Journal of Personality Disorders, 26, 2012, 513-523 © 2012 The Guilford Press COMPARISON OF HEXACO-PI-R AND NEO PI-R GAUGHAN ET AL. Examining the utility of general models of personality in the study of psychopathy: A comparison of the HEXACO-PI-R and NEO PI-R Eric T. Gaughan, PhD, Joshua D. Miller, PhD, and Donald R. Lynam, PhD The Five-Factor model is one of the most popular models of general personality but recently a competing model, the HEXACO, has been put forth as an alternative. In the current study, we compare the two models by examining the interrelations between their primary measures, the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and the Revised HEXACO Personality Inventory (HEXACO-PI-R), and their relations with psychopathy in a sample of undergraduates (N = 290). Results revealed good convergence between conceptually related personality traits. Both inventories accounted for substantial proportions of variance in psychopathy scores although the HEXACO-PI-R accounted for a larger proportion. The findings are discussed in relation to the HEXACO domain of Emotionality, which functions differently than NEO PI-R Neuroticism. The results suggest that both measures assess psychopathy-related traits, but the HEXACO-PI-R may offer a slight advantage. Psychopathy is characterized by traits such as egocentricity, callousness, manipulativeness, and impulsivity. Over the last 15 years, a number of researchers have explored the idea that psychopathy can be understood as a configuration of extreme levels of general traits (Benning, Patrick, Blonigen, Hicks, & Iacono, 2005; Miller, Lynam, Widiger, & Leukefeld, 2001). Research with a number of personality models, including Tellegen’s (1985) three-factor model and the Five-Factor Model (FFM; McCrae & Costa, 1990), has demonstrated that these models of personality are generally successful in predicting the different facets of psychopathy (e.g., Benning et al.; Gaughan, Miller, Pryor, & Lynam, 2009). The FFM, as operationalized by the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992), has been used in much of this research. Recently, a competing model of personality, the HEXACO model (Lee & This article was accepted under the editorship of Paul S. Links. From University of Georgia (E.T.G., J.D.M.); and Purdue University (D.R.L.) Address correspondence to Joshua D. Miller, Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602-3013; E-mail: jdmiller@uga.edu 513 514 GAUGHAN ET AL. Ashton, 2004), has been offered as an alternative. Although the HEXACO model, as operationalized by the Revised HEXACO Personality Inventory (HEXACO-PI-R; Lee & Ashton, 2006), includes five broad personality domains similar to those found in the FFM, it also contains a sixth factor, Honesty-Humility, which may be particularly relevant to psychopathy. Unfortunately, little research has simultaneously compared these two specific models in relation to psychopathy. Five-Factor Model and Psychopathy The FFM, as operationalized by the NEO PI-R, is composed of five broad domains, i.e., Neuroticism (N), Extraversion (E), Openness to experience (O), Agreeableness (A), and Conscientiousness (C) and each of these dimensions is further composed of six lower order facets, which are important to the precise characterization of personality pathology (Axelrod, Widiger, Trull, & Corbitt, 1997). A wealth of research has been devoted to the study of the relations between the FFM and psychopathy (e.g., Lynam, 2002) and the findings have been consistent across sources and populations. A metaanalytic review (Lynam & Derefinko, 2006) indicates that psychopathy is most strongly associated, inversely, with A (weighted mean r = -.52) and C (weighted mean r = -.38). In general, the scales associated with the interpersonal and affective features of psychopathy or traditional Factor 1 aspects of psychopathy (e.g., manipulativeness, lack of remorse) are primarily negatively related to A, whereas the scales related to antisocial behavior or Factor 2 aspects of psychopathy are negatively related