Psychopharmacology https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-021-05991-9 ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION Low doses of LSD reduce broadband oscillatory power and modulate event‑related potentials in healthy adults Conor H. Murray1 · Ilaria Tare1 · Claire M. Perry1 · Michael Malina1 · Royce Lee1 · Harriet de Wit1 Received: 28 May 2021 / Accepted: 20 September 2021 © The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2021 Abstract Rationale Classical psychedelics, including psilocybin and lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), are under investigation as potential therapeutic agents in psychiatry. Whereas most studies utilize relatively high doses, there are also reports of beneficial effects of “microdosing,” or repeated use of very low doses of these drugs. The behavioral and neural effects of these low doses are not fully understood. Objectives To examine the effects of LSD (13 μg and 26 μg) versus placebo on resting-state electroencephalography (EEG) and event-related potential (ERP) responses in healthy adults. Methods Twenty-two healthy men and women, 18 to 35 years old, participated in 3 EEG sessions in which they received placebo or LSD (13 μg and 26 μg) under double-blind conditions. During each session, participants completed drug effect and mood questionnaires at hourly intervals, and physiological measures were recorded. During expected peak drug effect, EEG recordings were obtained, including resting-state neural oscillations in scalp electrodes over default mode network (DMN) regions and P300, N170, and P100 ERPs evoked during a visual oddball paradigm. Results LSD dose-dependently reduced oscillatory power across delta, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma frequency bands during both eyes closed and eyes open resting conditions. During the oddball task, LSD dose-dependently reduced ERP amplitudes for P300 and N170 components and increased P100 latency. LSD also produced dose-related increases in positive mood, elation, energy, and anxiety and increased heart rate and blood pressure. On a measure of altered states of consciousness, LSD dose-dependently increased Blissful State, but not other indices of perceptual or sensory effects typical of psychedelic drugs. The subjective effects of the drug were not correlated with the EEG measures. Conclusions Low doses of LSD produced broadband cortical desynchronization over the DMN during resting state and reduced P300 and N170 amplitudes, patterns similar to those reported with higher doses of psychedelics. Notably, these neurophysiological effects raise the possibility that very low doses of LSD may produce subtle behavioral and perhaps therapeutic effects that do not rely on the full psychedelic experience. Keywords LSD · EEG · ERP · Psychedelic · Microdose Introduction The past decade has seen a resurgence of interest in human psychedelic research. Several clinical trials for treatment of anorexia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, addictions, and depression have been conducted, using single, relatively high doses of psilocybin and LSD (Nutt and * Harriet de Wit hdew@uchicago.edu 1 Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, 5841 S Maryland Ave MC3077, Chicago, IL 60637, USA Carhart-Harris, 2021). However, there have also been widely publicized anecdotal reports on the beneficial effects of “microdosing”—a practice of taking repeated, very low doses of LSD (10–15 μg) every few days to improve mood and cognition (Fadiman, 2011; Johnstad, 2018; Waldman, 2018; Fadiman and Korb, 2019; Kuypers et al., 2019; Polito and Stevenson, 2019; Kuypers, 2020). Initial studies indicate that these low doses of LSD produce detectable, but modest, effects (Bershad et al., 2019; Yanakieva et al., 2019; Family et al., 2020; Szigeti et al., 2021). Controlled laboratory studies indicate that single low doses increase ratings of vigor (Bershad et al., 2019), decrease attentional lapses (Hutten et al., 2020) and affect time perception (Yanakieva et al., 13 Vol.:(0123456789) Psychopharmacology 2019), but have few other effects on mood and do not impair cognition or proprioception (Family et al., 2020). A “selfblinding” study conducted outside the laboratory detected modest reductions in anxiety (Szigeti et al., 2021). Yet, even in the absence of pronounced behavioral effects, it is possible that very low doses LSD subtly change neural function, which ultimately affects behavior and perhaps contributes to users’ positive reports. In this study, we examine the neural signature of low doses of LSD using EEG. LSD is a partial agonist that acts at serotonin receptors, with some affinity to dopamine receptors (De Gregorio et al., 2018). Although LSD and psilocybin bind to several targets in the brain, their subjective and neurophysiological effects are thought to be predominantly driven by activation of the 5-HT2a receptor (Quednow et al., 2012; Preller et al., 2018; Preller et al., 2019). Upon activation, the 5-HT2a receptor facilitates release of the excitatory neurotransmitter glutamate and enhances overall excitability of cortical networks (Aghajanian and Marek, 1997; Beique et al., 2007; Nichols, 2016). Several studies have investigated the effects of psychedelic drugs on functional connectivity in the brain using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), most at relatively high doses. For instance, 100 μg LSD increased connectivity from the thalamus to cortical regions (Preller et al., 2019), while both 75 μg LSD and moderate doses of psilocybin increased measures of global brain connectivity (Lebedev et al., 2015; Tagliazucchi et al., 2016). In one study, increased “complexity” in connectivity after 75 μg LSD was correlated with subjective effects (Luppi et al., 2021). Together, resting-state fMRI studies indicate that classical psychedelic drugs increase functional connectivity across networks while decreasing connectivity within networks (Carhart-Harris et al., 2012; Carhart-Harris et al., 2016; Tagliazucchi et al., 2016; Mueller et al., 2017). This pattern, labeled neural “entropy,” is correlated with psychedelic drug-induced increases in trait openness (Lebedev et al., 2016). Some authors have suggested that this pattern represents a dismantling of maladaptive neuropsychological processes that underlies the drugs’ therapeutic effects (Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2019). Little is known about these neural effects at very low doses. In one of few such studies, LSD (13 μg) increased connectivity between the amygdala and middle frontal gyrus, and this was correlated with positive mood after