IE W EV PR IE W EV PR IE W EV PR Predicting Elementary English Learners’ Inferences From A Background Knowledge Threshold IE W A Thesis Presented to The Department of TESOL EV Emporia State University PR In Partial Fulfillment Of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts by Annette Putnam 2021 _________________________ Jim Persinger, Ph.D. PR EV IE W Approved by the Department Chair Cate Grundleger, Ph.D. Committee Chair Teddy Roop, Ph.D. Chair Member _________________________________________ Jerald W.Spotswood, Ph.D. Dean of the Graduate School and Distance Education ii Acknowledgements Since I am notoriously not a touchy-feely person, I shall keep this brief. I sincerely thank everyone who has played a role in my academic journey. This thesis would not have been possible without God, my parents, Dr. Cate Grundleger, Dr. Jim Persinger, Dr. Teddy Roop, Dr. Rajarshi Dey, Dr. Elizabeth Moss, and Dr. Gerry Coffman. PR EV IE W Thank you. iii TABLE OF CONTENTS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS……………………………………………………………...………………….iii TABLE OF CONTENTS…………………………………………………………………..………………iv CHAPTER I - INTRODUCTION………………………………………………………….…….1 Statement of the Problem………...………………………………………………………..2 Purpose of the Study………...…………………………………………………………….3 Research Questions and Hypotheses……………...………………………………………4 IE W Significance of the Study………………………………………………………………….5 CHAPTER II - LITERATURE REVIEW…………………………………………………..…7 Influential Theory…………………………………………...………………………….....7 L1 Reading Comprehension………………...………………………………….....7 EV Resonance Model………………………………………………………………...10 Predictive Modeling, Latent Semantic Analysis, and Knowledge Thresholds…..11 PR Reading Reconsidered: The Case for Knowledge…………………………………….....13 Qualities of Background Knowledge………………………………….................15 Background Knowledge as a Prerequisite to Inferences……………………...….16 Background Knowledge, Inferences, and Informational Text…………………...18 Group Differences………………………………………………………………………..20 L2 Reading Comprehension…...………………………………………………...20 English Learners, Background Knowledge, and Inference…………...…………22 iv CHAPTER II - METHOD……………………………………………………..……………....25 Research Design and Rationale…………………………………...…..………………....25 Participants and Setting…………………………………………………………..……...27 Instrumentation………………………………………………………………....………...……...27 Procedure…………………………………………………………………….…………..30 Data Analysis…………………………………………………………………………….31 Ethics……………………………………………………………………………………..32 CHAPTER III - RESULTS………………………………………………………………….....33 IE W CHAPTER IV - DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION…………………………………….....39 A Background Knowledge Threshold……………….…………………………..………39 Predicting English Learner Success……………………………………………………...40 EV Implications for Instruction……………………………………………...……………….40 Limitations of the Study………………………...……………….………….……………42 Suggestions for Future Research……………..………………………………………….43 PR References………………………………………………………………………………………..44 Appendix A…………………………………………………………………...……………….…60 Appendix B……………………………………………………………………………………....61 Appendix C………………………………………………………………………………………63 v 1 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION “If the topic...is outside [students’] experience or base of knowledge, they are adrift to an unknown sea.” (Aebersold & Field, 1997, p. 41) There is mounting evidence that K-12 English learners (ELs) are performing lower than their monolingual peers in reading on both high-stakes and classroom assessments (Koo et al., 2014). The National Assessment of Education Progress (2017) reports multilingual students read IE W 37 points below their non-EL peers. Considering there are almost 5 million ELs in the United States (Sanchez, 2017), this is an unacceptable gap. The current reality expects students to understand grade-level language, grade-level content, and meet rigorous Common Core State ELs (Wilson et al., 2016). EV Standards/English Language Arts academic standards, thus creating a challenge for teachers and The first nonfiction CCSS/ELA standard, 4.RI.1, is fundamental because it requires PR readers to independently make inferences (National Governors Association (NGA) Center for Best Practices & Council of Chief State School Officers (CCSSO, 2010). Years of overwhelming evidence suggest that strong inferences lead to comprehension (Oakhill & Cain, 2018; Shapiro, 2004), but this is a challenging skill for children (Omanson et al., 1978). Although a variety of factors influence inference-making, Yuill and Oakhill (1991) proposed a general knowledge deficit as one plausible explanation for variation in children's inference-making, and subsequent studies point in the same direction: proficient readers leverage sufficient prior knowledge to make accurate inferences regarding background information not outright stated by the author (Fisher et al., 2009; Graesser & Bertus, 1998). Unfortunately, this reality has not caught the 2 attention of reading practitioners and researchers in a way that is useful for teaching and learning. Statement of the Problem A knowledge deficit is present in our country. Background knowledge’s presence in instruction, curriculum, professional development, and scholarship does not reflect its significant role in making inferences during reading. Students in today’s classrooms are asked to conjure