to both A and C. HEXACO Model and Psychopathy The HEXACO model is the result of lexical studies of diverse languages that support six major dimensions of personality (Ashton & Lee, 2001; Ashton et al., 2004). Five of these dimensions represent variants of the broad FFM factors with the sixth factor believed to be absent from the Big Five (i.e., Honesty-Humility). As operationalized by the original HEXACO-PI (Lee & Ashton, 2004) and its revision the HEXACO-PI-R (Lee & Ashton, 2006), each of the six higher order domains is composed of four lower order facets. At this time there are limited extant data comparing the measures of the HEXACO-PI and the FFM. For example, research employing an abbreviated measure of the FFM (i.e., the NEO Five-Factor Inventory; NEO FFI; Costa & McCrae, 1992) demonstrated that HEXACO dimensions manifested expected convergent correlations with their counterparts in the FFM (e.g., Ashton, Lee, Visser, & Pozzebon, 2008). HEXACO Honesty-Humility, the sixth personality domain, was primarily associated with FFM A and to a lesser extent with C. Unfortunately, there is little published research that that has compared the bivariate relations between the full-length measures (i.e., NEO PI-R and HEXACO-PI-R of these models, cf., De Vries, De Vries, De Hoogh, & Feij, 2009). Moreover, the research comparing the statistical predictive abilities of measures of these two models with regards to impor- COMPARISON OF HEXACO-PI-R AND NEO PI-R 515 tant individual difference constructs has only recently started to accumulate. For example, Ashton and Lee (2008) found that the HEXACO-PI domains explained more variance in measures of materialism and delinquency than the NEO PI-R domains; De Vries and colleagues (2009) found a similar pattern of results for measures of egoism. Initial research examining psychopathy from the perspective of the HEXACO model has focused primarily on the Honesty-Humility dimension. Several studies (Ashton, Lee, & Son, 2000; Lee & Ashton, 2004; Lee & Ashton, 2005) have found significant correlations between the HEXACO domains and psychopathy traits, particularly the domain and facets of Honesty-Humility. For example, Lee and Ashton (2005) examined the relations between psychopathy and both the HEXACO model and the Big Five. In this study, the primary psychopathy scale showed the strongest correlation with Honesty-Humility and its four facets, although it was also significantly negatively correlated with Big Five A. It should be noted that the domains of the Big Five were measured with the Big Five Inventory (BFI; John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991) and not the NEO PI-R, which is important because two NEO PI-R Agreeableness facets, straightforwardness and modesty, have been found to be strongly related to Honesty-Humility (Ashton & Lee, 2005). Although the correlation between psychopathy and Big Five A was significant, this association likely would have been stronger with the inclusion of content found in these facets. Current Study The current study examines the utility of the HEXACO-PI-R and NEO PI-R personality inventories in assessing the traits related to psychopathy by testing their statistical predictive abilities. We first examine the convergent validity of the NEO PI-R and HEXACO-PI-R domains. Following this, we examine the correlations between the two inventories’ domains and facets and psychopathy scores. To determine whether either measure is better able to account for the various factors of psychopathy, the amount of variability explained by each inventory is then compared. Consistent with past research, we expect the HEXACO-PI-R and NEO PI-R to demonstrate good convergent validity and expect that HEXACO Honesty-Humility will be strongly correlated with NEO PI-R Agreeableness. We also expect that domains related to A (A; Honesty-Humility) and C will prove to be the strongest correlates of psychopathy. Method Participants and Procedure Participants were 290 undergraduate (51% female) students at a large Southeastern university. Of those participants for whom data on race is available (N = 237), 82% were White, 9% were Black, and 6% were Asian. The average age was 19.37 years (SD = 2.94). 