the drug (Bershad et al., 2020). Dampened amygdala responses and connectivity to the frontal lobe have also been observed after 100 μg LSD and psilocybin (Mueller et al., 2017; Grimm et al., 2018). Thus, low and higher doses of psychedelic drugs may produce similar effects on connectivity. Another way to study neural effects of psychedelic drugs is using EEG or magnetoencephalography (MEG). This technology provides better temporal resolution than 13 fMRI, but the primary measures are obtained from the surface of the brain. EEG measures are used to assess both resting-state oscillations and event-related potentials (ERPs). Several studies have examined effects of higher doses of psychedelic drugs on EEG. LSD (75 μg) and moderate doses of psilocybin reduce broadband cortical oscillatory power during resting state, particularly within the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013; Kometer et al., 2015; Carhart-Harris et al., 2016). Decreases in oscillatory power, particularly at lower frequencies, typically indicate greater neural function (Klimesch et al., 2007; Schroeder and Lakatos, 2009; Buschman et al., 2012). Several researchers have examined associations between EEG measures and behavioral effects. In one study (Carhart-Harris et al., 2016), LSD (75 μg) decreased alpha power in the occipital region, and this was correlated with intensity of visual imagery. The authors also reported that decreased alpha power in the PCC was correlated with reports of ego-dissolution. This is consistent with the fact that the PCC is a major hub of the default mode network (DMN), a major resting-state network implicated in self-referential processing (Raichle, 2015). ERPs are waveforms elicited in response to stimuli, including infrequent or “oddball” audio tones or visual images. Changes in the amplitudes and latencies of the components of these waveforms are taken to indicate changes in neural information processing. Several studies have examined effects of psychedelic drugs on ERPs. In one study, psilocybin (0.26 mg/kg) reduced amplitudes of perceptual (P100) and cognitive (P300) components during an auditory oddball task (Bravermanova et al., 2018), and in another study, the same drug reduced N170 responses to fearful, but not happy faces, in healthy subjects (Schmidt et al., 2013). Overall, these EEG findings are consistent with fMRI findings suggesting that psychedelic drugs increase neural complexity while reducing event-related neural responses. To our knowledge, however, the effects of very low doses on EEG measures have not been studied. Studying the effects of low doses of LSD on neural function, such as those used in microdosing, is important for several reasons. First, understanding how the drug alters brain function will help us to understand and study the possible therapeutic effects of the drug. Second, such studies will help to understand the relationships between behavioral or subjective drug effects and the drugs’ effects on neural systems. Such basic information will help to guide future drug discovery. Thus, the present study was designed to examine resting-state EEG and ERP responses in healthy adults after 13 μg and 26 μg LSD and to examine the relationship between these neural effects and the drug’s subjective effects. Psychopharmacology Methods Study design This study used a within-subject, double-blind design to test effects of low doses of LSD on mood and EEG. Healthy young adults participated in three 5-h sessions in which they received, in randomized order, placebo, 13, or 26 μg of LSD (sublingual). Subjective mood states and cardiovascular measures were recorded before drug administration and at 60-min intervals after drug administration. During the time of peak drug effect, 120 to 180 min after drug administration, EEG recordings were obtained to assess broadband oscillatory activity during resting state and event-related potential responses to infrequent (oddball) stimuli. The primary outcome measures were broadband oscillatory power during resting state and event-related potentials (ERP) during an oddball task. Subjects Healthy subjects (N = 22, 8 women) 18 to 35 years of age participated. They were screened for physical and psychiatric health with a physical examination, electrocardiogram, modified Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-5, and selfreported health and drug-use history. Inclusion criteria were English fluency, right-handedness, at least a high school education, body mass index of 18 to 32 kg/m2, and at least one prior use of a classical psychedelic drug (e.g., LSD, psilocybin, N,N-dimethyl-tryptamine [DMT]) or 3,4-methylenediox-ymethamphetamine (MDMA). Exclusion criteria were a history of psychosis, severe posttraumatic stress disorder or panic disorder, past-year substance use disorder (except nicotine), pregnant or nursing, working night shifts, regular medication aside from birth control, adverse reaction to a psychedelic drug, or unwillingness to use this type of drug again. Subjects provided written, informed consent prior to beginning the study, which was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Biological Sciences Division of The University of Chicago. Procedure before and 24 h after each session, from cannabis for 7 days before and 24 h after each session, and from alcohol for 24 h before and 12 h after each session. They were permitted to consume their normal amounts of caffeine and nicotine up to 3 h before sessions. Subjects were instructed to have a normal night’s sleep and fast for 12 h before the sessions. To minimize drug-specific expectancies, subjects were told they might receive a placebo, stimulant, sedative, or hallucinogenic drug. Experimental sessions Subjects attended three 5-h sessions from 8 am to 1 pm, separated by at least 7 days. Compliance to drug abstention was verified by urinalysis (CLIAwaived Instant Drug Test Cup, San Diego, CA; amphetamine, cocaine, oxycodone, THC, PCP, MDMA, opiates, benzodiazepines, barbiturates, methadone, methamphetamine, buprenorphine) and breath alcohol testing (Alcosensor III, Intoximeters, St. Louis, MO). Female subjects provided urine samples for pregnancy tests and were tested at any phase of the menstrual cycle. Subjects received a granola bar as a standardized breakfast. Pre-drug measures of subjective state and cardiovascular function were obtained, and then LSD (13 or 26 μg, tartrate solution in water) or placebo (water) was administered sublingually under double-blind conditions. Participants held the solution under the tongue without swallowing for 60 s, under observation of the research assistant. Subjective and cardiovascular