Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Behavior Therapy 40 (2009) 380 – 392 www.elsevier.com/locate/bt Treating Youths With Selective Mutism With an Alternating Design of Exposure-Based Practice and Contingency Management Jennifer Vecchio Christopher A. Kearney University of Nevada, Las Vegas Selective mutism is a severe childhood disorder involving failure to speak in public situations in which speaking is expected. The present study examined 9 youths with selective mutism treated with child-focused, exposurebased practices and parent-focused contingency management via an alternating treatments design. Broadband measures of functioning were employed, but particular focus was made on behavioral assessment of words spoken audibly and daily in public situations. Treatment ranged from 8 to 32 sessions and resulted in positive end-state functioning for 8 of 9 participants. Broader analyses indicated greater effectiveness for exposure-based practice than contingency management. The results support recent case reports of behavioral treatment for this population but in more rigorous fashion. Clinical and research challenges are discussed, including caveats about length and intensity of treatment for this population and need to develop standardized daily measures. SELECTIVE MUTISM IS A persistent and potentially debilitating condition in which a child fails to speak in public situations in which speaking is expected. Youths with selective mutism generally speak well in familiar situations such as home but fail to speak to people in situations such as school, restaurants, shopping centers, and parks and other recreational areas. Failure to speak must last at least 1 month. A diagnosis of selective mutism does not generally Address correspondence to Christopher A. Kearney, Ph.D., Professor and Director of Clinical Training, University of Nevada, Las Vegas, Department of Psychology, 4505 Maryland Parkway, Las Vegas, NV 89154-5030; e-mail: chris.kearney@unlv.edu. 0005-7894/08/380–392/$1.00/0 © 2008 Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. apply to youths with a communication disorder such as stuttering or to youths who lack knowledge or comfort with the primary language spoken in public situations (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). As such, selective mutism in the United States does not typically apply to new immigrants or to youths whose parents are non-English speakers (Krysanski, 2003; Vecchio & Kearney, 2007). Selective mutism affects 0.2 to 2.0% of children, impacts boys and girls equally, and begins typically during preschool and early school-age years (Bergman, Piacentini, & McCracken, 2002; Chavira, Stein, Bailey, & Stein, 2004; Elizur & Perednik, 2003; Garcia, Freeman, Francis, Miller, & Leonard, 2004; Kumpulainen, 2002). Treatment for many cases of selective mutism is delayed several years, however, because these children are simply considered shy by parents or because the children often speak well at home (Andersson & Thomsen, 1998; Ford, Sladeczek, Carlson, & Kratochwill, 1998; Schwartz et al., 2006). As the disorder progresses, children may experience debilitating problems such as peer rejection, few friendships, incomplete verbal academic tasks or standardized tests, or inadequate language or social skills (Cunningham, McHolm, & Boyle, 2006; Kumpulainen, Rasanen, Raaska, & Somppi, 1998). Selective mutism has a chronic course in some but not all cases, though additional longitudinal data are sorely needed (Cohan, Price, & Stein, 2006; Remschmidt, Poller, Herpertz-Dahlmann, Hennighausen, & Gutenbrunner, 2001; Steinhausen, Wachter, Laimbock, & Metzke, 2006). Research regarding selective mutism has burgeoned in recent years to better conceptualize and treat this often shadowy problem. Children with selective mutism have been historically described as shy, timid, socially withdrawn, inhibited, and selective mutism reticent (Kopp & Gillberg, 1997; Kristensen, 2001; Lesser-Katz, 1986; Steinhausen & Juzi, 1996). In more recent years, researchers have employed standardized measures with this population to discover a very high prevalence of comorbid social anxiety disorder (Sharp, Sherman, & Gross, 2007; Vecchio & Kearney, 2005; Yeganeh, Beidel, Turner, Pina, & Silverman, 2003). Researchers have also found many youths with selective mutism to have a developmental disorder or delay (Kristensen, 2000). Some have claimed as well that language and auditory processing impairments, memory problems, and oppositional behavior exist in selective mutism, raising the possible need for subtyping in this population (Arie et al., 2007; Kristensen & Oerbeck, 2006; Manassis et al., 2003; Yeganeh, Beidel, & Turner, 2006). Treatment for youths with selective mutism has generally gravitated toward procedures that target anxiety-and oppositional-based components of the disorder. Cohan, Chavira, and Stein (2006) conducted a comprehensive review of the treatment literature for selective mutism and found that researchers primarily used behavioral strategies to reduce anxiety, boost appropriate verbalizations, and reduce oppositional or inappropriate attentionseeking behavior. Key behavioral strategies for youths with selective mutism include exposurebased practices such as systematic desensitization, social skills training and modeling, self-modeling, shaping and stimulus fading, and parent-based contingency management. A number of other strategies have also been used, including family systems, psychodynamic, and multimodal approaches, but the authors concluded that behavioral interventions were primarily supported. Other recent studies have also supported the use of behavioral strategies for cases of selective mutism (Baskind, 2007; Fisak, Oliveros, & Ehrenreich, 2006; Vecchio & Kearney, 2007). Cohan, Chavira, and Stein (2006) also found substantial drawbacks with the extant treatment literature