516 GAUGHAN ET AL. Participants gave written informed consent, completed the battery of questionnaires (given in counterbalanced order), and received research credit for participating. All Institutional Review Board requirements were followed throughout the study. Measures NEO PI-R. The NEO PI-R (Costa & McCrae, 1992) is a 240-item selfreport measure of the FFM. Each of these five domains is underlain by six facets. In the current study, coefficient alphas for the facets ranged from .57 to .83 (median = .75) and from .89 to .91 for the domains. HEXACO-PI-R. The HEXACO-PI-R (Lee & Ashton, 2006) is a 200-item self-report measure of the HEXACO model of personality, which includes six broad domains of eXtraversion, Conscientiousness, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, Emotionality, and Honesty-Humility. Each of these six domains is underlain by four facets. In the current study, alphas for the facets ranged from .71 to .89 (median = .81) and from .89 to .92 for the domains. Self-Report Psychopathy Scale: Version III (SRP-III). The SRP-III (Williams, Paulhus, & Hare, 2007) is a 64-item measure of psychopathy that provides a global score (SRP-T), as well as scores for four subscales: Interpersonal Manipulation (SRP-IPM), Callous Affect (SRP-CA), Erratic Life Style (SRP-ELS), and Anti-Social Behavior (SRP-ASB). Alphas for SRP T, SRP-IPM, SRP-CA, SRP-ELS, and SRP-ASB were .94, .87, .85, .86, and .80, respectively. Results Relations between the Personality Inventories In order to control for Type 1 error, only results significant at p < .01 are interpreted. In general, the domains of each model converged with the nominally corresponding domains from the other model (see Table 1) with correlations ranging from .52 (N–Emotionality) to .86 (E–eXtraversion).1 NEO PI-R A manifested equally strong correlations with both HEXACO PI-R Agreeableness (r = .68) and Honesty-Humility (r = .67).2 Simultaneous regression analyses were conducted using the domains from each model to predict individual domains of the other model. Overall, the NEO PI-R domains accounted for between 50% and 80% of the variance in the HEXACO-PI-R domains, with an average of 62%; the HEXACO-PI-R domains accounted for between 63% and 76% of the variance in the NEO PI-R domains, with an average of .70. 1. All of the bivariate correlations were examined separately for men and women. As fewer than 3% of the correlations were significantly different across gender, all subsequent analyses were conducted with a combined data set that included both men and women 2. A complete table of correlations between the facets of the HEXACO-PI-R and NEO PI-R is available upon request from the corresponding author. COMPARISON OF HEXACO-PI-R AND NEO PI-R 517 TABLE 1. Correlations Between the Domains of the HEXACO-PI-R and NEO PI-R Honesty Humility N E O A C R2 -.23* -.11 .15 .67* .15 .50* Emotionality .52* .07 .18* .27* .11 .51* eXtraversion -.50* .86* .17* .03 .21* .80* Agreeableness -.38* .05 .12 .68* .11 .52* Conscientiousness -.13 .11 -.08 .22* .87* .76* Openness -.05 .09 .76* .16* -.05 .60* Adjusted R2 .64* .76* .63* .71* .76* (.70) (.62) Note. N = Neuroticism; E = Extraversion; O = Openness; A = Agreeableness; C = Conscientiousness; Parentheses = mean R2. *p ≤ .01. Relations between the Personality Inventories and Psychopathy Next, we examined the correlations between the personality domains and facets and the psychopathy scores. Both personality models manifested meaningful relations with the psychopathy factors. Overall, NEO PI-R A (mean r = -.55) and C (mean r = -.27) manifested the largest effect sizes of the FFM domains across the psychopathy scores (see Table 2).3 From the HEXACO-PI-R perspective (see Table 3), four of the six domains bore substantial relations with psychopathy; Honesty-Humility (mean r = -.48), Emotionality (mean r = -.42), Conscientiousness (mean r = -.30), and