Health Psychology Review ISSN: 1743-7199 (Print) 1743-7202 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rhpr20 Assessing the efficacy of appearance-focused interventions to prevent skin cancer: a systematic review of the literature Lorna J. Dodd & Mark J. Forshaw To cite this article: Lorna J. Dodd & Mark J. Forshaw (2010) Assessing the efficacy of appearance-focused interventions to prevent skin cancer: a systematic review of the literature, Health Psychology Review, 4:2, 93-111, DOI: 10.1080/17437199.2010.485393 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2010.485393 Published online: 13 Sep 2010. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 286 View related articles Citing articles: 17 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rhpr20 Download by: [Newman University] Date: 05 May 2017, At: 06:52 Health Psychology Review Vol. 4, No. 2, September 2010, 93111 Assessing the efficacy of appearance-focused interventions to prevent skin cancer: a systematic review of the literature Lorna J. Dodda* and Mark J. Forshawb a Department of Psychology & Counselling, Newman University College, Bartley Green, Birmingham B32 3NT, UK; bCentre for Health Psychology, Staffordshire University, College Road, Stoke-on-Trent ST4 2DE, UK (Received 11 July 2009; final version received 12 April 2010) Purpose. Ultraviolet (UV) radiation is associated with adverse health effects. Although the immediate appearance values of tanning seem to outweigh the longterm health consequences, interventions emphasising the negative effects of UV exposure on appearance may be effective in modifying such behaviour. Method. A systematic review assessing the efficacy of appearance-focused interventions in modifying UV exposure and/or protection. Relevant publications were identified from a comprehensive search strategy of 16 electronic bibliographic databases from inception to March 2009, hand searches of six subject relevant journals and references lists of identified articles. Results. Data were extracted from 12 studies that were found to be eligible. Significant effects were found for UV protection behaviour in favour of the interventions. Though the interventions only had moderate success in modifying UV exposure behaviour, plausible explanations were provided. There was no clear pattern to suggest that quality of the studies or type of intervention was associated with effectiveness of the interventions. Conclusion. Appearance-focused interventions represent a promising method for modifying tanning behaviour. There is scope to evaluate the generalisability of these interventions on different populations and regions. Keywords: appearance; intervention; review; skin cancer; UV Introduction Ultraviolet (UV) radiation is emitted from the sun and other sources (e.g., artificial tanning devices), and such exposure is associated with adverse health effects (e.g., skin cancer, premature aging, cataracts and immune suppression; World Health Organisation [WHO], 2009a, 2009c, 2009d). The two main types of skin cancer linked to UV exposure are non-melanoma and malignant melanoma (Cancer Research UK, 2009d). The incidence and prevalence of both these skin cancers are increasing globally each year, with 23 million and 132,000 cases reported yearly, respectively (WHO, 2009b). Worldwide, exposure to UV light is a significant contributor to ill health and mortality. Furthermore, because of its effects taking time to develop, that is the temporal separation of cause and effect, it represents a challenge to health promotion. In warmer climates such as Australia and the United States of America (USA) reports have shown skin cancer accounts for more than 80% and 50%, respectively, of all new cancers diagnosed each year (American Cancer *Corresponding author. Email: l.dodd@newman.ac.uk ISSN 1743-7199 print/ISSN 1743-7202 online # 2010 Taylor & Francis DOI: 10.1080/17437199.2010.485393 http://www.informaworld.com 94 L.J. Dodd and M.J. Forshaw Society, 2006; Cancer Council Australia, 2009). Notably, within the United Kingdom (UK) where UV levels and hours of sunshine are significantly less, latest figures show skin cancer still accounts for more than 30% of new cancer diagnoses each year (Cancer Research UK, 2009d). In fact, skin cancer deaths in the UK are greater than Australia (Cancer Research UK, 2009b). Furthermore, within the UK malignant melanoma is the second most common cancer in adults aged 1534 years (Cancer Research UK, 2009d); this is comparable to both Australia and USA statistics (American Cancer Society, 2006; Cancer Council Australia, 2009). Additionally, skin cancer is not gender or age specific as 5213 females and 4370 males were diagnosed with malignant melanoma in 2005 in the UK, and cases have been found in children as young as 10 years old (Cancer Research UK, 2009c; ISD, 2009; Northern Ireland Cancer Registry, 2009; Office for National Statistics [ONS], 2009). Consequently, UV protection represents a major, global health concern for all ages and sexes. Phenotypic features play a role in the development of skin cancer, most notably blue eyes, blonde or red hair colour, pale skin, tendency to sunburn rather than tan, freckles and moles/naevi (WHO, 2009c). Nevertheless, UV exposure should be easily modifiable through a number of primary prevention strategies such as avoiding sun exposure during peak UV levels, wearing protective clothing, applying sunscreen protection, seeking shade and avoiding artificial tanning devices (Cancer Research UK, 2009a). Previous studies reveal that western populations are aware of the risk factors associated with tanning behaviour and the preventable measures, though UV exposure is still frequent (e.g., Ddadlani & Orlow, 2008; Peacey, Steptoe, Sanderman, & Wardle, 2006). Primary prevention interventions for skin cancer such as community-based interventions (e.g., media campaigns to a target audience), public health campaigns (e.g., mass media campaigns) and educational interventions (e.g., lectures, presentations, workbooks in education-based settings) have been the main source for transferring such knowledge, primarily focusing on the health consequences (e.g., skin cancer) of such behaviour. Though to date such interventions have demonstrated that whilst they have been successful at enhancing knowledge and attitudes related to UV exposure and protection, few have been successful in modifying UV exposure/protection behaviours (e.g., Baum & Cohen, 1998; Buller & Borland, 1999; Cummings, Tripp, & Herrmann, 1997; Hart & DeMarco, 2008; Morris & Elwood, 1996; Saraiya et al., 2004). Despite, the adverse health effects associated with tanning, UV exposure seems to be gaining considerable popularity