prior knowledge while being withheld from systematic or planned instruction of background IE W knowledge. Before-reading background knowledge routines are typically spontaneous with a sampling of students sharing personal experience or incomplete information about the topic (Karpov, 2018), and rarely are individual marks collected on prior knowledge. Whole-group EV reading then swiftly moves into reading the selected text and students must immediately begin inferring complex concepts and ideas. Background knowledge is needed for inferences but is assumed to be acquired outside the school’s parameters and there is a push for reading skills and PR strategies instead (Neuman et al., 2014). However, it is “mistaken dogma”, leader-in-the-field Hirsch asserts (2006), to leave the acquisition of background knowledge to the rare field trip or hands-on activity. Without adequate background knowledge, teaching reading skills and strategies, like summarizing or visualizing, have limited effectiveness for the reader (Hirsch, 2006). A lack of knowledge building and measurement has resulted in a dependency on skill and strategy to increase reading scores while ignoring the return-on-investment of knowledge input (Cervetti & Hiebert, 2015; Wexler, 2019). Further, there remains a lack of quantitative evidence as to what renders a student’s background knowledge deficient or sufficient. Who or what 3 establishes such a consequential threshold? The generalizability of published research on background knowledge and inferences is problematic because it captures the experiences of monolingual learners and excludes the diverse public school classrooms that include ELs. Over time, however, the achievement gap widens and these traditionally underserved students are inadvertently sent to the sidelines in schools, work, and society (August et al., 2009; Hirsch, 2006). The logical question, then, is if the knowledge deficiency is more significant among students whose first language (L1) is not English? To help IE W ELs gain the most out of reading instruction, researchers and educators must understand how much background knowledge is needed for inference. Given the sine qua non of proficient reading, detailed empirical accounts of young ELs EV are vital to student learning and achievement. O’Reilly et. al. (2019) looked at how background knowledge and inference-making work in concert, but not specifically in elementary-aged students or English learners. To help elementary ELs gain the most out of reading instruction, PR researchers and educators need cognizance of how much background knowledge is needed for inference, especially since ELs have different background knowledge that should not be confused with a deficit of background knowledge (Vogt et al., 2010). If the aim is to provide a firm direction for K-12 educational settings, reading studies need to be updated and include ELs, as this thesis is designed to do. To remedy this, I will run a piecewise linear regression to reveal a knowledge threshold for expository text to shed light on the cruciality of measuring individual student levels of topic knowledge. 4 Purpose of the Study Despite the importance of literacy skills and strategies, ELs depend on background knowledge. The lack of background knowledge negatively impacts an EL’s willingness to communicate, comprehend, and grasp new learning (Shen & Byfield, 2018). A possible cause is that teachers do not assess or press enough on eliciting and then measuring how much knowledge students come to a topic with (Ambrose et al., 2010). This author’s experience in the classroom has been that activating background knowledge is a quick discussion rather than a preassessment that adjusts instruction for optimal learning. Nor has this author seen the systematic IE W building of background knowledge that is needed to deepen student understanding of new topics that will support their comprehension (Fugnitto, n.d.) In empirical research, prior knowledge is largely controlled, so it is neither an option to turn to the literature for guidance on how prior EV knowledge interacts or influences comprehension (Witherby & Carpenter, 2021). ELs require two knowledge sources for successful interpretation of a text: linguistic and background knowledge (Grabe & Stoller, 2019). The proposed quantitative non-experimental PR study using archival data is set up to investigate the unanswered question of whether there is a numerical value, a percentage point, at which the facilitating effect of background knowledge secures a good inference score. To control for cultural influence as much as possible, the purposeful decision was made to use the common experience of sleep. Specific sleep terms that most likely predict high inference scores will also be evaluated. Research Questions and Hypotheses Building on the studies above and the reality of a growing English Language Learner achievement gap, the purpose of this investigation is to predict inference correctness by determining whether or not a background knowledge threshold exists in children ages 9-10. 