Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e HOW TO WRITE 8 Psychology RESEARCH REPORTS AND ESSAYS Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e In memory of Associate Professor Jennifer Boldero (1955–2017) In recognition of your great contribution to research and teaching in psychology in Australia. Your passion and dedication inspired, supported, and educated hundreds of psychologists, psychology graduates, and one daughter. Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Findlay HOW TO WRITE Kaufmann 8 Psychology RESEARCH REPORTS AND ESSAYS Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 Pearson Australia 707 Collins Street Melbourne VIC 3008 www.pearson.com.au The Copyright Act 1968 of Australia allows a maximum of one chapter or 10% of this book, whichever is the greater, to be copied by any educational institution for its educational purposes provided that that educational institution (or the body that administers it) has given a remuneration notice to Copyright Agency Limited (CAL) under the Act. For details of the CAL licence for educational institutions contact: Copyright Agency Limited, telephone: (02) 9394 7600, email: info@copyright.com.au All rights reserved. Except under the conditions described in the Copyright Act 1968 of Australia and subsequent amendments, no part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, without the prior permission of the copyright owner. Senior Portfolio Manager: Stephen Heasley Sponsoring Editor: Simone Bella Development Editor: Chakira Lane Project Manager: Bronwyn Smith Production Managers: Bernadette Chang and Amandeep Kaur Content Developer: Stephen Razos Rights and Permissions Team Leader: Lisa Woodland Production Controller: Yijing Wu Lead Editor/Copy Editor: Marie-Louise Taylor Proofreaders: iEnergizer Aptara®, Ltd and Maryanne Phillips Indexer: iEnergizer Aptara®, Ltd Cover and internal design by Lamond Art & Design Cover illustration © Kraphix/Shutterstock Typeset by iEnergizer Aptara®, Ltd Printed in Malaysia 1 2 3 4 5 23 22 21 20 19 ISBN: 9781488618437 ISBN: 9781488618444 (eBook) Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd ABN 40 004 245 943 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Contents Preface����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������viii Introduction���������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������ix How to Use this Book�����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������xii Acknowledgments����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������xiv 1 Reasons for Writing Research Reports and Essays���������������������������� 1 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 2 Writing Research Reports and Essays – Getting Started ������������������� 7 2.1 2.2 2.3 2.4 2.5 2.6 2.7 2.8 2.9 2.10 3 Why Write Reports and Essays? ��������������������������������������������������������������������2 What Is a Research Report?����������������������������������������������������������������������������3 Writing Essays at University ��������������������������������������������������������������������������5 Chapter Summaries�����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������5 Collaborating Not Copying – The Distinction ����������������������������������������������8 Information Gathering�����������������������������������������������������������������������������������9 Evaluating Internet Resources ���������������������������������������������������������������������15 Critical Thinking �����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������16 Critically Evaluating Psychology Journal Articles ���������������������������������������18 Writing Style ������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������21 Using the Correct Tense��������������������������������������������������������������������������������27 Inclusive Language ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������28 Proofreading and Redrafting ������������������������������������������������������������������������28 The Physical Presentation of Your Assignment��������������������������������������������30 Referencing��������������������������������������������������������������������������������� 33 3.1 Plagiarism�����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������34 3.2 In-text Referencing ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������36 3.3 The Reference List ���������������������������������������������������������������������������������������42 3.3.1 Books������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������44 3.3.2 Journal Articles ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������45 3.3.3 Internet Sources��������������������������������������������������������������������������������47 3.3.4 Other Sources�����������������������������������������������������������������������������������48 v Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 4 Research Reports – How Do You Start? ������������������������������������������ 51 4.1 How to Approach a Research Report �����������������������������������������������������������53 4.2 How to Begin������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������54 4.2.1 Studies Designed by the Psychology Department�����������������������������54 4.2.2 Studies You Design���������������������������������������������������������������������������57 4.2.3 All Studies – A Brief Introduction to Key Concepts��������������������������58 4.3 Research Ethics ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������60 4.4 Where to Go From Here ������������������������������������������������������������������������������62 5 Sections of a Research Report ������������������������������������������������������ 67 5.1 Title Page and Abstract ��������������������������������������������������������������������������������68 5.2 Introduction��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������72 5.3 Method���������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������77 5.3.1 Participants���������������������������������������������������������������������������������������80 5.3.2 Materials��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������81 5.3.3 Design�����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������85 5.3.4 Procedure �����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������86 5.4 Results�����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������87 5.4.1 Your Very First Report�����������������������������������������������������������������������87 5.4.2 All Reports�����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������89 5.4.3 Numbers and Words ������������������������������������������������������������������������92 5.4.4 Decimals��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������93 5.4.5 Tables �����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������93 5.4.6 Figures����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������97 5.5 Discussion���������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������100 5.6 References���������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������105 5.7 Appendices�������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������105 5.8 Further Reading������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������106 5.9 Feedback����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������107 5.10 Summary����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������108 5.11 Checklist ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������108 6 Essays in Psychology ����������������������������������������������������������������� 111 6.1 6.2 6.3 6.4 How Are Essays Different From Research Reports? ���������������������������������113 Choosing an Essay Topic ���������������������������������������������������������������������������113 Deciding What the Essay Topic Asks���������������������������������������������������������114 Preparation – Reading, Note-taking, and Planning������������������������������������117 vi Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Contents 6.5 Structure of an Essay ���������������������������������������������������������������������������������118 6.5.1 The Title Page and Abstract������������������������������������������������������������119 6.5.2 The Introduction����������������������������������������������������������������������������119 6.5.3 The Body (Discussion) ������������������������������������������������������������������121 6.5.4 The Conclusion������������������������������������������������������������������������������122 6.5.5 The References �������������������������������������������������������������������������������124 6.6 Redrafting and Presentation������������������������������������������������������������������������124 6.7 Further Reading������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������125 6.8 Checklist ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������126 Appendix A Appendix B Appendix C Appendix D Sample of a Good Research Report�������������������������������������������������������129 Sample of a Poor Research Report���������������������������������������������������������139 Sample of a Good Research Report�������������������������������������������������������151 Sample of a Poor Research Report���������������������������������������������������������165 Glossary������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������175 References���������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������181 Index ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������187 vii Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Preface The Australian Psychological Society (APS) is pleased to continue its association with the publication of Bruce Findlay and Leah Kaufmann’s How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays, now in its 8th edition. Undergraduate psychology education aims to develop psychological literacy. Knowledge of research methods, critical thinking skills, and communication skills are valued competencies of psychology graduates. Undergraduate research projects and assignments are designed to help students master these skills; yet first- and ­second-year students may find writing research reports and essays daunting. Findlay and Kaufmann’s book is designed to make these learning tasks less challenging for students. It uses easy-to-read and student-friendly language. The book is intended to complement undergraduate textbooks and the current edition of the Publication Manual of the American Psychology Association (APA). The book will help students to plan and ­organise their research report, to express their ideas clearly, and to follow APA style. The book also alerts students to key issues, such as referencing sources correctly and avoiding plagiarism. It now includes information on electronic referencing, online sources, and submissions. Flowcharts, checklists, and sample reports provide further guidance on ­psychological writing. There are now additional examples for postgraduate students. I am pleased to recommend the new edition of this book as a valuable resource to students throughout their undergraduate studies. Students interested in a career in ­psychology are invited to explore the APS website and our resources for students, including Psych Student Headquarters (www.psychology.org.au). The APS website also shows the diversity of careers in psychology, opportunities to network, and resources provided for students and student subscribers by the APS. You can subscribe to the APS by visiting https://join.psychology.org.au. Sabine Wingenfeld Hammond, PhD, FAPS Executive Manager, Science, Education, and Membership Australian Psychological Society viii Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Introduction Introduction to Students If you’ve picked this up to see what it’s like – buy it! It’ll save you a lot of hassles in your undergraduate psychology career. While most psychology students do not go on to become the sort of psychologists who publish journal articles, most psychology departments treat you as if you will! For example, you will be expected to write research reports that differ from typical assignments and essays, and although they are not as long or complex as those an academic would write, they are designed to lead you towards that level. This book will give you all the information you need about how to prepare and write psychology research reports and essays in psychology at the undergraduate level, without overburdening you with all those extra touches that are needed for publication. There is a glossary of those words that are on the tip of your tongue but whose meaning escapes you at the moment. There is an index that will lead you to the help you need for any particular feature of a research report or essay that is bothering you. There are flowcharts of the processes involved in writing a research report and an essay. There are a couple of good examples of research reports that you can use as a sort of graphical index, and a couple of examples of badly written reports to show you what to avoid. If you want some global advice for undergraduate writing in psychology, it is – learn to plan ahead! It will save you a lot of anxiety of the sort that occurs when your computer fails at midnight and the assignment is due at 9 o’clock the next morning. This book will show you how to learn this valuable skill. Good writing! It’s not nearly as difficult as you might think. ix Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Introduction to Staff Members Purpose of the book. This book is intended to address the need for a set of guidelines for writing undergraduate-level psychology research reports and essays. It is aimed at first- and second-year students, although it may be useful to address weaknesses in preparation, writing, or even submitting assignments among more senior students. It is not, however, a guide to writing for publication. It does observe the conventions required by the 6th edition of the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (APA), and it includes the rationale for those conventions, as well as the steps to be taken in producing and presenting psychological research reports and essays. A student who grasps these concepts and learns the conventions will have a sound basis for presenting research in a professional manner, and writing well-argued essays, so that the later step of writing for publication should be an easier one to take. Why such a book is desirable. Increasing student numbers and dwindling resources (especially, face-to-face teaching opportunities) in Australian universities often result in students spending less time in classes on the more basic skills and approaches to writing research reports and essays. It can also mean students get less guidance on this (assessed) aspect of their learning and are less able to understand or make use of feedback on what they have written. This is particularly true at first-year level. Many psychology departments produce handouts on writing research reports and essays which, however brief, are time consuming to develop, and are a further burden (beyond core content development) on the demands of increasingly burdened academics. They may also be produced by staff members who have been writing for publication for some time and who may take for granted a good deal of information that first-year students, in particular, do not know. For this reason, many students find the conventions of psychological report writing especially difficult, since the requirements differ both from those of their other tertiary subjects and from their own previous experience. Moreover, when students are left to seek answers to very basic or seemingly obvious questions (which they can be too embarrassed to ask of staff members), they often find ad hoc or inaccurate answers (e.g., from the internet, other students, or even less experienced staff), and this can lead to complaints from students about what is expected in research reports. Students will find this book useful because it is considerably more detailed than the typical departmental handout, but not as overwhelming as books advising on writing for publication, such as the Publication Manual of the APA. Some of the more advanced books are mentioned towards the end of Chapters 5 and 6. Difference from the previous edition. Since the 6th edition of the Publication Manual of the APA was published in 2010, publishing in general has continued to change, especially in the accessibility and presentation of electronic resources. While the 6th edition of the Publication Manual updated some conventions to meet the changes in publishing, especially the use of the doi, the APA has also published, electronically, a more detailed style guide to electronic references. The latest edition of the current book includes some x Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Introduction of this material. In addition, we have updated guidance on issues like online submission and the use of online sources (e.g., online journals) and tools (e.g., Turnitin). The examples of opening sentences, summaries of prior research, and referencing examples are fairly recent material from Australian and New Zealand academics. We hope your students are tickled to see the names of academics they may know used as glowing examples still active at the time of writing, and relevant. The occasional URL used as examples have been updated to ensure they are still active and relevant. In response to requests from third- and fourth-year students, who say they are finding the book useful in their higher years, we have also included some examples of more sophisticated tables and figures and how they should be reported in text. Academics are encouraged to consider embedding and using this book in courses. It may save teaching time by recommending that students buy and frequently refer to this book. If you are using this book as an aid to your teaching and have any constructive criticisms to offer, especially about the newer sections, we would appreciate hearing from you. Bruce Findlay Senior Lecturer in Psychology Swinburne University of Technology PO Box 218, Hawthorn, Vic 3122 email: bfindlay@swin.edu.au Leah Kaufmann Senior Lecturer, School of Psychology Australian Catholic University Locked Bag 4115, Fitzroy MDC, Vic 3065 email: leah.kaufmann@acu.edu.au xi Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Use this Book This book has a number of design features which we hope will make it easier to understand the information you need to know. There are often good and poor examples cited. The examples will be in figures, because one of the conventions you need to learn is that illustrative material belongs in figures. The format of the figures and tables throughout this book is the format that you are expected to learn, but in addition there will be icons to remind you which are good and which are poor examples. They will look like this: Good examples for you to follow will have this “tick” beside them. Poor examples or formats that you should avoid using in your work will have this “cross” beside them. Notes, which will often accompany tables, will have this little “notepad” icon beside them. Please read them carefully! Finally, this icon will accompany comments or instructions that require even closer attention than usual. It will refer to things that are absolutely essential for you to be aware of. Please be aware that the examples throughout the book illustrating the conventions you need to know about are not exhaustive. You should not get the idea that these examples are the only way to express those particular conventions, but they are certainly acceptable ways. As you become more comfortable with writing in this style, you will be able to be more creative within the existing conventions. At the start of Chapters 4 and 6 there are flowcharts that indicate the sequence of activities for the successful writing of research reports and essays, respectively. These may serve as a useful summary and reminder of the process once you have familiarised yourself with the details. There are also checklists for research reports and essays, at the ends of Chapters 5 and 6 respectively, that will help you to plan and mark off your achievements in your first few assignments. Feel free to photocopy them and use them to measure your progress. There are good and bad examples of two research reports, starting on page 129. The first is a fairly straightforward one, such as you might expect in your first one or two assignments. The second is rather more complex, but you might encounter one like it late in first year or early in second year. They are not meant to be the only way you can write a research report, but they are acceptable ways; they can also be used as a sort of index, if you are looking for something but can’t think of the word that would allow you to use the normal index. You can look at the part of the report where you expect to find advice, then xii Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Use this Book be referred to the sections or pages where that advice can be found. The bad examples include many of the errors that students commonly make. Have a look at them, see if you can spot the errors, and check the answers on pages 147–149 and 172–174. At the beginnings of Chapters 2 to 6 there are lists of keywords. Before reading each chapter, look up these words in the Glossary at the back of the book. This will ensure that when you first encounter them in the context of the chapter they will already be a little familiar. Their meaning should become even clearer as you read through the chapter. Educator Resource – Test Bank The Test Bank provides a wealth of testing material. Updated for the new edition, each chapter offers a wide variety of question types, arranged by section. Questions can be integrated into Blackboard, Canvas or Moodle Learning Management Systems. xiii Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Acknowledgments Thanks to the many academics who adopted earlier versions of this book for their introductory psychology classes. Thanks also to the reviewers who made comments on the 7th edition. We have incorporated most of the changes they suggested. We are also very grateful to the many students who have expressed their appreciation for the earlier editions of this book, and whose comments and experiences have led us to clarify or expand some points. They more than offset the feeling we sometimes get that, although students are persuaded to buy the book, many seem not to pay attention to it. It’s many years ago now, but one first-year convenor set as a research report the topic used in previous editions as an example in the back of the book. The distribution of students’ marks, and the errors they made, suggested that a large number of students didn’t realise this! It may not be apparent to the casual reader, but the publications referred to as examples in reference lists, and from which we draw good examples of opening sentences, hypotheses, and summaries of prior research, are most frequently the work of Australasian psychology researchers. We are impressed by the number of publications produced by our academic colleagues, and the ease with which we can find good examples without looking beyond Australia and New Zealand. We like to think that we are reinforcing the inspiration they are to their students. At many universities, a high proportion of tutorial teaching, and therefore responsibility for instructions about, and marking of, research reports, is done by sessional staff, primarily higher degree students. We are continually impressed by, and grateful for, their dedication and professionalism. Similarly, we are blessed with congenial associates who offer advice in constructive ways. As ever, the responsibility for any remaining errors must be our own. In particular we are grateful to Ben Williams for his statistical and graphical advice, and to Aimee, Adam, Martin, Rose, Kelly, Ian, and Elisa, whose excellent reports allowed us to amalgamate them into the good report. xiv Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Reasons for Writing Research Reports and Essays 1 Reasons for Writing Research Reports and Essays IN THIS CHAPTER 1.1 Why Write Reports and Essays? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 1.2 What Is a Research Report? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3 1.3 Writing Essays at University . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 1.4 Chapter Summaries . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 1 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Reasons for Writing Research Reports and Essays How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 1.1 Why Write Reports and Essays? I N ANY SCIENTIFIC DISCIPLINE, the structure of established knowledge is, on rare occasions, shaken up by people of the stature of Einstein or Freud. More usually, knowledge is added to, brick by brick, by well-reported studies in some relatively specific area. When taken together, these all make up what we know – in the case of psychology – about how people think, feel, and behave. Hopefully, some of you will go on to contribute to this knowledge. This will involve considerable training in psychology, in research methods, and in publication. You may have begun psychology because you were interested in learning how to help other people, or perhaps in understanding yourself better, and so may not anticipate that you will publish research articles in your professional life. However, even if you don’t publish studies, you will have to read many published reports while completing your studies in psychology. Knowing the conventions and being able to critically evaluate these works will aid in this. Specifically, you will need to develop skills that allow you to decide whether the material you read is useful to your own area of work, as well as strengths and weaknesses within the work itself. You will need to understand how authors reached their conclusions, and to decide how sound or applicable those conclusions are, all of which require a familiarity with the conventions of psychological reporting. The majority of students who study an introductory psychology subject, or even complete a three-year degree with a major in psychology, do not go on to become psychologists. To become a psychologist, you will need to complete six years of training; that is, an accredited undergraduate major, a fourth year called an Honours year or a Postgraduate Diploma, and two more years which may be a Master’s degree, or professional supervision and training. In contrast, most students with an undergraduate degree in psychology find themselves working in management, human resources, human services departments, market research, journalism, the travel industry, or similar areas. This is because a degree with a major in psychology is well regarded in the commercial and industrial world, partly because of the experience of quantitative analysis that goes with writing research reports, but mainly because psychology graduates have had excellent training in the critical, disciplined, and concise reporting of the work they do. There are therefore three good reasons why you need to learn to write good research reports: • The first and most pragmatic reason is that you are expected to do so as part of an undergraduate degree in psychology and will be assessed on your ability to do so. It is something you need to know in order to complete your course successfully. • A better reason is that as a potential psychologist you need to appraise what other people have written and to report your own research in order to communicate to the community of scientists/practitioners and 2 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Reasons for Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays 1 academics what you have learned from your study of human, or perhaps animal, behaviour. • Finally, even if you do not intend to become a psychologist, any occupation will have its own conventions for preparing and writing reports of the work you do. Learning the conventions of psychology research reports demonstrates your ability to master the conventions of a discipline to potential employers. In addition, the generic skills of synthesising material requiring high levels of theoretical and conceptual understanding, as well as reporting your conclusions concisely and on time, are skills that will stand you in good stead in any professional career. Many undergraduate psychology students are also asked to write essays. In contrast to a research report, which expects you to describe a piece of empirical research, essays usually expect you to argue a point of view in some area of psychological interest. Essay writing is also a valued skill, since it also gives you an opportunity to demonstrate the generic skills of assembling and summarising relevant information, critically evaluating it, and coming to a considered conclusion about it in a succinct way. 1.2 What Is a Research Report? A research report (sometimes called a laboratory report, or lab report for short) is a summary of a research project, presented in a specific form including: (a) why you undertook this particular research; (b) what you expected to find; (c) how you actually did it; (d) what you did find; (e) what you think the results mean; and (f) the theoretical and practical implications of the conclusions you drew from those results. Like most disciplines, psychology has conventions for the reporting of research findings. This means that readers who are familiar with these conventions can quickly and easily find the information they need and evaluate the quality of this information. From there they can decide whether they want to read the report in more detail. As a result, the format of psychological reporting is very useful to readers and can help authors communicate with their audience. The reason that the conventions used in psychology conform to a “more or less” standard format is that the research presented differs in a number of important ways (e.g., number of studies, number and type of samples, use of apparatus, analyses), which results in slightly different information being presented. We encourage you to look through some of the major psychology periodicals in your library or browse through them on the web (and we encourage you to do so in the very near future), such as: Australian Journal of Psychology; Brain and Cognition; Child Development; Journal of Experimental Psychology; Journal of Personality and Social Psychology; or Psychological Science. You will find many articles in formats that are somewhat different to what is usually 3 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays taught in first- or second-year psychology, mostly as a result of more complex designs or methods than you encounter in your first couple of undergraduate years. Another reason is that a journal article expects a greater familiarity with existing research in the area than that expected of an undergraduate report, which means that some information may not be fully described, although you will do so in your early reports. However, by your third year you should be approaching a professional understanding of prior research, and your understanding of the conventions of presentation will conform more to the journal article standard. The qualitative difference between journal articles and undergraduate research reports is that the research report is primarily an exercise in communication and only secondarily, if at all, a contribution to research. It is important that the information you communicate to your marker reflects your knowledge and understanding of the topic. It is important that as an undergraduate you become expert at producing the standard format, and once you are completely in control of it then you can decide whether it is appropriate to “bend the rules” in particular cases. The basic rules are that a research report consists of the following sections in the sequence indicated: • • • • • • • • Title Page Abstract Introduction (section does not include this title) Method Results Discussion References Appendices (often not used in undergraduate reports) The Title Page clarifies what the report is about and contains information about you. The Abstract is an overview of the report. The rest of the report is the presentation of the full details of the research, using an hourglass structure (Kidder & Judd, 1986). Your Introduction should begin broadly, introducing the topic under study and why it is being studied. This will then begin to narrow down as you describe the work of previous researchers whose results have led to your own study. The end of the Introduction is like the approach to the waist of the hourglass, where you state the aims of your study and the specific hypotheses, which are predictions of what results you expected from your study. The Method and Results sections are the most specific sections of the report, since they state precisely what was done and what results were obtained. The Discussion is where the hourglass begins to spread out again. It starts with a description of results in relation to the hypotheses (i.e., were they supported?); your interpretation of the results; then broadens this to discuss how those results relate to previous work. Finally, you describe the implications of your research, including strengths and limitations, and finish at the broadest point with a general conclusion about the meaning and contribution of this research to the literature. The References allow others to check the accuracy of your assertions, and the Appendices contain additional material that doesn’t need to be included in the body of the report but is specifically referred to (e.g., in your Method). 4 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Reasons for Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays 1 1.3 Writing Essays at University An essay in psychology is no different in basic structure from the essays you will encounter in other tertiary humanities subjects, such as literature or social sciences like sociology. Unlike research reports, they are not usually divided into sections. They follow the standard essay pattern of having an introduction, a body or discussion, which is the bulk of the essay, and a conclusion. However, psychology essays may also need a Title Page, Abstract, and References. Essays at tertiary level are somewhat different from those many of you will have experienced at school. The basic difference is that at university you will be expected to be more critical of the material you use in your essays and systematic in your approach to creating an argument or viewpoint. Psychology essays typically ask you to discuss some research, to compare and contrast particular theoretical perspectives on some area of behaviour, or to critically evaluate some area of research. Chapter 6 describes what is required of each of these sorts of instructions, but in essence they expect you to read people’s work with a critical eye, and assess the strengths and weaknesses of a body of work. What is needed of you as a student with a major in psychology is that you demand particular standards of evidence for the assertions that researchers make before you give any weight to those assertions. This requires critical thinking on your part, and we describe some ways in which you can apply critical thinking to psychological articles in Chapter 2. 1.4 Chapter Summaries The following chapters will deal with: (a) where to look for appropriate references; information on critical evaluation of earlier research; and the process of producing an assignment, all of which are common to both research reports and essays (Chapter 2); (b) the expected conventions of referencing in text and in the Reference list (Chapter 3); (c) what you do to get started when assigned to write a research report (Chapter 4); (d) detailed instructions on what sort of material goes in which sections of a research report, and the conventions for presenting it (Chapter 5); and (e) advice about essay writing for psychology (Chapter 6). At the end of the book are examples of good and poor research reports, which you may find useful if looking at specific aspects, but can’t think of the word that will allow you to look it up in the normal index. 5 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e This page intentionally left blank. Writing Research Reports and Essays – Getting Started 2 Writing Research Reports and Essays – Getting Started KEYWORDS active voice, argument, assumptions, collaboration, copying, empirical, evidence, extraneous (nuisance) variable, factor, generic, hypothesis, methodology, operationalisation, organised scepticism, participant, plagiarism, problem (research problem), qualitative, questionnaire, reference, replication, representative, scientific study, study, theory IN THIS CHAPTER 2.1 Collaborating Not Copying – The Distinction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8 2.2 Information Gathering . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9 2.3 Evaluating Internet Resources . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15 2.4 Critical Thinking . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16 2.5 Critically Evaluating Psychology Journal Articles . . . . . . . . . . . . 18 2.6 Writing Style . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21 2.7 Using the Correct Tense . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 27 2.8 Inclusive Language . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 2.9 Proofreading and Redrafting . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 28 2.10 The Physical Presentation of Your Assignment . . . . . . . . . . . . . 30 7 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays T Writing Research Reports and Essays – Getting Started HERE ARE SEVERAL ELEMENTS and processes that are common to research reports and essays. Most notably, assignments that receive the best marks are those that not only follow the conventional format but are also written clearly and concisely. Importantly, get the best marks also meet the requirements of the instructions given, including word limits, and are submitted on time. This chapter addresses key issues in getting started: (a) the thorny problem of collaborating versus copying; (b) the question of the sort of material you should gather to support your research report or essay; (c) how to evaluate internet resources; (d) critical thinking and how to apply it to the material you read; (e) writing style, including the appropriateness of personal pronouns, fluency of expression, and punctuation; (f) the use of the correct tense; (g) inclusive language; (h) the need for proofreading and redrafting; and finally (i) the physical presentation of your assignment. 2.1 C ollaborating Not Copying – The Distinction Collaborative study has many benefits. It is good practice for your professional life, and it will make your university life more enjoyable and fruitful. However, collaboration in the production of assessable assignments can be confusing and even risky. You should always check what the expectations of your lecturer or tutor are regarding collaboration in the preparation of assessable work, but even when you work with another student or students, you should never produce one work which is submitted by multiple students. Instead, you might talk about the resources, your understanding of them, and the expectations of the task, and then you will write your own assignment. It is desirable for first- and second-year undergraduate students faced with writing research reports and essays to collaborate in study groups. For example, when working on research reports, it may help you to discuss hypotheses, if these were not already given in the instructions, and to discuss which references logically lead to them, and do or do not support the hypotheses. You may also like to discuss the results, and the strengths and weaknesses of the research, because sharing ideas can help your understanding of the material. Collaborating on the preparations for a research report also allows you to debate your ideas and opinions, and hopefully, that will give you a taste of the intellectual excitement to be experienced in academic research that makes much of the grind worthwhile. However, it is always important to check with your tutor or lecturer about what is acceptable and what is not acceptable at your institution. If your assignment is an essay, the possibilities for collaboration may be much more limited. Most lecturers would probably be happy for you to share resources, such as references. However, the essay probably needs to be a structured argument for your point of view, and your marker will expect that what you have written is your argument. If you want advice about whether you are on the right track with your essay plan, it is wisest to consult with the lecturer who set the essay topic, or your tutor. 8 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays – Getting Star ted 2 So, as we have discussed, there are benefits to collaborating with your fellow students, especially on the preparatory aspects of the research report or essay. In contrast, when it comes to the final written work, your marker needs to be confident that what is written is your own work, reflecting your own research and understanding, except where explicitly acknowledged. Using other people’s ideas, argument, or expression without acknowledgment is considered plagiarism. If you and a fellow student work too closely together, which leads to you both developing (or co-authoring) material, this is considered collusion. Both plagiarism and collusion are very serious matters and will typically mean a loss of marks, perhaps zero, for that report or essay, and a meeting with your lecturer or tutor to discuss issues of academic honesty. At worst, plagiarism and collusion can, and have led to expulsion from Australian universities. For more information about your university’s approach to academic honesty (or dishonesty), consult the university handbook or web page (e.g., code of conduct or student responsibilities) of your institution and any course guides produced by individual departments for subjects you are studying. Ignorance of these policies and procedures is not an acceptable excuse if you are caught submitting unoriginal material developed with or by a fellow student, or from books, journal articles, or the internet. It is also more detectable due to the use of originality-checking software such as Turnitin. There is more information about plagiarism from books, journals, and the internet in Chapter 3. If you have checked that collaboration is acceptable, there are several things you can do to make sure you stay on the right side of the collaboration versus collusion divide. First, collaboration is probably best in the planning and early (e.g., reading) stages of a research report or essay. For example, this will allow you to share practical information like planning your approach and sharing references. You may also like to discuss your understanding of such shared references once you have read them to clarify your comprehension. However, when it comes to writing the report or essay it is essential that it is your own work. For this reason, you should never share your drafts, or work on writing your reports or essays with another student(s). Also, while we will repeatedly stress the value of getting assignments proofread, your proofreader should not be a student completing the same assignment. You should be writing for an intelligent layperson, which means a non-psychology student is ideal as a proofreader. 2.2 Information Gathering All undergraduate essays and research reports in psychology will require you to read some material on the chosen topic. You need to decide what to read, and how much. There is an enormous amount of information available within the discipline of psychology and related areas. There are two important considerations that should help guide your thinking on this issue. First, the information you are given about your research report or essay is the best guide to the topic and scope of the project. Second, undergraduate psychology majors and graduate psychology training in Australasian universities is based on a scientist/practitioner model of psychology. The aim of these programs is the 9 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays theoretical and, at later stages, practical training for psychology professionals who understand the scientific method and base their practice on knowledge that has been gained using accepted methods of scientific inquiry. As a result, research reports and essays are designed to build skills in scientific inquiry and the scientific method. This broad aim will, therefore, guide the information you will use. The scientific method. You will learn more about the scientific method throughout your psychology studies, especially in your Research Design classes. The scientific method is an approach to learning about the world that, in the simplest terms, involves (a) observation; (b) hypothesis generation; (c) hypothesis testing using methods as objective as possible; and (d) the development of theory from findings (e.g., Popper, 1959) with the added conditions that the hypotheses and eventually the theory must be phrased in such a way that if they turn out to be false this can be discovered (i.e., falsification); and the method and findings must be reported in such a way as to be able to be replicated within a generally accepted paradigm (Eysenck, 2004). Even with these pretty strict rules, there are many ways of conducting research that broadly fit this ideal. In undergraduate psychology, you are likely to come across experiments, quasi-experiments, correlational methods, observational studies, case studies, and perhaps others that use the scientific method. There is also extensive qualitative research in psychology in recent literature, but you are less likely to encounter it at undergraduate level. An example of an experiment is to randomly assign participants to one of two or more groups, and to manipulate a single variable (e.g., the amount of alcohol consumed) across those groups. The groups are then compared on an outcome variable that is predicted to be dependent on amount of alcohol consumed (perhaps number of times a participant runs off the road in a driving simulator). If there are consistent differences between the groups, you can be confident that the amount of alcohol consumed caused the differences in driving performance. You can be confident because the random allocation to the groups should mean that all other possible causes of driving performance, such as time of day or differences between participants such as personality or skill levels, are equally distributed across the groups and so cannot account for the difference in results. Since it is not always possible to conduct formal experiments in psychology (we can’t randomly assign you to marry similar or dissimilar people and later test how satisfied you are), psychologists often use quasi-experimental studies. The groups they compare are naturally occurring ones, such as males and females, young and older, depressed and not depressed. We cannot be so confident about what causes what in such studies, because there may well be factors involved in those groups that affect our outcome measures other than the ones we manipulate. For example, age differences, especially in children, are associated with different levels of education and even developmental stages, all of which may be the actual cause of differences between younger and older children on a range of variables. In yet other studies, we measure variables such as self-esteem and satisfaction with life to find out whether the variables are associated with each other. That is, it may turn out that people with high self-esteem are generally more satisfied with their lives. A benefit of 10 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays – Getting Star ted 2 a correlational design of this type is that it allows exploration of existing relationships between variables that exist in the real world. However, this kind of design does not allow us to work out which one of those things causes the other or even if they are both caused by something else entirely, like holding down a good job, which is known to improve self-esteem and lead to greater life satisfaction. All of these sorts of designs, and more, are legitimate applications of the scientific method. You will learn about their strengths and limitations as you progress through your undergraduate psychology major, including that each of these contributes to our knowledge of psychology, as can be found in journal articles, books and textbooks, web pages, and even the popular press. The difficulty is to know where to look, and how to know which sources provide quality evidence for your research report or essay. What is a quality source? Different sorts of publications serve different purposes. Newspaper and magazine reports, as well as blogs, are written for the layperson who is not expected to be trained in the scientific method, while journal articles are usually written for people who are not only scientifically trained but may be working in the area that the study examines. In addition, journal articles are often very narrowly focused, considering only a very specific subject, whereas a textbook is likely to discuss a range of theories and findings related to a general topic, in much less detail than a journal article. How useful a particular reference is depends on what you need it for. A good place to start both research reports and essays is your textbook. It should provide you with a general overview of the area of interest, is likely to provide you with definitions, and should mention one or more studies in that area with the full references. It is usually acceptable to cite your textbook in your first few research reports or essays, especially in the Introduction section as you define the topic and key concepts of your paper. However, in later years your lecturer or tutor will expect fewer textbook citations and many more references to specific journal articles. If you are writing a research report, you will probably need to find journal articles on your specific topic or reviewing related topics. If you can find an article reviewing the area you are researching, that will help enormously because it summarises a good deal of knowledge in that area. In contrast, essays allow you to search more broadly, to include not only specific studies but also opinion pieces, which may be found in web pages, magazine articles, and perhaps even newspapers if the essay topic allows. These materials will be of particular use if you are interested in debate or opinion, as well as real-world applications or implications that may not have been directly studied. However, it is very important to understand that even well-researched and reasoned opinion pieces are not equivalent to research evidence as reported in journal articles. This is because the conclusion reflects different types of information (e.g., opinion versus data), as well as differences in the process of information gathering (e.g., the scientific method versus bibliographic research) and publishing (e.g., peer review versus edited media). Because psychology is very concerned with the quality of the evidence for the assertions it makes about how people think and behave, you need to be aware that 11 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays publications vary in the way they report information. To ensure that research is worth reporting in the first place, psychology, like other scientific disciplines, uses a method of peer review to decide if studies are worth publishing. This means that several well-qualified professionals make judgments about whether something is worth publishing in a professional journal. Therefore, although journals do vary in the quality of the material they publish, you can be confident that an article in a professional psychology journal has been through a process of peer review and is an acceptable one to cite in an assignment. That doesn’t mean that it is perfect or that you can’t criticise it, but it does mean that the article has been found to make a meaningful contribution to the discipline. How do you find relevant articles? You will almost always be given one or two articles as starter references. They may be in the Reserve section of your library, possibly in electronic form, for you to make a copy, or they may be accessible through web-based systems like Blackboard or WebCT. These references have been given to you because they are of specific relevance to your assignment. So, for this reason, it is essential you take the time to read them and understand why they have been assigned. These references will probably give you a feel for the area in question and will help you find additional articles of relevance either by looking through the references cited in these articles or by exploring articles which have subsequently cited them (e.g., by using the cited by link in Google Scholar). Your textbook may also provide a context for your report, usually the classic references for a particular area of psychology or authors whose other publications you can explore. The next place to look is one of the popular online databases. Commonly used ones are PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, Scopus, Web of Knowledge, Web of Science, and Academic Search Complete. Your library will probably have training sessions in-person or online on how to set up an account, undertake searches, and save articles. You can search databases by author, article title, abstract, or keyword. For your purposes, the keyword is most relevant. The more specific you can make the keyword, the better, because there is a huge body of literature available. If you just ask for “depression” you will get thousands of articles, and then how do you choose which to look at? If you are examining a particular group, say adolescents, add that to the keywords and limit the search (adolescents and depression). In this particular case, you will still get a large number of references, so refining what you ask for as much as you can is a good idea. Make a judgment, based on the title, of how relevant each reference is to your particular assignment, then select the most pertinent ones and read the Abstract. Do this for a small number and, if easily available, briefly look through the articles. You will often find that several of them cite the same earlier articles. That is a clue that the earlier work is central to the area and is therefore worth looking at. You should be able to access full-text versions of articles in the most frequently used journals dating at least back to the mid-1990s, and many more full-text references from much earlier. Because they are easily accessed, there is a temptation to use only modern references; this will often be a good idea, as it will give you the contemporary view of the phenomenon. However, it is valuable to bear in mind that many studies that are run as 12 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays – Getting Star ted 2 the basis of first-year research reports are replications of classic psychology research, which may mean you need to read several key papers from across the last five decades. This may involve locating hard copies held in the Periodicals section of your library (or those of nearby universities). One final point on finding articles for research reports is that although it is acceptable to use some secondary sources in first-year assignments, for example, Bandura, Ross, and Ross (as cited in Lilienfeld et al., 2014), they are often frowned upon by markers at other year levels. In later years, you are expected to have read the original study or to use another original source to make or support your argument. If you cannot access the primary source, do not simply cite it from the reference list of the article you do have, especially if this primary source is an older, foreign article or textbook, as this is easily detectable (e.g., a simple search of your library’s holdings will reveal the absence of the resource) and may also be considered a breach of academic honesty (i.e., you are claiming you have read material you have not read). Psychological essays allow wider scope in the use of material. In addition to academic materials like those just discussed, it may be appropriate to search internet sources such as Google or Yahoo for non-academic sources which demonstrate popular views or more general information currently occupying the minds of writers, researchers, and others. However, this will require considerable criticality from you, as it is important to be aware of the quality of evidence that is presented in such sources. Furthermore, it is crucial that you make clear whether you are reporting a matter of opinion, a public poll, or the result of (likely, unpublished) research. For example, if it is research, has it been reviewed by the authors’ peers in the discipline, or is it privately published (e.g., “none of the journals would accept it, so I put it on the web myself”)? There is more information on evaluating such material in the next section. In an essay, the way you might use internet material is to report that a particular topic is popular or has received considerable attention over a particular period (e.g., by using a date range in your search), in the sense of having many entries. You might briefly describe some of them, being careful to note the qualities of the material (e.g., opinion pieces, blogs, reports, professional online publications, etc.). Depending on the way the essay question is framed, you may argue for or against the opinions expressed, using evidence from other readings or research. Alternatively, you might use the internet to familiarise yourself with a topic, especially if your library has few resources in that area. This might allow you to narrow the scope of your inquiry, and possibly discover some of the names of researchers current in the field whose formal publications you can then access. The possibilities are limited only by your imagination, as long as you remember to acknowledge the work of others by properly citing them in your essay. Some web resources. Having said all that, the web is great for generally exploring a topic, and frequently contains material that is both more controversial and current than journal articles (e.g., publication biases like the tendency to only publish significant findings can affect what is available in academic journals), and much more current than 13 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays books. That is because almost all universities have websites, and many academics maintain their own pages discussing their current research, often with links to related material. Below are some web addresses that you may like to explore. They were all checked at the time of this publication and all were active then. You may want to use a search engine to find a specific topic, and that is fine, but this usually results in lots of extraneous material that is time-consuming to wade through. Your textbook publisher will also have a website, usually with links to ancillary material relevant to that text, and probably including study guides and examples of test questions. Such sites are very well worth exploring. Here are sites maintained by the peak bodies of psychology in Australia and America: Australian Psychological Society: www.psychology.org.au American Psychological Association: www.apa.org Association for Psychological Science: www.psychologicalscience.org These sites have a lot of information relevant to professional psychologists, which won’t be of immediate relevance to you, although they will give you a flavour of what is important to psychologists. However, they do have links to student resources. We’re reluctant to give just a few sites in case they are somehow thought to be special. They aren’t. They are just ones we’ve found when we’ve been looking. There are plenty more out there to be found, and they may well be better than these. Having said that, here is one site that has copies of some of the most famous papers in the history of psychology and allows you to search them by author or topic: http://psychclassics.yorku.ca A site for information within the general area of social psychology is: http://www.socialpsychology.org A similar site maintained by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, which has a students’ corner, is: http://www.spsp.org Some blogs that you may find interesting and useful are: https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2017/09/170911122615.htm @mindhacksblog http://www.spring.org.uk/ http://www.psychologicalscience.org/news/were-only-human http://www.badscience.net/ (more than psych, but good on science) Happy searching – but don’t get carried away! 14 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays – Getting Star ted 2.3 2 Evaluating Internet Resources This section has been called “Evaluating Internet Resources”, but the advice applies equally well to various media such as magazines, newspapers, blogs, and podcasts. The quality of such material is enormously variable, and you should not even consider citing newspapers or magazines for research reports. However, essays may be a different matter. Where you are asked to discuss, compare, or evaluate psychological theories, diagnoses or treatments, it is safer to stick to textbooks and journal articles. Occasionally, you may be asked to examine popular opinion or the impact of psychological practices on the general population. In such a case, it is acceptable to find summaries or opinions in the broader media, including the internet. The internet itself provides information on how to evaluate internet material. One website that we like (though there are others) is found at http://www.virtualsalt. com/-evalu8it.htm. It is by Harris (2015) and is specifically aimed at evaluating internet research sources. A more general one is an online tutorial from Monash University, found at http://www.monash.edu.rlo/tutorials, which is more a comprehensive look at evaluating any material you find, and also contains links to a huge number of other learning strategies. Harris (2015) suggests that a good way to evaluate material is to use a checklist with the acronym CARS, for Credibility, Accuracy, Reasonableness, and Support. Credibility involves whether the author is known, and how likely they are to be knowledgeable and trustworthy. Does the web page you are looking at name the author? If so, what biographical information do they give? Would you consider them to be expert in the field, or at least experienced? Do they provide contact details? Do they cite material from scientific sources, such as journal articles, which can be checked by you? Are they employed by a recognisable institution, and what sort (university, government department, research organisation)? While you are not expected to recognise the professional standing of particular individuals, you can at least be more confident of the trustworthiness of material on the home page of an academic at a university or major research institution than that of a person whose affiliation is unknown. Accuracy includes whether the material is comprehensive. Does it give you a rounded picture of the subject, or does it seem to present a partial or biased view? Does it admit alternative views, even if only to argue against them? Accuracy also includes a level of appropriateness. Is the material aimed at a general audience, or schoolchildren, or a specialist audience? Accuracy also includes timeliness (Harris, 2015). Especially in areas relating to some effects (e.g., biological effects on behaviour), some information becomes out of date fairly quickly. Does the document contain a date? However, don’t be too quick to judge journal articles published before you were born. They may have been updated but could form the basis of long-lasting and influential theories that are still relevant. Nonetheless, such “classics” will usually be referred to in your texts and, in the second and third years, you will be expected to read the original sources, not what some later author says was done. 15 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays The standard of Reasonableness includes judgments of whether the article is fair, objective, moderate, and consistent (Harris, 2015). Does the article present information in a fair and balanced way, including reporting information or arguments contrary to its own point of view? Is it trying to raise your emotions to the point where you cannot think clearly about its arguments? This approach can often be detected by the use of ridiculous counter examples (i.e., a “straw man”) or sarcasm, and while we cannot be totally objective, it is essential to beware of articles written in strongly slanted ways. One such influence is the particular political or ideological point of view that may influence the material, which can be harder to ignore if at odds with your own views, and equally hard to identify if consistent with your views. Try asking yourself, “What does the writer gain or achieve from convincing me of their point of view?” Also, run reality checks on what seem like unusual or extreme claims. Because psychology deals with how people think and behave, you are already a good amateur psychology theorist or you wouldn’t be able to function in society. If a claim seems contrary to your experience, look for more information, but try to keep an open mind. Moderate and conditional claims are more likely to be accurate. The final area of Support refers to the sort of evidence cited in support of the claims made in the material you find. Claims of fact and statistics frequently come from other sources (Harris, 2015), and an argument is more credible if those sources are also credible. Does the author cite their own sources of information? If you find several articles that agree with each other, you can be more confident of the claims being made, or they may all be citing the same dubious source (e.g., drinking eight glasses of water a day is widely cited as the ideal intake, but has no scientific support). Communication scholars talk about the “given-new” principle: when giving new information, it is better understood if linked to something already known (Haviland & Clark, 1974). Is what you are being told in the article consistent with things you already know from other areas of your study? If the material is untrustworthy about something you already know, you should suspect what it is telling you about something unknown to you. The process of choosing what material is relevant to your particular writing project needs more than just being careful about the source. It also requires you to think critically about the content. This is the subject of the next section. 