Agreeableness (mean r = -.28). Simultaneous regression analyses were then conducted in which the NEO PI-R or HEXACO-PI-R domains were used to predict the psychopathy scores (see Table 4). On average, the NEO PI-R domains accounted for 40% of the variance, whereas the HEXACO PI-R domains accounted for 49%.4, 5 Discussion General models of personality are useful for understanding psychopathy (Benning et al., 2005; Miller et al., 2001), however, the majority of this re3. Individual correlations between each personality trait and the five psychopathy scores were transformed using the Fisher-Z transformation before being averaged and then transformed back before reporting the mean effect size (e.g., the mean correlation between HEXACO Honesty-Humility and the five SRP-III psychopathy scores). 4. The NEO PI-R facets accounted for between 13% and 64% of the variance in the psychopathy scores with an average R-squared of .51. The HEXACO-PI-R facets accounted for between 22% and 71% of the variance in the five psychopathy scores with an average R-squared of .56. The analyses with the HEXACO-PI-R included the Altruism scale. 5. We did not conduct extensive analyses addressing incremental validity of the HEXACO-PI-R and NEO PI-R due to problems with multicollinearity. We did test whether both of the HEXACO domains of Honesty-Humility and Emotionality provided incremental validity over the FFM domains. Both HEXACO domains accounted for increased variance in all five psychopathy scores above and beyond that accounted for by the FFM. The corresponding FFM domains (i.e., N; A) also manifested incremental validity over the HEXACO domains for four of five scores but accounted for a smaller mean increment. 518 GAUGHAN ET AL. TABLE 2. Correlations Between the NEO PI-R and SRP-III Neuroticism Anxiety Angry Hostility Depression Self-Consciousness Impulsiveness Vulnerability Extraversion Warmth Gregariousness Assertiveness Activity Excitement Seeking Positive Emotions Openness Fantasy Aesthetics Feelings Actions Ideas Values Agreeableness Trust Straightforwardness Altruism Compliance Modesty Tendermindedness Conscientiousness Competence Order Dutifulness Achievement Striving Self-Discipline Deliberation SRP T SRP IPM SRP CA SRP ELS SRP ASB ES .06 -.16* .33* .08 -.12 .22* -.11 -.04 -.34* -.06 .19* .11 .26* -.37* -.08 .00 -.15* -.16* -.05 .13 -.07 -.66* -.38* -.60* -.56* -.52* -.39* -.43* -.34* -.22* -.17* -.38* -.12 -.24* -.41* .11 -.07 .34* .06 -.06 .18* -.03 -.02 -.27* -.02 .18* .12 .20* -.28* -.07 .06 -.15 -.06 -.13 .10 -.11 -.72* -.47* -.75* -.53* -.49* -.49* -.41* -.25* -.14 -.09 -.37* -.08 -.19* -.30* -.05 -.25* .34* .01 -.13 .00 -.23* -.17* -.47* -.15 .17* .07 .16* -.51* -.29* -.17* -.29* -.39* -.16* .07 -.25* -.64* -.41* -.41* -.57* -.49* -.35* -.56* -.15 -.09 -.07 -.17* -.05 -.11 -.18* .08 -.14 .20* .11 -.15* .35* -.06 .09 -.16* .04 .22* .16* .34* -.20* .11 .09 -.07 .00 .13 .20* .14 -.48* -.19* -.43* -.41* -.47* -.29* -.27* -.47* -.27* -.29* -.42* -.20* -.33* -.57* .07 -.07 .16* .08 -.02 .14 -.01 -.07 -.18* -.08 .03 .00 .12 -.20* -.01 .00 .03 -.10 .00 .03 -.01 -.27* -.13 -.32* -.26* -.21* -.10 -.14 -.20* -.22* -.06 -.23* -.04 -.13 -.24* .05 -.13 .26 .07 -.09 .17 -.08 -.04 -.28 -.05 .15 .09 .21 -.30 -.07 -.01 -.12 -.14 -.04 .10 -.06 -.55 -.31 -.50 -.45 -.42 -.31 -.36 -.27 -.18 -.13 -.30 -.09 -.19 -.33 Note. T = Total; IPM = Interpersonal Manipulation; CA = Callous Affect; ELS = Erratic Life Style; ASB = Antisocial Behavior. ES = Effect size (average correlations across the five SRP-III scores). *p ≤ .01. search has focused on the FFM. More recently, an alternative personality model, the HEXACO, has emerged and gained empirical support (e.g., Ashton & Lee, 2005). Although both models appear to be valuable platforms for examining personality configuration, little research has compared the respective models using their full-length inventories to determine their convergence and divergence and examine their relations with psychopathy. The present research sought to fill this void. Relations between Personality Inventories