and for many having ‘a tan’ is associated with attractiveness and feeling healthy (Cokkinides et al., 2001; Lazovich & Forster, 2005; Miles, Waller, Hiom, & Swanston, 2005; Miller, Ashton, McHoskey, & Gimbel, 1990). Subsequently, appearance-issues related to UV exposure have recently been at the forefront of research (e.g., Hillhouse, Turrisi, Stapleton, & Robinson, 2008; Mahler, Kulik, Butler, Gerrard, & Gibbons, 2008; Olson, Gaffney, Starr, & Dietrich, 2008). In a UK study assessing sun-safe knowledge and behaviour, 5077% of the sample reported they were aware of preventive measures of skin cancer (e.g., sunscreen, seeking shade, protective clothing), though only one-third reported that they adhered to such preventive practices. Furthermore, over 50% reported that a tan made them look healthier and more attractive (Miles et al., 2005). The appearance value of seeking a tan may further explain the popularity and growth of the artificial Health Psychology Review 95 tanning industry (e.g., Hillhouse, Turrisi, Holwiski, & McVeigh, 1999). Consequently, it seems that immediate appearance-related benefits of tanning outweigh the long-term health effects. As a result, the limited effectiveness of the previous interventions could be attributed to the lack of focus on appearance factors. There is now a growing body of literature on interventions emphasising the negative consequences of UV exposure on appearance by targeting the proximal effects of tanning, such as premature skin aging wrinkles and pigmented age spots (e.g., Barron et al., 2007; Gibbons, Gerrard, Lane, Mahler, & Kulik, 2005; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Mahler, Fitzpatrick, Parker, & Lapin, 1997; Mahler et al., 2008). Furthermore, such interventions seem to generate greater interest (e.g., Jones & Leary, 1994; Mahler et al., 1997; Weinstock & Rossi, 1998). The literature suggests that appearance-focused interventions should theoretically reduce UV exposure. To date such interventions have not been systematically reviewed. Thus, considering the serious health consequence of UV exposure, the enhanced popularity of this behaviour, and the limited effectiveness of previous skin cancer reduction programmes in modifying UV exposure/protection practices, assessing the efficacy of such interventions is paramount; thus this review is particularly timely. The objective of this review was to provide a systematic evaluation of the efficacy of appearance-focused interventions in modifying UV exposure and/or protection from baseline to the end of the intervention period or at follow-up. Method Search strategy Relevant publications were identified from a comprehensive search strategy of electronic bibliographic databases, hand searches of subject relevant journals and reference lists of identified articles. All the databases were searched for relevant publications from inception to March 2009. Search strategies were customised when required to meet the requirements of the databases. Where available, filters were imposed to capture-related terms, reviews and human-only studies. The following 11 Ovid electronic bibliographic databases were searched: AMED, British Nursing Index, Business Source Premier, CINAHL, Econlit, GreenFile, International Bibliography of the Social Sciences, MEDLINE, Pre-CINAHL, PsycARTICLES and SPORTDiscus. A further five bibliographic databases were scrutinised for relevant publications (the later three databases were searched for the purpose of identifying grey literature): Web of Knowledge, Cochrane Library, ProQuest Dissertation & Theses Databases, Index to Theses in Great Britain and ISI Proceedings. To identify relevant studies that may have not been added to these databases, the following six journals were hand-searched from 2008 to March 2009: Social Science & Medicine, Health Education Research, Health Psychology, Basic & Applied Social Psychology, Archives of Dermatology and Journal of Behavioral Medicine. The search strategy included keywords that focused on the desired intervention type ‘appearancefocused’ (e.g., age spots, appearance, appearance-diminishing, photoageing, photodamage, wrinkles), which were then combined with the health behaviour (e.g., skin cancer prevention, solar radiation, sun/UV protection, sunscreen, tanning, UV 96 L.J. Dodd and M.J. Forshaw exposure) and the associated illness terms (e.g., melanoma, skin cancer, skin tumour). Boolean operators were utilised to identify the most relevant literature. Selection strategy The inclusion criteria were kept fairly broad in order to capture all relevant articles. Initially, titles or abstracts were included and full manuscripts were retrieved for further inspection if the study was an intervention that focused on appearance and tanning (including artificial tanning). If these initial inclusion requirements were unable to be extracted from the title/abstract, the full manuscript was retrieved to enable a judgement about inclusion. The inclusion and exclusion criteria were defined in terms of the intervention, outcome measure and study design (Centre for Reviews and Dissemination [CRD], 2001). The full manuscripts were then scrutinised and included in the systematic review if the following criteria were met: (1) intervention was appearance-focused; (2) intervention evaluated change in intention and/or behaviour towards UV exposure and/or protection; (3) at a minimum the study design was quasi-experimental; and (4) the articles were published in English language. There was no restriction on population type or age group. Papers were excluded if they were: (1) discussions and/ or reviews (though their reference lists were examined); (2) if the intervention was a multi-component programme where there was little emphasis on aspects of appearance, for example, if a considerable number of the components focused on the health effects of the behaviour; (3) if there was no pre- and post-assessment of the desired outcomes; or (4) if they were not original articles. Review strategy A data extraction form was developed and applied systematically to all eligible articles that met the inclusion criteria. The form was used to extract the following information from the articles: article, study and intervention characteristics, outcome measures and analysis. A quality assessment checklist of 13 quality indicators was devised in accordance with published recommendations (e.g., Bellamy, 2005 QUEST criteria; Jacobsen, Donovan, Vadaparamphil, & Small, 2007; Ryan et al., 2007). All eligible studies were subjected to the quality assessment checklist. This process is essential for studies that are evaluated in review papers in order to limit bias, to allow for potential comparisons