2.4 Critical Thinking In the previous chapter we said that tertiary essays in most subjects, but especially in psychology, require you to demonstrate the skill of critical thinking. Critical thinking is not nit-picking or disagreeing with what you read. It is showing that you understand the strengths and weaknesses as well as the main ideas of the work in question. For the purposes of preparing a research report or essay in psychology, a definition of critical thinking is “a logical and rational process of avoiding one’s preconceptions by gathering evidence, contemplating and evaluating alternatives, and coming to a conclusion” (Smith, 1995, p. 2). This is essentially the generic skill of demonstrating your awareness 16 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays – Getting Star ted 2 of the strength of a position with regard to the evidence presented in support of it. The application of this skill maintains one of the major values of good science – an organised scepticism and a critical awareness of the need to support any claims you make with adequate evidence. Critical thinking and the scientific method. You will probably have been told in some of your early lectures that psychology is a science because of the way it investigates thought and behaviour. The scientific method generally starts from an observation, which leads to conjecture about the way the world works, which becomes the basis for a prediction about what will happen, at least in that situation, in future. A hypothesis is the operationalised form of this prediction using variables that can be measured, which is then tested. The data collected is then considered against the hypothesis and is interpreted as support for (or against) the hypothesis. If support is found, this leads to a testable theory (i.e., a series of predictions and mechanisms describing a larger phenomenon) which gains further support via the scientific method or is revised and tested further. Even when the evidence you find supports your hypothesis, you can only gain support for your hypothesis or theory. This may be described as a “proof”, but it does not mean your theory is proved. In this context, critical thinking is essentially a way of looking at the conclusions that psychologists have drawn on the basis of the hypothesis, evidence, or theory, and asking whether those conclusions are warranted by the evidence put forward in support of them. Deciding whether a researcher’s conclusions are reasonable requires several things. You need to identify and assess their assumptions; you need to decide if their hypothesis is a good operationalisation of their prediction and whether it is a reasonable derivation of the theory; you need to determine if their sample is appropriate (e.g., representative of the population they draw the conclusion about, and of sufficient size to support what they say); and you need to determine whether the evidence, statistical or otherwise, actually supports the claim they are making. You also need to decide whether or not there might be factors that limit or confound their finding, or whether there is another explanation that fits the evidence. Assumptions. It is common in psychological research to compare groups and look for differences. This is based on the assumption that there are differences to be found. It is good critical thinking to point out that a researcher appears to assume that there are differences, such as sex or age-based differences, or differences in ability between any other groups that are being compared. Identifying apparent assumptions shows that you are not simply accepting the researcher’s method, or results, but have thought about why they made those assumptions or adopted the approach that they did. Good critical thinking also means looking at both sides of a position (or perhaps more, since there are often many possible explanations for any particular set of observations). Remember that an experimental report is itself an argument, since it comes to a particular conclusion on the basis of an argument and evidence. Even if you want to argue a particular case, it is good to be able to point out arguments against it, if only to 17 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays say why they don’t apply or are not reasonable. Being able to anticipate and respond to possible objections to your case strengthens your position. Types of weak arguments. You should learn to recognise weak arguments in what you read, and avoid them yourself. There are many good books on critical thinking which describe such arguments in detail. One weak argument is the illegitimate appeal to authority. A famous or popular person saying something is not evidence of its truth. You would need to describe the evidence itself, rather than rely on the authority of the person making the assertion. The straw man argument is where someone misrepresents an adversary’s argument or states an extreme version of it to make that argument easier to refute. Another weak argument is appeal to popular belief. “As everyone knows, males are more aggressive than females” is such an appeal. This may well be a commonly held stereotype, but it depends heavily on what is defined as aggression. You would need to be much more specific and cite evidence, rather than make a general assumption that everyone else agrees with you. Ad hominem arguments attack the person rather than their argument or evidence. It may be tempting to dismiss someone as a sexist if they argue that men demonstrate lower scores on some tests of intelligence. However, you should restrain yourself from attacking the character of that person and confine yourself to the problems with their argument and the evidence. For example, perhaps the intelligence tests use items that are more familiar to women and this results in evidence that women tend to score higher on tests of this kind, suggesting that familiarity may be a confound for these tests. 2.5 C ritically Evaluating Psychology Journal Articles One reason that critical evaluation of journal articles is not more formally taught is that there are few clear guidelines for how to go about it. One person brave enough to put his thoughts into print is Sternberg (2010). (You know he’s brave because he has also published on subjects like intelligence, love, and wisdom!) He argues that there are (at least) eight standards for evaluating the contribution of psychology papers. Sternberg’s standards are aimed at practising researchers who are deciding whether something is useful enough to publish, and so they are broader than what is expected of student research reports. However, if you are planning a research career they are worth examining. When critically evaluating a psychological article, especially a report of an empirical investigation, you need to ask some general questions of the article as a whole, and some specific questions about particular parts of it. The general questions are about what the paper does. Does it: • replicate previous findings and/or support existing theories? • challenge previous assumptions? • address important social issues? 18 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays – Getting Star ted 2 • distinguish between theories that predict similar outcomes, or resolve an issue where previous theories make different predictions? • correct flaws in previous research, or introduce better methodology or stronger results (e.g., more appropriate sample, preregistration, where the research design has been peer reviewed before the study is undertaken)? • address a previously unidentified or unexplored issue? • present a promising new theory? You then need to consider how it achieves these improvements. You can do this by recognising whether the paper: • presents a novel/insightful/critical/extensive/contemporary review of relevant literature, • presents the pros and cons of its major argument, • is clear and unambiguous in its arguments, • adequately covers the relevant literature (this might be a big ask for undergraduates, but you should be reading more widely in your higher years). Having a satisfactory answer to some of these general questions is not the end of the story. You should then ask some more specific questions about the paper. For example: • Does the paper present clear hypotheses, and have these been well justified? • Are the hypotheses well operationalised? That is, is there a clear link between the phenomenon being studied and the variables chosen, and will testing them in the proposed ways produce an adequate answer to the questions the authors are asking? The adequacy of the operationalisation of the hypotheses has implications for the Method. For example, you could ask questions about the sample, like: • Is the sample appropriate (e.g., size, representativeness)? • If it is a fairly special class of people (e.g., university students), does testing it with this group restrict generalisation or will the result be universally meaningful? If the study addresses physiological measures or sensory perception, it may not matter that an “unrepresentative sample” has been used because such basic processes are likely to be common even to university students. If the question addressed is specific to a group (“How do university students perceive the quality of the teaching they get?”), then a specific sample such as university students is probably appropriate (depending on the representativeness of the university!). Alternatively, if the sample is (as is often the case) North American, predominately female, and under 30 years of age, can the results be generalised to “people” or are there cultural implications? If so, do the authors acknowledge this? • What are the validity checks used with online samples? For example, how are the authors certain all responses were not just one person who participated 150 times, or that respondents paid attention to the questions? 19 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Other methodological questions might include: • What measures are used? Are they standard scales or have the authors created their own? Have they reported the psychometric properties (reliability, validity)? Is there a chance the participants might not have answered honestly (e.g., self-presentation concerns)? • Is there a control group? Is one appropriate? • Have possible order effects been addressed? • Are the procedures clear enough that you could replicate the study? Criticising samples – types and sizes – can be overly simplistic. For example, even if a sample is small or unrepresentative, it doesn’t mean that the conclusion is implausible; rather, it may be weak or limited. You should therefore be cautious about dismissing small or student-only samples. You can acknowledge the weakness by saying things like “Jones’s (2016) conclusions, based on a small sample of college students, were that . . .”. Or, alternatively, of course, “Green’s (2017) conclusions, based as they were on a large, community sample from a major city, are more convincing than those mentioned by Black’s (2007) and Brown’s (2011) student-based findings.” Similarly, describing self-reported measures as flawed because they are subject to biased responding may be appropriate for sensitive topics about which people may feel uncomfortable disclosing, but this may be addressed by the use of anonymity in responding. When you consider the Results section of the article you are evaluating, don’t feel too bad if the statistics are a bit overwhelming! You are not usually expected to comment on the adequacy of the statistics, unless they are of the kind you have already encountered. You should, however, be able to comment on the completeness and clarity of the results that are reported. For example, if the authors are comparing groups, do they present the actual scores for each group, or just the comparison statistic (e.g., a t-test or F statistic)? The comparison statistic is affected by the sample size (e.g., small differences can be “significant” for large samples but not for small ones). Similarly, are tables or figures interpretable (e.g., well-labelled, consistent with hypotheses)? Does the text make clear what is to be expected from the tables or figures? When thinking about the Discussion section of the article, you should ask yourself questions like these: • How convincing are the results in testing the hypotheses? Are the results clear and unambiguous or are they confusing, with no clear pattern? Are any findings omitted, or added without reference to hypotheses? Do the authors admit that results are inconclusive, or, if the results turn out to be more complex than the hypotheses would have led you to expect, do the authors make this clear? • How well do the authors link their results with previous findings? Are similarities acknowledged and differences explained? • The implications of the results are very important. Do the authors suggest that the topic or theory should be thought about differently? Do they want to reframe a theory in the light of their results? Are you convinced that their 20 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays – Getting Star ted 2 explanation of psychological processes is an improvement over previous research? Do they make clear suggestions for clarifying unclear results or addressing other questions which the study may have raised but were not its original focus? • Are there any questions in your mind about the adequacy of the methodology in the light of the results? Do the authors admit methodological flaws or make suggestions for how different methodologies might confirm or refute their findings? • How well does the paper address the broader significance of the results (i.e., usefulness or significance to the “real world”)? • How well does the paper indicate whether particular issues need resolving? Does it suggest how this might be done? Does it identify other factors that may need to be examined to extend our understanding of the processes in question? Finally, just because a conclusion can logically be drawn from a particular result, it doesn’t mean that it is the correct one. If you were imaginative enough, you might think of an alternative explanation for the same observed facts. Beware of sounding too dogmatic! In writing a research report or essay, you will not have space to answer all these questions. The purpose of this sort of interrogation of an article is to help you to decide which articles to include (e.g., quality and relevance). This should also help you to develop hypotheses that are based on previous research and are well-operationalised and justified. In summary, you should always read research critically. You need to be continually alert to the assumptions behind the researchers’ claims; to the strength of the evidence presented in support of those claims; and to the conclusions drawn from the data reported. In short, good science presupposes genuinely critical thinking. 2.6 Writing Style Research reports and essays are professional presentations of your work. They should not be conversational, chatty, or colloquial. In the case of a research report, this is an argument presenting conclusions about your results, with little room for personal opinion or speculation. The reader is not interested in your unsupported beliefs. The reader wants to know what the results were, and what it means in light of previous research. If your findings are unexpected or inconsistent with previous findings, it may be necessary for you to consider alternative explanations – ideally drawing on research or evidence. Always justify a belief statement. You have a little more freedom with an essay, especially with regard to personal opinions. However, the opinions you put forward must be supported by evidence, theory, or reasoning, and presented in ways that are not dogmatic or in a conversational style. Remember that you are aiming at a professional presentation to your intellectual peers. While few scientists these days would argue that science is completely objective, your writing should be as objective as you can make it. Objectivity is helped if you keep your 21 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays sentences short and clear, avoid personal pronouns as much as possible, and give evidence for any assertions you make. Personal pronouns. The current APA Publication Manual (6th ed.) advocates for the use of active voice. That is, you are required to describe the actions you take in reviewing, researching, and reporting using verbs that reflect your active role. This results in a need to use personal pronouns (e.g., I, we) such as “In the next analyses we attempted to replicate previous findings on . . .”, or “To assess whether the . . . manipulations resulted in significant differences in perception . . . we first examined . . .”. This replaces the historical preference for the use of impersonal constructions, for example, “The author favours the argument that . . .” or “The investigators concluded that . . .”; or the passive voice, for example, “Participants were randomly assigned to groups . . .”. However, it is important to remember that this change reflects the active role of researchers, not support for subjectivity in your writing. An advantage of the older passive voice approach was that it seemed to encourage objectivity. However, the disadvantage was that it often sounded clumsy and implied the absence of the role or influence of the researcher(s). Active voice and personal pronouns will improve the simplicity and precision of your writing. However, it is not generally useful to make statements of opinion or speculation beginning “I believe that . . .”, or “I lean towards the conclusion that . . .”. Such expressions of opinion should be avoided or used sparingly, and only in the Discussion, as they rarely add to what can be inferred. For example, the statement “The evidence suggests that social facilitation is purely an arousal effect” is clearly a statement of the author’s opinion. Nothing is added by prefacing it with “In my opinion” or “I believe that”. Essays give you more scope for personal opinions, and therefore for the use of personal pronouns. They are usually expected. However, use them sparingly, and justify your expressions of opinion. Fluent expression. All other things being equal, the more fluently written your work is, the better your marks will be. Even if your point or argument is generally correct, poor written expression that your marker has to struggle to understand will lose you marks. While your sentences will vary in length to convey differentially complex ideas, it is generally better to keep your sentences short. Paragraphs should consist of several sentences (i.e., one sentence is NOT a paragraph). However, paragraphs should not be too long. The average paragraph should be about half a double-spaced page in length. A paragraph should contain only one main idea. In a review of research (e.g., the Introduction to a research report), you might describe a previous study in a single paragraph. It might describe the aim or hypothesis of the study, the size and nature of the sample, a succinct description of the method, and the major conclusions that are relevant to your own study. That can usually be done in about two-thirds of a double-spaced page (about 200 words). Alternatively, if you are describing how a particular theory has been applied to a variety of contexts, you might use a paragraph to state the theory, and then refer, in the next paragraph, to several studies that have applied the theory in various practical situations in only a sentence or two each. 22 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays – Getting Star ted 2 Fluency is also improved by avoiding jargon and preferring short words to long ones. You should also use the specific terms rather than more general ones (e.g., happy rather than positive mood), and you should use these terms consistently, since accuracy, even if a bit repetitive, is preferred over novelty. That said, markers are more impressed if you describe a theory in your own words than they are by fancy words or quotations from journal articles. If something is accurately described in simple words, your marker can be confident that you understand it. If a quotation is used, it is not always clear that you understand what it means. If unusual words are used, the marker may suspect plagiarism. Also, avoid using a thesaurus to help you create interest in your writing. Frequently, students who do this end up using words that are imprecise and it impairs the quality of the work. One way to test whether your writing is fluent is to read it aloud, or, better still, get someone else to do so. Although there is a difference between material written to be read and material written to be spoken (e.g., a speech or a lecture), anything that is poorly written will sound clumsy when read aloud. This is not a foolproof system, because normal conversation is often appallingly ungrammatical. However, it will help you identify long sentences and unexpected topic changes, as well as repeated words or incomplete sentences. Abbreviations and acronyms. A matter of relevance to fluent expression is the use of abbreviations or acronyms. You may find it useful to abbreviate commonly used names, most often in the Introduction and Method sections. Use abbreviations sparingly. It is asking too much of your marker to remember a lot of acronyms as they read, and it inhibits fluent expression. However, it may save you a few words when word limits are tight. Always introduce the abbreviation the first time it is used, thus: “Smith and Jones (2017) used the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) to test . . .”. When using the name of the test later, you must refer to it as the MMPI. Never do this if you don’t use the name again. Do not use an abbreviation for single words like extraversion and neuroticism, because the single letters E and N are counted as whole words, so you don’t save anything off your word count. Incomplete sentences. Markers are often irritated when students make an assertion in one sentence, then follow it with a supposed sentence containing only a participle. For example: “Bloggs (2018) found . . . to be the case. Thus proving Smith (2011) wrong.” That second sentence is not a sentence. It should be all one sentence, thus: “Bloggs (2018) found . . . to be the case, thus proving Smith (2011) wrong.” A related issue comes about from a writer using “they”, “this”, “those”, or “it” to refer to the subject of a previous sentence. The use of these terms often makes it unclear what topic is being referred to. Consequently, it is often better to restate the subject in each sentence. Similar words having different meanings. Students frequently use the words “effect” and “affect” interchangeably. They are words you will frequently find yourself using in both essays and research reports in psychology. They are easy to misuse because both can 23 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays be used as either a verb or a noun. However, “affect” is used as a verb (“level of lighting affects visual acuity”) except where it is used as a synonym for emotion or mood (“the procedure induced a positive affect”). In contrast, “effect” is usually used as a noun (“the effect of level of self-esteem on . . .”). Students also confuse “than”, which is used to describe a comparison (I am better than you), and “then”, which describes a timing (e.g., participants self-reported their height then their weight). Unjustified extension and conclusions. One other misuse of language that irritates markers is to start a sentence with “therefore”, “consequently”, “as a result”, or “thus” when the sentence does not follow logically from what has been said before. “Therefore” should only be used in drawing a conclusion from previously stated pieces of evidence. Similarly, “furthermore” and “additionally” should only be used to begin sentences that genuinely add to or clarify the previous point made, joining two related pieces of information. Commas. How is your punctuation? Although markers vary in how pedantic they are about punctuation, you never know how fussy yours is until you’ve upset them. For this reason, it is better to make efforts in the first place to get on top of this aspect of your writing. An amusing and instructive book on the subject is Eats, Shoots & Leaves: The Zero Tolerance Approach to Punctuation by Lynne Truss (2003), though there are also many others. The APA Publication Manual (6th ed.) has clear expectations about the use of commas. For example, commas are to be used in lists of three or more items, following each item. For example: Concentrating on the cognitive, affective, and social dimensions . . . in a study by Giddings, Bellamy, and Gott (2017) A comma is also appropriate when a clause could be omitted from the sentence without affecting the grammar and meaning of what is left. For example: Laughter, like smiling, can be a social response that serves to punctuate communications between individuals. You should also use a comma to separate two independent clauses which are joined by a conjunction such as “and” or “but”. For example: Age and tenure did correlate with job satisfaction, but the strongest predictors of job satisfaction were task variety and the extent of participation in decisions. You should not use a comma when the clause defines or limits the subject of the sentence. For example: The participants who held the picture cards responded faster than the others. 24 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays – Getting Star ted 2 A comma should also not be used where there are two parts to the predicate of the same subject. For example: All participants completed the first phase of the experiment and returned the following week for Phase 2. Note the contrast between this last example and the one beginning “Age and tenure . . .” on the previous page. In the “All participants . . .” example, both “completed the first phase of the experiment” and “returned the following week . . .” relate specifically to the subject “All participants”. We have only tackled the topic of commas, as they are essential to all writing. However, there are several other punctuation marks (e.g., hyphens, semicolons, colons) and rules that can enhance your writing if used correctly. Our advice regarding the placement of commas, and the use of other punctuation marks, is: if in doubt, leave them out. Apostrophes. Many students seem unclear about the correct use of apostrophes (’). In formal writing such as essays and research reports, you should never use an apostrophe for contractions. For example, write “it is”, never “it’s”, and “do not”, never “don’t”. (The use of such contractions in this book is an example of trying to be didactic, but in a userfriendly way. Such a tone is inappropriate in a formal essay or research report.) The acceptable use of apostrophes is to indicate possession. For almost all words, you simply add an apostrophe followed by s, as follows: “Galligan’s paper . . .”, “Each participant’s response . . .”, “. . . this study’s results . . .”, “These people’s opinions . . .”. This is true even when the word already ends with the letter s, as long as there is only one owner, for example, “Roberts’s article . . .”. However, when the noun is already plural, simply put an apostrophe after the s, for example: “All participants’ responses . . .”, “These studies’ findings . . .”. The other tricky situation is the possessive of compound groups. In such cases, you should add the apostrophe to the last word only, as in: “Walker and Broderick’s (2017) argument was that . . .”, “Crowe and Casey’s (2018) study found . . .”, “Fallon and Bowles’s (2012) suggestion that . . .”, “Bates et al.’s (2014) finding was that . . .”. If you are still unsure of the use of the apostrophe, you can almost always reword the sentence to avoid the need for one. The first example above can be avoided by saying: “It has been argued by Walker and Broderick (2018) that . . .”. The last example can be reworded as: “The results from Bates et al. (2014) suggested . . .”. Quotation marks. If you are using a word which is slang, or are making an ironic comment, put it in double quotation marks the first time it is used: . . . considered “normal” behaviour . . . If you use it subsequently (try not to overuse such words), do not put it in quotation marks. For more on quotations, see Chapter 3 (p. 42). 25 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Spelling. Many markers tend to put a line through misspelled words in assignments. They probably don’t consciously take marks off for it, but since most students use word processors, and most word-processing packages have easy to use spell-checkers, it indicates to your marker a lack of care or professionalism with the assignment, which will likely cost you marks! However, spell-checkers may not pick up psychological terms (e.g., “valence”) and authors’ names, which you will need to check yourself, or words that are correctly spelled but wrongly used, such as “their” and “there” or “affect” and “effect”. This is where it is advantageous to have someone else as a proofreader. Many spell-checkers use American spellings unless you change this setting, and markers may or may not be fussy about this, but you should at least be consistent throughout the report. Various lists of commonly misspelled words can be found on the internet (see also Burdess, 1991 for 1000 common psychology examples). They can be handy resources. Latin abbreviations. You will frequently see abbreviations of Latin words and phrases in professional articles. The ones most used in psychology reports and essays are e.g., i.e., and et al.. In psychology, the use of et cetera (and its abbreviation etc.) is best avoided. In common language, this is used to indicate that you have described part of the concept of interest and are relying on the reader’s familiarity or imagination to complete the full concept. In contrast, e.g. and i.e., respectively, provide an example of the concept, or all elements of the concept, and consequently demonstrate greater understanding and clarity on the part of the writer. They are therefore more appropriate in academic writing. E.g. and i.e. should only be used within parentheses. E.g. stands for “for example”. You should only use it when making a statement that you have backup for and which you wish to have the marker understand that there are many articles which say so. Here is an example: “Social identity relates to our understanding of our membership of a particular social group (e.g., Jenkins, 2014; Tajfel, 2010).” Note the full stops and the comma in the abbreviation. In an ordinary sentence, you should not use the abbreviation. Use the whole word, as we have done on the previous page when talking about apostrophes. I.e. stands for “that is”. Here is an example: “Study 2 tested a possible explanation for these findings, namely that the context (i.e., non-humorous components) . . .” Again, if you use it outside of parentheses, use “that is”. However, most markers are not that happy with this abbreviation, and you should avoid it if possible. Et al. stands for “and others”. It is used in its abbreviated form in psychology only to describe authors of articles or books, both in sentences and in parentheses. In flowing sentences, it always has a full stop after the “al” as in “Smith et al. (2016) argued that . . .” or “Jones et al.’s (2017) study found that . . .” Within parentheses it always has a comma after the full stop: “(Green et al., 2015)”. Use of different fonts. Another problem of working in changing times is how much you should take advantage of new technology. Many academics, being used to writing for publication, will argue that you should not use different font forms (like bold or italics) for emphasis in your research report or essay. If in doubt, don’t use them. There 26 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays – Getting Star ted 2 is a good case for arguing that you should be able to make the required emphasis by the appropriate use of language. The writing style in this book is also more conversational than you should use in a research report or essay, because this book is essentially an instruction manual and was written as if you were being spoken to. A research report or essay, however, is a formal report and so should not be chatty or frivolous. Use the same size and style font throughout your written work. Do not use larger font sizes for headings. However, note the use of bold type in headings, described in the Method section of Chapter 5. The Publication Manual of the APA expects you to use a 12-point serif font, such as Times New Roman. Your tutor or lecturer will probably be able to easily call to mind other examples of writing style that bother them. While not of great significance in terms of marks, the presence of such irritants may just tip the balance to your disadvantage if the marker is judging whether the assignment is worthy of, say, a high Pass or a Credit grade. 2.7 Using the Correct Tense An element of writing style that often confuses students is the tense used. The rule of thumb is that research reports should be written in the past tense. You are reporting research and theorising that it has already happened. This even applies to your own hypotheses which were developed before conducting the research (see Figure 4.2 in Chapter 4 for examples of aims and hypotheses). However, there are exceptions. The exceptions occur when you refer in your report to something that is happening at the time the reader is reading your report. For example, in the Method section you might say, “The questionnaire used is included as Appendix A.” Present tense is used because it is true as the reader is reading. Similarly, in your Results section you might say, “These results are summarised in Table 2”, since it is also true as the report is being read. Finally, in your Discussion, having reported (in the past tense) whether or not your hypotheses were supported, you could say, “These results suggest that . . .”. In this case, present tense is used because, although the results were supported (or not), they have implications for the present and future. In the same vein, you could say at the very end, “In summary, this study indicates that people judge corporations more harshly than individuals”, because it is a statement, in the present, of what the study suggests. Essays are typically written in the present tense, except where you are reporting something that has already happened including previous research or an argument that was made. This is because you are trying, in the present, to convince the reader of your point of view. For example, you might say that “Smith (2005) performed a study . . .” when describing what Smith did in the past, and “Smith (2005) argued that . . .”. In contrast, you might say “I argue, consistent with Smith’s contention”, which mixes these tenses, reflecting that you are active in presenting your argument, based on points of view other than your own. 27 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 2.8 Inclusive Language It is important, in the interests of accuracy and equity, to use neutral and respectful language. This means language that recognises people before their group memberships (e.g., people with a physical disability or mental disorder rather than disabled or mentally disordered people). For more on how this should be considered in writing research reports, see Chapter 5 (p. 81). The emphasis on inclusive language in the 6th edition of the APA Publication Manual highlights the need to address longstanding language biases, such as gender-biased language, which has accepted the use of masculine nouns and pronouns when the context includes both genders (e.g., mankind, chairman). Much research, especially Martyna (1978), suggests that readers of both genders think of male persons even when the context suggests that both genders are included. The best solution is to use plural pronouns. Instead of writing “The individual who displays prejudice in his personal relations is . . .”, write “Individuals who display prejudice in their personal relations are . . .”. Avoid using s/he, since it is impossible to pronounce, and he/she, which disturbs the flow of the writing. Further examples of gender-neutral writing are presented in Figure 2.1. On the other hand, avoid carrying this sensitivity to extremes. If all your subjects are male, of course you should use “he” and “him” when referring to individuals among them. For the use of gender-neutral language other than pronouns, a thesaurus should help keep you out of trouble. Don’t use the collective nouns “man” or “mankind”, use “people” or “humanity”. Don’t write “man an observation station”, say “staff” it. Children need “nurturing”, not “mothering”. The issue of sexist language is not trivial. It is vital in the interests of equity. It is also an important part of good communication. Unless you intend your message to refer to one gender only, you need to choose your words carefully so that it is quite clear that you are speaking of people in general. Ask your proofreader to watch out for language that is gender-exclusive, when the exclusion is not justified. 2.9 Proofreading and Redrafting Always proofread drafts. A distressing number of research reports are handed in for marking without being proofread. This behaviour is throwing marks away. It indicates to the marker that you do not care very much about the presentation of the report or have poor time-planning skills. Apart from minor evidence of carelessness, such as typographical errors (typos), it is not unusual to find some rather strange expression in undergraduate reports, such as long sentences, erratic punctuation, or the misuse of psychological jargon. A sign that you have done a good job on your report is that your work could be read and understood by a non-academic. We will say more in Chapter 4 about having a friend, parent, or sibling read your work to indicate just how clear your writing is. If they find that something you have written is unclear, don’t argue with them or try to clarify the problem verbally – you won’t 28 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays – Getting Star ted 2 Original: “If a subject has a strong tendency to give socially desirable responses in self-description, is it unreasonable to believe that he may also reveal this tendency in his behavior in a learning situation where he is aware of what would be considered socially desirable, namely to learn fast, to do his best?” (Edwards, 1957, p. 89) Revised: If participants have a strong tendency to give socially desirable responses in self-description, is it unreasonable to believe that they may also reveal this tendency in their behaviour in a learning situation where they are aware of what would be considered socially desirable, namely to learn fast, to do their best? Original: “With sufficient analytical subtlety we can tease out the connections from the behaviour of the man in his actual life situation – without the false situation of controlling and manipulating.” (Cattell, 1965, p. 20) Revised: With sufficient analytical subtlety we can tease out the connections from the behaviour of people in their actual life situations – without the false situations of controlling and manipulating. Original: “The goal of the individual includes his expectations for the future, his wishes, and his daydreams. Where the individual places his goals will be determined fundamentally by two factors, namely, by the individual’s relations to certain values and by his sense of realism in regard to the probability of reaching the goal.” (Lewin, 1948, p. 113) Revised: The goals of individuals include their expectations for the future, their wishes, and their daydreams. Where individuals place their goals will be determined by two factors, namely, by the individual’s relations to certain values and by their sense of realism in regard to the probability of reaching the goals. Figure 2.1. Examples of gender-neutral language. have this opportunity with your marker! If they have read it conscientiously, they are always right – by definition the writing is unclear. Even if their suggestions for fixing it are wrong or stupid, readers are never wrong if they have found something unclear. You may ask a fellow student to proofread for you, as long as they are not writing the same research report or essay. Of course, this means that you should also be prepared to do the same for them. This is essentially what happens in the real world of publishing. Writers usually get peer reviews before submitting their work for publication. Some students see their fellows as competitors, particularly third-year students. This is unfortunate and is inaccurate – your mark will not depend on others’ marks. Being prepared to give and receive criticism, and keeping that criticism focused on the writing and not the writer, is good preparation for later work experience. After proofreading, what then? Having received some constructive criticism, you need to start the long task of polishing and rewriting. Since essays are not constrained by the same conventions that research reports are, you may find that an essay will need even more rewriting! Make sure your time management has allowed for this. 29 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Time management. Most assignments are set so that you have several weeks before they are due. You should make a timetable of what needs to be done and do your best to stick to it. The checklists in Section 5.11 (research report) and in Section 6.8 (essay) are good starting points. There are no foolproof guidelines, because students work at very different rates. However, experience has shown that students are less stressed if they get some writing done early, and writing can even help to clarify what you do and don’t understand. It is easier to correct and elaborate on something already written than to try to hold material in your head then write it all at once. The steps involved in getting started are described in Section 4.4. Relation between effort and grades. A common complaint of many students is that they put an enormous amount of time and effort into their essay, but did not get the grade they expected. Although there is some relationship between effort expended and grade awarded, this assumes that the effort is being applied to things that will help (i.e., that drafts do improve). For example, reading more papers rather than starting writing won’t necessarily help your final product, and this is especially true for tasks you find difficult! Redrafting your writing is hard work. Make sure that the feedback you receive is really addressed in the next draft. What to look for when redrafting. The sorts of things to look for when rewriting a draft are: Is there a clear topic sentence in each paragraph, preferably at the start? Are all your sentences really sentences? Is there only one major point per paragraph? Do the paragraphs vary in length, without being either too short or too long? Avoid single-sentence paragraphs. Are the links between sentences or paragraphs logical or coherent? Not only should your drafts be read for clarity of argument and expression, your final version should also be proofread for typos or printing errors. The ease with which word-processing programs allow you to cut-and-paste words, sentences, or paragraphs leads to the possibility of all sorts of errors. If you can’t get someone else to proofread the final version, leave it for a day and proofread it yourself. Finally, do not forget to keep a copy, either on a USB or cloud-based backup service, or as a hardcopy. 2.10 T he Physical Presentation of Your Assignment Your university (e.g., library and the student union) will allow you to access word-processing, internet, and printing facilities if you need them. You should, however, be aware that, if you are working across multiple machines, you will need to check your final submission carefully for formatting errors (e.g., repagination, font changes, orphaned headings, even to make sure the file still opens!). Typing, spacing, margins, page numbers. The following suggestions may sound unduly simplistic, but too often they are ignored by undergraduates and can lead to 30 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Wr iting Research Repor ts and Essays – Getting Star ted 2 unnecessary deduction of marks. First, you may be required to submit electronically or in hardcopy, although the latter is increasingly uncommon. If you are submitting a hardcopy, check your institution’s or lecturer’s preference for single- or double-sided printing, or the need to use an institutional coversheet. Regardless of mode of presentation, you should always consult your institution’s requirements about line spacing and margins, as well as for indenting (i.e., the first line of each paragraph, except for the Abstract) using the tab key to ensure consistency. You should ensure your pages are numbered in the upper right-hand corner, so that if by some mischance the pages are separated or are carelessly assembled in the wrong order, it is immediately apparent and may save you marks. Use a 12-point serif font, such as Times New Roman. The APA Publication Manual expects you to use a serif font, claiming that it is easier to read in print. Many students, used to reading on a computer monitor, tablet or smartphone, tend to use sans serif fonts such as Arial. Check with your tutor or lecturer to see which they prefer. Twelve-point is the smallest easily read size for essays and reports. Some institutions insist that research reports should be exactly in the format of a submission to a journal. If this is the case at your institution, you should include a running head, repeat the title at the top of the first page of the Introduction, and have no spaces between paragraphs. Appendices. There are no consistent expectations about Appendices in undergraduate research reports, so check with your tutor or lecturer about their expectations. We recommend that for a research report you should include as Appendices all your raw data if you collected them yourself, and all statistical calculations if you have conducted them yourself. You probably do not need to include Appendices for material provided to you by teaching staff, unless you have specifically found a need to refer to it in the research report. Appendices that are included but are not referred to in the research report may be marked down. Essays do not normally require Appendices. The only exception might be if you have access to a rare or unusual document that would not be available to the average reader. In that case you could include it as an Appendix. Getting advice. Be aware that most lecturers and tutors will not usually read drafts. If you need advice on your work, you should consult your lecturer or tutor with specific questions ready. Lecturers and tutors may be available via consultation hours, although these can be limited, so do not be afraid to have questions written down. Your lecturer or tutor will appreciate your forethought. In addition, most institutions now have discussion boards accessible over the internet through programs like Blackboard or WebCT. Students can “post” questions or comments and tutors, lecturers, and fellow students can post replies. These are a very effective means of communication, since you can access them at your convenience, as can the staff member, and it saves the staff member answering the same question many times, since any other student can read the questions and answers. However, staff monitoring these discussion boards are frustrated if they do find 31 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays themselves answering the same question repeatedly, so be sure to glance through previous posts and see if your query has already been answered. Submit in a folder? This would only be relevant for hardcopy submissions, and most institutions do not require this. Check with your lecturer or tutor about whether your report should be in a folder or pocket. In any case, a report is much easier to mark if it is stapled only at the top left-hand corner. At all costs, avoid those folders where each page is in a separate sleeve. Taking pages out individually to write comments on them is guaranteed to try the fairmindedness of any marker! Make sure that your name, your tutor or lecturer’s name, the subject and year level, and day and time of your class are clearly marked on the Title Page of your report, and also on the outside of your folder. Figure 5.2 in Chapter 5 shows an example. 32 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e 3 Referencing Referencing KEYWORDS acknowledgment, citation, paraphrasing, plagiarism, primary source, quotations, reference, secondary source, Turnitin IN THIS CHAPTER 3.1 Plagiarism . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 34 3.2 In-text Referencing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36 3.3 The Reference List . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42 33 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays R Referencing EFERENCING, AND ARGUMENT OR narrative are the two components at the centre of any research (and any fact-based) assignments you will write at university. You can think of them as intertwined structures that support every paragraph and sentence, giving a coherent structure to the work as well as linking it to established knowledge on the topic of your assignment. Referencing serves an additional academic purpose in university assignments: in acknowledging sources you have read and used to complete your assignments, you demonstrate your research and reasoning skills. That is, referencing allows you to show the efforts you have made to seek out, identify, and incorporate relevant evidence in support of your claims, which is a core skill in the undergraduate psychology program (Australian Psychology Accreditation Council [APAC], 2014). In this chapter, we will discuss the mechanics of referencing within reports, and how to produce a reference list using the APA Publication Manual (6th ed.) style. Referencing is fairly uncomplicated once grasped and is identical for both essays and research reports. However, since it is ruled by the strict conventions of the APA Publication Manual, it must be adhered to or you will find your work penalised. Consequently, this chapter in this book is the one you will most likely refer to often, as it can be hard to remember the seemingly pedantic details of a referencing style. Nonetheless, it is worth bearing in mind that having an accurate reference list will help you gain a good grade for your work. However, before we get to those details, it is essential to understand the typical consequence of not referencing correctly or failing to acknowledge your sources. 3.1 Plagiarism Why it is important to avoid plagiarism. It is essential that you acknowledge other people’s ideas, work, and even opinions. Many students think that tutors and lecturers exaggerate the problems of plagiarism. Conversely, lecturers and tutors tend to think that students take the problem too lightly. The reason that academics treat plagiarism so severely is that the published word represents the product of their professional labour. If this intellectual effort is used by someone else as if it were that person’s own, it is equivalent to theft and is treated as such. Your marker will assume that if you don’t acknowledge another source, you intend the reader to believe that the ideas or opinions are your own. If they aren’t, you’ll be penalised, so be careful. What is plagiarism? There is no doubt that plagiarism has occurred if you use a slab of someone’s published work and do not acknowledge it. However, plagiarism can occur when you express someone else’s ideas and do not acknowledge them. Even when you paraphrase an opinion or description in your own words and style, you must say where the original idea or description came from. This might happen in the description of a concept, an effect or finding, or even an argument. For example, if you are making an original argument by bringing together claims and evidence proposed by another researcher, you need to clearly acknowledge the source of those ideas. 34 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Referencing 3 How plagiarism occurs. It is very possible that plagiarism, especially of ideas rather than word-for-word text, can occur inadvertently or unintentionally unless you have good research practices. For example, it is possible that, while doing your research for an assignment, you read a cogent argument which defines or links key concepts of relevance to your assignment. At the time, it was clear that this understanding was the work of the author of the article you have just read, but as you read a few more articles, you may forget which paper you read it in, and may even come to think you created the definition or put the ideas together, especially as you have now successfully written your assignment in your own words. In this case, you have unintentionally plagiarised and you are now at risk of the penalties resulting from such actions. A second, not uncommon cause of plagiarism is the poor writing practice of copying key material into your drafts with the intention of correcting the final version, including word-for-word quotes without proper acknowledgment of sources. Like the above example, this practice can lead to trouble because, while the source may be clear at the time, it is likely that later you will no longer be able to identify the quote or its source for paraphrasing or referencing. Again, it is unlikely that this practice reflects intent to plagiarise, but the outcome is exactly that, and you will again be at risk of the penalties resulting from such actions. In both of these cases, especially in an early assignment, you should be aware that lecturing and tutoring staff are not out to catch or punish you, although they will not give you credit (in marks) for other people’s work. Rather, they will adhere to their institutional policy, while trying to instruct you in good practice so that an unintentional error of this variety will not occur (or affect your results) in future. There are two further reasons plagiarism occurs, both of which are far less ambiguous. The first of these is copying from another student. Sometimes, when students are under pressure (e.g., lots of assignments, fallen behind because of work or illness), a kindly and well-intentioned student enrolled in the same unit, or who had previously completed the unit, may offer another student a copy of the completed assignment. It is unlikely that either party intends the “borrowed” assignment to be submitted as the struggling student’s own work; rather, that it might help guide the student to a quicker or more successful completion of their own assignment. However, when time is short and the pressure is high, students sometimes feel they are without options, and the need to submit something is overwhelming. The result can range from copying a phrase or two to copying the whole report, without acknowledgment – although this would be of little benefit in this case as another student’s work is not an appropriate source. In such cases, you need to be aware that your action will have jeopardised your result and the standing of the other student as well. If picked up (e.g., Turnitin will return results over multiple years), both students will likely be subject to academic honesty or integrity meetings with staff. The final and least defensible form of plagiarism is the purchasing of assignments. It is wholly unacceptable to purchase assignments from any source (e.g., individual, online, etc.) and submit them as your own work. If you choose to undertake this form of plagiarism and are caught, it is likely that you will face significant penalties for doing so. 35 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays How to avoid plagiarism. Avoiding plagiarism may be as simple as not using others’ words or ideas without appropriate acknowledgment. However, based on many years of experience with cases of plagiarism, we know that plagiarism is rarely intended and more commonly occurs as a result of one or more small errors at various points in time. For this reason, we will now discuss the riskiest stages of assignment preparation for plagiarism and how it can be avoided. Researching. It is not practical to record every idea and its source, just in case you might use it. However, you should cultivate a practice which allows you to keep track of what you have read, how it might be useful to your assignment, and any information that you might like to quote. You should avoid recording this information directly into drafts. Similarly, it is not a good idea to be drafting from original sources. For example, we advise that if you complete your researching and keep good and organised notes, you will be able to write your assignment from these and will be unlikely to unintentionally plagiarise. Paraphrasing. Be very careful when paraphrasing another author’s work. It is not enough just to change a few words around or replace them with synonyms. This is merely a subtler form of plagiarism and is identifiable. Try to understand what was meant and say it in your own style, making sure it fits with the text around it, and be sure to acknowledge the source. It can also be helpful to include an example when explaining important concepts or ideas. Although it may take up more words, it does indicate your understanding of the concept being described. Those students who convince their marker that they really understand what they have read and are not just regurgitating someone else’s work are the ones who get the better marks. See Figure 3.1 for examples of poor and better attempts at paraphrasing. Proofreading. Many institutions require that students use or submit their assignments through a software program like Turnitin, which tests for word-for-word plagiarism. These programs have the ability to check (exact and near) word-for-word similarity between your work and a large number of sources, including other students’ assignments – both past and present – internet sources (e.g., Wikipedia), online books, and journal articles. Frequently, institutions will provide you with the opportunity to submit your work to a program like this before your final submission so that you can correct inadvertent plagiarism, where you have not been sufficiently careful about paraphrasing material you’ve read. 3.2 In-text Referencing The only sure way to not plagiarise is to be diligent with in-text referencing. As we have said, the format of this can take a little getting used to. For example, there are differences in how you will acknowledge other people’s work in the main text of your writing compared 36 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Referencing 3 Original “. . . there is evidence that illusions and false-perceptions (or hallucinations) arise, at least in part, from the same mechanisms as ‘veridical’ perceptions” (Partos, Cropper, & Rawlings, 2016, p. 3). Poor Evidence exists suggesting that hallucinations and illusions partly result from the same mechanisms as true visual perception (Partos, Cropper, & Rawlings, 2016). Better (without example) Evidence suggests that the mechanisms that are responsible for accurate visual perception may also be partly responsible for illusions and hallucinations (Partos, Cropper, & Rawlings, 2016). Original “The concept of leadership is senseless when abstracted and decontextualized from the group” (Platow, Haslam, Reicher, & Steffens, 2015, p. 20). Poor Leadership, as a concept, is meaningless when is it distanced or separated from ideas about groups (Platow, Haslam, Reicher, & Steffens, 2015). Better (with example) Platow, Haslam, Reicher, and Steffens (2015) argue that the concept of leadership only makes sense if considered in conjunction with ideas about groups. For example, a leader is not a leader without people to lead. Figure 3.1. Poor and better versions drawn from an original reference. to in parentheses, and a further difference if you wish to quote an author. Then, to make things a little trickier, there are different rules about different types of sources (e.g., authored versus edited publications, and publications with many authors). The remainder of this chapter will describe each of these rules. How do you acknowledge another’s work? When summarising or paraphrasing a particular point of view, claim or argument made by another author or authors in your essay or the body of your report, primarily the Introduction and Discussion, give only the surname of the author or authors and the date of publication. For example: “Hosking (2013) stated that . . .” “According to Woodyatt, Wenzel, and Ferber (2017) . . .”