Conservative Protestant Child Discipline: The Case of Parental \elling* JOHN W. :P. BARTKOWSKI, Mississippi State University Princeton University BRADFORD WILCOX, Abstract Conservative Protestant child discipline has recently become thesubject ofconsiderable social research andpublic controversy. However, nosystematic empirical evidence has been brought tobear onconservative Protestant rates ofparental yelling, which weview as a keyindicator ofan authoritarian style ofparenting. We review parenting advice offered by conservative Protestant elites, who articulate child-rearing schemata grounded in both religious andpsychological rationales for the discipline ofyoungsters. Notably, conservative Protestant family specialists advocate corporal punishment while discouraging theparental use ofyelling at children. Data drawn from the 1987-88 National Survey of Families and Households (NSFH) indicate that conservative Protestant parents ofpreschoolers andschool-age children are significantly less likely to report yelling at their children. Moreover, the estimated effects of denominational affiliation on the parental use ofyelling are partly mediated byconservative theological views. We conclude by calling for research that analyzes the effects of the distinctive conservative Protestant approach todiscipline on child well-being. The distinctive disciplinary style of conservative Protestant parents has attracted considerable scholarly attention over the past several years (Bartkowski 1995; Bartkowski & Ellison 1995; Ellison 1996; Ellison & Bartkowski 1997; Ellison, Bartkowski & Segal 1996a, 1996b; Ellison & Sherkat 1993a, 1993b; Grasmick, Bursik & Kimpel 1991;Lienesch 1991;Wilcox 1998). This body of research suggests *Both authors, whose names are listed in alphabetical order, contributed equally to this article, whichwassupported in part by a grantfrom the Louisville Institute. Wegratefully acknowledge advice from RobertWuthnow and Xiaohe Xu, as well as comments offered by the anonymous reviewers. Please direct correspondence to ltv. Bradford Wilcox, Centerfor Research on Child Well-being, 221 Wallace Hall, Princeton University, Princeton, N] 08544. E-mail: wbwilcox@princeton.edu. © The University of North Carolina Press Social Forces, September 2000, 79(1):265-290 266 I Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 that conservative Protestants are more likely than other parents to value obedience from their children and to support the use of corporal punishment to discipline youngsters (Ellison & Sherkat 1993a, 1993b; Grasmick, Bursik & Kimpel 1991). Moreover, conservative Protestant parents are more likely to spank their preschool and school-age children (Ellison, Bartkowski & Segal 1996a, 1996b). Although there has been a great deal of research on the use of and support for corporal punishment among conservative Protestants, researchers have yet to examine one critical dimension of conservative Protestant discipline that might shed further light on child rearing within this religious subculture - verbal reproof (i.e., yelling). Our study, which examines religious variations in parental yelling at children, seeks to redress this gap in the scholarly literature on conservative Protestant child rearing. We focus on parents' patterns of yelling at children for three reasons. First, sociologists of the family and child-development specialists have begun to recognize that parental yelling is a key dimension of child discipline worthy of scholarly attention. Previous research suggests that yelling has a negative, independent effecton child well-being. Frequent yellingis linked to higher reported rates of antisocial behavior, lower levels of self-esteem, and psychopathology in children (Brenner & Fox 1998; Joubert 1991; Kazdin, Griest & Esveldt-Dawson 1984; Nagaraja 1984). Second, our study enables us to evaluate scholarship and popular commentary that has depicted conservative Protestant child discipline as an authoritarian form of parenting (cf. Baumrind 1971) consisting of harsh punishment, arbitrary assertions of power,and otherwise unresponsive child-rearing practices (Capps 1992, 1995; Gordon 1989;Greven 1990;Lifton & Strozier 1990;Maurer 1982;Maurer & Wallerstein 1980; for reviews, see Bartkowski 1995; Ellison 1996).1 We speak to this critical literature through the use of empirical data on patterns of parental yelling. If such portrayals of an authoritarian parenting style are indeed accurate, then the frequent use of corporal punishment among conservative Protestant parents (Ellison, Bartkowski & Segal 1996a, 1996b) should be accompanied by high relative rates of parental yelling and verbal intimidation. There may be some reason to anticipate such findings. Studies suggest that "power-assertive" disciplinary tactics such as corporal punishment and parental yelling are often highly correlated in practice (Parke & Slaby 1983;see also Hemenway, Solnick & Carter 1994;Kelley & Tsen 1992; Thomson, McLanahan & Curtin 1992). Moreover, a great deal of scholarship has linked this constellation of disciplinary tactics - marked by emotional outbursts, frequent recourse to corporal punishment, and minimal parental affect- to aversive developmental outcomes for children. Power-assertive discipline has been shown to increase the likelihood of antisocial behavior, poor scholastic performance, and emotional problems for developing youngsters (Baumrind 1971, 1997; Elder, Van Nguyen & Caspi 1985; Larzelere et al. 1989; Parke & Slaby 1983; Straus, Sugarman & Giles-Sims 1997). However, if we find Conservative Protestant Child Discipline / 267 that conservative Protestants are less likely to yell at their children despite their frequent use of corporal punishment, we would have some evidence that the disciplinary style in such homes is less authoritarian - and potentially less harmful- than critics of this religious subculture contend.' Finally, in turning our attention to religious variations in parental yelling, we extend a handful of studies that suggest that positive parental "emotion work" (cf. Hochschild 1979) is a key facet of conservative Protestant child rearing. Alongside the endorsements for corporal punishment featured within many conservative Protestant parenting manuals are equally strong affirmations for parental expressions of love, concern, and affection toward their young children (Bartkowski 1995; Wilcox 1998). Moreover, recent empirical research has revealed that conservative Protestant caregivers are significantly more likely than other parents to display positive, nurturant emotions toward their children (Wilcox 1998). Therefore, many of the same conservative Protestant parents who are more inclined to spank their children are also more likely to express love and affection toward them. In light of these empirical findings, we seek to assess the extent to which conservative Protestant parents may practice another kind of emotion work by regulating their own use of yelling as a means of child discipline. In exploring this new empirical domain of conservative Protestant parenting, we are guided by theoretical insights that reveal how family ideologies and practices are influenced by the interplay between schemata and resources - what Sewell (1992) calls the duality ofstructure (see also Sherkat 1998; Sherkat & Ellison 1997). Brietly, schemata are ideological frameworks that prescribe courses of appropriate action - in Sewell's terms, "recipes for group action." In the case of our study, the schemata of interest are the parenting ideals recommended by conservative Protestant family commentators as well as the child-rearing strategies embraced and employed by caregivers affiliated with this religious subculture. Our investigation, in part, aims to ascertain the degree of homology between these two forms of schemata - that is, the generation of elite conservative Protestant childrearing "recipes') as well as the consumption and implementation of such prescriptions by conservative religious parents. According to Sewell's (1992) framework, resources are cultural products or objects that actors with access to them can enlist "to enhance or maintain power" (9). Because resources are defined as meaningful within a particular cultural context, resource-rich actors are more capable of generating) disseminating, and legitimating schemata among group members. Sewell calls researchers' attention to different types of resources - including those that are animate versus inanimate in character - and links power to the accumulation of resources by particular factions of social actors. In the conservative Protestant context, the Bible serves as the key cultural resource for producing parenting schemata. However, this inanimate scriptural resource is made meaningful by animate networks of conservative Protestant interpretive authorities (i.e., leaders of national and local 268/ Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 family ministries) who disseminate "legitimate" scriptural