Case and Grammatical Functions: The Icelandic Passive Author(s): A. Zaenen, J. Maling and H. Thráinsson Source: Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, Vol. 3, No. 4 (Nov., 1985), pp. 441-483 Published by: Springer Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/4047609 . Accessed: 15/09/2014 12:55 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. . Springer is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Natural Language &Linguistic Theory. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON CASE AND GRAMMATICAL THE ICELANDIC FUNCTIONS: PASSIVE* 0. INTRODUCTION Recent attempts to capture the universalcharacteristicsof passive have moved away from relying on word order configurations.Ignoring proposals that have recourse to essentiallysemantic means of capturingthe active-passive relation (such as Gazdar& Sag 1981, Dowty 1982), there are two types of proposals that are currentlybeing debated. The first is that passive morphology inhibits case marking, and hence that the advancementof the object to subject in passive is essentiallyforced by the violation of the constraintrequiringthat all NPs have case. This idea is embodied in slightly different forms in various GB and GB-related proposals (see e.g., den Besten 1981, Kayne 1981, Chomsky 1981, Freidin & Babby 1982, 1983, and also Lieber 1979).1 The second * An earlyversionof this paperwas read at the WinterMeetingof the LinguisticSocietyof America in December, 1982. Later versions were presented at the Second Annual Workshopon ScandinavianSyntax at Biskops-Arno (Sweden) and at the University of Iceland in June, 1983. We are especially grateful to Anna Sigurbardottirand Johann G. Johannssonfor countless hours of insightfulwork as native speakerinformants,supported in part by NSF GrantNo. BNS80-14730. We thankthe followingpeople for commentsand suggestions: J. Bresnan, R. Cooper, A. Harris, R. Jackendoff,J. Kornfilt,L. Levin, J. Schindler,C. Watkins,and five anonymousreaders. 1 The case-dependentapproachis actually an adaptationof a traditionallymade observation, namely that oblique case is immune to passivization.Whereas the traditional observation is correct for Icelandic under the proper interpretationof 'oblique', its adaptationin the current GB literatureis not. To show that the two approachesare not empiricallyequivalent,we can comparethe followingtwo quotes. ... I want to express in a generative frameworkthe traditionalinsight among studentsof Germaniclanguagesas well as among grammariansof older phasesof Englishthat oblique case is immunefrom passivization.(den Besten 1981, p. 67) From most inflected languages we know that only those verbs which take an accusative object can form a personalpassive.This seems to be the generalrule, thoughin Old Greek also non-accusativeobjects can be made the subject of a passive sentence. In the Low German (...) of my hometown (Krefeld) there is no distinction between dative and accusative, or rather, there is only one objective case. So verbs which in High German govem the dative (e.g., helfen, kundigen:help, give notice)come to be construedwith the objective case in Lower German.In passive accordingly,we have personalconstructions: Ich werde geholfen, er ist gekundigtworden.(Marchand1974, p. 98) NaturalLanguage and LinguisticTheory3 (1985)1441-483. 0167-806X/85.10 1985 by D. Reidel PublishingCompany ? This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 442 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON proposal relies on grammaticalfunctions, and is embodied in both RG and LFG. In this paper we will argue on the basis of data from Icelandic that the case dependent approach is not general enough, whereas an approach based on grammaticalfunctions gives the correct result both for Icelandic, and also for languages like German for which the casedependent approach was developed. The main evidence for our argumentwill come from passives of the type illustratedin (1). (1) Honum var hjalpab. him(D) was helped We will argue that such sentences contain a non-nominativesubject.2 A second theoretical issue that we will address is whether the same grammaticalfunction can have more than one realization in the same clause. As we will see, the Icelandic data at first glance seems to lead to the conclusion that this can happen, but virtually all current syntactic theories propose a prohibitionagainst such double assignments.It is part of the theta criterionin GB (Chomsky1981); it has been proposedas the strataluniquenesslaw in RG (Perlmutter& Postal 1983, p. 92ff), and as the functionaluniquenesslaw in LFG (Bresnan 1982, p. 163, Grimshaw 1982, p. 91). While there are empirical differences among these formulations,they lie beyond the scope of this paper. As we will see, a more careful study of the Icelandic data shows that it is compatible with the For Marchand,the notion 'subject'seems to coincide with that of nominativeNP; hence his statementcan be given an interpretationthat allows it to apply correctlyto both German and Icelandic. Den Besten's statement,however, does not hold for Icelandic, as we will show. There are exceptions to Marchand'sstatement too, as he himself notes: Ancient Greek, but also Irishuntil it lost its dative case (C. Watkins,personalcommunication)and withinGermanic,Modem Faroese,as describedby Lockwood(1964), from whomwe quote the followingsentences: (i) Teir fagnabuDepilsmunnum vael. they receivedthe-depilsmen(D)well (ii) voru vel fagnabir. Depilsmenn the-Depilsmen(N)were well received(Nom.,pl.) 2 Before proceedingwith our analysishowever, we must remarkthat it would be all too easy to empty these theories of their distinguishablecontent. For example, if case is used not to refer to any morphologicallyobservablepropertiesof NPs, but simply to refer to whatever behaves syntacticallylike an OBJect, then the case-markingtheory of passivizationbecomes a notationalvariant of the function-basedtheory. Similarly,if OBJ is defined to be "whateverNP is assigned accusative case by the verb", then it will not be distinguishablefrom the case-assignmenttheory. In whatfollows, then, we will assumethat "case", and "function" are concepts with distinct morphologically and syntactically observableproperties. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 443 functional uniquenessprinciple and the assumptionthat only DOs passivize.3 Our attention will be focussed on what we will call the periphrastic passive, illustratedin a non-controversialinstance in (2): (2)a. fasta. Logreglan t6k Siggu the-police took Sigga(A) fast(A) The police arrestedSigga. b. af logreglunni. var tekin fost Sigga Sigga(N) was taken fast(N) by the-police(D) Sigga was arrested by the police.4 Icelandic also has some morphologically'middle'forms in the suffix-st, some of which have a passive meaning, as illustratedin (3):5 (3)a. b. fossinum. fyrir Ekkert heyrist nothing(N) can-be-heard on-account-of the-waterfall(D) Urib tyindist. the-watch(N) got-lost 3 The most problematic case of double objects discussed in the literature is that of Kinyarwanda(Gary & Keenan 1977); see also Dryer (1983) for argumentsthat these are not really double objects. Zaenen (1983) argues that Kikuyu verbs which seemingly allow the doubleobject constructionare better analyzedas allowingalternativeassignments of grammaticalfunctions; the argumentis based on the interactionof Passive with the prefixationof the direct object marker. 4 Agent af-phrasesare much less common in Icelandic passives than by-phrasesare in English. A sentence like (i) will alwaysbe interpretedas meaning(ii) ratherthan as (iii): (i) (ii) (iii) Bokin var tekin af J6ni. The book was taken from John. The book was taken by John. However, other sentences,such as (iv), are multiplyambiguousfor at least some speakers. (iv) Myndin var tekin af J6ni. The picturewas takenfrom/of/by John. 5 Example(3a) is taken from Einarsson(1945). Example(3b) shows that middleformation does not preserve oblique case; the verb tyna 'to lose' takes dative objects in the active voice, dative subjectsin the passive voice, but nominativesubjectsin the middlevoice. See Zaenen & Maling (1984) for discussion of oblique case markingwith respect to other quasi-productivelexical rules. Unaccusative verbs show that case preservation is not restrictedto fully productive 'syntactic' rules like passive ('major' rule in the sense of Wasow 1980). As far as we know, Helgi Bem6dusson(1982) was the firstto point out the existence of unaccusativeverbs preservingaccusativecase on themes. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 444 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON These will be ignored in this paper.6We assume that they are derived in the lexicon and not in the syntax. The general outline of the study is as follows: after giving some background information about Icelandic syntax in section 1, we will quickly review in section 2 the syntactic characteristicsof grammatical subjects in Icelandic. We then discuss in section 3 the problem of which NP's that are in post verbal position in unmarkedactive sentences can show up as the subject of a passive sentence. In section 4, we give an LFG account of passives in Icelandic, and outline the principles of casemarking and assignment of grammaticalfunctions that obtain in Icelandic. We will show that our account does not force us to the conclusion that Icelandic has double objects. We will then brieflydiscuss in section 5 whether German constructions like the one illustrated in (4) should be treated in the same way as their superficially similar Icelandic translations. (4) Ihm wurde geholfen. (German) Honum var hjalpab. (Icelandic) him(D) was helped We show that the two constructionsare syntacticallyvery differentin the two languages,and then extend our account of case markingto cover the German data. 1. SOME RELEVANT FEATURES OF ICELANDIC SYNTAX Icelandic has the richest inflectional system of any modern Germanic language. There are four cases (nominative, accusative, dative, genitive) and three genders (masculine, feminine, neuter). To make it easier to follow the examples, we give the paradigmfor personal pronounsin (5). (5) Singular masc fem N hann hun A hann hana D honum henni G hans neut pab Pab pvi hennar Pess masc Peir pa Peim Plural fem Twr Oaer Peim neut Pau Pau Peim Peirra peirra Oeirra 6 The use of the -st passive is less widespreadin Icelandicthan it is e.g., in Swedish.This is not meant to imply that there aren't some uses of the -st forms in Icelandic that are not found in Swedish,e.g., the one exemplifiedunder(i): (i) Deir sogbust ekki vilja gera Pab. they said-themselvesnot to-want to-do that This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 445 The basic word orderof Icelandic is SVO, both in main and in embedded clauses. The main verb directly follows any auxiliaries,as in English but unlike German.This is illustratedin (6). (6)a. b. Eg hafbi seb hana. I had seen her(A) *Eg hafbi hana seb. However, unlike English and like the other Germanic languages, Icelandic has the Verb-Second Constraint.That is, the subject inverts with the finite verb whenever another constituent is topicalized. This is illustratedin (7). (7)a. b. c. d. e. i skfiffunni. Olafur fann peysuna sina Olaf(N) found sweater(A) his[+REFL] in the-drawer(D) Peysunasina fsvnnOlafur i skuiffunni. *Peysunasina Olafurfann i skuiffunni. I skuffunnifann Olafurpeysuna sina. *I skuffunniOlafurfann peysuna sina. Thus far we have consideredonly simple transitiveverbs with accusative objects. Icelandic, however, also has transitive verbs with dative and genitive objects as illustratedin (8), and numerousditransitiveverbs (see section 3.2 below). (8)a. b. Eg hjalpabihonum. I helped him(D) DAT GEN Eg mun sakna hans. I will miss him(G) Icelandic has an impersonalpassive construction,illustratedin (9). When no topicalizationtakes place in impersonalpassives, a dummy iaboccurs in sentence-initial position, as shown in (9a). Whenever there is a topicalized constituent,dummypab cannot occur in the sentence, either before or after the finite verb. This fact is illustratedin (9b-d). (9)a. b. c. d. Dab var dansab i gxer. There was danced yesterday I gaervar dansab. *I gaervar Pab dansab. *I gxrrPab var dansab. When the auxiliaryis vera 'to be' or verba 'to be, become', the participle This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 446 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON agrees in numberand gender with the nominativeNP in the clause.7This is illustratedin (10). (10)a. til Islands. 