for youths with selective mutism. First, the use of wide-ranging, standardized, or systematic assessment techniques has been relatively uncommon, especially for key outcome variables. Second, many intervention studies were based on record review or individual cases with no control. Third, standardized treatments with measures of treatment integrity are virtually nonexistent. Fourth, very little information is available as to which specific treatment strategies are most useful for children with selective mutism. Dismantling studies are necessary to determine which treatment components are most active for promoting behavior change for this population. In particular, research- 381 ers have called for graphic display of treatment outcomes as well as effect size calculations to help make this determination (Pionek Stone, Kratochwill, Sladezcek, & Serlin, 2002). To partly address these concerns, we conducted a controlled outcome study using standardized and systematic assessments and a single-participantbased alternating treatments design. The design allowed us to examine the specific impact of exposure-based practice and contingency management in 9 youths with selective mutism. We chose exposure-based practice and contingency management for several reasons. First, these techniques have been used historically to increase a child's audible speech in public places via anxiety reduction (exposure) and modification of inappropriate attention-seeking or compensatory behaviors such as pointing (contingency management). Second, we wished to keep the study circumscribed to make a clearer determination about which technique was more effective for reducing selective mutism. Third, child-based exposure and parent-based contingency management allowed the therapist to clearly delineate treatment phases. Based on limited literature that exposure-based practices and contingency management are effective for reducing selective mutism, we hypothesized that the treatment techniques would be equally effective. Method participants Nine children aged 4 to 9 years and their parents voluntarily participated in this study. Children were mostly female (7), had a mean age of 6.6 years (SD = 1.9), and were European-American (4), biracial (2), Asian-American (2), or Hispanic (1). Most families (7) had two parents, and mean annual family income was $67,889. Participants were recruited from public and private schools (7), a preschool (1), and from a general press release to the community (1). All children met diagnostic criteria for primary selective mutism. Secondary diagnoses included social phobia (9), separation anxiety disorder (2), specific phobia (2), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (1), enuresis (1), generalized anxiety disorder (1), and oppositional-defiant disorder (1). Participants were excluded if (a) failure to speak was due to lack of knowledge or comfort with spoken language required in a social situation, (b) failure to speak was better accounted for by a communication disorder such as stuttering, (c) mutism occurred exclusively during the course of a pervasive developmental or psychotic disorder, (d) a child was currently receiving pharmacological or other treatment for selective mutism, (e) a child had non- 382 vecchio & kearney English-speaking parents and/or English was not the primary language spoken in the home, (f) a child had deaf parents, (g) a child had been absent for more than 20% of school days in their current academic year, (h) a child had a developmental disorder, (i) a child was less than age 4 years or greater than age 10 years, or (j) a child had comorbid diagnoses rated as more severe than selective mutism. Children with comorbid diagnoses equal in severity to selective mutism were eligible for the study. measures Given the furtive and fluid nature of selective mutism symptoms, a daily behavioral assessment approach was emphasized for this study. In addition, given the great difficulty in collecting daily data from young children with selective mutism and their parents, participants were asked to primarily complete two uncomplicated daily measures. However, data were collected from multiple sources, including children, parents, teachers, and clinicians. Child measures. Children completed two daily measures. First, children completed a one-item Daily Rating of Anxiety Scale. A 0-to-10 scale was used (0 = none, 5 = some, and 10 = extreme anxiety). Youths provided one rating per day. Second, children completed a Daily Rating of Behavior Scale to monitor speaking patterns. Children recorded number of words they mouthed, whispered, or spoke in school or other public settings. If applicable, children also rated how audible their spoken words were on a 0-to-10 scale (0 = not at all audible and 10 = completely audible). Children were asked to tell us how many words they spoke (or whispered or mouthed) at school or in public that day. We contacted the children at night and collected this information via telephone or parents asked their children in the evening and provided us with a number. Children were asked how many words they said in school and outside of school (public settings) but not at home. Some children told us of the phrases they said at school and we counted the words, and other children counted how many words occurred in stories they read in class. Other children were instructed to say certain phrases to their teachers or others, and these words were counted as well. The two youngest children (ages 4 and 6 years) did not provide child data, but we found no difficulty with the other children with respect to counting. Parent measures. Parents completed a demographic information sheet as well as the Child Behavior Checklist and two daily rating scales. The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) is a 118-item broadband instrument that yields parent rating scores for