between individual studies to be made and to allow for meaningful conclusions to be drawn (Higgins & Green, 2008). Unless specified, each quality assessment indicator was awarded 4 points for full detail, 3 for substantial detail, 2 for partial detail and 1 for no detail or providing insufficient detail for reasonable assessment. The following aspects of each paper were assessed and scored using the above criteria: study design (i.e., randomised controlled trial (RCT), randomisation without control group, control trial without randomisation, quasi-experimental, points awarded 4, 3, 2, 1, respectively), sample size, a priori estimate of sample size, sampling procedure, conditions comparable on measures of interest at baseline, refusal rate reported at baseline, allocation concealment/blinding, measurement tool of outcomes (i.e., objective measurement, self-report behaviour, report of intended behaviour, points awarded 4, 3, 2, respectively), intervention details, attrition rate at Health Psychology Review 97 follow-up, statistical techniques reported, statistical outputs reported and a discussion of generalisability. Scores were summated to create a score between 14 and 52. Studies were considered well-conducted and reported if they scored 42, average between 33 and 42 and poor if they scored less than 33 (refer to Table 2). The ranges were based on Bellamy’s (2005) QUEST criteria. Procedure and data synthesis Once full manuscripts of eligible studies (studies meeting the initial inclusion criteria at the title/abstract stage of searching) were retrieved, each study was scrutinised using the data extraction sheet. Fifty percent were scrutinised by the co-author as a reliability precaution. Only one discrepancy was reported between reviewers regarding the inclusion of a paper and a consensus was reached through further scrutiny of the paper and discussion. A meta-analysis was not undertaken because the desired outcome measure content was too heterogeneous to integrate statistically. For example, the studies’ assessments of UV exposure/protection timeframe varied from the previous weekend to the previous 12 months, and various studies’ interpretations of UV exposure/ protection were operationalised inconsistently. Furthermore, there were differences in methodological quality. Thus, findings are synthesised in a systematic descriptive form. Results Trial flow The initial comprehensive search strategy generated 815 potentially relevant citations. Of these, 148 were duplicates and thus excluded, and a further 628 were discarded for not meeting the initial inclusion criteria at the title/abstract stage of the search strategy. Full manuscripts were retrieved for the remaining 39 papers and were subjected to a full data extraction, of which 12 papers fulfilled the full inclusion criteria and are included in the review. Figure 1 depicts the output of the search strategy and reasons for exclusions. Study characteristics of the appearance-focused intervention studies Demographics characteristics A brief summary of characteristics of the included studies are described in Table 1. In eight of the studies the sample consisted of university students with an age range of 1744 years (Gibbons et al., 2005; Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Mahler et al., 2008; Mahler, Kulik, Gerrard, & Gibbons, 2007; Mahler, Kulik, Harrell, Correa, & Gibbons, 2005; McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001). Two studies consisted of adolescents (Novick, 1997; Olson et al., 2008), and two studies consisted of beach patrons, age range of 1867 years (Mahler, Kulik, Gerrard, & Gibbons, 2006; Pagoto, McChargue, & Fuqua, 2003). Seven of the studies (58%) included both male and female participants, although a greater proportion in each study were females ranging from 53 to 83% (range based on those studies that provided information on the gender split; Gibbons 98 L.J. Dodd and M.J. Forshaw Potentially relevant citations identified and screened for retrieval at title/abstract stage (N=815) Duplicate citations excluded (N=148) Non-duplicate citations retrieved (N=667) Articles excluded, did not fulfil initial inclusion criteria (N=628; see text for details) Full manuscripts retrieved for possible inclusion in review (N=39) Articles excluded not meeting full inclusion criteria (see text for details; N=27) Duplication of participants (n=1) Not an intervention (n=3) Desired outcome measures not addressed (n=4) Discussion/review/descriptive article (n=4) Post-test design only (n=4) Multiple component intervention with minimal appearance focus (n=2) Pre-post measures of desired outcomes not included (n=1) Intervention focus not appearance based (n=6) Not English (n=2) Studies included in final systematic review (N=12) Figure 1. Search strategy. et al., 2005; Mahler et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2008; McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001; Pagoto et al., 2003). Four studies included females only (Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Novick, 1997) and in one study sex of the sample was unable to be determined (Olson et al., 2008). Seven of the studies reported their samples’ ethnic origin as either entirely Caucasian or with a substantial bias towards Caucasian ethnic groupings (Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Mahler et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2008; Novick, 1997; Olson et al., 2008). All the studies included were carried out in the USA and four of the studies were carried out by the same first author. Therefore, it was deduced that the generalisability of the findings may be limited as the study samples are predominately drawn from university students, the sample is predominantly females, and data are collected from one, albeit well-populated, country. Health Psychology Review 99 Design characteristics The total sample size for each study ranged from 30 to 430 at baseline (median 133). For three of the studies, sample size for at least one of the conditions was relatively small (B30 per condition; Mahler et al., 2008; McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001; Novick, 1997), whilst the remaining studies’ condition size was moderate (30 toB200). A priori estimate for sample size was reported in two studies only (Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Mahler et al., 2008), thus for the remaining articles (83%) it is not clear whether the studies were adequately planned to detect for differences. Nine studies were described as RCTs (Gibbons et al., 2005; Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Mahler et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2008; Novick, 1997). One study utilised a randomisation design with a wait-list control group (McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001), one involved a controlled trial without randomisation (Pagoto et al., 2003) and the remaining study was a prepost comparison without a control (Olson et al., 2008). Eleven studies included a follow-up component ranging from 2 weeks to 12 months (Gibbons et al., 2005; Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Mahler et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2008; McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001; Novick, 1997; Pagoto et al., 2003). The maximum number of conditions (including control condition) found in a single study was five (Mahler et al., 2008). Recruitment characteristics Seven of the 10 randomised trials reported that the conditions were comparable at baseline assessment (Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Mahler et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2008). A non-random recruitment method was used by six of the 12 included studies (Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Mahler et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2008), three studies used a randomised sampling technique (Gibbons et al., 2005; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Novick, 1997), only one described how the technique was implemented (Hillhouse et al., 2008) and the remaining three studies did not report their sampling procedure (McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001; Olson et al., 2008; Pagoto et al., 2003). Of those studies using a non-random sampling procedure only 27% reported participants’ refusal rate (Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Mahler et al., 2006). Thus, it is not clear of the magnitude of bias that may have occurred in the sample (Clark-Carter, 2004). Intervention characteristics To some extent, type (e.g., booklet, workbook, essay, UV photograph), delivery (e.g., video, presentation, lecture) and content of the appearance-focused interventions varied between studies and varied depending on intervention condition (e.g., for those studies comparing more than one appearance-focused intervention type). In brief, intervention variation consisted of photoageing/appearance information and/ or UV photo/light (depicting underlying UV damage to skin; Gibbons et al., 2005; Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Mahler et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2008; McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001; Olson et al., 2008; Pagoto et al., 2003) and photoageing/appearance information and UV photo plus 100 L.J. Dodd and M.J. Forshaw supplementary information (e.g., sunless tanning lotion, social norms; Mahler et al., 2005, 2008). In one study, photographs of the face were taken and converted by computer imagery to demonstrate ageing and lesions (Novick, 1997). In those studies where some aspect of the intervention duration was reported, these aspects were of short duration, for example, duration ranged from 2 to 75 minutes long (medium 21 minutes; Gibbons et al., 2005; Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Mahler et al., 2005, 2007; McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001; Olson et al., 2008). Outcomes measured Included studies assessed intention and/or behaviour of the desired outcome measures; UV exposure and/or protection, assessing change from baseline to the end of the intervention period or at follow-up. Those studies that measured behaviour by both self-report and by an objective measure (e.g., skin reflectance spectrophotometer to assess change in skin colour), measured both UV exposure and protection behaviour and measured both intention and behaviour had both sets of data extracted. Outcome measure questions content varied in: quantity (e.g., number of questions per outcome measure), timeframe (e.g., outcome measures questions ranged from asking about the previous weekend to the previous 12 months, hours of partaking in the behaviour and/or frequency reported of the behaviour), type of behaviour (e.g., UV exposure and/or protection, indoor tanning/sun exposure, type of protection, i.e., sunscreen/wearing a hat/seeking shade) and measurement method (e.g., self-reported behaviour vs. objective skin-colour measure skin reflectance spectrophotometer). Quantitative analysis The statistical technique ANCOVA was predominantly utilised to analyse the data within the studies (with baseline data controlled for as the covariate). This is deemed an appropriate statistical test to assess for differences between conditions when examining changes from baseline to the end of the intervention or follow-up, when the assumptions of this test are adhered to (e.g., measurement level of the outcome variable is ratio/interval level). Quality assessment of the appearance-focused intervention studies Table 2 provides an overview of the scores awarded for the 13 quality indicators for each of the 12 studies. Quality assessment was rated good for one study (Jackson & Aiken, 2006), average for seven (58%) of the studies (Gibbons et al., 2005; Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Mahler et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2008) and poor for four (33%) of the studies (McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001; Novick, 1997; Olson et al., 2008; Pagoto et al., 2003). The following areas of the quality indicator: a priori estimate of sample size, allocation concealment/blinding and refusal rate reported at baseline, were given a low-quality score (e.g., 1) in the majority of the studies (83%, 83%, 67%, respectively). Table 1. Characteristics of included studies. Study (first author, year) Population Sample size at baseline Country Design Outcome measures Control condition(s) Intervention condition(s) Quality score Gibbons (2005) University students (male and female) Exp. 1 70 C 35 I 35 Exp. 2 134 C 67 I 67 USA RCT, one-month follow-up Indoor tanning behavioura Exp. 1 debrief with intervention information Exp. 2 received the same as the intervention group except the UV photo Exp. 1 and Exp. 2-Photoageing/ appearance information (communicated by oral presentation and brochures) and UV photo Hillhouse (2008) University students (female) 430 C 230 I 200 USA RCT, six-month follow-up Indoor tanning intentiona and behaviour No intervention 40 Photoageing/appearance information with an emphasis on norms (communicated by a written booklet) Hillhouse (2002) University students (female) 147 C ? I ? USA RCT, 2 weeks and two-month follow-up Indoor tanning intentiona and behavioura No intervention Appearance-focused information (workbook format) 38 Jackson (2006) University