measures were taken at 60, 120, 180, and 240 min. EEG recordings began 120 min after drug administration and lasted about 60 min. After the final time point at 240 min, subjects completed end-of-session questionnaires and were discharged. Drug The drug was manufactured by Organix and was prepared in solution with tartaric acid by the University of Chicago Investigational Pharmacy. Drug solution (or water) was administered in a volume of 0.5 mL. The doses were selected to be below the threshold for hallucinatory effects (Bershad et al., 2019) and within the range that is used in naturalistic settings (Polito and Stevenson, 2019). Drug effect measures Orientation session Subjects attended an orientation session to review the protocol, provide informed consent, receive presession instructions, and practice study tasks and questionnaires. They completed the DASS-21, a 21-item Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (Lovibond and Lovibond, 1995). They were instructed to abstain from drugs and medications for 48 h Mood states and subjective drug effects were assessed before and at regular intervals after drug administration using the Drug Effects Questionnaire (DEQ) (Fischman and Foltin, 1991; Morean et al., 2013), the Addiction Research Center Inventory (ARCI) (Haertzen et al., 1963; Martin et al., 1971), and Profile of Mood States during each session (POMS) (McNair et al., 1971). The DEQ consists of 5 13 Psychopharmacology questions assessing subjective drug effects using 100-mm visual analog scales: Do you feel a drug effect?, Do you like the drug effect?, Do you feel high?, Do you want more of what you received?, and Do you dislike the drug effect? The ARCI consists of 49 true/false questions measuring typical drug effects: amphetamine-like (ARCI-A; stimulant effects); benzedrine group (ARCI-BG; energy and intellectual efficiency); morphine-benzedrine group (ARCI-MBG; euphoric effects); LSD (ARCI-LSD; hallucinogen-like effects); and pentobarbital-chlorpromazine-alcohol group (ARCI-PCAG; sedative effects). The POMS consist of 72 mood adjectives rated on a Likert scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely), divided into 8 subscales, friendliness, anxiety, elation, anger, fatigue, depression, confusion, and vigor, and two composite scales: positive mood (elation minus depression) and arousal (vigor plus anxiety minus confusion plus fatigue). At the end of each session, subjects also completed an end-of-session drug identification questionnaire and the 5 Dimensions of Altered States of Consciousness (5D-ASC) questionnaire (Dittrich, 1998). Blood pressure and heart rate were monitored every 60 min using portable blood pressure cuffs (Critikon Dinamap Plus; GE Healthcare Technologies, Waukesha, WI). EEG measures EEG acquisition EEG recordings were collected using a 128 sintered Ag/ AgCl active electrodes (ActiveTwo™ system, BioSemi B.V., Amsterdam) placed according to equiradial layout on the head cap. Additional electrodes were placed at reference locations of the mastoids, around the eye to detect eye blinks, and on the chest to detect EKG artifacts (8 peripheral electrodes in total). The analog-to-digital box receiving the electrode leads was battery powered to electrically isolate participants. EEG data was acquired continuously, amplified, and digitized using Biosemi ActiveView software. Digitization of electrode placement reflecting actual head shape was conducted using a Patriot™ Digitizer Stylus (Polhemus Co., Colchester VT) and Locator software (Source Signal Imaging, Inc., San Diego CA). The stylus touches each electrode site until registered by the software (5–10 min total). EEG recordings occurred in a sound attenuated room, with the subject sitting comfortably. EEG recordings were high-pass filtered (1 Hz) and low-pass filtered (60 Hz, −12 dB/octave) to remove extraneous high and low frequency noise. EEG and electrooculogram (EOG) signals were processed by voltage-controlled amplifiers and digitized (16 bit/500 Hz sampling rate) for storage and analysis. Data was processed offline based on data stored on computer workstation hard drives. 13 Resting state EEG data were continuously recorded, while participants rested comfortably with eyes closed for 5 min, followed by eyes open for 5 min with eyes rested on a fixation cross. Subjects were instructed to minimize excessive blinking and head movements and remain awake. The primary outcome measure was oscillatory power across five frequency bands (delta, 1–4 Hz; theta, 4–8 Hz; alpha 8–13 Hz; beta 13–30 Hz; gamma 30–80 Hz). Scalp electrodes chosen for the analysis (Supp. Fig. 1) were selected to reflect default mode network hubs (medial prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, left temporoparietal cortex, and right temporoparietal cortex) observed in simultaneous fMRIEEG studies (Neuner et al., 2014). Emotional faces oddball task An oddball task was used to assess event-related potentials (ERP). In the task, subjects viewed frequent happy faces and infrequent neutral and angry faces in a 3:1 ratio (Eckman and Friesen, 1976). The neutral and angry faces depicted the same 2 models and were presented in separate blocks (i.e., all neutral or all angry) with order counterbalanced across participants. Participants were instructed to press the left button if they observed a happy face and the right button if they observed a non-happy face. EEG data from incorrect trials were discarded. Stimuli were displayed on a computer screen for 1500 ms, followed by a blank screen for 1000 ms, followed by a fixation cross for 1000 ms (Supp. Fig. 2). A total of 320 face stimuli were presented. Total task length was 15 min. The primary outcome measures were the amplitudes and latencies of task-evoked ERPs. For our purposes, the main analysis was the response to infrequent facial expressions, and in secondary analyses, we compared the rare neutral to rare angry faces. Data analysis Subjective and cardiovascular effects Visual inspection of the time course of each measure indicated that the effects of LSD peaked 2–3 h after administration, and the time course was similar for all measures examined. To reduce the number of analyses, peak change scores were calculated, subtracting the pre-drug values from the highest or lowest value in each session. These scores were analyzed using a one-way repeated measures analysis of variance (drug dose), with follow-up contrasts to compare each dose with placebo. Psychopharmacology EEG preprocessing Results Data were preprocessed using the EEGLAB extension v2021.0 (Delorme & Makeig, 2004) for MATLAB (Mathworks, Inc.). Data were filtered with a low-pass filter of 1 and a