See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/290315285 Psychology of Happiness and Flourishing, and the Problem of Evil Chapter · December 2012 CITATIONS READS 0 1,308 1 author: Alexander Poddiakov National Research University Higher School of Economics 68 PUBLICATIONS 82 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Non-transitive chess arrangements View project Evolution of creating difficulties for others View project All content following this page was uploaded by Alexander Poddiakov on 28 January 2016. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. Poddiakov, Alexander. Psychology of Happiness and Flourishing, and the Problem of Evil Translated from Russian. Original text: Poddiakov, A. (2012). [Psikhologiya schast'ya i protsvetaniya i problema zla]. In A.L.Zhuravlev, A.V.Yurevich (Eds.), [Nravstvennost' sovremennogo rossiiskogo obshchestva: psikhologicheskii analiz]. Moscow: Institut psikhologii RAN. Pp. 109-136. The past 20-30 years have seen an intensive development of psychological approaches to the study of happiness, well-being, flourishing and practical recommendations on how to become happier. These are worthy tasks. Depending on the degree of well-being/conflict-generating relations that prevail in society and the surrounding world, there arises the problem of organizing well-being and happiness in a way that would secure them against negative and hostile actors (robbers, international terrorists, invaders, etc.) who effectively impede flourishing. The aim of this article is to analyze the attitude to deliberate evil and its agents on the part of psychologists who study happiness and flourishing, and identify the features and dynamics of this attitude. General ideas of benevolence/malevolence of the world in psychological approaches to the study of happiness and well-being Beginning at least from the studies of Benedict Ruth in the first third of the 20th century psychology has been elaborating perceptions of two types of people’s attitude to the world. These types were succinctly described in “The Doctrine of Three Worlds” articulated by a character in Stanislaw Lem (Lem, 1990, p. 207): “In a benevolent world: It is easier to create than to destroy; It is easier to make happy than to torment; It is easier to save than to ruin; It is easier to revive than to kill. In a malevolent world: It is easier to destroy than to create; It is easier to torment than to make happy; It is easier to ruin than to save; It is easier to kill than to revive. In a neutral world it is equally easy (difficult) to do both.” Similarly, the works of psychologists examine and compare: - the perception of the world as largely a harmonious, low-conflict place where crimes and even serious conflicts are a deviation from the norm; - the perception of the world as riddled with contradictions and conflicts and unfriendliness which, even if it is not predominant, cannot be ignored (Druzhinin, 2000; Enikolopov, 2011; Poddiakov, 2007; Lee, 1995; Werner, 2004; Zabielski, 2007). Thus, Enikolopov analyzes Epstein’s concept of implicit theory of reality people automatically construct (Epstein, 1991). It comprises the following main blocks: sub-theories about the self, others, the inanimate world, and beliefs regarding their interactions. The personality-based 2 theory of reality treats the world as friendly or hostile; as meaningful, predictable, controllable, stable and just or the opposite; the attitude to other people as friendly, not threatening or as potentially threatening (Enikolopov, 2011). By no means all the works on the psychology of happiness and flourishing explicitly formulate the attitude to the problem of good and evil. I have picked the works whose authors are engaged with the problem and seek to get their opinion across to the reader. In terms of the two above-mentioned types of attitude to the world, the views of the psychologists who reflect on these matters can be divided into several groups depending on how they assess: • the present-day world; • the need and possibility of changing it; • ways of changing it (if it needs to be changed). 1. “The world is ideal, so there is no need to improve it, all your efforts are wasted.” That position is most clearly formulated by N.Linde, author of the book Basic Principles of Modern Psychotherapy (2002). In his Sutra on Happiness, he writes: “As my guru used to say: ‘The Gates of Hell are locked from within’. If they are locked from within, how can God lead people out of there? They want to be there and they fiercely resist being taken to Paradise. How can an angry person get into Paradise? How can a depressed person get there? How can a person afflicted by fears and anxieties get there? How can an ever fighting person get there? There are no nuclear warheads in Paradise. But there are people who cling to warheads so hard that they cannot make their way into Paradise. The best way to get rid of war and warheads is to learn to live in Paradise and to teach your enemies to do the same. If everyone lives in Paradise, there will be no need for warheads and there will be no enemies. But if the enemies are so foolish that they do not want to live in Paradise, so much the worse for them, though it’s a pity” (Linde, 2009). Linde does not say anything about criminals, terrorists and the like, but logically speaking the position should be the same. If they do not want to live in Paradise let them rape, rob, kill and do whatever they see fit, though it’s regrettable. 2. “The world is not ideal, but is logically becoming better.” By seeking happiness and improving ourselves we are working not simply for the future well-being of people, but also for his omnipotence, omniscience and righteousness. This position is embraced by Seligman, one of the founders of positive psychology. He writes that as humanity develops the number and significance of win-win situations, and not of win-loss situations increases. This echoes the ideas of A.Nazaretyan who argues that physical violence is resorted to less and less as civilization develops (Nazaretyan, 2006), and of C.Benson to the effect that the idea of abhorrence of intentionally caused suffering is gaining ground, if only slowly and gradually (Benson, 2001). In approvingly rendering the ideas of B. Wright, Seligman writes: “Progress in history is not like an unstoppable locomotive, but more like a balky horse that often refuses to budge and even walks backward occasionally: But the broad movement of human history, not ignoring such backward walks as the holocaust, anthrax terrorism, and the genocide against the Tasmanian aborigines, is, when viewed over centuries, in the direction of more win-win” (Seligman, 2013, p. 255)1. 