Inferential Beliefs in Consumer Evaluations: An Assessment of Alternative Processing Strategies GARYT, FORD RUTH ANN SMITH* The purpose o( this research is to investigate the processing strategies consumers use to form inferences about missing product informalion. We evaluate the relative effect of attribute information atxiut a partially described brand and about other fully described brands, the eflect of attribute intercorrelations, and the effect of prompting inferences. We find that attribute information about a partially described brand has a greater influence than that about fully described competitive brands, that highly correlated attributes more consistently influence inferences, and that prompting inferences produces substantially different results than tess intrusive measures. M elusions are supported. First, consumers' inferences appear to be based on a number of information sources. Huber and McCann (1982) and Meyer (1981. 1982) report that consumers infer the value of missing attributes from quality information about attributes of other brands in a product class (other-brand information). However, other authors (Johnson and Levin 1985b: Lutz 1975; Mazis and Adkinson 1976) offer evidence that inferences derive from the attribute ratings available about the partially described brand itself (samebrand information). Inferences based on these information sources may be described as using an otherbrand or a same-brand strategy, respectively. Second, regardless of the use of a same- or an otherbrand strategy, consumers appear to adjust their inferences depending on their perception of the level of uncertainty of the available information. Available information may have uncertain or unclear implications for inferences because it is perceived to be uncorrelated with the missing attribute (Huber and McCann 1982; Johnson and Levin 1985b) or because the value of the missing attribute is highly variable across the product class (Meyer 1981). In either case, the consistent finding is that greater uncertainty leads to greater discounting of the inferred attribute value (Johnson and Levin 1985b; Meyer 1981). While these findings offer some insights about how consumers process information to form inferences, some unanswered questions remain. First, in naturally occurring environments, both same- and other-brand attribute information is likely to be available. Therefore, inferences about missing product attributes may be subject to the simultaneous influence of both information sources. Since previous research implies that odels of consumers' evaluative processes often assume that available information completely describes each alternative. However, product information is often incomplete, and consumers' judgments may therefore be based on inferences about missing information. Inferences, or the construction of meaning that goes beyond what is explicitly given (Harris 1981). are pervasive in social cognition (Fishbein and Ajzen 1975; Fiske and Taylor 1984. Wyer and Carlston 1979). suggesting that they may occur frequently in response to incomplete product information. However, inferential processes are of recent interest to consumer researchers, and the processing strategies underlying inferences are as yet poorly understood. This study, therefore, was designed to clarify how consumers use available information to form inferences about missing product attributes. INFERENCES AND CONSUMER INFORMATION PROCESSING Although relatively few studies of consumers' inferential processes have been completed, two general con•Gar> T, Ford is Professor, Department of Marketing. Kogod College of Business Administration, the American tJniversity. Washington. D,C, 20016, Ruth Ann Smith is Assistant Proressor, Depanment of Marketing, The R.B, Pamplin College of Business. Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University. Blacksburg. VA 24061, The authors' names are listed alphabetically; each contributed equally lo this work. The assistance of Michael Mazis. Raheel Masood, Brian Ratchford. Deborah Strauss Salmond. Darlene Smith, and three anonymous reviewers is gratefully acknowledged. This research was partially funded by a grant Trom the Kogod College of Business. 363 RESEARCH • V o l , 14 •December 1987 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH 364 inferential processes are driven by whatever information is available, an important question concerns the relative impact of these two types of information on inferences when both are present. A second question concerns the influence of consumers' perceptions of attribute correlations on inferences about missing information. Either same- or otherbrand attributes perceived to have a low correlation with an unknown attribute may have less certain implications for inferences than do high-correlation attributes. Although the influence of attribute intercorrelations on inferences has been noted (Huber and McCann 1982; Johnson and Levin 1985b; Meyer 1981, 1982), studies to date have