high-pass filter of 60 Hz. Channels and periods of continuous data containing gross movement artifacts were selected and removed after manual inspection, and data were re-referenced to the average reference. Gross movements were defined by interruptions of signal that occurred across all electrodes in continuous data. Other artifactual noise, including eye blink, eye movement, muscle, or EKG related artifacts, were removed after independent component analysis (ICA) based on topography and morphology of ICA components. After ICA, peripheral electrodes were removed from analysis. Analysts were blind to drug condition during preprocessing steps to prevent bias during manual inspection. Demographic characteristics Resting‑state analyses Following ICA and removal of artifactual noise, oscillatory power in the delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), and gamma (30–80 Hz) bands was calculated using fast Fourier transform on scalp electrodes during eyes closed or eyes open resting state. Scalp electrodes chosen for the analysis (Supp. Fig. 1) were selected to correspond to default mode network hubs as observed in simultaneous fMRI-EEG studies (Neuner et al., 2014). For either eyes closed or eyes open, repeated measures analysis of variance was performed across each of the four default mode network regions and across the five frequency bands with dose as within-subject factor. ERP analyses Following ICA, data were epoched from −500 to 1000 ms around stimulus triggers. Trials with incorrect behavioral responses were discarded. Data files were analyzed using the EEGLAB Study function with multiple designs. ERP analyses were performed on Pz and PO 10 electrodes, selected a priori for analysis of the P300 component (parietal (Pz)), and visual processing components N170 and P100 (right parietooccipital (PO 10)), the latter chosen based on proximity to the fusiform face gyrus and N170 findings in the literature (Gao et al., 2019). For each component, peak amplitude and latency to peak amplitude were extracted and exported to SPSS software (version 25; SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL) for statistical analysis. Repeated measures analyses of variance were performed on either amplitude or latency from each subject, with dose as within-subject factor. Most subjects were in their 20s (mean age 25), with some college education (mean education: 15 years) (Table 1). Most reported current use of cannabis and alcohol, and their mean lifetime use of psychedelic drugs was about 11 times. Drug response measures LSD (13 and 26 μg) dose-dependently increased subjective (Fig. 1, Supp. Table 1) and cardiovascular measures (Fig. 2) over the course of the 5-h session. To illustrate the time course of effects on a representative value, Fig. 1 includes mean values on DEQ Feel Drug for each time point. Other rating scales and cardiovascular measures had a similar time course. The time course indicates that the EEG measures were obtained during the peak time of the drug effect. LSD significantly increased ratings on the DEQ Feel Drug (Fig. 1A), Feel High, Like Drug, and Want More (Supp. Fig. 3). LSD also significantly Table 1 Demographics and drug use characteristics of the participants in the study Category n or mean ± SD (range) Subjects (male/female) Age, years Education, years Body mass index, kg/m2 Race Caucasian African American Asian Other/>1 race DASS-21 Depression Anxiety Stress Current drug use in past month Cannabis, times/month (n = 17) Alcohol, drinks/week (n = 20) Alcohol, drinking days/week Caffeine, servings/day (n = 18) Tobacco, times/day (n = 2) Total lifetime drug use, nonmedical Psychedelic (n = 22) MDMA (n = 8) Stimulant (n = 8) Opiate (n = 5) 22 (14/8) 25 ± 5 (19–33) 15 ± 1 (14–18) 22.5 ± 2.9 (18–28.9) 18 1 1 2 5.0 ± 3.5 (0–14) 2.6 ± 3.5 (0–15) 5.9 ± 4.2 (0–16) 8.8 ± 8.7 (0–25) 4.7 ± 4.1 (0–17) 2.2 ± 1.5 (0–5) 1.1 ± 1.1 (0–3.5) 0.4 ± 1.8 (0–8.5) 10.9 ± 20.8 (1–100) 1.6 ± 3.9 (0–16) 24.3 ± 106.3 (0–500) 2.8 ± 8.8 (0–30) 13 Psychopharmacology Fig. 1 Mean (± SEM) subjective effects ratings after placebo (0 μg), 13 and 26 μg LSD. The top left panel shows mean scores on “Feel Drug” each hour after drug administration (A). Other scales showed a similar time course. Other panels show mean change from baseline scores for the Drug Effects Questionnaire (A), Profile of Mood States (B), and Addiction Research Center Inventory (C). ARCI-LSD: hallucinogen-like effects; ARCI-A: stimulant effects; ARCI-MBG: euphoric effects; ARCI-BG: energy and intellectual efficiency. Significant main effects of dose were obtained for all scales shown; the asterisks show which means differed significantly from placebo using independent follow-up t-tests (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001). increased Elation, Anxiety, and Positive Mood on the POMS (Fig. 1B), and on the ARCI, it increased measures of LSD-like effects (ARCI-LSD), amphetamine-like effects (ARCI-A), euphoric effects (ARCI-MBG), and energy and intellectual efficiency (ARCI-BG) (Fig. 1C, Supp. Fig. 3). Full time course and peak change from baseline data, including non-significant responses, are shown in Supp. Fig. 3. On cardiovascular measures, LSD increased heart rate and systolic and diastolic blood pressure (Fig. 2) (heart rate: dose, F1,21 = 5.11, p = 0.035; systolic BP: dose, F1,21 = 8.16, p = 0.009; 26 μg vs. placebo, p = 0.036; diastolic BP: dose, F1,21 = 4.72, p = 0.041). EEG recordings 13 During eyes closed resting state, LSD reduced broadband oscillatory power across four major hubs of the default mode network (medial prefrontal, posterior cingulate, and left and right temporoparietal cortices) and five frequency bands (delta [1–4 Hz], theta [4–8 Hz], alpha [8–13 Hz], beta [13–30 Hz], and gamma [30–80 Hz]) (Fig. 3A) (dose, F1,420 = 10.39, p = 0.001). A secondary analysis examining power for each of the four regions (Supp. Fig. 4) indicated that the effects of LSD were most pronounced with electrodes over the posterior cingulate cortex (dose, F1,105 = 15.71, p < 0.001). The drug also reduced broadband power for left Psychopharmacology Fig. 2 Mean (± SEM) cardiovascular measures after placebo (0 μg), 13 and 26 μg LSD. LSD increased heart rate, systolic BP, and diastolic BP (repeated measures ANOVA). The asterisk indicates significant difference between placebo and LSD using follow-up independent t-tests (*p < 0.05). E) (dose, F1,42 = 11.13, p = 0.002). No significant dose × face condition interactions were found for LSD’s effects on amplitude, latency, or error rate. Across all scalp electrodes, LSD attenuated electropositive and electronegative potentials associated with the P300 and