. As you can see in these examples, you do not use initials, the name of the article, or the page number. This example uses the APA 6th edition style, which is different to other styles you might have encountered (e.g., MLA) that use footnotes, op. cit., and/or loc. cit. Footnotes are not used to reference in psychology essays or reports. If you are discussing ideas or concepts but what you are writing doesn’t really afford mention of the author, you can describe only the content of interest and provide the reference in parentheses at the end of the sentence. For example, “reactive aggression 37 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays can be characterised as an anger-based response to a provoking event (Denson, 2015)”. This is not a direct quote, but your own paraphrase of another author’s position. The name or names in parentheses at the end of the statement or paragraph indicate to the reader that the idea is someone else’s. If not all the ideas in the paragraph came from someone else, the onus is on you, the writer, to make clear which ideas are those of other people. Keep a sense of proportion. The research report is primarily an exercise in communication. Continual interruption of the flow of expression with parenthetical references detracts from that communication. This must be balanced against the necessity to acknowledge your sources. The best compromise when integrating material from several sources is to express the ideas in your own words with the sources in parentheses at the end of the paragraph. The references should be in alphabetical order of the surname of the first author, with the works of any one author in chronological order, should not include any page numbers, and should end with the full stop outside the parentheses. For example: (Crafti, 2014; Dickson & Bates, 2012, 2013; Peyton & Critchley, 2015). Note that when multiple authors of a single reference are mentioned in parentheses, their names are joined by an ampersand (&). In contrast, when authors are named in the flow of a sentence, they are joined by “and”. This difference is an example of one of the conventions that you need to learn. First time citing primary sources (as a function of the number of authors). The first time you cite a reference with one to five authors, you must include each author’s surname, and the year of the publication. For references with only two authors, the authors names must be joined by “and” in-text or “&” in parentheses (e.g., Price & Harmon-Jones, 2015). For references with three to five authors, each author’s surname is followed by a comma, with an “and” or “&” between the last two authors’ surnames (e.g., Williams, Giddings, Bellamy, & Gott, 2017). If your article has six or more authors, you always use “et al.” after the first author, for example, “Cox et al. (2016) . . .”. If what you are citing comes from a chapter in an edited book, cite the author of the chapter and the year of publication of the book. See Section 3.3.1 for how it is referenced in the reference list. Repeat citations. You may find you have reason to cite a paper several times in your essay or research report. For example, you may present a definition or finding from one publication in one paragraph and an alternative finding in a subsequent paragraph. Then you may attempt to compare these in a further paragraph, which would require referencing each work again. In such a case, if the work has one or two authors, you use the same format as the first time it was cited. If the work has three to five authors, you use the first author’s surname and replace the remaining authors’ surnames with the term “et al.,” followed by the date. There is an exception to this rule. If you cited the work in the text the first time, and then cite it again in the same paragraph, you do not need the date. This is not the case if the second citation was in parentheses, in which case it must include the date. The 6th edition of the APA Publication Manual is adamant that all citations in parentheses must include the date. Figure 3.2 provides an example. 38 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Referencing Initial in-text citation and repeat citation Louis, Bastian, McKimmie, and Lee (2016) argue that the importance of class attendance has been largely overlooked as a factor affecting academic performance. This may be because of the difficulties separating this issue from factors such as opportunity and motivation. However, by controlling for previous academic performance, and using a modellingbased statistical approach, it was possible to determine that class attendance significantly positively contributed to undergraduate psychology students’ academic performance (Louis et al., 2016). 3 Initial parenthetical citation and repeat citation The importance of class attendance has been largely overlooked as a factor affecting academic performance (Louis, Bastian, McKimmie, & Lee, 2016). This may be because of the difficulties separating this issue from factors such as opportunity and motivation. As a result, Louis et al. (2016) examined this relationship while controlling for previous academic performance and determined that class attendance significantly positively contributed to undergraduate psychology students’ academic performance. Figure 3.2. Examples of references repeated within a paragraph. Internet-based titles. If you are referencing an internet source, the rules are identical to those of journal articles if the author and date can be identified. If the author is not identified, use the first few words of the entry in your reference list, which is usually the title, and the year (if known) or the date of accessing it (if the article is not dated). For example, “Health issues for men (2017) . . .”. Classic works. Occasionally you will come across a republication or retranslation of an older work, typically by writers like Freud or Piaget who have written classics in the field. Cite these in text like this: “According to Freud (1923/1961) . . .” where the first date is the original publication date, and the second is the date of the book you actually used. See page 48 for how this is cited in your reference list. Figures 3.3 and 3.4 give further examples of the conventions of in-text referencing. Secondary sources. It is sometimes necessary to refer to a secondary source; that is, a reference that one of your references has referred to but whose publication you cannot get hold of yourself. In such a case, you can say “Piaget (as cited in Lilienfeld et al., 2014) found that . . .” or “According to Burton, Westen, and Kowalski (2012), Piaget’s early work indicated . . .”. In such cases, you do not include the year for the original work in the citation, and only the work that you have actually read is listed in your references. In the above examples, Lilienfeld et al. (2014) and Burton, Westen, and Kowalski (2012) should be listed in your reference list, but not the authors they refer to. 39 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Type of citation First citation in-text Second in-text citation in same paragraph Subsequent in-text citations in new paragraphs First parenthetical citation Subsequent citations Work by one author Murray (2017) Murray Murray (2017) (Murray, 2017) (Murray, 2017) Work by two authors Peters and Kashima (2015) Peters and Kashima Peters and Kashima (2015) (Peters & Kashima, 2015) (Peters & Kashima, 2015) Work by three Ward, Sullivan, Ward et al. authors and Gilmore (2016) Ward et al. (2016) (Ward, Sullivan, & Gilmore, 2016) (Ward et al., 2016) Work by four authors Williams, Giddings, Bellamy, and Gott (2017) Williams (Williams, et al. (2017) Giddings, Bellamy, & Gott, 2017) Work by five authors Giofre, Giofre et al. Giofre et al. Cumming, (2017) Fresc, Boedker, and Trescoldi (2017) (Giofre et al., (Giofre, 2017) Cumming, Fresc, Boedker, & Tressoldi, 2017) Work by six or more authors Cox et al. (2016) Cox et al. Cox et al. (2016) (Cox et al., 2016) Work by a group (readily identified through abbreviation) as authors National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH, 2017) NIMH NIMH (2017) (National (NIMH, Institute of 2017) Mental Health [NIMH], 2017) Teen depression symptoms Teen depression symptoms (2017) (Teen depression symptoms, 2017) Internet page, Teen no author, depression no date symptoms (2017) Williams et al. (Williams et al., 2017) (Cox et al., 2016) (Teen depression symptoms, 2017) Figure 3.3. Examples of in-text citation of references. 40 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Referencing 3 Multiple citations by the same author(s): If citing more than one work by the same author, put them in chronological order. If published in the same year, identify them by lower case letters after the date both in the text and in your reference list: . . . was demonstrated (Haslam, 2015, 2017). Harmon-Jones, Bastian, & Harmon-Jones (2016a, 2016b) showed that . . . Two or more works by different authors within the same parentheses: When referring to several works within the same parentheses, order them in the same order they would appear in the reference list; that is, in alphabetical order of the first author or, if there are two by the same author, then in chronological order, separated by semicolons. Researchers have explored the effect of mindfulness meditation on implicit cognition (e.g., Brown & Ryan, 2003; Crescentini & Capurso, 2015; Sauer et al., 2011). Six or more authors: Use Cox et al. (2016) in text, as in Figure 3.3. The exception is where this would be confusing if there was another reference that also contracted to Cox et al. In such a situation, give the minimum number of authors needed to avoid confusion, e.g., Cox, Tsomides, et al. (2016). That is, the use of a second author’s surname is used to differentiate references. Figure 3.4. More examples of in-text citation of references. Secondary sources of any sort are generally frowned upon when you reach the higher undergraduate years, because you should be personally checking to make sure that you are accurately describing the work of the original author, not taking someone else’s word for it. However, if your institution does not have an extensive periodical collection, or time or library policy does not permit you to obtain copies from other libraries, a small number of secondary sources are usually acceptable in first and second year. If you want to be sure how your marker (i.e., lecturer or tutor) feels about this, you should check with them. Appropriateness of sources. We briefly revisit this issue from Chapter 2 (p. 11), but since you are reporting a scientific study, your sources should be appropriately scientific. Your marker will not be impressed by references in a research report to such publications as Reader’s Digest or Women’s Weekly and will definitely take off marks if you cite Wikipedia! 41 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Even Google Scholar is very uneven in quality and should not be used. Look back at Section 2.2 for some thoughts on the quality of references. Quotations. You should try to avoid directly quoting other people’s work. The exceptions are where you can’t say it in your own words without using many more words than the original; you can’t paraphrase it without distorting the meaning; it is so memorable that you can’t capture the meaning in a paraphrase; or you need an acknowledged expert to back up a conclusion. Given the general quality of writing of many published reports, these occasions are very few and far between. If you do use a quote, ensure that it is brief. Enclose the quote in double quotation marks (“. . .”) and include the page number after the year in the in-text reference so your reader can check to see if you’ve quoted the author correctly and in context. For example, “the key ingredient to genius is productivity – large in volume, extraordinary in longevity” (Albert, 1975, p. 144). If your quote includes quotation marks within it, they should be single quotation marks, as in: “APA cannot publish previously copyrighted material that exceeds the copyright holder’s determination of ‘fair use’ without permission” (American Psychological Association, 2001, p. 122). If you are quoting an internet source that does not give page numbers, give author, year, and paragraph number, abbreviated as “para 1”, or if subheadings are used, “Discussion section, para 2”. If you feel that you really do have to include a quotation longer than 40 words, it should be presented as an indented paragraph, without quotation marks, finishing with a full stop followed by the page number in parentheses. For example: Many people favour Pope’s (1980) definition of love: A preoccupation with another person. A deeply felt desire to be with the loved one. A feeling of incompleteness without him or her. Thinking of the loved one often, whether together or apart. Separation frequently evokes feelings of genuine despair or else tantalising anticipation of reuniting. Reunion is seen as bringing feelings of euphoric ecstasy or peace and fulfilment. (p. 4) Risks and rewards of quotations. As we have already said, it is much better to paraphrase ideas in your own words, and acknowledge their source. This demonstrates that you understand what the author was saying and will be much more likely to impress your marker than large undigested chunks of other people’s work for which you may receive no marks! For the same reason, you should avoid having more than one or two quotations in any essay or research report, even if they are brief. If you have a lot of quotations, it is not always clear to your marker that you understand what has been said, and you will lose marks. 3.3 The Reference List This section comes after the Discussion, is headed References in bold, sentence case (also known as title case), centred across the page and, unlike the earlier sections in the body of the report, does begin on a new page. It is important to recognise that this section 42 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Referencing 3 is not a bibliography. A bibliography is a list of all the references you have consulted, even if you have not included them in the final report. In contrast, a reference list includes only the references you have cited in the essay or report. Do not include a bibliography in a psychology research report. What you should include in your reference list. You should put into your reference list all those references, and only those references, that you have referred to in the body of your report, and that you have actually read. If you read in your textbook that James (1890) said something, it is the textbook (i.e., the primary source) that should be in the reference list, not James (1890) (i.e., the secondary source). If your marker sees a particularly interesting or obscure reference, they might ask you for a copy of it. If you don’t have it, you will be in trouble! Similarly, if you only read an abstract in PsycINFO and think the reference will be useful in your essay or report, you should read the whole article and then include it in your reference list. Note that all types of references, journal articles, books, and book chapters are all mixed in together in a single list. The categories in this chapter have been separated, but only for your convenience in checking out the conventional format. You should include all your references in the one list. Most markers are fairly strict about adhering to the conventions for reference lists as outlined in the Publication Manual of the APA. Many students are careless about this. Although it may appear pedantic to be concerned about punctuation, the conventions are easy to learn, and a good reference list is a simple way to pick up a mark, leaving your marker with the impression that you care enough about your work to attend to detail. Order of references. The reference list is in the alphabetical order of the surname of the first author, and if needed, additional authors of each work cited in the body of your report. If there are two authors of the same surname, it goes by alphabetical order of their first initial. If you cite two works by the same author, they go in chronological order of year of publication. If you cite two works by the same author published in the same year, call them, for example, (2013a) and (2013b), in both the body of the report and add them in this order to the References section. Articles by an author alone go before those by the same author with co-authors. Articles by an author with different co-authors go in alphabetical order of the second author’s surname. You must not change the order of authors in any one article. There is an academic pecking order which makes the order of authors important, since the assumption is that the first author usually has done the greater amount of work on the article. If you don’t see why this is important, think about how you refer to your textbook. Even if it has been written by several authors, the chance is that you refer to it by the name of the first author. Since professional kudos is gained by being well known, being first author of a book or article is important! Specific examples of the conventions for listing particular types of references that you are likely to come across follow. If you wish to use some of the more exotic examples of references, see the Publication Manual of the APA for details. 43 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Format and style of references. The Publication Manual of the APA was written to tell authors the conventions of preparing a manuscript for publication, and was designed to make life easy for typesetters working for publishers of journals. That is one reason why some of the fussier conventions regarding research reports have come into being. The current expectation is that the first line of each reference is flush with the left margin, and the second and later lines are indented one tab, or about 1 cm. Other formatting features include the use of author’s surnames and initials, separated by “&” and commas, as needed. For most reference types you will use, include the title of the work, and where it was published, in standard case (i.e., not sentence case). Finally, you will also include some details about the publication. For example, for journal articles you will report the volume number, page numbers, and the digital object identifier (doi). In contrast, for books you’ll report information such as edition, volume, page numbers and publisher information. The specifics of these references will now be discussed. 3.3.1 Books An entry in the reference list should contain all the information necessary for identification and library search, including place and date of publication, publisher’s name and the edition. Note also the location of commas and full stops (periods), as well as capitalisation (i.e., a capital letter is only used for the first letter of the first word in the title), and italics (i.e., used for the publication title). Like all entries in the reference list, the first line is flush with the left margin, and all following lines within that reference are indented about 1 cm. First editions Fine, C. (2017). Testosterone Rex: Myths of sex, science, and society. New York, NY: WW Norton & Co. If a new edition Vaughan, G. M., & Hogg, M. A. (2017). Social psychology (7th ed.). Frenchs Forest, Australia: Pearson Education. An edited book Thielking, M., & Terjesen, M. D. (Eds.). (2017). Handbook of Australian school psychology: Integrating international research, practice, and policy. Cham, Switzerland: Springer Nature. A specific chapter in an edited book von Hippel, W., Ronay, R., & Maddux, W. W. (2016). Of baboons and elephants: Inequality and the evolution of immoral leadership. In J. P. Forgas, L. Jussim, & P. A. M. van Lange (Eds.) Social psychology and morality (pp. 289–303). New York, NY: Psychology Press. 44 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Referencing 3.3.2 3 Journal Articles Note that both the title of the journal and the volume number (but not the page numbers) are italicised, and sentence case is used for the name of the journal. In some references you will read, you will see a number in parentheses after the volume number. Don’t do this. Issue number should only be used when the page numbers start again from 1 for every issue of that volume of the journal. This is rarely the case for most modern psychology journals, where the page numbers are typically continuous over the whole year’s issues. You may like to check with your tutor or lecturer about their preference here, since most electronic databases link their journals by issue number rather than page number, and it is difficult to guess whether a reference you’ve been given with pages 850–865 is in issue 6 or 7 or 8. However, the Publication Manual of the APA (6th ed.) is adamant that issue n ­ umbers are not included, so doing so will not be consistent with these guidelines. Most modern journal articles are available in electronic form and are being identified with a digital object identifier (doi), which indicates that it is the archival version of the article, and specifically identifies it. In most journals, when you click on the article title and get to the web page with the abstract and publication material and a further link to either an html or a pdf version of the article, the doi is somewhere on that page. In some journals, the doi is shown in the index of articles for that issue. You are expected to include the doi in your reference list. There may not be one in older print journals (e.g., before 1985), but even many of these are being scanned and added to electronic databases and given a doi number. Journal article, one author Denson, T. (2015). Four promising psychological interventions for reducing reactive aggression. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 136–141. doi:10.1016/j. cobeha.2015.04.003 Forgas, J. (2017). Can sadness be good for you? Australian Psychologist, 52, 3–13. doi:10.1111/ap.12232 Journal article, two authors Crone, D., & Williams, L. (2017). Crowdsourcing participants for psychological research in Australia: A test of Microworkers. Australian Journal of Psychology, 69, 39–47. doi: 10.1111/ajpy.12110 Journal article, three to seven authors If there are three to seven authors, all of them need to be included in the reference list. Confusingly, in text, you need to cite all of the authors the first time you use the reference, but only if there are three, four, or five authors. From then on, they can be cited in the text as, for example, Smith et al. (2017). If there are six or more authors, then you can use Jones et al. (2016) from the very first citation. Trounson, J., Critchley, C., & Pfeifer, J. (2015). Australian attitudes toward asylum seekers: Roles of dehumanization and Social Dominance Theory. Social Behavior and Personality, 43, 1641–1656. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2015.43.10.1641 45 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays More than seven authors Increasingly, journal articles are appearing with eight or more authors. In such a case, you can cite it in the body of your text as Jones et al. (2017) from the very first citation. In the reference list, you need to include the first six authors then an ellipsis (three dots . . .) then the last author, as in the following example. Knowles, S., Austin, D., Sivanesan, S., Tye-Din, J., Leung, C., Wilson, J., . . . Hebbard, G. (2017). Relations between symptom severity, illness perceptions, visceral sensitivity, coping strategies and well-being in irritable bowel syndrome guided by the common sense model of illness. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 22, 522–534. doi:10.1080/135 48506.2016.1168932 Article in an internet-only or open-access journal Buzwell, S., Farrugia, M., & Williams, J. (2016). Students’ voice regarding important characteristics of online and face-to-face higher education. Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain, and Culture, 12, 38–49. doi: dx.doi.org/10.7790/sa.v12i1.430 This article would be cited in the body of your text in the same way you would cite any journal article; that is, as Buzwell, Farrugia, and Williams (2016). Journal article accepted for publication, but not yet published. This convention applies to articles that a publisher has accepted but that have not yet actually been published. You are unlikely to come across these unless your tutor or lecturer gives you one of their own! This example is for a chapter in a book, but if it was a journal article, you would simply have the name of the journal, with major words capitalised, after the title. Keen, J., & Fitness, J. (in press). Can employers screen job applicants for potential white collar fraud offenders? In S. Boag & N. Tiliopoulos (Eds.), Personality and individual differences: Theory, assessment, and application. New York: Nova. Periodical published annually. Treat annually published series, such as the Annual Review of Psychology, that have specified, regular publication dates as periodicals, not as books. Broadbent, E. (2017). Interactions with robots: The truths we reveal about ourselves. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 627–652. doi: 10.1146/annurevpsych-010416-043958 Conference papers Sebastian, P., Kaufmann, L. M., & de la Piedad Garcia, X. (2017, January). In the nose, not in the beholder: Embodied cognition effects override individual differences. Embodied Cognition Preconference at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Psychology and Social Psychology. San Antonio, Texas, United States. 46 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Referencing 3 Abstract only If you find an interesting abstract using PsycINFO, you need to admit that you’ve only seen the abstract, not the entire article. Many such abstracts are doctoral dissertations. Brewster, M. K. (2000). Entering and exiting the corporation: A developmental study of women executives at mid-life. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering, Vol. 60(9-B), 4877. 3.3.3 Internet Sources Articles obtained from the internet. The APA Publication Manual (6th ed.) has included advice for referencing a large number of possible types of material obtained from the internet, and the APA also has a guide to electronic references, which is available, at a cost, on the web. Here is how you should reference the ones you are more likely to need. Document created by a private individual or organisation. If the article has a date of posting, use this format: Smith, A. (2016, September 26). This is the title. Retrieved from http://www.etc If there is no date of posting, use the date of retrieval and make it specific, thus: Jones, S. (n.d.). This is the title. Retrieved March 11, 2017, from http://www.etc In text, both of the above would be referenced like any other source with a clear author and date, as Smith (2016) and Jones (2017). If there is no obvious author, use any reasonable title from the site, as follows, for the reference list: Monash transition program. (2017). Retrieved from www.monash.edu.au/study/ parents/ transition Note that for sites maintained by universities, the date is usually available at the foot of the page, and you would reference this page in your text as “Monash transition program (2017)”. Sometimes you will find a page you wish to cite, but it has neither author nor obvious date. For example: Marriage equality facts at a glance (n.d.). Retrieved August 2017, from http://www. australianmarriageequality.org/get-informed/quick-facts/ In this case, the in-text reference would be enough of the title to allow your marker to find it in your reference list, with the retrieval date as the date, thus “Marriage equality facts at a glance (2017)”. The problem with using such a source is that there is no 47 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays guarantee that the page will not have changed when your reader looks it up, and so your use of it may not be able to be checked. Be sparing in using such sources. If you wish to reference a website but not a specific document within that site, you need only give the URL in the text, and not have it in the reference list at all. For example, you might say: An amusing site for aficionados of psychological tests is http://www.queendom.com. eBooks. Increasingly, books are available electronically, as eBooks. Sometimes they have a doi, but many do not. You’ve probably come across them in your library, but the university library web address for the book is probably four lines long. It is probably simpler to look it up on Google and find the publisher’s URL for that specific eBook and use it as follows: Bell, N. (2015) We are not amused. Failed humor in interaction. Retrieved from https:// www.degruyter.com/view/product/449175 If you can find a doi for an electronic book, the reference entry is simpler: Schiraldi, G. R. (2001). The post-traumatic stress disorder sourcebook: A guide to healing, recovery, and growth [Adobe Digital Editions version]. doi:10.1036/ 0071393722 You may also come across an electronic version of a republished book, especially from seminal authors such as Freud. Here is how you reference them: Freud, S. (1953). The method of interpreting dreams: An analysis of a specimen dream. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.). The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 4, pp. 96–121). Retrieved from http://books.google.com/books (Original work published 1900). 3.3.4 Other Sources Entry in an online encyclopaedia Haybron, D. (2011). Happiness. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Retrieved June 20, 2017, from http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/ happiness/ Unpublished theses These are often catalogued and available in the library from graduate students at your university and are a good source of recent research. Mizuno, H. (2017). Establishing and validating a measure of the Japanese concept of self. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Swinburne University of Technology, Melbourne, Australia. 48 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Referencing 3 Government reports Australian Bureau of Statistics (2015, August 8) Alcohol consumption. Retrieved from http://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/Lookup/by%20Subject/4364.0.55.001~201415~Main%20Features~Alcohol%20consumption~25 In the text, the first time this is mentioned, it would be cited as Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS, 2015), and the next time as ABS (2015). Newspaper articles You should never (or almost never) cite a newspaper article in a research report, but you may use one in an essay or other piece of assessment. Miller, N. (2017, May 20). UK migration squeeze hits Australians. The Age, p. 14. This would be cited in the body of your essay as Miller (2017), just like any journal article. Magazine article Again, you should never include a magazine citation in a research report, but the format is very similar to a newspaper article. Palmer J., & Gundlock, B. (2017). Dying of thirst. Good Weekend, pp. 14–19. Please note carefully in each of the above examples which parts are italicised, which words are given upper case letters and where there are punctuation marks. 49 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e This page intentionally left blank. 4 Research Reports – How Do You Start? KEYWORDS aims, dependent variable, endnote, ethics, experiment, extensions, hypothesis, Research Reports – How Do You Start? independent variable, manipulation, operationalisation, participant, replication, research ethics, study, survey, theory IN THIS CHAPTER 4.1 How to Approach a Research Report . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 53 4.2 How to Begin . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 54 4.3 Research Ethics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 60 4.4 Where to Go From Here . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62 51 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Research Reports – How Do You Start? The Process of Writing a Research Report Figure 4.1. The process of writing a research report. 52 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Research Repor ts – How Do You Star t ? 4 W ITH ANY RESEARCH REPORT, the starting point is: “What burning question do I want to answer?” From this starting point, a researcher will look back to the source of the question and then begin to identify the mechanics of answering it. 4.1 How to Approach a Research Report There are two general approaches to writing research reports. The first approach is to write the type of report you had in mind when you designed the study. Many psychologists, like other scientists, subscribe to the belief that advances in knowledge occur when a scientist, working from an existing body of knowledge, makes hypotheses about what would happen in particular situations. These hypotheses are then tested and the research report is written to publish the results of that experiment, indicating whether the original hypotheses were supported or not. If science always worked in a linear fashion, you could write the report before you even did the study, just leaving blanks for the results, with two Discussions, one for if the hypotheses were supported, and one for if the hypothesis were not supported. However, in the real world of the social sciences, things do not always work so simply. This can be because people are rarely so straightforward, and the concepts being examined are not always predictable. Consequently, research frequently leads to findings beyond those hypothesised. For this reason, we acknowledge a second approach to writing research reports, which is to write the report that makes the most sense after you’ve seen the results of your experiment. Writing the report based on known results can be criticised as being opportunistic or even unscientific, as it can encourage practices of “data dredging” or fishing, which ignores the theoretical background or even the motivating question of the research in favour of finding significant results. However, this isn’t always the case. Rather, theoretically guided examination of the results before writing the report can mean you have a comprehensive understanding of your data and the relationships in it, and that you don’t ignore interesting results that might not have been anticipated. Choosing which approach to use will be guided by your lecturer or tutor, but there are a couple of considerations. Of course, if your original hypotheses are important tests of one theory over another, you wouldn’t change the tone of your report to suit your results. On the other hand, many studies, especially in the area of people’s social behaviour, begin with “I wonder if . . .”, and it makes good sense to reorient your writing to the interesting things you have found. In this case, you are still expected to find theoretical justifications for your hypotheses in prior research. You may just have to look a bit harder to find them. As an undergraduate, you will rarely be required to design your own studies. Typically, your lecturer or tutor will run an experiment with your class as participants or will give you instructions on testing other people. The results will be collated by your lecturer or tutor and presented to your class to be used to test hypotheses. In this situation you 53 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays might like, as Kidder and Judd (1986) suggest, to regard your data as uncut jewels. Your job is to cut and polish them, selecting and highlighting the best facets, and craft the best settings. In spite of the apparent lack of flexibility of the conventional format, good report writing does allow ample scope for the use of good judgment and skill. 4.2 How to Begin The first thing is to decide what your general research question is, and then derive one or more hypotheses from it. Remember that a hypothesis is a specific, testable prediction of what you expect to happen when you run the study. Once you have decided on your question, you need to identify the research context for your question. You will then be in a position to build the links from this broad context to your specific hypothesis/es. This will also be true if you are given hypotheses, which may be the case for first-year research reports. 4.2.1 Studies Designed by the Psychology Department Many of the research reports you will have to write will be reports on studies which have already been designed by someone else. You will have been a participant in the study run by your tutor or lecturer, or you will have been given instructions on how to run the study on a participant or participants of your choosing. You will therefore be aware of what was actually done. This situation of having been a participant and the researcher is not typical in research, but it is essential to your training in psychology. However, when you come to write up the report, you are expected to do write the report as the researcher who designed and ran the whole study. Keep in mind, however, the need for objective writing. Reading. Begin with the major (key or assigned) reading. It is not unusual for student assignments to be replications of those that earlier researchers have published. That is, they are studies that exactly follow the procedures and materials used in research with the aim to provide students with the opportunity to understand the methods and approach of these studies. You will be referred to the journal article where the report originally appeared, and possibly to other related and more recent articles. However, we suggest you begin by reading the relevant section in your textbook for an overview and context of the area. It may also provide a guide for other useful references. Then carefully read the article to be replicated and any others given to you. You should see what the authors hypothesised and why, as well as whether their hypotheses were supported. You will need to read this article (and any others) critically. For example, before you get to the Method section of any particular report, ask yourself: “If I had to do an experiment to test their hypotheses, what would I do?” Then, when you’ve read the rest of the report, ask: “Does the Method actually test the 54 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Research Repor ts – How Do You Star t ? 4 hypotheses?”, “Would I draw the same conclusion from those results?”, “If not, how would I interpret them?”, “Is there any other explanation that would give the same results?” Make notes as you go. The sort of notes you should make will be dealt with later in this chapter. Developing aims and hypotheses. On the basis of your reading (and information provided by teaching staff), decide what the general aim of your study should be, and what specific results you expect to obtain when you replicate the experiment. Typically, you can expect the results to be similar to those of the original study, especially if the original participants were also university students. However, it may be that there is something different about the experiment in your study. Perhaps your study was undertaken in a different context or culture to that of the original, or your participants differed in gender or age to the original participants, or your method or experimental manipulation differs (e.g., even in subtle ways such as online versus face-to-face data collection). You might therefore expect a different result. Write your expectations in concrete and specific terms. These are your hypotheses. Figure 4.2 shows possible aims and hypotheses. Please do not confuse the two. Aims are general statements of the goals of the study, while hypotheses are specific predictions of what you expected to happen. Because you are reporting on a study that has already been performed, write your aims and hypotheses in the past tense. Aim: The aim of this study was to confirm that a conformity effect occurs during group discussion. Hypothesis: It was predicted that the range of opinions would be reduced after group discussion, compared with the pre-discussion range of opinions. Aim: The present study measured the differences in the ability of participants of varying ages to recall lists of digits. Hypothesis: It was hypothesised that for children and adolescents the number of digits able to be recalled would increase with the age of the participant. Aim: The aim of the current study was to produce the serial-position effect with free recall of a list of words. Hypothesis: The hypothesis was that words at the beginning and end of the list would be recalled by more participants than words in the middle of the list. Figure 4.2. Examples of possible aims and their corresponding hypotheses. Note: The aims are general statements of intent, but the hypotheses are worded in terms of the specific variables being measured. Also note that they are written in the past tense. 55 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays More reading. If other journal articles are recommended by your tutor or lecturer, read them and identify their relevance to your study. If no other articles are specifically provided, look at the reference list of the major article and see what articles are referred to in the argument of the reading you were assigned (and consider if these pieces are relevant to your study). You could also look to see if there are more recent articles that cite your major article. This can be achieved by doing a cited by search. Specifically, in many databases (e.g., PsychINFO, Scopus, Web of Science, Google Scholar) there is a “cited by” link which will produce a list of all of the articles in that database that have cited the paper you are looking at. This will include articles that have cited your paper for any reason, which may be of little relevance but can be searched for more recent relevant articles. The assessor of your research report will be impressed if you have looked for and found other relevant publications, and have made pertinent comments about them and their relationship to your study. Researching. There are a number of reference sources you can use when searching for information on a particular subject in psychology. Most libraries will have searchable research databases, including Scopus, PsycINFO, Academic Search Complete, and PsycARTICLES, which will allow you to search for psychological publications. These may include articles from the distant past, although you should generally confine yourself to recent articles. If you have access to these databases, you will find they are extremely useful in tracing articles by author, subject, or keyword. Most libraries regularly run training courses on how to search databases. Make use of them. Books. You will also find books referred to in reference lists. These are often hard to get hold of, and it is not usually cost-effective in terms of your time to run around looking for them. Use them if you can get them easily, since they often have overviews of the subject you are interested in, but journal articles are usually more accessible and up to date. How many references? (Or when to stop researching). Students frequently ask how many references they need for a research report. There is no simple answer to this. You should absolutely use the references you were given, and probably a couple more, as a minimum. Some departments may also have expectations (e.g., at least two additional references), so check with your tutor or lecturer. It is generally better to deal with a few references thoroughly than to superficially use many. We explore this in more detail in Chapter 5. Taking notes on references. Taking notes on what you read will help you keep track of what you have read, and build a clear understanding of the topic. You can make notes in a number of different ways. One way is to print or photocopy the relevant articles, read them carefully and highlight the most important parts, such as hypotheses, participants, the design of the experiment, and conclusions about whether the hypotheses were supported or not. The danger with doing this is that you are tempted to highlight anything even vaguely interesting. This approach does 56 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Research Repor ts – How Do You Star t ? 4 not usually reduce the information to manageable amounts, means you have to refer back to and maybe even re-read the paper itself to find this information when you need it, and increases the dangers of plagiarism. If you choose this approach, be strict about highlighting only material immediately relevant to your own study. In the interests of saving paper, you can usually download the articles as pdf files, read them on your computer or tablet, if you are comfortable with this, and only print the most relevant page(s) as well as the first page, which has the information you will need to reference the article. Another very effective way of making notes about articles you read is to create a summary table (e.g., in Microsoft Excel or in Word) which includes the citation precisely as it will appear in your reference list (see Chapter 5) and a brief summary of the relevant parts of the study using the major subheadings: type of participants; what was done; what was found; what was concluded; what you think is a better or different interpretation; and anything that the author omitted or questions the study raised. You should also decide whether the paper, as a whole, supports (or not) the study you are replicating. This could be done by colour-coding to help you clearly identify these papers. A slightly more sophisticated yet similar approach to the references tables is to make notes about relevant journal articles using one of the flexible databases on your computer. Many students find that having to type in the relevant information from articles is a more effective way of absorbing information than simply highlighting a photocopy. This system also allows you to cut-and-paste references from one document to the other and so saves some typing time. If you have access to a bibliographic program, such as Endnote or ProCite, this will make producing a good reference list much easier. This is a valuable skill to learn if you intend to go on to further study after your undergraduate degree, since you will then be expected to write a thesis, which is essentially a very large research report. 4.2.2 Studies You Design Your approach to preparing for a research report will be a little different if you have the freedom to design your own study. If you have this freedom, first look at what constraints are placed on you. For example, what is the general area you are expected to work in? How much time do you have? Are you expected to test something experimentally, or is a survey study acceptable? Once you have decided on the scope and type of study, ask yourself what question or questions you want answered. What answers would you expect? Then have a look through some recent journals in that subject area to see what other researchers have done. Look at Scopus, PsycINFO, Academic Search Complete, or PsycARTICLES under the subject area. If any articles look interesting, look at their reference sections for clues to similar works. Read and make notes on anything that looks as if it is relevant to what you want to do, especially if it supports what you expect to happen in your study (but don’t ignore contrary results; they’ll be handy if your hypotheses are not supported!). 57 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Make notes as mentioned in the previous section, and finally decide what you really predict will happen from the study as you conceive it. It is also good to make use of your lecturer or tutor’s consultation time or online discussion time to check you are on track. Once you have the context and aims of the study, you will develop hypotheses which can be justified on the basis of prior research. Your study design will then follow from your hypotheses. For example, a study predicting a difference between two groups means you intend to collect data from two identifiable groups on the variable you predict will differ. Your hypotheses will also need to be operationalised in such a way that it is practical to collect the data. That is, if the study is to be a questionnaire study, the scales you want to use need to be available. If the study is an experimental task, the facilities need to be available, as does a means of getting participants to become involved. You may have to apply to the ethics committee for permission to run this study, although if ethics approval is required, your lecturer will likely have already commenced this process. We discuss ethics in more detail in Section 4.3. Given the complexities of study design, it is exceedingly unlikely that you will be expected to design and run your own study in the first couple of years of an undergraduate degree in psychology. When you are expected to do this, you should be given a good deal of guidance. If you feel you don’t have enough guidance, ask for assistance. 4.2.3 All Studies – A Brief Introduction to Key Concepts Any study you conduct will be based on a literature and will need to reflect this. It is also important that your hypotheses derive from your researching and are specific and concrete. They must be expressed in terms that make it clear how you are going to measure the theoretical concepts you are interested in. Sometimes it is hard to operationalise hypotheses; that is, to formulate precise, concise, testable statements of what your theory would predict. You may find, if you are comparing groups of people, that it is useful to draw up a table of the independent and dependent variables. You can then be more specific in saying what you predict will happen. Such a table is illustrated in Figure 4.3. Independent variables (or IVs). Remember, independent variables are defined as variables that are theoretically unaffected by (i.e., independent of) the other variables in the Age (Independent) Younger Gender (Independent) Female Male Reaction time (in ms) Reaction time (in ms) Older Reaction time (in ms) Reaction time (in ms) Figure 4.3. Example of a matrix of independent and dependent variables. Note: Reaction time is the dependent variable. Age and gender are independent variables. 58 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Research Repor ts – How Do You Star t ? 4 study. Consequently, we often hypothesise that independent variables have some causal influence on whatever is being studied when undertaking experimental or quasi-experimental research. An independent variable can be varied or manipulated by you, the experimenter. This can be done by selecting categories of participants, such as people who differ in gender (men and women) or age (adolescent and middle-aged). Sometimes the independent variable is simply allowed to vary, such as a score on intelligence or satisfaction with body image, which is then measured and used to define the groups (e.g., high versus low scorers). Both of these approaches are considered quasi-experimental as the categories already exist prior to the experiment. This may mean that any differences found between groups (such as men compared to women) may be a result of gender, or some other variable also related to gender. Experimental studies use independent variables which created different groups of participants by randomly assigning individuals from the same group (e.g., students) to different conditions (i.e., manipulation). As a result, there were no predicted differences between the created groups prior to the manipulation, so any difference between these after the manipulation is most likely to be caused by the condition or manipulation. Dependent variables (or DVs). Dependent variables are variables that are affected by another variable, and are typically the main interest of the research. They are called “dependent” variables because you are hoping that the value of the dependent variable depends on your manipulation of the independent variables. As mentioned earlier, it is unlikely that you will have to design and run your own study before your second, or even your third year of psychology. It is far more likely that you have either been a participant in a study run by your tutor or lecturer, or have tested one or more participants using instructions from your tutor or lecturer. You will therefore know how the study was conducted and will be in possession of a description of the method, and the results of the study in the form of raw data or perhaps some form of summary data. You should note, however, that your experience as a participant or researcher may reflect only one aspect of the research (i.e., you only experienced one level of the independent variable, such as the control or the experimental group). The instructions you receive should make that clear, but in order to write your report, you will need to make sure you have all the details and understand the design of the research, including all conditions. Correlational studies. Strictly speaking, there are no independent and dependent variables in correlational studies. This is because correlational studies do not provide information about causation. Rather, correlational research can be used to identify the relationship between variables which may or may not be causally related. For example, by measuring the relationship between age and weight in children, it would be possible to find a significant positive correlation indicating that older children weigh more, and younger children weigh less. However, it is not possible to determine from this method that increasing age causes weight gain, and it is pretty clear from general knowledge that age is not the cause. Rather, growing and developing, itself highly correlated with age, is 59 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays likely to be the cause of weight gain, as well as increased height, strength, and even verbal intelligence. Similarly, a correlational study of education and knowledge may find a significant positive relationship between these variables, which may be tempting to describe as the effect of education on knowledge (i.e., causal, with education as the independent variable and knowledge as the dependent variable). However, this has more in common with the age–weight study we just described than it does with an experiment. That is, you may observe a relationship, but it is not possible to determine causation or even the independence and dependence of these variables. Thus, in correlational research, variables are not described as independent or dependent. 4.3 Research Ethics Any psychological study undertaken as research should have been reviewed by your departmental or university ethics committee. This means that studies run for the sole purpose of your teaching or learning may not have been reviewed by an ethics committee. However, such teaching and learning activities are typically reviewed by teaching and learning or curriculum committees. The purpose of ethics and curriculum reviews are to ensure the quality and appropriateness of activities for students, to protect the rights and well-being of participants, and to support and guide researchers and research when relevant. Research ethics is a formal process developed to reflect and uphold the values and principles that protect all of the people involved (i.e., participants, researchers, and consumers of research) and enhance the acquisition of knowledge and understanding. From the perspectives of participant and researcher, there are four cardinal rules of research ethics. The first is that participants’ agency and autonomy must be respected. This is achieved by informing participants of what they can expect, which allows them to freely give their consent to participate. Second, participants should be told (and it should be true!) that they can withdraw from the study at any time, without penalty. Third, consideration is given to the information obtained from participants. Specifically, confidentiality and privacy must be respected whenever possible, which means individuals should not be able to be identified from what is reported in the study without their express permission. Finally, any risks and benefits from the research must be balanced. For example, a study involving little or no predictable risks (e.g., a survey of attitudes to environmentally friendly cleaning products) requires fewer benefits than a study involving higher risks (e.g., perceptions of physical pain from exposure to very cold water). In the latter, a risk-management strategy would be essential, and participants would likely be compensated (e.g., a small monetary payment). These considerations seek to ensure that participants should suffer no ill effects from taking part in the study; that if for any reason participants do suffer distress, mechanisms are in place to deal with them; and if there are manageable risks to participants, that they are compensated at a level that does not compromise their ability to freely choose to participate. For example, participation is never paid at rates to induce participation. By the time you encounter a project, these considerations should have already been undertaken, and implemented using the resources of the psychology department. 60 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Research Repor ts – How Do You Star t ? 