interpretations and parenting schemata through their Christian child-rearing advice literature. Finally, Sewell (1992) argues that schemata are capable of being applied outside the social sphere in which they were originally generated. He terms this process the "transposability" of schemata. Hence, schemata that are apparently religious in character (e.g., theological beliefs that liken God to a parent) can have practical implications in other spheres ofsocial interaction (e.g.,parent-child relations within the home). Building on Sewell'sinsight, we contend below that the transposability of conservative Protestant religious child-rearing schemata from "virtual" pastoral discourse to «actual" family interaction is facilitated when these recipes for action meld rationales from both religious and nonreligious structures (namely, conservative theology and modern psychology). Accordingly, our investigation begins with a brief review of the ways in which elite conservative Protestant authors meld religious and psychological rationales into a coherent parenting schema that endorses corporal punishment while proscribing parental yelling. Next, we analyze data from the initial wave of the National Survey of Families and Households (NSFH-1) to examine the extent to which actual conservative Protestant caregivers adhere to the admonishments against parental yelling prescribed in conservative Christian child-rearing manuals. We conclude by outlining the implications of our study for scholarly understandings of conservative Protestant child discipline. The Religious and Psychological Determinants of Conservative Protestant Approaches to Parenting and Child Discipline Since the 1970s, conservative Protestantism has devoted increased pastoral and political attention to family life in an effort to shore up the «traditional" family, which is seen as a bulwark of religious faith and of the American way of life (Bartkowski 1997,2000; Bartkowski & Ellison 1995;Bartkowski, Wilcox & Ellison 2000; Bartkowski & Xu 2000; Ellison & Sherkat 1993a;Hunter 1987;Sherkat 2000; Wilcox 1998; Wuthnow 1988). Thousands of church and parachurch ministries have sprung up in response to this concerted focus on family living. For instance, the largest parachurch family ministry - James Dobson's Focus on the Familyoffers marriage and parenting schemata that reach an estimated 2.3 million people each month through its daily radio show (broadcast on more than 4,000 radio stations around the world), books, videos, and magazines. Thus, parachurch ministries such as Focus on the Family, as well as countless congregational ministries, provide resources - from books to support groups - that interact with their family-oriented schemata in ways that «mutually imply and sustain each other" (Sewell 1992:13). Conservative Protestant ChildDiscipline I 269 Despitethe explicitly religious nature of resources such as the Bible and faithbased social networks, the key to understanding conservative Protestant family schemata is to recognize how such ideologies at once engage and distinguish themselves from mainstreamAmerican culture (d. Smith 1998). A carefulreading of best-selling conservative Protestantfamily advice manualsreveals that this genre is produced at the intersectionof two differentstructures - namely, conservative Protestanttheologyand modern psychology. Given the "multiplicity of structures" that characterize contemporary social life (Sewell 1992: 16-17), the melding of religious and scientific structures into conservative Protestant family-oriented schemata increases the transposability of such ideologies and gives this religious subculture broader appeal than it might otherwiseenjoy. Tobegin,conservative Protestantfamily ministries drawfrom a fundamentalist tradition of biblical interpretationthat stresses "literal" understandings of scripture (Bartkowski & Ellison 1995; Ellison & Sherkat 1993a, 1993b). The interpretive strategies adopted by contemporary religious conservatives understand the Bible as not onlyinfallible regarding supernaturalmattersbut also inerrant wherehuman affairs and family relationships are concerned. Thus, conservative Protestantfamily ministries, as well as conservative Protestant theologians and pastors, draw heavily on the Bible - the primary religious resource in this subculture - to develop and disseminate schemata for familyliving that are readily distinguishable from mainstream American values. Conservative Protestant elitesgenerally insist that these schemata follow selfevidently from a plain readingof scripture. However, closer inspection reveals that elite interpretations of biblical texts are shaped by broader social and cultural trends - particularly the rise of modern psychology and therapeutic culture which these commentators selectively appropriate into their family manuals." On the one hand, the subjective, pietistictradition of conservative Protestantism has leftit open to what Rieff (1966) calls the"triumph of the therapeutic" - manifested most dearly in religious conservatives' appropriation of expressive and self-help psychology. This facet of conservative Protestantism is marked by a preoccupation with personalfulfillment and an expressive ethic of interpersonalrelations, usually shored up by religious language and imagery(Griffith 1997; Hunter 1987). On the other hand, the puritanicaland pragmaticroots of conservative Protestantism have made it particularly susceptible to a behaviorist approach to child rearing. Specifically, youngsters arethought to be bestsocialized through a stable, predictable system of punishments (e.g., physical discipline) and rewards (e.g., verbalpraise) (Lienesch 1991; Wilcox 1998). The ideological penetration of conservative Protestantism by modern psychology isalso evidenced in the fact that manypopular conservative Protestantfamily ministries are headed by trained psychologists (e.g., James Dobson, Gary Smalley, and John Trent). Even conservative Protestant elites whodo not boastacademic trainingintegrate the"theory" and"methods" of modern 270 I Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 psychology- for example, temperament theory and personality inventories - into their advice manuals for the edification of their Christian readership (see, e.g., LaHaye 1977; Littauer 1994). Elite conservative Protestants' melding of psychological and religious meaning systems is, in turn, supported by a wide array of pastoral resources generated from within this religious subculture. Such resources include Christian counseling services, books, videos, and educational institutions (most Christian colleges and seminaries have large programs in psychology). These resources and the distinctive family-oriented schemata they produce are then transposed into conservative Protestant domestic lifethrough the network of grassroots ministries and local clergy serving conservative religious parents. All of these experts - from elite to local conservative religious authorities - give family and parenting issues a prominent place in their preaching and counseling (Ellison & Sherkat 1993a; Hunter 1987; Wilcox 1998). In what follows, we rely on a sample of family advice manuals written by leading conservative Protestants to convey the general tenor of these parenting schemata, with special attention to elite commentators' proscriptions against yelling. We also seek to reveal how these manuals creatively legitimate a distinctive orientation toward child discipline by melding together "recipes for group action" from two otherwise competing structural sources - conservative theology and modern psychology. AGAINST VERBAL REPROOF: ELITE CONSERVATIVE PROTESTANT OPPOSITION TO PARENTAL YELLING Conservative Protestant child-rearing specialists are virtually unanimous in discouraging the use ofyelling or verbal intimidation as a means of child discipline (Christenson 1970; Dobson 1978, 1992; Fugate 1980; LaHaye 1977; Swindoll 1977).4 Conservative Protestant parenting experts who construe yelling as an illegitimate disciplinary strategy do so on two religious grounds. First, drawing on biblical passages that stress parental authority (Exod. 20:12;Eph. 6:1-2; 1 Tim. 3:45), these commentators argue that parents must demonstrate a spirit of self-control in their child-rearing style.Yelling, these specialists contend, tells the child that the parent has lost control of his or her faculties and that the youngster has emerged victorious from a familial conflict. Consequently, conservative religious childrearing experts view the parental use of yelling as a threat to the family's divinely ordained authority structure. According to these commentators, the youngster perceives yelling as a sign that the ill-tempered parent has become unworthy of the child's respect and obedience (Christenson 1970; Dobson 1978; Fugate 1980; LaHaye 1977). Richard Fugate - widely considered to be one of the most ardent conservative Protestant supporters of parental authority, child obedience, and the