Southern Speech Communication Journal ISSN: 0361-8269 (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rsjc19 The relationship of three types of communication apprehension to classroom achievement Michael D. Scott & Lawrence R. Wheeless To cite this article: Michael D. Scott & Lawrence R. Wheeless (1977) The relationship of three types of communication apprehension to classroom achievement, Southern Speech Communication Journal, 42:3, 246-255, DOI: 10.1080/10417947709372352 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10417947709372352 Published online: 01 Apr 2009. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 22 View related articles Citing articles: 24 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rsjc19 Download by: [Nanyang Technological University] Date: 04 June 2016, At: 01:29 THE SOUTHERN SPEECH COMMUNICATION JOURNAL 42 (SPRING, 1977), 246-255 THE RELATIONSHIP OF THREE TYPES OF COMMUNICATION APPREHENSION TO CLASSROOM ACHIEVEMENT Downloaded by [Nanyang Technological University] at 01:29 04 June 2016 MICHAEL D. SCOTT AND LAWRENCE R. WHEELESS This study was designed to assess the impact of oral, receiver, and writing communication apprehension on classroom achievement in an interpersonal communication course, and to determine the relationships among these variables. Three hypotheses and one research question were posited. Results indicated that oral and receiver communication apprehension have a significant and deleterious effect on student achievement. The results also suggested that questions regarding the additive effects of oral, receiver, and writing communication apprehension may be of little consequence. f ?{^ ommunication apprehension" has recently come to mean ' more than fear or anxiety about real or anticipated oral communication with people. Wheeless has identified and operationalized a type or dimension of apprehension for the receiving function of communication.1 Similarly, Daly and Miller report research suggesting that a type or dimension of apprehension also exists for the encoding of written messages.2 While the classroom effects of oral communication apprehension have been recently investigated, the study of classroom Michael D. Scott (Ph.D., University of Southern California, 1974) is an Assistant Professor in the Departments of Speech Communication and Educational Psychology at West Virginia University. Lawrence R. Wheeless (Ph.D., Wayne State University, 1970) is an Associate Professor and Director of Graduate Studies in the Department of Speech Communication at West Virginia University. 1 Lawrence R. Wheeless, "An Investigation of Receiver Apprehension and Social Context Dimensions of Communication Apprehension," The Speech Teacher, 24 (1975), 261-68. 2 John A. Daly and Michael D. Miller, "The Empirical Development of a Measure of Writing Apprehension," Research in the Teaching of English, 9 (1975a), 242-49. 246 Downloaded by [Nanyang Technological University] at 01:29 04 June 2016 Three Types of Communication Apprehension 247 effects of receiver and writing apprehension is still in the seminal research stage.3 Only one study has sought to assess the impact of receiver apprehension on student achievement, and the published literature contains only one report of research aimed at discerning the effects of writing apprehension on academic performance.4 Moreover, researchers have yet to investigate the question whether oral, receiver, and writing apprehension may have some cumulative effect on student achievement. REVIEW OF RELEVANT RESEARCH As it now stands, there are three strains of communication apprehension research—specifically, studies focusing on the impact of oral communication apprehension, receiver apprehension, and writing apprehension. The following discussion briefly reviews the findings of these studies. Oral Communication Apprehension Oral communication apprehension, a broadly-based fear or anxiety about real or anticipated communication with another person(s), appears to influence student achievement in a number of ways. The most obvious effects of oral communication apprehension on student achievement involve instructional strategies which require the highly apprehensive student to participate orally; for example, in public speaking and small-group discussion. A number of studies have yielded results that indicate 3 James C. McCroskey and Janis F. Andersen, "The Relationship between Communication Apprehension and Academic Achievement among College Students," Human Communication Research, 3 (1976), 73-81; James C. McCroskey and John A. Daly, "Teachers' Expectancies of the Communication Apprehensive Child in the Classroom," Human Communication Research, 3 (1976), 67-72; Michael D. Scott and Lawrence R. Wheeless, "Communication Apprehension, Student Attitudes and Levels of Satisfaction," Western Speech Communication (in press). 4 Lawrence R. Wheeless and Michael D. Scott, "The Nature, Measurement, and Potential Effects of Receiver Apprehension" (Paper presented at the International Communication Association Convention, Portland, Oregon, April, 1976); John A. Daly and Michael D. Miller, "Further Studies on Writing Apprehension: SAT Scores, Success Expectations, Willingness to Take Advanced Writing Courses, and Sex Differences," Research in the Teaching of English, 9 (1975b), 250-56. Downloaded by [Nanyang Technological University] at 01:29 04 June 2016 248 The Southern Speech Communication Journal highly apprehensive students perform substantially below the norm when required to participate in such activities.5 These studies also suggest that such students would avoid such activities altogether if given a choice. High degrees of oral communication apprehension also appear to affect students in less obvious ways. Among other things, research suggests that highly apprehensive students have lower grade-point averages and achieve less on standardized measures of achievement than do non-apprehensive students.8 Moreover, Scott and Wheeless, and