students (female) 211 C 105 I 106 USA RCT, two-week follow-up Intention to UV protecta and sunbath,a UV exposurea/ protectiona behaviour Stress management intervention Photoageing/appearance, image norm information (communicated by a variety of formats, e.g., video, worksheets) (Multi-component appearance-focused intervention) 100 37 Health Psychology Review 101 102 Table 1 (Continued ) Study (first author, year) Population Sample size at baseline Country University students (male and female) 125 C 25 I1 25 I2 25 I3 25 I4 25 USA Mahler (2007) University students (male and female) 133 C 34 I1 34 I2 35 I3 30 USA Outcome measures RCT, one-month UV protection follow-up behavioura RCT, 45 and 12 months follow-up Control condition(s) Intervention condition(s) 42 No intervention UV exposurea/ No intervention protectiona behaviour, actual skin coloura Quality score I1photoageing/appearance information (communicated by written material with visual material) and UV photo (the basic intervention) I2 basic intervention plus injunctive norm information I3 basic intervention plus descriptive norm information I4 basic intervention plus both the injunctive and descriptive norm information L.J. Dodd and M.J. Forshaw Mahler (2008) Design 40 I1 photoageing/ appearance information (communicated by video format) I2 UV photo I3 photoageing/ appearance information and UV photo Table 1 (Continued ) Study (first author, year) Population Sample size at baseline Country Beach patrons (male and female) 244 C 60 I1 61 I2 62 I3 62 USA Mahler (2005) University students (male and female) 146 C 50 I1 50 I2 46 USA McClendon University (2001) students (male and female) 58 C 30 I 28 USA Outcome measures Control condition(s) RCT, two-month UV protection follow-up behaviour,a actual skin colour No intervention RCT, one-month UV exposure/ follow-up protectiona behaviour No intervention Intention to UV protecta and sunbath,a UV exposure behavioura Wait-list control Randomised design with a wait-list control group Intervention condition(s) Quality score 42 I1 photoageing/ appearance information (communicated by laminated card) and UV photo I2 UV photo I3 photoageing/ appearance information 38 I1 photoageing/ appearance information (communicated by video format) and UV photo I2 photoageing/ appearance information and UV photo plus sunless tanning lotion Photoageing/appearance information (essay) and video focusing on someone who has skin cancer Health Psychology Review Mahler (2006) Design 31 103 104 Table 1 (Continued ) Study (first author, year) Population Sample size at baseline Country Adolescence 30 (female) C 14 I1 8 I2 8 USA Olson (2008) Adolescence 113 (?) Pagoto (2003) Beach patrons (male and female) a 257 C ? I ? Outcome measures Control condition(s) Intervention condition(s) Quality score 28 RCT, 6 weeks follow-up (weekly logs) UV exposure/ protection behavioura Provided with sunscreen and a photograph was taken, but the photo was not modified. USA Prepost comparison without a control Intention to use sunscreena No control group 23 Photoageing/appearance information, risk of UV exposure and strategies to protect (communicated by a session with visual material) and UV light (appearancefocused educational intervention) USA Controlled trial without randomisation, two-month follow-up UV exposure/ protectiona behaviour No intervention 24 Appearance-focused multi-component plus a UV photo Significant effects. ? denotes papers where sample size for the control group and the experimental group was not provided. I1 photograph demonstrating ageing I2 photograph demonstrating ageing and cancerous lesions (The photographs were computer-enhanced) All participants were provided with sunscreen L.J. Dodd and M.J. Forshaw Novick (1997) Design Table 2. Quality assessment of included studies. Study (first author, year) 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 4 2 4 1 2 4 1 4 1 3 1 4 3 4 4 4 1 4 1 4 1 4 1 4 1 4 1 2 Statistical technique and Attrition statistical rate outputs Conditions comparable at baseline Refusal rate reported at baseline Allocation concealment/ blinding Measurement tool Intervention details 3 1 3 1 3 4 4 4 4 4 1 3 4 3 2 4 3 1 3 4 3 3 4 1 3 3 4 4 3 4 1 2 3 4 3 3 4 1 1 4 4 4 3 4 3 1 4 4 4 4 4 1 1 3 4 4 1 1 1 1 4 4 4 2 1 1 1 3 3 1 1 1 1 1 1 4 3 1 1 1 1 2 4 1 4 3 4 3 4 3 4 4 4 3 4 3 3 3 3 3 4 3 3 3 2 3 3 Discussion of generalisability Overall quality score 2 37 1 40 3 38 4 45 3 42 3 40 4 42 2 38 1 31 2 28 1 23 2 24 Health Psychology Review 105 Gibbons (2005) Hillhouse (2008) Hillhouse (2002) Jackson (2006) Mahler (2008) Mahler (2007) Mahler (2006) Mahler (2005) McClendon (2001) Novick (1997) Olson (2008) Pagoto (2003) Sample size and a priori Study estimate of Sampling design sample size procedure 106 L.J. Dodd and M.J. Forshaw Findings of the appearance-focused intervention studies Effects of appearance-focused interventions on UV exposure and/or protection intentions All five studies assessing UV exposure and/or protection intention yielded a significant finding in favour of the appearance-focused intervention, either reporting significantly fewer tanning intentions and/or greater UV protection intentions (Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Jackson & Aiken, 2006; McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001; Olson et al., 2008). One study consisted of multiple components with a heavy focus on appearance factors (Jackson & Aiken, 2006). One study consisted of photoageing/appearance information and a UV light (Olson et al., 2008) and three studies favoured a photoageing/appearance information intervention (Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008; McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001). Two studies measured both UV exposure and protection intentions (Jackson & Aiken, 2006; McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001), two studies measured UV exposure intentions from indoor tanning (Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008) and one study measured UV protection intentions (e.g., sunscreen use; Olson et al., 2008). Three out of the five (60%) studies were rated as average to good on the quality assessment checklist (Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Hillhouse et al., 2008; Jackson & Aiken, 2006). Effects of appearance-focused interventions on UV protection behaviours All seven studies assessing behaviours towards UV protection yielded a significant finding in favour of the appearance-focused intervention in enhancing such behaviours (Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Mahler et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2008; Novick, 1997; Pagoto et al., 2003). For those studies with more