not systematically manipulated this variable, and its effect on the use of same- and other-brand inference strategies is unknown. Third, it is unclear how various experimental procedures used to evaluate inference formation may alter the very process they are designed to measure. Prior research has operationalized inferences in various ways. Some methods, such as obtaining overall product evaluations (Meyer 1981. Experiment 1), probability or frequency of choice (Meyer 1981, Experiments 2 and 3). or probability of purchase (Huber and McCann 1982) are relatively unobtrusive. However, studies where inferences are more directly measured by asking respondents to provide a rating for a missing attribute (Johnson and Levin 1985a, 1985b) may increase the salience of that information and thus create demand artifacts (Johnson and Levin 1985b). Therefore, the effects of prompting inferences should be compared with a less intrusive procedure. The present investigation was designed to address these issues. sumer context and in other settings have consistently demonstrated that inferences are inversely affected by negatively correlated information (Yamagishi and Hill 1983), while high positive correlations exert a direct effect on inferences (Huber and McCann 1982). While these findings imply that highly correlated same- and other-brand attributes should more substantially affect inferences than should attributes with low correlations, this prediction has not been explicitly tested. Thus, the second hypothesis states that: H2: Consumers' inferences will be influenced more by attributes with high correlations than by those with low correlations lo a missing attribute. The third hypothesis concerns the effect of prompting inferences about missing information on product evaluations. Sawyer (1975) suggests that research procedures that alter the salience of stimuli may contaminate dependent measures. This possibility is of particular concern in the present context, since prompting inferences may cause subjects to attend to information that may not otherwise have influenced their evaluations or that may lead to a heightened awareness ofthe researchers' purpose (Johnson and Levin 1985a). The impact of focusing respondents' attention on missing information by prompting inferences is addressed by the third hypothesis, which predicts that: H3: Consumers' inferential processes will be substantially different if inferences are directly prompted rather than indirectly measured. METHOD HYPOTHESES The first hypothesis concerns the relative influence of same- and other-brand attribute information on inferences, independent of the ecological correlations among known and missing attributes. Although prior research implies that both same- and other-brand attributes will influence an inference, it is reasonable to expect that the magnitude of their influence will differ when attribute intercorrelations are the same. Compared to same-brand attributes, other-brand attribute information has less certain implications for inferences. Since information that offers clear, unambiguous implications has been found to dominate ambiguous information in inference formation (Hinkle 1976), the influence of same-brand information is expected to substantially exceed that of other-brand information. More formally, this hypothesis is stated as; HI: Consumers' inferences will be influenced more by same- than by other-brand attribute information when attribute correlations are held constant. The second hypothesis focuses on the effects of attribute correlations on inferences. Studies in the con- Design The hypotheses were evaluated i n a 2 X 2 X 2 x 2 X 2 factorial design. The first two factors represent same-brand attribute (SBA) and other-brand attribute (OBA) information, respectively. Each source of information could assume either a high or a low quality rating. The third factor represents the correlation between the same-brand attribute and the missing attribute (CSBA), while the fourth factor is the correlation between the missing attribute and the other-brand information (COBA). Both CSBA and COBA assumed either a high or low level. These four factors were all treated as within-subjects factors whose levels were systematically varied for hypothesis testing. Specifically, two same-brand and two other-brand attributes with either an extreme high (close to +1) or extreme low (close to 0) ecological correlation with the missing attribute were assigned either high or low quality ratings. By operationalizing SBA and OBA in terms of specific attributes and varj'ing their quality level, the relative influence on inferences of same- and other-brand information with various correlations to a 36S INFERENTIAL BELIEFS missing attribute could be determined with the greatest clarity. The fifth factor, judgment task, was a between-subjects variable consisting of two levels. In one task condition, an inference about the value ofthe missing attribute was directly prompted while no prompt was