internaliz- ing, externalizing, and mixed behavior problems. T scores were used for this study. The CBCL is one of the most widely used standardized measures for assessing emotional and behavioral problems in children and has demonstrated excellent test-retest (r = .90) and interrater (r = .76) reliability as well as construct and criterion-related validity. The Daily Rating of Child Anxiety Scale was a one-item instrument rated on a 0-to-10 scale (0 = none, 5 = some, and 10 = extreme anxiety). Parents rated their perceived level of child anxiety on a daily basis. In addition, the Daily Rating of Child Behavior Scale was completed by parents daily to monitor their child's speaking patterns. Parents recorded number of words their child mouthed, whispered, or spoke in school or other public settings. If parents conducted an exposure in the school and took their child to the school (and thus observed their child speak to the teacher or others), then they counted the words spoken. Parents only recorded words they actually heard their child speak. If applicable, they also rated the audibility of their child's words on a 0-to-10 scale (0 = not at all audible and 10 = completely audible). Teacher measures. Primary teachers for seven participants completed the Teacher Report Form (TRF; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001) and primary teachers for six participants completed two daily rating scales, though only three teachers provided posttreatment TRF data. The TRF is a 118-item broadband instrument that yields teacher rating scores for internalizing, externalizing, and mixed behavior problems. T scores were used for this study. The TRF is one of the most widely used standardized measures for assessing emotional and behavioral problems in children and has demonstrated excellent test-retest (r = .90) and interrater (r = .60) reliability as well as construct and criterion-related validity. TRF data were obtained for seven participants at pretreatment (2–7 and 9), for three participants at posttreatment (4, 6, 7), and for four participants at follow-up (4, 5, 8, 9). The Daily Rating of Student Anxiety Scale was a one-item instrument rated on a 0-to-10 scale (0 = none, 5 = some, and 10 = extreme anxiety). Teachers rated their perceived level of student anxiety on a daily basis. In addition, the Daily Rating of Student Behavior was completed by teachers daily to monitor their student's speaking patterns. Teachers recorded number of words their student mouthed, whispered, or spoke in school. If applicable, they also rated the audibility of their student's words on a 0-to-10 scale (0 = not at all audible and 10 = completely audible). Clinician measure. Clinicians administered the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: selective mutism Child and Parent Versions (ADIS-C/P; Silverman & Albano, 1996). This semistructured diagnostic interview is widely used to assess anxiety-related disorders in youths, including selective mutism. The interview consists of individual sections on various mental disorders as well as yes/no items to assess symptoms and symptom severity, frequency, and duration. The interview accommodates young or mute children by allowing for nonverbal responses to items and via visual rating scales. The interviews have demonstrated good kappa reliability coefficients for major diagnostic categories based on composite information (.62–1.00) and parent information only (.65–1.00) (Silverman, Saavedra, & Pina, 2001). The interviews have also demonstrated good convergence with anxiety scale scores in an examination of concurrent validity (Wood, Piacentini, Bergman, McCracken, & Barrios, 2002). Children aged 9 years (n = 3) and parents were interviewed separately and data were later combined to derive composite diagnoses (see Silverman & Albano, 1996, for specific procedures). Children in the study less than age 9 years did not participate in the diagnostic interview process, so diagnoses in these cases were derived solely from parent report. The interviews were administered by an advanced graduate student in clinical psychology trained in the use of the measures. Clinical severity ratings of diagnoses were assigned on a 0to-8 scale to derive primary and comorbid diagnoses. Comorbid diagnoses were those with less severe clinical severity ratings but which also applied to a given child. assessment and consultation procedure Initial participant screening. Interested parents contacted the researchers and participated in an initial telephone screening. Parents were asked to identify their child's primary behavior problem and to stipulate whether their child met general criteria for selective mutism. In addition, the screener interviewed the parents regarding each exclusionary criterion mentioned earlier. If the initial screening indicated a child met diagnostic criteria for selective mutism and did not meet any exclusionary criteria, then a formal assessment session was scheduled. Thirty-seven sets of parents were screened in this fashion. Twenty were excluded because they met one or more of the exclusionary criteria: 10 were excluded because English was not the primary language or the child was not knowledgeable or comfortable with spoken language used in most public situations, 4 were excluded for currently receiving treatment for selective mutism, 4 383 were excluded for age constraints, and 2 were excluded for having deaf parents. In addition, one child refused to participate in the study. Of the 21 children excluded during initial screening, 14 were male and 7 were female. Formal assessment and consultation. Sixteen formal assessments and/or initial treatment were thus conducted. Parents were given the option to have the formal assessment conducted at the child's school, in their home, or at a specialized university clinic. Seven assessments were conducted at the clinic setting, and two were conducted