4 As a participant in a study, these considerations are given to you. You may feel somewhat pressured to participate as you will have to write up a report on the study as part of your educational experience. However, if this research is undertaken within your course, you should be afforded the opportunity to learn (i.e., participate) and the opportunity to choose to withdraw your data from the research. This usually involves seeking your explicit consent for the use of your responses either before or after you have completed participation, which will allow you to participate in the learning experience and preserve your choice to participate (or not) in the research. This is because research reports are an important part of a psychology training and being a participant in studies lets you discover how participants in your studies may feel if you get to the stage of running your own. It also gives you an idea of the ambiguities that participants in studies encounter, which will help you write your report. If answering questionnaires or taking part in studies is a turn-off, you might wish to reconsider your ambition to become a psychologist. Having said that, if you genuinely and sincerely feel uncomfortable about participating in a particular study, you are at liberty not to take part as a participant, and your tutor or lecturer will respect that right. Likewise, if you are the only 56-year-old male in the class and are concerned about confidentiality, do not feel obliged to hand in the questionnaire; or you could discuss with your lecturer how they will protect your anonymity (e.g., you or your lecturer could omit your age from the dataset). However, regardless of your participation, you are still expected to write the research report based on the study – that is part of your course requirement, and you will be provided with the required materials to complete the report. In situations where the study is an experimental task, if you feel uncomfortable about being involved as a participant it is usually possible to be allocated as a support person – a timer, a counter or an “instructor”. If you are feeling distressed or pressured, you should raise your concern with your tutor or lecturer, since others in the class may feel the same but be inhibited about speaking out. If you don’t wish to do this publicly, you should excuse yourself from the relevant class and contact your lecturer or tutor (e.g., via email). In our experience, it is rare that a classroom study is this uncomfortable for students. In the unlikely case that you feel your concerns are not treated appropriately, talk to the coordinator of the course. We say “unlikely”, because the tutor or lecturer is usually more concerned about ethics than most students. Ethics become even more important if you are collecting data from people outside the university. If you are interviewing a child, you will probably be given strict instructions about obtaining permission from the child’s parents, gaining the child’s confidence, and discontinuing the interview if the child becomes uncomfortable. People are more important than your study! Such experiences of having to meet the concerns of people in the real world are good practice for anyone wishing to become either a psychologist or a market researcher, and will allow you to view with a critical or perhaps an admiring eye some of the research you will read. You will usually be introduced to the ethics of research very early in your undergraduate course. Such an introduction may mention some (historical) failures, grey areas (e.g., use of deception), and exceptions to the rules, all of which are uncommon in 61 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays modern psychology research and unlikely to apply to undergraduate studies. The Australian Psychological Society (APS) encapsulates its expectations of the ethical behaviour of psychologists, both as researchers and practitioners, in a small booklet called Code of Ethics (APS, 2017), downloadable from the Society’s website (https:// www.psychology.org.au/about/ethics/) as a 32-page pdf. 4.4 Where to Go From Here What belongs in each section of the report will be dealt with at some length in Chapter 5. In the present section we deal with the general process of writing the research report, assuming you have already participated in some way in the study and have some data. Hypotheses. As a participant, it is unlikely that you were aware of the actual hypotheses at the time the study was run. It is also common practice for you, as an experimenter, not to be told the hypotheses at the time you test some other participant, which will help you not to influence the results. The hypotheses will become clear from your reading about the study you are replicating or from other reading, and from reflecting on what the study actually tested. By deciding on your hypotheses, you have already largely determined the shape of your report. Your Introduction must consist of a logical argument for why you have made those particular predictions. Your Method must describe a study that appropriately tested those predictions. In your Results section you need report only those results that are relevant to your hypotheses. Finally, your Discussion must say whether or not your hypotheses were supported, then interpret them and discuss their implications in relation to the literature which motivated them. Get something down in writing. You should begin by writing an outline of the structure of your report. The best research reports are thoroughly planned, which means you will have a good idea of the ideal length of each section, and ensure you don’t get caught up writing one section and end up leaving too little of the word count to complete the Discussion! A good starting point is to jot down the answers to the following questions: • • • • • What is the specific aim or purpose of studies in this area or topic? Which previous study or studies is this one like, and in what way? What did previous studies find, and am I predicting similar findings? What other studies relate specifically to my predictions? What is the overall aim of my study? We have used the first person pronouns here to reinforce the pretence that you have designed the study. The answers to these questions will form the Introduction to your study. 62 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Research Repor ts – How Do You Star t ? 4 The Method section is an answer to the questions: • Who were the participants and how were they recruited? • What materials were used in the conduct of the study? • How was the study conducted? To prepare to write the Results section, the overall question you need to ask is: • What results, including data and tests, are specifically relevant to my hypotheses? Finally, in preparing the Discussion, you need to answer these questions: • • • • • Do my results support my hypotheses? What do my results mean? How do they relate to the previous studies I’ve mentioned? What are the implications of my results for real-world situations? How could my study have been improved? The answers to all the above questions need to be tidied up, expanded and separated into the conventional sections of the research report, but they are the essentials of the report. You should note that the information in these sections is related, but never repeated. It is often difficult for students to grasp just which elements of a study are the important ones, especially if it has not been designed by the student. A plan helps to sort out your priorities. The next step is to start writing the report itself. Often just getting started is the most difficult part. You should get something down in writing as soon as possible. You can later edit, rewrite, expand, and tidy it up. Length of each section. Students frequently ask how long each section of the report should be. There are no formal rules about this, but a rule of thumb is that the Introduction should be about a third of the length of the total report. Although the lengths can vary with the type of study, the remaining sections are often roughly equal to each other in length. Word limits. We advise that, in an environment of online and electronic submissions, word limits are law and should be strictly adhered to. For example, if you are given a range, including a word limit with an allowance (e.g., 6 10%), then you should absolutely keep to this range. If you are given an upper limit, then this is the longest that your report should be. You should not assume there will be lenience or that your marker will not check. This can also be true for hardcopy reports, as markers may choose to count words, either by hand or via electronic scanning. It is also obvious when students submit a very long report, or a report with very narrow margins or tiny fonts, or that is not double-spaced. Penalties for long reports are almost certainly applied and may include a percent deducted from your final result or a percent deducted for each 10, 50, 100 words over the 63 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays limit. Alternatively, a marker may choose not to read the excess words, meaning any content not read is also not marked. For example, if five marks are awarded from a conclusion and yours is outside the word count, then you may not receive those marks. What do I have to do to get a Pass? Experience indicates that the average student needs to complete the following tasks satisfactorily in order to get a grade of clear Pass for their report: • Obtain and report on sufficient relevant references (at least those you were • • • • • • • • given, with some institutions expecting a minimum of additional references in the first year, so check with your tutor or lecturer). Develop and clearly state the hypotheses that your study actually tests. Calculate or identify the appropriate statistics and decide if your hypotheses are supported or not. Write an outline of your report as indicated above. Write a first draft, which may be pretty rough. Get your first draft read by another person, such as a friend, parent, or sibling who is not a psychology student. This is to ensure that your Introduction is a coherent argument for your hypotheses, that the Method and Results are understandable, and that the Discussion makes sense of what the results have told you. On the basis of feedback on your first draft, especially about whether it makes sense, add, subtract, or otherwise alter your draft. Type it up, and format it correctly (e.g., double-spaced, APA headings, cover page with your name and the report title as a minimum). Get the final draft proofread (checking that the reference list meets the conventions), tidy up, and submit on time (which might actually be 24 hours before it is due if you need a report from a program such as Turnitin). Redrafts. You should never submit the first draft of any written piece of assessment for marking if you are serious about your work or marks! The chances of producing even a sentence that is well written the first time are vanishingly small, let alone an entire report. An average student can improve their work by redrafting the work at least twice. How often you do that depends on how quickly you work, how important a good mark is to you, and how patient your proofreader is. However, the process of reviewing and redrafting is also subject to the law of diminishing returns, which says that you have to put a lot more work in each time to get only a slightly better mark. Still, with thoughtful redrafting, even average students can lift their marks each time the report is redrafted. Proofreading. If you do not have the luxury of someone else as a proofreader, time your work so that you leave 24 hours between writing something and proofreading it yourself. It is very easy in the heat of creation to read what you meant to say instead of what you 64 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Research Repor ts – How Do You Star t ? 4 have actually written and so overlook what may be quite gross errors of expression or logic. Your assessor can mark only what you have written, not what you meant to write! Time management. Time management is more thoroughly addressed in Section 2.9. However, it is worth saying that, although it is easier said than done, plan your time so that you allow more time than you might think you need. University timetables dictate that you will have assignments in several subjects due around or at the same time. Murphy’s Law suggests that if you leave everything until the last minute, the computer will choose that time to have a fatal breakdown, or the dog will chew up your final draft just as you sit down to type it up. Consequently, time your work to leave at least 48 hours between getting it finished and handing it in. That way, if there are any catastrophes, you do have some time up your sleeve in order to correct their effects. For similar reasons, back up your documents frequently, and in different places, such as a USB stick or cloud storage (e.g., Dropbox), as well as on your computer. You should aim to space your work time so that it can be done in several small chunks. Cooperate with other students to minimise the hassles of finding references and allow yourself time to discuss them with your colleagues (NB: this is a fun part!). Start early enough so that if you have to get advice from staff you can do so during their consultation times and make use of online discussion boards. Most student consultations with staff seem to revolve around statistics. You should have tackled these early on. Asking questions about statistical problems at the last minute indicates that your planning is poor, and tests the fairmindedness of your tutor or lecturer, who probably got where they are by being on top of their assignments when they were in your shoes! Extensions. Although institutions will grant extensions of time for genuinely unforeseen and uncontrollable difficulties, this does not generally include pressure of other demands, either paid employment or assignments in other subjects. In addition, most universities require you to submit supporting documentation on any problems you have. If your computer fails at the last minute, be prepared to show your tutor or lecturer your last draft or the USB with the final version on it. If you are encountering personal or academic problems that you feel are likely to affect the quality or timing of your work, talk to your tutor or lecturer early. Do not let problems pile up and overwhelm you. All universities have student counselling departments that can help you deal with problems. You might feel that you are unique, and you may be right, but it is very likely the counsellors have helped many students with similar problems in the past, and they can help you, too. Documentation from the counselling unit to your tutor or lecturer can be used to support extensions and does not need to include any personal or intimate details. Such services are free. Make use of them. Plagiarism checks. If you have the opportunity to run your research report through Turnitin or another plagiarism checker, do so. In the process of drafting and redrafting your report, it is easy to reword a sentence or two back to very much what the original 65 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays reference said. Turnitin will pick this up and allow you to correct it, thus saving you from trouble with your marker or your university’s Academic Honesty Policy. For more information on plagiarism, see Section 3.1 in the previous chapter. Copies of your report or essay. Always keep a copy of your final submission. It is a good idea to include the date in the filename of each draft, so that it is clear which is the final version. Most institutions have a policy that assignments gaining very high or very low marks are to be double-marked. Hopefully yours will be the former, but the policy results in reports being handed on by one staff member to another, and so they may occasionally be misplaced and you may be asked to produce a copy of your submitted work. In the next chapter we give detailed advice about the various sections of a research report, including examples of the conventions for writing each part. 66 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e 5 Sections of a Research Report KEYWORDS aims, apparatus, bibliography, construct, data, debriefing, degrees of freedom, dependent variable, descriptive statistics, design, figures, hypothesis, implicit measures, independent variable, inferential statistics, measurement, participant, post hoc, proof/prove, quantitative, sample, scale, self-report measures, significance level, statistic, tables, variable IN THIS CHAPTER Title Page and Abstract . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68 5.2 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72 5.3 Method . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77 5.4 Results Sections of a Research Report 5.1 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87 5.5 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100 5.6 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105 5.7 Appendices . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105 5.8 Further Reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106 5.9 Feedback . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 107 5.10 Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108 5.11 Checklist . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108 67 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays T HE PARTS OF THE report, in the order that they should appear in your final version, are as follows: • • • • • • • Title Page and Abstract Introduction Method Results Discussion References Appendices That is the order in which they will be treated in this chapter. However, you may find it easier to draft them in some other order. For instance, the Method and Results sections follow a set structure for presenting straightforward factual material. Specifically, the Method describes who the participants were, what materials were used, and the procedure the researcher used. The Results section describes the data you collected and the outcome of the analyses that were performed to test the hypotheses. For this reason, you may find it easier to write these two sections first, although you will have specified your hypotheses and completed (or reviewed) the analyses that test those hypotheses before you can do so. 5.1 Title Page and Abstract Title Page. The Title Page should contain a brief but comprehensive title that encapsu- Sections of a Research Report lates the essence of the research conducted. In the academic world many important reference sources, such as the Current Contents database, list only the titles of articles, which demonstrates how critical it is that a title accurately captures the research. This is best achieved by a title that includes the independent and dependent variables of your research, and the way they are related. Some real examples of article titles are shown in Figure 5.1. The title should be centred horizontally on the title page, and about a third of the way down the page. The APA Publication Manual (6th ed.) indicates the title should be in sentence case (i.e., capitalise important words including nouns, pronouns, adjectives, verbs, and adverbs, and use lowercase for conjunctions and prepositions; see their Section 2.01). You should note that sentence case (also known as title case) is used for your title, but not for the title of cited works in your reference list (see their Section 6.29). You should include your name two lines below the title and it should be centred. In addition, your tutor’s name, the subject or unit name and code, the due date, the day and time of your class, and your word count (if required) should be at the bottom of the page, flush with the left margin. One commonly used layout is shown in Figure 5.2. At this point, it is essential that you check with your tutor or lecturer about the conventions at your university or in your subject or unit. For example, institutions differ about whether to include a word count and a statement or declaration of originality. Similarly, assignments might differ in the use of word ranges (e.g., 1/– 10%) or limits, as 68 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 Can Sadness be Good for You?: On the Cognitive, Motivational, and Interpersonal Benefits of Mild Negative Affect. (Forgas, 2017) Sexual Objectification in Women’s Daily Lives: A Smartphone Ecological Momentary Assessment Study. (Holland, Koval, Stratemeyer, Thomson, & Haslam, 2017) Gender Differences in Weight-Related and Non-Weight-Related Appearance Concerns in a Community Sample. (Keating, Stephens, Thomas, Castle, & Rossell, 2016) Teaching Psychology in Australia: Does Class Attendance Matter for Performance? (Louis, Bastian, McKimmie, & Lee, 2016) Social-economic Theory and Short-Term Mate Preferences: The Effects of Gender Roles and Socioeconomic Status. (March & Grieve, 2016) A Preregistered Study of Competing Predictions Suggests that Men Do Overestimate Women’s Sexual Intent. (Murray, Murphy, von Hippel, Trivers, & Haselton, 2017). Sexual Behaviour in Early Adolescence: A Cross-National Comparison of Australian and United States Youth. (Prendergast et al., 2017) Trauma Exposure and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Within Fire and Emergency Services in Western Australia. (Skeffington, Rees, & Mazzucchelli, 2017) Stagnant or Successful, Carefree or Anxious? Australian University Students’ Goals and Beliefs About Adulthood and Their Current Well-Being. (Weier & Lee, 2016) Sleep Well Feel Well: An Investigation into the Protective Value of Sleep Quality on Subjective Well-Being. (Weinberg, Noble, & Hammond, 2016) Figure 5.1. Some titles of published articles by Australasian psychologists. The Effect of Group Discussion on the Polarisation of Attitudes in Group and Individual Decision-Making Kathy Smith Submitted as a Psychology 201 Practical Report Due date: 6th October, 2018 Tutor: Oliver Holmes Class: Wednesday, 10:30–12:30 Word count: 1987 words (excluding reference list) Figure 5.2. Example of a typical title page. 69 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays well as what is included in a word limit (e.g., abstract, tables, references). If you are required to include the statement of these components, be honest! Due to the increased use of online submissions and technologies such as Turnitin, dishonesty is likely to be easily identified and penalised, and may be subject to review under your Academic Honesty, Integrity, or Conduct policy. Finally, if your university demands that a research report be in the form required for submission to a journal, the page numbering will start on the title page. In addition, all pages, including the title page, will also carry a running head, which is a brief title of no more than 50 characters including letters, punctuation, and spaces. It is capitalised and is flush left. Abstract. The Abstract follows the Title Page on a separate page. As you will see from searching databases like PsycINFO, the Abstract contains sufficient information to describe all the key aspects of the research. However, since it is restricted in length to a single paragraph, it must pack a lot of information into a small space. Specifically, it is critically important that the Abstract contains information about: (a) the topic or aim(s) of the study; (b) the participants; (c) key aspects of the method such as what was measured and how; (d) what was found; and (e) what was concluded. For this reason, you will find it advantageous to write the Abstract section last. By then you will have written the extended version of each of these pieces of information, including the background and purpose of the research (Introduction), how your research was conducted and with whom (Method), what you found (Results), and what you concluded (Discussion). The Abstract requires a heading (i.e., the word Abstract), in bold sentence case, centred on the page. The Abstract itself should be formatted as a single paragraph of double-spaced typing, flush left, with the first line not indented and located at the top of its page. The APA Publication Manual (6th ed.) states that the Abstract is typically 150 to 250 words in length (see their Section 2.04), but this allows for the description of multi-study research, which will not be the case for undergraduate reports. For this reason, we recommend that your Abstract should be briefer (i.e., no more than 150 words); however, be sure to check the expectations of the staff member setting your assignment. In terms of the content of the Abstract, it is customary to begin with a sentence stating the topic, purpose, or aim(s) of the study. This is followed by a description of the participants, including the sample size and information about the participants that is essential to the conclusions of the research. For example, mention whether participants were university students or recruited from another source, but only include age or gender if they affect either the research question or the representativeness of the sample. You should then briefly describe what participants actually did (e.g., variables measured and/or procedures undertaken) and the major findings. Note that it may be possible to state whether the findings supported your hypotheses, but this is only advisable if it is clear to your reader (or marker) what your hypotheses were. As can be seen in the four examples given in Figure 5.3, describing results or discussing results in relation to hypotheses or expectations can be appropriate in different circumstances. A final sentence can then be written stating the conclusions that you drew from these results. 70 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 This study explored the relationship between Type A behaviour, stress, and task difficulty. Participants were 30 undergraduates selected from a larger study at a metropolitan university, whose stress levels were measured before and after a pattern-matching task which was either unpressured or deliberately designed to provoke Type A behaviours by providing an environment that was achievementoriented, time-pressured, and self-esteem threatening. Self-reported Type A behaviours were also measured. The hypothesis that people higher in Type A behaviour are more stressed during a more pressured task was supported, though they were not more stressed in general. It was concluded that measurement of Type A behaviour needs to focus on all aspects of this behaviour pattern, not just the time-urgency. This study examined “hoped for” selves across adulthood, and their relationships with current well-being. Two hundred and ninety-four introductory psychology students and their acquaintances responded to a survey about hoped for, and feared selves, and psychological well-being. Occupational hoped-for selves were most frequently reported, possibly reflecting the predominantly young sample and the current economic conditions. The area of family was also considered important. Contrary to previous findings, perceived potential to achieve hoped-for selves did not differ across age groups, nor did type of hoped-for self differ depending on current well-being. It was concluded that, although the study needs to be replicated with a more diverse representative sample, most people report being capable of achieving hoped-for selves. The aim of this study was to test whether there was a bias towards drawing left-facing profiles, and if so, whether this is affected by handedness. Under counterbalanced conditions, 431 undergraduate students were requested to draw a person’s head in profile, to report, from memory, which way the Queen’s head faced on a 20-cent coin, and to draw the head side of a 20-cent coin from memory. Results indicate that a substantial majority of people drew a left-facing profile, regardless of handedness. When asked which way the Queen’s head faced on a coin, they answered at no better than chance levels. It was concluded that a left-facing drawing bias exists which is unrelated to visualisation or handedness. The purpose of this study was to test the hypothesis of an inverted-U relationship between stress and arousal, and judgments of perceptual accuracy. The sample consisted of 211 women and 89 men undergraduate university students who judged the accuracy of repeated presentations of the horizontal–vertical illusion and reported their level of stress and arousal using an affect checklist. Results indicate that, contrary to expectations, there was no relationship between arousal and judgments of perceptual accuracy. It was concluded that there was no relationship between stress or arousal and this rather simple type of judgment, but that further studies should examine the effect of differing levels of task complexity. Figure 5.3. Four examples of Abstracts. 71 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays You will find it useful to look at some of the major journals for examples of Abstracts. Try Journal of Experimental Psychology or Brain and Cognition for areas that you may encounter in your very early studies. Later you may want to look at journals such as Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Child Development, Psychological Science, and Sex Roles for Abstracts of studies in later units. 5.2 Introduction By the time you sit down to write the Introduction, you will likely have completed a review of essential literature (see Chapter 2), will have decided on your hypotheses, tested them with the appropriate statistics, and determined what the results mean. At this point, you are ready to write the Introduction from the perspective of the researcher who designed the study. The purpose of the Introduction is to provide the reader (or marker) with the context and background for your research, and the path you took to decide why you tested the particular hypotheses you did test. As mentioned in Chapter 1, the structure of the Introduction is like the upper half of an hourglass, with the most general information (e.g., context, definitions) first, then gradually becoming more focused in the review of previous research, and concluding with specific, concrete hypotheses. The Introduction will begin on a new page but does not include the heading Introduction. The location of the Introduction tells the reader what it is. Institutions requiring a report in the form of a journal submission expect you to repeat the title, centred at the top of the page on which the Introduction begins. Some institutions suggest that the page on which the Introduction begins is not numbered, and numbering begins with “2” at the top right-hand corner of the second page of the Introduction. Ask your tutor or lecturer whether this style is expected. Opening sentences. These must be consistent with the purpose of the Introduction; namely, to explain why you have undertaken your study, and to set the scene for whatever you hypothesise. It is (surprise!) an exercise in communication, and for that reason should be written in English prose and should avoid psychological jargon wherever possible. As a result, you should not leap straight into the middle of your research problem or theory. Instead you should lead the reader step by step through your reasoning and evidence, beginning with a statement of the context, background, or broad issue. Kidder and Judd (1986) note that it is preferable that you open with a statement about human behaviour, not the behaviour of behavioural scientists or their research. However, do not allow this general statement to degenerate into cliché. Markers are not impressed by opening statements such as “Ever since humans came down from the trees, people have . . .”! However, the behaviour being studied ought to be relevant in some way to the real world. You should be able to make a one- or two-sentence statement of this relevance. Some recent Australasian examples are given in Figure 5.4. 72 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 “Time stood still” is a common expression describing the perception of time slowing down when one is exposed to dangerous or emotional stimuli; however, the processes that underlie the perceived duration of emotional stimuli (affective chronometry) are not fully understood (Davidson, 2015). One aspect of affective chronometry that is particularly unclear and has had inconsistent findings is the distortion of duration at intervals less than 1000 ms. Another aspect that is not fully understood is whether the distortions are caused by changes in arousal levels or by changes in attention (Grondin, Laflamme, & Gontier, 2014). (Corke, Bell, Goodhew, Smithson, & Edwards, 2016) Self-consistency, the degree to which self-concept elements are congruent across time, relationships, and situations (Boucher, 2010; English & Chen, 2007), has been associated with positive psychological adjustment. Specifically, lower levels of self-consistency have been found to be significantly associated with lower levels of self-esteem, positive emotion, and emotional adjustment, and higher levels of depression, neuroticism, anxiety, and stress (e.g., Block, 1961; Campbell, Assanand, & Di Paula, 2003; Donahue, Robins, Roberts, & John, 1993; Sheldon, Ryan, Rawsthorne, & Ilardi, 1997). (Gage, Coker, & Jobson, 2015) Initially, perfectionism was understood to be a negative trait where the actions of striving for excessively high standards were thought to lead to emotional distress (Burne, 1980; Hollander, 1965). Over the past two decades, a multidimensional model of perfectionism has been established (Dunkley, Zuroff, & Blankstein, 2003; Slade & Owen, 1998). According to this view, not everyone who has high standards experiences distress. Rather, it is those who attach not achieving their high standards to failure, who are inclined to experience anxiety, worthlessness and depression. (Mathew, Dunning, Coats, & Whelan, 2014) Substantial research into the physical and psychological effects of atypical weight has established common correlates and consequences of obesity. Despite this knowledge, weight loss programs are struggling to keep pace with biopsychosocial consequences of overweight and obesity. This has encouraged new approaches in an effort to better manage the problem. (Robertson, Davies, & Winefield, 2015) There is substantial evidence that the transition from elementary to middle school is a stressful time for young people (Eccles et al., 1993; Simmons & Blyth, 1987). This stress may be due in part to the challenge of renegotiating peer relationships and establishing oneself in a new social hierarchy, as well as a potential decrease in classroom emotional support (the affective tone of interactions within the classroom). (Shell, Gazelle, & Faldowski, 2014) Research has consistently outlined the contribution of alcohol in the facilitation of aggression, with alcohol involved in more than half of all violent crimes globally (Pernanen, 1991; World Health Organization, 2007), and recent Australian estimates indicating the presence of alcohol in up to 73% of all assaults (Collins & Lapsley, 2007). Further reports show an increase in verbal and physical violence outside licensed venues at times of high-alcohol use (i.e. between the hours of 8 pm to 6 am Friday to Sunday (Laslett et al., 2006)), with the majority of assaults occurring in the streets, lanes and footpaths outside venues (Ireland & Thommeny, 1993; Miller et al., 2011). (Zinkiewicz, Curtis, Meurer, & Miller, 2016) Figure 5.4. Some actual examples of opening sentences. Note: These are examples of opening sentences in publication. They are also in the format of block quotes. Note the full stop at the end of the quote and the source afterwards in parentheses. You should avoid using such quotes in your assignments. Instead rephrase the material using your own words, with an acknowledgment, to show that you understand the material (for more information on paraphrasing and quotation, see Chapter 3, Quotations, p. 42). 73 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Define the area under study. The initial statement should be followed by a statement giving the theoretical framework or context that has generated the particular questions you used in your study. Information about the context will usually be found in your assignment information, starter references, or even in the subject or unit textbook. This section should not be very long, but it is useful in this early part of your report to define concepts and other technical terms. You should write for an intelligent lay reader who is generally familiar with psychology but not with the specific area you are reporting on. Be careful when defining terms, because many terms used in psychology have technical meanings that are somewhat different from their everyday use. For this reason, use a dictionary of psychology, not an ordinary dictionary. If you already know that you intend to major in psychology, a dictionary of psychology is a good investment. Most textbooks also have technical terms defined either in the body of the text or in a glossary. Previous research. Usually the studies that you are assigned to read early in your undergraduate career will be replications or variations of previous research. If this is the case, then this will provide you with a place to begin. Specifically, you should describe what the previous research did, including their theoretical position or contention, a brief review of their method and finding, and what they have concluded from their research. You should be sure to describe all the relevant material from a particular piece of research in the one place. It is frustrating for your marker to read a paragraph about a specific study, then a couple of paragraphs about other research, then find more material about the first study. The reports that achieve the best marks are those that follow a logical sequence. You should be very selective about what you include. There are no strict rules about which research to review, but one possibility is to describe the most general findings first, followed by descriptions of the findings in previous research that are specifically relevant to your hypotheses. In addition, there is a temptation to write all you know about a previous study. Resist it! You need only include the material that is relevant to your hypotheses. Typically, that means giving only the briefest overview of the researchers’ aims, what they did, to whom, and what was concluded from it. When reading previous research, you should have applied the principles of critical evaluation already described in Section 2.4, both to your choice of previous studies and to the contents of them. In writing about previous research, you should briefly describe the elements of that research in a way that indicates the quality of the evidence you are using to justify the hypotheses that you will make at the end of the Introduction. Some examples of summaries of previous findings are shown in Figure 5.5. They are the work of Australasian students and are published here with permission. How much prior research should be included? Students often ask how much previous research they should include. It largely depends on the length allowed for the report. Check with your own tutor or lecturer, but most first-year research reports are based on a very limited number (i.e., one or two) of specific references. However, you are typically 74 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 An exhaustive body of research demonstrating similar concrete to abstract effects for other metaphoric representations exists. This includes research examining metaphorical associations between weight and importance (Ackerman, Nocera, & Bargh, 2010; Chandler, Reinhard, & Schwarz, 2012; Jostman, Lakens, & Schubert, 2009), rough textures and social interactions (Ackerman et al., 2010), hardness and rigid thinking (Ackerman et al., 2010), fluid movement and fluid thinking (Slepian & Ambady, 2012), spatial information or movement and time (Boroditsky & Ramscar, 2002; Cassanto & Boroditsky, 2007), cleanliness and moral purity (Lee & Schwarz, 2010, 2011), forward movement and goal achievement (Natanzon & Ferguson, 2012), height and power (Meier & Dionne, 2009), fishy smells and suspicion (Lee & Schwarz, 2012), and facial expressions and perceptions of brightness (Song, Vonasch, Meier, & Bargh, 2011). Reported adherence to the single session drinking guidelines ranged from 26–61%. For example, a UK study of female psychology students (N 5 235) measured adherence to the single session guideline by asking how many days per week they exceed three standard drinks in a single drinking session (Murgraff, McDermott, & Walsh, 2001). Almost 75% of participants reported consumption above the guideline periodically, with over 50% doing so two or more times per week. An Australian study (N 5 20,000) found similar adherence rates among males (35%), but much higher adherence among females (68%) to the single session guideline (Livingston, 2012). Finally, a study (N 5 992) of North American college students found 65% of participants do not adhere with the national guideline for single session drinking, and that over a year, both men and women exceed the guideline every second week on average (Hoeppner, Paskausky, Jackson, & Barnett, 2013). The ability to recognise facial expressions accurately is critical for effective social functioning (Blair, 2003). Facial expression recognition plays an important role in regulating behaviour and in guiding social judgements (Willis, Palermo, Burke, McGrillen, & Miller, 2010). Impairments in facial expression recognition can be detrimental to interpersonal relationships and wellbeing (Ruffman, Henry, Livingstone, & Phillips, 2008). However, there are limited effective interventions available to treat facial expression recognition deficits that are prevalent in a range of clinical populations (Willis, Murphy, Ridley, & Vercammen, 2015). Gay men and lesbians are subject to subtle and insidious forms of discrimination, including being paid less than straight people (Berg & Lien, 2002), being more likely to be overlooked for employment (Tilcsik, 2011), being less likely to receive help when needed (Gabriel & Banse, 2006), and being evaluated more harshly when they make decisions that are unsuccessful (Shepherd & Patzelt, 2014). As can be seen from these examples, there are social and cultural norms and practices that result in the unfair treatment of gay people (Kelley, 2001). Figure 5.5. Some real examples of summaries of previous (student) research. 75 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays expected to read beyond these assigned or starter references. Second-year research reports will often be assigned as many as five references, and you will certainly be expected to read at least as many more. A rule of thumb is that you will need to read at least as many more references as you have been given, although you may not use them all in the final report. In deciding what to include, you should review enough previous research to justify the hypotheses you make, either to show that previous researchers have supported similar hypotheses, or to show that they have found sufficient data for you to deduce the predictions that your study tests. It is common practice to read more references than you end up citing in your report. A good research report will cite only relevant work and will indicate why it is relevant. A quality report will demonstrate discernment. That is, it is better to cite a few well-chosen references than to have a long and unselective reference list including material that is only tangentially relevant. Moreover, reports will generally lose marks if the Introduction is little more than a collection of quotes and references. Using examples. In illustrating theoretical points or introducing technical terms, it is important to use examples. The more abstract the theory, the more important it is to use examples. The following poor and good examples (adapted, with permission, from a student report) highlight the way a single sentence can describe a general and ill-defined or a specific and clearly defined relationship between key variables, in this case, parental criticism and children’s body image: Poor: “Research has found that parents play a significant role in creating body image concerns for their children (Levine, Smolak, & Hayden, 1994).” Good: “Evidence suggests that the extent to which parents are critical of their children’s weight and shape is significantly correlated with their children’s body image dissatisfaction and their investment in thinness (Levine, Smolak, & Hayden, 1994).” Jargon. Try to avoid jargon. Some people justify the use of jargon as necessary linguistic shorthand. However, an undergraduate research report should be written for a more general audience, to demonstrate the author’s clear understanding of the content of the report. Direct quotes. For the same reason as avoiding jargon, you should avoid lengthy direct quotes from your references. It is not always obvious from the use of quotations that you know what they mean. Also, as mentioned in Chapter 3 on referencing, rephrasing them in your own words (and acknowledging the source) shows your marker that you clearly understand what you are saying. Introducing your own study. Once you have reported previous research, it is time to introduce your own study. First you need a statement of general aims. An example is: “The present study replicated Forgas’ (2017) finding that sadness can have positive 76 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 consequences on tasks requiring vigilant attention using a university student sample. In addition, we extended this work by exploring [other pertinent questions or alternative applications] . . .”. You should also add here any special or different treatments or measures you used, or ways your study has controlled for problems in previous research. Such statements show that you are aware of the shortcomings of previous studies. Hypotheses. Finally, you need concise, specific, concrete hypotheses. These are explicit statements of what you expected to find. Your expectations should be justified by what you have already said about previous research. Do not use the abbreviations “H1” or “H0”, and do not state or write about the null hypothesis (e.g., no difference, no change, or no relationship between groups or variables). Each hypothesis should be stated operationally. That is, the hypotheses should describe the variables used in your research (e.g., scores on the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale) rather than as the broad concepts they reflect (e.g., self-esteem). You should not number your hypotheses, as doing so typically complicates your writing (e.g., it is insufficient to refer to them by number alone elsewhere, and there are likely to be too few of them to warrant a list). Similarly, you should not present them in bulleted note form. They should be written in flowing English prose, with all hypotheses in the same paragraph. Remember to write your hypotheses in the past tense. The study has already been run or you would not be reporting on it. Therefore, say, “We/I predicted that participants would . . .”, or “We/I hypothesised that respondents would . . .”. You were given some examples of hypotheses in Figure 4.2 in Chapter 4. There are some more examples, both good and not so good, in Figure 5.6, which illustrate what the difference is! Length of sections. There are no formal rules about the appropriate length of each section of a research report, but a rough guide is that the Introduction should be about onethird of the length of the entire report. 5.3 Method When writing your first draft of a research report, it is easiest to write the Method first, because this section describes what was done and to whom. You will usually have been one of the participants in the study, or the person testing participants you have chosen or been assigned, so you should be well aware of what was done. If, for any reason (e.g., experimental conditions), you have experience of only one of the different treatments or materials, your tutor or lecturer should make this clear, so be sure to read through all of the information provided, and don’t rely solely on what you experienced. There are three reasons for having a Method section. One is to give your reader or marker enough information to judge whether your study really does test the hypotheses you claim that it tests. The second is to allow someone else to repeat your study if they so desire, and to test your hypotheses on their particular sample. You therefore need 77 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Poor: Hypothesis 1. Older adults will be less able to ignore the interspersed irrelevant material than younger adults. (Don’t give the hypotheses numbers or use subheadings like this. The dependent measure is not clear. The tense is wrong.) Better: It was anticipated that adults older than 45 years would be less able to ignore the interspersed irrelevant material and, as a result, would read significantly more slowly and show poorer comprehension than adults younger than 25 years. Poor: Perceived positive social support in personal relationships will be negatively associated with anxiety and mood disorder episodes. (Wrong tense, and how are these constructs measured?) Better: Scores on the XYZ scale of perceived social support were expected to be negatively correlated with the number of anxiety attacks reported and also with the number of mood disorder episodes. Poor: The hypothesis is that a mother’s positive perception of herself will decrease the intensity of her daily hassles via social support satisfaction. (How are these constructs measured? The tense is incorrect.) Better: It was hypothesised that scores on a mother’s self-esteem would negatively predict the intensity of her daily hassles, and this effect would be mediated by scores on social support satisfaction. Poor: The hypothesis is that student willingness to take a psychology course will depend on the gender of the instructor and the type of course being taught. (It is not clear what is actually being measured. The prediction is too general. The tense is wrong.) Better: [It was] hypothesised that student willingness to take a psychology course from a feminine, masculine, or androgynous instructor would vary with the type of course being taught. Feminine and androgynous instructors would be preferred for clinical courses, which tend to emphasise personal/social skills. Masculine and androgynous instructors would be preferred for experimental courses, which tend to emphasise analytic/objective skills (Freeman, 1992, p. 93). Figure 5.6. Good and poor examples of hypotheses. 78 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 sufficient detail so that someone else could be confident that by following your instructions they would be carrying out an identical experiment. The third reason is to allow your reader to judge if your results may be applicable to any other groups about which the reader is interested. The Method section is presented immediately after the Introduction without starting a new page. It is simply headed Method, in bold sentence case, centred across the page. It contains a maximum of four subheadings: • • • • Participants (or Subjects, if non-human) Materials or Measures (or Apparatus) Design Procedure Note that in contrast to previous editions of the APA Publication Manual, these are now in bold type. Levels of headings. The Method section is a good illustration of the need to distinguish between levels of headings. In most undergraduate research reports, there is a need to use only two or three levels of headings. For example, the section titles Method and Results are level 1 headings, in bold sentence case, and are centred across the page. Subsection headings are level 2 headings, which are bold sentence case, on a line by themselves aligned with the left margin, and do not have a full stop. Finally, if needed, the subsection Measures may use a level 3 heading, which is in bold sentence case, begins on a new line but is indented, and ends with a full stop. Examples of these headings in the context of a Method are presented in Figure 5.7. These conventions were introduced to make life easier for typesetters. Although you might argue that this reason doesn’t apply to undergraduate research reports, treat it as Method Participants Participants were 234 undergraduate psychology students, including 112 men . . . Measures Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI). Self-reported big five personality traits of Extraversion, Agreeableness, Emotional Stability, Conscientiousness, and Openness to Experiences were assessed by the TIPI (Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003). Procedure All participants were recruited in tutorial classes but participated via the secure online link at a time and location of their own choosing. Figure 5.7. Example of use of levels of headings. 79 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays part of the discipline of writing to a convention. Once you know about it, you don’t have to make decisions each time you write a research report. 5.3.1 Participants The participants subsection appears under its own (level 2) heading. In this subsection, you should state the number of participants tested; how they were selected; refusal rates (if known); how they were remunerated or compensated for their participation; and any relevant descriptive demographic variables such as age, gender, education, marital status, ethnic identity, occupational status, or other features. A demographic variable becomes relevant when it is used as an independent variable (such as when comparing groups of different ages) or when it specifically defines the population from which the sample is drawn. Your statement of these details allows the reader to assess how adequately you have controlled for some of these variables, and the extent to which your findings can be generalised to any other groups. Examples are shown in Figure 5.8. In describing the sample, you should note that you can say, “The sample comprised . . .” or “The sample consisted of . . .”, but it is poor English to say, “The sample comprised of . . .” or “The sample consisted 36 people . . .”. Similarly, don’t say, “The participants were divided into two groups . . .” because it gives the impression that each person was divided! Say, “The sample was divided into two groups . . .”. The participants were undergraduate introductory psychology students at a Perth university, 130 women (M 5 19.6 years, SD 5 2.9) and 46 men (M 5 19.7, SD 5 2.9), who were participating as part of a course requirement. The sample consisted of 268 women and 76 men who responded to an internet survey. Mean age was 35.4 years (SD 5 11.7). All participants had been diagnosed with . . . and the mean time since diagnosis was 11.2 years (SD 5 9.3). Sixty-four per cent were married or in de facto relationships, 18% were separated, divorced, or widowed. Approximately half the sample (54.8%) was currently employed. The respondents were 97 women (M 5 38.5 years, SD 5 9.4), each chosen non-randomly by an introductory psychology student at a Sydney university. All subjects were or had been married, and had a mean education level of 11.4 years. The sample comprised 78 children, 44 girls and 34 boys who were attending Year 6 classes in two Brisbane suburban State schools serving predominantly middle-class populations. Twelve students from the Australian National University participated, in return for a $10 Coles Myer voucher. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were native English speakers. (This was for an experiment on visual perception where vision was a relevant variable.) Figure 5.8. Examples of descriptions of participants in a psychology experiment. 80 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 Avoiding bias. The APA Publication Manual (6th ed.) seeks to increase accuracy and reduce language-based bias in the description of research. In fact, three general guidelines have been developed. These are: 1) promoting the specificity in the description of participants; 2) avoidance of “othering” labels; and 3) acknowledgment of participants’ agency in the description of procedures. A consequence of these guidelines is that there is a strong preference for collectively describing individuals who take part in your research as “participants” rather than “subjects”. This reflects the current view that these people are not passive objects of your research; rather, they are active partners in the research. As a result, participants should be said to have completed (or not) your measures, rather than described as having a measure “administered” to them or having their data “collected”. When describing who the participants were, the APA Publication Manual (6th ed.) encourages descriptions to be as specific as possible (e.g., rather than “over 65 years of age”, use “65 to 83 years of age”, and “women” rather than “females” unless you are describing participants’ biological sex) and to only include labels where absolutely necessary – especially labels that may not be chosen by people to whom they may be applied. One way of achieving this is to use more specific group memberships (e.g., “gay men” or “lesbians”), and to be generally sensitive in the use of social category labels (i.e., not “homosexuals”). The Publication Manual suggests this can be achieved by replacing collective nouns with adjectival forms (i.e., adjectives that further specify a less contentious social group term such as “older adults”, “people with a disability”). The Publication Manual has some very specific advice on terms that avoid biased language across a number of topics. Specifically, gender is typically used rather than sex, and should be inclusive whenever possible (e.g., strict avoidance of the use of “he” or “man” to refer to a general case for people). Likewise, sexuality should be described in terms of “sexual orientation” rather than as “sexual preference” and the terms “gay men”, “lesbians”, “bisexual men”, and “bisexual women” should be used to describe people who belong to these groups. Furthermore, prejudice towards gay men and lesbians should be described as “anti-gay bias” or “anti-gay prejudice” rather than “homophobia”. Racial and ethnic identity should be described using capitalised nouns (e.g., Australian, Sudanese) and, wherever possible, should be specified or endorsed by participants. Health and disability should acknowledge personhood and agency and then condition (e.g., “people diagnosed with PTSD” rather than “victims of trauma”). Finally, age-based groups are: “girl” and “boy” to refer to children aged to 12 years; “young men” and “young women” to describe 13 to 17 years of age; “men” and “women” to refer to people aged 18 years of age and over; and the terms “younger adults” and “older adults” can be used if needed to describe generational differences in research. 5.3.2 Materials Materials is the most general heading referring to the equipment, including measures, used in research. If you use equipment, it must be described in sufficient detail so that anyone wishing to replicate the study can buy, build, or otherwise acquire identical 81 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays equipment. If you use a computer program, give its name, version number, and source. If the layout of the equipment is either complex or theoretically important (e.g., for some perception experiments), it may be necessary to include a diagram of the layout. In this case, you should label the section Apparatus instead of Materials. In cases where the apparatus is used in conjunction with actions or tasks, it may even make communication clearer if you combine the Apparatus and Procedure subsection under the (level 2) heading Apparatus and Procedure. This allows you to say how the apparatus was used at the same time as you describe it, and is one of the exceedingly few occasions that justify the use of a combined subsection! Questionnaires and scales. In undergraduate studies, questionnaires are frequently the only materials used in a study. If so, this subsection may be described as Measures instead of the more general Materials; however, check with your lecturer or tutor on what is required. In describing the measures, be sure to report all information collected from participants, including demographics such as age, gender, education, and whatever else was asked. If participants completed well-known psychological tests, be sure to include the name of the measure and citation (i.e., author and date of publication), the constructs measured – especially if assessed by subscales – and the number of items assessing each, and the response format used. It can be useful to include an example item for each construct or subscale. You should also state briefly what you know of the test’s psychometric properties, such as demonstrated validity and reliability from prior, ideally relevant, research. Finally, you need to state how the test was scored, including any reverse scoring required, the full range of scores, and what a high or low score indicates. If the questionnaire is one that was developed or adapted for your study, include all the information you would include for a standard measure, as well as a description of the purpose or adaptation. You should also include the scale as an Appendix, and refer to it in this subsection (e.g., “see Appendix X”; We will say a bit more about this in Section 5.7). Several examples are included in Figure 5.9. Other measures. Across psychology, in line with other developments to enhance the rigour and quality of the discipline (e.g., statistical reporting standards, preregistration of studies), there has been an increase in the use of non-questionnaire measures in order to complement questionnaires and address issues such as self-report biases (e.g., the tendency for people to over-report desirable and under-report undesirable attributes or outcomes) and to assess non-reportable concepts such as implicit associations or reaction times. It should be noted that such measures cannot replace, and are not inherently superior to, questionnaires. Rather, when used appropriately, they allow consideration of a wider range of psychological phenomena, and complement questionnaire measures, often providing convergent support for hypotheses and theories. Such measures include archival (e.g., medical records) and observation measures, physiological measures such as heart rate, and implicit measures including priming and implicit association tests. It is unlikely that in the early years of your undergraduate studies you will encounter archival or physiological measures involving complex data sources or apparatus (e.g., 82 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 Field-independence was measured by the Hidden Figures Test (HFT; French et al., 1969). The 32-item HFT is a moderately difficult test requiring the ability to recognise simple geometric forms embedded within complex stimuli. Kardash et al. (1988) found that undergraduate students in educational psychology needed a score of 12 or better to be in the upper one-third of scores (field-independent), and a score of 8 or less to be in the lower one-third of scores (field-dependent). (Adapted from Townsend, Moore, Tuck, & Wilton, 1990) Participants completed the Quality of Relationships Inventory (QRI) developed by Pierce et al. (1991). The QRI has three scales, two of which were of interest in the present study. These were: (a) expectations for Social Support (e.g., “To what extent can you turn to this person for advice about problems?”) and (b) Conflict (e.g., “How often do you have to work hard to avoid conflict in this relationship?” and “How much does this person make you feel guilty?”). The authors report good reliability and validity in a large community sample. (Adapted from Pierce, Sarason, & Sarason, 1992) Rosenberg’s (1965) 10-item Self-esteem Scale (RSE) was used to assess self-esteem. Participants rated each item on a 4-point Likert-type scale, from 1 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). Overall self-esteem scores were calculated by summing across all items after reversing some items for consistency of direction of expression. Note that higher scores indicate higher self-esteem. The three attachment vignettes originally created by Hazan and Shaver (1987) were decomposed into 13 individual sentences, each of which was rated on a 7-point Likert scale, from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). To control for acquiescence response, five sentences were worded in a negative direction. An example is, “I’m nervous whenever anyone gets too close to me.” (Adapted from Simpson, Rholes, & Nelligan, 1992) Figure 5.9. Examples of overviews of scales in a Materials section. galvanic skin response, or functional magnetic resonance image or fMRI), specifically for reasons of limited access. However, you may be asked to report a heart rate or behavioural data (e.g., observation). In such cases, you will need to describe what is measured (e.g., the number of heartbeats) and how it is measured (e.g., the experimenter placed the index and middle fingers on the participant’s wrist and having located their pulse, counted all heartbeats occurring during a 20-second period as timed using a stopwatch). The description of implicit measures differs from the description of other measures because it involves several unique components. This is because implicit measures use trials, which present stimuli often under strictly timed conditions, and collect responses in the form of choices, accuracy, or reaction times. Moreover, as these tasks are not encountered in everyday life, they often require very specific instructions to participants. Each of these aspects needs to be described and, as with other information described in research reports, in more complete detail in your report than you might find in peer-reviewed publications. This is again to demonstrate your awareness of key aspects of such measures. Several examples are included in Figure 5.10. 