One of the primary aims of the current study was to examine how HonestyHumility would be related to the dimensions of the NEO PI-R. Based on the strong convergence between the domains (r = .67) of Honesty-Humility and NEO PI-R Agreeableness, it is clear that this sixth factor overlaps substantially with NEO PI-R Agreeableness and assesses similar content. At the facet level, Honesty-Humility demonstrated its strongest convergence with the NEO PI-R A facets of straightforwardness (r = .68) and modesty (r = COMPARISON OF HEXACO-PI-R AND NEO PI-R 519 TABLE 3. Correlations Between the HEXACO-PI-R and the SRP-III Honesty-Humility Sincerity Fairness Greed Avoidance Modesty Emotionality Fearfulness Anxiety Dependence Sentimentality eXtraversion Social Self-Esteem Social Boldness Sociability Liveliness Agreeableness Forgiveness Gentleness Flexibility Patience Conscientiousness Organization Diligence Perfectionism Prudence Openness Aesthetic Appreciation Inquisitiveness Creativity Unconventionality Altruism SRP T SRP IPM SRP CA SRP ELS SRP ASB ES -.58* -.39* -.66* -.28* -.48* -.51* -.49* -.20* -.33* -.50* .00 -.09 .22* .00 -.20* -.36* -.13 -.37* -.34* -.29* -.37* -.18* -.19* -.30* -.45* .02 -.15 .09 .08 .09 -.63* -.66* -.53* -.65* -.37* -.51* -.33* -.28* -.13 -.22* -.37* .01 -.04 .19* .02 -.17* -.39* -.22* -.41* -.36* -.25* -.28* -.12 -.16* -.23* -.35* -.02 -.13 .01 .07 .05 -.57* -.47* -.26* -.48* -.26* -.44* -.64* -.47* -.26* -.48* -.68* -.09 -.12 .14 -.12 -.24* -.34* -.12 -.34* -.34* -.29* -.22* -.13 -.08 -.20* -.25* -.10 -.26* .06 -.02 -.05 -.74* -.42* -.25* -.55* -.16* -.36* -.42* -.53* -.16* -.23* -.32* .10 -.02 .27* .08 -.08 -.27* -.06 -.30* -.28* -.22* -.48* -.27* -.26* -.35* -.58* .11 -.07 .14 .14 .19* -.40* -.31* -.20* -.44* -.11 -.21* -.23* -.27* -.07 -.12 -.23* -.05 -.13 .11 -.01 -.16* -.12 -.01 -.10 -.10 -.16* -.18* -.05 -.10 -.18* -.21* .07 .00 .09 .05 .09 -.30* -.48 -.32 -.54 -.23 -.39 -.42 -.39 -.16 -.27 -.42 -.01 -.08 .18 -.01 -.16 -.28 -.10 -.29 -.27 -.23 -.30 -.14 -.15 -.24 -.36 .02 -.12 .08 .06 .07 -.52 Note. T = Total; IPM = Interpersonal Manipulation; CA = Callous Affect; ELS = Erratic Life Style; ASB = Antisocial Behavior; ES = Effect Size (average correlations across the five SRP-III scores). *p ≤.01. .65). The current results suggest that the Honesty-Humility is reasonably well-captured by this measure of the FFM. In general, the remaining domains of each personality model demonstrated good convergence with the corresponding domains of the other model with convergent correlations ranging from .52 (HEXACO Emotionality and FFM N) to .87 (HEXACO and FFM C). As suggested by Lee and Ashton (2004), FFM N and HEXACO Emotionality are not identical. One substantial difference between the two is that FFM N includes content related to the experience of both internally (e.g., anxiety) and externally directed negative affect (e.g., anger), whereas the HEXACO Emotionality domain is limited to the experience of internally directed affect (e.g., fearfulness, anxiety). As noted below, this organizational difference appears to play a role in explaining why Emotionality is a stronger correlate of psychopathy than FFM N. This HEXACO domain also appears to include content that is of particular relevance to the study of psychopathy (i.e., sentimentality; fearfulness; dependence). It is also worth noting that, on average, the HEXACO accounted for greater variance in the NEO PI-R domains (mean adjusted R2: .70) than the NEO PI-R in the HEXACO-PI-R (mean adjusted R2: .62), which may also contribute to the predictive advantage manifested by the HEXACO in relation to psychopathy. 