corporal punishment ofyoungsters - is highly critical of verbal outbursts on the part of the parent. Fugate (1980) argues: "Chastisement [i.e.,punishment] is not a Conservative Protestant Child Discipline I 271 tongue lashing, threats, or screamingfits of anger; in other words, adult temper tantrums.These [attempts at verbal intimidation] do nothingbut support the child's disrespect for his parents' authorityand demonstratethe parents' inability to rule" (154). Utilizing a second form of religious reasoning, conservative Protestant parenting specialists argue that yelling violates the divinely ordained dignity of youngsters. Parents are supposedto treat their childrenwith respectboth because their children are made in the image of God (Ps. 139:13-16) and becausethey are charged with representing God to their children (Christenson 1992; Dobson 1978; LaHaye 1977; Swindoll1977). According to these authors, parents who yell and use verbal intimidation tactics are unable to model for their own youngsters the love that God has for his children (Dobson 1978; LaHaye 1977; Swindoll1977). Thus, yelling is construed by these child-rearing specialists as a moral affront to the child, who may be emotionally and spiritually damaged from such abusive encounters. Connecting themes of verbal intimidation with the emotional abuse of children, popular conservative Protestant spokesman Chuck Swindoll (1977) remarks,((I sayit again that it breaks my heart to think of how many children are whipped blackand blue with a sharp, stingingtongue" (94). Thesesame child-rearing specialists also oppose the parental use of yelling on threegrounds strongly related to the penetration of modem psychological and childdevelopment schemata into contemporary conservative Protestantism. First, relying on a behaviorist, pragmatic approachto childrearing, thesefamily specialists argue that the parental use of yelling does not work (Christenson 1970; Dobson 1992; LaHaye 1977). Many conservative Protestant parenting specialists contend that verbal intimidation is,quite simply, ineffective at eliciting behavioralcompliance from youngsters. According to these specialists, controlled corporal punishment administered immediately in the face of willful defiance is a superior - and, indeed, more effective - alternative to yelling. In this pragmatic vein, James Dobson (1992) contends: "Parents often use anger to get action instead of using action [spanking] to get action [compliance] .... Trying to control children by screamingis as utterly futile as trying to steer a car by honking the horn" (36). Second, many conservative Protestant parenting experts contend that verbal intimidation is a dangerous first step on the slippery slopeto physical child abuse. In the minds of many of these authors, yelling- rather than the judicious, deliberate use of corporal punishment - indicates an unhealthy escalation of parent-child conflict. When combined with the high likelihood of child noncompliance to verbalreproof,yelling is believed to increase the chances that a heated exchange may end in the physical abuse of the youngster by his or her frustrated parent. It is in this vein that conservative Protestant parenting experts articulate grave warnings against punishing children in anger. Beverly LaHaye (1977) warnsher readers to "firstgetvictoryovertheir own angerand hot tempers" before punishing a child, and she continues, "[Hot-tempered] parents need to 272 I Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 confess that angry spiritto God and askfor helpto change" (147). Gravitating deftly between psychological and religious admonitions against yelling, LaHaye also quotes a biblical passage designedto encouragea controlled parental responseto childmisbehavior: "He who is slowto anger is better than the mighty, and he who ruleshis spirit [is better] than he who capturesa city"(Prov. 16:32). And whileshe clearly endorses physical punishment, LaHaye (1977) strongly admonishes against administering such forms of discipline afteran angryescalation in conflict: "There are rightand wrongspankings. A wrongspanking wouldbe a cruel, sadistic beating that is given in a rage" (145). Finally, conservative Protestant experts oppose yelling on the expressive grounds that it harms the child's self-esteem. In their view, parentsmust be careful to respect the child's spirit; consequently, they should avoid any harsh verbal remarks that would do irreparable damage to that spirit (Dobson 1978, 1992; LaHaye 1977; Swindoll1977). Dobson (1978) counsels parents to avoid saying anything that implies that the "child is unwanted, unnecessary, foolish, ugly, dumb, [or] a burden" (78). laHaye (1977) quotesa psychiatrist who insists that, in a disciplinary situation, parents not give voice to disapproval of the child, onlythe child's behavior. Therefore, despite the fact that they advocate the controlled use of corporal punishment, conservative Protestant parenting specialists drawtogether forms of moralreasoning associated with conservative religion and modern psychology to admonishparents against the use of yelling or verbal reproof to discipline their maturing children. Data, Guiding Hypotheses, and Key Variables Guided by our review of conservative Protestant child-rearingspecialists' advice on the parental use of yelling, we now proceed to examine several different hypotheses using data from wave 1 of the National Survey of Families and Households (NSFH-1).TheNSFH-1 isa cross-sectional national probability sample of 13,017 adult men and women livingin the contiguous United States. Primary respondents completed an in-person interview as well as a self-administered questionnaire. Our subsample is restricted to two subsets of the overallNSFH-1 primary respondent sample: parents who have at leastone preschool child under the ageof five (N = 1,051) and parents with at leastone childagedfive to eighteen (N = 3,199).5 The statistics and analyses used for this study are based on weighted data to adjust for NSFH-1 oversamples of minorities, single-parentfamilies, and families with stepchildren. Our analyses of these data enableus to assess the following hypotheses: First,taking conservative Protestant affiliation as an indicator of respondents' exposure to the family-oriented schemata and resources found in this subculture, wepredictthat parentsclaiming a conservative Protestantaffiliation willbe less likelythan other caregivers to yellat their children. Conservative Protestant Child Discipline I 273 Second, given that much of the advice against yelling articulated in conservative Protestant child-rearing literature is grounded in this subculture's distinctive use of religious resources such as the Bible, we predict that theological conservatism (gauged by respondents' attitudes about the inerrancy of the Bible and beliefs that the Bible provides answers to human problems) will also be linked to lower levels of parental yelling. Third, given the conservative Protestant recourse to corporal punishment, we seek to determine the extent to which conservative Protestant effects on yelling are mediated by rates of corporal punishment (which has been associated with parental yelling in the general population). Fourth, we test the competing hypothesis that religious variations in yelling are an artifact of distinctively low female labor-force participation patterns within conservative Protestant families. We do so because the literature on conservative Protestantism suggests that mothers in this subculture, especially mothers of preschool children, are less likely to participate in the labor force (Ammerman & Roof 1995;Bartkowski 1999;Sherkat 2000). Moreover, previous research on conservative Protestant parenting suggests that female labor-force patterns are related to distinctive patterns of parenting (Wilcox 1998). Fifth, because conservative Protestant parents are encouraged to spank their children and refrain from yelling at their youngsters, we also test the hypothesis that the conservative Protestant parents who spank their children are significantly less likely to yell at their youngsters - compared with other parents who spank their children at similar rates. Parents surveyed for the NSFH-1 were asked several questions about how they interact with their children, including one item inquiring how often they "yell at [their] child." Response categories to this frequency-of-yelling item include the following: never (1), seldom (2), sometimes (3), and very often (4). Therefore, we treat the parental use ofyelling as a continuous dependent variable. The placement of this item on the actual survey (proximate to questions about the parental use of corporal punishment as well as physical and verbal expressions of affection) gives us confidence in its face validity and serves to limit the possibility for response bias (Joubert 1991; Sudman & Bradburn 1982). Many of our predictor and control variables follow closelyfrom previous NSFH1 analyses of religious differences