than one appearance-focused intervention, the most successful is reported. Of the seven, one study favoured an intervention that included photoageing/appearance information and a UV photo (Mahler et al., 2005). One study favoured an intervention that included photoageing/ appearance information, a UV photo, plus descriptive and injunctive norm information (Mahler et al., 2008). Two studies favoured an intervention of photoageing/appearance information (Mahler et al., 2006, 2007). In one study, a greater endorsement of UV protection was found in the intervention condition that viewed computer enhanced images of skin both aged and disfigured by cancerous lesions (Novick, 1997). Two studies consisted of multiple components with a heavy focus on appearance factors (Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Pagoto et al., 2003). All of these studies included a longitudinal/follow-up measure ranging from 2 weeks to 12 months (Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Mahler et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2008; Novick, 1997; Pagoto et al., 2003). Differences were still apparent at the follow-ups ranging from 2 weeks to 45 months, but no differences were found at 12 months follow-up (Mahler et al., 2007). Five out of the seven (71%) studies were rated as average to good on the quality assessment checklist. Effects of appearance-focused interventions on UV exposure behaviour Five studies out of the 10 measuring change in UV exposure behaviour yielded at least one significant finding in favour of the appearance-focused intervention Health Psychology Review 107 (Gibbons et al., 2005; Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Jackson & Aiken, 2006; Mahler et al., 2007; McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001). For those studies with more than one appearance-focused intervention, the most successful is reported. Four out of the five (80%) studies were rated as average to good on the quality assessment checklist. Of the five studies, one study favoured a UV photo intervention (Gibbons et al., 2005) and three studies favoured photoageing/appearance information intervention (Hillhouse & Turrisi, 2002; Mahler et al., 2006; McClendon & Prentice-Dunn, 2001). The information, however, was communicated through a number of different formats with varying content. In the Mahler et al. (2007) study, exposure to this information reduced incidental UV exposure and reduced skin darkening (observed by an objective indicator of skin-colour change). Similarly, using an objective indicator, McClendon and Prentice-Dunn (2001) revealed lighter skin post-intervention. One study favoured a multiple component intervention with a significant focus on appearance factors (Jackson & Aiken, 2006). Of those studies yielding a significant result, the longest significant follow-up assessment reported was 12 months measured by an objective indicator (Mahler et al., 2007). Of the five studies not yielding a significant result (Hillhouse et al., 2008; Mahler et al., 2005, 2006; Novick, 1997; Pagoto et al., 2003), one focused on photoageing/ appearance information communicated through a booklet format (Hillhouse et al., 2008). The study reported an increase in behaviour from baseline to follow-up, but it was argued that this was attributed to time of year when the assessment was carried out. Baseline assessment was carried out at a period where UV exposure behaviour (indoor tanning) is expected to be low, whereas the six-month follow-up assessment was carried during a period when such behaviour is expected to be high (February April). Although the control group did indulge in greater indoor tanning behaviour at follow-up than the intervention group, the difference was not significant (Hillhouse et al., 2008). However, as reported previously, intention to reduce indoor tanning was significant for the experimental group post-intervention. One study focused on a multiple component intervention with a significant focus on appearance factors including a UV photograph (Pagoto et al., 2003). In Mahler et al. (2006), the objective indicator of skin-colour change (the spectrophotometer device) revealed that there was no difference in skin-colour change post-intervention. Furthermore, an earlier study by Mahler et al. (2005) did not yield a significant finding in UV exposure behaviour reduction between intervention and control at follow-up. Similarly, Novick (1997) revealed a non-significant difference between the two intervention groups (who viewed a computer enhanced image photo of themselves with aged skin or both aged skin and cancerous lesions) and the control group in reduction of UV exposure behaviour between 10 am and 3 pm. The latter finding was attributed to the fact that the participants were working on summer camps, and they were required to spend a number of hours outdoors between 10 am and 3 pm. Amongst the publications reporting non-significant reductions in UV exposure behaviour post-intervention, three were rated of average quality (Hillhouse et al., 2008; Mahler et al., 2005, 2006) and two rated as poor quality (Novick, 1997; Pagoto et al., 2003). 108 L.J. Dodd and M.J. Forshaw Discussion The synthesis of results is promising; interventions focusing on appearance factors appear to have a relatively positive effect on modifying behaviour. All seven studies that assessed change in UV protection behaviours revealed a significant finding in favour of the intervention. Although, in contrast, not all the studies that aimed to reduce UV exposure behaviour experienced similar success. Only five out of the 10 studies measuring this behaviour yielded a significant result in favour of the intervention. Reasonable explanations for such findings were provided and should be evaluated when considering the papers and the interventions. Four of the five studies yielding a non-significant result also measured UV protection behaviours, where a significant increase occurred (Mahler et al., 2005, 2006; Novick, 1997; Pagoto et al., 2003). Pagoto et al. (2003) identified that such a finding is not unusual and have provided a very plausible explanation. They stated that individuals may simply perceive that UV protection is an adequate measure to reduce photoageing. Thus, future appearance-focused interventions should explicitly demonstrate that UV protection will not reduce the effects of photoageing if tanning duration and frequency are unchanged. There was no clear pattern to suggest that quality of the studies was associated with effectiveness of the interventions. However, the successful studies did have smaller sample sizes and slightly fewer conditions. Appearance-focused interventions were successful