N170 ERPs, respectively, as shown in scalp topographies (Fig. 5A, B). End of session questionnaires Fig. 3 Effects of LSD (13 and 26 μg) and placebo on oscillatory power during eyes closed resting state in the default mode network. Data are expressed as mean ± SEM. The drug decreased power across all frequency bands (repeated measures ANOVA; main effect of dose, p < 0.05; no interaction with frequency band). Follow-up independent t-tests compared each LSD dose to placebo (*p < 0.05). and right temporoparietal cortices (left: dose, F1,105 = 8.61, p = 0.004); however, the right temporoparietal cortex was only marginally affected (right: dose × band, F1,105 = 3.76, p = 0.055). This pattern of reduced oscillatory power also extended to the eyes open resting-state condition (Supp. Fig. 5) (dose, F1,420 = 5.78, p = 0.017). During eyes open, the drug decreased broadband oscillatory power in the posterior cingulate cortex (dose, F1,105 = 5.96, p = 0.016), in addition to the medial prefrontal cortex (dose, F1,105 = 8.62, p = 0.004), but not left or right temporoparietal cortices. In the oddball task, LSD dose-dependently reduced error rates (dose, F1,42 = 6.39, p = 0.015) (Supp. Fig. 6) and decreased amplitudes for the P300 and N170, but not the P100 ERP (Fig. 4A–D) (P300: dose, F1,42 = 7.52, p = 0.009; N170: dose, F1,42 = 4.87, p = 0.033). LSD increased the latency of the P100 component without affecting the latency of the later N170 or P300 components (Fig. 4A, On the end of session questionnaire (Table 2), most participants correctly guessed “placebo” on placebo sessions and “hallucinogen” on 26 μg LSD sessions. On 13 μg sessions, guesses were mixed between “placebo,” “sedative,” and “hallucinogen” responses. On the 5D-ASC, LSD (26 μg only) significantly increased scores on Blissful State and not any other subscale of the 5D-ASC (Fig. 6) (dose, F1,21 = 5.91, p = 0.024; 26 μg vs. placebo, p = 0.001). Discussion This study investigated the subjective and electrophysiological effects of low doses of LSD (13, 26 μg) and placebo in healthy adults. Using resting-state EEG, we detected a reduction of oscillatory power across frequency bands indicative of broadband cortical desynchronization during rest, both with eyes closed and eyes open. Using a visual oddball paradigm to assess modulation of ERPs, the drug decreased the amplitude and increased the latency of evoked responses to visual stimuli. Subjectively, the drug produced small increases in measures of energy, positive mood, elation, anxiety, and intellectual efficiency. On a measure of altered states of consciousness (5D-ASC), it increased the subscale Blissful State. The main finding in this analysis was that low doses of LSD reduced broadband oscillatory power. This finding is 13 Psychopharmacology Fig. 4 Event-related potentials (ERPs) in response to rare stimuli in the oddball task (angry and neutral faces). Grand averaged ERP traces are shown under Pz (used to assess P300) and PO 10 (used to assess N170 and P100) electrodes. Shaded regions indicate windows used for peak extraction (P300, 300–700 ms; N170, 160–200 ms; 13 P100, 90–140 ms) (A). Significant mean amplitudes or latencies for each ERP across subjects plotted (B). Repeated measures ANOVA showing significant main effects of dose (p < 0.05). No interaction with faces condition. Follow-up independent t-tests compared each LSD dose to placebo (*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001). Psychopharmacology consistent with reports with larger doses of classical psychedelics, including 75 μg LSD (Riba et al., 2002; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013; Kometer et al., 2015; Carhart-Harris et al., 2016). Findings of broadband cortical desynchronization after administration of a classical psychedelic were initially detected in an EEG study of dimethyltryptaminecontaining “ayahuasca” capsules (Riba et al., 2002) and later after moderate intravenous doses of psilocybin, using MEG (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2013). The authors of the MEG-psilocybin study detected decreases in oscillatory power over posterior and frontal associated cortices, with large decreases in areas of the DMN, including the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). They attributed desynchronization in the PCC to increased excitability of deeplayer pyramidal neurons that are rich in 5-HT2a receptors (Erritzoe et al., 2009). Moderate oral doses of psilocybin have also been shown to reduce oscillatory power during EEG from 1.5 to 20 Hz (affecting all frequency bands except for gamma) within the PCC, anterior cingulate cortex, and retrosplenial cortex (Kometer et al., 2015). Based on the inverse relationship between oscillatory power and neural activity, particularly at lower frequencies (Klimesch et al., 2007; Schroeder and Lakatos, 2009; Buschman et al., 2012), the authors suggested their findings indicate a shift in the resting-state excitation/inhibition balance toward excitation. LSD (75 μg) also decreased oscillatory power across delta, alpha, theta, and beta bands and increased “diversity” in neural signals (Carhart-Harris et al., 2016; Schartner et al., 2017). Diversity refers to distinct patterns that have been used to index levels of sedation and sleep (Schartner et al., 2017). The authors reported that decreases in alpha power in the occipital lobe correlated with intensity of visual imagery, while decreases of alpha in the PCC correlated with “egodissolution,” as measured by the 5D-ASC (Carhart-Harris et al., 2016). An important limitation of our analysis was that we did not include dipole or distributed source detection of intracranial spectral density, but rather for the sake of simplicity focused on scalp electrodes over DMN regions during resting state. Due to the inverse problem of inferring the source of scalp recorded signals from deeper structures, our interpretation of the results is limited until further confirmation using either intracranial EEG or source estimations. LSD reduced neural responses to unexpected stimuli in the visual oddball paradigm. The drug dose-dependently reduced amplitudes to both cognitive (P300) and perceptual (N170) ERPs and increased latency to the early perceptual P100 component. Because the oddball task used facial images as stimuli, these findings raise the possibility that low doses of LSD affect cognitive and perceptual processing associated with facial recognition, including allocation of attentional resources (P300), structural encoding of facial configurations (N170), and fast extraction of visual information (P100) (Posner, 1975; Schmidt et al., 2013). Notably, the low doses of