83 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays A Stroop task was used to assess participants’ ability to inhibit the irrelevant information (e.g., the semantic meaning) of targets presented in lowercase letters or rows of Xs presented in the centre of the computer screen in one of the four colours (i.e., red, purple, green, or blue) all in Arial 12-point. In the congruent condition, each target was a colour word presented in the matching colour (e.g., the word “red” in red colour). In the incongruent condition, each target was a colour word presented in a non-matching colour (e.g., the word “red” in purple, green, or blue). In the neutral condition, the target row of Xs was presented in one of the four colours. Accordingly, there were four different congruent stimuli, 12 different incongruent stimuli, and four different neutral stimuli. Each test block comprised 72 trials, composed of 24 congruent trials (each congruent stimulus presented six times), 24 incongruent trials (each incongruent stimulus presented twice), and 24 neutral trials (each neutral stimulus presented six times). Thus, a block contained all possible word/colour combinations, with one third of the trials being congruent, one third incongruent, and one third neutral. Participants were instructed at the beginning of each block that each trial would begin with a row of black Xs (i.e., cueing stimuli), followed by a symbol string in a colour, and that their task was to ignore the meaning of the string (e.g., word) and to classify the colour (using the keys: “Z” for red, “X” for purple, “N” for green, and “M” for blue). The cueing stimuli was presented for 460 ms and followed by a blank screen for 40 ms before the target was presented for 2000 ms or until the participant responded, whichever occurred first. Participants were given feedback on each response (e.g., correct, wrong, or no response) for 500 ms followed by a blank screen for 500 ms before the next trial commenced. Participant reaction time for each response was recorded, but only correct responses were analysed as a function of stimuli type (e.g., congruent, incongruent, and neutral). (Adapted from Kinoshita, de Wit, Aji, & Norris, 2017) An Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwarz, 1998) was designed to measure implicit attitudes as implicit associations between negative attributes and pictures of faces with visible differences compared to those without a visible difference. The stimuli consisted of photographs depicting faces of people with a visible difference including eight images displaying moderate acquired or congenital visible facial differences (e.g. cleft lip, neurofibromatosis, burn scars) and eight photographs of faces without visible differences matched for age, gender and facial expression, as well as eight positive and eight negative attributes drawn from qualitative studies of prejudice towards visible difference and focus groups conducted by the charity Changing Faces. Stimuli were presented individually, one per trial, in the centre of a computer screen. Faces were sized to 33% of screen size, and words were presented in black Arial 16-point font. The design of the IAT involves seven blocks including five 20-trial practice blocks for familiarising participants with the stimuli and response pairings (blocks 1, 2, 3, 5, and 6) and two 40-trial critical blocks (blocks 4 and 7). The order of blocks was face familiarisation, attribute familiarisation, congruent face and attribute practice block (i.e., difference faces and negative attributes paired with one response), congruent Figure 5.10. Examples of overviews of implicit measures in a Materials section. 84 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 face and attribute critical block, reversed attribute familiarisation, incongruent face and attribute practice block (i.e., no difference faces and negative attributes paired with one response), and the incongruent face and attribute critical block. Before commencing blocks, participants were instructed that the task involved sorting stimuli as quickly and accurately as possible. Specifically, either two stimuli as in familiarisation blocks, or four stimuli as in practice and critical blocks, were assigned to two keys (e.g., difference faces 5 “E” and no difference faces “I”) as indicated by the labels in the top left and right corners of the screen. Participants were told they would receive “correct” or “wrong” feedback for every response. Only correct responses for critical trials were analysed using a reaction time-based score with faster reaction times, and higher IAT D scores indicating stronger implicit associations. (Adapted from Roberts, Neate, & Gierasch, 2017) A lexical decision task was used to assess the implicit dehumanisation of women. Specifically, dehumanisation of women was operationalised as the faster correct classification of non-human words (e.g., “animal”, “instinct”) as real words, compared to human words (e.g., “human”, “culture”) following a “women” prime, compared to “men” or to a control word prime (e.g., “table”). Letter strings were used as targets to be judged and included 10 human-related words and 10 animalrelated words from Fasoli et al. (2015), and 20 pronounceable non-word letter strings. Blocks comprised equal numbers of word and non-word trials which began with the presentation of a fixation point ‘1’ (500 ms) followed by a prime (150 ms), blank screen (200 ms), and then the letter string which was terminated by the participant’s response and followed by an inter-trial interval of 1000 ms. Participants were told that the task was a simple word judgment task which required them to judge whether a letter string presented in the centre of the computer screen was a real word or a non-word. If they identified the letters as a word, they should press the “E” key, whereas they should press the “I” key if the string is a non-word, and this judgment should be made as quickly and accurately as possible. Participants were intentionally not informed of the true purpose of the measure, and instructed to ignore any other aspects of the task. The response times for correctly identified words were averaged as a function of target type, with faster average response times interpreted as facilitation of the target (e.g., non-human words) by the prime (e.g., women). (Adapted from Piccoli, Fantoni, Foroni, Bianchi, & Carnaghi, 2017) Figure 5.10. (Continued) 5.3.3 Design The purpose of the Design subsection is to explain the basic conditions of the experiment and define dependent and independent variables. If you are instructed to write this subsection, state the independent and dependent variables in their operationalised form; that is, in the units in which they are measured. If groups are being compared, describe how this comparison is related to the variables being assessed (e.g., “age as a two-level factor was operationalised by collecting responses from older and younger adults”). 85 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays When you are not expected to include a Design subsection, the operationalisation of the independent and dependent variables should be made clear in the Materials subsection where you describe how they are measured. If your study counterbalances the order of presentation of questionnaire items, say so in Procedure. If different groups of participants are expected to perform different tasks, or to do them in different orders, describe this in the Procedure subsection. 5.3.4 Procedure This subsection requires a fairly detailed description of how the study was carried out. Again, the detail should be such that other researchers could easily replicate it. Increasingly, this section begins with a description of how informed consent was given, and how or where participation was undertaken (e.g., “in laboratory classes”, “by accessing an online web link”). If any instructions are given to participants, these should be quoted in full if they are fairly brief, or overviewed and referred to as present in an Appendix if longer. Avoid abbreviations if you can. If you think abbreviations are necessary, use obvious ones that do not need tricky interpretations, and always define them the first time you use them. Other information to provide in the Procedure is the order of measures if not described elsewhere (e.g., Design), the duration of participation, and any debriefing used. Finally, include any other aspects of the execution of the research, for example, how you controlled for extraneous nuisance factors such as time and place. It is typically best to write the procedure in the sequence that a participant experienced it. If deception is involved, be sure to include any “cover story” you told the participants. It should be noted if your measures or procedure might have caused the participants to feel any embarrassment or discomfort, and what procedures you took to ensure anonymity (i.e., this may have been undertaken during data processing). Kidder and Judd (1986) put it nicely by saying that you should describe how you ensured that participants were treated with dignity and left with their self-esteem intact, and how any respect they might have had for you and psychology was enhanced rather than diminished. Combined (and confused) sections. It is important not to confuse the Materials and the Procedure subsections. The Materials subsection describes what equipment, measures, or tasks were used in the study, and the Procedure subsection describes how the data were collected. Many modern journal articles do combine Materials and Procedure sections, but that is usually because they are reporting multiple studies in one article with very strict word limits. At undergraduate level, you should only use combined sections if explicitly instructed to do so by your lecturer or tutor. Do not confuse Procedure with Results. You should not say at the end of the Procedure section that the data were analysed using SPSS, or say anything about the statistics used. The Procedure describes only how the data were collected. 86 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 Distinguishing roles. As part of your learning experience, you may have participated in the research as a participant. This means you are in a unique situation of being both a participant and the researcher, even though you may not have designed the research or undertaken all the analyses. This means you have insight about the experience of participating and may not feel fully responsible for all aspects of the research. For example, remembering how you felt while completing a questionnaire or taking part in an experiment may remind you that not only are participants active, but their attitude and even response to the study may not be what the experimenter really wanted. This should help you to not make assumptions about the participants’ behaviour or responses, allowing you to regard the results with a critical and sceptical eye. However, while this should inform your critical thinking about the research, your experience as a participant should not be an explicit aspect of how you write up the research, unless explicitly instructed otherwise. Similarly, you should not state that your tutor or lecturer designed the research, or collated or analysed the results for you, even though it may be true. Remember that you are writing the report as if you were the researcher who designed and carried out the study. 5.4 Results This section follows immediately after the Method section, and should not begin on a new page. The heading is simply Results, in bold sentence case, and centred across the page. The purpose of the Results section is to present all the evidence relevant to your hypotheses. You should report your results in the same order that you presented the hypotheses in the Introduction. 5.4.1 Your Very First Report If this is your very first Results section, ask yourself: “What information do I need to report to convince the reader that I really have tested the hypotheses?” Usually your first report will require few, if any, statistics. Typically, you are required to report data such as frequency counts or proportions. For example, you might have compared the proportion of men compared to women drivers who fail to stop at a stop sign. In this case you simply report that, for example: “Of 100 drivers observed, 60 were men, and 11 of these failed to stop at the stop sign. Of the 40 women drivers, three failed to stop at the stop sign.” That allows readers to make judgments of whether the sample size is reasonable, and to have the information to decide for themselves if your hypothesis was supported or not. If you are reporting the proportions of more than two groups, it may be simpler to show them as a bar chart. One example is shown in Figure 5.11, and the conventions for figures are described in Section 5.4.6. Some early research reports expect you to calculate the mean and standard deviation of scores for two groups. For example, a classic study is one in which participants may have been asked to learn a list of pairs of words, where one group was given no explicit 87 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 550 Self-report Official report 500 450 400 Frequency 350 300 250 200 150 100 50 0 Motor vehicle theft Assault Sex offence Robbery Murder Charge type Figure 5.11. Lifetime frequency of self-reported offending and officially recorded charges for (n 5 1184) convicted offenders. (Adapted from Payne & Piquero, 2016.) instructions and the other was given instructions to imagine bizarre connections between the word pairs. They might then have been given one word from each pair in the list and been expected to remember its matching pair. If your hypothesis was that the bizarre instructions should lead to better recall of the paired words, then reporting the mean number of words recalled correctly in each group will allow you to judge whether the instructions made a difference. A good way to report these results is: “The bizarre imagery instructions resulted in more word pairs being correctly recalled (M 5 13.20, SD 5 4.30) than did the no instructions (M 5 8.60, SD 5 5.10).” The conventions for reporting statistics are described on page 90, but the M is the convention for the arithmetical mean (note that it is in italics) and the SD for standard deviation. We would strongly discourage the use of graphs or tables for reporting only one or two statistics; however, make sure you refer to any instructions provided to you by your lecturer or tutor. Nonetheless, it is essential that you are able to construct grammatically correct sentences to say what you have found. If reporting the statistics, you should insert the appropriate statistics into the sentence. If you are presenting a figure or table, you should refer to it at the end of the description and be sure not to also report the statistics in text. In reports where the statistics are numerous and reporting them in sentence form is tedious and repetitive to read, then tables or graphs are appropriate. However, every table or figure must be introduced first by a prose description of the results that alert the reader to what they can expect. Students will lose marks for having a table unless it is properly introduced, if statistics are repeated in the text, and if the table or figure is not correctly formatted (see Sections 5.4.5 and 5.4.6). 88 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5.4.2 5 All Reports In research reports for which statistics are calculated, especially if calculated using a computer package, it is advisable to begin the section with a statement about how the raw data were processed to produce the summary data. Our academic colleagues are divided on whether you should say, “Data were analysed using SPSS” since it is often taken for granted in later year reports, and published articles do not include such statements. Ask your tutor or lecturer which they prefer. In any case, you should not present the raw data themselves in this section. Again, you should not say that your department or tutor or lecturer collated and summarised the data. Pretend that the data were manipulated by you in such a way that you could make inferences about the variables you have measured. If there were no effects due to counterbalancing, you might include this at the start of the Results section. There are some examples of introductory statements about data in Figure 5.12. In later year reports, you may be expected to say whether the data included missing values and how this was dealt with. You will also be expected to say whether the assumptions were met for the statistical tests used. However, this is not usually required of your early reports, since the staff usually cleans the data before presenting it to you. Check with your lecturer or tutor if these details are required. Mean scores for the groups of interest were compared using t-tests. All sets of items were analysed as a function of the 3 (condition: verbal instruction, written instruction, no instruction) between subjects by 2 (gender: men, women) within subjects factors using analysis of variance (ANOVA). Scheffé tests were used to compare means where appropriate. Individual-participant mean latency data, collapsed over the eight trial blocks, were submitted to a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) to assess the effects that were due to judgment-type and stimulus complexity. Of 530 respondents at Time 1, three (0.6%) were excluded from analyses because they had missing data on 10 or more items of the NEO-FFI. Item-level analyses were therefore performed on a sample of 527 participants. (Adapted from Murray, Rawlings, Allen, & Trinder, 2003) “The responses to the four initial on-line judgments were averaged to form an index, as were the four later memory-based judgments. Means are displayed in Table 4.” (Schul & Burnstein, 1990, p. 969) “Characteristics of participants and nonparticipants from the original cohort are presented in Table 1. Compared with those from the original cohort who did not take part in the current study (n 5 1648), those participants who took part in the current study (n 5 1220) were more likely to have older mothers, older fathers, and to have come from families with an income above the poverty line during pregnancy with the study child.” (Tearne et al., 2016, p. 4) Figure 5.12. Examples of introductory statements in the Results section. 89 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays You should introduce and present descriptive statistics first. This usually entails describing and presenting the means and standard deviations, or perhaps medians and semi-interquartile ranges. Then you should describe any inferential tests of significance that you have calculated, such as correlations, t-tests or analyses of variance (ANOVAs), and any post-hoc comparisons of means. You should not assume that your reader has remembered everything already said. It is important to express your results in a way that makes clear that your hypotheses are being appropriately tested. You need to express yourself clearly so the reader is reminded of why the data are important and why they are being compared, but do your best not to be too repetitive. For studies where there are only one or two hypotheses, it is sufficient simply to report what you found, as shown in the previous section. For reports with more than two hypotheses, you might remind the reader what each hypothesis was by saying something like: A one-tailed paired-samples t-test found that respondents reported the quality of their own relationship to be significantly higher (M 5 4.40, SD 5 0.81) than that of the average person (M 5 3.67, SD 5 0.68), t(120) 5 8.58, p , .001. If there were six paired comparisons made, you might put the means, standard deviations, and t-statistics (t, degrees of freedom, and p) into a table. In such a case, having introduced the table by saying something like “Means, standard deviations, and t-statistics for all pairs of judgments are shown in Table 1”, you could follow that with: “As can be seen in Table 1, on average, respondents reported that their own relationships were significantly better than those of the average typical person.” Do not repeat statistics from a table in your text. If your table has a large number of means and standard deviations and no room for t-statistics, you may say in the text: “Table 2 shows that men reported lower levels of emotional stability than women. This was found to be significantly different, t(108) 5 4.32, p , .001.” You do not need to give the means again. It also saves on word count, since tables may not be considered part of your word count. Reporting statistics. There are strict conventions regarding how statistics are reported. Any ordinary letter representing a statistic (M, SD, r, t, F) should be italicised, and you should state the nature of the test, the degrees of freedom, the value of the statistic, and the level of significance, in that order. Greek letters, such as Chi-square (x2), are not italicised. There are several examples of the conventions for reporting frequently used inferential statistics in Figure 5.13. Journal articles usually only report statistics if they are significant. However, students are typically required to report all statistics in research reports to indicate that you know that they are non-significant. Check with your tutor or lecturer to see what they expect. Significance levels. The 6th edition of the APA Publication Manual expects significance levels to show the exact probability for any value larger than p 5 .001 (e.g., p 5 .023). Of course, this system needs a statement early in the Results saying that the significance level has been chosen as p , .05 so that you can tell which probabilities are significant 90 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 A one-tailed t-test for independent samples indicated that the younger adults group had a significantly faster reaction time (M 5 320 ms, SD 5 48 ms) than the older adults group (M 5 392 ms, SD 5 56 ms), t(38) 5 4.37, p , .001. With lower levels of marital satisfaction, husbands tended to exhibit more avoidant behaviours than wives, r(n 5 120) 5 –.28, p 5 .023. Although the corresponding correlation for wives was in the expected direction, their marital satisfaction was unrelated to their avoidant behaviour, r(n 5 145) 5 –.07, p 5 .69. As shown in Table 3, people with intellectual disabilities were more than twice as likely as people without disabilities to be the victims of a personal offence, x2(1, N 5 300) 5 32.22, p 5 .003. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed on participants’ rated satisfaction with their group’s performance. There was no interaction between own group and other group feedback, F(1,52) 5 2.03, p 5 .095. The ANOVA did reveal a significant main effect of own group feedback, F(1,52) 5 120.35, p , .001, but the main effect of other group feedback was not significant, F(1,52) 5 1.49, p 5 .59. Figure 5.13. Examples of the conventions of reporting statistical findings. Note: Roman letters indicating statistics are italicised, and the general information to be stated is: (a) the type of statistic, (b) degrees of freedom, (c) the calculated statistic itself, and (d) the significance level. and which are not. The only exceptions are where the exact probability is less than p 5 .000, when you should write p , .001, or in tables where there are many statistics. In the latter case you can use an asterisk system, with a note below the table saying that * 5 p , .05, ** 5 p , .01, *** 5 p , .001. Examples of both significant and non-significant results of statistical tests are also shown in Figure 5.13. Please note that you should always say “non-significant” and never “insignificant”. Non-significant means that you have done a statistical test and the resulting level of significance is greater than your criterion. Insignificant is a non-technical term implying “meaningless”. A statistical test may give you a result that is statistically significant (say a difference in IQ between two groups of only one or two IQ points) but is, in a practical sense, meaningless. When you come to interpret your results, you are expected to know the difference. Confidence intervals and effect sizes. Most undergraduate psychology research reports use directional hypotheses, for example, that there is a predicted difference or relationship between two values in a specified direction (e.g., “women will score higher than men on . . .”, “self-esteem will be positively correlated with . . .”). The probability of this hypothesis being supported is then tested at some significance level, usually p , .05. Psychologists have long been engaged in a complex argument about significance testing. The APA Publication Manual (6th ed.) states that this should only be a starting point, and that more information in the form of confidence intervals and effect sizes should be reported. However, these are not always taught in first-year psychology or statistics 91 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays classes. Unless you have been taught them, you cannot be expected to use them in your first few reports. More advanced statistics classes do teach them, and, if this applies to you, you will be expected to report them in your later year research reports. Stating whether hypotheses are supported. There is no formal convention for when you should state whether a hypothesis is supported or not. If you only have one or two straightforward hypotheses, which are simply tested, it is best to leave such a statement until the start of the Discussion. In a more complex study, it may be acceptable to make a statement of support (or otherwise) in the Results section. You can do this succinctly by beginning your statements: “As predicted . . .”, or ending them with “. . . as hypothesised”. In a study where you report the results of, say, an analysis of variance, you would normally have the means and standard deviations in a table. Report the main effects, something like this: There was a significant main effect of type of relationship, F(1, 180) 5 15.31, p 5 .002. As shown in Table 1, own relationships were reported to be better than others’ relationships, as predicted. However, if the interaction effect is significant, it is often meaningless to discuss the main effects. Assuming that you hypothesised such an interaction, you could remind the reader of it, as follows: A significant interaction was found, F(2, 203) 5 10.64, p 5 .006. An inspection of the means in Table 1 indicated, as hypothesised, that the difference reported between own and others’ relationships was greater for romantic relationships than for parental ones. Note that in more advanced research reports you may be expected to perform post-hoc tests to determine whether the above effect was statistically significant, not just a simple inspection of the means. However, do not make any further interpretation of the results beyond the simple statement of support, as illustrated. Check with your tutor or lecturer about their preference. 5.4.3 Numbers and Words When reading journal articles, you will sometimes see numbers written in words (three, eight), and sometimes as Arabic numerals (11, 124). The convention is that you should use numerals to express numbers 10 and above (e.g., “the remaining 10 participants”, “25 years old”), but use words for the numbers one to nine. There are, however, exceptions to this rule. In the following examples, Arabic numerals should be used: where numbers below 10 are grouped for comparison with a number of 10 or above (e.g., “3 of 21 analyses”), and when they precede a unit of measurement (e.g., “2 cm”, “5 mg dose”); when describing mathematical functions (e.g., 3 times as many), quantities, percentages or proportions (e.g., “5%”); when describing a time, date, age, sum of money, or score on a scale (e.g., “scored 5 on and 6-point scale”). 92 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 You should use words rather than numerals when beginning a sentence (e.g., “Seventyfour children participated . . .”), for numbers below 10 that do not fit the previously mentioned criteria (“a total of six items . . .”), for ordinal numbers (e.g., “first”), and for common fractions (“one-fifth of the class . . .”, “reduced by two-thirds . . .”). In addition, a combination of numbers and words should be used to express rounded large numbers (“almost 3 million people . . .”) and considered for back-to-back modifiers (“two 2-way interactions”) if this improves readability (e.g., “the first three items were excluded”). 5.4.4 Decimals The number of decimal places reported should reflect the precision of your measurement, with numbers typically reported to two decimal places even if your analyses provides you with more! It gives a false impression of the accuracy of your measurement but is unlikely to be meaningful. The convention is to round up the second decimal place if the third digit is 5 or greater, and to round down if it is 4 or less. For example, 4.235 should round to 4.24, but 4.234 should round to 4.23. An exception to using two decimals is when reporting probability (i.e., p) values (see Significance levels on pp. 90–91). Finally, you should use a zero before the decimal point when reporting values that are less than one, except where the number, by its nature, cannot be greater than one, such as correlations, proportions, and levels of statistical significance. For example, use “0.23 cm” but “r 5 .74, p 5 .023”. 5.4.5 Tables One of the most frequent errors that students make in presenting results is to assume that a table or figure is sufficient, or even essential. This is not true. Tables or figures should be regarded as tools that should be used to summarise complex results which are repetitive to read, and which allow the easy comparison of multiple values. As a result, they must be described, highlighting the important results or comparisons, and cannot be used to replace the text, although the actual values should never be expressed in both a table and in text. As a simple rule, a table should not be used for a single (e.g., t-test or correlation) result. Such results are best reported using a standard sentence. There is a good deal of evidence indicating that even experts are poor at interpreting many tables and figures without clear instructions. You should therefore tell the reader what to look for in your table or figure and give enough explanation to make the table easily understandable, before presenting the table in your report. The APA Publication Manual (6th ed.) reminds us that if you describe every value that appears in a table, the table is not necessary, so your description should highlight and summarise key findings. Some examples of introductions to tables are shown in Figure 5.14. Tables should be shown as soon as possible after you introduce them. This will usually be before the next paragraph starts, with a space before and after the table or figure to set them off from your text. However, it is important that tables and their titles are not 93 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Table 5 shows the proportions of participants who responded in each category in each condition. Table 4 presents the correlations between the two subscales of the Marital Satisfaction Scale and the three subscales of the Eating Attitudes Scale. The participants’ payoffs for the two choices as a function of the number of others who invest in the joint account are shown in Table 1. Figure 5.14. Introductions to tables. split over two pages, so it is acceptable to separate them from when you introduced them by another paragraph. There are a number of conventions for the presentation of tables that you must adhere to. Tables are numbered successively, using Arabic integers only (1, 2, 3 . . .), with each referred to in text. Each has a title at the top, which should be clear and explanatory, preferably stating which variables are presented in the table and how this is arranged (e.g., “Means and standard deviations for self-esteem and well-being as a function of participants’ gender”). The title begins with the word “Table” and the table number and should be on a line of its own, not underlined or in italics. The title should begin on the next line and should be in italicised sentence case. Neither line of the table title has a full stop at the end. There are some examples of good titles for tables in Figure 5.15. When preparing an article for publication, tables are expected to be double-spaced. This is less critical in an undergraduate report, but avoid small, cramped tables. Separate major sections of a table with horizontal lines, but never include vertical lines in your tables (so avoid the temptation to show off your computer’s grid-drawing capabilities!). If your table contains comparisons between groups, include the sample sizes of the groups. Use a lowercase n for group sizes and a capital N for the total sample size. Use a note below the final horizontal line to explain anything that might not be Table 3 Percentage of Correctly Classified Stimuli for Younger and Older Adult Participants Table 1 Mean (and Standard Deviation) Anagram Solution Times for Three Groups of Differing Task Complexity Table 2 Correlations Between Components of Defensiveness Composite Score and Independent Variables Figure 5.15. Examples of titles of tables. Note: For table titles, major words are capitalised and there is no full stop. 94 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 immediately obvious, such as abbreviations, although it is preferable to use the full names of variables in the table unless impossible due to length. An example of a typical simple table is shown in Table 5.1, and a more complex one in Table 5.2. A table of correlations is shown in Table 5.3. Table 5.4 shows the means and standard deviations Table 5.1 Mean (and Standard Deviation) Anagram Solution Times for Three Groups of Differing Task Complexity Task N M time in seconds (SD) Difficult Moderate Easy N 5 34 12 13 9 51.66 (12.17) 43.84 (11.35) 29.21 (9.98) Note: For any report, except perhaps the very first one you do, you need to include standard deviations. Table 5.2 Mean (and Standard Deviation) Ratings of Individual and Corporate Wrongdoing by High and Low Conservative Participants Conservatism Judgment of Wrongdoing Items Individual Knew beforehand Recklessness Morally wrong Deserves punishment Harm to workers Fairness of criminal charge Corporation Knew beforehand Recklessness Morally wrong Deserves punishment Harm to workers Fairness of criminal charge N 5 40 High (n 5 23) M (SD) Low (n 5 17) M (SD) 1.33 (1.10) 2.33 (0.87) 2.56 (0.71) 2.22 (0.94) 2.89 (0.63) 2.67 (0.49) 1.44 (0.98) 2.11 (0.88) 2.11 (0.61) 2.22 (1.02) 3.56 (0.65) 1.89 (0.31) 2.09 (1.21) 3.18 (0.88) 3.18 (0.74) 3.46 (1.33) 3.46 (0.54) 3.37 (0.22) 2.00 (1.00) 4.09 (0.94) 3.73 (0.71) 3.55 (1.12) 3.46 (0.49) 3.46 (0.24) Note: In more advanced reports you would be expected to include standard deviations in parentheses beside each value (as shown here), and perhaps a t-statistic and significance level if you used one to compare the high and low conservative means for each category. 95 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Table 5.3 Intercorrelations Between Lemon Sourness and Personality Variables 1. Sourness 2. Extraversion 3. Neuroticism 4. Arousability (SEA) 5. Arousal 1 2 3 4 5 2 2.34* .09 .08 .25* 2 2.16* 2.08 2.10 2 .56** .17* 2 .17* 2 Notes: N 5 324, * 5 p , .05, ** 5 p ,.01. SEA 5 Scale of Emotional Arousability (Braithwaite, 1987) Note: The correlation of any variable with itself is, of course, 1.0, but since everyone knows that, there is no need to report it. In a more advanced report, you could put the Cronbach alpha reliabilities for the appropriate subscales on the diagonals instead of a hyphen. If you performed correlations separately for two groups, say adolescents and elderly people, you could put one lot of correlations above the diagonal and one below, telling the reader which was which in a note. Table 5.4 Means (and Standard Deviations) of Personality Factors by Humour Type Humour Type Indis. high Neuroticism Extraversion Psych. well-being 36.61 45.03a 22.62a (8.72) (5.32) (2.14) Optimum 33.13a 42.84b 23.31b (7.92) (5.21) (2.54) Low 38.91a 37.84ab 20.32ab (8.50) (5.21) (3.13) Note: Figures in parentheses are standard deviations. Means in any row having identical superscripts are different at p , .05 (Scheffé). Psych. well-being 5 Psychological well-being, Indis. high 5 Indiscriminately high. expected from an analysis of variance. If you include a note, it should be placed directly under the table, beginning with the italicised word Note. As can be seen in these examples, notes can explain several things, such as: abbreviations presented in parentheses (Table 5.3); the meaning attached to an asterisk, particularly in a table of correlations to show how significant some correlations are (Table 5.3; note that the level of significance is included in the note at the foot of the table); that the superscripts indicate which means in any row are significantly different (Table 5.4); and the meanings of abbreviated words used in the table (Table 5.4). It is acceptable to have the note in a smaller font than the rest of the table. Tables most often present quantitative data. In undergraduate reports they are usually collections of numbers, appropriately identified. However, a table could include any other material that organises information to be presented under two or more headings simultaneously, for comparison (e.g., systematic literature reviews). Anything else is called a figure. 96 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5.4.6 5 Figures In an undergraduate research report, a figure will usually be a graph (e.g., a histogram), or occasionally an example of pictorial stimuli, diagram of apparatus, or the depiction of a trial (e.g., order of presented stimuli and associated durations). In journal articles, figures could include photographs or drawings, as in the example research report in Appendix A. Like tables, figures are numbered sequentially in Arabic numerals and must be introduced in text. Unlike tables, the title of a figure should go below the figure. The figure number should be italicised and the number is followed by a full stop and followed by the title itself on the same line, also ending in a full stop; the title is not italicised. Furthermore, a capital letter should only be used for the first letter of the first word of the title. Look at Figure 5.16 for some examples of figure titles. In undergraduate research reports, the figure should not be smaller than about 8 cm by 12 cm or larger than about half a page. Leave enough space above and below the figure so that it stands clear of the text. Never continue text alongside a figure, or extend a figure over a page break, or split the figure and its title over two pages. The height of the figure should be about two-thirds its width. Each axis of a graph or histogram should be clearly labelled. It is conventional to plot the independent variable on the horizontal axis and the dependent variable on the vertical axis. Break the axis with a double slash if the origin of the coordinates is not zero, but this should be avoided in the vast majority of cases as it misrepresents the effect or comparison. Similarly, every attempt should be made to ensure figures are presented using axes that allow comparisons when appropriate. Remember to mark accurately the plotted points on a graph. Figure 5.17 is a typical figure from an undergraduate report. When producing figures, you should ensure that they convey your findings accurately. If a result is statistically significant, ensure that the figure shows the difference. This is largely a matter of choosing the appropriate scales for your axes. The importance of this is shown by comparing Figures 5.18 and 5.19. In contrast, a statistically non-significant result should also be reflected in your figure, and if all results being reported are non-significant, then a figure is not necessary. Rather, you could just say so in words. Examining the examples, Figure 5.18 does not give any idea about how different the findings are as the scale hides the range of the effect, where Figure 5.19 does show Figure 1. Mean number of correctly recalled nonsense syllables and meaningful words over 10 trials. Figure 3. Means Ratings (and Standard Deviations) of marital satisfaction by age and stage of family life cycle. Figure 4. Proportion of utterances for friends and strangers marked by each cue. Figure 5.16. Examples of figure titles. Note: For figure titles, major words are not capitalised and there is a full stop. 97 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 10 Words Nonsense syllables Mean number of correct responses 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Number of trials Figure 5.17. Mean number of correctly recalled nonsense syllables and meaningful words over 10 trials. the difference but suggests several results may, in fact, be significantly different because the slashes allow you to focus on the small range of the vertical axis (i.e., does not show the whole scale range). For this reason, both Figure 5.18 and 5.19 are inferior to Figure 5.20, which gives an accurate illustration of the effect because it includes error bars. As a result, we can see clearly that there is no significant difference between the lexical decision and naming conditions when the independent variable is words, but that there is a difference when the independent variable is non-words. One advantage of the use of error bars indicating confidence intervals is shown in Figure 5.20. This shows the same data as in Figures 5.18 and 5.19, but with error bars added. The lengths of the error bars, and whether they overlap, give a visual indication of the significance of the result. The scale of the vertical axis, so that the height of the histograms largely fills the figure, also helps you make sense of the differences in variable means. If results are presented in different figures, as might be the case for multiple studies, they should be able to be compared, so make sure they have exactly the same axes. This will save your readers having to make complicated transformations in their heads, which will hinder your message and lose you marks. Avoid showing off what you can do with the fancier graphical capabilities of Microsoft Excel or SPSS. Do not use three-dimensional or coloured graphs. Similarly, there are reasons for using pie charts (e.g., proportions of a total) and line graphs (e.g., indicating that there has been a change between one point and the next, such as improvement for an individual or group over time). However, unless this is the kind of result you are presenting, you should use a histogram. In sum, it is best to use the simplest graphical design that illustrates whether your hypothesis has been supported or not. This process should also help you decide when to use a table or figure. 98 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Reaction time (seconds) S ections of a Research Repor t 7 Lexical decision 6 Naming 5 5 4 3 2 1 0 Non-words Words Figure 5.18. Mean reaction time for word and non-word targets as a function of decision type. The data shown in this figure are the same as in Figure 5.19, but are given in seconds, so any differences are minimised. Reaction time (milliseconds) 740 Lexical decision Naming 700 660 620 580 540 500 0 Words Non-words Reaction time (milliseconds) Figure 5.19. The data shown in this figure are the same as in Figure 5.18, but are shown here in milliseconds so that differences are accentuated. 900 Lexical decision 800 Naming 700 600 500 400 300 200 100 0 Words Non-words Figure 5.20. The data shown in this figure are the same as in Figures 5.18 and 5.19 but are scaled to show the entire range, and to show error bars. If you had this figure in a report, you would need to add to the title a statement saying that the error bars are confidence intervals or standard deviations, whichever you use. 99 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 5.5 Discussion The Discussion, as in the Method and Results sections, follows straight on with the single (level 1) heading Discussion in bold sentence case, centred across the page. It is not necessary to start a new page. Remember that in Chapter 1 the report was described as being like an hourglass in shape? The Discussion can be viewed as the other end of the hourglass from the Introduction. Where the Introduction begins with a broad statement of the area under study and becomes more specific as you argue the case for your particular hypotheses, the Discussion should begin specifically, discussing your hypotheses, and then broaden out to discuss the more general implications and conclusions of your research. The Discussion begins with specific statements about whether the hypotheses were supported. These statements are followed by interpretation of the results, and discussion of how your results relate to the previous research, most likely discussed in your Introduction. At the end of the Discussion, the hourglass is again at its broadest, with discussion of the implications your results have for the “real world”, although you should avoid such a flippant expression! Let’s look at each of these parts of the Discussion in more detail. State whether hypotheses have been supported. You should not expect your reader to remember exactly what your hypotheses were, so it is advisable to restate them. However, avoid word-for-word repetition from your introduction as well as repeating information already in the Results section. For example, you could say: “The results of the current study support the hypothesis that parents perceive greater similarity between their children than those children perceive between themselves.” Be very careful about what your results tell you. Never use the words “prove” or “disprove”. A single experiment does not unequivocally disprove a hypothesis, and no hypothesis can ever be proved. Someone could always come along and think of a different explanation that also fits the observed facts. What you should say is that your results support, or fail to support, your hypotheses. If you have several hypotheses, it is best to begin your Discussion with a paragraph in which you state whether each of the hypotheses is supported or not, and how you know. But do not repeat numbers or statistics from the Results. In the following paragraph(s) you should then discuss each hypothesis to completion before interpreting the next. Discussing each hypothesis in turn gives your Discussion greater coherence and continuity. The danger is in repeating your hypotheses frequently, which gets boringly repetitive. It is acceptable to have an introductory sentence stating: “Results indicate that all (or most of) the hypotheses were supported.” Or, “While the hypothesis that . . . was supported by the results, there was only partial support for the prediction that . . .”. The following paragraphs could elaborate on the details, and whether your results support previous findings. Interpretation of results – support for previous research. The interpretation of results relating to any one hypothesis should be dealt with in two ways. The first way is 100 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 to relate your results to previous research. Make a brief but clear statement of whether the results are or are not consistent with the research you described in your Introduction, and which led you to make that particular hypothesis. For example: “The observation that the groups asked to rate whether words were pleasant or unpleasant had better recall than those asked to judge whether the words contained the letter ‘e’ is consistent with the findings of Hyde and Jenkins (1969).” This is the second time in the Discussion that the hypothesis has been stated, although perhaps in a slightly different form. This should be the last time, as constant repetition of the hypothesis is irritating to a reader. You need to find a balance between reminding the reader what the hypothesis is and boring them to death with it. Note that your results support previous research and not vice versa. Theirs came first, so it is not accurate to say that their work supports your findings! Interpretation of results – explanation of findings. The second way that your results need to be dealt with is at the level of explanation. In the first few research reports you will encounter, the chances are that you will be replicating a previously published study. Replication is one of the aspects of the scientific method, and its importance should not be underestimated. You will therefore know how the author or authors of that study interpreted their results. In that case, you can put their explanation into your own words. Following the sentence in the previous example, you could say: “Hyde and Jenkins interpreted such findings as supporting the depth of processing theory, that material processed at the semantic or deep level would be better remembered than material processed at the surface level of appearance.” Remember that since you have just referred to Hyde and Jenkins (1969) in the previous sentence, you do not have to repeat the year of publication. However, if it were a new paragraph you would be expected to include the year of publication. If your study is not a replication of a previous study, then you have to be more thoughtful in finding a possible explanation for the results you obtained. There are two major types of explanation possible for a particular set of results. One explanation is based on theoretical considerations and the other on methodological considerations. The distinction is illustrated by the following example from Macrae (1989). Suppose a psychologist, Jane Bloggs, theorises that due to a genetic factor, men demonstrate superior spatial ability compared to women, and she finds that several findings exist to support this theory. Suppose that you have just designed a new test of spatial ability and have administered it to a large number of psychology undergraduates. There are three possible outcomes and a number of interpretations. The first possible outcome is that you find that men do perform better on your test. Based on the Bloggs theory, you hypothesised that men would do better and your results supported your hypothesis and, by inference, the theory is further supported. However, it is not possible to rule out the explanation that a third variable associated with gender is responsible for this finding. For example, your spatial test and most others are biased because the items tend to be selected from material that men are more likely to have encountered at school. Thus, the finding that there is a gender difference is not due to genetic factors, but to differential socialisation of men and women. 101 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays The second possible outcome is that you find no difference between mean scores for men and women. One explanation is that, since you had hypothesised that men would do better, based on the Bloggs theory, and your results do not support your hypothesis, that casts doubt on the theory. A finding of this kind pushes you to consider how your research differs from previous research (e.g., sample, details of the measure, procedure such as instructions). For example, if Bloggs’ research is the only one to use a university sample, then perhaps women university students are not representative of the general population of women but actually have higher spatial ability than most women, while men university students are more typical of the general population of men, at least in spatial ability. Your failure to find a gender difference actually reflects inadequate sampling from the population to which the Bloggs theory applies. However, if there are no clear or systematic differences between your method and previous findings, it might be necessary to look for an alternative explanation in previous research (e.g., what other visuo-spatial tasks do men and women typically demonstrate similar performance on?). This kind of researching will likely be expected for students in later years of study. The third possibility is that women score better on spatial ability than men. As in the previous paragraph, your results do not support your hypothesis, and so doubt is cast on the Bloggs theory. Again, as in the previous paragraph, a different explanation is that women university students are better at spatial ability than most other women, while, in contrast to the previous paragraph, men university students are actually worse than most other men. Once again, your failure to support your hypothesis reflects inadequate sampling from the general population and may require you to return to the literature for further explanation. In each possible outcome, the first explanation is a straightforward theoretical one. Either the theory is supported or it is not. The second explanation for each outcome reinforces the importance of not being dogmatic about proving things. A different but quite logical explanation fits the same observed facts. In the second explanation for each outcome, there is a clear methodological difference (i.e., the new measure) affecting the results. These considerations are themselves capable of being tested in later research. A good research report would recognise that there are alternative explanations, acknowledge the limitations of the current research to distinguish between these, and would recommend ways of testing them. New references in the Discussion? A question that is frequently asked is whether it is permitted to introduce reference material into the Discussion if it has not already been mentioned in the Introduction. Academics disagree on this, so always check with your tutor or lecturer as to what they expect. In your first one or two research reports, we would discourage this practice. Such reports are often replications of well-established studies, and, whatever your results, you should not need any material in the Discussion that you have not already dealt with in the Introduction. In later-year research reports, where the outcome of the study is less certain, you may find that your hypotheses turn out not to be supported, in spite of you having carefully justified them in the Introduction using relevant references. In casting around for an 102 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 explanation for why your expectations turned out not to be true, you may occasionally come across a reference that provides some alternative evidence. If it is directly in the area you are researching, it should have been mentioned in the Introduction as a possible alternative outcome. However, if it has some bearing on your findings, but belongs to a slightly different literature, it may be useful to introduce it in the Discussion. If so, you will need to explain what the relevance is and how it might inform the interpretation of your finding, but briefly. Implications. Having discussed the compatibility of your results with those of previous research, and given an explanation for the results, you need to discuss the implications of the results. The explanation you gave for the results you obtained will presumably be applicable more widely than for the specific instance you tested. Say how this might be so, especially if the results allow you to extend or consolidate a theory. Usually, undergraduate studies have a fairly applied flavour, so you should be able to say what the implications of your results are for the wider community. Keep in mind, however, that there may be other explanations, so avoid overstating where possible. For example, in a questionnaire study of attitudes to corporate or individual responsibility for wrongdoing, you may find that people judge corporations more harshly. You could then state that such a finding has implications for how monetary compensation is calculated in legal settlements. However, to conclude that individuals would be unlikely to sanction would be an overstatement. Limitations. You also need to deal with other possible methodological considerations in your Discussion section. Doing so indicates that you are aware of extraneous factors that may have affected your results. However, you need to distinguish between faults in design and problems in procedure. An example of a design fault is an investigation of the effectiveness of two different lecturers that uses one class where the students are on average 10 years older than those in the other class. The effects of lecturer are confounded with the effects of student age. It is unlikely that you will find such design flaws in a study designed by psychology staff. An example of a procedural problem is running a well-designed study under crowded and noisy conditions. However, while this may not be ideal, it is not necessarily a confound and may be quite typical of “real-world” settings in which people perform attentionally demanding cognitive tasks. A common problem with students’ reports is that they waste space in the Discussion in criticising the conduct of the experiment. Many undergraduate experiments run as teaching exercises are impossible to run under ideal conditions. However, unless there is a specific reason such an environment could compromise your specific results, this is unlikely to be a limitation of your study. That is, procedural problems such as varying ages, times of testing (day and evening students), crowded conditions, noise, and similar factors rarely have systematic effects on the results. Rather, you should focus on faulty design or methodological confounds, specifically describing any systematic effects this may have had on the results. Unless the effect is clearly visible and has an explicitly explained effect on your results, you cannot say that the problem has occurred. Simply 103 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays saying that something “may” have affected the results will lose you marks. If you want to let your reader know that you have thought about these factors, you should mention that no obvious systematic effects on the results were observed. In many studies using undergraduate psychology students as participants, an obvious potential limitation is the gender imbalance, since there are usually three times as many women in the course as men. However, unless there is a good theoretical reason why gender may have made a difference to the results, there is no point in mentioning this. Similarly, unless there is a good reason to expect that university students may be different from the wider community on the variables measured, there is no point in saying that the sample is unrepresentative. Although modern research is increasingly finding cultural differences in areas previously considered universal, it is unrealistic to expect any study to account for all possible cultural differences. Unless you are prepared to cite research describing such differences, you will lose marks for merely saying that the result may not hold in other cultures. Finally, students sometimes cite the sample size as a limitation. If your results are significant, this is not a statistical limitation. However, it may imply that the sample is not representative of a wider population. Conclusion. Finally, you need a concluding paragraph that sums up what the study has found. Be careful not to repeat statements from earlier in the report. The conclusion should be a summary or overview. It is sometimes easier to state what future research should be done to take your findings a step further, or to control for faulty design that became apparent in the running of the experiment. This should be done carefully. It is boring and glib simply to say that more research needs to be done. You must be specific in saying what variables need to be accounted for or controlled. It is better to have a concluding paragraph that is a neat précis of the study, emphasising the conclusions you have reached. An example from a first-year report might look like this: In conclusion, this study has revealed that people perceive their future selves most often in terms of their occupational or career opportunities. This was particularly evident in the younger age groups. The area of family was also considered important. Relative levels of well-being seemed to generate no real difference in participants’ perceptions of possible future selves. All participants, irrespective of age, felt capable of achieving their future selves. The Discussion is the last section in the body of the report. As mentioned earlier, it is usually easiest to write the Method section first, then the Results, then the Introduction, and finally the Discussion and Abstract. Having produced a first draft of all these sections, you should give them, in the correct order, to someone else to read critically. Based on the feedback you receive from this person, you should redraft your report. Then repeat the process, getting someone else to read your second draft. Any student serious about achieving a Pass should do at least this much: a rough draft, then a revised draft that will need fewer or minor modifications following proofreading, which specifically means reading it aloud, ideally (but not necessarily) to another person. 