at the participant's home. Parents provided written consent, and children provided nonverbal assent prior to data collection. Parents also completed the diagnostic interview, CBCL, and a demographic information form. Parents provided consent to contact their child's teacher to derive TRF and other data. Seven children were excluded from the study during this formal assessment/early intervention process: 1 child failed to meet diagnostic criteria for selective mutism, 1 child had a non-Englishspeaking parent, 2 children met criteria for selective mutism but their parents decided not to participate in the study, and 3 children met criteria for selective mutism but dropped out of the study within 3 sessions. Children excluded from the study during the formal assessment/early treatment process were referred for treatment, received treatment from the researchers separate from the study, or were provided with contact information for other mental health providers in the community. Of these 7 children, 5 were male and 2 were female. The remaining 9 children who completed formal assessment and who remained in the study did so for 8 to 32 sessions designed to be administered twice per week (therapy time range, 2 to 5 months). Eligible families were then scheduled for a consultation session to review assessment results and learn more about the treatment study. Families were informed of the time commitment involved, including two formal treatment sessions per week (scheduled at least 3 days apart) and daily rating scales. The basic principles and goals of each treatment component were described. Parents then provided additional consent for the treatment phase of the study, and the first formal session was scheduled. An additional release of information form was completed to allow the therapist to coordinate treatment with each child's teacher. The initial assessment and consultation session constituted the baseline period of this study. Daily ratings of anxiety and speech were then collected from baseline to posttreatment. 384 vecchio & kearney treatment procedure A single-participant alternating treatment design was employed for this study. Treatment A involved child-focused, exposure-based practice and Treatment B involved parent-focused contingency management. The order in which treatments were provided was randomized and counterbalanced. Five children began with Treatment A and followed an ABBABAAB pattern. Four children began with Treatment B and followed a BAABABBA pattern. Between sessions, the alternating treatment was not used. When a child received Treatment A, parents were instructed not to employ treatment B until the next Treatment B session. The therapist provided reminders of this requirement after each treatment session. Regular parent-therapist discussions regarding the order, progress, and assignment of treatment revealed no instances in which parents or children reported difficulty setting aside one set of techniques (e.g., A) to implement the other set of techniques (e.g., B). Treatments A and B were not employed concurrently. Treatment A. Treatment A consisted of childfocused, exposure-based practices that involved shaping, modeling, prompting, and in vivo exposure along an established hierarchy of key situations. Shaping included the production of vocalizations with mouth closed, vocalizations with mouth open, mouthed words without verbalization, whispered single words, whispered sentences, and sentences with progressively increased audibility until normal spontaneous communication was achieved. Modeling and prompting were used by the therapist to promote target responses. Youths were required to achieve adequate speech in this manner in a progressively more difficult series of settings: home, clinic setting, community settings such as restaurants or shopping centers, and school. Examples of initial tasks for a child along a hierarchy included playing games with verbal input, reading aloud, giving short reports, asking and answering questions, practicing short conversations, and ordering food. Examples of later tasks at school included speaking and reading to the therapist in an empty classroom, speaking and reading with a teacher present in the classroom, speaking and reading with a few classmates in the classroom, reading to a teacher or peers in a small reading group, and speaking in class when expected to speak, such as being called on to answer a question. Homework assignments were also provided to youths to practice what had been accomplished during the past treatment session. Treatment A thus involved a proactive, therapistdirected introduction to a speaking situation with concurrent anxiety-reduction techniques such as breathing retraining. Youths were required to, and did, enter arranged exposure situations to engage in a formal anxiety extinction process. Expectations and prescribed opportunities for speaking in these situations were high. Exposure situations were highly anxiety-provoking and exposures were conducted in situations other than settings common to a child's everyday life. During exposure-based homework assignments between sessions, parents were permitted to contact the therapist for assistance. No formal rewards were provided for speaking during exposures. Treatment B. Treatment B consisted of parentfocused contingency management that involved establishing a consequence system for speaking and failure to speak. Parents were asked to reward youths for appropriate speech in public places and punish youths for failure to speak in public places during situations the child was expected to speak. Punishment involved loss of privileges such as early bedtime or loss of toys or television time based on parent observations or teacher reports of failure to speak during the day in public settings. Youths were asked by parents as appropriate to speak audibly to others in a preestablished