LSD did not impair behavioral responses to facial stimuli, but instead improved responses by reducing error rates. It remains to be determined whether similar effects would be observed with non-facial stimuli at low doses. In higher doses, several studies have shown that LSD and psilocybin affect ERPs as well as facial recognition. Specifically, LSD and psilocybin often reduce the recognition of negative facial expressions (Rocha et al., 2019). Psilocybin also reduced N170 amplitudes in response to fearful, but not happy faces (Schmidt et al., 2013). The authors noted that N170 amplitudes for fearful faces were greater than for happy faces under placebo conditions, suggesting that psilocybin induced a shift away from the bias to fearful faces. Although our study was not designed to assess changes in recognition of the valence of emotional face processing, LSD did not differentially alter N170 amplitudes after neutral or angry faces in the present study. Other studies have reported that psilocybin (at relatively high doses) also reduced amplitudes in P300 ERPs during an auditory oddball task (Bravermanova et al., 2018), and LSD has been found to reduce the amplitudes of visual average and photic evoked responses (Rodin and Luby, 1966; Shagass, 1966). The findings from this study suggest that even microdoses of LSD have effects on brain processing of visual stimuli that parallel that seen with higher doses of LSD and other psychedelic drugs. The subjective reports in our study showed that low doses of LSD increased positive mood and produced some stimulant-like effects. Specifically, LSD (26 μg) increased elation, positive mood, and anxiety on the POMS, as well as amphetamine-like effects on the ARCI. The subjective reports on stimulant-like effects are consistent with the known action of LSD at dopamine receptors (Pieri et al., 1974; Kelly and Iversen, 1975) raising the possibility that some of these effects are mediated by dopamine rather than serotonin. However, other studies (Preller et al., 2017) have shown that the subjective effects of higher doses of LSD are completely blocked by a 5-HT2a antagonist. Whether dopamine receptors are engaged at these low doses of LSD (De Gregorio et al., 2018) remains to be determined. We also note that most of the participants correctly identified the 26 μg dose as a “hallucinogen” at the end of the session. The subjective effects they experienced during the session presumably led to this identification. However, we recognize that effects experienced early in the session could also have influenced responses later in the session (i.e., through “unblinding”). Future analyses examining relationships between direct subjective effects and drug identifications may help resolve this issue. The present findings can also be related to EEG studies with other drugs. D-amphetamine, like classical psychedelics, reduces resting-state neuronal oscillations in delta, theta, and alpha bands, predominately localized to frontal 13 Psychopharmacology 13 Psychopharmacology ◂Fig. 5 Scalp topographies averaged across subjects for P300 (A), N170 (B), and P100 (C) event-related potential amplitudes for angry and neutral conditions in the emotional faces oddball paradigm. Right: red electrodes indicate electrodes where there were significant differences between placebo and 26 μg LSD condition (p < 0.05). in positive mood after LSD (13 μg) was correlated with connectivity between the amygdala and middle frontal gyrus during resting state. It remains to be determined how fMRI measures of connectivity and EEG measures of broadband oscillatory power after low doses of LSD can be reconciled. Table 2 End of session questionnaire. Number of subjects who labeled the drug they received as a placebo, hallucinogen, stimulant, sedative, or opioid Conclusions Study session (LSD dose) Placebo Hallucinogen Stimulant Sedative Opioid Placebo 13 μg 26 μg 12 2 2 5 1 6 6 2 5 3 2 13 2 4 1 and central regions (Albrecht et al., 2016). Conversely, sedative-like drugs such as alcohol or thiagabine (a GABA reuptake inhibitor) robustly increase delta, theta, and alpha power (Lansbergen et al., 2011; Muthukumaraswamy and Liley, 2018). Subanesthetic doses of ketamine, which enhance cortical excitatory signaling (Gerhard et al., 2020), also robustly reduce delta, alpha, and beta band power similarly to classical psychedelics (Muthukumaraswamy and Liley, 2018). Within the greater context of pharmaco-EEG studies, our findings indicate that low doses of LSD facilitate cortical neuronal activity. In the present study, EEG activity (resting state or ERPs) was not related to subjective or behavioral responses. This lack of correlation may be due to the limited statistical power of this study to detect such a relationship. In a previous study with fMRI (Bershad et al., 2020), we found that the increase These data add to the literature on classical psychedelics by describing acute effects of low doses of LSD from a neurophysiological perspective. We found that 13 and 26 μg doses of LSD have subtle, mostly positive subjective effects related to mood and cognition and yet produce some of the same EEG effects reported at higher doses of LSD (75 μg) and moderate doses of psilocybin. It has been proposed that classical psychedelics produce therapeutic effects by altering spontaneous cortical activity, inducing neural entropy that counters maladaptive neuroplasticity and associated mental rigidity (Carhart-Harris and Friston, 2019). Although we did not assess clinical outcome, our findings suggest that low doses of LSD produce neural effects that are similar to higher doses, raising the possibility that these low doses may lead to beneficial outcomes without producing psychedelic states. Future studies are needed to extend these observations to populations with psychiatric symptoms, and after repeated doses of the drug, as it is used in microdosing. Future studies should also examine the effects of these low doses when combined with psychotherapy. If effective, repeated low doses may provide an alternative model alongside high dosing procedures that have shown therapeutic benefits when used with additional support and preparation (Griffiths et al., 2016; Ross et al., 2016). Fig. 6 Mean scores (± SEM) on subscales of altered states of consciousness after placebo, 13 μg LSD, and 26 μg. LSD (26 μg only) increased scores on Blissful State (ANOVA *p < 0.05). 