in child discipline and parent-child interaction (Ellison, Bartkowski & Segal 1996a, 1996b; Wilcox 1998). Conservative Protestant affiliation is determined by a dummy variable that identifies respondents affiliated with the following groups: Southern Baptist, fundamentalist Baptist, Assembly of God, Missionary Alliance, Church of the Brethren, Evangelical Free Church, Pentecostal, Holiness, and other fundamentalist or evangelical churches (see Kellstedt & Green 1993; Roof & McKinney 1987). Consistent with the extant 274/ Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 research on conservative Protestant discipline) we do not includemeasures for other religious affiliations (e.g., mainline Protestant) Catholic) Iewish)." Our measure of theological conservatism is composed of a two-item index (r = .74) P < .001) Cronbach's alpha = .85)measuredby the respondents) agreement with the following statements: (1) "The Bible is God's word and everything happened or willhappen exactly as it says," and (2) "The Bible is the answerto all important human problems."We use the mean score from the responsesto each item (from strongly disagree [1] to strongly agree [5]) to measure theologicalconservatism. Parentswere alsoaskedhow often they "spank or slap [their] child," Responses (never [1]) seldom [2]) sometimes [3]) or very often [4]) were used to measure frequency of corporal punishment. We use this measure to indicate the broader disciplinary contextwithin whichparentsmayyell or) conversely) refrainfrom doing so. We control for a variety of variables that previous research indicates might otherwise confound the effect of religious involvement on parental yelling (see) e.g.) Ellison) Bartkowski & Segal 1996a) 1996b): respondent's gender (l = female), age of respondent (in years), race/ethnicity of parent (l = black) 1 = Hispanic) 0= white/Anglo), primary respondent's marital status (l = singleparent), parent's education (from some high school [1] to Ph.D. [6]))total household income (in tens of thousands of 1986 dollars) logged), and)for female respondents) their laborforce participation (l = mother works full-time [35 hours or more per week] or part-time [1-34 hours per week], 0 = mother not employed). Wealsocontrol) where appropriate) for the number of preschool children (birth through four years) and total children (birth to eighteen years) in the household. Since our dependent variable is not measured with referenceto a particular focalchild)we rely on two sets of measures to control for biologicalties within the family (1 = all biological children or a blend of biologicaland step- or adopted children)0 = all step- and/ or adopted children) and the genders of children in the family (1 = all male or both genders) 0 = all female). Findings Descriptive statistics for all variables used in this study are provided in Table 1. Of particular note are the means presentedfor our dependent variable - the reported parental use of yelling. On average) parents of preschool children report yelling at their youngsters a bit less frequently than parents of school-age children. Nevertheless) the mean rates of yellingfor both of these subsamples lie near the middleof the continuum for our dependentvariable - that is) somewhere between "seldom" and "sometimes." Among parents of preschool children) 180/0 report a conservative Protestant denominational affiliation; 21% of the parents of schoolage children are affiliated with a conservative Protestant denomination. Conservative Protestant Child Discipline I 275 Table 2 presents zero-order religious differences in the parental use of yelling. For parents of preschool children only, Table 2 provides tentative support for our first and second hypotheses. Mean differences in yelling among parents of preschool children (column 1) are marginally significant (p < .10), such that parents who affiliate with conservative Protestant denominations and embrace inerrantist theological views (with average scores of 4 or higher on our index of theological conservatism) report lower overall rates ofverbal reproof. We note more robust mean differences (p < .05) in reported rates of parental yelling among caregivers of school-age children (column 2), such that parents affiliated with a conservative Protestant denomination and theologically conservative respondents report utilizing verbal reproof significantly less than other respondents in our sample. Thus, for parents of school-age children, Table 2 provides stronger support for our first and second hypotheses. However, we can be confident of our findings only after controlling for a range of potentially confounding sociodemographic predictors of parental yelling. We rely on ordinary least squares (OLS) regression to estimate the effects of religious affiliation, theological conservatism, and various control variables on the parental use of yelling. In Table 3, we report the likelihood of parents yelling at their preschool children. Model 1 indicates that parents who are affiliated with a conservative Protestant denomination are significantly less likely to yell at their preschool children than are nonconservative Protestant parents. The findings in this model support our first hypothesis, which links conservative Protestant affiliation to lower likelihoods of parental yelling. Model 2 estimates the effect of theological conservatism on the frequency of parental yelling. This model demonstrates that theologically conservative parents are significantly less likely to use verbal reproof in disciplining their preschool children. This finding lends support to our second hypothesis by revealing that theological conservatism is related to a significantly lower incidence of yelling among parents of preschool children. The addition of theological conservatism to the model also reduces to insignificance the net effect of denominational affiliation on our dependent variable. This result suggests that the denominational effect is indirect for parents of preschoolers, operating primarily through this subculture's use of religious resources - specifically, scriptural passages that are used to discourage (and, it would seem, actually lessen) parents' use of yelling. Models 3 and 4 explore the possible mediating effects of corporal punishment and female labor-force participation rates, respectively, on the relationship between our respondents' religious involvement and their reported use of yelling. Corporal punishment is positively and significantly related to parents' use of yelling.Notably, the inclusion of corporal punishment as an indicator of the disciplinary context within homes more than triples our model fit. This pronounced increase in model fit suggests an especiallyclose linkage between parents' use of corporal punishment and yellingas means of disciplining their preschool children. However,the addition 276 I Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 TABLE 1: Means and Standard Deviations ofVariables Dependent variable Frequencyof yelling(continuous) Religious factors ConservativeProtestant affiliation Theological conservatism Disciplinarycontext Corporalpunishment Parent and householdcharacteristics Female Age Black Hispanic Singleparent Education Householdincome (logged) Preschoolchildren (aged0-4) Children Aged1-4 (Only) Children Aged5-18 2.39 (.85) 2.67 (.80) .18 (.38) 2.82 (1.13) 2.92 (1.09) 2.17 (.85) 1.81 (.79) .60 (049) 28.21 (5.79) .10 (.30) .08 (.28) .17 (.38) 2.56 (1.26) .22 (2.54) 1.38 (.58) .57 (.50) 38.27 (8.07) .13 (.34) .09 (.29) .16 (.37) 2.38 (1.21) 045 (2045) Children (aged0-18) Mother works full- time" Mother workspart-time" Familycharacteristics Allbiological Blend .20 (040) .09 (.29) .96 (.19) .01 (.06) .21 (AI) 2.13 (1.08) .24 (042) .10 (.30) .83 (.37) .04 (.18) Conservative Protestant Child Discipline / 277 TABLE 1: Means and StandardDeviations of Variables (Continued) Children Aged 1-4 (Only) Family characteristics (cont'd) Allmale Bothgenders a .42 (.49) .26 (.44) .21 (.40) (.50) 1,051 N Children Aged 5-18 .50 3,199 Refers to female respondentswhoareworking of corporal punishment does not change the direction or significance of religious effectson parental yellingat preschool children. Nevertheless,in line with hypothesis 3, the strength of theological conservatism increases once we control for corporal punishment, which suggests that theologically conservative parents of preschool children are even less likelyto yell at their children after controlling for their greater propensity to spank. Model 4 indicates that mothers who work in the labor force are more likely to yell at their preschool children. It also reveals that the inverse relationship between theological conservatism and yelling is reduced to marginal significance (compared to model 2) when we add mothers' work-force participation rates into our model. In line with hypothesis 4, this finding suggests that lower rates of yelling among theologically conservative parents are partly an effect of lower labor-force