1 In this translation references to the edition of 2013 are given (and not to the Russian edition of 2006). 3 “A process that continually selects for more complexity is ultimately aimed at nothing less than omniscience, omnipotence, and goodness. This is not, of course, a fulfillment that will be achieved in our lifetimes, or even in the lifetime of our species. The best we can do as individuals is to choose to be a small part of furthering this progress. This is the door through which meaning that transcends us can enter our lives. A meaningful life is one that joins with something larger than we are—and the larger that something is, the more meaning our lives have. Partaking in a process that has the bringing of a God who is endowed with omniscience, omnipotence, and goodness as its ultimate end joins our lives to an enormously large something. <…> The good life consists in deriving happiness by using your signature strengths every day in the main realms of living. The meaningful life adds one more component: using these same strengths to forward knowledge, power, or goodness. A life that does this is pregnant with meaning, and if God comes at the end, such a life is sacred.” (Ibid., p. 260). 3. The third position is as follows: “The universe is not hostile, nor yet is it friendly. It is simply indifferent” (J. H. Holmes, cited by: Csikszentmihalyi, 2013, p. 23)2. “The flow experience, like everything else, is not ‘good’ in an absolute sense… Optimal experience is a form of energy, and energy can be used either to help or to destroy. Fire warms or burns; atomic energy can generate electricity or it can obliterate the world. Energy is power, but power is only a means. The goals to which it is applied can make life either richer or more painful… The task is to learn how to enjoy everyday life without diminishing other people’s chances to enjoy theirs” (Csikszentmihalyi, 2013, p. 83-84). This is the position of another founder of positive psychology, M.Csikszentmihalyi. Positive psychologists refer to him very selectively and cautiously stressing some aspects of his approach (for example, considering a flow as a source of positive sensations) and studiously side-stepping other elements of this approach, as will be shown later, including the obvious attention to the negative aspects of human being and the problem of intentional evil which few positive psychologists share. He writes that the state of a flow of positive emotions and happiness was probably characteristic of Marquis de Sade, a humble Chinaman hacking cattle carcasses, the spectators of gladiatorial battles, the Golden Horde warriors notorious for their brutality and of modern soldiers killing their enemies; criminals stealing a car, those who take part in mass acts of vandalism, etc. (Csikszentmihalyi, 2013). Csikszentmihalyi believes it is necessary to take into account the overall balance of order and chaos created by different people and social groups pursuing their opposite goals. Society must maximize the chances of all its members achieving their goals and minimize chaos. М. Csikszentmihalyi stresses, however, that even that does not guarantee that what is happening is ethical because it can be achieved at the expense of other societies (e.g., in Nazism). 4. Finally, the last position: “You can’t be healthy on a sick planet” (Servan-Schreiber, 2009; p. 81; cited by: Wong, 2011, p. 77). “You cannot live a healthy and fulfilling life in a sick world contaminated by crime, corruption, injustice, oppression, and poverty. Such evils can destroy individuals and societies like cancer cells” (Wong, 2011, p. 77). This is the position of P.Wong who promotes the projects “Radical Positive Psychology for Radical Times” (Wong, 2007) and “Positive Psychology 2.0” (Wong, 2011). He stresses the need to 2 In this translation references to the edition of 2013 are given (and not to the Russian edition of 2011). 4 contribute to the development of good and worthy people, and of the civil society as a whole, and to overcome and transform negative phenomena. To understand life in all its complexity, it is necessary to study the paradoxical effects of the interaction of the negative and the positive. The Positive psychology 2.0 project enlarges the American branch of Positive psychology as represented by Seligman providing the basis for the development of good, kindly people and psychologically healthy institutions, development that challenges negatives and the finiteness of human existence (Wong, 2011, p. 77-78). It has to be stressed that the above-quoted propositions belong to the level of basic philosophical ideas of the world (which give rise to various specific practices). The researchers of positive psychology are making a serious mistake when they insist that it is simultaneously positivist-scientific in the traditional sense, that is, is concerned only with facts and methods and does not purport to philosophize and build a certain picture of the world. The above-quoted ideas are, if anything, philosophical reflections on Good (“Partaking in a process that has the bringing of a God who is endowed with omniscience, omnipotence, and goodness as its ultimate end joins our lives to an enormously large something”, “if God comes at the end, such a life is sacred”, etc.) and Evil (genocide, terrorism, crime, etc.) and on the dynamics of these phenomena. A critique of ungrounded claims of positive psychology to be a positivist science is found in Friedman and Robbins (2012). The books on positive psychology nowhere suggest that the methods of coping with situations caused by the premeditated actions of others (murder, arson, treachery) can (and often must) be special. In the concluding part of this article it will be shown that coping with intentionally created difficulties differs in substantial ways from coping with the difficulties that arise due to natural causes beyond anyone’s control. Positive psychology can probably recommend a rape victim how to cope with what happened and to go on living happily, but it does not offer any recommendations on what to do if gangsters raid your village regularly and beatings, burglaries and violence continue with the connivance of those whose duty it is to protect you. The possible advice “go to the police” assumes the existence of institutions working towards positive goals. These are the kinds of institutions positive psychologists are interested in. Deviations in the work of these institutions (of which gangster raids are evidence) do not interest positive psychologists and indeed some of them consider excessive and lamentable the very fact that these deviations engage the minds of “ordinary psychologists”. Interestingly, in the rare cases when positive psychologists write not about the slings and arrows that stand in the way of happiness in general, but about a clash with active evil, the instances of coping they describe all share one feature. In these examples people do not confront evil, but do something about themselves, i.e., find a distraction. “Alexander Solzhenitsyn describes how one of his fellow prisoners in the Lefortovo jail mapped the world on the floor of the cell, and then imagined himself traveling across Asia and Europe to America, covering a few kilometers each day. <…> An acquaintance who worked in United States Air Force intelligence tells the story of a pilot who was imprisoned in North Vietnam for many years, and lost eighty pounds and much of his health in a jungle camp. When he was released, one of the first things he asked for was to play a game of golf. To the great astonishment of his fellow officers he played a superb game, despite his emaciated condition. To their inquiries he replied that every day of his imprisonment he imagined himself playing eighteen holes, carefully choosing his clubs and approach and systematically varying the course. This discipline not only helped preserve his sanity, but apparently also kept his physical skills well honed”. <…> “Solzhenitsyn describes very well how even the most degrading situation can be transformed into a flow experience: ‘Sometimes, when standing in a column of dejected prisoners, amidst the shouts of guards with 5 machine guns, I felt such a rush of rhymes and images that I seemed to be wafted overhead. . . . At such moments I was both free and happy. . . . Some prisoners tried to escape by smashing through the barbed wire. For me there was no barbed wire. The head count of prisoners remained unchanged but I was actually away on a distant flight’.” (Ibid, p. 104-106). After these descriptions, Csikszentmihalyi writes a very interesting conclusion to them. “Richard Logan proposes an answer based on the writings of many survivors, including those of Viktor Frankl and Bruno Bettelheim, who have reflected on the sources of strength under extreme adversity. He concludes that the most important trait of survivors is a ‘nonself-conscious individualism’, or a strongly directed purpose that is not self-seeking. People who have that quality are bent on doing their best in all circumstances, yet they are not concerned primarily with advancing their own interests. Because they are intrinsically motivated in their actions, they are not easily disturbed by external threats. With enough psychic energy free to observe and analyze their surroundings objectively, they have a better chance of discovering in them new opportunities for action” (Ibid.). So, objective analysis and finding opportunities for action. But where are the actions that are supposed to be preceded by cerebration and flights of fantasy? We do not find any such examples or descriptions. To give a polemical edge to my argument, let me mention Air Force pilot Mikhail Devyatayev. He spent more than half a year in a Nazi prison camp during the Second World War and led a group of prison mates in seizing a German plane, found out how it worked within minutes (he too was going over it in his mind in advance, but it was not a game of golf but his actions in the unknown plane), managed to break away from pursuing planes and reach the friendly side with the mates (https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Mikhail_Devyatayev). Are people like Devyatayev much less interesting for M.Csikszentmihalyi than the airman who spent all the time he was in prison playing golf in his mind, was liberated by somebody and continued to play golf after he was freed? Perhaps М. Csikszentmihalyi has not heard about Devyatayev, but what is really surprising is that he seems to be unaware of any other similar cases that involved something more than flights of intellect. Similarly, Seligman does not cite any examples of mutual help that involves real resistance to violence. P.Wong takes the most balanced position. One of the provisions of his manifesto of radical positive psychology is bringing freedom to those who are in captivity and justice to those whose rights are infringed upon. Yet he too does not write anything about how this demand is likely to be perceived by the opposite side, those who bully and oppress and how they should be treated if they refuse to be persuaded and do not want to change. On the whole over the years there has been a sense that while positive psychologists know about resilience, they do not know about resistance or know about it so little that it is not worth writing about. However, as the international situation was changing (the early works that staked the claim for positive psychology as a trend in its own right appeared at the beginning of 2000s when the world was living through a favorable period) the approach of positive psychology also changed. From positive psychology of flourishing to positive military psychology The case for positive psychology at the early stages In his early texts laying down the groundwork for positive psychology Seligman posited it as a counterweight to what he called negative psychology. In a 2000 article he wrote: “I am first 6 going to discuss the notions of negative psychology and negative social science and contrast them to the notions of positive psychology and positive social science” (Seligman, 2000, p. 415). He argued that, for example, the political leaders of Florence in the 15th century, the richest country in Europe, decided to invest their profits not in building up military muscle, but in creating beauty. According to Seligman, the USA is living through a similar world historic period: it can either commit itself to defense or to creation, but not of art monuments, but a monument of a different kind, “humane scientific monument: positive psychology” (Ibid., p. 417). “Negative” psychology, Seligman argues, has a “medical”, “clinical” approach: it is overly concerned with depressions, schizophrenia, alcoholism and other deviations. Accordingly, people need a positive science which studies positive phenomena (Ibid., p. 342). “Sociology, political science, anthropology and economics are the proper home of such investigations, but these disciplines (like psychology) are also pervaded by the study of the disabling institutions, such as racism, sexism, Machiavellianism, monopolies and the like. These social sciences have been muckraking, discovering a good deal about the institutions that make life difficult and even insufferable. At their best, these social sciences tell us how to minimize these disabling conditions” (Seligman, 2002, p. 266). Thus, at the turn of the millennia Seligman was critical of the social sciences that studied various manifestations of deliberately perpetrated evil (racism, Machiavellianism, etc.) and methods of countering them as sciences engaged in “muckraking”, though today a positive attitude to the world is more relevant. However, over time the rhetoric and practice of positive psychology changed substantially. Current situation: flexible adaptation of positive psychology to war According to publicly available official information on the Martin Seligman Positive Psychology Center website (https://www.authentichappiness.sas.upenn.edu/learn/soldiers), in recent years the Center has been running a Comprehensive Soldier Fitness и Resilience Training program for US soldiers and officers in active military service. This is the most ambitious program in the history of psychology: since 2009 the Department of Defense allocated $120 million for it, about a million soldiers have taken the course and eventually every service man and woman will go through it (Friedman, Robbins, 2012). An entire issue of the journal American Psychologist published in 2011 was devoted to the program. Seligman is an invited editor and co-author of several articles, including the summing up article under the tell-tale title “Comprehensive Soldier Fitness and the Future of Psychology” (Seligman, Fowler, 2011). Let us look at its content. The article is very important. It provides a brief history of military psychology in the USA, sets immediate tasks and makes longer-term forecasts. To begin with, it has to be stressed that Seligman casts aside his former critical attitude to preoccupation with sickness and, together with his co-author writes that the project has been prompted by battle fatigue afflicting many soldiers, the high rate of post-traumatic syndrome cases and other pathologies. It is worth recalling that here he reproduces the “medical” logic for which he criticized “negative” psychologists in his capacity of a progressive “positive” psychologist. The article also sets some basic value benchmarks. The authors write that American soldiers carry our government missions implementing the will of the nation. It would be wrong to deny scientific and professional support to the military who defend the nation. The American Psychological Association has never denied them such support. Seligman and Fowler write that the methods of psychological training they propose for the army are based on the principles of positive psychology. They catalog the objections of critics to the program and respond to criticisms. 7 One of the main critical objections goes like this: “Psychology should do no harm: Aiding the military will make people who kill for a living feel better about killing and help them do a better job of it” (Ibid., p. 86). The answer is the following. “If we had discovered a way of preventing malaria—mosquito netting, draining swamps, quinine—and our soldiers were fighting in a malaria-infested theater, would these voices also counsel withholding our discoveries? We would not withhold our help: The balance of good done by building the physical and mental fitness of our soldiers far outweighs any harm that might be done… Three ideologies have arisen in the past century that have sought to overthrow democracy by force: fascism, communism, and jihadist Islam. It should be noted that without a strong military and the will to use force responsibly in self-defense, our victories would not have happened, and defense against current and future threats would be impossible. Psychology materially aided in the defeat of the first two threats, and in doing so it carved out its identity. We are proud to aid our military in defending and protecting our nation right now, and we will be proud to help our soldiers and their families into the peace that will follow” (Ibid., p. 86). Let us now pass on to analysis. In the passages quoted Seligman in fact reveals to what extent he believes in the benevolence of the world and in win-win games for all the participants at the current stage: protection from current and future threats is impossible without practical use of weapons and the military. This is perfectly true. But the surprising thing about Seligman’s earlier texts is precisely the clever way in which he sidesteps the topic of self-protection of the happy and prosperous from negatively-minded actors. The reality is that if you are attacked by murderers you often have to kill the murderers. If thugs regularly raid your village, something has to be done about it. What is interesting, though, is not just military psychology, but the military psychology which preens itself on being positive. When reading Seligman one gets a (certainly false) impression that he, the author of texts on positive military psychology, seems to be only dimly aware of the fact that the decision-making logic in the battlefield is different from the ordinary logic and that the price even of absolutely correct decisions is totally different. There people kill people. Even if there is positive thinking it is of a very different, peculiar kind. Let us start explaining the special conditions of decision making in the battle-field with examples of a type which for some reason is not encountered in positive military psychology. Physiologist O.G.Gazenko, who served as chief of an army sick bay during the Second World War described the following problem. “What should a doctor on the frontline do about the wounded if the situation on that section of the front is very hard? If I retire all the wounded and send then to the rear, the enemy will break the front and both the wounded and all the rest will die. So, if a guy is wounded in the arm I’ll treat the wound and send him to hospital. If a guy is wounded in the leg I’ll leave him to man the machine-gun. I cannot relieve him. It’s a huge risk: he may develop a sepsis and die. … (Besides, a soldier with a leg wound cannot maneuver during the battle, which increases the risks – A.P.) This is the price of preventing the worst from happening, in other words, we are dealing with a problem with an indefinite question, and the answer is as follows: if I act in one way I win this and that at such and such a price. The doctor must each time consider the price.” (quoted from Feigenberg, 2009). 8 It has to be stressed that a military psychologist who conducts all-round psychological training of servicemen must prepare the commanders and doctors for taking decisions like the one described above and the soldiers for obeying the orders in such situations. Likewise, in military confrontations the commander may decide to sacrifice part of the unit (for example, the group covering retreat) to fulfill the mission and save the other part of the unit. (This situation underlies one of L. Kohlberg’s moral dilemmas that diagnose the level of moral development.) Servicemen must be psychologically trained to handle such situations. Finally, the most general problem is described by V.Druzhinin: “Most of those who saw action admit that it is extremely difficult to kill an enemy face-to-face. Whatever ethologists, the followers of K.Lorenz, may say, there is the notorious psychological barrier that holds back an individual from killing a representative of his own species. It is another question that this barrier may be broken” (Druzhinin, 2002, p. 118). It is still unclear how these and similar problems can solved in military psychology which is positive. And they have to be solved because even refusing to address them is a conscious solution. Is positive military psychology possible? This is an existential question. As far as I know Seligman himself tactfully avoids using the collocation “positive military psychology” which is uncomfortably reminiscent of Lem’s phrase “association of forces of public good-heartedness” (Lem, 1990). Lem emphasized grotesque of such collocations which hide realities very far from good-heartedness. But I see no other way of referring to military psychology based on positive principles than “positive military psychology”. One can well imagine separately positive psychology (psychology of happiness and flourishing) and military psychology. One can imagine an article titled “Military Psychology in the Terrorist Attack at … (follows the place name)” and “Genocide and Military Thinking”, but it is hard to imagine articles titled “Psychology of Happiness and Flourishing in the Terrorist Act at…” or “Genocide and Positive Thinking.” Whereas a number of texts on positive psychology avoid mentioning unpleasant things positive military psychology studiously avoids mentioning the attitude to the enemy. Even when articles on positive military psychology are devoted to the attitude to another person they consider relationship within an in-group, the attitudes of acceptance, empathy, respect, trust, openness, tolerance etc., etc. within the unit, but not the attitude to the members of an out-group, the adversary, the concrete enemies with regard to whom the authors have formed a conspiracy of silence (see, for example, Cacioppo et al., 2011). But then the question arises, why have all the soldiers gathered in the battle zone? To demonstrate empathy for each other? All these are existential questions. Murder corresponds to an extreme negative attitude (with the exception of very few cases of euphanasia). Can the attitude to the enemy be positive in the context of military actions and during the course of the actual battle? How can one have a positive attitude to someone seen through the gunsight just before firing, to someone observed on the screen before launching a missile, etc.? It bears repeating that even if there is something positive about these situations, it is positive in a very special way. It can safely be said that victory in war is achieved to a large extent due to the blend, on the one hand, of a positive optimistic attitude to the world, and on the other hand, of a sharply negative attitude (not stopping short of physically destroying ) the enemies, the murderers who are destroying the world. Such positive psychology of happiness for the oppressed is a psychology of unhappiness for the enemies. It would not be irrelevant to quote S.Lem once more: “Of course, making it impossible to cause harm is itself evil for many people, those who are very unhappy unless others are unhappy. But let them be unhappy” (Lem, 1990, p. 247). However, do we find analysis and tips on how to form this kind of attitude to the other in Seligman’s positive military psychology? The attitude need not necessarily take the shape of “I love bloody battles” or “the science of hatred.” The position may be