way. Consequences were established for successfully or unsuccessfully engaging in and practicing therapeutic homework assignments. Initial tangible reinforcers for speech were later replaced with greater social reinforcement. Routines were also established so a child would have increased opportunities to speak in public; examples include accepting a call from the therapist or family members, asking a child to greet someone in public, or taking a child to recreational activities that require some social interaction. Parents were also encouraged as well, even during end stages of treatment, to continue placing their child in interactive settings and restrict accommodations for unwillingness to speak. Parents were also taught to engage in short, specific commands to their children and to ignore inappropriate compensatory behaviors, especially as treatment progressed. Teachers were instructed as well to engage in appropriate contingency management procedures during and following treatment. Treatment B thus involved a reactive, largely parent-directed response to a child's actions in everyday speaking situations without concurrent anxiety reduction techniques such as breathing retraining. Youths were not required to enter an arranged exposure situation to engage in a formal anxiety extinction process. Instead, a child's speech or lack of speech was linked to incentives or disincentives, respectively, in situations common to his or her everyday life such as school. During contingencymanagement-based homework assignments between selective mutism sessions, parents did not contact the therapist for assistance. A full description of the treatment protocols is available from the first author. treatment integrity and reaction One therapist conducted all assessment and treatment sessions. Integrity raters attended 76% of treatment sessions to observe the therapist and provide yes/no answers to key questions. Integrity raters were four undergraduate and two graduate psychology students who read and reviewed each treatment protocol (A and B) and who were taught the different techniques in each treatment protocol. Raters were given a copy of the treatment protocols and the treatment integrity form for evaluating the sessions. For Treatment A, raters answered the following questions: If necessary, were shaping, modeling, and prompting used? Was the hierarchy mentioned and discussed? Did the in vivo exposures follow the hierarchy? Were homework assignments pertaining to the exposure given? Did the session focus solely on Treatment A? For Treatment B, raters answered the following questions: Was the treatment session held only with the parents? Were the concepts of contingencies, reinforcement, and shaping discussed? Were consequences, rewards, and target behaviors clearly identified? Were homework assignments pertaining to the plan given to the parents? Did the session focus only on Treatment B? Treatment reaction questions were presented to parents at the end of the study. Parents were asked to rate two questions on a 0-to-10 scale (0 = completely not due to the treatment approach and 10 = completely due to the treatment approach). The following questions were posed: How would FIGURE 1 385 you attribute the changes in your child's behavior to the exposure-based treatment approach? How would you attribute the changes in your child's behavior to the contingency management-based treatment approach? posttreatment and 3-month follow-up Treatment continued until criteria were met for positive end-state functioning, when parents withdrew from the study after at least three treatment sessions, or following 6 months of treatment, whichever came first. Positive end-state functioning was defined as speaking in school when expected to speak, speaking in a free manner, and speaking at an audible level to teachers and peers and in all mandated social settings such as restaurants, recreational areas/playgrounds, and school. Participants received 8 to 32 treatment sessions split equally across Treatment A and Treatment B (M = 18.1, SD = 7.7). Diagnostic interviews and broadband measures of behavior (CBCL, TRF) were readministered at posttreatment. Participants were contacted as well 3 months following posttreatment. Formal posttreatment data were unavailable for two families and follow-up data were unavailable for three families due to moving or failure to respond to therapist requests for information. Results interrater reliability of interview and daily measures An additional graduate student attended 67% of ADIS-C/P interviews, and 100% interrater diag- Words spoken per day in public by Participant 1. 386 vecchio & kearney FIGURE 2 Words spoken per day in public by Participant 2. nostic agreement was obtained. Regarding daily measures, interrater reliability for ratings of number of words spoken was initially calculated across all three data sources: children, parents, and teachers. Interrater reliability was .86. Interrater reliability for daily ratings of number of words spoken was also calculated for only children and parents: .92. Child and parent ratings of child anxiety and mouthing and whispering of words were largely zero across all cases at all points, so these variables were not included in interrater reliability, individual, or collective analyses. individual participant data Participants improved greatly during treatment. Individual participant data regarding words spoken per day in public are presented graphically in Figures 1 through 9. At posttreatment and 3- FIGURE 3 month follow-up, eight of nine participants met criteria for positive end-state functioning. One participant (#6) met criteria for selective mutism at posttreatment and 3-month follow-up based on parent but not child report. However, this participant's parent indicated her daughter was speaking more often in public and with greater ease in several social situations