participation rates among conservative Protestant mothers of preschool children. To test hypothesis 5, we divided the full sample into two subsamples - one consisting of conservative Protestant parents and the other of nonconservative Protestant respondents. We used this methodological strategy to determine if conservative Protestants who spank their preschool children are significantly less likely to yell at them when compared to other parents who spank their children at similar rates. In so doing, we were able to ascertain if spanking is less predictive of yelling for conservative Protestants than for other parents. Slope tests of corporal punishment coefficients revealed no significant differences between the subsamples (models are available upon request). This finding suggests that increased rates of corporal punishment among parents of preschool children are associated with increased rates of parental yelling without regard for religious tradition. Turning briefly to our control variables, the models in Table 3 generally indicate that Hispanic caregivers and more educated parents report yelling at their children less frequently.Mothers are somewhat more likelyto yell at their youngsters, though this relationship is no longer significant when we control for women's labor-force 278 I Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 TABLE 2: Religious Differences in Parental )flling Means Religious affiliation Conservative Protestant Preschool Children School-Age Children 2.29+ 2.56* (.81) 2.68 (.79) (.84) Nonconservative Protestant 2.40 (.86) Religious ideology Theologicallyconservative Nontheologically conservative 2.32+ (.87) 2.57* (.81) 2.41 2.70 (.84) (.78) 1,051 N 3,199 Note: Standarddeviations arein parentheses. + p < .10 * P < .05 (differentfrom homogamous nonconservative couples) participation. Finally,yelling is more likely to be reported in those families with a greater number of preschool children. Next, we estimate the effects of our predictor and control variables on the frequency of yelling among parents who have at least one school-age child aged five to eighteen. Table 4 lends further credence to our first hypothesis, as model 1 indicates that parents affiliated with a conservative Protestant denomination are less likely than their counterparts to yell at their school-age children. As with parents ofpreschoolers, the addition of theologicalconservatism (model 2) produces a significant negative effect on the frequency ofyelling. Our second hypothesis is again confirmed, although the findings on this score tell a somewhat different story for parents of school-age children. The effects of these parents' affiliation with a conservative Protestant denomination remain significant despite the addition of theological conservatism to this model. 7 Model 3 indicates, once again, that corporal punishment is positivelyassociated with yelling among parents of school-age children. Moreover, the inclusion of corporal punishment more than doubles our model fit, which once again highlights the close linkage between corporal punishment and yelling for parents in general. For this group of parents, however, the addition of corporal punishment does not alter the direction or overall strength of the effect associated with conservative denominational affiliation or theological conservatism. Mothers' work patterns in this group (model 4) do not produce significant effects on the dependent variable and, for that matter, do not cause noteworthy changes in our model (when compared with model 2). These findings for mothers ConservativeProtestant Child Discipline /279 of school-age children contrast with the results of analyses of mothers of preschoolers, in which the latter's labor-force participation reduces the significance of theological conservatism in predicting yelling. The consistent significance of conservative Protestant affiliation across all of our models in Table 4 suggests that religious affiliation exerts a more enduring influence on the parents of school-age children. To test hypothesis 5, we again divided the full sample into two subsamples one conservative Protestant and the other nonconservative Protestant - to see if spanking is less predictive ofyelling for conservative Protestant parents of schoolage children than for other parents. Slope tests of corporal punishment coefficients indicated no significant differences between the subsamples (models are available upon request). Thus, once again, increased rates of corporal punishment are associated with increased rates of parental yelling without regard for religious tradition. Several other covariates in Table 4 also are related to statistically significantly differences in parental yelling. Mothers are more likely to yell at their youngsters, as are single parents. Also, parents with more children and!or all biological children are significantly more likely to yell at their children. By contrast, older parents of school-age children, Hispanic parents, and those with greater educational capital are less likely to report yelling at their youngsters. Discussion We began this study by noting that the distinctiveness of conservative Protestant child discipline has led some scholars, child-rearing specialists, and social commentators to conclude that conservative Protestant parenting is marked by arbitrary assertions of power and by harsh, abusive punishments (e.g., Capps 1992, 1995; Gordon 1989; Greven 1990; Lifton & Strozier 1990; Maurer 1982; Maurer & Wallerstein 1980; for reviews of such critiques, see Bartkowski 1995; Ellison 1996). Among our other aims in this study, we sought to evaluate such portrayals of conservative Protestant child discipline by examining religious variations in parental yelling. Our analyses of empirical data concerning this important dimension of child discipline indicate that these portrayals of conservative Protestant parenting are in need of modification. Specifically, our study suggests that the disciplinary style of conservative Protestant parents is not as authoritarian as has often been charged. Conservative Protestant parents, especially those embracing theologically conservative beliefs, are less likely than other parents to yell at their children. However, our investigation provides no evidence that the conservative Protestant parents who use corporal punishment are particularly less likely to yell at their children than are parents who use corporal punishment at similar rates. Our study, combined with recent work that reveals conservative 280 I Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 TABLE 3: OL5 Regression Models for ~lling among Parents with Children Aged 0-4 (Only) Religious factors Conservative Protestant (1) (2) -.146* (.068) -.111 (.070) -.096 (.059) -.110 (.069) -.049* (.024) -.127*** (.021) -.047+ (.024) Theological conservatism Disciplinary context Corporalpunishment Parentand household characteristics Female (3) (4) .551*** (.026) .144** (.055) .148** (.055) .093* (.047) .034 (.066) Age .008 (.005) .007 (.005) .009* (.005) .007 (.005) Black -.083 (.089) -.065 (.089) -.002 (.075) -.086 (.089) Hispanic -.395*** (.093) .113 (.079) -.090*** (.024) .014 (.012) -.373*** (.093) .114 (.079) -.096*** (.024) -.202* (.079) .149* (.067) -.055** (.020) -.366*** (.093) .098 (.079) -.100*** (.024) .014 (.012) .012 (.010) .005 (.012) Single parent Education Household income (logged) Motherworkspart-time" .282** (.096) Motherworksfull- time" .183* (.075) Family characteristics Preschool children .431*** (.056) .437*** (.056) .244*** (.048) .453*** (.056) Allbiological -.169 (.140) -.169 (.140) -.052 (.141) -.184 (.139) Blend -.537 (.427) -.571 (.427) -.296 (.366) -.600 (.425) Allmale .076 (.057) .078 (.057) .072 (.048) .073 (.056) Bothgenders .059 (.085) .062 (.084) .034 (.071) .055 (.084) Conservative Protestant Child Discipline I 281 TABLE 3: OLS Regression Models for ~lling among Parents with Children Aged 0-4 (Only) (Continued) Intercept (1) (2) (3) (4) 1.851 H* 2.666*H 1.046H* 2.048 H* (.221) (.233) (.223) (.232) .107 .109 .372 .117 1,051 1,051 1,047 1,051 Adjusted R' N Note: Standarderrors arein parentheses. a Refers to female respondentswhoareworking + p < .10 * P < .05 H P < .01 *** P < .001 Protestants are more likely than other parents to praise and hug their children (Wilcox 1998), leads us to wonder if the unique cultural context of conservative Protestant parenting may mitigate some of the adverse outcomes typically associated with corporal punishment. This hypothesis deserves further scholarly investigation. A growing body of research on child discipline now suggests that contextual factors - that is, the circumstances under which discipline and punishment are administered - often act to mediate the relationship between disciplinary tactics and child outcomes. This literature on parental discipline suggests that the restrained use of corporal punishment - when combined with reasoned explanations and a warm, responsive style of parenting - may not necessarily be harmful to children (Larzelere et al. 1989, 1996, 1998; Simons, Johnson & Conger 1994; for reviews, see Baumrind 1997; Larzelere 1996). The importance of disciplinary context is further underscored by research that revealsvariations in the effectsof corporal punishment byethnicity, family structure, and the age of the punished child. In one recent study (Gunnoe & Mariner 1997), corporal punishment produced no discernible negative outcomes for black children and youngsters aged four to seven; and, while physical punishment was shown to be harmful for white children aged eight to eleven, these effects were eliminated by controlling for household structure (with physically punished children of single parents at greater risk of negative outcomes). When interpreted in light of our findings, these studies underscore the need for future research to examine how the context of conservative Protestant parenting may mitigate some of the negative effects associated with conservative Protestant caregivers' propensity to rely on corporal punishment. 