more detached emotionally. But in any case it is not entirely positive with regard to the enemy. Perhaps the closest approximation to a real positive military psychology is exemplified by Anka the Machine Gunner in the Soviet film “Chapayev” (by Vasilyev brothers, 1934) 9 (https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Chapaev_(film) ). In one of the key episodes this positive character mows down with bursts of machine-gun fire the rows of advancing White Guard troops – she does it with skill and concentration and is in the state of flow – described by Csikszentmihalyi but, for some reason, not by Seligman. Let me elaborate. I respect positive science which helps to make the positive feelings of a happy and prosperous individual still more positive. I respect military psychology and consider that military psychologists are necessary. Moreover, reading extracts from the reactions of the sergeants Seligman and his co-authors (Reivich, Seligman, McBride, 2011) quote I am prepared to believe that resilience training is a very relevant program of military psychological training. However, Friedman and Robbins do not believe in its relevance and argue that the consequences of the program could be quite amoral notwithstanding all the declarations because it ignores the ethical element and besides, has no scientific grounding. They analyze its fundamental flaws of the ethical and scientific character (Friedman, Robbins, 2012). They focus in particular on the very concept of resilience. In the opinion of Friedman and Robbins, Seligman wrongly elevates it to the rank of a virtue out of context and without any connection to other qualities. A vivid example is the resilience of Hitler that he displayed over much of his life. The authors stress that this is an extreme example, but it has to be understood that the concept of resilience of a soldier (they cite an army field manual) carries a different meaning than the concept of resilience used in most psychological writings (on the study of childhood traumas that need to be healed, etc.). The key meaning of a soldier’s resilience in accordance with the quoted US Army Field Manual is fulfilling the combat mission in spite of any difficulties and obstacles. The authors point out that training resilience in this way – without a clearly formulated ethical element – may lead to disaster. For all that, Friedman and Robbins express their respect for the US Army and believe that an open dialogue on critically important issues is necessary if optimum results are to be achieved (Ibid.) A detailed historical analysis of the conceptualization of the phenomenon of resilience in psychology and analysis of the various ways in which it is used is also provided by P.Kessman et al. (2012a). These authors subscribe to the opinion of Friedman and Robbins that decontextualization of the concept is extremely dangerous from the moral point of view. In general, the question is about how to sit on two stools – positive psychology of happiness and military psychology. Let us sum up the interim results. With the change of the social and political situation the branch of positive psychology headed by Seligman has managed to: • flexibly and almost imperceptibly revert to the main thrust of the “clinical model” which relies on too many cases of deviation; • turn to the work (war) which according to Seligman should be considered dirty work; it will be recalled that he criticized the former “pre-positive” social sciences which “have been muckraking, discovering a good deal about the institutions that make life difficult and even insufferable» (Seligman, 2002, p. 266); • quietly introduce into the study of a happy and flourishing individual the study of an individual engaged in physical elimination of enemies and calling for special psychological preparation, support and subsequent rehabilitation. Conclusion Seligman writes: “Lying awake at night, you probably ponder, as I have, how to go from plus two to plus seven in your life, not just how to go from minus five to minus three and feel a little less miserable day by day” (Seligman, 2013, p. 9). Granted, going from +2 to +7 is a worthy task. But how easy, if at all possible, is it to go from +2 to +7 without analyzing the situation of transition from 0 до -50 (an estimated number of girls, established from the number of bodies found, killed by a Russian gang for refusing to become 10 prostitutes), or transition from 0 до -937 000 (official number of victims of genocide in Rwanda) etc.? As S.Lem writes, “he who studies human being cannot exclude from the train of being mass homicide without renouncing his calling.” (Lem, 1990, p. 448). K.Benson argues that the psychological and the moral are inseparably bound up and that the key feature of the human Self is the ability of purposive and conscious work to expand and develop human worlds as well as their purposeful amoral constriction and destruction. Human psychology cannot be explained without that capacity (Benson, 2001). If one goes along with that, one has to admit that the psychology of happiness and flourishing, if it claims to cognize the full reality, and to help all the needy and the miserable, should inevitably engage and perhaps directly deal with those who bring suffering and death and represent malignant aggression--according to Erich Fromm or “actively attacking evil” (Prokofiev, 2008, 2009), and those who live in accordance with the “life against life” principle, according to V.N.Druzhinin. It is worth repeating P.Wong to the effect that “evil may destroy individuals and societies like cancer cells” (Wong, 2011, p. 77). Speaking about the positions of P.Wong, I.Boniwell and some other scholars, one should emphasize the following. Back in 2005 B.Held noted the emergence of “the second wave” of positive psychologists who are more oriented towards integration than division of psychology into positive and negative (Held, 2005). Apparently, I.Boniwell belongs to this second wave because she quotes the vehement criticism of the opponents of positive psychology (Lazarus, 2003a, 2003b), (Tennen, Affleck, 2003) and agrees with them on the need to synthesize