6lafur var farinn Olaf(N) was gone(masc.sg.) to Iceland(G) b. var farin Sigga til Islands. Sigga(N) was gone(fem.sg.) to Iceland c. Barnib var farib til Islands. the-child was gone(neut.sg.) to Iceland 2. Two HYPOTHESES: SUBJECT VERSUS ToPic IN ICELANDIC Given these basic features of Icelandic syntax, there are two possible analysesof sentences like the ones in (11), where a DAT or GEN is in the first position, and the tensed verb in second position is 3rd person neuter, sg. (11)a. b. Deim var hjailpab. them(D) was helped Hennarvar saknab. her(G) was missed The question is whether these forms are instances of topicalization in an impersonalpassive construction, or personal passives in which the first NP is a bona fide subject. These two analysesare sketched in (12a) and (12b). S (12)a. NP Aux VP V var hjailpab NP feim 7 Strongverb participleslike fwa end in -inn, -in, -ib; one class of weak verbs (e.g. telja) also has participlesof this form, but most weak verb participlesend in -abur,-ub, -b. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE b. AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 447 S NP Aux VP Deim var hjalpab Under the first hypothesis, sentences like (11) are simply examples of impersonal passives combined with the topicalization of the dative or genitive NP; under this analysis,the sentences are theoreticallyunproblematic and uninteresting.Under the second hypothesis, however, the dative NP is indeed a grammaticalsubject, and the construction therefore presents difficultiesfor those accounts of passive that are based on case-marking.In what follows, we will argue that the correct analysisis (12b).8 Before tackling that problem we will first review the arguments that have been amassed during the last years to the effect that nominative casemarkingis not a necessaryprerequisite(nor a sufficientone) for subjecthood in Icelandic. Recent research has shown that there are several syntactic properties that distinguishbetween topics and subjects in Icelandic (Andrews 1976, 1982a, 1982b, Thrainsson 1979, Maling 1980, Zaenen 1980). Andrews (1976) was the first to point out that this distinction does not coincide with that between nominative and non-nominative NPs, and that contraryto traditionalbelief, a sentence like (13) must be analysedas having a simple subject-predicatestructurewith a dative subject and a nominative object. (13) Henni hefur alltaf her(D) has o6tt Olafur leibinlegur. always thought Olaf(N) boring(N) Arguments for that view were further developed in Maling (1980), Andrews (1982a), and Thrafinsson(1979), who summarizesmost of the tests. We will refer to these non-nominativepreverbal NPs as "oblique 8 This destroysthe argumentgiven in section 4.2 of Maling & Zaenen (1978) againstthe applicationof the that t-filterin Icelandic.However, the generalconclusionreachedin that paperstill holds. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 448 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON subjects". We illustrate here each property with an example of a nominative subject and an example of oblique subject, and refer to the relevant literaturefor furtherdiscussion. 2. 1. Raising Only subjects can raise, as illustratedby the contrast between (14b and d). The verb sakna "to miss" takes a nominativesubject and a genitive object. (14)a. Gubruin saknarHaraldar. Gudrun(N)misses Harold(G) b. Eg taldi Gubruinu i barnaskapminum sakna to-miss I believed Gudrun(A)in foolishness my Haraldar. Harold(G) c. Haraldar saknar Gubruin. Harold(G) misses Gudrun(N) d. *EJgtaldi JHaraldar IHarald J sakna JGubru'n IGubrtunu I believedJHarold(G)l to-miss JGudrun(N) 1Harold(A)J IGudrun(A) Since i barnaskap minum 'in my foolishness' in (14b) is an adverbial belonging to the matrix clause, its presence between the NP Gubruinu and the infinitive complement sakna Haraldar 'to miss Harold' is good evidence that the NP has been raised into matrix object position. (See Thrafinsson1979, pp. 389-393, for furtherdiscussion.)Of course, predicate nominalscannot be raised,despite theirnominativecase marking,as illustratedin (15).9 9 Non-NPs cannot in general undergoRaising, as shownin (i): (i) i barnaskapminumhafa veribmargt f6lk. *Eg taldi a fundunum I believed[at the-meeting]ppin foolishnessmy to-have been many people However,just when the PP can be analyzedas the grammaticalsubject,then it can undergo Rtaising,as illustratedin (ii): (ii) Eg taldi undir ruiminui bamaskap minumvera gobanfelustab. I believedunderthe-bed in foolishnessmy to-be good hiding-place. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE (15)a. AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 449 Olafur er bondi. Olaf(N) is a-farmer b. Bondi er 1lafur.(Topicalization) a-farmer is Olaf c. Olaf Eg tel vera bonda. I believe Olaf(A) to-be a-farmer(A) bonda vera Olaf. d. *Eg tel I believe a-farmer(A) to-be Olaf(A) However, so-called oblique subjects can raise, as shown in (16): (16) Eg tel henni hafa alltaf p6tt I believe her(D) to-have always thought Olafur leibinlegur. Olaf(N) boring(N) We can conclude that whatever the surface case-marking,all and only grammatical subjects can raise. (See Andrews 1982b and Thra"insson 1979 for furtherdiscussion.) 2.2. Reflexivization A second test is reflexivization.Many speakers of Icelandic allow only grammatical subjects to be the antecedents of reflexive pronouns, or more accurately, allow objects to be antecedents only if the reflexive occurs in a predicate complement predicatedof that object (see Maling 1982 for discussion).For such speakerswe find the same sort of contrasts with respect to Reflexivizationas we did with respect to Raising. We will give the judgmentsfor those speakerswho allow only subject-controlled reflexivization.(For other speakers the contrasts are more subtle: subjects control obligatoryreflexivization,whereasobjects control reflexives only optionally; see Thraiinsson(1976) and Maling (1982) for further discussion.)Boldface indicates intended coreference. (17)a. b. Sigga barbimig met duikkunnisinni/*hennar. Sigga(N) hit me(A) with doll(D) her(*[-REFL]) Sigga hit me with her doll. Eg barbiSiggu meb duikkunnihennar/*sinnm. I hit Sigga(A) with doll her(*[+REFL]) I hit Sigga with her doll. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 450 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON c. Siggu barbieg meb duikkunnihennar/*simn. Sigga I hit with her doll. Again the non-nominativesubject can (in fact, must) control reflexivization, as shown in the following example: (18)a. b. Henni Pykir brobir sinn/*hennar leibinlegur. her(D) thinks brother(N)her(*-REFL) boring fagur. (Proverb) fugl Hverjum pykir sinn everyone(D)thinks his[+REFL] bird(N) beautiful 2.3. Topicalizationand Subject-VerbInversion Subjects appear immediatelyafter the finite verb if another constituent has been preposed;whenever an object has been topicalized, no further Topicalizationcan take place. (l9)a. Refinn skaut Olafur meb pessaribyssu. the-fox(A) shot Olaf(N) with this shotgun Olafur. b. *Meb Pessaribyssu skaut refinn with this shotgun shot the-fox(A) Olaf(N) Similarlyin direct questions the tensed verb is immediatelyfollowed by the subject; hence direct questions and topicalizationsare incompatible as shown in (20): hafbi aldrei hjilpab Haraldi. (20)a. Sigga Sigga(N) had never helped Harold(D) b. Hafbi Sigga aldrei hjilpab Haraldi? (Yes-No-Question) Had Sigga(N) never helped Harold(D) c. *HafbiHaraldiSigga aldrei hjalpab? d. Hvenar hafbi Sigga hjalpabHaraldi? (Wh-question) When had Sigga helped Harold(D) e. *Hvenxr hafbi HaraldiSigga hj'alpab? f. Haraldihafbi Sigga aldrei hjalpab.(Topicalization) The same is true of oblique subjects. Unlike topics, oblique subjects can This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 451 immediately follow the finite verb.10As shown in (21b), topicalization can apply to sentences like (13), in which case the oblique subject inverts with the finite verb. (21)a. Hefur henni alltaf t0tt has b. Olafur leibinlegur? she(D) always thought Olaf(N) boring(N) Olafur hefur henni alltaf frtt leibinlegur. Olaf(N) has she(D) always thoughtboring(N) c. *Hefur Olafur henni alltaf tott leibinlegur? has Olaf(N) she(D) always thoughtboring 2.4. Extraction Most speakers of Icelandic do not generally allow Topicalization in binding domains; in other words, they allow Topicalizationto occur in embedded ab 'that'-clauses, but not under indirect questions, comparatives, relatives, etc. This is illustratedby the contrast in (22). (For furtherdiscussionsee Zaenen 1980, Rognvaldsson 1984.) (22)a. i gaer. Jon telur ab Maria hafi kysst Harald Jon believes that Mary(N) has kissed Harold(A) yesterday b. ab Maria hafi kysst Harald?. Hvenawrtelur Jon when believesJon(N) that Mary(N) has kissed Harold c. Jon telur ab Haraldhafi Maria kysst i gxer.(Topicalization) d. *Hvenxr telur Jon ab Harald hafi Maria kysst? Whendoes Jon believethat Harold, Mary kissed? Again, oblique subject NPs pattern here with the nominativesubjects as "' Note that the superficially similar Ist dir kalt geworden? in German does not necessarily have to be analyzed as an instance of Subject-Verb Inversion. According to German word order, both (nonsubject) dir and kalt c;anoccur between the finite verb ist and the nonfinite geworden even without this rule, as illustrated in (i). (i) Es ist dir it (ii) kalt geworden. (German) is you(D) cold become * ab er Pjr kalt orbib. * Pab er per orbib kalt. (Icelandic) It is clear that the Icelandic construction differs significantly from its German counterpart. See section 5 below for an account of certain syntactic differences between German and Icelandic. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 452 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON opposed to topicalizedNPs, as shown in (23): (23)a. Jon telur ab henni hafi alltaf frtt Jon believes that she(D) has always thought Olafur leibinlegur. Olaf(N) boring(N) b. ab henni hafi p06tt Hvenar telur Jon when believesJon(N) that she(D) has thought Olafur leibinlegur? Olaf boring When does Jon believe that she considered Olaf boring? c. Jon telur ab Olafur hafi henni alltaf frtt leibinlegur. (Topi-. calization) Jon believes that Olaf she has alwaysfound boring. at Olafur hafi henni d. *Hvenar telur J6n when believesJon(N) that Olaf(N) has she(D) 00ott leibinlegur? thoughtboring Note in particularthat in (23d), the object Olafuris not patterninglike a subject, despite its nominativecase-marking. 2.5. IndefiniteSubjectPostposing When a subject is indefinite, it can be postposed by a rule of indefinite NP postposing (plus pab-insertion).This cannot be done when a nonsubject is in first position. (24)a. Dab hefur pj6fur stolib hjolinu minu. there has a-thief(N) stolen bicycle(D) mine(D) b. hefur jojfurinnstolib. Hjoli a-bicycle has the-thief stolen c. *Dab hefur hjoli pjofurinn stolib. there has a-bicycle(D) the-thief(N) stolen d. * Dabhefur hjoli stolib Ojofurinn. Again oblique subjects pattern like nominative subjects rather than like topicalized NPs. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE (25)a. AND FUNCTIONS GRAMMATICAL 453 Olafur leibinlegur. Pab hefur einhverjum frtt there has someone(D) thought Olaf(N) boring(N) Someone found Olaf boring. b. Olafur hefur einhverjuml06ttleibinlegur. leibinlegur. c. *Dab hefur Olafur einhverjum f6tt there has Olaf(N) someone(D) thoughtboring 2.6. SubjectEllipsis Another syntactic rule which distinguishessubjects from non-subjectsin modern Icelandic is the kind of ellipsis which allows the subject of a coordinated clause to be deleted under identity with the subject of the preceding conjunct clause.11(The examples in (26c, d) are grammatical only if the verb grafa is interpretedintransitivelyas 'to dig' rather than transitivelyas 'to bury'.) (26)a. b. c. d. fluttu llkib og heir grofu pab. Deir they(N) moved the-corpseand they buriedit Deirfluttu likib og _ grofu tab. D likib Peir fluttu og Peir grofu Likib var flutt og peir grofu e. *Likib hrxddi pa og - . (Subj-Subj) (Obj-Obj) . (Subj-Obj) grofu pab. The corpse scared them and __ (Obj-Subj) buried it. Now consider the coordination of sentences, one of which takes a nominative subject, the other an oblique subject. Consider the contrast between (27a) and (27b). Oblique subjects can be deleted under identity with nominativesubjects and vice versa, but objects cannot be deleted in 1' It is well-knownthat Old Norse allowed ratherfree ellipsis of NPs under identitywith some NP in the previous sentence. Faarlund(1980, p. 70) observes that ellipsis in Old Norse does not seem to be sensitive to either subjecthoodor nominativecase, but simply deletes any unstressedrecoverablepronoun;one could argue that the basic conditionwas not strict identitybut simplycoreference.However,in the modernScandinavianlanguages, such ellipsis or coordinationis sensitive to grammaticalrelations.The examplesin (26) are based on Faarlund'sNorwegianexamples.Note furtherthat sentences like (i) where both subject and object are elided are grammatical,if somewhatstilted: (i) Ieir fluttu likib og they moved the-body and.. grofu buried The syntacticconstraintson this kind of 'conjunct-splitting'merit furtherinvestigation. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 454 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON this fashion,even when their case is nominative.This is illustratedby the followingexamples: (27)a. Hann segist vera duglegur, en he(N) says-self to-be diligent, but..(D) finnst finds of Oungt. (Subj-Subj) verkefnib the-homeworktoo hard. He says he is diligent, but finds the homeworktoo hard. b. *Hann segist vera duglegur,en mer he(N) says-self to-be diligent, but I(D) latur. (Subj-Adj) find ..(N) lazy He says he is diligent, but I find [him] lazy. finnst It is clear that ellipsis is sensitive to grammaticalrelationsratherthan to morphologicalcase. In (27a), ellipsis is perfectly acceptable even though the cases differ, because both NPs are subjects. However (27b) is unacceptable, despite the fact that the coreferential NPs are both nominativecase, because only one of the two is a grammaticalsubject. (For further discussion see Eirikur Rognvaldsson 1982b, Bresnan & Thrainsson 1984.) 2.7. InfinitiveComplements Only subjects can be the target of EQUI-NP-Deletion, or be understood as 'arbitrary'or anaphoricallycontrolled PROs in Icelandic. (28)a. Eg vonast til ab fara heim. I hope for to go home b. Ab fara heim snemmaer ovenjulegt. to go home early is unusual Oblique subjects can also be the target of EQUI or be arbitraryPRO subjectsof infinitives,as shown in (29). The verb vanta 'to lack' takes an accusative subject and an accusative object. (29)a. Mig vantar peninga. me(A) lacks money(A) b. Eg vonast til I hope for ab vanta ekki peninga. (A) to lack not money(A) This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE c. AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 455 Ab vanta peninga er alltof algengt. to lack money is all-too common In conclusion, there are (at least) seven different syntactic criteria to distinguishsubjects from topics in active sentences in Icelandic, and we see that these tests do not necessarilypick out the nominativeNP. 3. WHICH POSTVERBAL NPs CAN PASSIVIZE IN ICELANDIC? 3.1. ObliqueSubjectsin IcelandicPassives We are now in a position to return to the question posed above: what is the grammaticalfunction of the preverbalNPs in passive sentences like those in (11) (repeatedhere for convenience): (11)a. b. var hj'alpab. Peim them(D) was helped Hennarvar saknab. her(G) was missed Note that regardlessof the person or numberof the initialNP, the verb is always in the 3rd person singular. Verbs agree in person and number with a nominativeargument;if there is no nominativeNP, then the verb occurs in the 3rd person (neuter) singular, which we take to be the unmarkedform. We will now test the syntacticbehaviorof passivizedoblique NPs using the same tests that we used above in section 2 for active sentences. As can be seen from the examples in the following sections, the preverbal dative NP in a sentence like (1 la) behaves in all respects like a grammatical subject. 3.1.1. Raising A first such test is Subject-to-ObjectRaising. (30) hafa verib hjalpabi profinu. Eg tel peim I believe them(D) to-have been helped in the-exam The preverbalNP undergoes Raising and keeps its dative case-marking. 3.1.2. Reflexivization A second test is reflexivization.Many speakers of Icelandic allow only This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 456 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON subjects to be the antecedents of reflexive pronounsor more accurately, allow objects to be antecedents only if the reflexive occurs in a predicative complement predicated of that object (Maling 1982). For such speakers, we find the same sort of contrastswith respect to Reflexivization as we did with respect to Raising. Note that the reflexive is obligatoryin (31). (31) Honum var oft hj'alpabaf foreldrum sinum/*hans. he(D) was often helped by parents his[+REFL]/ *[-REFL] 3.1.3. Subject-VerbInversion Third, the preverbal dative can immediatelyfollow the finite verb, in direct questions,or if another constituenthas been preposed. (32)a. b. Var honum aldrei hjalpabaf foreldrumslnum? was he(D) never helped by parents his I pr6finu svar honum vist hjalpab. in the-exam was he(D) apparentlyhelped 3.1.4. Extraction Fourth,many speakersof Icelandic do not generally allow Topicalization in bindingdomains;in other words, they allow Topicalizationto occur in embedded ab clauses ('that'-clauses),but not under indirect questions, comparatives, relatives, etc. However, these oblique NPs can appear preverballyin these contexts. (33)a. b. Hann telur, ab henni hafi verib hjalpabi gaer. he believes that she(D) has been helped yesterday Hvenaertelur hann ab henni hafi verib hjalpab? when believeghe that she(D) has been helped 3.1.5. IndefiniteSubjectPostposing Fifth, the preverbaldatives under considerationpatternlike grammatical subjects ratherthan like topicalizedNPs with respect to indefinitesubject postposing. (34) Dab hefur morgum studentum verib hjalpabi profinu. there has many(D) students(D) been helped on the-exam This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL 457 FUNCTIONS 3.1.6. SubjectEllipsis Sixth, the preverbaldative can be deleted by subject ellipsis. (35) Hann segist vera saklaus en he(N) says-self to-be innocentbut __(D) hefur vist has apparently verib hjalpab? i profinu. been helped on the-exam 3.1.7. InfinitiveComplements Seventh, just like other grammaticalsubjects the passivizeddative object can be the target of EQUI as illustrated in (36a); it can also be understoodas an arbitraryPRO subject of an infinitive, as in (36b). (36)a. Eg vonast til ab ver?a hjailpab. I hope for to be helped b. Ab vera hjailpabi pr6finu er oleyfilegt. to be helped on the-exam is un-allowed. Thus, by the above seven tests, the preverbaldative and genitive NPs in (11) are clearly grammaticalsubjects and not topicalizedobjects, and the sentences in (11) must be analyzed as ordinarypersonal passives. It is clear that for Icelandic, oblique case is not immune to passivization. 3.2. Passive and DitransitiveVerbs Icelandic has numerousditransitiveverbs. The possibilitiesare illustrated in (37) (from Thriainsson1979, pp. 21-22). (37)a. Deir leyndu Olaf sannleikanum. they concealed[from]-Olaf(A) the-truth(D) ACC-DAT b. Jon bab mig bonar. Jon asked me(A) a-favor(G) ACC-GEN c. Eg sagbi per soguna. I told you(D) a-story(A) DAT-ACC d. Olafur lofabi Mariu pessum hring. Olaf(N) promisedMary(D) this(D) ring(D) DAT-DAT e. ails Maria oskab 61afi gobs. Mary wished Olaf(D) everything(G)good(G) DAT-GEN This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON 458 We see that virtually any combination of two case-marked postverbal NPs is possible, except that the first NP can never be genitive case. There are several different types of ACC ACC verbs, but it is debatable whether any of them are truly ditransitive. First, there are transitiveverbs like kalia 'to name, call', as illustratedin (38). (38)a. Deir k6llubuhana Kiddu. they called her Kidda(A) b. Hun var kollub Kidda. she(N) was called Kidda(N) But this type of verb is not a true ditransitiveverb, since the second postverbalNP is clearly an (objective) complementrather than a verbal object. This can be seen from the passive version in (38b) where the 'retained complement' becomes nominative, agreeing with the subject NP it is predicatedof. Second, there are also simple transitiveverbs like hoggva 'to hit' and kyssa 'to kiss', which:'cantake an additionalargumentwhich might be describedas a cognate object. (39)a. hoggva einhvern banahogg to-hit someone(A) a-deadly-blow(A) b. sla' einhvern kinnhest to-hit someone(A) a-blow-on-the-cheek(A) c. kyssa einhvern rembingskoss to-kiss someone(A) a-deep- kiss(A) Only the first postverbal NP in such constructions can passivize, as illustrated in (40), in which case the second object not surprisingly