Hurt, Preiss, and Davis report research suggesting that highly apprehensive students generally have less favorable attitudes toward school than non-appreherisive students.7 Thus, to conclude that oral communication apprehension has a pervasive influence on classroom achievement, does not appear unreasonable. Receiver Apprehension Unlike oral communication apprehension, receiver apprehension concerns the degree to which individuals are fearful about misinterpreting, inadequately processing, and/or being unable to adjust psychologically to messages. Receiver apprehension, which is assessed with a sixteen-item, self-report measure, is not conr ceptually tied to any one particular facet of information processing. In the one study aimed at assessing the impact of receiver apprehension on student achievement, Wheeless and Scott 5 See, for example: Paul R. Hamilton, "The Effect of Risk Proneness on Small Group Interaction, Communication Apprehension, and Self-Disclosure," Master's thesis, Illinois State Univ. 1972; James C. McCroskey, and Michael E. Sheahan, "Seating Position and Participation: An Alternative Explanation" (Paper presented at the International Communication Association Convention, Portland, Oregon, April, 1976); Gail A. Sorensen and James C. McCroskey, "The Prediction of Interaction Behavior in Small Groups," Communication Monographs (in press). 6 McCroskey and Anderson, "The Relationship between Communication Apprehension and Achievement among College Students." 7 Scott and Wheeless, "Communication Apprehension, Student Attitudes, and Levels of Satisfaction"; H. Thomas Hurt, Raymond Preiss, and Bren Davis, "The Effects of Communication Apprehension of Middle School Children on Sociometric Choice, Affective and Cognitive Learning" (Paper presented at the International Communication Association Convention, Portland, Oregon, April, 1976). Three Types of Communication Apprehension 249 Downloaded by [Nanyang Technological University] at 01:29 04 June 2016 found that students who were highly apprehensive about receiving information did not perform as well on objective measures of achievement and outside class projects as did students exhibiting low levels of receiver apprehension.8 While causal inferences cannot be safely drawn from this single study, receiver apprehension and student achievement did appear to be meaningfully related. Writing Apprehension Writing apprehension concerns the degree to which individuals are fearful or anxious about encoding written messages, as well as their predispositions toward writing activities. Like oral and receiver communication apprehension, writing apprehension is also assessed via a self-report measure. Daly and Miller investigated the relationships among writing apprehension, SAT scores, and success-expectations in writing courses.9 While causality could not be inferred in each instance, Daly and Miller found that students who reported low levels of writing apprehension scored significantly higher on the verbal dimension of the SAT than did students who reported high levels of writing apprehension. In addition, these researchers found that students reporting high levels of writing apprehension expected to be less successful in their writing endeavors than did students reporting low levels of writing apprehension. As was the case with receiver apprehension, writing apprehension also appears to be meaningfully related to student achievement. RATIONALE, HYPOTHESES, AND RESEARCH QUESTION The preceding studies have been primarily concerned with assessing the relationships between a particular "type" of communication apprehension and student achievement or other variables associated with student achievement. In addition, many of these studies fall under the rubric of ex post facto research; that is, research in which measures of the particular type of 8 Wheeless and Scott, "The Nature, Measurement and Potential Effects of Receiver Apprehension." 9 Daly and Miller, "Further Studies on Writing Apprehension: SAT Scores, Success Expectations, Willingness to Take Advanced Writing Courses, and Sex Differences." Downloaded by [Nanyang Technological University] at 01:29 04 June 2016 250 The Southern Speech Communication Journal apprehension under investigation were administered after subjects had completed the dependent measures. Although each type of communication apprehension has been found to be meaningfully related to student achievement (albeit ex post facto, or otherwise), the relationships among the preceding types of communication apprehension and student achievement have yet to be investigated. Obviously, more information needs to be gathered about the individual effects of the preceding types of communication apprehension, including the variance in student achievement which can be attributed to them. Therefore, this study was initially concerned with testing the following directional hypotheses: H x : Students who are highly apprehensive about oral communication will achieve significantly less than students who are not apprehensive about oral communication. H2: Students who are highly apprehensive about receiving communication will achieve significantly less than students who are not apprehensive about receiving communication. H3: Students who are highly apprehensive about encoding written communication will achieve significantly less than students who are not apprehensive about encoding written communication. A second major aim of this study was to examine the relationships among the three types of apprehension in relation to the criterion of student achievement. While it might be argued that these three types of communication apprehension have a cumulative impact on student achievement, the lack of previous research in this regard suggested that directional predictions were not warranted. As a result, the following research question was posited: Qj: What is the nature of the relationships among oral, receiver, and writing apprehension, and student