employed in the second task condition. As explained in more detail below, the within-subjects factors were fully crossed to create experimental stimuli describing various brands of 10-speed bicycles. This product class was selected because pretests revealed it to be familiar to college students serving as subjects, and complex enough to involve multiple attributes with correlations of different magnitudes with a missing attribute. These stimuli were evaluated by subjects randomly assigned to one ofthe two judgment tasks. Stimulus Materials Experimental stimuli consisted of brief profiles of two comparable brands of bicycles, each described with respect to the quality of two attributes. One bicycle in each pair was incompletely described in that one attribute rating was provided and one was missing ("not reported"). For the second brand, both attribute ratings were available. Therefore, same- and other-brand information were simultaneously available for the judgment tasks. An example ofthe kind of information provided to subjects is presented in Figure A. In developing these stimuli, four combinations of attribute correlations were created by fully crossing CSBA and COBA. The specific attributes exhibiting high or low intercorrelations with the missing attribute were selected during pretesting in which 10 attributes were identified as quite important in selecting a bicycle {x 2. 5.5; s.d. £ 1.5 on a 7-point scale). The perceived intercorrelations among all pairs of these attributes were obtained from the estimates of a convenience sample {n = 19). Respondents were asked to assume they knew the quality level of one attribute in a pair and to indicate the probability that they could predict the quality level ofthe other attribute given this knowledge. Probabilities close to 1.0 were interpreted to indicate a high perceived correlation between attributes. Four attributes exhibiting relatively symmetrical conditional probabilities [p {a\b) = p (b\a)] of various magnitudes and with low standard deviations {s.d. £ 0.25) were selected for use in the brand descriptions. One of these, workmanship, was always the missing attribute ofthe partially described brand. The second attribute describing this bicycle (i.e., SBA) was either durability or weight, which reflected high and low correlations, respectively, as indicated by mean conditional probabilities of 0.74 and 0.33. The fully described brand in each pair included a rating of an attribute with either a high or a low correlation with the missing attribute, plus a rating of a filler attribute. Workmanship (which was also the miss- FIGURE A SAMPLE EXPERIMENTAL STIMULUS Brond D Brond N Workmanship Workmanship 10 Reputation of Authorized Deolert 5 ? Durability B NOTE. In Ifas sUmuKrt Dotti tfie »ame-U»ry) (dur»t]*btyl and oOiB>-t)rftna (wortimanimpl •!• lnbute« ara higtily corretaied wiih tri« missinQ ami&ute. and txith am raied si a riigri quality Wvsl (Roputaton of aumoruM) OMWrs Is afliterattiibut*.) Each tuOtact r*c«iv«a a total ol ing attribute ofthe partially described brand) was selected as the high correlation other-brand attribute under the assumption that it would be the most closely related to the missing information. Appearance, which exhibited a moderate conditional probability of 0.62, was the low correlation attribute ofthe fully described brand.' The filler attribute, reputation of authorized dealers, was the second attribute of every fully described brand and was always rated at the scale midpoint on quality. The combined effects of its moderate importance (x - 4.09) and constant neutral rating were expected to produce a consistent, but neutral effect on all judgments. The specific combinations of attributes constituting each level of CSBA and COBA are presented graphically in Figure B. The quality ratings ofthe available attribute information {SBA and OBA) were also treated as withinsubjects factors. These ratings were systematically manipulated by assigning either a high value (7, 8, 9. or 10 where 10 = best imaginable), or a low value (0, I, 2, or 3 where 0 = worst imaginable). The missing attribute received no rating and reputation of authorized dealers was always rated at the scale midpoint of 5. Four 'A medium, rather than a low, correlation was preferred for this attribute since it is intuitively implausible that an unrelated attribute of another brand would have any implications for inferring a missing product dimension. THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH 366 FIGURE B STIMULUS MATERIALS O t h t r - b r a n d Ottributt (08A) COBA • High Work month ip COBA - Low Workmanship ? Workmanship Apptaronci ? CSBA = High Durability Dtoltrt' . Rtpulotion Durabil ity 5 Somt Brand Attribuli (SBA) Work month ip Workmon«hip Daoltrt' Raputollon 5 Workmonihip Apptoronct ? r> CSBA = Low Wtight Dioltrt' Reputation Wtighl 5 Dtoltrt' Riputot ion 5 •OuraDility. weq-it ana wofhrnansfnp wefe assigootJ o>th«r r»gh (7 8 9 . 