remainsaccusative. banahogg. (40)a. Skarphehinn hjo Draiin hit a-deadly-blow(A) Thrain(A) Skarphedin(N) b. Drainn var hoggvinn banahogg. a-deadly-blow(A) Thrain(N) was hit c. *Banahoggvarnh6ggvib Prain. We will brieflydiscuss the analysisof this type of ACC ACC verb below in section 4.4. A third possible type of ACC ACC verb, pointed out to us by an anonymous reviewer, consists of simple transitive verbs like keyra This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 459 'drive', which can optionally be followed by a second accusative NP indicatingthe thematicrole of 'path'. Here, too, only the first postverbal NP can passivize, and the second NP remainsaccusative. (41)a. Hann keyrbibilinn Pessa leib. he drove the-car(A) this route(A) b. Billinn var keyrburpessa leib(A). the-car(N) was driven this route(A) var keyrb bilinn. c. *Dessileib this route(N) was driven the-car(A) Similar facts obtain for the verb aka 'drive', which takes DAT rather than ACC case on 'car'. We will discuss the analysisof these verbs, too, in section 4.4. To the extent that none of the above-mentioned types are truly ditransitive,12there seem to be no triadic verbs in Icelandic where both objects are marked accusative. We will assume this to be an accidental gap. Nothing in our account of case marking would prevent such a combination. Ditransitiveverbs fall into two classes with respect to passive. In the first class, only the first postverbal NP passivizes, as illustratedby the following exampleswith the DAT DAT verb skila 'to return': (42)a. Eg skilabi henni peningunum. I returnedher(D) the-money(D) b. Henni var skilab peningunum. she(D) was retumedthe-money(D) c. *Peningunumvar skilabhenni. d. Eg tel henni hafa verib skilab peningunum. I believe her(D) to-have been returnedthe-money e. *Eg tel peningunumhafa verib skilabhenni. 12 The well-known class of exceptions to passive like weigh includes one superficially ditransitiveACC ACC verb, namely,kosta 'to cost' illustratedin (i). (i) Dabkostabimig eina kronu. it cost me(A) one kronur(A) Just as in English, neither postverbalNP passivizes.Our guess is that this is because the subject is not agentive enough. The classificationof kosta thus depends on whether one thinksthat a necessarycriterionfor (di)transitivityis having a passivizableobject. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 460 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON The behavior of these verbs is straightforward,the only noteworthy detail being that a DAT object passivizes; but this should not be at all surprisingafter the discussionof the previous section. Examples (42a-c) contrast nicely with the following, where the verb skila is used as a simple transitiveverb, taking a DAT theme but with the goal realized as a PP complementinstead of a DAT NP. (43)a. Eg skilabi peningunum til hennar. I retumedthe-money(D) to her(G) b. Peningunum var skilab til hennar. the-money(D) was returnedto her We see that when. peningunum is the sole bare NP argument, it can passivize. These facts seem to suggest that the mapping between grammatical functions and phrase structure positions in Icelandic is fairly fixed: subjects are mapped onto the preverbalNP and direct objects are mapped onto the immediatelypostverbalNP. However, this mapping is not the only possibilityfor objects, as shown by the passivizationpattern of the second class of ditransitives,namely DAT ACC verbs like gefa 'to give'. In some sense, the DAT ACC pattern represents the core class of ditransitiveverbs: only these verbs survive as ditransitivesin English and the Scandinavian languages other than Icelandic, i.e., in related languages without morphological case marking. All the other ditransitive verbs have become instead verbs taking NP PP complements (presumably because at least one of the NPs is semanticallyquite predictable,e.g. a recipient). For DAT ACC verbs, either postverbalNP can passivize, as illustrated in (44). Note that in (44a) the retained object is nominative (ratherthan accusative); this rather surprisingfact will follow from the principlesfor default case markinggiven in (61d) below. amb'attir. (44)a. Konunginumvoru gefnar the-king(D) were given(f.pl.) maidservants(N.f.pl.) The king was given female slaves. b. var gefin Amb'attin konunginum the-maidservant(N,sg)was given(f.sg.) the-king(D) As noted above, the verb agrees with the nominative argument even when this argumentis not the grammaticalsubject, as in (44a). To show that in both (44a) and (44b), the object has indeed become a This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 461 subject, we will run the examples through the tests for subjecthood summarizedpreviously. (45) Raising ambattir. a. Eg tel konunginumhafa verib gefnar I believe the-king(D) have been given(f.pl.) slaves(N) I believe the king to have been given slaves. b. ambattina hafa verib gefna Eg tel konunginum. I believe the-slave(A) have been given(A) the-king(D) I believe the slave to have been given to the king. (46) Reflexivization a. Konunginumvoru gefnar amb'attiri holl the-king(D) were given slaves in palace sinni/?hans. his(+REFL/?-REFL) b. Ambattin var gefin konunginumvegna fegurbar the-slave(N) was given the-king(D) because-of beauty sinnar/?hennar. her(+REFL/?-REFL) (47) Subject-VerbInversion a. Um veturinn voru konunginumgefnar ambaittir. in the-winterwere the-king(D) given slaves(N) In the winter, the king was given (female) slaves. b. Um veturinn var ambfittin gefin konunginum. in the-winterwas the-slave(N) given the-king(N) In the winter, the slave was given to the king. (48)a. Voru konunginumgefnar ambaittir? were the-king(D) given slaves(N) Was the king given slaves? b. Var amb'attin gefin konunginum? was the-slave(N) given the-king(D) Was the slave given to the king? This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 462 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. T14RAINSSON (49) Extraction a. Hvaba ambaittir heldur pui ab konunginum verbi gefnar? which slaves(N) think you that the-king(D) will-be given b. Konunginum held eg ab verbi ambattin gefin. the-king(D) believe I that the-slave(N) will-be given To the king I think that the slave will be given. (50) IndefiniteSubjectPostposing a. Dab voru konungi gefnar ambattir i vetur. there was king(D) given slaves(N) in winter There was a king given slaves this winter. b. Pab var ambatt gefin konunginum i vetur. there was slave(N) gefin konunginum(D)in winter (51) SubjectEllipsis a. Konungarnir f6ru viba og - voru the-kings(N) traveledwidely and -(D) were oft gefnar ambattir. often given slaves b. Ambattin kom fra Irlandi og - the-slave came from Ireland and -(N) var was gefin konunginum. given the-king(D) (52) InfinitiveComplements a. Ab vera gefnar ambattir var mikill heibur. to be given slaves(N) was great honor miklumvonbrigbum. b. Ab vera gefin konunginumolli to be given the-king(D) caused great disappointment To be given to the king caused great disappointment. c. Ambattin vonast til ab verba gefin konunginum. the-slave(N) hopes for to be given the-king(D) The slave hopes to be given to the king. These tests show that both the dative object and the accusative object can be made grammaticalsubjects by passive. When the dative object is passivized, the postverbal 'retained object' appears in the nominative case, and the verb agrees with it in number. At first glance, this might This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 463 seem to contradict our conclusion that it is the preverbal dative NP which is the subject, but as we will show below, the nominative case markingis predictable.The accusative object of the active becomes the nominativesubject of the passive, and passes the same subjecthoodtests with equal success. 4. PASSIVE IN LFG In lexical-functional grammar, actives and passives are related by a lexical redundancyrule. Given the existence of impersonal passives in Icelandic, it seems reasonable to dissociate the part of the rule that relates the active subject to the passive by-phrasefrom the part of the rule that associates the active object with the passive subject. Hence we have the two rules in (53), whose effect is illustratedin (54): (53)a. b. SUBJ~- AF-OBJ/0 OBJ +SUBJ (54) agent theme taka: V, 'take' (SUBJ, OBJ) tekinn: V[+part] (AF-OBJ, SUBJ) We ignore here the fact that the agent is only rarelyexpressedin passive sentences in Icelandic. A treatmentof optional agents,can be found in Bresnan (1982a); for the use of 0 in Lexical rules, see Bresnan(1982b, p. 166). The auxiliaryverbs vera and verbawill be treated as 'raising'verbs taking a SUBJ and an XCOMP. No thematic role is associated with the subject of these verbs and they get whatever subject their XCOMP gets (see Kaplan & Bresnan (1982) for furtherexplanationof the formalism used in LFG). The morphological principles assigning case will be discussed below. 4.1. GrammaticalFunctions,ThematicRoles and Case-Marking in Icelandic Our account of passive in Icelandic is based on the LFG analysis of passive sketched above. Note in particularthat only NPs bearing the grammaticalfunction OBJ passivize. To develop a full account of the passive construction, we need to specify how these rules interact with other components of the grammar.In this section, we will outline our view of this interaction, particularlywith respect to thematic roles and case-markingrules. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 464 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON 4.1.1. The Lexical Component For the sake of clarity, we restate here the distinction made in LFG between semantically restricted and semantically unrestricted Grammatical Functions (GFs). The same distinction is made in Relational Grammarbetween 'obliques' and 'terms'. SUBJ, OBJ, and 2OBJ are semantically unrestrictedfunctions because they can bear any type of thematic role, dependingon the verb. For example, in (55a) the SUBJ is an agent, whereas in (55b) it is an instrument. (55)a. The presidentkissed the baby. b. The wrench opened the safe. This characteristic of SUBJs, OBJs and 2OBJs contrasts with the behavior of GFs such as with-phrasesand to-phrases.While these PPs may bear more than one function, the functions are restricted by the form of the PP: there is no verb in English that expresses its goal by means of a with-phrase,or its instrumentby means of a to-phrase.(This is necessarily an oversimplification;see Bresnan (1982c), for a more elaborate discussion, and Levin (1985) for further developments.)Thus the nonverbal constituents of a sentence can be classified as follows into two kinds of verbal argumentsplus a category of adjuncts. (56) ARG: (i) semanticallyunrestricted(SUBJ, OBJ, 2OBJ) (ii) semanticallyrestricted all types of adjuncts ADJ: 4.1.2. Case Assignment Case can be assigned in (at least) three different ways,13which we will call semantic, lexical or idiosyncratic, and functional case assignment. SEMANTIC case-markingincludes such traditional'adverbial'categories as accusatives of time or duration, and instrumentaldatives; these are illustrated in (57).14 (57)a. b. Straikurinn beib allan dagin. the-boy waited all(A) day(A) Hann tok vini sinum opnum ormum. he took friend his[+REFL] open(D) arms(D) He greeted his friend with open arms. 13 Although our terminology may differ, these are the same three types of case distinguished,e.g., in Hjelmslev,and more recently in a GB-frameworkin Babby (1980) and Freidinand Babby (1983). 