periods, F(3,44) = as teams gained experience, 3.84, P < 0.05. Task-related approaching the level seen alpha suppression is associin experts. Transition matriated with increased attention ces (a plot of the NS_E being demands, specifically, inexpressed at time t versus creased alertness and expecthat at time t + 1) showed tancy [3]. A greater change in FIGURE 2 The DFJDM scenario conducted at Washington State that the expert teams use alpha power between resting University. (Photo courtesy of Scott L. Oplinger, Washington more of the available NS_E and scenario most likely in- State University.) patterns available to them, dicates that experts are more possibly indicating a more flexible team and one that does efficient at matching their psychophysiological state (e.g., state not frequently get locked into a restricted pattern of enof alertness) to task demands (scenario versus rest). gagement. In Figure 3, the entropy metrics for two novices and one Team Neurodynamics expert team are shown. Submarine Officer Advanced Course Our goal for modeling team neurodynamics is to rapidly de(SOAC) novice team 1 did not complete session 5, while novtermine the functional status of a team to assess the quality ice team 2 had technical issues that resulted in session 1 being of performance and decision with the potential to adaptiveunavailable. However, the graphs in Figure 3(a) clearly demonly rearrange the team or task components to better optimize strate that novices increase in the entropy measure as they gain teamwork. Neurophysiologic synchronies (NS) are low-level experience and plateau in sessions 3–5. Figure 3(b) compares data streams that combine individual team members’ EEGthe expert entropy to the novice entropy in sessions 1–2 verbased measures of engagement into an aggregated profile sus 3–5. A significant difference is shown between experts’ and of team engagement. The metric of EEG engagement from novices’ sessions 1–2. which these team patterns are derived is related to informaNS expressions appear to be important constructs for studytion gathering, visual scanning, sensory load, and sustained ing team dynamics as attention [4]. Team engagement NS are normalized and pattern classified by self-organizing artificial neural networks. ▼ they change rapidly in response to short- and long-term The temporal expression of these patterns can be mapped changes in the task [5] to team events such as the frequency of team conversation. ▼ they relate to the task and some established aspects of team Team NS metrics can be collected and analyzed in near real cognition such as speech [6] time and realistic training settings such as submarine pilot▼ they can be rapidly reported for use by educators/training and navigation, high-school team problem solving, or ers [7] antisubmarine warfare tactical training. The team NS met▼ they can distinguish some aspects of novice/expert perforrics that are expressed during team performance have been mances proven to provide insight into the dynamics of the team. For ▼ they are sensitive to training effects. 62 IEEE PULSE ▼ JANUARY/FEBRUARY 2012 Team engagement NS are normalized and pattern classified by self-organizing artificial neural networks. SOAC Team T1 SOAC Team T2 4.5 NS_E Entropy NS_E Entropy 4.5 4.3 4.1 3.9 3.7 3.5 4.3 4.1 3.9 3.7 3.5 1 2 3 4 Session Number 1 2 3 4 Session Number Adrienne Behneman (abehneman@b-alert.com), Chris Berka (chris@b-alert.com), Veasna Tan (vtan@b-alert.com), Robin R. Johnson (rjohnson@b-alert.com), and Giby Raphael (graphael@b-alert.com) are with Advanced Brain Monitoring, Inc., Carlsbad, California. Ronald Stevens (Immex_ron@hotmail.com) and Trysha Galloway (Trysha@teamneurodynamics.com) are with the Learning Chameleon and UCLA IMMEX Project, Culver City, California. Bryan Vila (vila@wsu.edu) is from Washington State University, Spokane, Washington. (a) References 4.3 ** NS_E Entropy 4.2 (n = 5) (n = 5) 4.1 4.0 **P < 0.001 Kruskal–Wallis ** 3.9 (n = 9) 3.8 Expert Teams SOAC SOAC Teams Teams (Sessions (Sessions 1 and 2) 3–5) (b) FIGURE 3 Entropy metric for two SOAC novice teams and one expert team: (a) the Shannon entropy levels for two SOAC teams that performed four SPAN simulations at different times during their nine-weeks training and (b) the mean (± S.D.) entropy levels for SPAN sessions performed by expert submarine navigation teams (Expert) and for SOAC teams on their first two SPAN performances (sessions 1 and 2) and subsequent performances (sessions 3–5). Acknowledgment This work was supported by the DARPA (government contract numbers NBCHC070101 and NBCHC090054) and National Science Foundation Small Business Innovative Research award 0822020. The views expressed are those of the author and do not reflect the official policy or position of the Department of Defense or the U.S. Government. [1] C. Berka, A. Behneman, and N. Kintz, “Accelerated training using interactive neuro-educational technologies: Applications to archery, golf and rifle marksmanship,” Int. J. Sport Soc., vol. 1, no. 4, pp. 87–104, 2010. [2] M. Malik, J. T. Bigger, A. J. Camm, R. E. Kleiger, A. Malliani, A. J. Moss, and P. J. Schwartz, “Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use,” Eur. Heart J., vol. 17, no. 3, pp. 354–381, 1996. [3] W. Klimesch, “EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis,” Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev., vol. 29, no. 2–3, pp. 169–195, 1999. [4] C. Berka, D. Levendowski, M. Lumicao, A. Yau, G. Davis, V. Zivkovic, R. Olmstead, P. Tremoulet, and P. Craven, “EEG correlates of task engagement and mental workload in vigilance, learning and memory tasks,” Aviat. Space Environ. Med., vol. 78, no. 5, sec. II, suppl. 1, pp. B231–B244, 2007. [5] R. H. Stevens, T. Galloway, C. Berka, and A. Behneman, “Temporal sequences of neurophysiologic synchronies can identify changes in team cognition,” in Proc. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 54th Annu. Meeting, 2010, pp. 190–194. [6] R. H. Stevens, T. Galloway, C. Berka, A. Behneman, T. Wohlgemuth, and J. Lamb, “Linking models of team neurophysiologic synchronies for engagement and workload with measures of team communication,” in Proc. 20th Conf. Behavioral Representation in Modeling and Simulations, 2011, pp. 122–129. [7] R. H. Stevens, T. Galloway, C. Berka, and P. Wang, “Developing systems for the rapid modeling of team neurodynamics,” presented at the 14th Int. Conf. Human–Computer Interaction, Orlando, FL, 2011. JANUARY/FEBRUARY 2012 ▼ IEEE PULSE 63