282 I Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 TABLE 4: OLS Regression Models for ~lling among Parents with Children Aged 5-18 Religious factors Conservative Protestant (1) (2) (3) (4) -.152*** (.035) -.118** (.036) -.131*** (.035) -.119** (.036) -.046** (.014) -.047*** (.013) -.046** (.014) Theological conservatism Disciplinary context Corporalpunishment Parentand household characteristics Female .310*** (.018) .070* (.029) .078** (.029) .073** (.028) .080* (.038) Age -.012*** (.002) -.012*** (.002) -.003 (.002) -.012*** (.002) Black -.017 (.044) -.004 (.044) -.044 (.043) -.004 (.044) Hispanic -.381*** (.050) .109** (.042) -.367*** (.050) -.369*** (.051) .102* (.042) -.325*** (.049) .111** (.040) Education -.019 (.012) -.027* (.012) -.025* (.012) -.026* (.012) Household income(logged) .007 (.006) .006 (.006) .012* (.006) .006 (.006) Single parent .099* (.042) Motherworkspart-time" -.006 (.052) Motherworksfull- time" -.003 (.042) Family characteristics Children .081*** (.018) .085*** (.018) .032* (.015) .068*** (.015) Allbiological .108** (.037) .110** (.037) .095** (.035) .117** (.037) Blend .126 (.086) .119 (.086) .083 (.083) .128 (.086) Allmale -.028 (.037) -.025 (.037) -.055 (.036) -.026 (.037) Bothgenders .056 (.037) .057 (.037) .035 (.035) .066 (.036) Conservative Protestant Child Discipline / 283 TABLE 4: OLS Regression Models for Yelling among Parents with Children Aged 5-18 (Continued) (1) (2) (3) (4) Intercept 2.915*** (.106) 3.038*** (.112) 2.310*** (.115) 3.072*** (.110) Adjusted R' N .067 3,199 .070 3,199 .146 3,178 .069 3,199 Note: Standarderrors are in parentheses. a Refers to female respondentswhoareworking ,.. p < .05 ** P < .01 *** P < .001 Conclusion Our study illuminates contextual issues of child discipline that have sustained the interest of sociologists of the family for some time. A great deal of sociological research has compared the contours of authoritarian and authoritative parenting styles. Our investigation has taken verbal reproof (i.e, parental yelling) as an indicator of the kind of erratic and irrational form of discipline flagged by Baumrind (1997) and others as potentially harmful to children. At the same time, we have noted that the extant research on conservative Protestant child discipline has led some scholars and parenting commentators (e.g., Capps 1992; Maurer 1982; Maurer & Wallerstein 1980) to assume that conservative Protestant caregivers engage in authoritarian or abusive parenting. In an effort to evaluate such assumptions, we analyzed conservative Protestant child-rearing discourse and practice regarding parental yelling. Our analyses of NSFH -1 data were guided by recent theoretical insights that examine the interplay between "schemata" (i.e., transposable recipes for social action) and "resources" (culturally meaningful objects or products used to legitimate and disseminate normative schemata within distinct social groups) (Sewell 1992; Sherkat 1998; Sherkat & Ellison 1997). This perspective attuned us to the various structural rationales (i.e., conservative interpretations of the Bible, selective tenets from expressive and behaviorist psychology) that leading religious conservatives utilize to produce parenting schemata that are at once distinct from and engaged with contemporary American parenting standards. More specifically, our analysis of best-selling conservative Protestant childrearing manuals revealedthat - for both theological and psychologicalreasons the same conservative Protestant parenting specialists who enthusiastically endorse the corporal punishment of youngsters actuallyoppose the use of yellingas a means of disciplining children. Mindful that the discourse of conservative Protestant 284 I Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 parenting might contradict the actual practice of child rearing in such homes, we then turned our attention to national survey data that gauge the frequency with which parents yell at their children to discipline them. Our findings indicate that parents affiliated with a conservative Protestant denomination are significantly less likely to report yelling at their preschoolers and school-age children when compared with their nonconservative Protestant counterparts. Our findings also reveal that theological conservatism mediates this relationship between conservative Protestant denominational affiliation and frequency of yellingfor parents of both preschoolers and school-age children (more so for the former than the latter). Our study has important implications for research on child discipline and highlights several fruitful avenues for future research. First, our findings suggest that emotionally controlled child discipline may be part of a distinctive conservative Protestant orientation toward child rearing. It seems that parents from this subculture discipline their children in a substantively different fashion than other parents. The schemata that underlie this emotionally controlled child discipline, as well as the conservative Protestant cultural resources that support this orientation (e.g., religious ideologies, faith-based social networks), seem well worth pursuing in light of our findings. Consequently, as scholars begin to explore subcultural variations in disciplinary decision-making processes, we urge them to consider parents' use of yelling as an important - yet heretofore understudieddimension of child discipline.f Second, as noted previously in this study, we are left to wonder if the lower rates of yelling found here among conservative Protestants may counteract some of the negative effects of more frequent corporal punishment in this subculture. Although a great deal of previous research has linked the use of corporal punishment in general with short-term and long-term negative child outcomes (e.g., increased aggression, poor academic performance, delinquent behavior) (Straus 1994), the distinctive aspects of conservative Protestant child discipline highlighted both in previous research (Bartkowski 1995; Wilcox 1998) and in our investigation might buffer the otherwise adverse effects of physical punishment in conservative Protestant homes. Although we have seen no published study to date that has examined the potentially distinctive effects of child discipline (and specifically corporal punishment) in conservative Protestant homes, this issue should become a top priority for researchers interested in cultural variations in child discipline. To the degree that researchers can take into account the distinctiveness associated with other aspects of conservative Protestant child discipline (including lower relative rates of parental yelling), such inquiries may help us adjudicate the controversies that currently surround conservative Protestant parenting. Using the concepts of schemata, resources, and social practice, our analysis has sought to advance sociologicalunderstandings of the ways in which factors such as elite discourse, theological convictions, and child-rearing ideals inform family Conservative Protestant Child Discipline I 285 interaction within conservative Protestant homes (d. Bartkowski 1995, 1997, 1999; McNamara 1985a, 1985b; Sherkat 1998, 2000). Yet it is important to recognize that structures, schemata, and cultural resources are not static entities but rather lend themselves to "transformation" and social change. Indeed, Sewell (1992) argues that social transformations often emerge because: (1) divergent structures can be melded to produce new social forms; (2) schemata are often altered when transposed to a new social context; and (3) the accumulation of resources - and the emergence of newfound resource bases - by social actors is an unpredictable process. It is with this insight in mind that we call attention to the fact that the child-rearing advice and theological justifications articulated in conservative Protestant parenting manuals are consumed, evaluated, and implemented by