14 These examplesare taken from Fribjonsson(1978). This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 465 We will not consider this type of case assignment in any detail here. Depending on their function, semanticallycase-markedconstituents are either semanticallyrestricted functions or adjuncts. IDIOSYNCRATIC or lexical case marking is an idiosyncratic property of a lexical item, assigned by a verb, preposition or adjective. We assume that idiosyncratic case is associatedwith a particularthematicrole, and that this case marking is assigned before thematic roles are associated with grammaticalfunctions.FUNCTIONAL case markingis what is widely referredto. as regular or "default" case marking, which results in nominative subjects and accusative objects. It is sensitive to surface grammatical relations, and hence applies after all association principles and reassociationrules, includingpassive. 4.1.3. The MappingbetweenThematicRoles and GrammaticalFunctions Our account of passive relies on the existence of association principles which establish a mapping between thematic roles and grammatical relations. The existence of such association principles is assumed in various current linguistic theories, but has not been fully fleshed out.15 Our account makes use of both universal and language-specific association principles. We postulate a level of representationat which the valency of a verb is determinedand its argumentscan be distinguishedin terms of thematic roles. At this level of thematic structure, verbs like dansa 'dance' and lesa 'read' have the following representations: (58) dansa 'dance' (agent) lesa 'read' (agent, theme) The relationship between different verb forms can be represented in termsof the optional suppressionof an argument.For example,we might want to express the relation between the causative and non-causative forms of a verb like faekka 'decrease' as follows: (59) fa!kka 'decrease' ((agent), theme) 15 It is clear that this general schema owes much to the work of Anderson (1977) and Wasow (1977, 1980), to Ostler (1979) and Marantz(1981), and to Amritavelli (1980), Rappaport (1983) and Levin (1985). We do not have the time here to spell out the importanceof these various contributions.These accounts assume the existence of something like a thematiclevel of representation.It is not completelyclear to us that this is a purely linguisticlevel; it seems ratherto be a level that mediates between cognitive and linguisticrepresentations.In this paper,however,we will treat it as a linguisticlevel. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 466 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON Idiosyncratic case-marking takes this level as its input. Thus we are assuming idiosyncratic case is assigned to thematic roles and not to grammatical functions. This assumption is motivated by the fact that for a given verb, the idiosyncratic case-marking associated with a given thematic role is preserved under rules like passive and raising when the GF changes. Hence verbs like gata 'take care of' or lofa 'promise'get the following representationsafter the assignmentof idiosyncraticcase: (60) gzeta 'take care of' (agent, theme) GEN iofa 'promise'(agent, theme, goal) DAT DAT A set of association principles determines which thematic role will be mapped onto which grammatical function. Some of the association principles seem to be universal, whereas others are language-specific. Where the universal and the language-specific principles interact, the language specific ones take precedence, and the universal ones act as elsewhere conditions. One universal principle that has often been proposed is that the agent role is associated with the SUBJECT function. We will assume that this principle is by and large correct.16 We also assumea hierarchyof grammaticalfunctions.17 Given these background assumptions, we can now formulate the following set of associationprinciplesfor Icelandic. (61) IcelandicAssociationPrinciples a. If there is only one thematic role, it is assigned to SUBJ; if there are two, they are assignedto SUBJ and OBJ; if there are three, they are assigned to SUBJ, OBJ, 2OBJ. (Universal) b. AGENTS are linked to SUBJ. (Universal) c. Case-markedTHEMES are assigned to the lowest available GF. (Language Specific) d. Default Case Marking:18the highest available GF is assigned NOM case, the next highest ACC. (Universal) 16 We have ignoredthroughoutthe problemposed by the existence of ergative languages. It is not clear to us whetherergativityshould be thoughtof as a difference in association principles('deep' ergativity)or in case-markingprinciples('surface'ergativity)or both. 17 The hierarchyof grammaticalrelationsstems from work in Relational Grammar(see Perlmutter(ed.) 1983, Cole & Sadock 1977). 18 The default case-marking is in a certain sense the equivalent of "structuralcase marking"in GB, except that we allow for exceptionssuch as nominativeobjects (see 62b). This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 467 4.2. Applyingthe AssociationPrinciples These association principles are not specific to passives; they also account for preverbaloblique NPs in active sentences, as in (62). (62)a. b. Mer er kalt. me(D) is cold. Henni hefur alltaf pott Olafur leibinlegur. her(D) has always thought Olaf(N) boring(N) We assume that Icelandic does not have any impersonal verbs in the sense of 'subjectless'verbs except for those with no semantic arguments, e.g., weather-verbs,or those with prepositionalphrase complementsbut no argumentsrealized as bare NPs. Now note that the association principles given here allow for two different assignmentsof GFs in the case of accusative THEMES which do not bear idiosyncraticcase. The associationconvention in (61c) does not say anythingabout them, since it only specifies that idiosyncratically markedthemes will be associatedwith the lowest available GF. Hence in the ditransitive cases where we have a non-case-marked theme and another nonsubject argument,we can assign the GFs in two ways. For a verb like gefa 'to give', we will get the following two possibilities. (63) a. b. gefa: V(agent theme goal) [+dat] SUBJ OBJ 2OBJ SUBJ 2OBJ OBJ Note that this type of dual association resolves the problem of double objects. Although we'can associate either the THEME or the GOAL with the direct object in the lexical form of a verb like give, we will not be able to associate both argumentswith the direct object at the same time because the association principles respect functional uniqueness. Whichever GF the GOAL is linked to will get markedwith the (lexically assigned) dative case; whichever GF the THEME is linked to, it will get marked accusative in the active version by virtue of the principle of default case marking. This solution is not empiricallyequivalent to a solution in which we have two direct objects. It predicts that whatever othercharacteristicof OBJ we might find in the language, it will not apply to the immediately postverbalNP (the 'retainedobject') of a ditransitiveverb in the passive, because that NP must have been assigned to 2OBJ. To test this predic- This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 468 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON tion, other syntactic rules that single out OBJs as opposed to all other postverbal GFs in Icelandic must be found. Rognvaldsson (1982) discusses some data which might illustrate such a rule, and which support our analysis of DAT ACC verbs.19 The evidence is based on the interaction of heavy NP shift and reflexivization (at least for those speakers who allow nonsubject antecedents). Rognvaldsson shows (pp. 133-135) that the reflexivizationpossibilitiesreflect the underlyingorder of the two postverbal NPs. This is illustratedfor an ACC DAT verb, svipta 'deprive', by the examples in (64), taken from Rognvaldsson (his [74a, b] and [80].) (64)a. Sjorinnsvipti hannii [manni sinumi] the-sea deprivedher(A) husband(D) her[+REFL] The sea deprived heri of heri husband. b. *Sj6rinnsvipti [konu sinai] manninumi the-sea deprived wife(A) his[+REFL] the-man(D) The sea deprived hisi wife of the mani. c. *Sj6rinnsvipti manninumi [gomlu konuna sinlai... ]NP the-man(D) old the-wife his[+REFL] The sea deprived of the mani hisi old wife. Sentence (64b) is ungrammatical because the supposed antecedent doesn't precede the reflexive; however, crucially, applying heavy-NP shift to the first NP, as in (64c), does not make the reflexive possible. One possible interpretationof this fact (not Rognvaldsson's)is that an OBJ but not a 2OBJ may be the antecedent of a reflexive. This generalization(togetherwith linearprecedence) accounts for the contrast noted by Rognvaldssonin footnote 25 (pp. 231-2). The DAT ACC class of ditransitive verbs appear to have not one but two reflexivization patterns: i.e., either object can be an antecedent. The following sentences are based on Rognvaldsson'sexamples. (65)a. Eg gaf konungi [ambaittinasina.] I gave a-king (D) slave(A) his[+REFL] 19 We would like to thank an anonymousreader for pointing out the relevance of these data. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 469 b. Eg gaf amb'attina [konungi sinum]Np I gave the-slave(A) king(D) his[+REFL] Example(65b) contrastswith (64c) above. [Note that the shifted (Dative) NP in (65b) need not be especially heavy.] Given our analysis of DAT ACC verbs as getting dual assignmentsto GFs, these facts receive a very simple and natural explanation: an OBJ but not a 2OBJ can be the antecedent of a reflexive. Given the under-determinationof GF assignments embodied in the principles(61a-d) and the alternativeassignments displayedin (63), this follows naturally. As pointed out by an anonymousreviewer, further support for alternative assignmentsas in (63) seems to come from the facts discussed in Bernodusson(1982, pp. 37-8). He observes that for many active DAT NOM verbs (verbs with DAT subjects and NOM objects) either order feels natural ("eblileg"); this intuition about 'unmarked' word order correlateswith the fact that either argumentcan occur between the finite and non-finiteV, a position reservedfor grammaticalsubjects. The facts for actives are analogous to the facts for DAT-NOM passives derived from DAT ACC ditransitiveverbs. The solution proposed here is preferable to one in which we would allow 2OBJ to passivize in Icelandic. Under such a proposal, we would need a different principle to exclude double passivizationin the case of ditransitiveverbs other than with DAT ACC objects,20a restrictionthat was illustratedabove in (42). We conclude this section by presentingChart I, which summarizesthe case markingand grammaticalfunctionsassignedto the argumentsof the various classes of ditransitiveverbs. The default case-markingprinciple stated in (61d) correctly accounts for a rather surprisingfact already noted in passing. Recall that in the passive sentence in (44a), the postverbal theme is marked nominative, even through it is not the grammaticalsubject but rathera 2OBJ. Since the theme is not assigned idiosyncraticcase by this class of ditransitive verbs, whetheror not it becomes the subject in the passive version, it will be the highest GF not alreadyassignedcase. Hence it will get nominative case by the default case-markingprinciple. 