520 GAUGHAN ET AL. TABLE 4. Predictive Validity of the NEO PI-R and HEXACO-PI-R Domains SRP T SRP IPM SRP CA SRP ELS SRP ASB -.15* -.08 -.26* -.08 -.04 -.04 Mean R2 NEO PI-R domains Neuroticism Extraversion .01 .03 -.15* .16* Openness .04 .06 -.12* .13* .04 Agreeableness -.66* -.73* -.64* -.45* -.25* Conscientiousness -.24* -.13* -.06 -.42* -.14 R .50* .55* .49* .43* .10* .41 Adjusted R2 .49 .54 .49 .42 .08 .40 Honesty-Humility -.48* -.58* -.34* -.31* -.31* Emotionality -.44* -.27 -.63* -.32* -.20* eXtraversion -.06 -.05 -.16* .08 -.10 Agreeableness -.17* -.16 -.21* -.14* -.01 Conscientiousness -.19* -.12* -.01 -.36* -.08 2 HEXACO-PI-R domains Openness .12* .10 .00 .15* .12 R2 .64* .57* .65* .50* .17* .51 Adjusted R2 .63 .56 .64 .49 .15 .49 Note. T = Total; IPM = Interpersonal Manipulation; CA = Callous Affect; ELS = Erratic Life Style; ASB = Antisocial Behavior. *p ≤ .01. Reported values are standardized regression coefficients except where otherwise noted (e.g., Rsquared). The good convergence between the dimensions of the two inventories was also reflected in findings from the regression analyses. Despite differences in organization and the inclusion of an additional sixth factor in the HEXACO-PI-R, the current findings indicate substantial (but not perfect) convergence among the measures. Personality Inventories and Psychopathy The correlations between the NEO PI-R with the SRP-III (e.g., Williams et al., 2007) were extremely consistent with the pattern found in a recent meta-analytic review of the relations between the FFM and psychopathy (Decuyper, De Pauw, De Fruyt, De Bolle, & De Clercq, 2009). Specifically, the scales related to the interpersonal and affective features of psychopathy were primarily associated with low A, whereas the scales related to the behavioral and deviance aspects of psychopathy were associated with both low A and C. The psychopathy scores demonstrated more varied relations with the HEXACO-PI-R dimensions. Similar to previous work with the HEXACOPI-R (e.g., De Vries, Lee, & Ashton, 2008; Lee & Ashton, 2004), psychopathy was most strongly associated with low Honesty-Humility, although the current research found significant negative relations with the domains of Emotionality, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. The strength of the correlations between HEXACO Emotionality and the psychopathy factors was somewhat surprising given that Emotionality has shown only weak correlations with psychopathy in some (Lee & Ashton, 2004, 2005) but not all (De COMPARISON OF HEXACO-PI-R AND NEO PI-R 521 Vries et al., 2008) previous research. This discrepancy is most likely due to differences in content assessed by various self-report psychopathy measures that differ with regard to the emphasis on the role of low neuroticism. Although both the NEO PI-R and HEXACO-PI-R domains accounted for substantial variance in the psychopathy scores, the HEXACO-PI-R demonstrated stronger predictive validity (mean adjusted R2: 49% vs. 40%); these differences were smaller at the facet level (mean adjusted R2: 56% vs. 51%). Results of the correlation and regression analyses with the HEXACOPI-R suggested that the domain of Emotionality and Honesty-Humility are the domains most responsible for the additional variance explained by the HEXACO (rather than just Honesty-Humility). The findings for Emotionality were more unexpected but suggest that the Emotionality facets of sentimentality and fearfulness (both inversely related to psychopathy) may be particularly important to the assessment of psychopathy, at least as assessed by the SRP-III. Implications The present research suggests that there is substantial overlap in content between the FFM, as assessed by the NEO PI-R, and the HEXACO model, as assessed by the HEXACO-PI-R. Both inventories capture traits associated with an antagonistic disposition (e.g., manipulativeness, deceitfulness/ insincerity, callousness, immodesty, lack of empathy) and poor behavioral inhibition (e.g., Conscientiousness) which have been considered the core personality features of psychopathy (Lynam & Derefinko, 2006). With regards to relations between the inventories, the additional sixth dimension of the HEXACO-PI-R, Honesty-Humility, overlaps substantially with FFM Agreeableness. Moreover, the corresponding dimensions of the two inventories assess similar content, though they organize certain traits somewhat differently, e.g., externalizing forms of negative affect included in N (NEO PI-R) or