members of a religious subculture that is itself undergoing noteworthy transformations. Conservative Protestants are increasinglylikelyto enjoy high levels of human and cultural capital (i.e., greater educational achievement, rising occupational status) (see Hunter 1987;Wuthnow & Lawson 1994), and previous research reveals how structural changes occurring within this religious subculture have altered many of its members' views on social, political, and familial issues (Bartkowski 1998; Hunter 1983, 1987; Schmalzbauer 1993; Shibley 1996; Smith 1998). As research on this topic continues to add depth to scholarly portraits of conservative Protestant parents, we urge social scientists to explore the full gamut of dynamics - cultural and structural, ideological and practical, traditional and progressive - that converge to produce what might best be described as a "neotraditional," though not necessarily"authoritarian:' parenting style within this distinctive religious subculture. Notes 1. Donald Capps (1992) advanced these very claims in a provocative presidential address to the Society for the ScientificStudy of Religion, titled "Religion and Child Abuse: Perfect Together" (see also Capps 1995). Drawing on biographical accounts of conservative Protestant leaders such as James Dobson and Billy Graham, Capps (1992) argued that conservative Protestant parents have taken the notion of "breaking the will" of the child as a license to "inflict severe physical punishment, usually with a leather belt or hickory stick, and sometimes before the child is even able to crawl" (3). Capps and other critics of conservative Protestant parenting are deeply concerned that child discipline within such homes is harsher, more arbitrary, and hence potentially more harmful than the forms of discipline favored by nonconservative Protestant parents. 2. This possibility is not altogether unfathomable. Despite the fact that some sociologists continue to conflate the use of corporal punishment with authoritarian parenting, Diana Baumrind (1997) has recently noted that virtually all of the parents in her classic parenting study - including the caregivers she categorized as authoritative - used corporal punishment. Moreover, recent studies (see Baumrind 1997;Larzelere 1996) reveal that a variety of contextual factors may mitigate the negative child outcomes typically 286 / Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 associated with corporal punishment (Hotaling, Straus & Lincoln 1990; Straus 1994). Such research underscores the importance of conceptualizing child discipline as a multidimensional construct and highlights the need to ascertain the household context within which discipline is administered. 3. Our reference to elite conservative Protestants' selective appropriation of modern psychology is not necessarily at odds with other research that has revealed the greater inclination for rank-and-file religious conservatives to be suspect of scientific epistemologies and advanced educational pursuits (with the latter acting as a conduit for the dissemination of the former) (Darnell & Sherkat 1997; Ellison & Musick 1995). We argue here that elite religious conservatives bolster their prospective readership and negotiate a collectiveidentity by strategically engaging (i.e., simultaneously rejecting and invoking) various aspects of modern psychology on child rearing. These authors are, of course, careful to enlist psychological approaches that affirm their primary epistemological framework of scriptural inerrancy. 4. The manuals selected and analyzed for this study are written by best-selling conservative Protestant advice authors who are highly visiblewithin conservative religious circles. Many of these same manuals have been examined in previous studies that focus on conservative Protestant parents' use of corporal punishment and their propensity for affective child rearing (e.g., Bartkowski 1995; Ellison, Bartkowski & Segal 1996a; Ellison & Sherkat 1993a;Wilcox 1998). However, previous scholarship has not analyzed these authors' advice on the subject of parental yelling. 5. We divide the sample into subsets of parents of preschool children (only) and schoolage children because the NSFH poses parenting questions separately to these two groups. 6. Ancillary analyses (results not shown) revealed that the addition of other religious affiliation measures into our models does not affect the direction or the significance of the conservative Protestant coefficient. These analysesdemonstrate that (1) other religious affiliations are insignificantly related to yelling among parents of preschool children, and (2) Catholic and Jewish affiliations are positively related to yelling among parents of school-age children. 7. Why would denominational affiliation have pronounced significant inverse effects on the use of yelling for parents of school-age children but not for parents of preschool children? Perhaps parents of school-age children are more tightly integrated into religious networks that discourage using verbal reproof or intimidation as a means of discipline. If these parents are more tightly integrated into such networks, their child-rearing behavior might be more closely monitored (and more subject to social sanctions) than that of parents who avoid yelling purely on theological grounds. Alternatively, these discrepant findings could stem from the disparate number of cases across our two samples of parents. Our sample of parents of school-age youngsters is over three times greater than that for preschoolers. 8. Using a socialinformation processing model popular in child-development scholarship, Ellison (1996) has hypothesized that conservative Protestant caregivers may engage in culturally specificdecision-making processesduring the course of parent-child disciplinary encounters. According to this model, social information is assimilated via four successive cognitive processes: (1) appraisal (during which child behaviors are apprehended and Conservative Protestant Child Discipline I 287 categorized), (2) response search (during which parents may consider the range of possible responses before them in the face of perceived child misbehavior), (3) response decision (during which parents narrow the field of possible parental reactions and evaluate that, or those, deemed most appropriate), and (4) implementation (the stage at which cognitive decisions are enacted through social behavior). It may be at stages 2 and 3 that prevailing conservative Protestant child-rearing schemata encourage conservative Protestant parents to discount parental yelling as a viable disciplinary option even while the merits of corporal punishment are considered. References Ammerman, Nancy Tatom, and Wade Clark Roof. 1995. "Introduction: Old Patterns, New Trends,FragileExperiments." Pp. 1-20 in Work, Family, andReligion in Contemporary Society, edited by Nancy T. Ammerman and Wade Clark Roof. Routledge. Bartkowski, John P. 1995. "Spare the Rod ... , or Spare the Child? Divergent Perspectives on Conservative Protestant Child Discipline." Review of Religious Research 37:97 -116. - - - . 1997."Debating Patriarchy: Discursive Disputes overSpousalAuthority among Evangelical Family Commentators." Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 36:393-410. - - - . 1998. "Changing of the Gods: The Gender and Family Discourse of American Evangelicalism in Historical Perspective:' History of the Family 3:97-117. - - - . 1999. "One Step Forward, One Step Back: 'Progressive Traditionalism'and the Negotiation of Domestic Labor within Evangelical Families." Gender Issues 17:37-61. - - - . 2000."BreakingWalls,Raising Fences: Masculinity,Intimacy, and Accountability among the Promise Keepers." Sociology of Religion 61:33-53. Bartkowski, John P.,and Christopher G. Ellison. 1995. "Divergent Perspectives on Childrearing in Popular Manuals: Conservative Protestants vs. the Mainstream Experts." Sociology of Religion 56:21-34. Bartkowski, John P., W Bradford Wilcox, and Christopher G. Ellison. 2000. "Charting the Paradoxes of Evangelical Family Life: Gender and Parenting in Conservative Protestant Households." Family Ministry. In press. Bartkowski,John P.,and Xiaohe Xu. 2000."Distant Patriarchs or Expressive Dads? The Discourse and Practice of Fathering in Conservative Protestant Families." Sociological Quarterly 41:86585. Baumrind, Diana. 1971. "Current Patterns of Parental Authority." Developmental Psychology Monographs 4:1-1 02. ---.1997. "Necessary Distinctions." Psychological Inquiry 8:176-82. Brenner, Viktor, and Robert A. Fox. 1998. "Parental Discipline and Behavior Problems in Young Children." Journal of Genetic Psychology 159:251-56. Capps, Donald. 1992. "Religion and Child Abuse: Perfect Together." Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 31:1-14. - - - . 1995. The Child's Song: The Religious Abuseof Children. Westminster/John Knox. Christenson, Larry. 1970. The Christian Family. Bethany House. Darnell, Alfred, and Darren E. Sherkat. 