20 Dual assignmentshave been shown to be independentlymotivatedin other languages, and hence preferableto analyseswhich assumeeither that 2OBJs can passivizeor that the same sentence can contain two (direct)OBJs. One such languageis Kikuyuas discussedin Zaenen (1983). Given that this possibility of alternative assignmentsdoes exist in the world's languages, we can make use of it for Icelandic. Our conclusion, then, is that Icelandicdoes not violate the functionaluniquenessprinciple,and can be analyzedalong lines that are knownto be independentlynecessaryfor other languages. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A. ZAENEN, 470 J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON CHART I Verb Type Thematic Roles DAT-ACC Agent Theme Goal ACC-DAT Agent Theme Source ACC-GEN DAT-DAT DAT-GEN Agent Theme Goal Lexical Case marking Grammatical Functions Default Case-marking NOM ACC DAT SUBJ (61b) 2OBJ/OBJ (61a) OBJ/20BJ (61a) SUBJ (61b) 2OBJ (61c) OBJ (61a) NOM DAT NOM GEN SUBJ (61b) 2OBJ (61c) OBJ (61a) ACC ACC Agent Theme Goal SUBJ (61b) 2OBJ (61c) OBJ (61a) NOM DAT DAT Agent Theme Goal SUBJ (61b) 2OBJ (61c) OBJ (61a) NOM GEN DAT 4.3. Verbsof VariablePolyadicity The associationprinciplesmake some furtherpredictionsthat are borne out by the data. As in English, many verbs can be used with variable numbersof grammaticalarguments.For example, the verb oska 'to wish' can be either a simple transitive verb taking a genitive THEME, or a ditransitivetaking a dative GOAL and a genitive THEME; in other words,the goal argumentis optional. (66)a. Du' hefur Oskab henni Pess. you have wished her(D) this(G) Ni hefur Oskabpess. b. c. * Pi hefur Oskabhenni. Given the association principles for Icelandic in (61), the mapping between theta-rolesand GFs is determinedby the polyadicityof the verb. Hence we predict that for such verbs, the ability of a NP with a given thematic role (e.g., the THEME) to passivize will also depend on the polyadicityof the verb. This can be seen from the different assignments of the OBJ-functionin (67): This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL (67) oska: V(agent FUNCTIONS 471 theme (goal)) [+gen] [+dat] a. SUBJ 2OBJ b. SUBJ OBJ OBJ Only when a NP is assigned to OBJ will it passivize.7 As an idiosyncratically case-marked theme, bess can only be assigned to the lowest available GF. When the verb is used as a simple transitive,this will be the OBJ function, and hence tess will passivize and raise as shown in (68a). But when oska is used as a ditransitive,pess will be assigned to 2OBJ, and hence will be unable to become the subject of a passive and subsequentlybe raised. This is illustratedby the contrastin (68). (68)a. Dess var Oskab (*henni). this(G) was wished(her(D)) b. Eg tel Pess hafa verib Oskab (*henni) I believe this(G) to-have been wished (her(D)) c. Henni var Oskabpess. d. henni hafa verib 'skab tess. Eg tel I believe her(D) to-have been wished this(G) Other verbs that work the same way include leyna 'to conceal' ACC DAT; bibja 'to ask' ACC GEN; skila 'to return' DAT DAT; hota 'to threaten' DAT DAT; synja 'to deny' DAT GEN.22The judgments are 21 Note furtherthatpassivein Icelandicis not restrictedto NPs that are thematicallylinked to the passiveverb. We also find passivesin the so-called raisingconstructions,as shownin (i). (i) Hun er talin vera dugleg. she(N) is believedto-be conscientious(N). Such examplesindicatethat passive in Icelandicis not a lexical rule in the sense of Wasow (1977) or Chomsky(1981). We have not taken Raisingand Equi constructionsinto account here; see Andrews(1982b) for a fuller treatmentof case-assignmentin ModernIcelandic. 22 We can speculateabout the reason for the distributionof DOs in Icelandic:on the one hand all immediatelypostverbalNPs that are argumentsof the verb are DOs, regardlessof their case marking;on the other hand all postverbalaccusativesthat are neitherXCOMPs nor have a semantically restricted function can passivize. It is difficult to resist the temptationto link these observationsto the fact that Icelandicis a languagethat has at the same time a ratherrigidwordorderand a rich case markingsystem.It has often been noted that languages have two major strategies to encode grammaticalrelations,the one being word order,and the other case marking.Icelandic does both, and it can be assumedthat it is this convergenceof propertiesthat is illustratedin the notion of direct object that is used in the language: a DO is an argumentthat either immediatelyfollows the verb or is an accusativeobject. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 472 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON extremely clear-cut. These fact show clearly that passive is not sensitive to thematic roles but must be expressed as operations on grammatical functions. 4.4. Some FurtherAlternations A few furtheralternationsremainto be accountedfor. Since we have not studied these alternationsextensively, our proposals here are very tentative, but we want to give some indication of how such alternations could be analyzedwithin the general frameworkwe are assuming. In section 3.2, we noted the existence of a few different types of ACC-ACC verbs, some of which were arguablyditransitive.The first type, which was illustrated in (38) and is repeated here for convenience, is clearly not ditransitive: (38)a. Deir k6llubu hana Kiddu. they called her Kidda(A) b. Hun var kdllub Kidda. she(N) was called Kidda(N) In verbs of this type, the second NP is neither an OBJ nor a 2OBJ, but rather an XCOMP, i.e., an (objective) complement, and hence falls outside the scope of this paper, since predicative complements never passivize. The status of Ahe second type, illustrated in (39), isn't so clear: (39)a. hoggva einhvern banahogg to-hit someone(A) a-deadly-blow(A) b. slai einhvern kinnhest to-hit someone(A) a-blow-on-the-cheek(A) c. kyssa einhvern rembingskoss to-kiss someone(A) a-deep-kiss(A) Here we have an alternationbetween a simple transitiveform and what looks like a ditransitiveform of the verb. For ease of exposition, let us refer to the second accusative, e.g., 'a deadly blow', as a COGNATE ACCUSATIVE. The cognate accusative might be analyzed as (some kind of) an object; alternatively,it might be that the cognate accusative is actually a semanticallyrestricted GF, i.e., one restricted to a particular thematic role. As such, it would be neither an OBJ nor a 2OBJ, and hence would not participatein any of the alternationswe are discussing. The principles we have already formulated give us a way to test the This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 473 correctness of this analysis:if alternatingforms exist in which only the cognate ACC is present, do these monotransitive forms have passive counterparts?If the cognate accusative in sentences like (39) is assigned to a semantically unrestrictedfunction, then its function is simply the highest available function given the polyadicity of the verb; we would then expect it to passivizewhen it is assigned to the OBJ-function.If, on the other hand, it does not passivize when it is the sole postverbal NP, then we would want to look for aguments that indeed in both the ditransitiveand monotransitiveforms the cognate accusative is either a semanticallyrestricted (OBLique) argument, or not an argumentat all. It turns out that the cognate accusative can at least marginallybe the sole postverbal argument,and in such examples it can be passivized, as illustratedin (69). (69)a. b. c. d. Njall hj6 aldrei banahogg. Njal hit never a-deadly-blow(A) var hoggvib 'a mibnatti. ?Banah6ggib the-deadly-blow(N) was struck at midnight J6n kyssir bara rembingskossa. Jon kisses only deep-kisses(A) ?Rembingskossareru oft kysstiri astars6gum. deep-kisses(N) are often kissed in love-stories Crucially,the ACC case markingturnsinto NOMinative,showing that in these examples at least, it was not idiosyncraticallyassigned. These facts suggest that the cognate accusative should be analyzed as a 2OBJ in exampleslike (39), but as an OBJ when it is the sole postverbalNP, as in (69a, c).23 If this is right, the question becomes: what is its thematic role? Consider the example in (39a). The relation of hoggva 'hit' to the animateobject einhvern 'someone' seems to be the same with or without the cognate accusative; moreover, the thematic role of the cognate accusative is presumablythe same with or without the 'hittee'. Although the 'hittee' is arguably a GOAL rather than a THEME, it seems unreasonableto assign the THEME role to 'a deadly blow' in either type 23 This is not the only possibleway to look at the data. We leave it to the readerto devise other possibilities,too tedious to spell out at this point. The one we are exploringhere, i.e., the analysisas 2OBJ, is the one that seems the most problematicfor our generalaccountof passive, but also the one that intuitivelyseems to us to be the most likely. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 474 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON of sentence. For most ditransitiveverbs, the THEME is an obligatory argument. The cognate accusative, on the other hand, is merely an optional complement; moreover it certainly does not undergo the movement or action described by the verb 'hit'. It seems to be a semanticallyempty argument,one which cannot really be questioned or focussed. In fact its semantic contributionto the meaning of the verb is ratherlike a manneradverbial.Just what role it bears is not clear to us. What is clear is that such cognate accusativesnecessitate the formulation of anotherassociationprinciple,to make sure that they are alwayslinked to 2OBJ if anotherNP-argumentis present,since this assignmentwill not follow automaticallyfrom the principlesin (61) if the cognate accusative is not an idiosyncraticallycase-markedtheme. It also appears that these NPs are part of the VP. Consider the placementof an adverblike sjaldan'rarely'in sentences with an auxiliary verb, as illustratedin (70). (70)a. b. *Hann hefur gefib barninu sjaldanpelann. he has given the-child(D) rarely the-bottle(A) *Hann hefur hoggvib ovini sjaldanbanahogg. he has struck enemies(A) rarely a-deadly-blow(A) Thrainsson(1984) argues that there is a class of adverbsin Icelandic (including sjaldan),that can occur anywherein S as an immediatedaughter of S (as long as other word order constraints,such as the verb-second constraintare observed), but not embedded at a lower level, e.g., within VP. He argues that in sentences with an auxiliary,there is a VP-node following the (finite) auxiliary and dominating the verb and its (postverbal) arguments. If his arguments are correct, the examples in (70) indicate that cognate objects like banahogg 'deadly blow' in (70b) are within the VP just like regular objects such as pelann 'the