achievement? METHOD Procedures and Sampling Students enrolled in mass lecture sections of a universitylevel communication course were administered measures of oral Downloaded by [Nanyang Technological University] at 01:29 04 June 2016 Three Types of Communication Apprehension 251 communication apprehension, apprehension about receiving communication, and apprehension about writing. Oral and writing apprehension tests were administered on the first day of class. The receiver apprehension test was administered before mid-term. At the completion of the semester, achievement scores were obtained for students enrolled in laboratory sections concerned with dyadic, interpersonal communication. The three data sets were merged to produce 194 complete data observations (265 5s had at least partial data reported). Attrition was due to student absences during one of the times data were collected, and failure to match data sets occasioned by errors in student identification numbers reported on IBM answer sheets. Measurement Oral communication apprehension was assessed with a 25item version of the Personal Report of Communication Apprehension (PRCA).10 This measure displayed the following descriptive characteristics: split-half reliability = .92; mean = 72.30; and standard deviation = 15.49. Receiver apprehension was assessed with the Revised Receiver Apprehension Test (RRAT),11 a 16item self-report measure which displayed the following characteristics: split-half reliability = .89; mean = 43.64; standard deviation = 13.42. Finally, writing apprehension was assessed with the Writing Apprehension Test (WAT)12 which, in this study, displayed the following characteristics: split-half reliability = .94; mean = 55.09; standard deviation = 13.42. Achievement was operationalized as examination average and average score on projects which involved oral communication as well as writing and receiver-oriented behaviors. Three criterion-referenced examinations were given on specified cognitive objectives for the course. The multiple choice (5-option) examinations consisted of two 50-item examinations and one 25-item examination. The first examination (50-item) displayed the following descriptive statistics: reliability (KR-20) = .86; 10 James C. McCroskey and Lawrence R. Wheeless, An Introduction to Human Communication (Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 1976), 85-86. 11 Wheeless and Scott, "The Nature, Measurement, and Potential Effects of Receiver Apprehension." 12 Daly and Miller, "The Empirical Development of a Measure of Writing Apprehension." Downloaded by [Nanyang Technological University] at 01:29 04 June 2016 252 The Southern Speech Communication Journal mean = 39.47; standard deviation = 6.83; standard error of measurement = 2.74. The second examination (50-item) displayed the following characteristics: reliability (KR-20) = .82; mean = 35.18; standard deviation = 6.61; standard error = 3.12. The third examination (25-item) displayed the following descriptive statistics: reliability (KR-20) = .70; mean = 20.15; standard deviation = 3.21; standard error = 1.91. All examination items were developed with discriminant-item analyses during the previous semester. The examination average was derived by weighting the 50-item tests twice as heavily as the 25-item test, which was converted (via linear transformation) to a compatible fifty-unit scale. The resulting examination average had the following characteristics: mean = 38.42; standard deviation = 5.78; range = 7.20-48.80. Projects were graded on the following criteria: not attempted = 0, attempted = 1, completed as assigned = 2, and completed as assigned, plus extra, relevant project report on the topic = 3. For ease of analysis and interpretation, project averages (0-3 point scale) were multiplied by the constant of 16.6667 (linear transformation) in order to transform them to the same metric as the examination average. The resulting project average had the following characteristics: mean = 41.91; standard deviation = 6.90; range = 14.34-50.00. Design and Statistical Analysis Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) was used to test all hypotheses at the .05 level of statistical significance. For the first three hypotheses, high and low levels of PRCA, RRAT, and WAT served as the classification/independent variables in three, one-way analyses, and the two achievement scores were treated as the criterion/dependent variables. High- and lowapprehensives (PRCA, RRAT, and WAT) were operationalized as one standard deviation above and below the mean, respectively. For the research question, a three-way multivariate analysis of variance was attempted (High vs. Low PRCA X High vs. Low RRAT X High vs. Low WAT). Again, achievement means were the dependent variables (examination and project averages). A Pearson product-moment correlation between criterion/dependent variables was ascertained in order to check the validity of assumptions underlying MANOVA in regard to Three Types of Communication Apprehension 253 dependent variables. Roy's maximum root criterion and generalized F-distribution were used to determine significance in all MANOVA's. Downloaded by [Nanyang Technological University] at 01:29 04 June 2016 RESULTS AND INTERPRETATION The correlation coefficient between the two criterion/ dependent variables measuring achievement (examination average, project average) was significant (r = .49). Thus, assumptions regarding the relatedness of dependent variables in MANOVA were met. The MANOVA of achievement scores for high and low Oral Communication Apprehensives (PRCA) revealed a significant difference on the transformed F-ratio (F = 5.22, 2/69 d.f., p < .008). The mean of the canonical variate representing achievement was significantly lower for high-communication apprehensives (X = 1.01, n = 46) than for low-communication apprehensives ^X~= 1.11, n = 26), (if = 1 - \ = .13). The 24 percent classified as high apprehensives was well above expected norms of