and integrate positive and negative psychology and what they know about people (Boniwell, 2009). Some scholars also draw attention to the fact that modern positive psychology is increasingly differentiating into various subareas of study. Some of these sub-areas are increasingly concerned with suffering, resilience and positive coping under difficult conditions (Leontiev, 2012; Hart, Sasso, 2011). The polemic with positive psychology started by R.Lazarus and B.Held in the early and mid-2000s, continues. Reports are delivered at conferences (Kessman et al., 2012b), methodological articles of external critics are published (Friedman and Robbins) which see positive psychology as a simplified but successfully marketed version of the humanist psychology, as well as articles by internal (Wong) critics. Friedman, Robbins and Wong agree on several key points: positive psychology ignores the holistic approach, simplifies reality and the virtues it identifies may have a destructive character. However, as P.Wong briefly notes, the problem of positive psychology is that if it takes up the problems of suffering (and the problem of “negative individuals” who bring suffering), it would lose its identity. One must give due to P.Wong as a positive psychologist: he tries not to turn to his theme of “negative traits” that lead to “negative outcomes,” more than he can help, merely pointing out that this is not the subject area of positive psychology. On the whole, the attitudes of the representatives of the psychology of happiness and flourishing to premeditated evil and those who are committed to it can be divided into several types (the list may be incomplete, but it gives an idea of the issue) 1. Re-educating the enemies and ignoring those of them who refuse to be re-educated (“The best way to get rid of war and warheads is to learn to live in paradise and to teach your enemies to do the same… If the enemies are so foolish that they do not want to live in paradise, so much the worse for them, though it’s a pity” (Linde, 2009). One can be quite happy in spite of them. 2. The understanding that “you cannot live a healthy and fulfilling life in a sick world contaminated by crime, corruption, injustice, oppression, and poverty” (Wong, 2011, p. 77). The theme of studying these negative phenomena which make complete well-being impossible, is not discussed because it is beyond the realm of positive psychology and may dilute its identity. 3. A pragmatic attitude to formulating and discussing the problem of evil based on the flexibility of practical intellect. To wit: in conditions of well-being the value is declared of win-win games and no interest is shown in the dynamics in the non-positive numbers zone (only transitions from positive to still more positive numbers are considered). Whenever the socio-political and economic situation takes a turn for the worse the rhetoric quickly switches to global threats, a 11 reversal to the clinical model and work with professional fighters against evil who physically destroy the carriers of threats – but in the framework of advanced positive psychology, the monument of the psychology of the future. No psychological analysis of the structure of the activity to destroy the enemy or analysis of its central relationship – the attitude to the enemy – seems to be conducted. Equally, no analysis is offered of the goals, relations and strategies of the subjects opposing “our” fighters. Yet such analysis is necessary if the struggle is to be effective. More precisely, such problems are unlikely to be ignored during the course of actual training, but they are not discussed in publications on positive psychology because such discussion would unpleasantly dilute the identity of the positive psychology. Meanwhile coping with the difficulties deliberately created by the enemy (most of which in a war are of this kind) differs dramatically from cooping with the difficulties that arise due to natural causes beyond people’s control. Even if we work only with people who are coping with difficulties, in order to understand the features of coping and behavior as a whole, one has to understand the features of the difficulties created for these people, and understand the behavior of the one who creates them – to understand even if this was not the initial task of positive work (Poddiakov, 2008, 2011). It would therefore be interesting to trace how the studies in positive psychology which are, to use Wong’s expression, spreading like a bush fire, will raise, discuss and solve (or otherwise) the problems of interaction with the proponents of aggressive evil. In his book Variants of Life: Essays in Existential Psychology V.N. Druzhinin wrote: “There are three roles worthy of a human: the role of savior, protector and creator. The creator, constructor, worker, artist, scientist, pupil and teacher, mother and father, a friend reproduce and renew life. The protector, policeman, solider, fireman and watchman protect life from external threats. The savior, doctor, psychologist and the priest prolong physical and spiritual life. In the duel with destructive aggression and meaningless existence, the sole meaning of individual being is not illusory: it is extending the life of humanity” (Druzhinin, 2000, pp. 133-134). But the role of defender against destructive aggression often implies alternative altruism (Poddiakov, 2007): humaneness with regard to some (the protected) at the expense of inhumanity with regard to others (the aggressors), inhumanity because killing a human being is inhumane with regard to that human (and in extreme cases we are talking about killing). Alternative altruism may also include inhumanity with regard to oneself and other members of an in-group. The fight against terror, crime, selfless defense of another person against non-physical and physical violence, etc. are examples of such activities. The question is, will the psychology of happiness and well-being be able to forego these manifestations of humanity in order not to dilute its identity or will it preserve and even develop them? References Benson, C. (2001). The cultural psychology of self: place, morality and art in human worlds. London; NY: Routledge, Taylor and Francis Group. Boniwell I. (2009). [Keys for well-being: what positive psychology can do.] Moscow: Vremya. (Russian). Cacioppo, J.T., Reis, H.T., & Zautra, A. J. (2011). Social resilience: the value of social fitness with an application to the military. American Psychologist. Vol. 66(1), 43-51. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2013). Flow: The Psychology of Happiness. Random House eBooks. Druzhinin, V. N. (2000). [Variants of Life: Essays on Existential Psychology.] Moscow: Per Se. (Russian). Enikolopov, S.N. Psychology of evil. In A.L. Zhuravlev & A.V. Jurevich (Eds.), Psihologicheskie issledovanija duhovno-nravstvennyh problem. Moscow: Institut psihologii RAN. Pp. 308-335. (Russian). 12 Epstein, S. (1991). Cognitive-experiential self-theory: An integrative theory of personality. In R. Curtis (Ed.), The relational self: convergences in psychoanalysis and social psychology. NY: Guilford Press. Pp. 111-137. Fejgenberg, I. (2009). [Memoires]. Nezavisimyj psihiatricheskij zhurnal. No. 2. http://www.npar.ru/journal/2009/2/feinverg.htm. (Russian). Friedman, H.L., & Robbins, B.D. (2012). The negative shadow cast by positive psychology: Contrasting views and implications of humanistic and positive psychology on resilience. The Humanistic Psychologist. Vol. 40(1), 87-102. Hart, K.E., & Sasso, T. (2011). Mapping the contours of contemporary positive psychology. Canadian Psychology. Vol. 52(2), 82–92. Held, B.S. (2004). The negative side of positive psychology. Journal of Humanistic Psychology. Vol. 44(1), 9-46. http://www.bowdoin.edu/faculty/b/bheld/pdf/JHP-held-2004.pdf. Held, B.S. (2005). The “virtues” of positive psychology. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology. Vol. 25(1), 1-34. Kessman, P., Howe, K., Arcangeli, B., & Goodman, D. (2012a). “Resilient” terminology: a genealogy of the wellness literature. Paper presented at the Fifth Annual Humanistic Psychology Conference of the Society for Humanistic Psychology, Division 32 of the American Psychological Association. March 29 - April 1, 2012. Pittsburgh, PA, USA. Kessman, P., Howe, K., & Goodman, D.M. (2012b). Sifting through the “Good Life”: The Downside of Positive Psychology. Paper presented at the Third Biennial Midwinter Meeting Society for Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology. March 1–3, 2012. Austin, Texas, USA. Lazarus, R.S. (2003a). Does the positive psychology movement have legs? Psychological Inquiry. Vol. 14(2), 93-109. Lazarus, R.S. (2003b). The Lazarus Manifesto for positive psychology and psychology in general. Psychological Inquiry. Vol. 14(2), 173-189. Linde, N. (2009). [Sutra on Happiness]. http://www.voppsy.ru/Linde.htm. (Russian). Lee, R.M. (1995). Dangerous fieldwork. London: Sage. Lem, S. (1990). [Inspection at the Scene of the Crime.] In: Lem, S. [Iz vospominaniy Iyona Tihogo]. Moscow: Knizhnaya palata. (Russian). Leontiev, D.A. [Remarks during the course of the debate “The Problem of Evil and Positive Psychology: Alexander Poddiakov vs Dmitry Leontiev”]. Moscow, NRU HSE. 18 May 2012. URL: http://video.hse.ru/video/925. (Russian). Nazaretyan, A. (2006). Violence and non-violence at different stages of world history: A view from the hypothesis of techno-humanitarian balance. In Leonid Grinin, Victor C. de Munck & Andrey Korotayev (Eds.), History & Mathematics: Analyzing and Modeling Global Development. Moscow: URSS. Pp. 127–148. http://cliodynamics.ru/index.php?option=com_content&task=view&id=130&Itemid=70. Poddiakov, A. (2007). [Alter-altruism]. Psychology. Journal of the Higher School of Economics. Vol. 4(3), 98–107. http://psy-journal.hse.ru/en/2007-4-3/26978281.html. (Russian, abstr. in English). Poddiakov, A. (2008). Deliberate creation of difficulties and their coping. Psikhologicheskie Issledovaniya, 1(1). http://psystudy.ru/index.php/eng/2008n1-1e/95-poddiakov1e.html (Russian, abstr. in English). Poddiakov, A. (2011). [Complicology—study of creating difficulties: from biological mechanisms to moral reasoning]. In A.L. Zhuravlev & A.V. Jurevich (Eds.), Psihologicheskie issledovanija duhovno-nravstvennyh problem. Moscow: Institut psihologii RAN. Pp. 435-479. http://www.hse.ru/data/2011/11/21/1272684575/complicology.doc. (Russian). Prokofiev, A.V. (2008). Kant, lie, and violence… Logos, 5, 60-90. (Russian). Prokofiev, A.V. (2009). [Choosing the Lesser Evil and the Problem of the Limits of the Morally Admissible]. Eticheskaya mysl: Yearbook. Moscow. RAS Institute of Philosophy. Vol. 9, pp. 122-145. (Russian) 13 Reivich, K.J., Seligman, M.E.P., & McBride, S. (2011). Master Resilience Training in the U.S. Army. American Psychologist. Vol. 66(1), 25-34. Seligman, M.E.P. (2000). Positive psychology. In J.E. Gillham (Ed.), The Science of Optimism and Hope: Research Essays in Honor of Martin E.P. Seligman. Radnor, PA: Templeton Foundation Press. Pp. 415-430. Seligman, M.E.P. (2002). Authentic happiness. NY: Free Press. Seligman, M.E.P. (2013). Authentic happiness. NY: Atria Paperback. Seligman, M.E.P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist. Vol. 55 (1), 5-14. Seligman, M.E.P., & Fowler, R.D. (2011). Comprehensive Soldier Fitness and the Future of Psychology. American Psychologist. Vol. 66(1), 82-86. Servan-Schreiber, D. (2009). Anti-cancer: A new way of life. New York: Viking. Tennen, H., & Affleck, G. (2003).While accentuating the positive, don't eliminate the negative or Mr. In-Between. Psychological Inquiry. Vol. 14 (2), 163-169. Werner, H. (2004). Comparative psychology of mental development. NY: Percheron Press. Wong, P.T.P. (2007). Radical positive psychology for radical times. Кeynote address at the International Council of Psychologists. San Diego, August 13, 2007. http://www.meaning.ca/archives/archive/art_radicalPP2_P_Wong.htm. Wong, P.T.P. (2011). Positive psychology 2.0: towards a balanced interactive model of the good life. Canadian Psychology. Vol 52 (2), 69–81. Zabielski, S. (2007). Deception and self-deception in qualitative research. Paper presented at the conference “Psychology of the coping behavior”. May 16-18, 2007. Kostroma, Russia. View publication stats