bottle' in (70a). A third type of double accusative can be found with verbs like keyra 'drive'. Here we find the following alternations, pointed out by an anonymousreviewer: (71)a. Hann keyrbibilinn pessa leib. he drove the-car(A) this route(A) b. Hann keyrbiPessa leib. he drove this route(A) The second accusative in (71a) behaves in some respects like an object, This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 475 namely, it passivizeswhen it is the sole postverbalNP: (72) Iessi leib hefur aldrei verib keyrb. this route(N) has never been driven However, when two postverbalNPs are present, as in (71a), only the first NP passivizes: (73)a. Billinn var keyrburpessa leib. the-car(N) was driven this route(A) b. *lessi leib var keyrb bilinn. this-route(N)was driven the-car(A) Here again, we have a pattern in which only the first postverbal NP passivizes, confirmingprinciple (61a). (Similarlyfor another verb, aka 'drive', but with DAT ratherthan ACC case on 'the-car'.) We will assume that some transitive verbs allow the PATH-role, normally realized as a PP, to be assigned to the OBJ function when that position is otherwise unfilled. It is not entirely clear, however, how to analyze the accusative Pessa leib 'this route' when it is the second postverbal NP and the OBJ function is filled, as in (7 Ia). Such accusatives are frequently referred to as adverbials or semantic accusatives, which would suggest that they are not ARGuments (20BJ) of the verb, but rather some sort of ADJunct. If that were the case, one might expect them to contrast with regular verbal arguments with respect to the VP-test discussed above. While most speakers agree that there is some difference, the judgments are unfortunately not very clear. (74)a. ?*Jon hefur gefih barninu oft pennan pela. Jon has given the-child(D) often this bottle(A) b. ?Omar hefur ekib bilnum oft Oessaleib. Omar has driven the-car(D) often this route(A) In addition, the placement of manner adverbs gives a similar (weak) contrast: (75)a. ??Hannmun gefa barninu mjog varlega pennan pela. he will give the-child(D) very carefully this bottle(A) b. Omarmun keyra bilinn mjog varlega bessa leib. he will drive the-car(A) very carefully this route(A) It is not obvious what to make of these rather subtle differences. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON 476 However, within our framework, the fact that Iessa leib remains accusative when the dative object of aka 'to drive' is passivized clearly dictates treatingit as an adverbialadjunctratherthan a verbal argument. To summarize,we have proposed the following representationsat the thematic level for the two classes of verbs discussed in this subsection. (76)a. hoggva 'strike' (agent, (theme), (cognate-obj)) b. keyra 'drive' (agent, theme)or (agent, path) c. aka 'drive' (agent, theme)or (agent, path) [+DAT] The possible associationswith GFs are shown in (77). (77)a. hoggva: (agent (theme) (cognate-obj)) SUBJ OBJ 2OBJ SUBJ SUBJ OBJ OBJ b. keyra: (agent theme) SUBJ OBJ or (agent path) SUBJ OBJ c. aka: (agent theme) [+DAT] SUBJ OBJ or (agent path) SUBJ OBJ If both the theme and the path-role for the verbs keyra or aka are realized, then the path is an ADJunct and not an ARGument of the verb. 5. GERMAN ASSOCIATION PRINCIPLES Having demonstrated that in Icelandic oblique NPs can be made grammatical subjects by passivization, we need to address the question of whether the same is true of German, which has constructions that are superficially similar. Consider the German equivalent of an Icelandic sentence like (4), repeated here for convenience. (78) Ihm wurde geholfen. Honum var hjailpab him(D) was (German) (Icelandic) helped. Although the work on German syntax has not led to as extensive a study of the syntactic properties of subjects as has been made for Icelandic, it is clear that the German facts do not parallel the Icelandic ones. There are no reasons to assume that ihm in (78) above is a grammatical subject. Thus the German examples, while superficially very similar to the Icelandic ones, are functionally quite different. Some of the evidence This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 477 supportingthis conclusion is illustratedin the following examples,mostly from Cole et al. (1978). Subjects of infinitivesin Germancan be controlled (either functionally or anaphorically),but this is not possible with the passives of verbs taking oblique case marking. ArbitraryPROs can be found as the understood subject of German infinitivals,as shown in (79a), but not with idiosyncraticallymarkedNPs, as shown in (79b). (79)a. Im Sommerzu reisen ist angenehm. in summer to travel is agreeable To travel in the summeris nice. b. *Geholfen zu werden ist angenehm. is agreeable helped to be To be helped.is nice. c. Aufgenommenzu werden ist angenehm. admiited to be is agreeable To be admittedis nice. Similarly,EQUI-control is not possible with the passives of verbs taking oblique objects. German has a rule of EQUI control, illustratedin (80). (80) Er hofft weg zu gehen. he hopes away to go As shown in (81a), this rule applies to passives; however, as illustrated (81b), it does not apply to the dative NP of a passive sentence like (78). (81)a. Er hofft aufgenommenzu werden. he hopes admitted to be b. *Ihm/*Er hofft geholfen zu werden. him(D)/he(N) hopes helped to be Further examples can be found in Cole et al. (1978); for example, the preverbal oblique NPs cannot be deleted under identity was a (nominative) subject. (82)a. b. Er kam und (er) besuchte die Kinder. he(N) came and (he) visited the children Er kam und (er) wurde verhaftet. he came and (he) was arrested c. *Er kam und - he came and __(D) wurde geholfen. was helped This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 478 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, d. *Er sah die Damen und - he saw the ladies and AND H. THRAINSSON gefielen sie. (D) pleased them(f.) e. *Er sah die Damen und sie gefielen he saw the ladies and they pleased (D) Nor can the oblique NP be deleted by relative clause reduction. (83)a. Der das Buch lesende Junge heisst Wilhelm. the [the book reading] boy is-named Wilhelm b. *Das der Junge lesende Buch heisst Sieben Legenden. the [the boy reading book] is-named Seven Legends c. Das vom Jungen gelesene Buch heisst the [by-the boy read] book is-named Sieben Legenden. Seven Legends d. *Der vom Lehrer geholfene Junge bekam eine gute Note. the by-the teacherhelped] boy got a good grade c. *Der das Buch gefallende Junge sitzt da in der Ecke. the [the book pleasing] boy sits there in the corner Thus the same type of tests that show that oblique NPs can be grammatical subjects in Icelandic show that the Germananalogues cannot be analyzedas such. As in Icelandic, the 'quirky'argumentsof passive verbs do not behave differently in this respect from the quirky arguments of active forms. Consideractive constructionslike the one illustratedin (84). (84) Mir ist ubel. me(D) is nasty I am nauseated. Here too it can be shown that mir does not behave like a grammatical subject, as illustrated in the following examples. (85)a. *Mir hofft ubel zu sein. me(D) hopes nauseated to be *Ich hoffe ubel zu sein. I(N) hope nauseated to be b. *Ubel zu sein ist unangenehm. nauseated to be is disagreeable This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 479 In spite of the superficialsimilaritiesbetween Icelandic and German,the analysis of constructions without nominative arguments (be they active or passive voice) in the two languages must be quite different. We can account for these differences by means of one language-specific association principle for German,which replaces the language-specificprinciple for Icelandic in (61c): (86) Case-markedTHEMATIC ROLES are assigned to 2OBJ. In other words, idiosyncraticallymarked argumentsare associated with the function 2OBJ regardlessof the valence of the verb. Thus the entire set of Association Principlesfor Germanis as given in (87): (87) GermanAssociationPrinciples a. If there is only one thematic role, it is assigned to SUBJ; if there are two, they are assignedto SUBJ and OBJ; if there are three, they are*assigned to SUBJ, OBJ, 2OBJ. (Universal) b. AGENTS are linked to SUBJ. (Universal) c. Case-marked THEMATIC ROLES are assigned to 2OBJ. (Language Specific) d. Default Case Marking: the highest available GF is assigned NOM case, the next highest ACC. (Universal) Remember that universalprinciplesare interpretedas elsewhere conditions applying after any language-specificprinciplesor assignmentsof restricted GFs. Hence all idiosyncraticallymarked argumentswill have the statusof 2OBJs, and thus will not passivizeunder the assumptionthat German has only the unmarked passive rule according to which only (direct) OBJs passivize. Thus they will never be subjects in either passive or active sentences. This is exactly the result that we want for German. Together with the functionaluniquenesslaw, the associationprinciples for German make some further predictions: namely that in German (unlike Icelandic) we cannot get two idiosyncraticallycase-markedNPs after the verb. This follows from (87c) given functionaluniqueness:two different idiosyncraticallycase-marked thematic roles cannot both be assigned to the same GF, namely 2OBJ. Thus, under the assumptionthat both DAT and GEN case are assigned idiosyncraticallyby the verb in German, there should be no DAT-DAT, DAT-GEN or GEN-GEN verbs in that language.24Two accusative argumentsare allowed if one is 24 This same predictionis made within the GB frameworkby HartmutCzepluch (1982); his modificationsof the GB theory will not extend to Icelandic however, where many of these combinationsdo occur. There are no GEN-GEN verbs in Icelandiceither. We have not ruledout this possibility,and treat it as an accidentalgap in that language. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 480 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON an OBJ and the other is idiosyncraticallymarked,and hence a 2OBJ. In fact, DAT-GEN combinationsdo not exist in contemporaryGerman,so the prediction made by our analysis is correct in that respect. Our predictions are also correct with respect to the behavior of verbs with two accusatives. The few cases that still exist allow only for the passivization of one of the objects, as shown by the following examples.25 das Gedicht abgehort. (88)a. Ich habe ihn I have him(A) the poem(A) heard I had him recite the poem. b. Er ist das Gedicht abgehort. c. *Das Gedicht ist ihn abgehort. So we can account for the difference between Icelandic and Germanby means of one language-specific association convention for each language. This account also correctly predicts the non-occurrence of a certain number of combinations in German. (Some other gaps remain unexplained, however; for example, nothing in our account would prevent Icelandic from 'havingGEN GEN or ACC ACC combinations in postverbal position.26)We have shown that superficiallyvery similar In support of the claim that verbs assign at most one lexical (i.e., idiosyncratic)case, Holmberg (1985) suggests that while the case markingon themes is truly idiosyncraticin Icelandic, the apparent instances of two lexical cases actually reflect predictablecase marking associated with particularthematic roles: for first objects, dative case marks [+recipient]and accusativecase marks[-recipient]. Holmbergattributesthe lack of GEN first objects to the fact that genitive case is not associatedwith any particulartheta role. The hypothesisthat lexical items can assign at most one lexical case is clearly desirable from a theoreticalstandpoint,and Holmberg'scharacterizationcomes close to descriptive adequacy.Unfortunately,there are counterexampleswhich clearly requirethe assignment of lexical case to two arguments.Among the DAT GEN ditransitivesare at least three verbs whose first (DAT) objects are not recipients:synja 'to deny', varna 'to deny' and frfja 'to question'. 