13 Psychopharmacology Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at https://d oi.o rg/1 0.1 007/s 00213-0 21-0 5991-9. Acknowledgements We thank Robin Nusslock for the helpful comments on the manuscript. Funding This research was supported by the National Institutes of Health [DA02812]. CHM was supported by the National Institutes of Health [T32DA043469]. Additional support was received as a pilot grant from the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago. Declarations Conflict of interest The author declare no competing interests. References Aghajanian GK, Marek GJ (1997) Serotonin induces excitatory postsynaptic potentials in apical dendrites of neocortical pyramidal cells. Neuropharmacol 36:589–599 Albrecht MA, Roberts G, Price G, Lee J, Iyyalol R, Martin-Iverson MT (2016) The effects of dexamphetamine on the resting-state electroencephalogram and functional connectivity. Hum Brain Mapp 37:570–588 Beique JC, Imad M, Mladenovic L, Gingrich JA, Andrade R (2007) Mechanism of the 5-hydroxytryptamine 2A receptor-mediated facilitation of synaptic activity in prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104:9870–9875 Bershad AK, Schepers ST, Bremmer MP, Lee R, de Wit H (2019) Acute subjective and behavioral effects of microdoses of lysergic acid diethylamide in healthy human volunteers. Biol Psychiatry 86:792–800 Bershad AK, Preller KH, Lee R, Keedy S, Wren-Jarvis J, Bremmer MP, de Wit H (2020) Preliminary report on the effects of a low dose of lsd on resting-state amygdala functional connectivity. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 5:461–467 Bravermanova A, Viktorinova M, Tyls F, Novak T, Androvicova R, Korcak J, Horacek J, Balikova M, Griskova-Bulanova I, Danielova D, Vlcek P, Mohr P, Brunovsky M, Koudelka V, Palenicek T (2018) Psilocybin disrupts sensory and higher order cognitive processing but not pre-attentive cognitive processing-study on P300 and mismatch negativity in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 235:491–503 Buschman TJ, Denovellis EL, Diogo C, Bullock D, Miller EK (2012) Synchronous oscillatory neural ensembles for rules in the prefrontal cortex. Neuron 76:838–846 Carhart-Harris RL, Friston KJ (2019) REBUS and the anarchic brain: toward a unified model of the brain action of psychedelics. Pharmacol Rev 71:316–344 Carhart-Harris RL, Erritzoe D, Williams T, Stone JM, Reed LJ, Colasanti A, Tyacke RJ, Leech R, Malizia AL, Murphy K, Hobden P, Evans J, Feilding A, Wise RG, Nutt DJ (2012) Neural correlates of the psychedelic state as determined by fMRI studies with psilocybin. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 109:2138–2143 Carhart-Harris RL et al (2016) Neural correlates of the LSD experience revealed by multimodal neuroimaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113:4853–4858 Delorme A, Makeig S (2004) EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods 134:9–21 13 De Gregorio D, Enns JP, Nunez NA, Posa L, Gobbi G (2018) d-Lysergic acid diethylamide, psilocybin, and other classic hallucinogens: mechanism of action and potential therapeutic applications in mood disorders. Prog Brain Res 242:69–96 Dittrich A (1998) The standardized psychometric assessment of altered states of consciousness (ASCs) in humans. Pharmacopsychiatry 31(Suppl 2):80–84 Eckman P, Friesen W (1976) Pictures of facial affect. Consorting Psychologies' Press, Palo Alto CA; Folkman S, Chesney M, McKusick L, Ironson G, Johnson DS, Coates TJ (1991) Translating Coping Theory into an Intervention. The Social Context of Coping, S:239-260. Erritzoe D, Frokjaer VG, Haugbol S, Marner L, Svarer C, Holst K, Baaré WF, Rasmussen PM, Madsen J, Paulson OB, Knudsen GM (2009) Brain serotonin 2A receptor binding: relations to body mass index, tobacco and alcohol use. Neuroimage 46:23–30 Fadiman J (2011) The psychedelic explorer’s guide: safe, therapeutic, and sacred journeys: Inner Traditions/Bear. Fadiman J, Korb S (2019) Might microdosing psychedelics be safe and beneficial? An Initial Exploration. J Psychoactive Drugs 51:118–122 Family N, Maillet EL, Williams LTJ, Krediet E, Carhart-Harris RL, Williams TM, Nichols CD, Goble DJ, Raz S (2020) Safety, tolerability, pharmacokinetics, and pharmacodynamics of low dose lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) in healthy older volunteers. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 237:841–853 Fischman MW, Foltin RW (1991) Utility of subjective-effects measurements in assessing abuse liability of drugs in humans. Br J Addict 86:1563–1570 Gao C, Conte S, Richards JE, Xie W, Hanayik T (2019) The neural sources of N170: Understanding timing of activation in faceselective areas. Psychophysiology 56:e13336. Gerhard DM, Pothula S, Liu RJ, Wu M, Li XY, Girgenti MJ, Taylor SR, Duman CH, Delpire E, Picciotto M, Wohleb ES, Duman RS (2020) GABA interneurons are the cellular trigger for ketamine’s rapid antidepressant actions. J Clin Invest 130:1336–1349 Griffiths RR, Johnson MW, Carducci MA, Umbricht A, Richards WA, Richards BD, Cosimano MP, Klinedinst MA (2016) Psilocybin produces substantial and sustained decreases in depression and anxiety in patients with life-threatening cancer: a randomized double-blind trial. J Psychopharmacol 30:1181–1197 Grimm O, Kraehenmann R, Preller KH, Seifritz E, Vollenweider FX (2018) Psilocybin modulates functional connectivity of the amygdala during emotional face discrimination. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 28:691–700 Haertzen CA, Hill HE, Belleville RE (1963) Development of the Addiction Research Center Inventory (Arci): selection of items that are sensitive to the effects of various drugs. Psychopharmacologia 4:155–166 Hutten N, Mason NL, Dolder PC, Theunissen EL, Holze F, Liechti ME, Feilding A, Ramaekers JG, Kuypers KPC (2020) Mood and cognition after administration of low LSD doses in healthy volunteers: A placebo controlled doseeffect finding study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 41:81–91 Johnstad PG (2018) Powerful substances in tiny amounts: an interview study of psychedelic microdosing. Nordisk Alkohol Nark 35:39–51 Kelly PH, Iversen LL (1975) LSD as an agonist at mesolimbic dopamine receptors. Psychopharmacologia 45:221–224 Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S (2007) EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev 53:63–88 Kometer M, Pokorny T, Seifritz E, Volleinweider FX (2015) Psilocybin-induced spiritual experiences and insightfulness are associated with synchronization of neuronal oscillations. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 232:3663–3676 Psychopharmacology Kuypers KP, Ng L, Erritzoe D, Knudsen GM, Nichols CD, Nichols DE, Pani L, Soula A, Nutt D (2019) Microdosing psychedelics: more questions than answers? An overview and suggestions for future research. J Psychopharmacol 33:1039–1057 Kuypers KPC (2020) Microdosing with psychedelics: what do we know? Tijdschr Psychiatr 62:669–676 Lansbergen MM, Dumont GJ, van Gerven JM, Buitelaar JK, Verkes RJ (2011) Acute effects of MDMA (3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine) on EEG oscillations: alone and in combination with ethanol or THC (delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol). Psychopharmacol (Berl) 213:745–756 Lebedev AV, Lovden M, Rosenthal G, Feilding A, Nutt DJ, CarhartHarris RL (2015) Finding the self by losing the self: neural correlates of ego-dissolution under psilocybin. Hum Brain Mapp 36:3137–3153 Lebedev AV, Kaelen M, Lovden M, Nilsson J, Feilding A, Nutt DJ, Carhart-Harris RL (2016) LSD-induced entropic brain activity predicts subsequent personality change. Hum Brain Mapp 37:3203–3213 Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH (1995) The structure of negative emotional states: comparison of the depression anxiety stress scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav Res Ther 33:335–343 Luppi AI, Carhart-Harris RL, Roseman L, Pappas I, Menon DK, Stamatakis EA (2021) LSD alters dynamic integration and segregation in the human brain. Neuroimage 227:117653. Martin WR, Sloan JW, Sapira JD, Jasinski DR (1971) Physiologic, subjective, and behavioral effects of amphetamine, methamphetamine, ephedrine, phenmetrazine, and methylphenidate in man. Clin Pharmacol Ther 12:245–258 McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF (1971) Manual profile of mood states. Morean ME, de Wit H, King AC, Sofuoglu M, Rueger SY, O’Malley SS (2013) The drug effects questionnaire: psychometric support across three drug types. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 227:177–192 Mueller F, Lenz C, Dolder PC, Harder S, Schmid Y, Lang UE, Liechti ME, Borgwardt S (2017) Acute effects of LSD on amygdala activity during processing of fearful stimuli in healthy subjects. Transl Psychiatry 7:e1084 Muthukumaraswamy SD, Liley DT (2018) 1/f electrophysiological spectra in resting and drug-induced states can be explained by the dynamics of multiple oscillatory relaxation processes. Neuroimage 179:582–595 Muthukumaraswamy SD, Carhart-Harris RL, Moran RJ, Brookes MJ, Williams TM, Errtizoe D, Sessa B, Papadopoulos A, Bolstridge M, Singh KD, Feilding A, Friston KJ, Nutt DJ (2013) Broadband cortical desynchronization underlies the human psychedelic state. J Neurosci 33:15171–15183 Neuner I, Arrubla J, Werner CJ, Hitz K, Boers F, Kawohl W, Shah NJ (2014) The default mode network and EEG regional spectral power: a simultaneous fMRI-EEG study. PLoS One 9:e88214 Nichols DE (2016) Psychedelics. Pharmacol Rev 68:264–355 Nutt D, Carhart-Harris R (2021) The current status of psychedelics in psychiatry. JAMA Psychiatry 78:121–122 Pieri L, Pieri M, Haefely W (1974) LSD as an agonist of dopamine receptors in the striatum. Nature 252:586–588 Polito V, Stevenson RJ (2019) A systematic study of microdosing psychedelics. PLoS One 14:e0211023 Posner MI (1975) Chapter 15 - Psychobiology of attention. In: Handbook of Psychobiology (Gazzaniga MS, Blakemore C, eds), pp 441-480: Academic Press. Preller KH, Razi A, Zeidman P, Stampfli P, Friston KJ, Vollenweider FX (2019) Effective connectivity changes in LSD-induced altered states of consciousness in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 116:2743–2748 Preller KH, Herdener M, Pokorny T, Planzer A, Kraehenmann R, Stampfli P, Liechti ME, Seifritz E, Vollenweider FX (2017) The fabric of meaning and subjective effects in LSD-induced states depend on serotonin 2A receptor activation. Curr Biol 27:451–457 Preller KH, Burt JB, Ji JL, Schleifer CH, Adkinson BD, Stampfli P, Seifritz E, Repovs G, Krystal JH, Murray JD, Vollenweider FX, Anticevic A (2018) Changes in global and thalamic brain connectivity in LSD-induced altered states of consciousness are attributable to the 5-HT2A receptor. Elife 7. Quednow BB, Kometer M, Geyer MA, Vollenweider FX (2012) Psilocybin-induced deficits in automatic and controlled inhibition are attenuated by ketanserin in healthy human volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacol 37:630–640 Raichle ME (2015) The brain’s default mode network. Annu Rev Neurosci 38:433–447 Riba J, Anderer P, Morte A, Urbano G, Jane F, Saletu B, Barbanoj MJ (2002) Topographic pharmaco-EEG mapping of the effects of the South American psychoactive beverage ayahuasca in healthy volunteers. Br J Clin Pharmacol 53:613–628 Rocha JM, Osorio FL, Crippa JAS, Bouso JC, Rossi GN, Hallak JEC, Dos Santos RG (2019) Serotonergic hallucinogens and recognition of facial emotion expressions: a systematic review of the literature. Ther Adv Psychopharmacol 9:2045125319845774 Rodin E, Luby E (1966) Effects of LSD-25 on the EEG and photic evoked responses. Arch Gen Psychiatry 14:435–441 Ross S, Bossis A, Guss J, Agin-Liebes G, Malone T, Cohen B, Mennenga SE, Belser A, Kalliontzi K, Babb J, Su Z, Corby P, Schmidt BL (2016) Rapid and sustained symptom reduction following psilocybin treatment for anxiety and depression in patients with life-threatening cancer: a randomized controlled trial. J Psychopharmacol 30:1165–1180 Schartner MM, Carhart-Harris RL, Barrett AB, Seth AK, Muthukumaraswamy SD (2017) Increased spontaneous MEG signal diversity for psychoactive doses of ketamine. LSD and psilocybin. Sci Rep 7:46421 Schmidt A, Kometer M, Bachmann R, Seifritz E, Vollenweider F (2013) The NMDA antagonist ketamine and the 5-HT agonist psilocybin produce dissociable effects on structural encoding of emotional face expressions. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 225:227–239 Schroeder CE, Lakatos P (2009) Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection. Trends Neurosci 32:9–18 Shagass C (1966) Effects of LSD on somatosensory and visual evoked responses and on the EEG in man. Recent Adv Biol Psychiatry 9:209–227 Szigeti B, Kartner L, Blemings A, Rosas F, Feilding A, Nutt DJ, Carhart-Harris RL, Erritzoe D (2021) Self-blinding citizen science to explore psychedelic microdosing. Elife 10. Tagliazucchi E, Roseman L, Kaelen M, Orban C, Muthukumaraswamy SD, Murphy K, Laufs H, Leech R, McGonigle J, Crossley N, Bullmore E, Williams T, Bolstridge M, Feilding A, Nutt DJ, CarhartHarris R (2016) Increased global functional connectivity correlates with LSD-induced ego dissolution. Curr Biol 26:1043–1050 Waldman A (2018) A really good day: how microdosing made a mega difference in my mood, my marriage, and my life: Anchor Books. Yanakieva S, Polychroni N, Family N, Williams LTJ, Luke DP, Terhune DB (2019) The effects of microdose LSD on time perception: a randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 236:1159–1170 Publisher's note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. 13