1997. "The Impact of Protestant Fundamentalism on Educational Attainment." American Sociological Review62:306-15. 288 I Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 Dobson,James. 1978. The Strong-Willed Child: Birth through Adolescence. Living Books/Tyndale House. --.1992. TheNewDare to Discipline. Living Books/Tyndale House. Elder, Glen, TriVanNguyen, and Avshalom Caspi. 1985. "LinkingFamily Hardship to Children's Lives." Child Development 56:361-75. Ellison, Christopher G. 1996. "Conservative Protestantism and the Corporal Punishment of Children." Journal for theScientific StudyofReligion 35:1-16. Ellison, Christopher G.,and John P. Bartkowski. 1997. "Religion and the Legitimation of Violence: The Caseof Conservative Protestantismand Corporal Punishment." Pp. 45-67in TheWeb ofViolence: From Interpersonal toGlobal, edited by Lester R. Kurtzand Jennifer1. Turpin. Universityof IllinoisPress. Ellison, Christopher G., John P. Bartkowski, and Michelle 1. Segal. 1996a."Conservative Protestantism and the Parental Use of Corporal Punishment." Social Forces 74:1003-29. - - - . 1996b. "Do Conservative ProtestantParentsSpankMore Often? Further Evidence from the National Surveyof Families and Households." Social Science Quarterly 77:663-73. Ellison, Christopher G., and Mark A. Musick. 1995. "Conservative Protestantism and Public Opinion toward Science." Review of Religious Research 36:245-62. Ellison, ChristopherG.,and Darren E.Sherkat 1993a. "Conservative Protestantism and Support for Corporal Punishment."American Sociological Review 58:131-44. - - - . 1993b. "Obedience and Autonomy: Religion and ParentalValues Reconsidered:' Journal for the Scientific Studyof Religion 32:313-29. Fugate, J. Richard. 1980. What theBible Says aboutChild Training. Alpha-OmegaPublications. Gordon, Thomas. 1989. Discipline that Works: Promoting Self-Discipline in Children. Plume. Grasmick,Harold, Robert Bursik,and M'lou Kimpel. 1991. "Protestant Fundamentalism and Attitudes toward Corporal Punishment of Children." Violence and Victims 6:283-97. Greven, Philip. 1990. Spare the Child: TheReligious Roots ofPunishment and thePsychological Impact of Abuse. Alfred Knopf. Griffith, R. Marie. 1997. God's Daughters: Evangelical Women and the Power of Submission. Universityof CaliforniaPress. Gunnoe,MarjorieLindner, and Carrie LeaMariner. 1997. "Toward a Developmental-Contextual Model of the Effects of Parental Spankingon Children'sAggression." Archives ofPediatric Adolescent Medicine 151:768-75. Hemenway, D.,S.Solnick, and J.Carter. 1994. "ChildrearingViolence." Child Abuse andNeglect 18:1011-20. Hochschild, Arlie R. 1979. "EmotionWork, Feeling Rules, and Social Structure:'American Journal of Sociology 85:551-75. Hotaling, Gerald T., Murray A. Straus, and Alan J. Lincoln. 1990. "Intrafamily Violence and Crime and Violence outside the Family." Pp. 431-70 in Physical Violence in American Families: Risk Factors and Adaptions in Violence in 8,145 Families, edited by Murray A. Straus and RichardJ.Gelles. Transaction. Hunter,JamesDavison. 1983. American Evangelicalism: Conservative Religion andtheQuandary ofModernity. Rutgers University Press. - - - . 1987. Evangelicals: TheComing Generation. Universityof ChicagoPress. Conservative Protestant Child Discipline I 289 Joubert, Charles E. 1991. "Self-Esteemand Social Desirability in Relations to College Students' Retrospective Perceptions of Parental Fairness and Disciplinary Practices." Psychological Reports 69:115-20. Kazdin, Alan E., Douglas 1. Griest, and Karen Esveldt-Dawson. 1984. "Perceptions of Psychopathology among Psychiatric InpatientChildren." Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 13:147-56. Kelley, Michelle 1., and Hui MeiTsen. 1992. "Cultural Differences in ChildRearing: A Comparison of Immigrant Chinese and Caucasian American Mothers." Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 23:444-55. Kellstedt, LymanA, and John C. Green. 1993. "KnowingGod's ManyPeople: Denominational Preference and Political Behavior:' Pp. 53-71 in Rediscovering theReligious Factor inAmerican Politics, edited by David C. Leege and Lyman A Kellstedt. M.E.Sharpe. LaHaye, Beverly. 1977. How to Develop Your Child's Temperament. Harvest House. Larzelere, Robert E. 1996. "A Review of the Outcomes of Parental Use of Nonabusive or Customary Physical Punishment." Pediatrics 98:824-28. Larzelere, Robert E., Michael Klein, Walter R. Schumm, and Samuel A. Alibrando. 1989. "Relations of Spanking and Other ParentingCharacteristics to the Self-Esteem and Perceived Fairness of Parental Discipline." Psychological Reports 64:1140-42. Larzelere, Robert E.,Paul R. Sather, William N. Schneider, DavidB.Larson, and Patricia 1. Pike. 1998.«Punishment Enhances Reasoning's Effectiveness as a Disciplinary Response to Toddlers." Journal of Marriage and the Family 60:338-403. Larzelere, Robert E.,William N. Schneider, David B. Larson, and Patricia 1. Pike. 1996. "The Effects of Discipline Responses in Delaying ToddlerMisbehavior Recurrences." Childand Family Behavior Therapy 18:35-57. Lienesch, Michael. 1991. "Train Up a Child': Conceptions of Childrearing in Christian Conservative Social Thought." Pp. 203-24 in Comparative Social Research. Vol. 13,edited by CraigCalhoun. JAlPress. Lifton, Robert J., and Charles B. Strozier. 1990. "Waiting for Armageddon." New York Times Book Review, August12. Littauer,Fred. 1994. Wake Up Men! Headship Doesn't Mean Lordship. Word. Maurer, Adah. 1982. "Religious Values and Child Abuse." Pp. 57-63 in Institutional Abuse of Children and Youth, edited by Ranae Hanson. Haworth Press. Maurer,Adah,and James Wallerstein. 1980. TheBible and theRod. Committee to End Violence against the Next Generation. McNamara, Patrick. 1985a. "Conservative Christian Families and Their Moral World: Some Reflections for Sociologists." Sociological Analysis 46:93-99. - - - . 1985b. "The New Christian Right'sViewof the Familyand It's SocialScience Critics: A Study in DifferingPresuppositions." Journal of Marriage and the Family 47:449-58. Nagaraja, Iaya, 1984. "Non-Compliance- A Behavior Disorder." ChildPsychiatry Quarterly 17:127-32. Parke,Ross D., and Ronald G. Slaby. 1983. "The Developmentof Aggression." Pp. 547-647 in Handbook of ChildPsychology, edited by E. MavisHetherington. Wiley. Rieff, Philip. 1966. The Triumph of the Therapeutic: Uses of Faith afterFreud. Harper and Row. 290 I Social Forces 79:1, September 2000 Roof, Wade Clark, and William McKinney. 1987. American Mainline Religion: Its Changing Shape andFuture. RutgersUniversityPress. Schrnalzbauer, John. 1993. "Evangelicals in the NewClass: Class versus SubculturalPredictors of Ideology." Journal for theScientific Studyof Religion 32:330-42. Sewell, William H., Jr. 1992. ''A Theory of Structure: Duality,Agency, and Transformation:' American Journal ofSociology 98:1-29. Sherkat, Darren E. 1998. "Counterculture or Continuity? Competing Influences on Baby Boomers'Religious Orientations and Participation." Social Forces 76:1087-1115. - - - . 2000. "That They Be Keepers of the Home': The Effect of Conservative Religion on Earlyand LateTransitionsinto Housewifery." Review ofReligious Research 41:344-58. Sherkat, Darren E., and Christopher G. Ellison. 1997. "The Cognitive Structure of a Moral Crusade: Conservative Protestantism and Oppositionto Pornography." Social Forces 75:95782. Shibley, Mark A. 1996. Resurgent Evangelicalism in the United States: Mapping Cultural Change since 1970. Universityof South Carolina Press. Simons, Ronald1., ChristineJohnson,and Rand D.Conger. 1994. "Harsh Corporal Punishment versusQuality of Parental Involvement as an Explanationof Adolescent Maladjustment." Journal of Marriage and theFamily 56:591-607. Smith, Christian, with Michael Emerson, Sally Gallagher, Paul Kennedy, and David Sikkink. 1998. American Evangelicalism: Embattled and Thriving. Universityof ChicagoPress. Straus, Murray A. 1994. Beating theDevil OutofThem: Corporal Punishment inAmerican Families and Its Effects on Children. Lexington Books. Straus, Murray A., David B. Sugarman, and Jean Giles-Sims. 1997. "Spankingby Parents and Subsequent Antisocial Behavior of Children." Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine 151:761-67. Sudman, Seymour,and Norman M. Bradburn. 1982. Asking Questions. Iossey-Bass, Swindoll, Charles R. 1977. You and Your Child. Thomas Nelson. Thomson, Elizabeth, Sara S. Mcl.anahan, and R.B. Curtin. 1992. "Family Structure, Gender, and Parental Socialization." Journal of Marriage and theFamily 54:368-78. Wilcox, W.Bradford. 1998. "Conservative ProtestantParenting: Authoritarianor Authoritative?" American Sociological Review 63:796-809. Wuthnow,Robert. 1988. The Restructuring ofAmerican Religion. Princeton University Press. Wuthnow, Robert,and MatthewP. Lawson. 1994. "Sources of Christian Fundamentalismin the United States." Pp. 18-56 in Accounting for Fundamentalisms: The Dynamic Character of Movements, edited by Martin Marty and R. Scott Appleby. University of ChicagoPress.