0 ' ' 0 | ot low (0 t 2 ex 31 rating*. •The rBling o' ifvs lil*t attnbute was always S combinations of aitribuie ratings were created, consisting of high same- and other-brand ratings, low sameand other-brand ratings, high same- and low otherbrand ratings, and low same- and high other-brand ratings. These combinations were then fully crossed with the four combinations of attribute correlations so that in the final set of stimulus materials, each combination of attributes in the four cells of Figure B assumed each level of attribute ratings. By crossing the within-subjects factors, descriptions of 16 brand pairs were created (2 SBA x 2 OBA X 2 CSBA X 2 COBA), For subjects to evaluate all 16 pairs would ha\e been excessive, so each subject completed only a one-fourth replication, consisting of four brand pairs. The specific stimuli presented to each subject were selected randomly, subject to the constraint that each stimulus represented a different cell in Figure B and a different combination of attribute ratings. Stimuli were prepared in booklet form, with order of presentation varied systematically. Dependent Measures The primary dependent variable of interest was subjects'evaluations of the four partially described brands in their stimulus set. These evaluations were based on two different judgment tasks, with task constituting a between-subjects factor. Approximately one-half of ihe subjects were specifically asked to estimate the value of the missing attribute of the partially described brand (the "direct" inference condition), while the remaining subjects provided an overall assessment of each of the partially described brands in the absence of any specific reference to the missing attribute (the "indirect" or unprompted inference condition). In both conditions, subjects also provided overall evaluations of each of the fully described brands, although these judgments were not an integral part of the hypothesis tests. Rather, they were obtained to conceal the purpose of the experiment and to facilitate certain manipulation checks. All judgments were made on an 11-point scale with anchors of zero (worst imaginable) to 10 (best imaginable) that appeared directly below the descriptions of each brand pair. Subjects and Procedure Data were collected from undergraduate college students in an introductor\ marketing management course at a large university. Subjects met in smail groups in a 367 INFERENTIAL BELIEFS classroom setting, performing all tasks under the supervision of one ofthe investigators. No interactions among participants were necessary, so demand effects due to the group setting are assumed to be negligible. As a cover story, subjects were told that the study involved decision making. Booklets containing stimulus materials were distributed in random order. The instructions preceding the stimuli reiterated the cover story and established comparability among the brands to be evaluated by indicating that all were among the 10 best sellers to college students. This was also intended to establish the relevance ofthe other-brand information for evaluating the partially described bicycle. Then a thorough description ofthe meaning ofthe attribute ratings was provided, and subjects completed a rating task as a comprehension check. Next, a sample stimulus was presented, which served to both familiarize subjects with the format and explain the meaning ofthe "not reported" rating ofthe partially described brand. Upon completion ofthe main experimental task, various manipulation checks and demographic information were obtained. Data were collected from a total of 407 subjects, of whom 200 made direct inferences, while the remaining 207 were in the indirect inference condition.' Analysis ofthe hypothesis guessing question revealed acceptance of the cover story and no knowledge ofthe researchers' purpose. In addition, no statistically significant differences were found in either the demographic characteristics of the two groups or their knowledge and experience with lO-speed bicycles. consistent quality cues (both high or both low ratings). Previous findings (Huber and McCann 1982: Meyer 1981) imply that the inferred value ofthe missing attribute would be a discounted mean, resulting in a less favorable evaluation of partially described brands relative to comparable fully described alternatives. This assumption was supported with 95 percent confidence in all but one case, in which the partially described brand was rated lower with 90 percent confidence. It should be noted, however, that some of this difference may be due to a generalized tendency to devalue an incompletely described alternative (Yates, Jagacinski, and Faber 1978).^ Third, the attribute intercorrelations perceived by experimental subjects were evaluated by obtaining estimates ofthe conditional probabilities for all attribute pairs. The probabilities of predicting the missing attribute, workmanship, conditional on the assumption of knowledge about durability, appearance, or weight were high (.v = 0.76), medium (.v = 0.47), and low (.v = 0.30) respectively, consistent with the levels observed in pretests and desired for the attribute correlation manipulation. RESULTS Manipulation Checks where: Before testing the hypotheses, checks on various manipulations and assumptions were conducted. First, since the attribute ratings used to operationalize SBA and OBA assumed several values within a level (except for reputation of authorized dealers), it was necessary to verify that no systematic differences were present in the mean high or low ratings assigned. A series of oneway ANOVAs was conducted, and in no case was the magnitude ofthe difference statistically significant (p > 0.40). Second, it was necessary to determine whether subjects in the indirect inference condition had incorporated inferential beliefs about the missing attribute into their evaluations ofthe partially described brands. This was accomplished by comparing the overall ratings of brand pairs in which CSB.A and COBA were either both high or both low and in which SBA and OBA provided 'The sample sizes used in this research provided high power under even small effect sizes. For example, the power for the teMs of the main effects and two-way interactions was approximately 0.82 with alpha = 0.05, even assuming a small effect size (Cohen 1977. p. 312), Hypothesis Tests The research hypotheses imply the following model of inference formation: A'j = SBA + OBA + CSBA + COBA + SBA . CSBA + OBA • COBA (I) A'J = inferred value of attribute A of brand j SB.A = quality rating of any other attribute ofthe same brand' i.e.. SBA * .A, OB.A = quality rating of any attribute of some other brand, inclusive of OBA = Aj CSBA = ecological correlation between Aj and SBA COBA = ecological correlation between Aj and OBA The model suggests that consumers' inferences are a function of same- and other-brand attribute information, the correlations between available and missing attributes, and the interaction between the information source (sameor other-brand) and correlation. In the context of this model, suppon for Hypothesis 1 would consist of a significant main effect of SBA and no significant effect of OBA since the influence of same-brand attribute ratings 'Table 1 and Figures C and D offer further evidence that inferences were incorporaied into evaluations of partially described brands. The similar configuration of results in the indirect and direct inference tasks argues thai even when inferences were not prompted, they were nevcnheless incorporated into respondents' judgments- The lower R- for the direct condition, however, suggests less consistcncv in prompted inferences. 368 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH TABLE 1 ANALYSIS OF VARIANCE RESULTS Direct inference condition Source of variation Entire model SBA OBA CSBA COBA SBA.OBA SBA.CSBA SBA.COBA OBA.CSBA OBA.COBA CSBA.COBA SBA.OBA.CSBA SBA.CSBA.COBA OBA.CSBA-COBA SBA.OBA.CSBA.COBA Error R^ Sums of squares 2244.39 1932.25 14.61 .61 1.45 2,99 281,49 .34 333 7.21 .04 .00 1.05 .29 .00 2806.57 Indirect inference condition d.f. ' a 15 1 1 .0001 .0001 .0439 .6811 .5254 ,3617 ,0001 .7578 .3354 .1567 .9195 1,0000 .5895 .7780 1,000 ^^} 1 ^ 1 1 1 1. tl 1 1 1 1 2 797 .443 is expected to e.xceed that of other-brand information in inference formation. Similarly, significant SB,A«CSB,A and OBA •COBA interactions would be consistent with Hypothesis 2, which predicted that highly correlated attributes will influence inferences more than attributes with low correlations.* Hypothesis 3. which posited that directly prompting inferences will substantially alter the processes underlying their formation, cannot be directly tested in the context of this model. However, a different configuration of results for the direct and indirect judgment tasks would constitute indirect suppon for this prediction. Analysis of variance was used to test the model, and the results are summarized in Table 1. The ratings used to operationalize SBA and OBA were treated as dichotomous (high or low) rather than continuous (7. 8, 9. 10 or 0, 1. 2, 3) in the analysis, and the data for the direct and indirect inference conditions were analyzed separately. Preliminary inspection of the results indicates that the ANOVAs for both judgment tasks are highly significant {p < O.OOOI). Moreover, the R's are relatively high (0.443 in the direct and 0.656 in the indirect inference condition), indicating that the significant results are not merely a result of the large sample size. Hypothesis 1 stated that consumers' inferences would be influenced more by same-brand attribute information than by other-brand information when attribute correlations are held constant, or that the main effect *None of the other effects was predicted to be statistical!) significant. Significant main effects of CSBA and COBA were noi expected since the treatments contained under each included both high and low attribute quality ratings that were expected to neutralize each other. Sums of squares d.f. 3452,66 3183.72 