• • • • • • • • Is syntax always king? Syntactic and semantic processing in Chinese Yi Cheng Kang ÉhAsaEq@az Columbia University, New York, NY Sentence processing Sentence processing is complex – it builds up from orthographical and phonological processing of individual words to syntactic and semantic processing of sentences. With Indo-European languages, syntax seems to be king in most cases – (1) syntax is processed first (ELAN), and (2) when syntactical errors occur, semantic errors are ignored (N400 is not observed) (A. Friederici, 2002) (A. Friederici et al., 2004). • • – – a Hypothesis Earlier studies of German and French showed that the N400 is not observed if the sentence also has a syntactical error. This was explained in terms of syntax primacy, with a syntactical error “blocking” further semantic processing (Friederici et al., 2004)(Friederici & Weissenborn, 2007). ERP studies demonstrate that this effect is not seen in Chinese over a range of different syntax violations (e.g. syntactical category, transitivity). As long as a semantic violation has occurred, an N400 is seen (Yu & Zhang, 2008; Zhang et al., 2010, 2013) (Zeng et al., 2016). • • – – While semantics appears to be important in Chinese sentence processing, syntax is not irrelevant or strictly contingent on semantics either. Structural priming: exposure to a certain structure (a syntactic form, in this case) promotes subsequent reuse of that same form. a a a a Subjects were significantly more likely to “copy” the syntactic structure of the priming sentence (preposition-object vs double object) in describing the picture prompt (Fig 1). However, with non-Indo-European languages, there are good reasons to expect reduced syntax primacy in sentence processing. For example, Mandarin Chinese is (1) more analytical than English, with little to no inflection, and yet (2) has freer word order and (3) word categorization than English. Strictly syntax-first processing in Chinese has therefore been argued to be implausible, and semantic information is thought to be essential even in early processing of a sentence (Ping Li, 1996). Circumstantial evidence: identification of the agent in a sentence is achieved primarily via word order in English (i.e. syntax) vs animacy in Chinese (i.e. semantics) (Bates et al., 1982; P. Li et al., 1993). a Semantics is not dominant either – evidence from structural priming Semantic processing occurs even with syntactic violations – – • • Semantic violations are immediately noticed – evidence from eye-tracking • • Sentence processing in Mandarin Chinese is less syntax-dominant than Indo-European languages, involving both syntax and semantic information even in early processing. ERP evidence against syntax primacy Visual rather than auditory cues – subjects read a sentence and their eye movements are tracked. Chinese script is logographic, requiring distinct neural processes from alphabetic languages (Gandour et al., 2002; Schirmer et al., 2005). Moreover, word boundaries are not explicitly marked in written form – readers must use contextual cues to identify whether a character is a standalone word or part of a multi-character word – e.g. (new) (fresh) (beginner) (Chen & Zhou, 1999). Ye et al. (2006) observed independent ELAN (~50 ms) and early-onset N400 (at 150 ms) for syntactic and semantic violations respectively. Integration of syntax and semantic information appears to occur by 250 ms, with a more negative N400 in the combined syntactic/semantic violation than in either case alone. No statistically significant P600 was found in all cases. Taken together, this suggests a significantly earlier integration of syntax and semantics, in line with the importance of semantic context in Chinese syntactical processing. a a While controlling for animacy and semantic meaning, the authors found that syntax alone primes the subjects to produce similar syntactical structures. Demonstrates that Chinese speakers do have ‘autonomous’ syntactic representations as well – semantics might be important, but it is not dominant over syntax in sentence processing. Syntax can exist (and be processed) independently of semantics in a Chinese sentence (Zeng et al., 2016). Conclusions • • • Hypothesis broadly validated – both ERP and eye-tracking studies provide strong evidence of (1) faster semantic processing in Chinese and (2) parallel and independent processing of syntactic and semantic information (no ‘blocking’ of the N400 with a syntactic violation). Syntax is not king in Chinese sentence processing. This makes sense – Chinese is highly analytical and syntactically flexible. The vast majority of words have no inflections, words