5 Thus, the specific research questions and null hypotheses addressed in this study are: Research Question 1: Can a background knowledge threshold be identified below which correct inferences will be limited and not predicted by background knowledge but above which there is a correlation? Null hypothesis 1: There will not be a background knowledge threshold that affects accurate inferencing. Research Question 2: Using a latent semantic analysis hybrid, can a background IE W knowledge score of topic-specific words determine which ELs will have difficulty with making correct inferences about an informational text? Null hypothesis 2: Background knowledge activation via topic-specific words cannot Significance of the Study EV predict whether a student will be able to make correct inferences about a specific topic. Background knowledge is too often discussed as a have-or-have-not entity, but this PR undermines the depth of academic and topic-specific background knowledge (Ambrose et al., 2010). Previous studies have demonstrated the qualitative advantages of having ample academic background knowledge, but there remains limited evidence pointing to how much background knowledge is needed to ensure successful inferencing. By quantifiably finding a threshold, at which a lack of topic background knowledge will severely limit the inference ability of readers, this study could be of importance to the following: Mainstream and English as a Second Language (ESL) teachers: When background knowledge is externally and individually elicited, teachers are given a glimpse into the depth, breadth, and organization of the knowledge that will be used for higher-order reading processes, such as inferencing (Kim & Clariana, 2015). There may be a strong possibility of practical use for teachers across many content-areas to identify ELs who may struggle to generate correct inferences based on insufficient background knowledge. The outcomes of this study will help them make decisions about pre-reading strategies and interventions for science topics. Additionally, data given will help ESL specialists better know how to evaluate students for conceptual understandings and vocabulary knowledge. TESOL research: The researcher intends that this small study will improve the knowledge base of teacher educators in best practices for assessing and teaching background PR EV IE W knowledge. A key takeaway will be fostering the next generation of teachers of ELs. 6 CHAPTER II 7 LITERATURE REVIEW Reading studies have looked at preconditions affecting inference outcomes, from reading purpose (Nahatame, 2014) to vocabulary knowledge (Beck et al., 1982; Neuman et al., 2014) to decoding skills (Oakhill & Cain, 2018). There is also a close connection between a reader’s activated background knowledge, inference, and comprehension (Anderson & Pearson, 1984; Hirsch, 2006; Neuman et al., 2014), an assertion which is supported by the classic ConstructionIntegration Model (Kintsch, 1988) and the resonance model (O’Brien & Cook, 2016). However, IE W few studies have looked at the quantity of background knowledge needed for inferencing during reading. This review synthesizes relevant background knowledge and inference studies to provide the context as to what led to this study’s research questions. An overview of theoretical EV reading perspectives is considered first followed by an examination of the characteristics of background knowledge and its effect on inferencing in informational texts. The chapter proceeds PR to highlight intra-reader variation, including difficulties of the EL. Influential Theory L1 reading comprehension One primary goal of reading research is to understand how readers represent a text after they process it. In education, this representation is commonly referred to as reading comprehension and, according to the RAND Reading Study Group (RRSG) and Snow (2002), consists of three interrelated parts: the reading purpose, the text, and the reader. Interactive reading models are based on Bartlett’s schema model (1932) which puts forth a theoretical structure of how knowledge is processed, modified, and retrieved from a reader’s long-term memory (Al-Issa, 2006; Duke et al., 2011; Rumelhart, 1980). Schema is not organized linearly, like printed text, but in a combined network of knowledge. A founding principle of schema 8 theory is that of bottom-up and top-down (knowledge-driven) processes working interactively as opposed to serially. Conceptual-level processing does not lie in the text, as the text itself does not carry meaning but instead guides the reader as to what to retrieve from schemata (Carrell & Eisterhold, 1983); essentially, more information is supplied by the reader than the text (Clark & Silberstein, 1977). Accordingly, comprehension is achieved when a schema that “provides a coherent explanation of the relations between the objects and events mentioned in a discourse” is constructed. (Espinosa, 1996, p. 240) IE W One view of successful comprehension at the discourse level is the well-developed construction-integration (CI) model (Carlson, van den Broek, McMaster, Rapp, Bohn-Gettler, Kendeou, White, 2014). As can be seen in Figure 1, the comprehension process is born from two EV levels of representation: integrating concepts across sentences, the textbase, and the subsequent act of supplementing that textbase with background knowledge and inferences, the situation PR model (Cain & Oakhill, 2018; Duke, 2011; Wolfe & Woodwyk, 2010). Reproduced with permission of copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.