15 1 .09 1 1 10.67 .24 .02 236.44 .00 4.19 2.96 .00 .59 .05 .39 13,50 1808,82 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 a .0001 .0001 .8449 .0289 .7449 .9236 .0001 1.0000 .1704 ^491 1.0000 .6059 .8790 .6756 .0489 612 .656 of SB,A would be significant while that for OBA would not. Table 1 reveals a highly significant main effect of SBA in both the direct and indirect inference conditions {p < 0.0001). In fact. 86 percent of the sums of squares accounted for by the direct condition model and 92 percent by the indirect condition model are attributable to the quality ratings of the same-brand information, indicating its strong influence on respondents' inferences. As expected, the main effect of OB.A was not significant in the indirect inference condition, but in the direct condition, the other-brand information did exert a significant influence on inferences {p < 0.05). However, this effect accounts for less than 1 percent of the total variance explained, suggesting that the significance is attributable mainly to the large sample size raiher than to any practical importance of other-brand information in inference formation. Figure C presents a more detailed analysis of the relative effects of same- and other-brand information on inferences. The top panel of the Figure presents the hypothesized configuration of means. It was expected that when SBA was rated high, the inferred value of the missing attribute, (A'j), should exceed the value inferred when SBA was rated low. However, there should be no difference in A'j between the high and low levels of OBA. The observed means for the direct and indirect inference conditions are displayed in the lower panel. Both are highly consistent with the predicted pattern. The mean value of^^ varies directly with the level of SBA. but is virtually the same regardless of the level of OBA. Thus, despite the statistical significance of OBA in the direct condition, the results overw helmingly support the greater influence of same-brand information in the 369 INFERENTIAL BELIEFS FIGURE C FIGURE D HYPOTHESIS 1 HYPOTHESIS 2 Hypothesized configurotian of tneans .Hypothesized configuration of meons High ' quality rating Same-brond attribute (SBA) Other-brand attribute (OBA) Low ' quqiity rating Low quality rating High quollty roling High corre l o tion Low correlation 2. Observed configuration of means Direct inference condition 2. Observed configurotion of means Indirect inference conditian Direct inference condition Indirect inference condition High TT quality I 6 3 0 rating 587 • SBA . High ^ ^ quality ^,-'''''^6,56 rating 522 5,06 3.99 2,37 3,14 257 Low 2,(X) quollty roting Low quality rotmg High quality roting Low quality rating High quality roting process of inference formation, and Hypothesis 1 is therefore accepted. Hypothesis 2 predicted that consumers' inferences would be influenced more by attributes with high correlations than by those with low correlations to a missing attribute. Evidence in support of this hypothesis would consist of significant SBA*CSBA and OBA •COBA interactions. More specifically, the configuration of means for A, should conform to that presented in the top panel of Figure D. That is. if attribute intercorrelations influence inferences as predicted, a highly correlated attribute with a high rating should result in a higher value for A) than would a low correlation attribute with a similar high rating. Conversely, a highly correlated attribute with a low rating should produce a lower value for,*l] than would a low correlation attribute with a low rating. In other words, there should be less discounting of inferences based on highly correlated attributes. The ANOVA results in Table 1 reveal that for both the direct and indirect inference conditions, the SBA • CSBA interactions are highly significant {p < 0.0001), as expected. Moreover, in both cases, the interaction accounts for a substantial portion of total Low correlotion High correlation Low ,53 quOhty foting 2 Low COrrelO- tion High correlation variance explained by the models (12 percent and 7 percent in the direct and indirect inference conditions, respectively), indicating that the effects are not spurious. However, the OBA«COB,A interactions are not significant for either of the judgment tasks, probably reflecting the overwhelming reliance on same-brand information for inference formation as described previously. Regardless, the discussion ofthe cell means will be confined to those for the same-brand interactions. The observed cell means for the inferred value ofthe missing attribute for various levels of CSBA are displayed in the lower panels of Figure D for the direct and indirect inference conditions, respectively. As hypothesized, the means for the highly correlated attributes are more extreme than those for low correlation attributes in both judgment tasks. Since there were no differences in the values ofthe available attributes, these results indicate that respondents relied more heavily on cues