previous research.13 Examination of the canonical variate of achievement revealed that project average and examination average were correlated with the variate at .88 and .84, respectively. Likewise, both univariate analyses indicated significant hypothesized differences, with the project average producing the major difference. The first hypothesis was supported. The MANOVA of achievement scores for high and low Receiver Apprehensives (RRAT) revealed a significant difference on the transformed F-ratio (F = 3.61, 2/49 d.f., p < .03). The mean of the canonical variate representing achievement was significantly lower for high-receiver apprehensives (X = 1.29, n = 29) than for low-receiver apprehensives (X~ = 1.40, n = 23) (JJ2 = 1 -X = .13). While 78 5s were classified into high and low apprehension categories, the merging of data sets resulted in 51 Ss with complete data (78 of 265 5s = 29 percent, which approximates expectations from previous research). This indicates that S attrition was not due to receiver apprehension levels. Examination of the canonical variate of achievement re13 James C. McCroskey, "Measures of Communication-Bound Anxiety," Speech Monographs, 37 (1970), 269-77. Downloaded by [Nanyang Technological University] at 01:29 04 June 2016 254 The Southern Speech Communication Journal vealed that project average and examination average were correlated with the variate at .85 and .62 levels, respectively. Likewise, the univariate analyses indicated significant, hypothesized differences, with the project average producing the major difference. The second hypothesis was supported. The MANOVA of achievement scores for high and low writing apprehensives revealed no significant difference (F = 0.59, 2/64 d.f., p > .05). No significant multivariate effect or univariate effects on achievement were observed to result between high and low writing apprehension categories. Because the 19 percent classified as high apprehensives were slightly above previous norms, failure to find significant differences was not due to subject attrition. The third hypothesis was not supported. The attempted exploration of the research question was unsuccessful. The three-way MANOVA (High-Low PRCA X HighLow RRAT X High-Low WAT) was able to classify a total of only 16 Ss into the relevant cell conditions. Two conditions had no Ss and three conditions had only one S each. Only 8 percent of the 5s (16 of 192) were high and/or low across the three types of apprehension (8 cells). Ninety-two percent were moderate in apprehension on at least one of the three measures— a datum which effectively prevented the classification scheme needed to test additivity. It should be noted that a three-way analysis including moderate levels would not solve this problem, since the 8 cells common to both analyses would still contain insufficient N's. Moreover, the lack of significance on writing apprehension probably precluded this variable from having an additive effect in relation to the other forms of apprehension. These observations point, however, to a meaningful interpretation. The relatively few persons displaying categorical combinations of the three types of high- and low-apprehension minimize the importance of examining the additive effects of the three in combination. Because so few people fit the categorical combinations (8 percent), oral, receiver, and writing communication apprehension are probably not additive for most people (92 percent). CONCLUSIONS This study was concerned with the individual effects of three Downloaded by [Nanyang Technological University] at 01:29 04 June 2016 Three Types of Communication Apprehension 255 types of communication apprehension on student achievement, as well as the relationships among these variables. Consistent with previous research, oral and receiver communication apprehension were found to have a deleterious effect on student achievement. Unlike Daly and Miller (1975b), however, the present investigators did not observe such an effect for writing apprehension. This finding, coupled with research suggesting that writing apprehension is significantly related to student attitudes and levels of satisfaction with writing activities,14 implies that the writing apprehension instrument may be more of a measure of predispositions toward writing in classes where writing is a secondary concern, than a measure of anxiety or fear. While oral, receiver, and writing apprehension were not found to have any cumulative impact on student achievement, the findings of this study may have implications for instruction. Clearly, oral and receiver communication apprehension have an undesirable influence on student achievement, regardless of whether the classroom environment is performance oriented. Teachers, in their attempts to assist students, need to assess the degree to which students in their classes are apprehensive about orally communicating or receiving information. Such assessment would facilitate the use of instructional strategies which are less likely to elicit the fears of communicatively anxious students. Treatment of the problem of communication apprehension itself would be most desirable. Finally, we need to point out that researchers should begin to investigate the classroom impact of "communication apprehension" in controlled experiments. Much of the research, including that reported here, has not controlled for mediating variables such as self-esteem, prior academic success, intelligence, or student attitudes toward the content being taught. Obviously, reciprocal causality between variables like "communication apprehension," intelligence, and the like is at issue here. Consequently, until such variables are controlled for in "true" experimental studies, the most that can be said about "communication apprehension" and student achievement is that the two appear to be meaningfully and negatively related. 14 Scott and Wheeless, "Communication Apprehension, Student Attitudes, and Levels of Satisfaction."