at follow-up compared to pretreatment. Her clinical interference rating for selective mutism also declined from 8 at pretreatment to 4 at post-treatment. The family terminated treatment after 16 sessions due to a family emergency. treatment effect size Cohen's d was calculated to measure treatment effect size based on child, parent, and teacher reports. Effects of Treatment A (exposure-based Words spoken per day in public by Participant 3. selective mutism FIGURE 4 Words spoken per day in public by Participant 4. practice) and Treatment B (contingency management) were compared by pooling data across participants (Table 1). Three paired-sample t-tests were computed comparing Treatment A and Treatment B means per participant for mean number of words spoken per day. Children displayed significantly greater speech during Treatment A than Treatment B based on child (t = 4.96, pb .01), parent (t = 3.70, p b .01), and teacher (t = 2.82, p b .05) reports. Dunlap and colleagues (1996) presented a formula for calculating effect size across data points. This formula accounts for the correlation between measures so effect size is not overestimated. Cohen's d was thus computed from the paired-sample t-tests with consideration of the correlation between Treatments A and B. Results indicated a large effect size based on child report FIGURE 5 387 (0.83), a moderate effect size based on parent report (0.41), and a small effect size based on teacher report (0.25). associated symptomatology Data from broadband measures were not subjected to formal analysis due to limited sample size. However, participants generally showed improvement from pretreatment to posttreatment and 3-month follow-up. CBCL internalizing, externalizing, and total T scores decreased for seven participants from pretreatment (66.7/49.3/ 56.7) to posttreatment (62.7/47.5/53.2) and remained stable at 3-month follow-up (61.3/48.8/ 53.7). TRF internalizing, externalizing, and total T scores for three participants decreased from pretreatment (66.3/51.0/58.0) to posttreatment Words spoken per day in public by Participant 5. 388 vecchio & kearney FIGURE 6 Words spoken per day in public by Participant 6. (63.3/43.0/54.0). TRF internalizing, externalizing, and total T scores for three different participants remained stable from pretreatment (60.3/47.0/ 53.7) to 3-month follow-up (62.0/50.0/55.0). treatment integrity and reaction Treatment integrity was determined by raters to be 100%. The therapist in all observed sessions maintained strict adherence to the appropriate treatment and did not introduce aspects of the opposing treatment. Treatment reaction ratings were obtained from seven participants. Treatment A (child-focused, exposure-based practice) received a mean rating of 9.5 on a 0-to-10 scale (range 7–10, SD = 1.1). Treatment B (parent-focused contin- FIGURE 7 gency management) received a mean rating of 7.9 on a 0-to-10 scale (range 5–10, SD = 2.0). Discussion This study is one of the first to employ systematic assessment and treatment procedures in a controlled investigation of outcome utilizing exposurebased practices and contingency management for selective mutism. Results indicated that both treatment approaches may be effective for increasing audible speech in this population, though exposurebased practices may be significantly more effective. Results of the study match those of several previously published case reports, but do so in more rigorous fashion (Baskind, 2007; Fisak et al., Words spoken per day in public by Participant 7. selective mutism FIGURE 8 Words spoken per day in public by Participant 8. 2006; Vecchio & Kearney, 2007). The results also provide further empirical evidence for earlier claims that behavioral procedures for selective mutism produce positive end-state functioning (Cohan, Chavira, & Stein, 2006). Results of the present study suggest that exposure-based practices may be more effective than contingency management, though both procedure sets were helpful. This may provide some preliminary support for conceptualizing selective mutism as an anxiety disorder because exposure-based practices are commonly and effectively used for problems such as panic disorder, social phobia, and generalized anxiety disorder (Compton et al., 2004; James, Soler, & Weatherall, 2005; In-Albon & Schneider, 2007). In addition, the most common comorbid diagnoses in the present study were anxiety-based, particularly social phobia. This FIGURE 9 389 finding replicates prior diagnostic studies in this area (Sharp et al., 2007; Vecchio & Kearney, 2005; Yeganeh et al., 2003). Parents also found exposurebased practices to be somewhat more useful than contingency management practices. Some researchers, however, claim that elements other than anxiety contribute to selective mutism (Arie et al., 2007; Kristensen & Oerbeck, 2006; Manassis et al., 2003; Yeganeh et al., 2006), and two key findings from the present study may provide some support for these claims. First, the utility of contingency management, though not as powerful as exposure-based practices, may illustrate an oppositional or other component of selective mutism that requires external consequences for behavior change. In the case of one participant who continued to meet criteria for selective mutism, she reported at follow-up that she Words spoken per day in public by Participant 9. 