A STUDY OF THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN INSTRUMENTAL MUSIC EDUCATION AND CRITICAL THINKING IN 8TH- AND 11TH-GRADE STUDENTS by Ryan M. Zellner NANCY LONGO, PhD, Faculty Mentor and Chair WILLIAM CAMERON, PhD, Committee Member CATHERINE MCCARTNEY, PhD, Committee Member David Chapman, PsyD, Dean Harold Abel School of Social and Behavioral Sciences A Dissertation Presented in Partial Fulfillment Of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy Capella University April 2011 © Ryan M. Zellner, 2011 Abstract The purpose of this study was to explore the possible relationship between instrumental music education in Grades 8 and 11 and critical thinking as assessed by the Pennsylvania System of School Assessment. The subsets that were examined included Reading (B): Interpretation and Analysis of Fictional and Nonfictional Text, which assesses the academic standards 1.1, Learning to read independently, standard 1.2 Reading critically in all content areas, standard 1.3 Reading, analyzing and interpreting literature, and Mathematics, sections C.1 Geometry—Analyze characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes, D.2 Algebraic concepts—Analyze mathematical situations using numbers, symbols, words, tables and/or graphs, and E.1 Data analysis and probability— Interpret and analyze data by formulating answers or questions (Pennsylvania Department of Education, 2009–2010). The sample consisted of Instrumental students (N = 50) and Noninstrumental music students (N = 50) over 2 graduated high school classes. The results indicated that the Instrumental music sample consistently outscored the Noninstrumental music sample when comparing the Reading B, Mathematics M.C.1, M.D.2, and M.E.1 subsections of the Pennsylvania System of School Assessment with significant increases noted from 8th to 11th grade. Dedication All the work and desire that I have today, I owe to my father, Kenneth Zellner, who passed away during the doctoral process. For someone who never stepped foot inside of a college classroom, you are much wiser than I will ever be and I can never thank you enough for all the values you instilled in me. iii Acknowledgments I would like to thank all the support of my family and friends, especially my wife, Sandra; without your support and consistent encouragement I could not have finished my doctoral journey. In addition, a special thank you to my mentor, Dr. Nancy Longo, who has supported me through this whole process form start to finish and to the members of my committee, Dr. William Cameron and Dr. Catherine McCartney—without your assistance and professionalism this dissertation would not have been possible. iv Table of Contents Acknowledgments iv List of Tables vii List of Figures ix CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION 1 Introduction to the Problem 1 Background of the Study 5 Statement of the Problem 8 Purpose of the Study 8 Research Questions 9 Significance of the Study 10 Definition of Terms 15 Assumptions and Limitations 18 Expected Findings 20 CHAPTER 2. LITERATURE REVIEW 22 Music and Academic Achievement 23 Music and Personality 28 Critical Thinking 32 Music Education and Intelligence 38 CHAPTER 3. METHODOLOGY 43 Purpose of the Study 43 Research Design 44 Target Population and Participant Selection 48 v Procedures 50 Ethical Considerations 51 Instruments 52 Research Questions and Hypotheses 56 Data Analysis 57 Expected Findings 59 CHAPTER 4. DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS 61 Introduction 61 Description of the Stratified Sample 61 Conclusion 94 CHAPTER 5. RESULTS, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS 97 Introduction 97 Summary of the Results 98 Discussion of the Results 104 Limitations 105 Recommendations for Further Research 107 Conclusion 108 REFERENCES 110 vi List of Tables Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations (8th Grade) 65 Table 2. Test of Homogeneity of Variances (8th Grade) 67 Table 3. Between-Subjects ANOVA (8th Grade) 68 Table 4. Descriptives for 11th Grade 70 Table 5. Test of Homogeneity of Variances (11th Grade) 71 Table 6. Robust Tests of Equality of Means (11th Grade) 72 Table 7. Between-Groups ANOVA (11th Grade) 74 Table 8. Group Means and Standard Deviations (8th Grade) 76 Table 9. Independent-Samples t Test for Means of Samples (8th Grade) 76 Table 10. Means and Standard Deviations (11th Grade) 77 Table 11. Independent-Samples t Test for Means of Samples (11th Grade) 78 Table 12. Means and Standard Deviations for Combined Samples 80 Table 13. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Tests of Within-Subjects Effects 81 Table 14. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Tests of Within-Subjects Contrasts 82 Table 15. Levene’s Test of Equality of Error Variances (Combined) 82 Table 16. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Between Subjects (Group Cumulative Means) 83 Table 17. Skewness and Kurtosis of 8th and 11th Grades 84 Table 18. Means and Standard Deviations (Reading) 85 Table 19. Levene’s Test of Equality of Error Variances (Reading) 85 Table 20. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Tests of Between-Subjects Reading 86 Table 21. Means, Percentage Correct, and Maximum Score (Reading) 86 vii Table 22. Means and Standard Deviations (M.C.1) 87 Table 23. Levene’s Test of Equality of Error Variances (M.C.1) 88 Table 24. Means and Percentage Correct (M.C.1) 88 Table 25. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Test of Between-Subjects M.C.1 89 Table 26. Means and Standard Deviations (M.D.2) 89 Table 27. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Tests of Within-Subjects Contrasts (M.D.2) 90 Table 28. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Tests of Between-Subjects M.D.2 90 Table 29. Means, Percentage Correct, and Maximum Score (M.D.2) 91 Table 30. Means and Standard Deviations (M.E.1) 92 Table 31. Levene’s Test of Equality of Error Variances (M.E.1) 93 Table 32. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Tests of Between-Subjects M.E.1 93 viii List of Figures Figure 1. Mean raw scores of both samples (5th, 8th, and 11th) 63 Figure 2. Percentage of correct reading answers (5th, 8th, and 11th) 64 Figure 3. Percentage of correct math answers (8th and 11th) 64 Figure 4. Percentage of correct answers for both samples (5th, 8th, and 11th) 73 Figure 5. Means of both samples 8th to 11th grade 80 ix CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION Introduction to the Problem Traditional music education trains students to perform on their instrument by recognizing both rhythmic and tonal patterns within a structured musical experience. The making of music seeks to go beyond notes on a page. It seeks to energize and create musical compositions that make aural sense to the untrained listener. The instructional design of music education is to teach the student both melodic and rhythmic patterns through the process of active learning. This focuses the responsibility of learning on the learner, thereby allowing the learner to engage in the processing of information in an interactive environment where the teacher uses activities that promote student engagement through problem-based, collaborative and cooperative learning (Prince, 2004). An example of active learning includes interactive lectures, which incorporate activities that encourage discourse between students using demonstrations, visual aids or peer interactions. Prince cited a study by Ruhl, Hughes, and Schloss that examined a traditional classroom lecture of 45 minutes versus a lecture that allowed the students three pauses of 2 minutes each to check their notes with a peer. The results indicated increased short and long term retention in the group that allowed for the breaks. Other examples of active learning include the use of analogies, contemplation, student groups, 1 class discussions and verbal studying. These activities must promote thoughtful engagement and promote learning outcomes (Prince, 2004). Interactive engagement activities have been shown to increase conceptual understanding. Hake (1998) examined 6,000 undergraduate students in introductory physics courses that utilized substantial interactive engagement methods. Those students who studied under this method outscored their peers in conceptual understanding scores by nearly two to one. The origins of active learning can be traced back to hands-on learning theories, which are derived from master–apprenticeship models that are based on knowledge through experience. The theory of active learning is at the very core of music education and in becoming a musician. The student is an active and functional learner who is contributing directly to the output of a product, much like that of an apprentice. The first step of the musical process is to be able to master the instrument. This mastery process was famously established in the 1920s by Suzuki, resulting in his own pedagogical method called Talent Education. Suzuki was the son of the first and most prolific Japanese violin maker. Suzuki credits this early exposure to music as well as his father’s willingness to learn from others and strong moral fortitude for shaping himself as a person and educator (Cooney, Cross, & Trunk, 1993). The philosophy of Shinichi Suzuki’s educational method is that all students are capable of learning music just as they are capable of learning and mastering a language. Suzuki was mesmerized by the language acquisition of children and the capabilities that are displayed in language versatility by the ages of 5 and 6. All learners must be nurtured through the learning process, and it is the process that is at fault when there is a slow learner not the individual (Cooney et al., 1993). Suzuki was convinced, however, that the 2 path to any learning begins with the creation of character, and this character is the precursor to ability. The