can switch between nouns, verbs, adjectives, etc. without any morphological indication (i.e. they remain the same character), and the topic prominence of Chinese allows for much looser word order (e.g. OSV sentences). Chinese syntax is far more ambiguous than English without the context provided through semantic processing. However, we cannot overstate the case for semantic importance in Chinese processing – syntax does still exist, and it can be represented independently of semantic meaning in some contexts. References • • Criticism: syntactic violation examples were created by omission of a noun phrase – some have argued that this also constitutes a semantic violation (Yang et al., 2009). • Both semantic and combined semantic/syntactic violations led to immediate disruptions in eye movements. Subjects had increased gaze duration, showed a tendency to look back to earlier words, etc. beginning from the incorrect word itself. The disruptions continued as the subjects read the next few words as well, and overall reading speed was decreased. There was no distinction in onset speed for the disruptions for semantic violations relative to combined violations, although this may be because the temporal resolution of eye-tracking is not as high as ERP studies (Yang et al., 2009). Again, this suggests that syntax is not dominant over semantics in Chinese. Bates, E., McNew, S., MacWhinney, B., Devescovi, A., & Smith, S. (1982). Functional constraints on sentence processing: A cross-linguistic study. Cognition, 11(3), 245–299. https://doi.org/10.1016/00100277(82)90017-8 Chen, H.-C., & Zhou, X. (1999). Processing East Asian Languages: An Introduction. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(5–6), 425–428. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909699386130 Friederici, A. D., Gunter, T. C., Hahne, A., & Mauth, K. (2004). The relative timing of syntactic and semantic processes in sentence comprehension. NeuroReport, 15(1), 165–169. Friederici, Angela D. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01839-8 Friederici, Angela D., & Weissenborn, J. (2007). Mapping sentence form onto meaning: The syntax–semantic interface. Brain Research, 1146, 50–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.038 Gandour, J., Wong, D., Lowe, M., Dzemidzic, M., Satthamnuwong, N., Tong, Y., & Li, X. (2002). A Cross-Linguistic fMRI Study of Spectral and Temporal Cues Underlying Phonological Processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(7), 1076–1087. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902320474526 Li, P., Bates, E., & Macwhinney, B. (1993). Processing A Language without Inflections: A Reaction Time Study of Sentence Interpretation in Chinese. Journal of Memory and Language, 32(2), 169–192. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1993.1010 Li, Ping. (1996). The temporal structure of spoken sentence comprehension in Chinese. Perception & Psychophysics, 58(4), 571–586. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213091 Schirmer, A., Tang, S.-L., Penney, T. B., Gunter, T. C., & Chen, H.-C. (2005). Brain Responses to Segmentally and Tonally Induced Semantic Violations in Cantonese. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929052880057 Yang, J., Wang, S., Chen, H.-C., & Rayner, K. (2009). The time course of semantic and syntactic processing in Chinese sentence comprehension: Evidence from eye movements. Memory & Cognition, 37(8), 1164–1176. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.37.8.1164 Ye, Z., Luo, Y., Friederici, A. D., & Zhou, X. (2006). Semantic and syntactic processing in Chinese sentence comprehension: Evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Research, 1071(1), 186–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.085 Yu, J., & Zhang, Y. (2008). When Chinese semantics meets failed syntax. NeuroReport, 19(7), 745–749. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282fda21d Zeng, T., Mao, W., & Lu, Q. (2016). Syntactic and semantic processing of Chinese middle sentences: Evidence from event-related potentials. NeuroReport, 27(8), 568–573. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0000000000000569 Zhang, Y., Li, P., Piao, Q., Liu, Y., Huang, Y., & Shu, H. (2013). Syntax does not necessarily precede semantics in sentence processing: ERP evidence from Chinese. Brain and Language, 126(1), 8–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.04.001 Zhang, Y., Yu, J., & Boland, J. E. (2010). Semantics does not need a processing license from syntax in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(3), 765– 781. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019254