from highly correlated attributes when forming inferences, but placed less emphasis on low correlation attributes. Thus. Hypothesis 2 is partially supponed in that the predicted effect of attribute intercorrelations was observed for the same-brand information. Howe\er, respondents appear to rely so little on other-brand in- 370 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH formation that any effect of attribute correlations was swamped by the impact ofthe same-brand information. In addi;ion to the predicted effects, statistically significant effects were observed in the indirect inference condition for CSBA and the four-way interaction. Since both effects account for less than 1 percent ofthe total \ariance explained, and since the interaction is largely uninterpretable, these effects are assumed to be a function of the sample size and to have no practical implications for interpreting the data. Hypothesis 3 predicted that consumers" use of available information in forming inferences will differ if inferences are prompted (as in the direct inference condition) rather than unprompted (as in the indirect inference condition). Although no direct test of this hypothesis is possible, the ANOVA results in Table I offer indirect support in that the overall R^ is substantially lower in the direct condition (0.443) than in the indirect condition (0.656). This would seem to suggest that when inferences are prompted, respondents use available information in a less consistent manner, increasing the noise in their estimates ofthe value ofthe missing information. In particular, the effect of prompting inferences seems to focus attention some\^hat less strongly on the same-brand information. For example, although the main effect of SBA was high!> significant in both conditions, it accounted for 86 percent ofthe variance explained in the direct condition compared to 92 percent in the indirect condition. This may be due to the nature of the judgment tasks performed in the two conditions. In the indirect inference condition, respondents were asked for an overall assessment ofthe partially described brand, for which they knew one attribute rating. In ihe direct inference condition, they were asked to estimate the value ofthe missing attribute. While SBA seems to have been the dominant source of information to perform both tasks, it may have seemed less relevant with respect to OB.A for the direct task than when forming an overall evaluation of a partially described brand. Thus, consistent with the hypothesis, prompting inferences seems to alter the salience of available same- and other-brand information. DISCUSSION The results of this investigation show that consumers consistently employ a same-brand strategy in inference formation, even when other-brand attributes are more highly correlated to missing information than are samebrand attributes. It was also found that more highly correlated attributes exert a more consistent effect on inferences than do low correlation attributes, and that prompting inferences produces greater heterogeneity in their value than do less direct measures. These results are important for several reasons. First. it is overly simplistic to conclude that correlations bemissing and available attributes explain inference formation. If only same- or only other-brand information is available, correlations may provide an adequate account of inferences. When both sources of information are present, however, the effect of attribute correlations appears to be swamped by the observed preference for same-brand information in forming inferences. Specifically, even low-correlation information substantially affects inferences if it pertains to the partially described brand. Other-brand information, regardless of its correlation, does not appear to affect inferences when same-brand information is present. Second, the results have several methodological implications. It is clear that research on consumers' inferential processes must explicitly control for ecological correlations between available and missing product attributes. Moreover, the operationalization of the dependent variable in such studies matters. In the present study, prompting an inference about the specific value of a missing attribute produced results quite different in magnitude, if not in overall pattern, from that produced by not prompting. This finding underscores the need to develop good, unobtrusive measures of inferential processes. The approach used in this study was similar to that found in other studies (Huber and McCann 1982: Meyer 1981) and involved obtaining overall evaluations of partially described brands. Although a manipulation check indicated that these judgments did reflect inferences about missing product attributes, greater consistency in judgments was observed among respondents in this condition compared to the direct inference condition. This seems counterintuitive since the inferences would be expected to introduce some noise into the