390 vecchio & kearney Table 1 Means and standard deviations for number of words spoken across informants for Treatments A and B Report Treatment A Treatment B Child Parent Teacher 62.8 (17.3) 62.1 (18.6) 28.0 (30.9) 44.2 (22.1) 45.0 (30.0) 19.6 (27.2) Note. Treatment A = child-focused, exposure-based practice; Treatment B = parent-focused contingency management. simply chose not to speak in certain situations and was not fearful. Second, daily anxiety ratings and rates of mouthing and whispering for all participants were extremely low, to the point that formal analyses of these variables were rendered moot. One possibility is that participants felt the comprehensive rating system was cumbersome and thus concentrated on the most important variable: audible words spoken in public. In addition, anxiety ratings were not necessarily specific to speaking situations. Still, the low ratings may provide evidence of limited daily distress for this sample. Parents provided anecdotal reports, even after education about anxiety components and endorsement of social anxiety symptoms, that they believed their children to be more shy than anxious in social situations. Further research is needed to explore potential discrepancies between assignments of anxiety-based diagnoses and relatively low daily ratings of anxiety in this population. Some speculate that selective mutism affects self-reported anxiety because a child chooses not to answer others or to place herself in many anxietyprovoking situations during the day (Beidel & Turner, 2005; Yeganeh et al., 2003). Clinicians who address youths with selective mutism may have several daunting challenges before them. First, developing collaborative efforts with children, parents, and teachers is likely imperative for generalizing treatment gains, especially in cases involving school-based mutism. Second, intense clinical efforts are generally necessary for this population, including frequent sessions, an extended treatment time line, exposurebased practices in community and school settings, and frequent conversations with parents and consulting professionals. Third, use of daily behavioral assessment measures, especially with respect to words audibly spoken, will likely provide more information about treatment effectiveness than global measures of functioning. Researchers who address youths with selective mutism also have several daunting challenges before them. Because selective mutism is a particularly fluid and furtive problem, developing standardized, daily, and sensitive measures for this population remains a priority. Larger-scale investigations of behavioral and nonbehavioral treatments for youths with selective mutism remain necessary as well. Of particular value would be interventions designed to fit the demands of everyday clinical practice, or eliminating time-consuming techniques that have less efficacy and that limit family choices regarding treatment providers. As mentioned earlier, longitudinal data and longer-term follow-up data regarding this population are also needed. Finally, early screening for selective mutism in an era when many children attend day care or preschool would seem to be a useful practice. Limitations of the current study should be considered when interpreting results. First, teacher compliance to daily measures regarding selective mutism was spotty, and these measures may have been too taxing in some cases. Second, the scope of the daily dependent measures may have been a bit ambitious for children and parents, though compliance was generally good. Still, children and parents may have eschewed attention to lesser variables such as daily anxiety or mouthing of words in favor of more obvious and relevant variables such as words audibly spoken. Multiple ratings throughout the day may have been preferable as well. Third, as with any alternating treatments design, the possibility of carryover effects must be considered. Finally, various specific techniques were included under the broad rubrics of child-focused, exposure-based practice and parentfocused contingency management. Research is needed to further dismantle these techniques to identify individual effectiveness. Despite these limitations, the present study does provide new though preliminary evidence regarding differential effectiveness of child-and parent-based treatments for youths with selective mutism. The study provides some evidence that exposure-based practices could remain a priority when addressing this population, but that contingency management practices may be a useful adjunct. Parent-based approaches for selective mutism could be particularly important in cases involving high rates of compensatory behavior, language differences between home and school, or when young children depend on frequent exposures established by parents. As with many childhood behavior disorders, including children and parents in the treatment process is likely preferable than either party alone. References Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles. Burlington, VT: selective mutism University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th ed text revision). Washington, DC: Author. Andersson, C. B., & Thomsen, P. H. (1998). Electively mute children: An analysis of 37 Danish cases. Nord Journal of Psychiatry, 52, 231–238. Arie, M., Henkin, Y., Lamy, D., Tetin-Schneider, S., Apter, A., Sadeh, A., et al. (2007). Reduced auditory processing capacity during vocalization in children with selective mutism. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 419–421. Baskind, S. (2007). A behavioural intervention for selective mutism in an eight-year-old boy. Educational and Child Psychology, 24, 87–94. Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (2005). Childhood anxiety disorders: A guide to research and treatment. New York: Routledge. Bergman, R. L., Piacentini, J., & McCracken, J. T. (2002). Prevalence and description of selective mutism in a schoolbased study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 938–946. Chavira, D. A., Stein, M. B., Bailey, K., & Stein, M. T. (2004). Child anxiety in primary care: Prevalent but untreated. Depression and Anxiety, 20, 155–164. Cohan, S. L., Chavira, D. A., & Stein, M. B. (2006). Practitioner review: Psychosocial interventions for children with selective mutism: A critical evaluation of the literature from 1990–2005. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 1085–1097. Cohan, S. L., Price, J. M., & Stein, M. B. (2006). Suffering in silence: Why a developmental psychopathology perspective on selective mutism is needed. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, 341–355. Compton, S. N., March, J. S., Brent, D., Albano, A. M., Weersing, V. R., & Curry, J. (2004). Cognitive-behavioral psychotherapy for anxiety and depressive disorders in children and adolescents: An evidence-based medicine review. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 930–959. Cunningham, C. E., McHolm, A. E., & Boyle, M. H. (2006). Social phobia, anxiety, oppositional behavior, social skills, and self-concept in children with specific selective mutism, generalized selective mutism, and community controls. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 15, 245–255. Elizur, Y., & Perednik, R. (2003). Prevalence and description of selective mutism in immigrant and native families: A controlled study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1451–1459. Fisak, B. J., Oliveros, A., & Ehrenreich, J. T. (2006). Assessment and behavioral treatment of selective mutism. Clinical Case Studies, 5, 382–402. Ford, M. A., Sladeczeck, E., Carlson, J., & Kratochwill, T. R. (1998). Selective mutism: Phenomenological characteristics. School Psychology Quarterly, 13, 192–227. Garcia, A. M., Freeman, J. B., Francis, G., Miller, L. M., & Leonard, H. L. (2004). Selective mutism. In T. H. Ollendick, & J. S. March (Eds.), Phobic and anxiety disorders in children and adolescents (pp. 433–455). New York: Oxford University Press. In-Albon, T., & Schneider, S. (2007). Psychotherapy of childhood anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 76, 15–24. James, A., Soler, A., & Weatherall, R. (2005). Cognitive behavioural therapy for anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 19, CD004690. 391 Kopp, S., & Gillberg, C. (1997). Selective mutism: A population-based study: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 257–262. Kristensen, H. (2000). Selective mutism and comorbidity with developmental disorder/delay, anxiety disorder, and elimination disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 249–256. Kristensen, H. (2001). Multiple informants' report of emotional and behavioural problems in a nation-wide sample of selective mute children and controls. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 10, 135–142. Kristensen, H., & Oerbeck, B. (2006). Is selective mutism associated with deficits in memory span and visual memory? An exploratory case-control study. Depression and Anxiety, 23, 71–76. Krysanski, V. (2003). A brief review of selective mutism literature. Journal of Psychology, 137, 29–40. Kumpulainen, K. (2002). Phenomenology and treatment of selective mutism. CNS Drugs, 16, 175–180. Kumpulainen, K., Rasanen, E., Raaska, H., & Somppi, V. (1998). Selective mutism among second-graders in elementary school. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 7, 24–29. Lesser-Katz, M. (1986). Stranger reaction and elective mutism in young children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 458–469. Manassis, K., Fung, D., Tannock, R., Sloman, L., Fiksenbaum, L., & McInnes, A. (2003). Characterizing selective mutism: Is it more than social anxiety? Depression and Anxiety, 18, 153–161. Pionek Stone, B., Kratochwill, T. R., Sladeczek, I., & Serlin, R. C. (2002). Treatment of selective mutism: A best-evidence synthesis. School Psychology Quarterly, 17, 168–190. Remschmidt, H., Poller, M., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Hennighausen, K., & Gutenbrunner, C. (2001). A follow-up study of 45 patients with elective mutism. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 251, 284–296. Sharp, W. G., Sherman, C., & Gross, A. M. (2007). Selective mutism and anxiety: A review of the current conceptualization of the disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 568–579. Silverman, W. K., & Albano, A. M. (1996). The Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for Children for DSM-IV, child and parent versions. Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX. Silverman, W. K., Saavedra, L. M., & Pina, A. A. (2001). Testretest reliability of anxiety symptoms and diagnoses with the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: Child and parent versions. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 937–944. Steinhausen, H., & Juzi, C. (1996). Elective mutism: An analysis of 100 cases. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 606–614. Steinhausen, H., Wachter, M., Laimbock, K., & Metzke, C. W. (2006). A long-term outcome study of selective mutism in childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 751–756. Vecchio, J. L., & Kearney, C. A. (2005). Selective mutism in children: Comparison to youths with and without anxiety disorders. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 27, 31–37. Vecchio, J. L., & Kearney, C. A. (2007). Assessment and treatment of a Hispanic youth with selective mutism. Clinical Case Studies, 6, 34–43. Wood, J. J., Piacentini, J. C., Bergman, R. L., McCracken, J., & Barrios, V. (2002). Concurrent validity of the anxiety disorders section of the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: Child and parent versions. Journal 392 vecchio & kearney of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 335–342. Yeganeh, R., Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (2006). Selective mutism: More than social anxiety? Depression and Anxiety, 23, 117–123. Yeganeh, R., Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., Pina, A. A., & Silverman, W. K. (2003). Clinical distinctions between selective mutism and social phobia: An investigation of childhood psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1069–1075. R E C E I V E D : January 14, 2008 A C C E P T E D : October 12, 2008 Available online 3 November 2008