at modifying behavioural intention. Furthermore, there was no clear pattern within the results where one could make a confident conclusion about the most effective appearance-focused intervention. Whilst they were all appearance-focused the delivery mode varied (e.g., booklet, workbook, essay, UV photograph, video, presentation, lecture, information based, etc.). One must re-state at this point that even though the review of studies is promising and that it is tempting to make comparisons between the studies, the studies were not fully homogeneous; there were clear methodological differences. The reliability and validity of the assessment measures are questionable; nine of the studies (75%) utilised a self-report measure. Future research should adopt more objective skincolour measures such as the spectrophotometer. Furthermore, outcome measure question content varied between studies, for instance, outcome questions ranged from asking about the previous weekend UV exposure/protection to the pervious 12 months, type of behaviour measured, i.e., UV exposure (e.g., incidental and/or intentional, indoor tanning or sun exposure), protective behaviour (e.g., sunscreen, protective clothing and/or seeking shade) and measurement method (e.g., self-report and/or objective method). Thus the heterogeneity of question content is worth considering. It is clear that standardised measures are limited in this area and future researchers should consider developing more valid measures. There were a number of limitations to the studies that future research should address, many of which were noted by the authors themselves. All of the reviewed studies were conducted in the USA, a considerable percentage of the population were female, eight of the samples (67%) were university students and the samples were predominantly Caucasian. Thus, future research should assess whether such interventions are effective in different climate/regions (e.g., a worldwide sample) and on different populations. As the interventions are showing promise, younger age Health Psychology Review 109 groups should also be targeted to discourage the idea that a tan is attractive. However, out of the 12 studies reviewed, four of the studies were conducted by the same first author (Mahler), another potential source impacting upon the review findings. Additional caveats to note include the fact that only two studies made a priori estimate of sample size at the design stage. Thus, without such an estimate it is not clear whether the studies were adequately planned to detect for differences, and therefore the efficacy of the findings is potentially undermined. The majority of the studies included a follow-up component that ranged from 2 weeks to 12 months. Changes in UV protection behaviour were found at the follow-ups thus suggesting that the intervention has a long-term impact. However, one must acknowledge that the follow-up assessments predominantly relied on self-report measures of this behaviour. Reduction in UV exposure behaviour was not as successful at the followups. It was noted by some of the authors that if individuals were able to keep the intervention material (e.g., UV photo) as a continual reminder this may encourage a long-lasting behavioural change. This is a very plausible explanation and should be considered in future research. Before concluding, it is important to consider the aim of the review and the potential limitations. The possibility remains that even with the use of a multiple search strategy some studies may not have been identified. Furthermore, whilst studies with multiple components were identified and viewed carefully by the authors to assess the quantity/emphasis on appearance factors, omissions of possible relevant studies may have occurred. These should be considered when drawing conclusions from the review findings. In addition to addressing the improvement elements suggested above, the authors would further recommend that to truly assess the potential of appearance-focused interventions comparisons should be made with health-focused interventions. Finally, a systematic review evaluating the efficacy of the mediating variables (that were measured in many of the studies, e.g., attitudes towards UV exposure/ protection, perceived susceptibility to photoageing and skin cancer) on the interventions to understand behaviour change would be useful for health practitioners designing such interventions. In conclusion, the review findings suggest that appearance-focused interventions hold promise for motivating individuals to modify their tanning behaviour. Future research focusing on different populations and regions as well as designing adequately powered RCTs will provide a better insight into the effectiveness of such interventions. References American Cancer Society (2006). Cancer facts & figures. Retrieved from http://www.cancer. org/downloads/STT/CAFF2006PWSecured.pdf Barron, D., Kreiger, S., Butler, H.A., Mahler, H.I.M., Kulik, J., Gerrard, M., & Gibbon, F. (2007). Motivating safer sun protection behaviors through an appearance based intervention in two distinct regions. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33, S205S205. Baum, A., & Cohen, L. (1998). Successful behavioral interventions to prevent cancer: The example of skin cancer. Annual Review of Public Health, 19, 319333. Bellamy, R. (2005). A systematic review of educational interventions for promoting sun protection knowledge, attitudes and behaviour following the QUEST approach. Medical Teacher, 27, 269275. 110 L.J. Dodd and M.J. Forshaw Buller, D.B., & Borland, R. (1999). Skin cancer prevention for children: A critical review. Health Education & Behavior, 26, 317343. Cancer Council Australia (2009). Skin cancer facts & figures. Retrieved from http:// www.cancer.org.au//cancersmartlifestyle/SunSmart/Skincancerfactsandfigures.htm Cancer Research UK (2009a). Looking after your skin. Retrieved from http://www.cancerhelp. org.uk/help/default.asp?page 4368 Cancer Research UK (2009b). Skin cancer facts. Retrieved from http://info.cancerresearchuk. org/healthyliving/sunsmart/skin-cancer-facts/about-skin-cancer/ Cancer Research UK (2009c). UK skin cancer incidence statistics. Retrieved from http:// info.cancerresearchuk.org/cancerstats/types/skin/incidence/ Cancer Research UK (2009d). CancerStats key facts on skin cancer. Retrieved from http:// info.cancerresearchuk.org/cancerstats/types/skin/ Centre for Reviews and Dissemination (CRD) (2001). Report 4: Undertaking systematic reviews of research on effectiveness: CRD’s guidance for those carrying out or commissioning reviews (2nd ed.). York: The University of York. Clark-Carter, D. (2004). Quantitative psychological research a student’s handbook. Hove: Psychology Press. Cokkinides, V.E., Johnston-Davis, K., Weinstock, M., O’Connell, M.C., Kalsbeek, W., Thun, M.J., & Wingo, P.A. (2001). Sun exposure and sun-protection behaviors and attitudes among US youth, 11 to 18 years of age. Preventive Medicine, 33, 141151. Cummings, S., Tripp, M.K., & Herrmann, N.B. (1997). Approaches to the prevention and control of skin cancer. Cancer and Metastasis Review, 16, 309327. Ddadlani, C., & Orlow, S.J. (2008). Planning for a brighter future: A review of sun protection and barriers to behavioral change in children and adolescents. Dermatology Online Journal, 14, 114. Gibbons, F.X., Gerrard, M., Lane, D.J., Mahler, H.I.M., & Kulik, J.A. (2005). Using UV photography to reduce use of tanning booths: A test of cognitive mediation. Health Psychology, 24, 358363. Hart, K.M., & DeMarco, R.F. (2008). Primary prevention of skin cancer in children and adolescents: A review of the literature. Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing, 25, 6778. Higgins, J.P.T., & Green, S. (2008). Cochrane handbook for systematic reviews of interventions version 5.0.1. Retrieved from http://www.cochrane-handbook.org Hillhouse, J.J., & Turrisi, R. (2002). Examination of the efficacy of an appearance-focused intervention to reduce UV exposure. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25, 395409. Hillhouse, J.J., Turrisi, R., Holwiski, F., & McVeigh, S. (1999). An examination of psychological variables relevant to artificial tanning tendencies. Journal of Health Psychology, 4, 507516. Hillhouse, J.J., Turrisi, R., Stapleton, J., & Robinson, J. (2008). A randomized control trial of an appearance-focused intervention to prevent skin cancer. Cancer, 113, 32573266. ISD (2009). Cancer incidence and mortality data. Retrieved from http://www.isdscotland.org/ isd/183.html Jackson, K.M., & Aiken, L.S. (2006). Evaluation of a multicomponent appearance-based sunprotective intervention for young women: Uncovering the mechanisms of program efficacy. Health Psychology, 25, 3446. Jacobsen, P.B., Donovan, K.A., Vadaparamphil, S.T., & Small, B.J. (2007). Systematic review and meta-analysis of psychological and activity-based interventions for cancer-related fatigue. Health Psychology, 26, 660667. Jones, J.L., & Leary, M.R. (1994). Effects of appearance-based admonitions against sun exposure on tanning intentions in young adults. Health Psychology, 13, 8690. Lazovich, D., & Forster, J. (2005). Indoor tanning by adolescents: Prevalence, practices and policies. European Journal of Cancer, 41, 2027. Mahler, H.I.M., Fitzpatrick, B., Parker, P., & Lapin, A. (1997). The relative effects of a healthbased versus an appearance-based intervention designed to increase sunscreen use. American Journal of Health Promotion, 11, 426429. Mahler, H.I.M., Kulik, J.A., Butler, H.A., Gerrard, M., & Gibbons, F.X. (2008). Social norms information enhances the efficacy of an appearance-based sun protection intervention. Social Science & Medicine, 67, 321329. Health Psychology Review 111 Mahler, H.I.M., Kulik, J.A., Gerrard, M., & Gibbons, F.X. (2006). Effects of two appearancebased interventions on the sun protection behaviors of Southern California beach patrons. Basic and Applied Psychology, 28, 263272. Mahler, H.I.M., Kulik, J.A., Gerrard, M., & Gibbons, F.X. (2007). Long-term effects of appearance-based interventions on sun protection behaviors. Health Psychology, 26, 350360. Mahler, H.I.M., Kulik, J.A., Harrell, J., Correa, A., & Gibbons, F.X. (2005). Effects of UV photographs, photoaging information and use of sunless tanning lotion on sun protection behaviors. Archives Dermatology, 141, 373380. McClendon, B.T., & Prentice-Dunn, S. (2001). Reducing skin cancer risk: An intervention based on protection motivation theory. Journal of Health Psychology, 6, 321328. Miles, A., Waller, J., Hiom, S., & Swanston, D. (2005). SunSmart? Skin cancer knowledge and preventive behaviour in a British population representative sample. Health Education Research, 20, 579585. Miller, A.G., Ashton, W.A., McHoskey, J.W., & Gimbel, J. (1990). What price attractiveness? Stereotype and risk factors in suntanning behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 20, 12721300. Morris, J., & Elwood, M. (1996). Sun exposure modification programmes and their evaluation: A review of the literature. Health Promotion International, 11, 321332. Northern Ireland Cancer Registry (2009). Cancer incidence and mortality. Retrieved from http://www.qub.ac.uk/research-centres/nicr/Data/OnlineStatistics/ Novick, M. (1997). To burn or not to burn: Use of computer-enhanced stimuli to encourage application of sunscreens. CUTIS, 60, 105108. Office for National Statistics (ONS) (2009). Cancer statistics: Registrations. Retrieved from http://www.statistics.gov.uk/downloads/theme_health/MB1-37/MB1_37_2006.pdf Olson, A.L., Gaffney, C.A., Starr, P., & Dietrich, A.J. (2008). The impact of an appearancebased educational intervention on adolescent intention to use sunscreen. Health Education Research, 23, 763769. Pagoto, S., McChargue, D., & Fuqua, W.R. (2003). Effects of a multicomponent intervention on motivation and sun protection behaviors among midwestern beachgoers. Health Psychology, 22, 429433. Peacey, V., Steptoe, A., Sanderman, R., & Wardle, J. (2006). Ten-year changes in sun protection behaviors and beliefs of young adults in 13 European countries. Preventive Medicine, 43, 460465. Ryan, R., Hill, S., Broclain, D., Horey, D., Oliver, S., & Prictor, M. (2007). Cochrane consumers and communication review group. Study quality guide. Retrieved from www.latrobe.edu.au/ cochrane/resources.html Saraiya, M., Glanz, K., Briss, P.A., Nichols, P., White, C., Das, D., et al. (2004). Interventions to prevent skin cancer by reducing exposure to ultraviolet radiation: A systematic review. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 27, 422466. Weinstock, M.A., & Rossi, J.S. (1998). The rhode island sun smart project: A scientific approach to skin cancer prevention. Clinics in Dermatology, 16, 411413. World Health Organisation (WHO) (2009a). Health effects of UV radiation. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/uv/health/uv_health2/en/index.html World Health Organisation (WHO) (2009b). How common is skin cancer. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/uv/faq/skincancer/en/index1.html World Health Organisation (WHO) (2009c). Who is the most at risk of getting skin cancer? Retrieved from http://www.who.int/uv/faq/skincancer/en/index2.html World Health Organisation (WHO) (2009d). Artificial tanning sunbeds: Risk and guidance. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/uv/publications/sunbedpubl/en/index.html