104 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 Having had your second draft critically read, you are ready to add the finishing touches. The Abstract and Title Page have already been mentioned. You now need to deal with the References and the Appendices. 5.6 References This section comes after the Discussion, has the heading References in bold sentence case, centred across the page and, unlike the earlier sections in the body of the report, does begin on a new page. It is important to recognise that this section is not a bibliography. A bibliography is a list of all the references you have consulted, even if you have not included them in the final report. Do not include a bibliography in a psychology research report. Your reference list should include all (and only) references that you have referred to in the body of your report, and that you have actually read. If you read in your textbook that James (1890) said something, it is the textbook that should be in the reference list, not James (1890), although many of these texts can now be accessed at York Classics in Psychology (http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/) if you are interested. If your marker sees a particularly interesting or obscure reference, they might ask you for a copy of it. If you don’t have it, you will be in trouble! Similarly, if you only read the abstract in PsycINFO but not the entire article, be honest in your reference list. So, remember that if you have written “Stoner (cited in Turner, Hogg, & Wetherell, 1991)” then only the Turner, Hogg, and Wetherell (1991) reference is included in the reference list. Note that all types of references, including journal articles, books, and book chapters are presented in a single alphabetically ordered list. The categories covered in Chapter 3 have been separated, but only for your convenience in checking out the conventional format. The details of the conventions of how to report your references were included in Chapter 3. 5.7 Appendices It is rare for a journal article to include any Appendices at all, except occasionally for a list of stimuli or the items in a scale used in that study. However, it is not unusual for student research reports to do so. In many psychology departments, students are requested to include their statistical analyses, either handwritten or as computer printouts. This is to allow the marker to check that what you stated in the Results section has been correctly calculated and accurately reported. It also allows for realistic damage control. If you have made a mathematical error that affects your interpretation of the test of a hypothesis, then you will typically be penalised only for that one mistake, even though it affects the remainder of your report, as long as the remainder of the report is consistent with the mistaken interpretation. Another occasion where the inclusion of statistical calculations is especially important is in cases where you have been expected to create one or more hypotheses to test, 105 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays rather than having them given to you. Your marker needs to know that the statistic quoted to test a hypothesis has been correctly applied and calculated. Please note, you should not include tables or figures in an Appendix. If tables or figures relating to your results are worth drawing up, then they belong in the Results section. This will also include assumptions, if required (i.e., at higher levels). Format of Appendices. If there is only one Appendix, label it Appendix in bold sentence case, centred across the page, and starting on a new page. If there are several appendices, each should start on a new page, and you should call them Appendix A, Appendix B, etc., followed by the title, centred across the page, as follows: Appendix A List of anagrams and their solutions used in group size experiment When you have got this far, you can breathe a sigh of relief. All you need to do is hand in the completed report. If you have been efficient with your time management, you should still have 48 hours or so before the report is due. At this point, if you are using Turnitin, you should submit your final draft. You should then check back in approximately 12 to 24 hours for the Originality Report, and make changes as needed (e.g., correcting incorrectly attributed quotations or improving paraphrasing). Finally, proofread (again, preferably aloud) your final draft and check for orphaned headings (i.e., where section, table, and figure heading have become separated from the following material). Also, remember to keep a copy of the final report, either a hardcopy or a file on a USB stick or cloud-based backup service. 5.8 Further Reading The major arbiter of current convention in psychological report writing is the APA Publication Manual (6th ed.). Here is the full reference: American Psychological Association. (2010). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. This book gives minute details of the conventions for writing articles intended to be published in APA journals. If you intend to become an academic psychologist, this book is an essential purchase. You can usually find a copy on Reserve in your library, and we encourage you to have a look at it. If you are happier looking at material online, you might like to explore Purdue University’s Online Writing Lab, which summarises a lot of the APA Publication Manual’s conventions. Here’s the link: https://owl.english.purdue.edu/owl/resource/560/01 106 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 Another useful book is Sternberg’s on writing scientific reports. Although it says it is for students, it is really aimed at researchers at the beginning of their research publication careers. However, it is quite readable, and if your library has a copy it is well worth having a look at it. Here is the reference: Sternberg, R. J. (2010). The psychologist’s companion: A guide to scientific writing for students and researchers (5th ed.). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Some libraries have Sternberg’s book as an eBook, which can be read online. Alternatively, your bookshop will order one for you. Study skills books that we have found useful are the following: Forshaw, M. (2012). Critical thinking for psychology. West Sussex, UK: BPS Blackwell. Goodson, P. (2017). Becoming an academic writer. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Your library will have many books on student skills. Search the catalogue for “study skills” or look around call number 371 in your library. 5.9 Feedback As markers we are frustrated if we have written what we believe to be helpful comments on students’ reports and find that students look only at the grade they receive. It will help you if you read the marker’s comments when you get your report back. However, it is unwise to rush up to your tutor or lecturer demanding an immediate explanation of your mark or even the comments. Some institutions have a policy that assignment grades will not be discussed for 24–72 hours after their return. Take the report home and think about what has been commented on, especially as it relates to what you wrote. If you are still unclear about what is meant, make an appointment to speak to your tutor or lecturer about the report at a mutually convenient time, if possible. Make notes about that discussion and keep them with the report. Next time you have to write a report, all the material will be together, and this will save you having to start from the beginning again. It is certainly distressing to have put in a lot of work and then find that it has not been rewarded with the grade you expected. Unfortunately, effort is not marked and may not be clearly visible to your markers. However, combined with systematic and supported practice (e.g., across drafts and even assignments), it will lead to improvement both in your understanding and in your performance. Psychology research reports are different from most forms of assignment that you will be familiar with, and it usually takes time to become good at them. It will be a cumulative process over your undergraduate career. One thing to bear in mind is that reports function differently in different units (e.g., in first-year units there may be a focus on APA style, while at second year the focus may be on literature review). Consequently, it is essential for you to review the information you are given about a report, which will typically include a description of the marking (e.g., a rubric) that will 107 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays make such priorities clear. Nonetheless, it is important that you keep all your marked reports and refer to them on the next attempt so that you may continue to make progress. Students frequently complain that the comments and feedback they get are inconsistent across various tutors or lecturers. Different staff members may have their pet peeves about specific aspects of conventions (some fuss about misplaced apostrophes, while others ignore them), so what is commented on in detail may vary from one marker to another. However, there are two reasons for feedback. First, markers are trying to provide you with information to improve your writing, which can be a little idiosyncratic. Second, markers are clarifying what you have received marks for and where marks have been deducted. These comments tend to be more consistent and more related to the actual marks you receive. Moreover, all universities take considerable pains to train markers, and to moderate or check results, usually by undertaking cross- or double-marking to ensure consistency in grading. If you are unclear about the comments on your report and their relationship to your marks, you may wish to check with your lecturer or tutor. 5.10 Summary In conclusion, the following eight general points summarise the important steps you should follow when writing a psychology research report: 1. Plan ahead to make best use of the time available. 2. Search for relevant references other than those given. 3. Be crystal clear about your hypotheses. Did the study test them? Did the results support them? 4. Write an outline of your study. 5. Get something comprehensive down on paper as soon as possible. This will usually be the Method section. 6. Have someone critically read your first draft and be prepared for a fair amount of rewriting. Also have someone critically read the revised draft. Stick to the word limit. 7. Get your typed final presentation proofread for typos (and submit to Turnitin, if used). 8. Get it in on time. If there is a genuine trauma, seek an extension before the due date. 5.11 Checklist For the first two or three reports that you write, you may find it helpful to check off these steps as you do them (although you may choose to do them in a different sequence to the one that follows): Preparation Do you have all the handouts from your department (word limit, due date, etc.)? Do you have the details of the research (i.e., what was done, how many participants, groups, a copy of the questionnaire, etc.)? 108 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e S ections of a Research Repor t 5 Have you read (obtained copies of) the references given? Have you decided on your hypothesis or hypotheses? This requires some critical evaluation of the references, unless you’ve been told what hypotheses are being tested. Are they specific and testable, written in terms of the variables as measured in your study? Did the study actually test them? Have you obtained more references with results relevant to your hypotheses? Again, some critical thinking is needed, directed at these references. Have you calculated the appropriate statistics? Do they really test your hypotheses? Were the hypotheses supported? Method (see p. 77) Do you have material covering the following subsections: (a) Participants? (b) Design? Do you need a Design section? (c) Materials? (d) Procedure? Results (see p. 87) Have you included a statement of how the data were processed? Does your report include descriptive statistics? Do you need a table or figure? Have you included a description of results of statistical tests? If you’ve used a table or figure, does it meet APA conventions? Introduction (see p. 72) Have you included an opening statement of the area under consideration (but not a cliché)? Have you introduced the theoretical framework for the study, including only the relevant parts of previous research, and defined terms where needed? Have you provided evidence that your reading of previous research has been critical, rather than just taking it at face value? Have you introduced the present study, stating the aims of it and how it differs from previous research? Have you concluded with your operationalised hypotheses, written in the past tense? Discussion (see p. 100) Have you begun by stating whether the hypotheses were supported? How do the results relate to the previous research mentioned in the Introduction? What do the results mean? What are the implications for everyday life? Are there any methodological issues? Are problems systematic and observable in the results? If not, do you have a statement saying that there were no obvious methodological problems? Have you concluded with a brief summary of what you found, or a suggestion for correcting any problems you encountered? 109 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Title Page, Abstract, References (see pp. 68, 105) Have you produced a Title Page, and written an Abstract? Have you ensured that your References section includes all and only the references cited and meets APA conventions? Presentation Rearrange the sections into the proper order. Have you numbered the pages? Did you have your first draft read by someone else? Was there anything that was unclear? What do you need to do to make it clear? Have you written a complete second draft? Is it clearer to someone else than the first one? Have you read it aloud? Does the overall draft exceed the word limit? If so, cut it back. Have you typed it up double-spaced? Have the entire report proofread and submit it to Turnitin. If you are really keen, or really anxious, do as many more drafts as you can cope with! Keep a copy of the final, proofread version, either on a USB or cloud-based backup service or as a hardcopy. Submit your final report – if electronically, via the portal; or attach the institution, faculty, or departmental cover sheet and hand it in on time (or even early!). Celebrate a good job well done. You deserve it! (It’s important to celebrate achievements.) 110 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e 6 Essays in Psychology KEYWORDS analytical categories, argument, evidence IN THIS CHAPTER 6.1 How Are Essays Different From Research Reports? . . . . . . . . . . 113 6.2 Choosing an Essay Topic . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 113 6.3 Deciding What the Essay Topic Asks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 114 6.4 Preparation – Reading, Note-taking, and Planning . . . . . . . . . . 117 6.5 Structure of an Essay . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 118 6.6 Redrafting and Presentation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124 6.7 Further Reading . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 125 111 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Essays in Psychology 6.8 Checklist . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 126 How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Essays in Psychology The Process of Writing an Essay Figure 6.1. The process of writing an essay. 112 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Essays in Psychology 6 6.1 H ow Are Essays Different From Research Reports? C HAPTER 2 HIGHLIGHTED THE features that research reports and essays have in common. However, the differences between these formats are considerable. For one thing, there are fewer conventions used in essays, which allows you much more opportunity to be creative and to demonstrate your narrative skill. Similarly, essays are not usually divided into sections the way research reports are. Furthermore, psychology essays shorter than about 2500 words do not usually include subheadings; this doesn’t mean there is no structure, but the structure is simpler and more flexible. Other than the Title Page, Abstract (if your institution requires one – many do not), and References, as long as there is an introduction, a body (or discussion, or argument), and a conclusion, you have met the requirements of format. Writing a good essay is not, however, only about meeting the format requirements. A good essay clearly and succinctly presents a viewpoint or argument, supported by good evidence, and provides a good summary and conclusion. This is achieved by the use of a clear structure to the writing. For example, the Introduction to a research report typically outlines the particular sort of argument you intend to present – to justify the claims or predictions you are posing. As a student faced with an essay topic, one of the problems you must solve is to review the available evidence and decide on what to present and how to structure your argument. Since an essay doesn’t have a Results section to focus your attention on the evidence, you need to find a focus for yourself, and make it an integral part of the structure of the essay. To do this you need to carefully examine the question asked. 6.2 Choosing an Essay Topic If you are fortunate, you will be given a choice of essay topic. This is typically easier than being allowed to choose your own topic, because the staff member who set the question will have already identified answerable questions. However, how should you choose which one to do? When asked this, some students say they choose the one that looks easiest. This is a mistake. Typically, questions are designed to be of similar difficulty, which means that if one seems easy, you may not have understood this topic, or the ­others. Alternatively, if there is a comparatively straightforward topic, it is likely that markers will expect essays on this topic to be executed at a very high level for it to achieve top marks, whereas a topic which is more difficult to answer may be marked more leniently and the markers may be more accepting of less-polished answers. In essence, there is no advantage to choosing a topic just because it seems easy. Other strategies students use include attempting to decode the directive words used in the question. For example, students often report that there is a perception that essays that ask you to “discuss” something are easier than those that ask you to “critically evaluate”. This is not true. For any topic, you are expected to critically evaluate what you read. 113 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays For example, you should consider the strength of arguments presented by other authors or the quality (e.g., relevance, applicability) of the evidence. These are the very issues you will need to discuss in an essay asking you to “discuss” a topic. So, the general description of what is required may not be a great basis for choosing a topic. A third approach to choosing an essay topic is to identify a topic of interest to you. Specifically, since you will be expected to do a fair bit of reading and thinking for any essay topic, you might as well choose an area that you are interested in or would like to learn more about. There is a small risk with this approach; namely, that you might get carried away with researching or, if the topic is something you feel very strongly about, that you may have trouble being objective or critical. Try this: imagine you were given the opportunity to evaluate a topic you believed in strongly. Would you be able to fairly and critically evaluate counter evidence? This can even be true for topics you simply hold a strong point of view about. For example, students who are given the opportunity to evaluate the evidence that suggests using laptops in lectures (i.e., for accessing/taking notes) actually impairs academic performance (Patterson & Patterson, 2017) often find this is contrary to their personally held point of view and consequently dismiss this evidence, sometimes using the flawed “person who” argument (i.e., the evidence suggests use of laptops in lectures impairs academic performance, but this is not true for me . . .). You should also think twice about choosing an area in which you think you already have expert knowledge. If your brother suffers from dyslexia and you are therefore attracted to the essay question on this topic, be wary of making general statements on the basis of your single example. Your brother may not be typical. Many students, particularly mature age students, sometimes fall into the trap of writing about areas in which they have personal experience, and not always sticking to what the question asks of them. Be particularly careful of ideological hobby-horses. Although we are happy to encourage passionate belief in a good cause, it is usually very difficult to write objectively about such a topic, or to stick to what the question requires. We suggest that if you are given a list of choices, you reduce the possibilities to two or three topics which look interesting, then have a look at the references given for each and see how readable they are. Don’t be too easily discouraged, and do think about the topics you have previously studied (across any units of study). Students often forget that studying psychology is meant to be cumulative. That is, knowledge acquired in one unit can be useful in other units. That does not mean it’s okay to submit multiple assignments on the same topic – although the opportunity to do so is unlikely to present itself within a course – but it may mean you have read something relevant previously that may help you choose a good topic for an essay. 6.3 Deciding What the Essay Topic Asks There are relatively few ways that essay topics are commonly worded in psychology. Figure 6.2 gives examples of some of the sorts of essays that you might encounter in your first year or two of psychology. Look at the directive words in these topics, which are the 114 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Essays in Psychology 6 Discuss research into sibling relationships in children with a focus on the development of pro-social behaviour, jealousy, and trust. Discuss the role of deception in social psychological research. To what extent is deception a necessary methodological component of social psychological experiments? What is the impact of genetic factors on the development of self-esteem or selfworth in adolescents? Discuss the evidence. It is widely believed that ageing causes a decline in cognitive skills. Evaluate the assertion in relation to the experimental evidence on memory and skilled performance. “Being an only child is a disease in itself.” Using current research, evaluate the accuracy of this statement made by the eminent American psychologist G. Stanley Hall. Critically evaluate some of the evidence which suggests that under some conditions rewarding people can actually cause them to become less motivated. Developmental dyslexia is not a uniform disorder. Critically review the literature so as to illustrate the major sub-types of developmental dyslexia and the underlying cognitive and phonological deficits that have been identified with various types of dyslexia. Compare and contrast the following theories of love: “Love as attachment” and Sternberg’s triangular theory of love. Define, compare, and contrast the concepts of “conformity”, “compliance”, and “obedience”. Under what circumstances is conformity desirable and when is it undesirable social behaviour? Describe the processes involved in solving the problem of the Hobbits and the Orcs in terms of the three-store memory model and IDEAL problem solving. Anorexia nervosa is an eating disorder that affects an individual’s physical as well as psychological health. What are the major theories of the etiology of anorexia nervosa? The modern conception of companionate marriage puts heavy expectations on couples. What factors improve the odds of maintaining a satisfactory marital relationship? Support your assertions with research evidence. Why do out-group members seem to “all look alike”? Examine the reasons behind the perception of out-group homogeneity. Critically examine research based on attribution theory into attributions of victim responsibility for rape. To what extent does such research support attribution theories and which attribution theories are most useful in this context? Are males better equipped to be leaders than females? Critically assess whether it is just childhood interactions that influence leadership or whether other influences are involved. Figure 6.2. Examples of psychology essay titles. 115 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays words telling you to do something: discuss, evaluate, critically evaluate, critically review, compare and contrast, define, or describe. It is important that your essay does what is asked of it; that is, that you answer what is asked, not what you might like to write about the topic. If it is not clear to you exactly what the question requires, talk to the lecturer who set the topic. Discuss means to describe the relevant perspectives and evidence for each as related to the topic or question under consideration. Typically, you would find journal articles related to the topic, some in favour of a particular perspective or outcome and some that are critical of that perspective or outcome, possibly presenting an alternative perspective. For instance, the second example in Figure 6.2 is about the role of deception in research. Some writers argue that deception is necessary. Others argue that it should not be necessary; and these writers suggest some alternatives to deception. Your essay should describe what these opposing viewpoints are, but, in view of the second sentence in the question, would then go on to look at why the proponents of deception say that deception is necessary. Evaluate or critically evaluate are similar to the direction to “discuss”, but there is an expectation that you would end up with a judgment in favour of one side of the argument. You are being asked for an appraisal of the relative strength or weight of the arguments as a basis for drawing a conclusion. Here is a chance for you to express your evidenced opinion. However, don’t get carried away. Markers are usually not impressed by dogmatic personal assertions. It is wiser to use the evidence you find and critically evaluate to draw your conclusion. For example, “The weight of evidence favours . . .”. Review or critically review means to make a survey of the topic. Such an instruction usually expects a wide reading in the area. For many other instructions you might only need a fairly small number of references, but if you are asked to review an area, you must make an effort to get a fairly comprehensive coverage of the topic. This may mean there is a review of the topic available to you (e.g., a paper in the Annual Review of Psychology or a meta-analysis or systematic review). You will be expected to briefly summarise the work of previous researchers, while being critical of poor arguments or methodological problems like small sample sizes or unrepresentative samples, or any other systematic biases that appear, and summarise the current state of the topic, including any weaknesses or gaps in the literature. Compare or compare and contrast means that you are expected to look for similarities and differences between the theories or approaches described in the topic. These will most likely be named. Again, you are expected to be critical of what you read, but here you are not being asked to make a personal judgment between the two. You are really reporting what other authors have to say about the relative merits or disadvantages of the different approaches. You must come to some conclusion, at least about the importance or implications of major areas of similarity and difference. Define, describe, and examine are instructions expecting you to present the information in some depth, and, importantly, in your own words. Define means to say exactly what the concept, term, or phrase means. For example, if asked to define “conformity”, you would be expected to say that it is the tendency to change one’s behaviour, attitudes, 116 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Essays in Psychology 6 opinions, or even perceptions to match those of the bulk of others around you. You would also need to provide a reference for your definition, acknowledging where this understanding comes from. This will assist you to contextualise your definition. For example, you would also need to distinguish this tendency in particular situations from the more general personality trait of conformity. The further instruction to describe or examine means that you would go on to present a detailed account of the theory or term, including the evidence for this, and also implications for real-life situations. Note that all these directives expect you to be critical of what you read. This does not necessarily mean that you should be negative, but that, as was said in Chapter 2, you should apply critical thinking principles to what you read. Are the conclusions drawn by the authors reasonable on the basis of the evidence they report? It is perfectly acceptable to compliment authors on the representativeness of their sample or the imagination employed in operationalising some concept, just as it is to be critical of a small or unrepresentative sample, or an interpretation that ignores likely alternatives. 6.4 P reparation – Reading, Note-taking, and Planning Having decided what the topic expects of you, you need to spend quite a lot of time in preparation. As in any effortful exploit, the better the preparation the better the finished product; so in the case of your essay, time spent in preparation will pay off in the final product. For the typical psychology essay, you will have been given only one or two references to start you off. You will already have skimmed through these in order to decide which topic to choose. Now read what your textbook has to say about the area in question. This will give you a general overview of what conventional thinking is on the subject and will point you in the direction of further references. So too will the reference lists in the articles you’ve been given. With the question in mind, you might do a brief references search – consider looking for papers that have cited the reference you were given – and make a list of a handful of references that look as if they might be relevant. Take notes. Many students find it helpful to use a separate sheet of paper, or an index card for each article, and write the author(s), date, title, and journal details at the top of each sheet. Alternatively, you could use Microsoft Excel to set up a table of references or referencing software such as Endnote. Most universities have an institutional licence which allows students to download and use Endnote for free. Check with your library or information technology services. This makes life easier when identifying ideas and quotes, and correctly citing these in your essay, as well as simplifying writing your reference list. The notes you should take are the major points made by the author or authors, any key definitions or quotes, the evidence presented, and the interpretations or conclusions drawn by the authors. This information may be apparent from their Abstract or from their subheadings, but you will eventually need to read the 117 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays article in depth to find all the key information. However, during preparation, focusing on the broad strokes to identify only those papers that are entirely relevant is very helpful. Many students download or photocopy anything that might appear even remotely useful. The danger here is that you end up with an indigestible mass of paper or electronic files, which can be distracting. Once you have good summary notes, you should then build an essay plan. If the directive is to “compare and contrast” or “evaluate”, you might separate the references into heaps of “for” and “against”. If it is “discuss”, you might do the same, or you might organise them chronologically to show the evolution of thinking about the topic. You should then jot down the broad outline of what you intend your argument to be, and where you will use the references. One way to achieve this is to write down the point or claim of each paragraph and the associated reference(s). You will eventually need an introduction and a conclusion, but planning is primarily for the main body of the essay and these sections will be much clearer once your argument is fully elaborated. Now go back and read your references more thoroughly. This time, your aim is to flesh out the specific arguments you are going to make, or pieces of research you are going to describe. Here is where you should highlight parts of what you printed or photocopied. Never write on or highlight books that aren’t your own. Make a copy of the relevant bits and write on or highlight your copy, remembering to note the title, author, and date details for your reference list. Make a note of good page numbers on the summary sheet or in your table or referencing software. As with research reports, keep direct quotes to a minimum. It is always better to put the expression into your own words to indicate that you understand what is meant (acknowledging its source, of course!). Now that you have a plan, you are ready to start writing. You will probably find at first that it is pretty sketchy. You might wish to start with the section that is clearest to you, rather than the introduction – but this is a matter of writing style and preference, and you should do what works for you. You should note that you will have to go back to your references again and again to find evidence to back up assertions that you are making, or to check the accuracy of your summaries of research. You may also find that you need another look through PsycINFO or other sources to find more evidence to support some of your claims. Finally, you will definitely need to write, and rewrite your work before you are finished. 6.5 Structure of an Essay As mentioned earlier in this chapter, the essay structure is to have a Title Page, possibly an Abstract, an introduction, a body (or discussion), a conclusion, and a reference list. As we said about writing a research report, it may be easier to write your first draft in a different order, in this case drafting the body first, then the conclusion, then the introduction and last of all the Abstract. However, we’ve described them in the order in which they will eventually be presented. 118 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Essays in Psychology 6 6.5.1 The Title Page and Abstract The format of the Title Page and Abstract is the same as for a research report. Look back at Section 5.1 for the details. Many universities do not require an Abstract for an essay. Check with your tutor or lecturer about their expectation. If they do require one, and since the essay is not describing a study, you won’t have information about participants and hypotheses in your Abstract. What you will summarise, in less than 150 words, are your contention or position, and the major arguments that led you to your conclusion. The conclusion itself should also be succinctly stated. 6.5.2 The Introduction Although it comes first when you submit the final version, it is advisable to leave the writing of the introduction until you have a reasonable draft of the essay body and conclusion. By then you know what you have argued and concluded and can write an appropriate introduction. Opinions vary on the format of introductions, but one thing is clear: avoid wasting words with too general an opening statement. Similarly, although some map of the territory ahead is advisable, avoid too much detail which interrupts the flow or clarity of the argument. Introductions to essays should not be longer than one long paragraph, perhaps 150 words or 10% of the word limit. The introduction should not restate the title but should say why the area being examined or evaluated is important or interesting. It should then go on to give the reader an idea of what you are going to argue. There are some examples of introductory paragraphs in Figure 6.3, but be aware that they are not the only ways to introduce an essay. One approach to avoid is the detective novel structure – starting with the description of a problem, followed by a sort of gradual unfolding of information and evidence, leading up to a final burst of explanation at the end. This approach can be confusing to your marker who has to wait to the end to find out what is going on! In contrast, more journalistic styles tell the reader the important information at the very start, and gradually increase the level of detail. This approach means that the relevance of the details is always apparent and the argument is always clear. This does not mean that you should be too journalistic in expression or style (e.g., your claims need to relate to the evidence), since it is an academic essay. However, the overall approach is a good one. This means there is a lot to be said for beginning with a direct a statement of point or claim in your introduction, then using the body of the essay to build the argument that supports your point or claim. It is worth noting that your lecturer or tutor will not be all that happy with personal opinions, which means you should probably stick to supported conclusions. However, if you are required to evaluate or compare, you are being asked to state an opinion based on evidence. In such cases, you can make it clear that you have done as asked by saying something like: “The weight of evidence from Bloggs (2015) and Smith (2016) suggests that . . . is the case, in spite of some contrary evidence from Jones (2014).” 119 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Title: Critically examine some of the evidence which suggests that under some conditions rewarding people can actually cause them to become less motivated. While many people might consider that rewarding an individual for a particular activity would serve to enhance motivation, a number of studies have shown that under some conditions rewarding people can actually cause them to become less motivated (Deci, 1971, 1972; Harackiewicz, Manderlink, & Sansone, 1984; Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973). This essay will describe the early studies of Deci, the criticisms of those studies by Lepper and colleagues, and the recent work by Harackiewicz and colleagues which attempted to address the earlier concerns, especially the variable of perceived control by the participant over the activity in question. Title: Are males better equipped to be leaders than females? Critically assess whether it is just childhood interactions that influence the leadership potential of both sexes or whether there are any other influences involved. Leadership has been traditionally regarded as a male domain (Dunlevy, 1991). Many theorists have attempted to explain the gender differences pertaining to leadership success (e.g., Singer, 1991). One such explanation has been offered by Maccoby (1990), a developmental theorist, who suggests that sex role differences occur primarily through childhood interactions. That is, during the childhood years, patterns of behaviour emerge that may be seen to better equip males than females to be leaders. Although childhood interactions seem to affect leadership potential, they are not the only influences involved in determining leadership success. Parental, teacher, and media influences, workplace practices and expectations, and biological factors are also important considerations in any comprehensive attempt to explain gender differences in leadership. Title: Discuss research evidence regarding the development of complex emotions in children. A large proportion of the research on the emergence of emotions has focused on the development of the basic emotions in early infancy, which has resulted in the temporal development of complex emotions being a comparatively uncharted area of study (Lewis, Sullivan, Stranger, & Weiss, 1989). The general consensus seems to consider that happiness, interest, surprise, fear, anger, sadness, and disgust are the basic emotions, although there is still some debate as to whether disgust is an emotion or a reflex (Bootzin, Bower, Crocker, & Hall, 1991). Complex emotions may be defined as those emotions that rely on an awareness of self, examples being jealousy, shame, embarrassment, envy, and pride (Lewis et al., 1989). In an attempt to trace the sequence of development of complex emotions, this essay will briefly examine the three alternative views of emotional development set out by Lewis et al. in their study of self-development and self-conscious emotions. Figure 6.3. Examples of introductory paragraphs from undergraduate essays. 120 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Essays in Psychology 6 Title: Religion: Is it a source of increased acceptance of others, or a source of increased prejudice and intolerance? The popular maxim “never discuss politics or religion” suggests that both topics, far from generating a lack of interest, generate much interest but much difference of opinion amongst people (Batson & Ventis, 1982). Because most religions preach unconditional love and acceptance of others, it has been presumed that religious people are generally less prejudiced than non-religious people (Herek, 1987). However, according to Batson (1976), major religions have often fostered doctrines leading to violent, inhumane, and antisocial acts. Results of even the earliest studies (Allport & Kramer, 1946; Rosenblith, 1949) have revealed that religious involvement correlates positively with prejudice. This essay will critically evaluate the above claims in the light of subsequent research evidence. Figure 6.3. (Continued) 6.5.3 The Body (Discussion) This is the largest section of your essay. Your essay plan should guide this section. Depending on the directive you’ve been given in the question you have chosen, you will have a selection of previous research or theory to summarise, criticise, compare, or all three. The tricky part is deciding on the order in which this is to happen. Burdess (1991) talks about finding appropriate analytical categories, then dealing with each in turn. If you are comparing and contrasting approaches or theories, it is a good idea to deal with them one at a time, elaborating on their strengths, weaknesses, and areas of applicability. If asked to critically evaluate a body of literature, the analytical categories might be particular pieces of research. For example, perhaps a methodological theme (e.g., deception) has been used in a range of contexts (e.g., developmental psychology, social psychology, and cognitive psychology). In this case, you might consider the reasons, risks, consequences, and outcomes for each of these contexts in turn. If the topic has developed over time, it might be appropriate to consider key pieces of research in a chronological sequence. This approach respects the development of knowledge on the topic, since much research is cumulative and builds on previous work. You can either point this out, or show how it isn’t true (e.g., sometimes research on a topic seems to diverge, leading to little straightforward progress). Alternatively, if you are dealing with a literature that is fractured or controversial, you could write first about several pieces of research that support a point of view, then write about other research that doesn’t support that viewpoint, or at least not so well, then come to some decision about what you believe on the basis of this evidence. How much to include? One difficulty is deciding what material to include and what to omit. It is not unusual to feel that, since you have read an enormous amount of research, you must say something about everything you’ve read. Resist this temptation, especially if you have not been particularly discerning in your initial identification of relevant research to read. If you cite everything you have read, the result is likely to be superficial and 121 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays even incoherent or inaccurate, since you will be aware that word limits are strictly enforced; and if you have a lot of things to talk about and limited space, you can only say a little about each. Your marker will be much more impressed with in-depth description and critical appraisal of a few references than a superficial treatment of many. If you are having trouble reducing the amount of material you feel you need to talk about, you may be interpreting the question too broadly. It is usually acceptable in your introduction to say that an area is very broad and applicable to a wider range of phenomena or contexts, and consequently that the essay will limit consideration to a particular scope (e.g., context, period). If you are in doubt about the wisdom of this approach for a particular essay, talk to the lecturer who set it. Occasionally you may have the opposite problem. If you make an essay plan, write a first draft, correct and tidy it up, and are still far short of the word limit, then it is a signal that you need to do more reading. Have you described research in sufficient detail that the reader can see the logic of the conclusions reached or the criticisms made? Have you been fair in arguments for and against, or are your own prejudices showing? While it is acceptable, in fact expected, that you have an opinion, it is not acceptable to ignore evidence that you don’t like! Even when you’ve written a proper length essay, these are shortcomings that should not be there! Another problem in many student essays is the lack of linking sentences so that paragraphs flow logically. Such essays are a collection of largely unrelated paragraphs. You need to have a sentence at the end or beginning of each paragraph that allows the reader to understand the link between paragraphs. This can be done as simply as starting a new paragraph with “In contrast to Brown’s (2016) findings, Jones (2015) found that . . .” and using that paragraph to deal with a different idea from that in the previous paragraph. Alternatively, if you wish to explore one study in one paragraph, and someone else’s work in the next paragraph, you might say “Additional support for Allen’s (2014) proposition comes from Bloggs’s (2016) study, where . . .”. This is what we have done with this paragraph and the preceding one. The body of the essay will be the major part. As mentioned before, the introduction should not be more than about 150 words. The conclusion should be similar in length, about 150 words, and certainly not more than about 10% of the word limit. So if your word limit is 1500 words, the body of the essay should be about 1200 of them. Don’t be paranoid about counting words, but use these numbers as a guide. 6.5.4 The Conclusion The conclusion is a single-paragraph summary of the major points of your argument, and a final statement of your point, having evaluated conflicting or at least comprehensive evidence. What you have to do is find a balance between an abrupt ending which looks like you just ran out of ideas, and a repetition of points that you seem to have just made, particularly in a fairly short essay. For example, avoid any word-for-word repetition from the body of the argument. However, the conclusion should not introduce any new material or ideas. 122 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Essays in Psychology 6 The main reason that students have difficulty with conclusions is that they often seem to have no view or opinions about the material. Once you have seriously addressed the critical reading of the evidence, you should find that you do have an opinion on the topic. The conclusion is the place to state it, remembering that even here you need to back up your opinion with evidence. Since I’ve just said not to introduce new material, you need to refer back to work in the body of the essay, even if only by citation. Figure 6.4 has several examples of concluding paragraphs. Title: The facial feedback hypothesis suggests that the experience of a particular emotion can follow from a particular facial expression. Laird has used the manipulation of facial muscles to induce specific emotions as an independent variable in experiments. Critically evaluate some of this literature, and evaluate how effective you think the method has been. In summary, existing research on the muscle by muscle technique of testing the facial feedback hypothesis is contradictory. However, so many faults have been pointed out by researchers such as Buck (1980) and Tourangeau and Ellsworth (1979), and many of the arguments for the hypothesis are so weak, that those studies which showed positive results are unconvincing. Obviously more research needs to be done, particularly using between-subjects designs, but the muscle by muscle technique studies that have been carried out so far simply appear to contain too many methodological flaws to be considered effective. Title: How do the gender stereotypes we hold affect our interaction with a member of the opposite sex? Critically evaluate some of the recent research in this area. In conclusion, recent research has indicated that differences in behavioural style do exist between men and women when interacting with each other. These behavioural differences are parallel to and influenced by gender stereotypes. Whether the stereotypes are accurate or not, they function as part of sex role expectations, and so influence the behaviour of men and women. An understanding of the differences in interaction styles, and the stereotypes that pervade and direct them, can be helpful in overcoming misunderstandings between the sexes and so narrow the communication gap of the gender divide. Title: Are there gender-specific differences in the ways in which parental socialisation influences the development of empathy in children? Overall, the proposition that there are gender-specific differences in the influence of parental socialisation on the development of empathy in children could be considered highly controversial. In previous research there is both a lack of consistency in findings and a lack of control for, and consideration of, numerous possible confounding influences. Although the existing literature tends to favour the notion that mothers are particularly influential in developing greater empathic understanding and responsiveness in their daughters, the validity of this finding is questionable on the basis of the limitations and problems discussed above. Figure 6.4. Examples of concluding paragraphs from undergraduate essays. 123 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 6.5.5 The References The reference list at the end of an essay is formatted in exactly the same way as for a research report, so look back at Section 3.3. In-text referencing is also the same as for a research report, and this was dealt with in Section 3.2. You may find markers slightly more lenient as to what counts as evidence for an essay compared with a research report. For instance, a newspaper article would certainly indicate current interest in a particular topic and that may be a valid point in an essay, whereas you would never reference a newspaper article in a research report. Don’t get carried away, though. Avoid references to radio or television programs in psychological essays, however interesting or topical they may be. A newspaper is acceptable, because it is a reference that can be checked. Usually it is not practical for an interested reader to check on what a radio or television program said, so as a standard of evidence it is not very good. The same is true for many internet references. If they are opinion pieces (e.g., personal blogs or vlogs) or home pages that may change between when you cite them and when a marker may want to check them, then avoid them. If they are pages with copyright dates, as with many university sites, or are electronic journals, then they are much more acceptable. Students sometimes complain that although they’ve referred to many references, they haven’t received as good a grade as a friend who used fewer. As with research reports, there are no firm rules about the number of references to use, but generally speaking it is better to write sensibly about a few references than to be promiscuous with many. When reading the references you’ve found, you will often find that many of them say the same things. It is not necessary to make a general statement then put six references in parentheses, unless you are specifically making the point that the statement is a well-supported one. Your marker is more interested in your critical evaluation of specific references than in the fact that you’ve read six of them. It is acceptable to make a statement, then refer to just a couple of representative references like this: (e.g., Moulding, 2010; Nedeljkovic, 2011). The “e.g.,” indicates that these are examples of researchers who have said such things, but that many more researchers agree with your statement. Check with the lecturer or tutor who set the question, but as a rule of thumb a first-year essay should not require more than about six or eight references, and a second-year essay should not require more than about 12 references. Some lecturers are happy with fewer, especially if they are asking you to criticise a particular author’s thoughts on a topic. 6.6 Redrafting and Presentation As with a research report, redrafting is crucial. When you have a first draft written, however brief, congratulate yourself. Then get someone else to cast an eye over it for you, with a view to pointing out areas which lack clarity or contain poor argument or insufficient evidence to back up assertions. Look back at Section 2.9 for more information about redrafting. 124 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Essays in Psychology 6 Presentation is the same as for a research report. As we said in Section 2.10, most institutions these days expect your assignments to be typed, double-spaced, with wide margins, and to include a title page that includes your name, your tutor’s name (please spell it correctly!), and the day and time of your lab class. You may also need an official coversheet if submitting in hardcopy. If you can run your essay through a plagiarism detector like Turnitin before final submission, that should also help keep you out of trouble. Finally, get your essay in on time. It is very rare that students can do enough work on an essay to make the penalty for any extra day(s) worthwhile! This means it is a good idea to aim to get your essay completed 48 hours early, so that last-minute hassles with computers can be overcome, or at least addressed. Back up each draft locally and on a separate device (e.g., USB, cloud-based backup such as Dropbox, etc.) so you always have the previous draft you wrote available if something terrible happens to your final draft. You may find it useful to include the date, or the version number in your filename, in order to keep track of the latest draft (e.g., psychessay_v3 or essay02May18). Finally, reward yourself for keeping to your schedule. It is hard, given all the competing demands on your time, so when you are successful in sticking to your timeline you’ve earned a special treat. Psychology staff often put more emphasis on due dates than staff in other subjects, and that is because psychology is an accredited course leading (eventually) to a professional vocation where time is important. However, if you have a genuine, documentable reason for an extension, don’t be afraid to ask before the due date. 6.7 Further Reading There are many books available that address essay writing in the social sciences, and your library will probably have copies of them. Although none of these were specifically written for psychology students, they contain much good advice. Ones that we have found to be especially useful are: Betts, K., Seitz, A., & Farquharson, K. (2006). Writing essays and research reports in the social sciences (3rd ed.). South Melbourne, Victoria: Thomson. Burdess, N. (2007). Good study. Frenchs Forest, NSW: Pearson Australia. Marshall, L. A., & Rowland, F. (2013). A guide to learning independently (5th ed.). Frenchs Forest, NSW: Pearson Australia. All these books are by Australian academics for Australian students. Burdess’s book is particularly good on general study skills such as preparing for lectures and tutorials, taking notes, examination techniques, and general writing skills. Marshall and Rowland’s book is very good for making it clear that studying at university is different from studying at school. One of us found an earlier edition of it valuable as a mature age student coming back to study after many years away from it. Betts, Seitz, and 125 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Farquharson’s book is especially good on essays, though its material on research reports is aimed more at sociology students. 6.8 Checklist For the first two or three essays that you write, you may find it helpful to plan your time. You should aim to sit down to write consistently, for a minimum of 45 (uninterrupted) minutes. To make sure this writing time is put to good use, check off these steps as you do them before you start writing (although you may choose to do them in a different sequence from the one that follows): Preparation Do you have all the handouts from your department (word limit, due date, etc.)? Have you chosen an essay topic? Have you read (obtained copies of ) the references given? Are you clear on what the topic is asking you to do (Evaluate? Discuss?)? Have you obtained enough references to allow you to have a reasonable opinion on the area in question? Have you read them critically, taking appropriate notes? Have you jotted down an essay plan – at least the rough sequence in which you are going to argue your case? Body of the essay (see pp. 121–122) Have you listed the arguments for your case? What about the arguments against? Are you confident of backing up your opinion with evidence? Conclusion (see pp. 122–123) Can you fairly state the reasons for your decisions? Is it a single brief paragraph? Writing Introduction (see pp. 199–121) Have you begun by stating the area under question and your opinion? Have you given the reader/marker a verbal map of what to expect in the way of your argument? Is it brief (not more than about 150 words)? Title Page, Abstract, References (see pp. 119, 124) Have you produced a Title Page, and written an Abstract (if required)? Have you ensured that your References include all (and only) the references cited and meets APA conventions? 