25 Of course, one must explain the fact that such forms existed in earlier stages of the language. Unfortunately,it is difficultto determinewhat their status was. They seem to have always been rather bookish (see Curme 1922, for discussion). Our guess is the following:in earlierstages, the dative case was not just an idiosyncraticcase markingon a 2OBJ, but could also encode semanticallyrestricted grammaticalfunctions (e.g., instrumentals).Hence there would have been no violation of the functionaluniquenesslaw. One might speculateabout why the evidence in the modernstages of both Icelandicand German points to an analysis in which bare NPs tend to be analyzed as occupying a semanticallyunrestrictedfunction. But it seems true that even in the Germaniclanguages that have maintained overt case, semanticallyrestricted grammaticalrelations are in generalrealizedas PPs. 26 As noted above in sections 3.2 and 4.4, it is debatablewhether any of the superficially ACC-ACC verbs are trulyditransitive. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 481 sentences in two rather closely related languages must be analyzed in quite different ways. In doing so, we have shown that a possibilitythat has been rejected on the basis of German data, namely that of oblique subjects and non-accusative direct objects, actually exists within Germanic. This leads to an interesting question in historical syntax: was English in its earlierstages more like Germanor more like Icelandic?We will not try to answer that question, but merely note that it cannot be assumedon the basis of universalprinciplesof language that English was like German,as has been done in some currentwork (see e.g., Lightfoot 1979, 1981).27 REFERENCES Allen, Cynthia: 1982, 'Dative Subjects in Old and Middle English', unpublishedmanuscript,AustralianNationalUniversity. Amritavelli,Raghavachari:1980, Aspectsof the Organizationof RedundancyRules in the Lexicon,unpublishedPh.D. dissertation,SimonFraserUniversity. Anderson,Stephen: 1977, 'Commentson the Paper by Wasow',in Culicover et al. (eds.), pp. ;361-377. Andrews, Avery: 1976, 'The VP ComplementAnalysis in Modem Icelandic', NELS 6, 1-21. 1982a, 'Long Distance Agreement in Modem Icelandic',in P. Jacobson& G. K. Pullum(eds.), The Natureof SyntacticRepresentation, Reidel, Dordrecht,pp. 1-31. : 1982b, 'The Representationof Case in Modem Icelandic',in J. Bresnan(ed.), The Mental Representation of GrammaticalRelations,MIT Press, Cambridge,pp. 427-503. Babby, Leonard:1980, 'The Syntaxof SurfaceCase Marking',CornellWorkingPapersin Linguistics,No. 1, pp. 1-32. Bern6dusson,Helgi: 1982, 'Opersonulegarsetningari islensku',('ImpersonalSentencesin Icelandic')cand. mag. thesis, Universityof Iceland. den Besten, Hans: 1981, 'A Case Filterfor Passives',in A. Belletti, L. Brandiand L. Rizzi (eds.), Theoryof Markednessin GenerativeGrammar,Scuola normaleSuperioredi Pisa, Foris, Dordrecht. Breckenridge,Janet: 1975, Rules WhichNothing Undergoes,unpublishedBachelor'sthesis, HarvardUniversity,Cambridge. Bresnan,Joan: 1982a, 'The Passive in Lexical Theory',in Bresnan(1982d), pp. 3-86. : 1982b, 'Polyadicity',in Bresnan(1982d), pp. 149-172. : 1982c, 'Controland Complementation',in Bresnan(1982d), pp. 282-390. (ed.): 1982d, The Mental Representationof GrammaticalRelations, MIT Press, Cambridge. and HoskuldurThrainsson:1984, 'A Note on IcelandicCoordination',unpublished manuscript,StanfordUniversityand Universityof California,Santa Cruz. Butler, M.: 1980, GrammaticallyMotivatedSubjectsin Early English, unpublishedPh.D. dissertation,Universityof Texas at Austin. 27 That such preverbaloblique NPs may have been grammaticalsubjects in earlierstages of Englishhas been.hintedat in Butler (1980), who gives statisticsshowingthat they do not behave like normalimpersonalconstructions.See also Allen (1982), who shows that this is true for some but not all such verbs, and also Harris(1975). This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 482 A. ZAENEN, J. MALING, AND H. THRAINSSON Chomsky,Noam: 1981, Lectureson Governmentand Binding, Foris, Dordrecht. Cole, P. and J. Sadock (eds.): 1977, GrammaticalRelations, Syntax and Semantics 8, AcademicPress,New York. , W. Harbert, G. Hermon, and S. N. Sridhar: 1978, 'On the Acquisition of Subjecthood',in Studiesin the LinguisticSciences8, 42-71. Culicover,Wasowand Akmajian(eds.), 1977: FormalSyntax,Academic Press,New York. Curme,G. O.: 1922, A Grammarof the GermanLanguage, MacMillan,New York. Czepluch,H.: 1982, 'Case Theory and the Dative Construction',The LinguisticReview 2, 1-38. Dowty, David: 1982, 'GrammaticalRelationsand MontagueGrammar',in P. Jacobsonand G. Pullum(eds.), The Natureof SyntacticRepresentation, Reidel, Dordrecht,pp. 79-130. Dryer,Matthew:1983, 'IndirectObjectsin KinyarwandaRevisited',in D. Perlmutter(ed.), Studiesin RelationalGrammar,Universityof Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 129-140. Einarsson,Steffin:1945, Icelandic,Grammar,Texts, Glossary,Johns Hopkins,Baltimore. Faarlund,Jan Terje: 1980, 'Subject and Nominative in Old Norse', ScriptaIslandica: Islandskasallskapetsarsbok31, 65-73. Freidin,Robert and LeonardBabby: 1982, 'On the Interactionof Lexical and Structural Properties',unpublishedpaper, Cornell University,Ithaca. :1983, 'On the Interactionof Lexical and SyntacticProperties:Case Structurein Russian',CornellWorkingPapersin Linguistics,No. 6. Fribjonsson,J6n: 1978, A Coursein ModernIcelandic,TimaritibSkak, Reykjavik. Gary, J. 0. and E. Keenan: 1977, 'On Collapsing GrammaticalRelations in Universal Grammar',in P. Cole and J. Sadock(eds.), GrammaticalRelations,Syntaxand Semantics 8, AcademicPress,New York, pp. 83-120. Gazdar,Geraldand Ivan Sag: 1981, 'Passivesand Reflexives in Phrase StructureGrammar', in J. A. G. Groenendijk,T. N. V. Janssen,and M. B. J. Stokhof (eds.), Formal Methodsin the Studyof Language, AmsterdamMathematicalCenter Tract No. 135. Grimshaw,Jane: 1982, 'RomanceReflexive Clitics', in Bresnan(ed.), pp. 87-148. Harris, Alice: 1975, 'On the Mechanism of Syntactic Rule Loss', unpublishedpaper, HarvardUniversity,Cambridge. Holmberg,Anders: 1985, 'Case and Word Order in Icelandic and Swedish',unpublished manuscript,Academyof Finland;paperpresentedat GLOW,April, 1985. Kaplan,Ron and Joan Bresnan:1982, 'A FormalSystemof GrammaticalRepresentation', in J. Bresnan (ed.), The Mental Representationof GrammaticalRelations, MIT Press, Cambridge,pp. 173-281. Kayne, Richard,S.: 1981, 'ECP Extensions',LinguisticInquiry12, 93-133. Keenan,E.: 1976, 'Towardsa UniversalDefinitionof "Subject"',in C. N. Li (ed.), Subject and Topic,Academic Press, New York, pp. 303-333. Levin, Lori: 1981, 'Lexical Representationsof Quirky Case in Icelandic', unpublished paper,MIT, Cambridge. 1985, 'Operationson Lexical Forms: Unaccusative Constructionsin Germanic Languages',Ph.D. dissertation,MIT, Cambridge. and Jane Simpson: 1981, 'Quirky Case and Lexical Representationof Icelandic Verbs', ChicagoLinguisticsSociety17, 185-196. Lieber,Rochelle: 1979, 'The English Passive:An Argumentfor HistoricalRule Stability', LinguisticInquiry10, 667-688. Lightfoot, David: 1979, Principlesof Diachronic Syntax, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. :1981, 'The Historyof Noun PhraseMovement',in Bakerand McCarthy(eds.), The LogicalProblemof Language Acquisition,MIT Press,Cambridge,pp. 86-119. Lockwood,W. B.: 1964, An Introductionto ModernFaroese,Munksgaard,Copenhagen. Maling,Joan: 1980, 'Inversionin EmbeddedClausesin ModernIcelandic',Islensktmdl og almennmdlfraebi 2, 175-193. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CASE AND GRAMMATICAL FUNCTIONS 483 1982, 'Clause-BoundedReflexivesin Modem Icelandic',paper read at the Trondheim Workshopon ScandinavianSyntax, June, 1982; to appear in L. Hellan and K. Koch-Christensen(eds.), Topicsin ScandinavianSyntax,Reidel, Dordrecht. - and Annie Zaenen: 1978, 'The Nonuniversalityof a Surface Filter', Linguistic Inquiry9, 475497. Marantz,Alec: 1981, On the Natureof GrammaticalRelations, Ph.D. dissertation,MIT; revised version publishedby MIT Press,Cambridge,1984. Marchand,H.: 1951, 1974, 'The Syntactic Change from Inflectional to Word Order Systemsand Some Effects of this Change on the Relation Verb/Object in English. A Diachronic-SynchronicInterpretation',in Studies in Syntax, Wilhelm Finks Verlag, Munchen,pp. 98-117. Ostler, N.: 1979, Case Linking:A Theoryof Case and VerbDiathesis, unpublishedPh.D. dissertation,MIT, Cambridge. Perlmutter,David: 1978, 'ImpersonalPassivesand the UnaccusativeHypothesis',BLS 4, Universityof California,Berkeley. (ed.): 1983, Studiesin RelationalGrammarI, Universityof Chicago Press,Chicago. and Paul Postal: 1983, 'Some Proposed Laws of Basic Clause Structure', in Perlmutter(ed.), pp. 81-128. Rappaport,Malka: 1983, 'On the Natureof Derived Nominals',in Levin, Rappaportand Zaenen (eds.), Papersin Lexical-FunctionalGrammar,IULC, pp. 113-142. Rognvaldsson,Eirikur:1982a,'Orbar6bogf;ersluri islensku',('WordOrderand Movement in Icelandic')cand. mag. thesis, Universityof Iceland. 1982b, 'We Need (Some Kind of a) Rule of ConjunctionReduction', Linguistic Inquiry13, 557-561. :1984, 'IcelandicWord Orderand Pab-Insertion',WorkingPapersin Scandinavian Syntax8, Universityof Trondheim. Thrafinsson,Hoskuldur: 1976, 'Some Arguments against the Interpretive Theory of Pronounsand Reflexives',HarvardStudiesin Syntaxand Semantics2, 573-624. in Icelandic,Garland,New York. : 1979, On Complementation : 1984, 'On Auxiliaries,Aux and VPs in Icelandic',to appearin Hellan and Koch Christensen(eds.), Topicsin ScandinavianSyntax,Reidel, Dordrecht. and the Lexicon', in Culicover, et al. (eds.), pp. Wasow,Thomas: 1977, 'Transformations 327-360. 1980, 'Major and Minor Rules in Lexical Grammar',in Hoekstra et al. (eds.), LexicalGrammar,Foris, Dordrecht,pp. 285-312. Zaenen,Annie: 1980, ExtractionRules in Icelandic,Ph.D. dissertation,HarvardUniversity, Cambridge;to be publishedby Garland,New York. : 1983, 'Double Objects in Kikuyu?',CornellWorkingPapersin LinguisticsNo. 5, pp. 199-206. and Joan Maling: 1984, 'Unaccusative,Passive and QuirkyCase', in Cobler et al. (eds.), Proceedingsof 3rd West Coast Conferenceon FormalLinguistics,StanfordUniversity,pp. 317-329. Received 5 December 1984 Revised 7 June 1985 Maling Dept. of Psychology BrandeisUniversity Waltham,MA 02254 U.S.A. This content downloaded from 137.99.78.63 on Mon, 15 Sep 2014 12:55:50 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions