Topics Enculturation By Dr. Priyadarshini • Socialization & enculturation • Parenting • Peer influence • Math achievement Department of Psychology Sunway University Socialization & Enculturation Socialization - The process by which we learn and internalize the rules and patterns of society in which we live. This process probably starts from the very first day of life and occurs over a long period of time. Enculturation - A process by which youngsters learn and adopt the ways and manners of their specific culture. Products of the socialization process (i.e., the subjective, underlying psychological aspects of culture that become internalized through development). It involves learning and mastering cultural norms, attitudes, values and belief system. Actual process and mechanisms by which people learn the rules of society and culture (e.g., what is said to whom and in which contexts). Examples: 1. Using the words lu and gua in Indonesia. 2. Right-hand minded in Asian culture. 3. Food serving. • Socialization (and enculturation) agents are the people, institutions, and organizations. • Parents are the first and most important Help instill and reinforce cultural tradition and values in their children when they are learned and practiced well and correcting mistakes in that learning. • What about you? Other agents are siblings, extended families, friends, and peers. • School, church, and social groups (e.g., Boy or Girl Scouts). 1 Microsytem – the immediate surrounding, such as family, school, peer group, with which children directly interact. • Mesosystem – the linkages between microsystem, such as between school and family (e.g., SES level). Human development is a dynamic, interactive process between individuals and various ecologies (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). Exosystem – the context that indirectly affects children, such as parents’ workplace (e.g., Daycare centre). Macrosystem – culture, religion, society. Chronosystem – the influence of time on the other systems (e.g., Era of technology). We are not simply socialized by our families, peer groups, and educational and religion institution; we also contribute to our own development by affecting the people and contexts around us. • • Parenting goals & beliefs • Parenting goals provide the motivation and framework for what parents think is the best way to raise their children. • Middle-class mothers in Berlin, Germany vs. New Delhi, India (Keller, 2010). • Germans = autonomous. A separate person with unique thoughts, wishes, desires and needs. Focus on objects. • Indians = autonomous but closely interdependent with other people, especially family. Emphasize physical contact, emotional closeness and indulgence. Focus on interaction. Parents’ beliefs concerning their role as caregivers also influence their behaviors. Parents in the U.S. believe that they play a very active, goal-directed role in the development of their children (Coll, 1990; Goodnow, 1988). Traditional Turkish mothers believing that their children “grow up” rather than are “brought up” (Kagitcibasi, 1996) This range of parenting beliefs will be reflected in how parents interact with their children • E.g., Whether parents share cultural knowledge primarily through verbalization and direct instruction or by observation and imitation. • Parental ethnotheories (parental cultural beliefs system) – “a basis for guiding parenting practices that structure children’s daily lives” (Harkness & Super, 2006). • Family day? Family dinner? Americans – special time with each children (e.g., play baseball with the boy and watch their ballet recital for the girl). Dutch – family time with their children (e.g., sitting down for dinner every night). 2 Parenting styles Baumrind (1971) three major patterns of parenting: 1. Authoritarian - expect unquestioned obedience and Ethnotheories motivate and shape what parents think is the “right” way to parent their children. view the child as needing to be controlled. They have also been described as being low on warmth and responsiveness toward their children. 2. Permissive - warm and nurturing to their children, however, they allow their children to regulate their own lives and provide few firm guidelines. 3. Authoritative - sensitive to the child’s maturity and are firm, fair, and reasonable. They also express a high degree of warmth and affection to their children. Authoritarian Maccoby & Martin (1983) 4. Uninvolved - often too absorbed in their own lives to respond appropriately to their children and may seem indifferent to them. They do not seem committed to caregiving, beyond the minimum effort required to meet the physical needs of their child. An extreme form of this type of parenting is neglect. Authoritative Permissive Which of these styles is optimal for a child’s development? • American children seem to do well with the authoritative parenting style. • Children of authoritarian parents - more anxious and withdrawn, lacking spontaneity and intellectual curiosity. • Children of permissive parents - immature; they have difficulty controlling their impulses and acting independently. • Children of uninvolved parents - fare the worst, being noncompliant and demanding. Demonstrate more positive mood, self-reliance, selfconfidence, higher emotional and social skills, and secure attachment to caregivers. Promote psychologically healthy, competent, independent children who are cooperative and at ease in social situations. Higher school achievement for European American, African American, and Hispanic American adolescents, but not for Asian Americans. 3 Authoritarian However, Baumrind’s parenting styles may not adequately capture parenting in other cultures. Permissive • Chinese parents have been often described as authoritarian. • However, the significance and meaning attached to this parenting style may originate from a set of cultural beliefs that may differ greatly from the European American cultural belief system (Chao, 1994; Gorman, 1998). • Based on Confucian philosophy, Chinese parenting may be distinguished by the concept of ‘training’ ( jiao xun) in child rearing. Authoritative Culture & peers How much do your peers influence your development? Margaret Mead (1978) culture types with differing levels of peer influence on the socialization of its young people: 1. Post-figurative cultures – Cultural change is slow and socialization occurs primarily by elders transferring their knowledge to their children. 2. Configurative Cultures – Cultural changes occurs more rapidly, adults continue to socialize their children, but peers play a greater role in socializing each other. 3. Pre-figurative cultures – Culture is changing so rapidly that young people are the ones to teach adults. The knowledge that adults hold may not be sufficient for the next generation and adults may need to look to younger people for advice and information. Social and cultural factors that influence Math achievement Exposure to peer groups • American teenagers spend more hours (18hrs) with their peers compared with Japanese (12hrs) and Taiwanese (8 hrs) (Fuligni & Stevenson, 1995). • Children from solitary farm settlement have limited options to interact with a wide range of potential playmates. • Children from hunting/gathering society may have peers from multi-age groups instead of the same-age group (e.g., U.S. age stratified schooling system). • Depending on the culture, the extent to which children interact with their peers may be quite significant. • Cross-national differences in math achievement are related to secondary (i.e., culture) rather than primary (i.e., inherent – natural abilities that are shaped by evolutionary processes that all people presumably share) math abilities (Geary, 1996). 4 Cross-national differences among Chinese, Japanese, and American children in counting and memory exercises may be largely a function of differences in those languages related to counting and numbers (Stigler et al., 1986). • Possible contributing factors are: Language School systems Parental and familial values Attitudes & appraisals of students Teaching styles & Teacher-student relationships Mandarin Japanese 1 = ichi 11 = juchi 1 = yi 11 = shi yi 2 = ni 12 = juni 2 = er 12 = shi er 3 = san etc. 3 = san etc. • The Japanese language has unique verbal labels only for the numbers 1 through 10. Number 11 is then “ten-one,” 12 is “ten-two” and so forth. • English, however, has unique labels for number 1 through 19 as well as decade numbers (20, 30,40..) Research has shown that East-Asian students make fewer errors than Americans in counting, and understand some basic math concepts related to counting and numbers better (Miller et al., 2005; Miura et al., 1999). Research has shown that the educational system which children take part plays an important role in producing cross-national differences in math abilities, while at the same time imparting cultural values. • The content of what is taught in the school reflects a priori choices by that culture or society regarding what it believes is important to learn. Different cultures believes different topics to be important for later success in that society. 4 = si 4 = yon 5 = go 20 = niju 5 = wu 20 = er shi 6 = roku 21 = nijuichi 6 = liu 21 = er shi yi • Environmental setting in which education occurs (Formal – schools vs. informal – taught by the elders). • Organization, planning and implementation of lesson plans (e.g., praises, mistake focused, special classes). 7 = qi 7 = nana 8 = hachi 30 = sanju 8 = ba 30 = san shi 9 = kyu 31 = sanju ichi 9 = jiu 31 = san shi yi 10 = ju 10 = shi Cultural differences in parenting beliefs about education have an impact on children’s education experiences (Chao, 2000). • Japanese and Chinese parents consider all children as equal, with no differences between them; U.S. parents recognize differences and find reasons to treat their children as special. • U.S. parents consider innate ability more important than effort; Japanese and Chinese, effort is far more important than ability (Stevenson & Zusho, 2002). • U.S. parents tend to be more easily satisfied at lower levels of competence than either the Japanese or the Chinese. Students around the world approach their academic work with quite different worldviews, attitudes, and attributional styles. • Asian American middle-school students exhibited a stronger desire to please parents, greater parental pressure, but also higher levels of parental support, than did the European American students (Pang, 1991). • American undergraduate and graduate students attributed academic achievement more often to ability than did Asian students (Yan & Gaier, 1994). American students also believed that effort was more important for success than lack of effort was for failure, whereas Asian students considered effort equally important for success or failure. 5 • Differences in the use of classroom time appear to underlie math performance differences. • The Japanese and Chinese spend more days per year in school, more hours per day in school, a greater proportion of time in school devoted to purely academic subjects, and a greater proportion of time devoted to math (Takahasi & Takeuchi, 2006). American teachers tend to use praise to reward correct responses. Teachers in Japan & Taiwan tend to focus on incorrect answers, using them as examples to lead into discussions of the computational process and math concepts. These teaching differences is in line with the cultural emphasis in the U.S. on rewarding uniqueness and individualism & the emphasis in Japan and China on finding ways to engage in group process and sharing responsibility for mistakes with members of the group. END 6