126 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Essays in Psychology 6 Revision and Submission Presentation Rearrange the sections to ensure the clarity and coherence of the argument. Did you have your first draft read by someone else? Was there anything that was unclear? What do you need to do to make it clear? Have you written a complete second draft? Is it clearer to someone else than the first one? Does the overall draft exceed the word limit? If so, cut it back. Have you typed it up double-spaced? Have you had the entire report proofread, and corrected any mistakes? If you are really keen, or really anxious, do as many more drafts as you can cope with! Keep a copy of the final, proofread version, either on a USB or cloud-based backup service or as a hardcopy, as well as on your computer. Hand it in on time (or even early!). Celebrate a good job well done. You deserve it! 127 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e This page intentionally left blank. Appendix A Sample of a Good Research Report This is the first of two examples of a good research report. It is fairly typical of what you might encounter in your first research report in your first semester of psychology. We have always been hesitant to include these sample reports. In the case of the examples of good research reports, we did not want students to think there was only one way of writing a research report, or of expressing particular aspects of one, other than the standard conventions of the sequence of sections. However, we were persuaded because it seemed desirable to have a graphic index. If you are after a quick reference for something and know it belongs in, say, the Introduction, but can’t think of a word to allow you to look up the real index, you can quickly and easily find what you want from the sample research report. The numbers referred to in the boxes are the chapter, section, and subsection numbers in this book. We have also included examples of badly written research reports on the same topics. Again we have previously hesitated to include these for the reason that students sometimes have trouble evaluating the quality of their own writing – once in print, the neatness of presentation overwhelms the critical sense. However, we hope that the abundant sprinkling of numbers in superscript, indicating that something is wrong, will alert you to the shortcomings. The numbers refer to notes on pages 147–149 and 172–174 explaining just what is wrong. Please note that such a system of numbers indicating notes should not be used in your own research reports, even though you will occasionally see them in published articles. The other important thing to note is that the first poor example (Appendix B) is unusually short, even for a first-year research report. It is only there to give you an example of all the things you should not do, and, since we found 102 of them, that should be enough of a dire warning! We would like to acknowledge our gratitude to Dr Ben Williams who designed and ran this first good example report with a cohort of first-year students. Thanks are also due to students Kelly Stepney, Ian Baxendine, and Elisa Spiller, who kindly gave permission to use parts of their excellent reports to synthesise the good example. The second good report (Appendix C) is probably a bit more complex than those you will encounter early in first year, but we thought it worth including to show the variety of figures. The number of references is also a bit over the top for a first-year report. By 129 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays second year, your reports will be longer and more complex, so don’t use it as a template, simply as a way to find what you want in the body of the book. We would again like to acknowledge our gratitude to Dr Ben Williams who designed and ran this report with a cohort of second-semester, first-year students, and to students Eliza Griffiths, Jaime Lee, Aimee McCarten, and Rachael Young, who kindly gave permission to use parts of their excellent reports for our version of the good example. 130 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix A S ample of a Good Research Repor t Exploring the Prevalence of Weight Stigma and its Impact on Gender Katie Smith Title 5.1 1234567 Submitted as an HAY101 Research Report Due date: 15th October 2018 Tutor: Gordana Bruce Class: Monday 13:30–14:30 Word count: 1732 Note: Check whether your tutor or lecturer wants your student number rather than your name on the title page. 131 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Note no page number Abstract 5.1 Abstract Note no indent p. 70 Numbers 5.4.3 In a between-groups design study replicating Harris, Harris, and Bochner (1982), 220 women and 96 men Australian university students rated a fictitious target on several personality and appearance traits using a series of 7-point rating scales. The target’s weight and sex were manipulated between groups. As expected, results demonstrated a marked decrease in participants’ ratings on positive traits when the target was overweight, confirming the existence of an obesity stereotype; however, there was no significant difference between these ratings between target sexes. A comparison between present results and Harris et al.’s findings showed a decrease in participant ratings for both average weight and overweight targets. These results suggest that, while stereotypes for obesity do exist and may be increasing, this stereotyping is also experienced by average weight targets. 132 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix A S ample of a Good Research Repor t Note no page number Introduction 5.2 General opening statement p. 72 Figure 5.4 Obese individuals are stigmatized, rejected, and discriminated against in our society because of their weight (Rothblum, Miller, & Garbutt, 1988). One factor that may influence this stigmatization is the obesity stereotype people may hold. The obese stereotype is defined as the consistent negative belief that an overweight person’s condition is a socially deviant, self-inflicted and disabling characteristic (Maddox, Back, & Liederman, 1968). First lines indented p. 31 In-text referencing 3.2 Olds, Tomkinson, Ferrar, and Maher’s (2010) study suggested that obesity rates have only slightly increased since 1985, with 21–25% of the population classed as overweight or obese, and only 5–6% classed as obese. Despite the ‘and’ or ‘&’ p. 38 stability of obesity and overweight rates, there are suggestions that the bias and stereotyping against this body type is increasing (Puhl & Heuer, 2009), and the prevalence of such stigma is suggested as a major contributing factor to the mental distress that overweight and obese individuals face (Puhl, Schwartz, & Brownell, 2005; Wott & Carels, 2010). The term “weight stigma” is used to identify the ridicule, negative attitudes, and biases that are faced by individuals who are overweight or obese (Wang, Brownell, & Wadden, 2004). This is manifested in many ways within society; such as the perceived and actual biases against overweight individuals within the health care system (Puhl, Andreyeva, & Brownell, 2008; Puhl & Heuer, 2009); the prevalence of negative stereotyping of overweight individuals in television and film (Puhl & Brownell, 2001); and stereotypes casting overweight individuals as lazy, sloppy, noncompliant, undisciplined, and lacking willpower. (Puhl, Andreyeva, & Brownell, 2008; Puhl & Brownell, 2001). Overview of previous research p. 74 Tiggemann and Rothblum (1988) investigated attitudes about body weight and appearance in a group of 65 men and 218 women undergraduate psychology students at two universities. In one of the conditions participants were to rate a thin man, thin woman, fat woman, and fat man on eight qualities. The results were consistent with other studies, and indicated negative stereotyping of obese people. Although the idea of obese people as Use of quotation marks p. 25 “fat jolly people” was confirmed, being rated higher on warmth and friendliness, they were rated as more self-indulgent, less self-disciplined, lazier, and less attractive. Similarly, Maddox et al. (1968) found that overweight people were evaluated negatively in their study, and suggested that this may be due to the view that the overweight condition appears to be selfinflicted and a voluntary condition. An interesting aspect of the study was the pervasiveness of the negative evaluation, as the samples were chosen to maximise variability in attitudes towards overweight people, yet still seemed to evaluate them negatively. 133 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Page numbering starts here with p. 2 2 To understand why the stigma is so strong against overweight people Klaczynski, Goold, and Mudry (2004) investigated the “thin ideal” in relation to obesity stereotypes and self-esteem. This is because self-esteem and the “thin ideal” can both lead to the development of the obese stereotype. The thin ideal is the message that people constantly receive from media and friends suggesting that the ideal body is a thin one, and that those who do not meet these standards are to be perceived as weak, lacking in skills, and possessing characteristics that are undesirable. Their results indicated that the belief that obesity is the result of personal flaws mediates the influence of self-esteem on negative attitudes towards obesity. In a study exploring these stereotypes, Harris, Harris, and Bochner (1982) had 46 men and 101 women university students complete a “Person Perception Study” where each student rated one of eight descriptions of a person who was either a man or woman, average weight or overweight, wore glasses or did not, but was otherwise of medium height, had dark curly hair, brown eyes, a pleasant smile, preferred casual clothes, and often preferred the colour blue. Harris et al. found that overweight individuals are stereotyped as less active, intelligent, hardworking, attractive, popular, successful, athletic, and appropriately sex-typed than individuals of average weight. Interestingly, neither sex of participant nor sex of target was significant in their results. However, more recent studies have suggested that overweight women face much greater discrimination than overweight men within the workforce (Puhl & Heuer, 2009); and there are suggestions of differential treatment of overweight children, with girls facing greater discrimination than boys (Puhl Introducing the present study p. 76 Hypotheses p. 55 Figure 4.2, Figure 5.6 & Latner, 2007). The aim of the present study was to partially replicate that of Harris, Harris, and Bochner (1982) and explore the strength of the obesity stigma in a modern Australian context. It was hypothesised that overweight targets would be negatively stereotyped compared to those of average weight. It was also predicted, given Puhl and Latner’s (2007) findings, that women would experience harsher weight discrimination than men, measured on a set of trait scales. Finally, given suggestions of increasing bias (Puhl & Heuer, 2009) it was hypothesised that the stereotypes in the present study would be more negative than those in the original study by Harris et al. in 1982. Note bold, no full stop Method 5.3 Participants 5.3.1 Figure 5.8 Method Participants The sample consisted of 316 undergraduate psychology students, 220 women and 96 men. The mean age was 20.6 years (SD 5 4.39 years). The students Method does not start on a new page Italicise letters representing statistics p. 90 134 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix A S ample of a Good Research Repor t 3 took part in the study as a voluntary requirement for one of their course units with no reward for participation. The sample comprised students from one single Victorian university. As BMI was taken into account, 9.5% were underweight, 62% were of average weight, 17% were considered overweight, 4.5% were obese, and 7% failed to report this. Materials 5.3.3 Materials and Design The testing instrument in this study was a single-page questionnaire identical to the one used in Harris et al.’s (1982) original study, with a single minor change. In the original study, participants were given a description of a fictional person named “Chris Martin”; this has been changed to “Chris Mortin” in the present study. All other details of the study were kept consistent with Harris et al.’s original design, such that each participant saw a description of one individual who was either average weight or overweight, man or woman, and wore glasses or did not. The wearing of glasses was not analysed in this study. Procedure 5.3.4 Procedure Students were randomly assigned to a target by being given randomly ordered booklets and tested in their usual tutorial groups. Results 5.4 Results As with the original study by Harris et al. (1982), the key traits identified to Note that Results does not start on a new page correspond with obesity stereotyping were collapsed into a single Obesity Stereotype value, which was calculated thus: (active 1 intelligent 1 hardworking – outgoing 1 attractive 1 popular 1 successful 1 athletic 1 appropriately sex-typed 1 7)/9, giving a variable where lower scores mean more negative stereotyping. Introduction of Figures 5.4.6 Figure 1 shows the mean obesity scores for overweight and average weight targets, as well as the means split by sex of target. As seen in Figure 1, overweight target people were scored more negatively than average weight ones, supporting the first hypothesis. There appeared to be little difference in scores depending on the sex of the target, indicating that the second hypothesis was not supported. The obesity score means for the present study and those obtained by Introduction of Tables p. 75 Table 4.12 State whether hypotheses are supported p. 92 Harris et al. (1982) are shown in Table 1. They indicate that there was less of a negative stereotype towards overweight people in 1982 than there is now. The hypothesis that obesity stereotype scores obtained in this study would be lower than the results of Harris et al.’s study was supported. 135 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 4 Obesity stereotype scope 4 3 No frames around histograms 2 1 0 Obese Figure titles 5.4.6 Figure 5.17 Table titles 5.4.5 Note there are no vertical lines Average weight Obese– female Average weight– female Obese– male Average weight– male Target weight status (overall and by gender) Figure 1. Mean obesity stereotype score based on weight. Table 1 Mean Stereotype Scores for Overweight and Average Weight Individuals for the Present Study and those of Harris et al. (1982) The present study (N 5 316) Average weight (overall) Obese (Overall) 3.79 3.09 Harris et al. (N 5 157) 4.24 3.79 Discussion A comparison of the obesity stereotype score between the weight State whether hypotheses are supported p. 100 categories demonstrates support for the hypothesis that obese people would be subject to more negative stereotyping than those of average weight. As the Leave space before and after figures and tables Italics for number representing statistics Note that Discussion does not start on a new page obesity stereotype score is a calculated score collapsing participants’ ratings on several attributes, a lower mean obesity stereotype score means that participants rated these individuals as, on average, less active, less hardworking, less athletic, less appropriately sex-typed, less successful, less popular, less attractive, and less intelligent, but more outgoing than those of average weight (Harris, Harris, & Bochner, 1982). The difference in mean obesity stereotype scores supports the first hypothesis, suggesting that weight stigma is still present and demonstrable within this sample. Support for previous research pp. 100–101 The suggestion that women would be viewed more harshly than men was not supported. This is consistent with Harris et al.’s (1982) study, but contrary to that of Puhl and Latner (2007). However, Puhl and Latner’s study was of 136 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix A S ample of a Good Research Repor t 5 children, and it may be that although girls are judged more severely than boys if overweight, by adulthood this differential disappears. Alternatively, since much of the obesity research has been done in North America, it may be that Australians are more equitable in their stereotyping. The hypothesis that the severity of stereotyping has increased since the study of Harris et al. (1982) was supported. This supports the findings in the review by Puhl and Heuer (2009). However, it should be noted that the mean scores for an average weight individual decreased by a similar amount. It may be, as Klaczynski et al. (2004) argue, that the current ideal of thinness makes Implication of results p. 103 even an average weight person susceptible to harsh judgment. An implication of the present study is that since negative stereotyping is more severe, and since the effects of it include mental health issues and employment concerns, among others (e.g., Puhl & Heuer, 2009), society should seriously address the causes of such stereotyping. Some limitations to the present study must be admitted. The sample is relatively small and consisted only of psychology students, with a severe imbalance between the sexes. Results cannot therefore be generalised even to university students overall. It would be desirable for future research to obtain a sample with a more even balance between the sexes, and preferably from a wider age and socioeconomic range. A further concern, perhaps relevant to differences in the sex of the target, is that participants were only given a written description of the target person, and the weight variable was simply a distinction between “average weight” and “overweight”. If a picture had been shown, possible interactions of perceptions of obesity and physical attractiveness may have come into play. Although “attractive” was one of the variables measured, the lack of a visual Concluding paragraph p. 104 stimulus could be seen as a limitation. In conclusion, the current study has demonstrated that, at least for psychology students, stereotyping on the basis of weight is more severe now than 30 years ago, though it did not appear to be more severe for women than men. Since the latter result differs from North American research, further research is warranted, especially into the causes of stereotyping. Given that severity of judgments of average weight people had also increased, more attention should be given to how realistic expectations of various weight levels are. 137 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 6 References 5.6 Format of journal articles 3.3.2 References Harris, M. B., Harris, R. J., & Bochner, S. (1982). Fat, four eyed, and female: Stereotypes of obesity, glasses, and gender. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 12, 503–516. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1982.tb00882.x Klaczynski, P. A., Goold, K. W., & Mudry, J. J. (2004). Culture, obesity stereotypes, self-esteem, and the “thin ideal”: A social identity perspective. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33, 307–317. doi: 10.1023/B:JOYO.0000032639.71472.19 Maddox, G. L., Back, K. W., & Liederman, V. R. (1968). Overweight as social deviance and disability. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 9, 287–298. doi: 10.2307/2948537 Olds, T., Tomkinson, G. R., Ferrar, K. E., & Maher, C. A. (2010). Trends in the prevalence of childhood overweight and obesity in Australia between 1985 and 2008. International Journal of Obesity, 34, 57–66. doi: 10.1038/ ijo.2009.211 Puhl, R. M., Andreyeva, T., & Brownell, K. (2008). Perceptions of weight discrimination: Prevalence and comparison to race and gender discrimination. International Journal of Obesity, 32, 992–1000. doi: 10.1038/ ijo.2008.22 Puhl, R., & Brownell, K. D. (2001). Bias, discrimination, and obesity. Obesity Research, 9, 788–904. doi: 10.1038/oby.2001.108 Puhl, R. M., & Heuer, C. A. (2009). The stigma of obesity: A review and update. Obesity, 17, 941–964. doi: 10.1038/oby.2008.636 Puhl, R. M., & Latner, J. D. (2007). Stigma, obesity, and the health of the nation’s children. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 557–580. doi: 10.1037/00332909.133.4.557 Puhl, R. M., Schwartz, M. B., & Brownell, K. D. (2005). Impact of perceived consensus on stereotypes about obese people: A new approach for reducing bias. Health Psychology, 24, 517–525. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.24.5.517 Rothblum, E. D., Miller, C. T., & Garbutt, B. (1988). Stereotypes of obese female job applicants. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 7, 277–283. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(198803)7:2,277::AID-EAT2260070213.3.0. CO;2-2 Tiggemann, M., & Rothblum, E. D. (1988). Gender differences in social consequences of perceived overweight in the United States and Australia. Sex Roles, 18, 75–86. doi: 10.1007/BF00288018 Wang, S. S., Brownell, K. D., & Wadden, T. A. (2004). The influence of the stigma of obesity on overweight individuals. International Journal of Obesity, 28, 1333–1337. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802730 Wott, C. B., & Carels, R. A. (2010). Overt weight stigma, psychological distress and weight loss treatment outcomes. Journal of Health Psychology, 15, 608–614. doi: 10.1177/1359105309355339 Reference list does begin on a new page p. 105 Note digital object identifier (doi) p. 45 138 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix B Sample of a Poor Research Report Weight Stigma and Gender1 Katie Smith HAY101 2 139 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 3 This study looked at weight stigma in 316 men and women.5 They each 4 got a description of someone who was average weight or overweight. There were eight different targets. 6 Results showed that targets were rated worse7 on positive traits, just like8 Harris, Harris & Bochner’s study. There were no differences between men and women.9 Also, judgments were tougher10 in this study compared to 1982.11 140 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix B S ample of a Poor Research Repor t Introduction13 12 Obese people are stigmatised because of their weight (Rothblum, Miller and14 Garbutt, 1988). This is due to the obesity stereotype.15 This is the belief that people are overweight because they are socially deviant, and bring it on themselves16 (Maddox17 Back & Liederman, 1968). Olds, Tomkinson, Ferrar &18 Maher’s 201019, 20 found that obesity rates have only slightly increased since 1985. Others say that obesity is increasing21 (Puhl & Heuer, 2009), and this causes22 the mental distress that individuals23 face (Wott & Carels, 2010). This24 is shown in biases against overweight individuals within the health care system, in television and film, and assumptions that overweight individuals are lazy, sloppy, undisciplined, and lacking willpower.25, 26 Tiggemann and Rothblum (1988) investigated undergraduate psychology students27 Subjects28 rated a thin man, thin woman, fat woman and fat man.29 Obese people were rated as more self-indulgent, less self-disciplined, lazier and less attractive. Maddox et al.,30 (1968) found that overweight people were evaluated negatively and said this was because31 the overweight condition was 32 self-inflicted and a voluntary condition. Klaczynski, Goold, and Mudry (2004) said33 that the ideal body is a thin one, and that those who do not meet these standards are34 weak, lacking in skills and possessing characteristics that are undesirable.25 Harris, Harris and Bochner17 (1982) had university students27 complete a “Person Perception Study” where each student rated35 descriptions of people.36 Harris et al. found that overweight individuals are stereotyped as less active, intelligent, hardworking, attractive, popular, successful, athletic, and appropriately sex-typed than individuals of average weight.37 Our study38 replicated Harris, Harris and Bochner (1982). It is39 hypothesised that overweight targets will be39 stereotyped harshly.40 It is39 also predicted that women would experience harsher weight discrimination than men. Finally it is39 hypothesised that our stereotypes will be39 more negative than Harris’ et al.41 in 1982. 141 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 42 Method 43 44 Participants45 46 Participants consisted of 47 316 undergraduate psychology students, 220 females48 and 96 males.48 The mean age was 20.6 (SD 5 4.39).49, 50, 51 Materials and design46 The testing instrument in this study was a single-page questionnaire identical to the one used in Harris et al’s52 (1982) original study, with a single minor change. In the original study, participants were given a description of a fictional person named “Chris Martin”; this has been changed to “Chris Mortin” in the present study. All other details of the study were kept consistent with Harris et al.’s original design. Each participant saw a description of one individual who was either average weight or overweight, man or woman, and wore glasses or did not.53 Procedure Students were randomly assigned54 by being given randomly ordered booklets and tested in their usual tutorial groups. 142 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix B S ample of a Poor Research Repor t 42 Results55, 56 57, 58, 59 Obesity stereotype scope 4 3 2 1 0 Obese Average weight Obese– female Average weight– female Obese– male Average weight– male Figure 1. Mean obesity stereotype score based on weight.62 Target weight status (overall and by gender) 60, 61 Figure 1 shows the obesity scores for overweight and average weight targets, as well as split by sex of target.63 64 The obesity scores63 for the present study and those obtained by Harris et al52 (1982) are shown in Table 1. They indicate that there was less of a negative stereotype towards overweight people in 1982 than there is now.65 Table 1. 6642 Mean Stereotype Scores for Overweight and Average Weight Individuals for the Present Study and those of Harris et al. (1982).68, 69 The present study Harris et al. 3.79 3.09 4.24 3.79 70 Average weight Obese 143 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Discussion71 The first hypothesis was supported.72 The suggestion that women would be viewed more harshly than men was not supported. This is consistent with Harris et al.’s (1982) study, but not Puhl &18 Latner’s.73, 74 The hypothesis that the severity of stereotyping has increased since the study of Harris et al. (1982) was supported. This supports the findings in the review by Puhl and Heuer (2009). It may be, as Klaczynski et al. (2004) argue, that the current ideal of thinness makes even an average weight person susceptible to harsh judgment.75 What this means76 is that since negative stereotyping is more severe, and causes77 mental health issues and employment concerns, among others (e.g., Puhl & Heuer, 2009), society should seriously address the causes of such stereotyping. A problem with the study78 is that the sample is relatively small and consisted only of psychology students, with mostly females.79 Future research should get a better sample.80 Another problem81 is that participants were only given a written description of the target person, and the weight variable was simply a distinction between “average weight” and “overweight”. A picture would have been better82 because “attractive” was one of the variables measured.83 In conclusion, we found84 that for psychology students, stereotyping on the basis of weight is more severe now than thirty85 years ago, though not for females than males.86 Since the latter result differs from North American research, maybe Australians are different.87 Since average weight people were judged more severely than they used to be,88 more attention should be given to how realistic people’s judgments are.89 References90, 91 144 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix B S ample of a Poor Research Repor t 42 Harris, M. B., Harris, R. J., and92 Bochner, S. (1982). Fat, four eyed, 93and female: Stereotypes of obesity, glasses, and gender. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 12,94 503–516.95 Klaczynski, P. A., Goold, K. W., & Mudry, J. J. (2004). Culture, obesity stereotypes, self-esteem, and the “thin ideal”: A social identity perspective. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33, 307–317. 96 Olds, T.97 Tomkinson, G. R. Ferrar, K. E. & Maher, C. A. (2010). Trends in the prevalence of childhood overweight and obesity in Australia between 1985 and 2008. International Journal of Obesity, 34, 57–66. 98 Puhl, R. M., & Latner, J. D. (2007). Stigma, obesity, and the health of the nation’s children. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 557–580. Puhl, R. M., & Heuer, C. A. (2009). The stigma of obesity: a99 review and update. Obesity, 17(5), 941–964. Rothblum, E. D., Miller, C. T., & Garbutt, B. (1988). Stereotypes of Obese Female Job Applicants.100 International Journal of Eating Disorders, 7, 277–283. Tiggemann, M., & Rothblum, E. D. (1988). Gender differences in social consequences of perceived overweight in the United States and Australia. Sex Roles, 18, 75–86. 101 Wang, S. S., Brownell, K. D., & Wadden, T. A. (2004). The influence of the stigma of obesity on overweight individuals. International Journal of Obesity, 28, 1333–1337. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802730. 145 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 42 Wott, C. B., & Carels, R. A. (2010). Overt weight stigma, psychological distress and weight loss treatment outcomes. Journal of health psychology,102 15, 608–614. 146 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix B S ample of a Poor Research Repor t Problems: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 Titles should be in normal; that is, 12-point font, not bold, and should be centred across the page. The Title Page should not have borders or decorations. There should be sufficient information to allow your tutor to return the report to you; that is, day and time of tutorial class. See the good example on page 129. The Abstract should be labelled Abstract, centred across the page, and start at the top of the page. The first line of an Abstract should not be indented. “. . . looked at weight stigma” is too vague. Look at the good example, page 129. You need more details of the sample, numbers of men and women, and that they were university students. You also need more details of the method; that is, that the targets were rated on personality and appearance. The Abstract should be a single paragraph. “. . . rated worse” is sloppy expression. Better to say that judgments were more severe on positive traits. Don’t say “just like Harris . . .” say: “. . . supporting Harris, Harris, and Bochner (1982).” Note the “and” rather than an ampersand (&). Some academics don’t like references in Abstracts, though there is no APA rule about it. But it’s wise to check your lecturer’s preference. “There were no differences . . .” is vague. Add “in judgment scores” to the end of the sentence. “. . . judgments were tougher . . .” is also sloppy. Say something like “. . . judgments were more severe . . .”. Better to cite the study: “. . . than in Harris et al.’s (1982) study.” Overall, the Abstract needs to describe aims, method, hypotheses, results, and provide a conclusion. Page numbering starts at 2 on the second page of the Introduction. Do not have a heading for the Introduction. It is obvious what it is from its location. References in brackets need an ampersand (&) not “and”. Too sweeping. Say “One factor may be the obesity stereotype.” “. . . socially deviant” is ambiguous, as is “bring it on themselves”. See the good example for better wording. Multiple authors need commas after their names (Maddox, Back, & Liederman, 1968). When not in brackets, references need “and” not “&”. The date should be in brackets. Since there is an apostrophe, the word “study” needs to be added: “Olds, Tomkinson, Ferrar, and Maher’s (2010) study found . . .”. Ambiguous. Are some people getting fatter, or are more people getting fat? Avoid using “causes” since we can rarely be so dogmatic. Say “may contribute to . . .”. Again, too sweeping. Say “. . . that some individuals face . . .”. To what does “This” refer? It does not belong at the start of a new paragraph. It should be added to the previous one. Don’t use single-sentence paragraphs. These assertions need references. See the good example. It would be advisable to state the number of students. And this sentence needs a full stop. The preferred term is “participants”. It is not clear whether each participant rated each of these targets or whether different groups each rated a single target. A reference in the body of the text like this does not need the comma: “Maddox et al. (1968) . . .” “. . . said this was because . . .” is sloppy. Better to say “. . . suggested that . . .”. The authors did not say the overweight condition was self-inflicted. They said that people in general thought that it was. The authors didn’t say that the ideal body is a thin one. They suggested that this is what people in general think. Be careful to distinguish between what the authors report and what they assert themselves. They are not necessarily weak, but are “perceived to be weak . . .”. How did they do the rating? What sort of people? It would be informative to also know if the sex of the rater or the sex of the target person being rated were also relevant. Avoid personal pronouns as much as possible. Say “The current study . . .”. Hypotheses should be in the past tense. “. . . harshly” is imprecise. Say “more harshly than those of average weight.” 147 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 Apostrophes for the studies of multiple authors belong on the last author’s name or on the “al” thus: “Harris et al.’s study . . .”. Alternatively, avoid the apostrophe by saying “. . . in the study by Harris et al. in 1982”. Needs a page number. The Method section does not start on a new page. The title Method should be in bold type. This level of subheading should not be in italics. This heading should be in bold type. It is the sample that consists of the men and women, not the individual participants! Current conventions prefer “women” and “men” rather than “females” and “males”. Say “20.6 years . . .” to confirm what the units are. You should add whether the students were completing the task voluntarily, and if they were rewarded. Other demographics should be included, such as whether they were from a single university, and whether their weight was measured. This reference needs a full stop – “. . . Harris et al.’s (1982) . . .”. In this study, say that the wearing of glasses was not analysed. Randomly assigned to what? Results does not start on a new page. Results should be in bold type. Check with your lecturer/tutor, but many universities expect a statement that data were (note that “data” is plural) analysed using SPSS. In this study, the dependent variable had to be calculated from a number of scores. You need to describe that process here. The figure needs to be introduced. There needs to be a label on the vertical axis. There needs to be a label on the horizontal axis. The Figure 1 needs to be in italics and not bold, and the body of the title should be normal font. These are mean scores. Say so. If sex differences were tested for, state whether there was a difference. State whether the hypothesis was supported. The table title should be in normal font, not bold. There is no full stop after the table number. The format eliminated this error. Table titles should be in italics. There is no full stop after a table title. It is a good idea to show sample sizes where possible. See the good example. Discussion does not need a new page, and should be in bold type. State what the hypothesis was; that is, “The hypothesis that . . . was supported.” This reference needs a date. Remind the reader (marker!) what those references found. “Harsh judgment” is a bit vague. Say what that judgment is, in this case “susceptible to the obesity stereotype”. This single-sentence paragraph should be attached to the previous paragraph. Beware of being dogmatic about causes. Better to be tentative and say “. . . contributes to . . .”. Better to talk about a limitation. The basic study design was fine, but the sample is limited. Say women, not females. And why is it a problem, since the study did not find sex differences? Possibly because the number of men is too small to be representative, even of male psychology students. What sort of a better sample? More? Yes. Better to say “A further limitation may be . . .”. What sort of a picture? Why is “attraction” a possible confounding variable? Cite a reference. Try to avoid personal pronouns, though this example would probably be okay. For numbers greater than 10, you can use numbers, so “. . . than 30 years . . .”. This expression is confusing. Is what is meant “More severe now than 30 years ago for men, but just as severe for women”? “. . . maybe Australians are different.” needs expanding. Is there any reason to expect this, other than your study, which has just admitted some limitations, especially in numbers of men? “. . . than they used to be.” is too vague. Say “. . . than Americans were 30 years ago . . .”. 148 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix B S ample of a Poor Research Repor t 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 Where did this assertion come from? What evidence might there be that people’s judgments are unrealistic? If referring to the obesity stereotype, perhaps it could be said that attention might be given to how to address such a stereotype. The reference list should be on a new page. The heading References should be centred and in bold type. In the reference list, only use an ampersand (&). References need the second and later lines to be indented. The volume number should be in italics. You should include a doi number if one can be found. Maddox et al. (1968) was cited in the text, but is omitted from the reference list. A comma is needed between names; that is, Olds, T.,. If there are multiple references by one author with colleagues, they should be in alphabetical order of second author, so Puhl and Latner should go after Puhl and Heur (remember to use an “&” in the reference list!). There should be a capital after a colon, so “. . . obesity: A review . . .”. Titles of journal articles do not have capital letters for major words. Wang et al. (2004) is not cited in the study. Even though it may have been read, if not cited it should not be in the reference list. Titles of journals do have capital letters for major words. 149 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e This page intentionally left blank. Appendix C Sample of a Good Research Report 151 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Comparison of Gender Performance on Mental Rotation Tasks Between (Animal-like) Greebles and Traditional Cube Shapes Anna Jones Title 5.1 Submitted as HAY101 Research Report Due date: 15th October 2018 Tutor: Gordana Bruce Class: Monday, 13:30–14:30 Word count: 2012 ote: Check whether your tutor or lecturer wants your student N number rather than your name on the title page. 152 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix C S ample of a Good Research Repor t Note no page number Abstract 5.1 Abstract Not indented p. 70 The aim of this study was to extend the work of Jansen-Osmann and Heil (2007) by comparing the performance of men and women on mental rotation tasks using animal-like stimuli called greebles and traditional Shepard and Numbers 5.4.3 Metzler (1971) cubes. Participants were 162 women and 59 men undergraduates of a metropolitan university in Melbourne, Australia, mean age 24.3 years (SD 5 8.31). They were presented with two blocks of trials consisting of pairs of either greebles or cube shapes, which were either the same copy, or a different image, rotated at one of 12 angles, and were required to decide if the objects were the same. As predicted, men had faster comparison times, and faster mental rotation speeds than women when judging pairs of cube shapes. When judging pairs of greebles, both genders had faster comparison times than they had when judging cube shapes, but, contrary to predictions, women were not faster than men. It was concluded that stimulus type did affect comparison speed, but that further research is required to determine what strategies led to this difference. 153 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Note no page number Introduction 5.2 General opening statement p. 72 Figure 5.4 Humans seem to be able to represent knowledge and reason in a number of ways. One form of mental representation is visual imagery in which pictorial representations of problems can be manipulated (e.g., Kosslyn, 1994, 2005). Early work leading to this view came from a study by Shepard and In-text referencing 3.2 Metzler (1971) which presented participants with pairs of perspective drawings representing three-dimensional cube shapes. Image pairs could be the “same”, depicting equivalent three-dimensional objects differing by a rotation (Figure 1, panel a), or “different”, depicting two different objects. For the different condition in their original study, Shepard and Metzler used shapes which were mirror images of each other to prevent participants developing a strategy of merely identifying a unique feature possessed by one of the objects (Figure 1, panel b). Participants had to identify, as quickly as Referral to same authors within a paragraph p. 38 possible, whether or not the pair of images represented the same object. They found that reaction time for same decisions increased linearly as a function of rotation. This was interpreted as evidence that participants were literally rotating a visual mental analogue of one image to align it with the other image to make a decision, hence the term mental rotation task (MRT). This result has been demonstrated for other kinds of stimuli, including (a) (b) Figure 1. “Same” (a) and “different” (b) (mirror-image) pairs of cube shapes differing by rotation: Adapted from Shepard and Metzler (1971). Abbreviations p. 86 5.3.4 Location of Figures (or Tables) p. 97 5.4.6 Figure titles p. 97 5.4.6 Figure 5.17 154 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix C S ample of a Good Research Repor t Page numbering starts here with p. 2 2 ‘&’ or ‘and’ p. 38 polygons (Cooper, 1975) and body parts (Cooper & Shepard, 1975), and the linearity of the reaction time function further suggests that the rotation is done at a constant angular rate. The mental image must contain some threedimensional information since the effect is observed for rotations in both the picture plane and “depth” plane (that is, perpendicular to the image plane, see Shepard & Metzler, 1971). Subsequent research found that men could perform MRTs faster than women (e.g., Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974). Accumulated results from a variety of cognitive tasks (including those that have found women to be superior to men on verbal tasks), combined with the male advantage for MRTs, have been used to support the argument that men have superior spatial reasoning skills (e.g., Halpern, 1992). Meta-analyses indicate that the gap has not diminished since the original finding, despite claims to the contrary (Masters & Sanders, 1993). Comparatively little work has examined the mechanisms responsible for the gender differences in MRTs (see Peters, 2005). The male advantage Summary of prior research pp. 74–76 cannot be explained by an overall faster male cognitive system, because there are tasks which women can perform faster. There must be some specific component in MRTs at which men excel. Jansen-Osmann and Heil (2007) argued that there are at least three cognitive components that could create differences in the task of deciding whether two images are the same or different: (a) rotating one stimulus to align it with another, (b) comparing the two rotated images, and (c) an essentially attentional cost associated with task characteristics. They called these three factors rotational speed, perceptual comparison and rotational uncertainty respectively. Rotational speed can be determined from the slope of a graph plotting reaction time as a function of rotation angle (steeper graphs indicate slower rotation). Comparison time and rotational uncertainty are estimated from the reaction time for 08 (i.e., no rotation required) – a smaller value indicating faster comparison. In-text referencing of multiple authors p. 38 3.2 The form of the test can influence the size of the male advantage (Linn & Petersen, 1985). Parsons et al. (2004), using virtual reality to present threedimensional Shepard-Metzler-like cubes, found small but non-significant 155 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 3 gender differences in MRTs. Jansen-Osmann and Heil (2007) conducted an experiment to determine what aspect of MRTs men perform faster and used five different stimulus types: letters, abstract symbols, animal drawings, polygons and cube figures. Somewhat surprisingly, they found an overall male advantage only for polygons, with no significant gender differences for the other kinds of stimuli, although there was a trend towards women being slightly faster at farm animals and men being slightly faster for symbols. When Jansen-Osmann and Heil analysed the cognitive components of the task, they found that men had a significantly faster rotational speed for polygons, but that women were significantly faster than men at the perceptual comparison portion of the task for farm animals. Since most studies have used geometric or “mechanical” shapes, presumably to avoid using stimuli which are highly familiar, it is hard to tell Quotation marks p. 25 whether the class of stimuli is important. Given that different types of objects seem to be represented in different brain areas (e.g., Martin, Wiggs, Ungerleider, & Haxby, 1996), it is possible, for example, that some particular male brain areas assist in MRTs but only for abstract, rigid or mechanical objects. It would be interesting to see whether objects other than geometric shapes elicit a different pattern of gender differences in MRTs. A novel stimulus which could be called “organic” or pseudo-animate is the greeble (Gauthier & Tarr, 1997) – an animal-like object created for studying visual object recognition. The greebles need to be made asymmetric so that mirror and same versions of the stimuli can be created (see Tarr & Cheng, 2003 for examples). The present study sought to extend the work of Jansen-Osmann and Heil (2007) by comparing the performance of men and women on mental The present study pp. 76–77 rotation tasks using these animal-like shapes called greebles compared to the traditional Shepard and Metzler (1971) cubes. Consistent with past research, it was hypothesised that men would have faster comparison times than women for cube shapes, and also have a faster Hypotheses p. 55 Figure 4.2, Figure 5.6 rotational speed. Consistent with Jansen-Osman and Heil (2007), we expected women to perform an MRT with greebles as fast or faster than men. In particular we hypothesised that women would have a faster mean comparison time than men for greebles. There was no specific prediction for gender differences 156 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix C S ample of a Good Research Repor t 4 in rotational speed for greebles. However, if men performed this component faster it would argue for a general male advantage at mental rotation. Method 5.3 Participants 5.3.1 Figure 5.8 Levels of headings p. 79 Materials 5.3.3 Method does not start on a new page Method Participants The sample consisted of a convenience sample of 221 undergraduate psychology students. There were 162 women (M 5 24.6 years, SD 5 8.82) and 59 men (M 5 23.9, SD 5 7.53) who voluntarily participated in their own time. Materials A computerised program was used to measure speed and accuracy of Italicise letters representing statistics p. 90 mental rotation. The program used was Inquisit2 web edition by Millisecond Software (2006) and was made available through the student portal. Within this task there were four asymmetric “greebles” (see Figure 2(a)) and four different Shepard and Metzler (1971) cubed shapes (see Figure 2(b)), provided by Tarr (2006). Each stimulus was rotated through 12 different angles from 08 to 3308 in increments of 30 degrees. (a) (b) Figure 2. Examples of stimuli: (a) Three-dimensional greeble: Adapted from Gauthier and Tarr (1997); (b) Three-dimensional cube shape: Adapted from Shepard and Metzler (1971). Procedure p. 86 5.3.4 Procedure After approval by the Ethics Committee, students were asked to complete the mental rotation task in their own time. Submission of the task was taken as consent to participate. The task was divided into two sections of cube shapes and greeble stimuli, with a break in between, with the order of presentation randomly Counterbalancing p. 86 5.3.3 counterbalanced between participants. An original image was presented on the left of the computer screen with either a same, rotated copy, or a different 157 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 5 (mirror-reversed) and rotated image. Participants were required to decide, as quickly as possible, whether the images were the same (regardless of rotation) or different, by pressing either the “z” key or the “/” key respectively. Each stimulus pair remained on the screen until a response was made. Feedback was then immediately given in the form of a green circle for a correct decision or a red cross for an incorrect one. The feedback was displayed for 350 ms. Participants’ age and gender were also collected. Trials were grouped into blocks by stimulus type (all greebles or all cubes). Within each block the four images, by two variations (same and mirror-reversed), by 12 angles of rotation were shown in every possible combination, resulting in 96 possible image pairs. Each pair was shown twice. Results Results 5.4 To examine whether the two types of stimuli produced different mental Note that Results does not start on a new page rotation patterns, the mean reaction time with respect to each angle of rotation for same pairs was calculated then compared across genders. Individual trials were removed if they were more than four standard deviations from the mean, or if participants made errors on more than half a stimulus block. There were no differences depending on whether participants viewed a greeble block or a cube block first. Introduction of Figures p. 97 5.4.6 As shown in Figure 3, men had faster reaction times at all rotations for cube Report effects of counterbalancing p. 86 5.3.3 shapes, as predicted. At 08 men took, on average, 1325 ms (SD 5 749 ms), while Cubes Mean reaction time (miliseconds) 4500 M/Shapes F/Shapes 4000 3500 3000 2500 2000 1500 1000 0 30 60 90 120 150 180 210 Rotation in degrees 240 270 300 330 Figure 3. Mean reaction times for cube shapes as a function of rotation angle by gender. Error bars indicate confidence intervals using standard error. 158 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix C S ample of a Good Research Repor t 6 women took 1623 ms (SD 5 967 ms). The steeper graph for women indicates slower rotation speed than for men. The only exception was at the 1808 comparison, where women’s reaction time approached that of men. Figure 4 indicates that there was very little mean difference in comparison times or rotational speed for greebles, except that men appeared to respond faster at 1808. Greebles 4500 Mean reaction time (miliseconds) Leave reasonable space before figure 4000 M/Greebles F/Greebles 3500 3000 2500 2000 1500 1000 0 30 60 90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 Degrees of rotation Figure 4. Mean reaction times for greeble shapes as a function of rotation angle by gender. Error bars indicate confidence intervals using standard error. State whether hypotheses are supported p. 100 Discussion The results supported the hypotheses that men would have faster comparison time and faster rotational speed than women when rotating cube Leave reasonable space after figure Discussion does not start on a new page shapes. These results support previous research by Voyer, Voyer, and Bryden Support for previous research pp. 100–101 (1995) and Roberts and Bell (2003). The men’s faster reaction time at 08 rotation indicates that they are faster at comparison, while their shallower slope of the reaction times as the angle of rotation increases indicates that men’s rotational speed is faster. While women did perform the 08 comparison task for greebles at a faster rate than men, this difference was very small, and was not apparent at other rotations. The overlapping confidence intervals and the very similar slopes of the reaction times at varying angles of rotation suggest that there was no gender difference in 159 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 7 the mental rotation of the organic greeble shapes. In general, these results tend to support Jansen-Osmann and Heil (2007), who found, as did the present study, that reaction times were faster for organic shapes than for cube stimuli. Jansen-Osmann and Heil (2007) found that fastest reaction times were for alphanumeric characters, suggesting that familiarity is a factor in mental rotation. Although greebles are artificial characters, they are more organic than Possible reasons for your findings pp. 101–102 the cube stimuli. Rilea (2008) suggested that Jansen-Osmann and Heil’s findings were due to the animals possessing meaning, and that women were faster at processing meaningful stimuli than abstract ones. Sherman (1978, as cited in Casey, Nuttall, & Pezaris, 1999) argued that men have innate spatial interests, Secondary sources pp. 39–41 and are therefore more likely to participate in spatial activities, increasing their spatial experience. In support, increasing women’s spatial experiences has been found to improve their mental rotation performance (Moe, 2009). Implication for everyday life p. 103 The results of the current study imply that, while there are gender differences in mental rotation of cube shapes, these differences reduce with organic shapes. Mental rotation is an important aspect of spatial ability, which is strongly related to high mathematics and engineering science ability (Feng, Spence, & Pratt, 2007). In order to increase the proportion of women entering high-status professions such as engineering and architecture, strategies to improve mental rotation skills at younger ages for women would be desirable. Limitations pp. 103–104 A possible limitation of this study is that the timing of it coincided with an assessment requiring exposure to literature on gender differences in mental rotation, which emphasises a male advantage. This may have induced a stereotype threat effect (Moe, 2009), where some women may have felt less competent, particularly given the instant feedback, if they made several incorrect responses in a row. Incorrect responses were not analysed in this study. While not a limitation, accuracy in mental rotation should be a factor for consideration in future gender difference studies. In addition, the decrease in reaction time at 1808 for men in the greebles block, and for women in the cubes block, suggests that comparison at this rotation may be performed using different strategies, and this should be further investigated with other stimuli to see whether the findings are the same. 160 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix C S ample of a Good Research Repor t 8 Concluding paragraph pp. 104–105 In conclusion, this study aimed to extend the work of Jansen-Osmann and Heil (2007) by comparing the performance of men and women on the mental rotation task using two different classes of stimuli – cube shapes and greebles. The results provide further evidence that stimulus type does play a role in affecting performance outcomes. The insight gained should encourage future research to focus on other mechanisms, such as performance strategies and biological and environmental influences, which are responsible for the gender differences observed in mental rotation tasks. 161 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Reference list does begin on a new page References 5.6 Format of journal articles 3.3.2 9 References Casey, M., Nuttall, R., & Pezaris, E. (1999). Evidence in support of a model that predicts how biological and environmental factors influence spatial skills. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1237–1247. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.35.5.1237 Note no issue p. 46 Cooper, L. A. (1975). Mental rotation of random two-dimensional shapes. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 20–43. doi: 10.1.1.211.9457 Cooper, L. A., & Shepard, R. N. (1975). Mental rotation in the identification of left and right hands. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 48–56. doi:10.1037/ 0096-1523.1.1.48 Feng, J., Spence, I., & Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychological Science, 18, 850–855. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01990.x Gauthier, I., & Tarr, M. J. (1997). Becoming a “greeble” expert: Exploring mechanisms for face recognition. Vision Research, 37, 1673–1682. digital object identifier (doi) p. 45 doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(96)00286-6 Format of books 3.3.1 Halpern, D. F. (1992). Sex differences in cognitive abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Jansen-Osmann, P., & Heil, M. (2007). Suitable stimuli to obtain (no) gender differences in the speed of cognitive processes involved in mental rotation. Brain and Cognition, 64, 217–227. doi: 10.1016/j. bandc.2007.03.002 Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Mental images and the brain. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 333–347. Linn, M. C., & Petersen, A. C. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56, 1479–1498. doi:10.2307/1130467 Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press. 162 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix C S ample of a Good Research Repor t 10 Martin, A., Wiggs, C. L., Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. G. (1996). Neural correlates of category specific knowledge. Nature, 379, 649–652. doi:10.1038/379649a0 Masters, M. S., & Sanders, B. (1993). Is the gender difference in mental rotation disappearing? Behavior Genetics, 23, 337–341. doi: 10.1007/ BF01067434 Moe, A. (2009). Are males always better than females in mental rotation? Exploring a gender belief explanation. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 21–27. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2008.02.002 Parsons, T. D., Larson, P., Kratz, K., Thiebaux, M., Bluestein, B., Buckwalter, J. G., & Rizzo, A. A. (2004). Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial rotation in a virtual environment. Neuropsychologia, 42, 555–562. Peters, M. (2005). Sex differences and the factor of time in solving Vandenberg and Kuse mental rotation problems. Brain and Cognition, 57, 176–184. Rilea, S. L. (2008). Sex and hemisphere differences when mentally rotating meaningful and meaningless stimuli Laterality, 13, 217–233. doi: 10.1080/13576500701809846 Roberts, J. E., & Bell, M. A. (2003). Two- and three-dimensional mental rotation tasks lead to different parietal laterality for men and women. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50, 235–246. doi: 10.1016/ S0167-8760(03)00195-8 Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–703. doi: 10.1126/science.171.3972.701 Format of internet sources 3.3.3 Tarr, M. J. (2006). Greebles. Retrieved September 27, 2009, from Tarrlab: http://titan.cog.brown.edu:8080/TarrLab/stimuli/novel-objects/greebles-2-0-symmetric.zip/view Tarr, M. J., & Cheng, Y. D. (2003). Learning to see faces and objects. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 23–30. Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 250–270. doi: 10.1037//00332909.117.2.250 163 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e This page intentionally left blank. Appendix D Sample of a Poor Research Report Mental Rotation Tasks1 Anna Jones HAY 101 2 165 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 3 4 This study looked at mental rotation5 by gender, using 221 university students6 looking at greebles and cube shapes.7 8 We extended Jansen-Osmann’s9 study because of the greebles.10 Shapes were either the same or different and rotated.11 Men were faster on cubes and women were not faster on greebles.12 So our13 hypotheses were partially supported.14 This means that men are still faster on cubes, but different stimuli make a difference.15 166 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix D S ample of a Poor Research Repor t 16 Introduction17 18 Shepard and Metzler (1971) found that men could mentally rotate cube shapes faster than women.19 Both comparison speed and rotation speed.20, 21 (a) (b) 22 20 Figure 1. “Same” and “different” pairs of cube shapes differing by rotation. Lots of studies23 have found that males have superior spatial reasoning skills.24 This hasn’t25 changed since the early studies (Masters and26 Sanders, 1993). We don’t25 know why there are gender differences in MRTs.27 It’s25 not that males are always faster cognitively, because there are some tasks which females can perform faster. Jansen-Osmann &26 Heil (2007) argued that there are three things28 that could create differences:29 1)30 rotating one stimulus to align it with another, 2) comparing the two rotated images, and 3) task characteristics.31 These are called rotational speed, perceptual comparison and rotational uncertainty.32 You tell rotational speed33 from the slope of a graph.34 Comparison time and rotational uncertainty are estimated from the no rotation reaction.35 167 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Parsons, Larson, Kratz, Thiebaux, Bluestein, Buckwalter, &26 Rizzo36 (2004) used virtual reality and cubes,37 and found small but non-significant gender differences in MRTs. Jansen-Osmann and Heil (2007) used five different stimulus types38, 39 and found men were better40 only for polygons. Women were a bit faster41 at farm animals and men a bit faster41 for symbols.42 Greebles43 might be a good stimulus to test.44 The45 study compared men and women on greebles compared to cubes.46 The first hypothesis is47 that men will48 be faster than women for cube shapes.49 Women will be48 as fast or faster than men with greebles.50 168 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix D S ample of a Poor Research Repor t Method51 Participants52 Participants consisted of 162 women53 (M 5 24.6,54, 55 SD 5 8.82) and 59 men (M 5 23.9, SD 5 7.53). Materials56 The Inquisit57 program was used for the experiment.58, 59 There were four cube shapes and four greebles.60, 61 Each was rotated from 08 to 3308.62 There were two blocks.63, 64 Students had to press z or / keys to decide if the pictures65 were the same. There was immediate feedback.66 Procedure.67 Students completed the task in their own time, on their home computer or one at uni.68, 69 169 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 70 Results71 Men72 were faster all the time for cubes.73 Their slope was shallower.74 This supports the hypothesis, which means that men are better at mental rotation of cube shapes.75 76 There were no differences between men and women for greebles.77 78, 79 Cubes Mean reaction time (miliseconds) 4500 M/Shapes F/Shapes 4000 3500 3000 2500 2000 1500 1000 0 30 60 90 120 150 180 210 Rotation in degrees 240 270 300 330 Figure 2. 80Mean reaction times for cube shapes as a function of rotation angle by gender. Error bars indicate confidence intervals using standard error. Greebles Mean reaction time (miliseconds) 4500 4000 M/Greebles F/Greebles 3500 3000 2500 2000 1500 1000 0 30 60 90 120 150 180 210 240 270 300 330 Degrees of rotation Figure 3. 