A (HEXACO-PI-R). We believe that the organizational advantage of the HEXACO PI-R lies not in the addition of the sixth factor, but rather in the composition of Emotionality/Neuroticism which differs across the HEXACO PI-R and NEO PI-R models. In previous work using the NEO PI-R (see Lynam & Widiger, 2007), psychopathy has manifested complex relations to the facets of Neuroticism, characterized by low scores on some facets (e.g., anxiety, depression) and high scores on the other facets (e.g., angry hostility). At the domain level, such divergent relations among the N facets often results in negligible relations between psychopathy and N, a finding at odds with traditional conceptions of psychopathy. The facets of Emotionality within the HEXACO PI-R, which do not include anger or poor impulse control, are uniformly negative in their relations to psychopathy. Although the HEXACO-PI-R accounted for more variance than the NEO PI-R in psychopathy scores, it should be noted that both measures were successful at predicting psychopathic traits. As has been found with similar comparisons (e.g., NEO PI-R vs. MPQ; Gaughan et al., 2009), the differences in predictive ability were greater at the domain level than at the 522 GAUGHAN ET AL. facet level. Although some have suggested that constructs like psychopathy are “poorly accommodated by the Big Five” (Lee & Ashton, 2005, p. 1580), the current study and previous research suggest that this conclusion is limited to measures of the Big Five (i.e., Big Five Inventory). Because the FFM, as operationalized by the NEO PI-R, includes traits related to straightforwardness and modesty, which are not included in measures of the Big Five (Ashton & Lee, 2005), it appears to be better suited at capturing psychopathy and other maladaptive traits. The wealth of research demonstrating its utility for understanding psychopathy attests to its ability to accommodate a range of traits associated with psychopathy. Limitations and Conclusions There are several limitations of the present research which may limit the generalizability of the findings. First, the sample was predominantly White and included only college students. Future research should examine these inventories in alternative samples (e.g., community, forensic) with greater diversity with regard to race, ethnicity, age, and education. Second, the use of a college sample might have resulted in some degree of restriction of range, which may have attenuated the current effect sizes. Future studies would be strengthened by utilizing other sources of information, including interviews and informant reports. Third, due to the length of the protocol we included only a single measure of psychopathy. In sum, the HEXACO-PI-R and NEO PI-R are significantly overlapping inventories and are both successful at accounting for the variance in psychopathy at both the domain and facet level. The use of measures such as these in psychopathy research is helpful in understanding the basic personality traits underlying the various dimensions of psychopathy. References Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2001). A theoretical basis for the major dimensions of personality. European Journal of Personality, 15, 327–353. Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2005). Honesty-humility, the big five, and the five-factor model. Journal of Personality, 73, 1321–1353. Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2008). The prediction of honesty-humility-related criteria by the HEXACO and five-factor models of personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1216–1228. Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Perugini, M., Szarota, P., de Vries, R. E., Di Blas, L., et al. (2004). A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: Solutions from psycholexical studies in seven languages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 356–366. Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., & Son, C. (2000). Honesty as the sixth factor of personality: Correlations with Machiavellianism, primary psy- chopathy, and social adroitness. European Journal of Personality, 14, 359–369. Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Visser, B. A., & Pozzebon, J. A. (2008). Phobic tendency within the five-factor and HEXACO models of personality structure. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 734–746. Axelrod, S. R., Widiger, T. A., Trull, T. J., & Corbitt, E. M. (1997). Relations of five-factor model antagonism facets with personality disorder symptomatology. Journal of Personality Assessment, 69, 297–313. Benning, S. D., Patrick, C. J., Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., & Iacono, W. G. (2005). Estimating facets of psychopathy from normal personality traits: A step toward community epidemiological investigations. Assessment, 12, 3–18. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO personality inventory (NEO PI-R) and the NEO five-factor inventory (NEO- COMPARISON OF HEXACO-PI-R AND NEO PI-R FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Decuyper, M., De Pauw, S., De Fruyt, F., De Bolle, M., & De Clercq, B. J. (2009). A metaanalysis of psychopathy-, antisocial PDand FFM associations. European Journal of Personality, 23, 531–565. De Vries, R. E., De Vries, A., De Hoogh, A., & Feij, J. (2009). More than the big five: Egoism and the HEXACO model of personality. European Journal of Personality, 23, 635–654. De Vries, R.E., Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2008). The Dutch HEXACO Personality Inventory: Psychometric properties, self-other agreement, and relations with psychopathy among low and high acquaintanceship dyads. Journal of Personality Assessment, 90, 142–151. Gaughan, E. T., Miller, J. D., Pryor, L. R., & Lynam, D. R. (2009). Comparing two alternative measures of general personality in the assessment of psychopathy: A test of the NEO PI-R and the MPQ. Journal of Personality, 77, 965–996. John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The big five inventory-versions 4a and 54. Berkley, CA: University of California, Berkley, Institute of Personality and Social Research. Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO personality inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39, 329–358. Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2005). Psychopathy, Machiavellianism, and narcissism in the five-factor model and the HEXACO model of personality structure. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1571–1582. Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2006). Further assessment of the HEXACO personality inven- 523 tory: Two new facet scales and an observer report form. Psychological Assessment, 18, 182–191. Lynam, D. R. (2002). Psychopathy from the perspective of the five-factor model of personality. In P. T. Costa & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (pp. 325–348). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Lynam, D. R., & Derefinko, K. J. (2006). Psychopathy and personality. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of the psychopathy (pp. 133–155). New York: Guilford Press. Lynam, D. R., & Widiger, T. A. (2007). Using a general model of personality to identify the basic elements of psychopathy. Journal of Personality Disorders, 21, 160–178. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1990). Personality in adulthood. New York: Guilford Press. Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., Widiger, T. A., & Leukefeld, C. (2001). Personality disorders as extreme variants of common personality dimensions: Can the five-factor model adequately represent psychopathy? Journal of Personality, 69, 253–276. Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to assessing anxiety, with an emphasis on self-report. In A. H. Tuma & J. D. Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 681–706). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Williams, K. M., Paulhus, D. L., & Hare, R. D. (2007). Capturing the four-factor structure of psychopathy in college students via selfreport. Journal of Personality Assessment, 88, 205–219.