data. leading to less consistency. .Mthough the precise cause of this is difficult to ascertain with certainty, it ma> indicate that prompting inferences substantially alters the normal same-brand strategy. Regardless, further evaluation of the procedure seems appropriate. From a theoretical perspective, the observed preference for a same-brand strategy implies that for decisions within a product category, a particular instance ofthe categor\' rather than the entire category may be more influential in inference formation. Thus, a specific brand would have difficulty "free riding" on market information, such as product class averages. Moreover, this finding implies that policy decisions on information disclosure should consider the perceived correlations between information already available in the environment and information for which disclosure is being considered. If adequate signals are provided by other attributes for which information is available, the need for disclosure is diminished. However, since consumers seem uncomfortable using other-brand signals, arguments based on the availability of product class information are less compelling, especially considering that information is discounted substantially. Finally, several issues for further research emerge from this study. First, subjects" almost complete reliance 371 INFERENTIAL BELIEFS on same-brand information raises questions about whether the other-brand cues were strong enough. Perhaps a stronger treatment, such as the product class average, would have produced greater reliance on the other-brand cues. Second, since only intrinsic cues were provided in the brand descriptions, an obvious extension ofthis study would be to consider the impact of extrinsic cues on inferential strategies (Szybillo and Jacoby 1974). Finally, it would be useful to determine if there are certain conditions under which the samebrand inference strategy becomes less desirable than an other-brand strategy. For instance. little or no variance about the product class mean for a certain attribute might enhance the implications of the mean for inferring the value of the attribute for a specific brand compared to information about highly correlated attributes of the partially described brand itself. The pervasiveness of consumers' inferences in product evaluations provides ample justification for efforts in each of these areas. [Received July 1985. Revised May 1987.] REFERENCES Cohen. Jacob (1977), Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, Rev. ed.. New York: Academic Press. Fishbein. Martin and leek Ajzen (1975). Belief Attitude. Intention, and Behavior, Reading. MA: Addison-W'esley. Fiske, Susan T. and Shelicy E. Taylor (1984). Social Cofmiuon, Reading. MA: Addison-Wesley. Harris, Richard J. (1981), "Inferences in Information Pro- cessing." in ThePsycholosyofLearninf;and.\foiivati(in. Vol. 15, ed. Gordon A. Bower, New 'I'ork: Academic Press. 81-128. Hinkle. Ronald Leiand (1976), "The Role of Stimulus Clarit> in Impression Formation." unpublished M.A. thesis. University of Illinois. Urbana-Champaign, IL 61820. Huber, Joel and John McCann (1982), "The Impact of Inferential Beliefs on Product Evaluations," Journal of Marketing Research. 10 (August), 324-333. Johnson, Richard D. and In^in P. Levin (1985a), "Explicit Inferences in Price-Quality Judgments." paper presented at the annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago. and Irwin P. Levin (1985b), "More Than Meets the Eye: The Effect of Missing Information on Purchase Evaluations," Journal of Consumer Research. 12 (2). 169-177. Lutz. Richard (1975), "First-order and Second-order Cognitive Effects in Attitude Change," Communication Research. 2(3), 289-299. Mazis. Michael B. and Janice E. Adkinson (1976). "An Experimental Evaluation of a Proposed Corrective Advertising Remedy," Journal of Marketing Research. 13 (May). 178-183. Meyer. Robert J. (1981), "A Mode! of Multiattribute Judgments Under Attribute Uncertainty and Informational Constraint," Journal of Marketing Research. 18 (November). 428-441. (1982),) "A Descriptive Model of Consumer Inforp mation Search Behavior," Marketing Science. 1 93-121. Sawyer. Alan G. (1975). "Demand Artifacts in Lahoraior\ Experiments in Consumer Research," Journal of Consumer Research. 1 (4). 20-30. Szybillo. George J. and Jacob Jacoby (1974). ••Intrinsif versus Extrinsic Cues as Determinants of Perceived Product Q u a l i t y . " Journal of .Applied Psychology. 59(1), 74-78. Wyer. Robert S., Jr. and Donal E. Carlston (1979), Social Inference and Attribution. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 1'amagishi, Toshio and Charles T. Hill (1983). "Initial impression Versus Missing Information as Explanations of the Sei-Sizc Eff"ecl." Journal of Personality and Soctal Psychology 44 (5). 942-951. Vates, }. Frank, Carolyn M. Jagacinski. and Mark D. Fabcr (1978). "Evaluation of Partiall> Described MuliiattHbute Options." Organizational Behavior and Human Performance. 21 (April). 240-251.