80Mean reaction times for greeble shapes as a function of rotation angle by gender. Error bars indicate confidence intervals using standard error. 170 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix D S ample of a Poor Research Repor t Discussion81, 82 The results supported the hypotheses for cubes.83 These results support previous research.84 Men were faster at comparison,85 and at rotational speed.85 Women were a bit faster at comparison for greebles.85, 86 But not at other angles, see Figures 2 & 3.87, 88, 89 These results support Jansen-Osmann and Heil.90, 91 Maybe men are more used to spatial stuff92 because they get to play with Lego and computer games more than women.93 Maybe greebles are more like dolls so women compare them faster.94 A limitation is that students could do the task at any time of the day or night, and this might have affected the results.95, 96 In conclusion, this study compared males and females97 on mentally rotating greebles and cubes, and found men better98 on cubes but no difference99 on greebles. So type of stimulus makes a difference. More research should be done on this important topic.100 References101,102 Jansen-Osmann, P., & Heil, M. (2007). Suitable stimuli to obtain (no) gender differences in the speed of cognitive processes involved in mental rotation. Brain and Cognition, 64,103 217–227.104 Masters, M. S., & Sanders, B. (1993). Is the gender difference in mental rotation disappearing? Behavior genetics,105 23, 337–341.106 Parsons, T. D., Larson, P., Kratz, K., Thiebaux, M., Bluestein, B., Buckwalter, J. G., and107 Rizzo, A. A. (2004). Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial rotation in a virtual environment. Neuropsychologia, 42, 555–562. Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects.108 Science, 171, 201–203. 109 171 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Problems: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 Titles should be in normal, that is, 12-point font, and should be centred across the page. Similarly, the Title Page should not have borders or decorations. There should be sufficient information to allow your tutor to return the report to you, i.e., day and time of tutorial class. See the good example on page 151. The Abstract should be labelled Abstract, centred across the page. All sections of the research report should be double-spaced. The first line of an Abstract should not be indented. “. . . mental rotation” of what? This is too vague. It is best to include dependent and independent variables, so it would help to mention gender and type of stimulus. There should be adequate description of participants, i.e., at least how many, and possibly ages or other demographics. Although some academics don’t like to see references in the Abstract, the latest APA manual does permit them (Section 2.04, p. 26). You could say “. . . Shepard and Metzler (1971) cube shapes, and Gauthier and Tarr (1997) greebles.” The Abstract should be a single paragraph. If you must have a reference, it needs a date. “. . . because of the greebles” is too vague and chatty. Better to say that you extended Jansen-Osmann and Heil’s (2007) study to include a more organic stimulus, Gauthier and Tarr’s (1997) greebles. Give a bit more detail. “. . . faster” on what? How did this relate to the hypotheses? Try to avoid personal pronouns where possible. “The hypotheses” implies they were yours – it is your report! This relates to the previous point. What was supported and what was not? “This means that . . .” is too chatty. And you really need a statement of what you concluded. Overall, the Abstract needs to describe aims, method, hypotheses, results, and provide a conclusion. Do not show a page number on the first page. The Introduction does not have a title. You should have an introductory statement describing why this area is interesting or important to report on. In fact, it would be better to have a whole introductory paragraph describing mental rotation in the Shepard and Metzler (1971) tradition. In this report you should distinguish between comparison speed and rotational speed. This is not a sentence. The idea should have been dealt with at greater length. You need to introduce the figure. The figure title should go below the figure, and Figure 1 should be in italics. This is too chatty. You need some references for such a sweeping statement. It is acceptable to have (e.g., . . .) and give two or three references to indicate the many that you are claiming found this. Do not use contractions. Spell out “This has not . . .”. You need to know when to use “and” (outside brackets!) and “&” (inside brackets). See Section 3.2, p. 38. You need to define an abbreviation the first time you use it. Say “mental rotation tasks (MRTs) . . .”. “. . . things” is too vague. It would be better to say “factors” or “variables”. Again vague. Be explicit. Say “differences in speed of mental rotation”. Series within a paragraph should be letters, not numbers, e.g., (a) (b) (c). “Task characteristics” is a bit vague for what Jansen-Osmann and Heil said. This is ambiguous. Say “These factors are called . . .”. “You tell . . .” is too chatty. Better to say that “Rotational speed is indicated by the slope . . .”. “. . . of a graph.” What graph? The graph of reaction time by rotation angle. Better to say “. . . estimated from the reaction time at 08.” If there are six or more authors, you can use Parsons et al. (2004) from the very first time used. “. . . used virtual reality and cubes” is too telegraphic. You need much more detail of what was done using virtual reality. Say what the five stimulus types were. State what they did with the five stimulus types. 172 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Appendix D S ample of a Poor Research Repor t 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 “. . . better” at what? “. . . faster” at what? “. . . symbols” would make more sense if you’d said that they were one of the five types of stimuli. Did they suggest why men and women might have been faster at mental rotation of the different stimuli? Introduce what a greeble is, who did so, and why they did it (especially since they were not designed with mental rotation in mind). Say why greebles would be a good stimulus for mental rotation studies. Don’t say “the study” if referring to your own. Make clear that you are referring to “the current study” or “the present study”. This is an ambiguous sentence. You need to make clear that your study used these different stimuli for a mental rotation study, comparing the performance of men and women. Better to start “It was hypothesised that . . .”. Use past tense for hypotheses. Be more specific about what you expected men to be faster at – comparison speed and rotational speed. The sentence is ambiguous (does having a greeble slow men down?). “. . . as fast or faster . . .” is not a specific hypothesis. Much better to have a directional one, i.e., women would be faster at comparing greeble images. The heading Method is centred across the page, and does not start on a new page. Participants should be in bold type. It is the sample that consists of women and men. Statistics letters (M, SD) should be in italics. Say “years” the first time, just to clarify what it is a mean of, i.e., (M 5 24.6 years, SD 5 8.82). Be consistent with subheadings. Materials should be bold, not italic. Give a reference for any software used. Which experiment? How was the software used in the current study? In this case, to run the mental rotation testing program online via the student portal. Why were there four of each shape? Spell out that they needed to be asymmetric in order to force mental rotation, and allow for mirror images. You need to be more specific. In this case in increments of 308. Two blocks of what? Two blocks of trials – cubes and greebles. Say that the order of presentation of the blocks was counterbalanced. “. . . pictures” is too chatty – be consistent with use – say stimuli. Also, what participants had to do belongs in Procedure. Say what the feedback was, but this also belongs in Procedure. Be consistent with subheadings. There is no full stop after the Procedure subheading. It is wise to mention ethics approval at the start of Procedure. More detail is expected. Describe what the participant saw on the computer, how they responded, what the feedback was, and how many trials they had. Results does not begin on a new page. The title Results should be in bold. Indent paragraphs. Be more specific. What were the men faster at? What slope? It is acceptable to state that a hypothesis is supported, but “better at mental rotation” is not specific enough. Mention both comparison speed and rotational speed. Introduce Figure 2 illustrating what you have just reported. This is also too vague. Be more specific. Introduce Figure 3. Do not use heavy lines around figures. Figure titles should start, e.g., Figure 2 in italics. Discussion should not start on a new page. Discussion should be in bold type and centred across the page. Remind the reader what the hypothesis was. Give references for the previous research you claim it supports, so the reader can check if your claim is accurate. 173 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 Be a bit more specific. They were faster at comparison and rotational speed for cube shapes. Again, for women and greebles. Spell it out! Looking at Figure 3, women’s speed may have been faster, but barely. Say so, and that it was only at 08. This is not a sentence. Do not refer back to tables or figures in the Discussion. Do not use ampersands (&) in text. Describe the way in which your results support previous work. Give a date for any reference used. This is far too chatty for a formal report. Unless you have a reference to support your assertion, be tentative in making suggestions. Say “Perhaps this is because they get to play . . .”. Again, be very tentative in your own suggestions. Here, you could refer back to Jansen-Osmann and Heil’s findings for animals. Don’t make “this may have affected the results” statements without saying how, and then only if the evidence is in the results. See Section 5.4 Results for ways of describing possible other reasons for your results. Use this to suggest that future research test for them. It compared their performance. Since you’ve used “men” and “women” throughout, don’t revert to males and females now. “Better” is an emotionally laden word. Say “faster” for comparison and rotational speed. “ . . . but no difference . . .” is poor expression. Say “but no different in performance . . .”. This is clichéd. You should have suggested what sort of research could be done earlier in the Discussion. You should also have said why it is important. References should be on a new page. The heading References should be centred across the page. Journal titles and volume numbers should be in italics, and second and later lines indented. Missing doi numbers. Journal titles should have major words capitalised. Page numbers should not be in italics. Use ampersands (&) in reference lists. Article titles should not have major letters capitalised. Reference lists should be double-spaced, just like the rest of the report. 174 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Glossary acknowledgment See citation. active voice A style of writing that assumptions These are beliefs or suppositions of the researcher, or even the whole society. acknowledges the person who is doing the Assumptions may not be explicitly stated, action described. In psychology writing, this either by intended omission or because the means using “I” and “we” when writing about researchers lacks conscious awareness. It your ideas/actions (e.g., “I propose that . . .”). should be noted that assumptions can affect aims The aims of a study are the statements, in the questions we ask, our attitudes to people broad general terms, of the goals that you as and research, and our behaviour. When the researcher expect to achieve. conducting research, it is useful to identify the analytical categories These are the small and simple categories that you compare and assumptions embedded in the research you are reading. contrast or analyse in your essay. You develop them from larger ideas or topics which define bibliography A list of all the journal articles the scope of the question. For example, if a and books you consulted in the preparation of question asked you about the role of deception a report. You do not include a bibliography in in social psychological research, you could a psychology research report. See also break this down into the more manageable references. analytical categories such as lack of awareness (e.g., participants who don’t know they are citation A citation is the mentioning or participants), omission (e.g., not describing acknowledgment in your text of the work of the full purpose of the study), minor deception another person. If you write “Smith and Jones or commission (e.g., like a misleading heading (1989) suggested that …” you have cited their on a questionnaire), and more serious work. Your reference list must include all the deceptions (e.g., leading people to believe they citations you have made in your report. are inflicting electric shocks on other people). apparatus The instruments or equipment used collaboration Collaboration means working together with one or more people in some in conducting the research. Typically, joint activity. Check with your tutor or apparatus is used to describe experimental lecturer about the amount of collaboration equipment (as distinct from paper and pencil acceptable when preparing for a research or online tests, which are usually called report. “materials”). argument An argument is a series of logical construct A construct is generally considered synonymous with the term “concept”. In claims, some of which are called premises and psychological terms, a construct is something one of which is called the conclusion. that is not itself observable, but is assumed to 175 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays exist because it can explain observable descriptive statistics This phrase is a general behaviour. An example is anxiety, which is not label for procedures that organise and itself observable, but is assumed to be a summarise collections of raw data. The most mechanism that gives rise to emotions such as commonly reported descriptive statistics are apprehension, distress, uneasiness, and dread, means and standard deviations. The as well as physiological symptoms such as procedures are descriptive because they increased heartrate and sweating. simply describe the data; they do not draw copying As distinct from collaboration, copying is definitely unacceptable. It means any inferences about it. design The design of an experiment refers to using the work of another person, including how the experiment was organised; that is, another student, and passing it off as your how many groups of participants you had and own. Do not do this. See also plagiarism. what procedures were carried out on each. In complex studies, there is a separate data Data are the body of facts gathered in an experiment or a study. The singular is subheading in the Methods section to explain the design. “datum”. “Raw data” are data as they have been collected, before you have summarised them or analysed them in any way. debriefing Debriefing is the explanation you empirical This term is used very loosely to cover a number of concepts. Most commonly it refers to the collection, analysis, or give to a participant at the completion of a evaluation of data. An empirical finding is study or experiment. Such an explanation something that has been actually observed, as should include an overview of the theory the distinct from something inferred or deduced. research relates to, the research question, your An empirical construct is one that has been hypotheses, and the relevance of what was inferred from observation of behaviour rather done to the participant. If any deception was involved, the debriefing provides an accurate than deduced from a theory. endnotetm A software program that helps you description of the research, and clarifies the manage references as you write your report. It nature and necessity of hiding this. may be free to you as a student; check with degrees of freedom These refer to the your library. independent pieces of information known ethics See research ethics. about a sample for which we are about to evidence In the context of psychology research calculate a statistic. They are important because reports and essays, evidence is the data they give an idea about how powerful or collected by researchers. The conclusions they informative the statistic is, and that is why they have drawn from that data are a sort of are reported when any statistical test is made. evidence, as is any publication that is available dependent variable A dependent variable is a variable whose values are the result of changes in the values of other variables. In an to be checked, but such information is more questionable. experiment An experiment is a set of experiment, the dependent variable is what conditions and procedures for testing a you actually measure. You are hoping that the hypothesis. It emphasises control over values of it are “dependent” on the other independent variables and the measurement of variables that you already know about (the dependent variables so that cause-and-effect “independent variables”). relationships can be determined. 176 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Glossar y explicit measures See self-report measures. can be subjected to empirical test and then extension supported or not supported by the evidence. In the context of research reports, an extension is the allocation of more time to complete the assignment than was implicit measures Implicit measures are originally allowed. This situation will measures that assess a construct of interest normally be permitted only in traumatic without requiring introspection or self-report circumstances that can be documented in (See self-report measures). Popular implicit some way. Requests for extensions should be measures include the Implicit Association Test made to your tutor or lecturer before the (IAT), priming and the Stroop Test, all of original due date. which assess concepts such as attitudes or extraneous (nuisance) variable Sometimes called “chance error”, this term refers to the effects of variables that arise from chance, self-esteem as the relative speed with which words can be correctly identified. independent variable This term refers to a such as running the experiment on an variable where the values it can take are uncomfortably hot day. Usually, with real-life manipulated or controlled or recorded by an experiments and large numbers of experimenter, so that their effect on some participants, such errors tend to cancel each other variable (the dependent variable) can be other out. In the context of student observed. It is called “independent” because, experiments, they may affect the outcome of in principle, its values are not dependent on the study, and you should be aware of their changes in the values of other variables. possible effect. inferential statistics Statistics of this kind are used when making inferences. They use the factor A factor is anything that has a causal mathematics of probability theory to infer influence on something else. It is often used generalisations about populations from data interchangeably with “independent variable”, taken from samples of those populations. The especially in writing about statistical ones you will first come across are t-tests and procedures. Some of those procedures talk analysis of variance. about “levels” of a factor. This term refers to the magnitude of whatever the factor is, such as decibel level or dosage level. figures The graphical depiction of an idea. Figures are most commonly used to present data in psychology reports, but they can also be used to represent models or to depict experimental materials or methods. manipulation (experimental) This term usually refers to controlling the values of an independent variable; for example, having three groups of participants, each of which is given a different treatment. measurement Measurement is the assigning of numbers to events or objects within strict rules. It might be as simple as obtaining generic Used in the sense of “generic skills”, it people’s height in centimetres, or as means more widely applicable than just to complicated as recording their intelligence psychology. according to IQ scores. methodology Strictly speaking, methodology hypothesis A hypothesis is any statement that refers to the way in which knowledge and could serve as an explanation of certain facts. understanding are achieved. Many people use It should be presented in such a form that it the term as synonymous with “scientific 177 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays method”. It is a much broader term than effects and the psychological effects operating “method”, which refers to the particular on subjects who believe they are being given procedures used in a specific investigation. the real drug. plagiarism In simple terms, plagiarism means operationalisation Operationalisation refers to “copying”. It is used to refer to copying from the procedure by which a theory gives rise to a someone else’s published work without specific hypothesis that is concrete and acknowledging the real source of the work. It testable. For example, you might have a is unacceptable because it is the academic theory that says that attention is directly equivalent of theft: taking the work of other related to proximity of a stimulus. Such a people and using it as if it were yours. It does statement is very broad. You could not simply apply to quoting the exact words of operationalise it to hypothesise that students someone’s published work without at the front of the class would maintain more acknowledgment. Using someone’s published eye contact with the lecturer than students at ideas without acknowledgment is also the back of the class. This statement is now a plagiarism. You are encouraged to express specific and testable hypothesis. If supported, other people’s ideas in your own words, but it adds support to your theory. you must make a reference in your report organised scepticism This is the attitude that you won’t believe what is said unless there is indicating that this is what you have done. post hoc Latin for “after this”. In the context of good evidence to back up the assertion. psychology, it usually refers to statistical Organised scepticism is a fundamental procedures used after an experiment has been property of good science – that it is the run and some overall statistical significance evidence that counts, not the authority of the found. These procedures determine which of scientist making the pronouncement. several factors contribute to the overall significance. paraphrasing Expressing someone else’s ideas in your own words. participant A participant is someone who takes part in a study. Until the APA 5th edition, participants were frequently primary source A primary source is a reference, such as a journal article or book, that you have actually read. Contrast with secondary source. problem (research problem) A research referred to as subjects. However, the term problem is the question that you want to participant is now used because it answer by running your study. In general acknowledges the role of consent and terms, it is a situation where some of the agency in participation. In the Particants components are known, and others need to be section of a report, you need to describe discovered. your participants, at least in general terms, proof, prove (evidence) Very loosely, proof is so that your reader can decide how any demonstration that provides logical representative your results are for whatever support for a belief. In the context of a research group the reader is interested in. report, it is important to emphasise that you placebo A treatment or preparation with no cannot prove anything! Your hypothesis can be medicinal or pharmacological effects. It is supported if the observed facts are in agreement used in studies of the effects of drugs to with the propositions in your hypothesis, or it distinguish between real pharmacological can fail to be supported if the observed facts are 178 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Glossar y not in agreement with the hypothesis. Neither replicate, replication To replicate an the hypothesis nor the theory from which it is experiment is to reproduce exactly the derived can ever be proved. method of the earlier experiment on a new sample of subjects. Because conclusions are qualitative Qualitative refers to differences in drawn from experiments on the basis of kind, so that apples are qualitatively different probability, replication is important. Repeated from oranges. In psychological terms, similar findings add to the support for the qualitative research might involve asking hypotheses made, and by inference to the participants about their experiences of joy. theories that produce the hypotheses. It is quantitative Something is quantitative if it is because replication is important to the capable of being measured in quantities. To scientific method that the Method section contrast quantitative with qualitative, your needs to be accurate enough for other anxiety at being asked to recall the name of experimenters to be able to reproduce your your 5th grade teacher is likely less (i.e., a 1 experiment exactly. out of 5) than your anxiety if handling a large spider (i.e., possibly 5 out of 5!). representative A “representative sample” means that the characteristics of the sample questionnaire A questionnaire is any set of are sufficiently like those of the population questions designed to be answered by a that you can make realistic judgments about person, who is usually called a “respondent” the population on the basis of what you find in this situation. Typically, the questions will about your sample. deal broadly with a single area of interest or a small number of such areas. research database(s) These provide an online and searchable index of research papers. quotations This is where, in your report or research ethics Research ethics in the context essay, you use the exact words from a of this text refers to the process of review reference. It is important that they can be that is undertaken to ensure the project recognised as the words of someone else and upholds the values and practices of ethical that your reader can find the source of them. research including merit and respectfulness (see https://www.nhmrc.gov.au/ random sample A sample is a subset of a guidelines-publications/e72) population. The students in your psychology class are a sample of the university population. sample A sample is a subset of a population. However, they may not be representative of The students in your psychology class are a that population. A random sample is a sample sample of the university population. However, taken so that every member of the population they may not be representative of that has an equal, independent probability of being population. See also random sample. scale, subscale selected. reference In the context of a research report, a In the sense in which you will most frequently encounter it, a scale is a reference is any material to which you refer in collection of questionnaire items that measure the body of the report. Typically, references some psychological construct. An example will be journal articles or books, but in an would be an intelligence scale. A subscale is a essay they could also be newspaper articles, subset of these items that measure some government reports, or television programs. component of the overall variable, say the See also primary source and secondary source. verbal ability subscale of an intelligence scale. 179 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays scientific method An approach to developing often used as a more general term than knowledge through the process involving “experiment”, because a study does not observation, data collection, and reporting. necessarily record or manipulate independent secondary source A secondary source is a variables. A study may be an observation of reference that you have not read yourself but behaviour or a collection of information by has been referred to in something you did questionnaire, or it may be an experiment. read. If you read in your textbook that Piaget subject See participant. (1932) came to some conclusion, but you do survey A survey is a search for particular kinds not have access to Piaget’s actual article, then of information. It generally refers to a study any statement you make about what Piaget using questionnaires, interviews, or said in that article must acknowledge where established scales, for example, intelligence or you read it, namely in your textbook, not in aptitude scales. Piaget’s original work. self-report measures Self-report measures tables Used to summarise and compare include questionnaires and rating scales that information, typically data. Columns are given require the participant to report their views or headings to indicate the common information evaluations. to be compared (e.g., mean and standard significance level This is an arbitrary value, deviations, or questionnaire used), and the which you use as a criterion for deciding rows are the individual instances being whether your observed data are different compared (e.g., experimental condition, enough from what you could expect if only chance factors were operating to consider your journal article). theory A theory is a general principle or set of data to be statistically significant. It is contentions advanced as an explanation for a frequently set at 5%. If p is the probability of set of known facts or empirical findings. Good the observed results or more extreme ones theories can be used to deduce many happening by chance alone, then “p , .05” hypotheses in different areas of application, shows that your data indicate a statistically which are then supported when tested significant result. empirically. statistic A statistic is a number derived from Turnitin Turnitin is an online teaching and some mathematical manipulation of your data, learning tool used at many universities which which summarises a characteristic of your is designed to identify word-for-word sample, and theoretically reflects the similarity between students’ assignments as population from which the sample was drawn. well as online texts, articles and websites. An example is the mean of your sample. The term “statistic” is also applied to the number variable A variable is some characteristic of resulting from a statistical procedure. For people or things that can change, either example, the number you obtain when within the person or between persons, things, applying a t-test is a statistic. or situations. Variables can be as obvious as study A psychological study is an investigation height or eye colour, or as complex as into some aspect of the thought or behaviour intelligence, ability to cope, or marital of people or, sometimes, other animals. It is satisfaction. 180 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e References Albert, R. S. (1975). Toward a behavioral definition of genius. American Psychologist, 30, 140–151. doi:10.1037/h0076861 American Psychological Association (2001). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychological Association (2010). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Australian Bureau of Statistics (2015, August 8) Alcohol consumption. Retrieved from http://www.abs.gov.au/ ausstats/abs@.nsf/Lookup/by%20Subject/4364.0.55.001~2014-15~Main%20Features~Alcohol%20 consumption~25 Australian Psychological Society (2017). Code of ethics. Victoria, Australia: Author. Retrieved from http:// www.psychology.org.au Australian Psychology Accreditation Council. (2014). APAC rules for accreditation. Melbourne, Australia: Author. Retrieved from https://www.psychologycouncil.org.au/Assets/Files/2016_05%20APAC%20 RULES%20FOR%20ACCREDITATIONAmended%20AT%20edit%20formatted_June_2016.pdf Bell, N. (2015) We are not amused. Failed humor in interaction. Retrieved from https://www.degruyter.com/ view/product/449175 Betts, K., Seitz, A., & Farquharson, K. (2006). Writing essays and research reports in the social sciences (3rd ed.). South Melbourne, Victoria: Thomson. Brewster, M. K. (2000). Entering and exiting the corporation: A developmental study of women executives at mid-life. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering, Vol. 60(9–B), 4877. Broadbent, E. (2017). Interactions with robots: The truths we reveal about ourselves. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 627–652. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-043958 Burdess, N. (1991). The handbook of student skills. Sydney: Prentice-Hall. Burdess, N. (2007). Good study. Frenchs Forest, NSW: Pearson Australia. Buzwell, S., Farrugia, M., & Williams, J. (2016). Students’ voice regarding important characteristics of online and face-to-face higher education. Sensoria: A Journal of Mind, Brain, and Culture, 12, 38–49. doi:dx.doi.org/10.7790/sa.v12i1.430 Cattell, R. B. (1965). The scientific analysis of personality. Baltimore: Penguin. Corke, M., Bell, J., Goodhew, S. C., Smithson, M., & Edwards, M. (2016). Perceived time slows during fleeting fun or fear. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 1–7. doi:10.1080/17470218.20 16.1264000 Crone, D., & Williams, L. (2017). Crowdsourcing participants for psychological research in Australia: A test of Microworkers. Australian Journal of Psychology, 69, 39–47. doi:10.1111/ajpy.12110 181 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Denson, T. F. (2015). Four promising psychological interventions for reducing reactive aggression. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 136–141. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.04.003 Edwards, A. L. (1957). The social desirability variable in personality research. New York: Dryden. Eysenck, M. W. (2004). Psychology: An international perspective. New York: Psychology Press. Fine, C. (2017). Testosterone Rex: Myths of sex, science, and society. New York: WW Norton & Co. Forgas, J. (2017). Can sadness be good for you? Australian Psychologist, 52, 3–13. doi:10.1111/ap.12232 Forshaw, M. (2012). Critical thinking for psychology. West Sussex, UK: BPS Blackwell. Freeman, H. R. (1992). Effect of instructor gender and gender role on student willingness to take a psychology course. Teaching of Psychology, 19, 93–95. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1207/ s15328023top1902_7 Freud, S. (1953). The method of interpreting dreams: An analysis of a specimen dream. In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.). The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 4, pp. 96–121). Retrieved from http://books.google.com/books (Original work published 1900). Gage, E., Coker, S., & Jobson, L. (2015). Cross-cultural differences in desirable and undesirable forms of self-consistency and influence on symptoms of depression and anxiety. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46, 713–722. doi:10.1177/0022022115578475 Goodson, P. (2017). Becoming an academic writer. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Harris, R. (2015). Evaluating internet research sources. Retrieved from http://www.virtualsalt.com/evalu8it. htm Haviland, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1974). What’ s new? Acquiring new information as a process in comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 13, 512–521. doi:10.1016/S00225371(74)80003-4 Haybron, D. (2011). Happiness. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Retrieved from http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/ happiness/ Holland, E., Koval, P., Stratemeyer, M., Thomson, F., & Haslam, N. (2017). Sexual objectification in women’s daily lives: A smartphone ecological momentary assessment study. British Journal of Social Psychology, 56, 314–333. doi:10.1111/bjso.12152 Hyde, T. S., & Jenkins, J. J. (1969). Differential effects of incidental tasks on the organization of recall of a list of highly associated words. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82, 472–481. doi:10.1037/h0028372 Keating C., Stephens, J., Thomas, N., Castle, D. J., & Rossell, S. L. (2016). Gender differences in weightrelated and non-weight-related appearance concerns in a community sample. Australian Journal of Psychology, 68, 11–19. doi:10.1111/ajpy.12092 Keen, J., & Fitness, J. (in press). Can employers screen job applicants for potential white collar fraud offenders? In S. Boag & N. Tiliopoulos (Eds.), Personality and individual differences: Theory, assessment, and application. New York: Nova. Kidder, L. H., & Judd, C. M. (1986). Research methods in social relations (5th ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Kinoshita, S., de Wit, B., Aji, M., & Norris, D. (2017). Evidence accumulation in the integrated and primed Stroop tasks. Memory and Cognition, 45, 824–836. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-017-0701-8 Knowles, S., Austin, D., Sivanesan, S., Tye-Din, J., Leung, C., Wilson, J., . . . Hebbard, G. (2017). Relations between symptom severity, illness perceptions, visceral sensitivity, coping strategies and well-being in irritable bowel syndrome guided by the common sense model of illness. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 22, 522–534. doi:10.1080/13548506.2016.1168932 182 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e References Levine, M. P., Smolak, L., & Hayden, H. (1994). The relation of sociocultural factors to eating attitudes and behaviors among middle school girls. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, 471–490. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431694014004004 Lewin, K. (1948). Resolving social conflicts. New York: Harper & Row. Louis, W. R., Bastian, B., McKimmie, B., & Lee, A. J. (2016). Teaching psychology in Australia: Does class attendance matter for performance?, 68, 47–51. doi:10.1111/ajpy.12088 Macrae, K. (1989). Psychology 1 course handbook. Melbourne, Australia: Department of Psychology, La Trobe University. March, E., & Grieve, R. (2016). Social-economic theory and short-term mate preferences: The effects of gender roles and socioeconomic status. Australian Journal of Psychology, 68, 241–250. doi:10.1111/ ajpy.12102 Marriage equality facts at a glance (n.d.). Retrieved from http://www.australianmarriageequality.org/ get-informed/quick-facts/ Marshall, L. A., & Rowland, F. (2013). A guide to learning independently (5th ed.). Frenchs Forest, NSW: Pearson Australia. Martyna, W. (1978). What does ‘He’ mean? Journal of Communication, 28, 131–138. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1978.tb01576.x Mathew, J., Dunning, C., Coats, C., & Whelan, T. (2014). The mediating influence of hope on multidimensional perfectionism and depression. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 67–71. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.008 Miller, N. (2017, May 20). UK migration squeeze hits Australians. The Age, p. 14. Mizuno, H. (2017). Establishing and validating a measure of the Japanese concept of self. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Swinburne University of Technology, Melbourne, Australia. Monash transition program. (2017). Retrieved from www.monash.edu.au/study/ parents/transition Murray, G., Rawlings, D., Allen, N. B., & Trinder, J. (2003). NEO Five-Factor Inventory scores: Psychometric properties in a community sample. Measurement & Evaluation in Counseling Development, 36, 140–149. Murray, D. R., Murphy, S. C., von Hippel, W., Trivers, R., & Haselton, M. G. (2017). A preregistered study of competing predictions suggests that men do overestimate women’s sexual intent. Psychological Science, 28, 253–255. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616675474 Palmer J., & Gundlock, B. (2017). Dying of thirst. Good Weekend, pp. 14–19. Partos, T. R., Cropper, S. J., & Rawlings, D. (2016). You don’t see what I see: Individual difference in the perception of meaning from visual stimuli. PLoS One, 11, e0150615. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0150615 Patterson, R. W., & Patterson, R. M. (2017). Computers and productivity: Evidence from laptop use in the college classroom. Economics of Education Review, 57, 66–79. doi:10.1016/j.econedurev.2017.02.004 Payne, Jason & Piquero, Alex. (2017). The concordance of self-reported and officially recorded criminal onset: Results from a sample of Australian prisoners. Crime & Delinquency. doi:10.1177/0011128716682440. Piccoli, V., Fantoni, C., Foroni, F., Bianchi, M., & Carnaghi, A. (2017). Automatic female dehumanization across the menstrual cycle. British Journal of Social Psychology, 56, 270–280. doi:10.1111/bjso.12178 Pierce, G. R., Sarason, B. R., & Sarason, I. G. (1992). General and specific support expectations and stress as predictors of perceived supportiveness: An experimental study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 297–307. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.297 183 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Platow, M. J., Haslam, A. S., Reicher, S. D., & Steffens, N. K. (2015). There is no leadership if no one follows: Why leadership is necessarily a group process. International Coaching Psychology Review, 10, 20–37. Pope, K. S. (1980). On love and loving. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Popper, K. R. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. London, UK: Hutchinson & Co. Prendergast, L. E., Leung, R. K., Toumbourou, J., Taft, A., McMorris, B. J., & Catalano, R. F. (2017). Sexual behaviour in early adolescence: A cross-national comparison of Australian and United States youth. Australian Journal of Psychology, 69, 3–11. doi:10.1111/ajpy.12118 Roberts, R. M., Neate, G. M., & Gierasch, A. (2016). Implicit attitudes towards people with visible difference: findings from an Implicit Association Test. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 22, 352–358. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2016.1163399 Robertson, S., Davies, M., & Winefield, H. (2015). Why weight for happiness? Correlates of BMI and SWB in Australia. Obesity Research & Clinical Practice, 9, 609–612. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/j. orcp.2015.04.011 Schiraldi, G. R. (2001). The post-traumatic stress disorder sourcebook: A guide to healing, recovery, and growth [Adobe Digital Editions version]. doi:10.1036/ 0071393722 Schul, Y., & Burnstein, E. (1990). Judging the typicality of an instance: Should the category be accessed first? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 964–974. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.964 Sebastian, P., Kaufmann, L. M., & de la Piedad Garcia, X. (2017, January). In the nose, not in the beholder: Embodied cognition effects override individual differences. Embodied Cognition Preconference at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Psychology and Social Psychology. San Antonio, Texas, United States. Shell, M. D., Gazelle, H., & Faldowski, R. A. (2014). Anxious solitude and the middle school transition: a diathesis 3 stress model of peer exclusion and victimization trajectories. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1569–1583. doi:10.1037/a0035528 Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support seeking and support giving within couples in an anxiety-provoking situation: The role of attachment styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 434–446. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.62.3.434 Skeffington, P. M., Rees, C. S., & Mazzucchelli, T. (2017). Trauma exposure and post-traumatic stress disorder within fire and emergency services in Western Australia. Australian Journal of Psychology, 69, 20–28. doi:10.1111/ajpy.12120 Smith, R. A. (1995). Challenging your preconceptions: Thinking critically about psychology. Pacific Grove, California: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company. Sternberg, R. J. (2010). The psychologist’s companion: A guide to scientific writing for students and researchers (5th ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511762024 Tearne, J. E., Robinson, M., Jacoby, P., Allen, K. L., Cunningham, N. K., Li, J., & Mclean, N. J. (2016). Older maternal age is associated with depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms in young adult female offspring. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125, 1–10. doi:10.1037/abn0000119 Thielking, M., & Terjesen, M. D. (Eds.). (2017). Handbook of Australian school psychology: Integrating international research, practice, and policy. Cham, Switzerland: Springer Nature. Townsend, M. A. R., Moore, D. W., Tuck, B. F., & Wilton, K. M. (1990). Headings within multiple-choice tests as facilitators of test performance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 60, 153–160. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1990.tb00932.x 184 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e References Trounson, J., Critchley, C., & Pfeifer, J. (2015). Australian attitudes toward asylum seekers: Roles of dehumanization and Social Dominance Theory. Social Behavior and Personality, 43, 1641–1656. doi:10.2224/sbp.2015.43.10.1641 Truss, L. (2003). Eats, shoots & leaves: The zero tolerance approach to punctuation. London: Profile Books. Vaughan, G. M., & Hogg, M. A. (2017). Social psychology (7th ed.). Frenchs Forest, Australia: Pearson Education. von Hippel, W., Ronay, R., & Maddux, W. W. (2016). Of baboons and elephants: Inequality and the evolution of immoral leadership. In J. P. Forgas, L. Jussim, & P. A. M. van Lange (Eds.) Social psychology and morality (pp. 289–303). New York: Psychology Press. Weier, M., & Lee, C. (2016). Stagnant or successful, carefree or anxious? Australian university students’ goals and beliefs about adulthood and their current well-being. Australian Journal of Psychology, 51, 422–430. doi:10.1111/ap.12169 Weinberg, M. K., Noble, J. M., & Hammond, T. G. (2016). Sleep well feel well: An investigation into the protective value of sleep quality on subjective well-being. Australian Journal of Psychology, 68, 91–97. doi:10.1111/ajpy.12098 Zinkiewicz, L., Curtis, A., Meurer, H., & Miller, P. (2016). Demographic risk factors for alcohol-related aggression in and around licensed venues. Alcohol, 51, 196–200. doi:10.1093/alcalc/agv083 185 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e This page intentionally left blank. Index Page numbers in bold indicate glossary entries Page numbers in italics refer to tables and figures abbreviations 23, 86 Latin 26 Abstract section 4, 70–2 checklists for 110 citing 47 reference list 42–9 assumptions 17–18, 175 referencing within reports 34 Australian Psychological Society on statistics 90 on titles 68 on writing style 22 14, 62 Australian Psychology Accreditation Council 34 in essays 113, 118, 119, 126 apostrophes 25 authority, illegitimate appeal to 18 examples of 71, 72 apparatus 82, 175 authors, order of 43 function of 70 appeal to popular belief 18 length of 70 Appendices 4, 105–6 Academic Search Complete 12, backup copy 66, 106, 125 figures and tables in 106 bar charts 87, 88 format of 106 belief statements 21 accuracy 15 procedures in 86 Betts, K. 125 acknowledgment see citations questionnaires in 86, 87 bibliographic software 57 acronyms 23 statistics in 105 bibliography 105, 175 active voice 22, 175 whether to include 31 Body section of essays 113, 118, 56, 57 ad hominem arguments 18 area of study, defining 74 advice seeking 31–2 argument 18, 175 bold type 27 aims 55, 55, 175 articles books 44, 56 121–2, 126 alphabetical order 38, 43 electronic 45–8 chapters in 44 American Psychological journal see journal articles eBooks 48 Association 14 style manual see APA Publication Manual analyses of variance (ANOVAs) 90 analytical categories 108, 175 magazine 49 newspaper 11, 49 textbooks 11, 14, 74 Burdess, N. 26, 121, 125 unpublished 46 assignments see also essays; research reports capitalisation of titles 44, 46, 68, 97 annual periodicals 46 backup copy of 66, 106, 125 CARS checklist 15 APA Publication Manual 106 marks see marks chapters, in books 44 on abstract length 70 Pass requirements 64 checklists on fonts 26–7, 31 physical presentation of in-text referencing 38 on language-based bias avoidance 81 30–2, 110, 124–5 Association for Psychological Science 14 CARS 15 essays 126–7 research reports 108–10 children, as participants 61 187 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays chronological order 38, 41, 43, 118, 121 citations 36–42, 175 ‘critically review’ (in essay topic) 116 cultural differences 104 classic works 39 direct acknowledgment 37–8 preparation of 63 writing style 27 distressed participants 61 doi (digital object identifier) data 176 data analysis 89 44, 45 due dates 125 see also time in essays 124 databases 12, 56, 57 examples of 40, 41 debriefing 86, 176 online titles 39 decimals 93 Eats, Shoots & Leaves (Truss) 24 plagiarism and 34, 35, 36 ‘define’ (in essay topic) 116–17 eBooks 48 quotations 42 definition of terms 74 edited books 44 relevance of sources 41–2 degrees of freedom 90, 176 effect sizes 91–2 repeat 38, 39 demographic variables 80, 82 electronic articles 45–8 secondary sources 39, 41 departmental studies 54–7 electronic books (eBooks) 48 classic works 39 dependent variables 58, 59, 176 empirical 176 clichés 72 ‘describe’ (in essay topic) encyclopaedias 48 code of ethics 62 116–17 management endnote 57, 176 collaboration 8–9, 175 descriptive statistics 90, 176 equipment (apparatus) 81–2, 175 commas 24–5, 44 design 85–6, 176 error bars 98, 99 ‘compare’ (in essay topic) 116 comparisons between groups 94 Conclusion section essays 113, 118, 122, 123, 126 faults in 103 detective novel structure 119 errors, typographical 28–30 essays dictionaries 74 appendices 31 digital object identifier (doi) checklist for 126–7 44, 45 collaboration on 8–9 direct acknowledgment 38 directive words for 114–17 directive words 114–17 further reading on 125–6 conclusions, unjustified 24 discipline 9 information gathering conference papers 46 ‘discuss’ (in essay topic) 113, research reports 104–5 confidence intervals 91–2, 98 114, 116 confidentiality 61 discussion boards 31 consent 60 Discussion section 4, 100–5 for 9–14 length of 113, 122 physical presentation of 30–2, 124–5, 127 construct 175–6 checklists for 109, 126 preparation for 117–18, 122 contractions 25 conclusion 104–5 proofreading see proofreading ‘contrast’ (in essay topic) 116 in essays 113, 118, 121–2, reasons for writing 2–3 copying 176 see also plagiarism versus collaboration 8–9 126 evaluation of 20 redrafting 28–30, 64, 124–5 references in 12–13, 113, 117, correlational studies 59–60 hypotheses in 62, 100 124, 126 see also correlations 90 implications of results 103 references counterbalancing 89 interpretation of results 100–2 versus reports 3, 113 credibility 15–16 versus Introduction structure of 5, 113, 118–24 critical thinking 16–21 ‘critically evaluate’ (in essay topic) 113–14 section 100 limitations 103–4 new references in 102–3 body 113, 118, 121–2, 126 conclusion 113, 118, 122–3, 123, 126 188 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Index introduction 113, 118, 119, gender imbalance 104 interaction effects 92 gender-neutral language 28, 29 internet see online resources titles of 115 generic skills 3, 16, 177 interpretation of results 100–2 topic of 113–14 given-new principle 16 in-text referencing see citations at university level 5 Good study (Burdess) 125 Introduction section 4, 72–7 writing process 112 Google Scholar 12, 56 area of study, defining 74 writing style 21–7 government reports 49 checklists for 109, 126 ethics see research ethics grades see marks versus Discussion section 100 ethics committee 58, 60 graphs 87–8, 88, 98, 98–9 in essays 113, 118, 119, ‘evaluate’ (in essay topic) 113–14 Greek letters 90 120–1, 122, 126 evidence (proof) 11, 176, 178–9 group work 8–9 hypotheses in 62, 77 ‘examine’ (in essay topic) A guide to learning independently length of 22, 63, 122 120–1, 122, 126 116–17 experiment 10, 176 (Marshall & Rowland) opening sentences 72, 73 125 preparation of 63 explanation of findings 101–2 explicit measures see self-report measures extension 65, 177 extraneous variable 86, 103, 177 previous research in 74–6, 75 headings introductory statements 89, 89 in Method section 79–80, 79 issue numbers 45 typeface for 27 italics 26, 44, 45 highlighting 56–7, 118 histograms 97–8 jargon 23, 76 hypotheses 17, 54, 177 journal articles factor 177 aims and 55, 55 authors 43, 45–6 Farquharson, K. 125 in Discussion section 62, 100 databases of 12 feedback 107–8 examples of 78 electronic 45–7 figures 97–9, 177 identification of 62 evaluation of 12–13, 18–21 in appendices 106 in Introduction section 61, 76–7 format of 3, 31, 70 graphs 88, 88, 98, 98–9 multiple 90, 100 note-taking on 56–7 introductions to 97 specificity of 58–60 referencing 11, 45–7 see also presentation of 98, 98–9 support for 92, 100 titles of 97, 97 References versus research reports 3–4 findings, explanation of 101–2 illegitimate appeal to authority 18 first editions 44 implications of results 103 flowcharts for writing 52, 112 implicit measures 83, 84–5, 177 fluency of expression 22–3 inclusive language 28, 29 keywords 12 folders 32 incomplete sentences 23 knowledge 11 fonts 26–7, 31 independent variables 58–9, format 58, 177 appendices 106 index cards 117 essays 4, 113, 118–24 inferential statistics 90, 177 journal articles 4, 31 information gathering 9–14 references 105 informed consent 60 research reports 3–4, 68 in-press articles 46 full stops (periods) 44 instructions for participants 86, 87 titles of 45, 68, 69 journalistic style 119 laboratory reports see research reports language inclusive 28, 29 word meanings 23–4 language-based bias, avoidance of 81 189 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays Latin abbreviations 26 Pass requirements 64 encyclopaedias 48 law of diminishing returns 64 reference lists and 105 evaluation of 15–16 lecturers relation to effort 30 journal articles 45–7 on appendices 31, 107 Marshall, L. A. 125 on collaboration 8–9 Materials section 81–3, 83 on essay topics 114, 116 matrix of variables 58–9, 58 open-access journals 46 feedback from 107–8 mature age students 114 opening sentences 72, 73 on opinion statements 119 mean 87, 90, 90 operationalisation 17, 58, 178 on plagiarism 34 meanings, confusing 23–4 opinion statement 119, 122 on presentation 30–2, measurement 177 order of references 38, 43 68–70, 119 on references 45, 74, 102, 124 on research design 76, 85–6, 103 referencing 39, 124 textbook-linked 14 scales 82, 83, 179 organised scepticism 178 subscales 82, 179 outlines median 90 essays 118, 122 Method section 4, 77–87 research reports 62–3 seeking advice from 31–2 checklist for 109 on statistical analysis 88, evaluation of 19–20, 54 page numbering 30–1, 70, 72 90, 92 function of 68, 77–9 paragraph structure 22–3, 122 studies run by 54–8, 60, 86 hypotheses in 62 paraphrasing 36, 178 on time management 65 preparation of 63 participants 79, 178 on writing style 22, 26, 70–2 subheadings in 79 consent by 60 writing style 27 description of 80, 80 length of abstracts 70 methodology 177–8 gender imbalance in 104 of essays 113, 122 discussion based on 101 instructions for 86, 88 of introductions 22, 63, 77, 122 flaws in 103–4 number of 80 of report sections 63, 76 Monash University tutorial 15 roles of 87 library resources 12, 56 multiple authors 44–5 students as 60 limitations 103–4 multiple hypotheses 90, 100 linking sentences 122 passive voice 22 Pass requirements 64 newspaper articles 11, 49 past tense 27 magazine articles 49 non-significant results 90, 97 peer review 12 main effects 92 note-taking periodicals 13, 46 see also manipulation (experimental) 55, 59, 177 for essays 117–18 for references 56–7 journal articles periods (full stops) 44 margins 30–1 nuisance variable 86, 177 personal opinions 119, 122 marks 8 numbering personal pronouns 22, appendices and 105–6 Discussion section and 103 double-marking 66 pages 30–1, 70, 72 tables 94 numerals, versus words 92 essay topics and 113 objectivity 21–2 inconsistent 108 online resources 13–14 74, 76 physical presentation 30–2, 110, 124–5 placebo 178 feedback from 107–8 Introduction section and 72, 28, 62 plagiarism 9, 34–6, 178 avoiding 36 databases 12 causes of 35 discussion boards 31 checking for 36, 65–6, 125 190 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e Index versus collaboration 8–9 qualitative 10, 179 defined 34 quantitative 96, 179 checklist for 108–10 quasi-experiments 10 collaboration on 8–9 questionnaires 61, 179 defined 3–4 planning essays 117–18, 122 research reports 62–3 plural pronouns 28 research reports in appendices 82 versus essays 3, 113 as materials 82 examples of popular belief, appeal to 18 quotation marks 25, 42 good 129–38, 151–63 postgraduate training 2 quotations 23, 42, 76, 179 poor 139–49, 165–74 post hoc procedures 90, 92, 178 further reading on 106–7 predictions see hypotheses radio programs 124 how to approach 53–4 preparation checklists 108–9, 126 random sample 179 how to begin 54–60 preparation for essays 117–18 reading-aloud test 23 information gathering for 9–14 presentation of assignments reality checks 16 versus journal articles 4 reasonableness 15–16 physical presentation of 30–2, 110, 124–5, 127 present tense 27 previous research in Discussion section 100 in Introduction section 74–6, 75 redrafting 28–30, 64, 104, 124–5 References 4, 11–12, 105, 179 30–2, 110 proofreading see proofreading reasons for writing 2–3 checklists for 110, 126 redrafting 28–30, 64, 104 primary sources 13, 178 for departmental studies 54–7 references for 11–12 see also probability 90–1 in Discussion section 102–3 problem 178 in essays 113, 117, 124, 126 procedural problems 103 format of 42–9 Procedure section 86–7 in-text see citations pronouns 22, 28, 62 major 54 proof (evidence) 11, 176, 178–9 note-taking for 56–7 proofreading 28–30, 36 number of 56, 124 Reserve section of library 12 order of 38, 43 Results section 4, 87–98 choosing someone for 9, 29, 64–5 importance of 9 for self-designed studies References structure of 4, 68 see also specific report sections writing process 52, 62–6, 105, 108 writing style 21–7 checklist for 109 57–8 evaluation of 20 referencing 33–49 first reports 87–8 PsycARTICLES 12, 56, 57 relevance 12–13, 41–2, 76 function of 68 Psychclassics (website) 14, 105 replication 10, 179 see also hypotheses in 62 time management 64–5, 106 The Psychologist’s Companion (Sternberg) 107 previous research reports see research reports introductory statements 89, 89 statistics in see statistics psychology, as a discipline 9 representative references 124 ‘review’ (in essay topic) 116 psychology organisations 14 representative sample 179 rewriting 29 psychometric properties 20, 82 republished books 48 Rowland, F. 125 PsycINFO 12, 56, 57 research, previous running heads 70 Publication Manual see APA Publication Manual in Discussion section 100 in Introduction section 74–6, 75 sample 20, 80, 179 punctuation 24–5, 43 research database 56, 179 scales 82, 83, 179 Purdue University, Online research ethics 60–2, 179 scepticism, organised 178 research problem 178 scientific method 10–11, 17, 180 Writing Lab 106 191 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e How to Wr ite Psychology Research Repor ts and Essays scientist/practitioner model 9 style tutors see lecturers Scopus 12, 56, 57 journalistic 119 typing 30–1 search engines 14 writing 21–7 typographical errors 28–30 secondary sources 13, 39–41, 180 subjects see participants subscales 82, 179 undergraduate training 2 Seitz, A. 125 support 15–16 universities self-report measures 20, 82, 180 survey 180 semi-interquartile ranges 90 sentences essay writing at 5 library resources 12 tables 88, 93–6, 180 student counselling 65 incomplete 23 in appendices 106 unjustified conclusions 24 linking paragraphs with 122 examples of 94–5, 95–6 unpublished articles 46 numerals starting 92–3 introductions to 93, 94 unpublished theses 48 opening 72, 73 presentation of 94–5 unrepresentative samples 19, serif font 27, 31 titles of 94, 94 sexist language 28, 29 television programs 124 significance levels 90–1, 180 tense 27 slang 25 terms, definition of 74 social psychology 14 textbooks Society for Personality and Social Psychology 14 spacing 30–1 spelling 26 standard deviation 87–8, 90, 92 statistics 87–92, 180 104, 116, 117 URLs 48 variables 10, 180 demographic 80, 82 as references 11 dependent 59, 176 term definitions in 74 extraneous (nuisance) 103, 177 websites associated with 14 independent 58–9, 177 theory 17, 180 discussion based on 102 thesaurus 28 in Appendices 105 theses, unpublished 48 confidence intervals 91–2 time management 30, 65, 106 matrix of 58 voice 22 volume numbers 45 weak arguments 18 data analysis 89–90 essays 125 Web of Knowledge 12 descriptive 90, 176 extension 65, 177 Web of Science 12, 56 proofreading 64, 106 websites 48 see also online in Discussion section 100 effect sizes 91–2 Title Page 4, 68–70 resources inferential 90, 177 checklists for 110, 126 word limits 63–4, 68–70, 108, 122 presentation of see figures; for essays 113, 118, 119, 126 word meanings 23–4 example of 69 word-processing software 30 graphs; tables reporting 90, 91 significance levels 90–1, 180 titles words, versus numerals 92–3 capitalisation of 44, 68, 97 Writing essays and research Sternberg, R. J. 107 of essays 115 reports in the social straw man argument 18 of tables 94, 94 sciences (Betts, Seitz, & student counselling of journal articles 45, 69, 70 Farquharson) 125 departments 65 studies 180 topic of essay 113–14 writing process Truss, Lynne 24 essays 112 departmental 54–7 t-tests 90, 91 research reports 43, 52, 62–6, self-designed 57, 76–7 Turnitin 36, 55–6, study groups 8 65–6, 125, 180 107 writing style 21–7 192 Copyright © Pearson Australia (a division of Pearson Australia Group Pty Ltd) 2019 — 9781488618437 — Findlay & Kaufmann/How to Write Psychology Research Reports and Essays 8e This page intentionally left blank. This page intentionally left blank.