motivation of the learner in this method is paramount, and when the motivation is no longer present, the activity ends. Suzuki insisted that students learn his Talent Education method by imitating an exact model while learning each step perfectly before advancing to the next. By the end of the system, the student has synthesized each lesson so that the playing of the music had become a habit. In the Suzuki method, the child masters each new composition acquiring new skills with each piece preparing the student for the ones to follow (American Suzuki Center, n.d.). Even within this regimented approach to education, Suzuki believed that education should be for everyone, appeal to the child’s interest and should in turn work to develop a total person. The Suzuki model insists on the mastery of each skill before a new skill is added. While this quest for perfection would seem to create monotony in adults, children work in the environment that they are given. By their inherent nature of curiosity and their inquisition for understanding they work toward perfection through repetition without hesitation. Many times in contemporary education, goals are sought rather than the development of the process of acquiring information. End results are measured as tests and other assessments; however, as Suzuki stated, these are more a measurement of the teacher than of the student (Cooney et al., 1993). The clear application for modern education is the perfection of process rather than the perfection of the student. If students have committed themselves to the process of learning and develop along this path, their success will breed success. 3 Suzuki’s Talent Education spoke much to the development and mastery of skills and this learning method utilizes the teacher-centered method to focus and hone the students’ transfer of knowledge. While the students are actively engaged in the learning process is does little to address student’s prior knowledge and incorporate discovery learning. Teachout (2007) recognized this disparity in music education. He stated, “Music education should be about developing such musical knowledge, skills and dispositions to demystify music and afford all students opportunities for higher levels of independent engagement with music” (p. 21). Traditional music education, which is based mainly on large ensemble settings, utilizes the teacher-centered model, in which the instructor is responsible for providing feedback. The instructor guides rehearsals with the goal of these rehearsals being an excellent musical performance. The performance is then a reflection of the instructor’s skill and expertise both in the field of music as well as leadership (Teachout, 2007). However, this philosophy does little to impart the student with problem-solving or critical thinking skills. Teachout stated that music education should not only be about what students can do, but what they know and how they value music. To accomplish this task, a sequenced and spiraled curriculum model must be established in order for the active learning model to be complete. One of these skill sets is the ability to develop critical thinking skills. The construct is that as students have exposure to musical training their ability to think critically increases. The ability to think critically in music allows for the performer not only to interact within a musical composition but also to anticipate and solve problems in action. The performance of music is a delicate balancing act between the entire melodic, harmonic and rhythmic acts that occur at the same point in time, thus allowing for a 4 seamless composition to occur. Without active critical thinking on the part of the performer, the music itself would not be allowed to be a serendipitous exchange between the musicians. Background of the Study “Critical thinking is the use of those cognitive skills or strategies that increase the probability of a desirable outcome. It is used to describe thinking that is purposeful, reasoned, and goal directed” (Halpern, 1997, p. 4). In 1998, Halpern proposed a four component model for the transfer of critical thinking skills. The first component is attitude, which she describes as the recognition of critical thinking as a skill and then the implementation of such by the individual. Next is that the instruction and practice of critical thinking as a skill in that a person will recognize the benefit of the utilization of critical thinking skills. The third component is the ability to transfer the elements of problem and the application of them to a new contextual situation. The last component is the use of metacognition to enhance and facilitate the process of critical thinking. In order to apply Halpern’s model, the delineation between music listener and performer must be recognized with the latter of which being the point of reference for this research. Being that music is an active endeavor that involves a multisensory experience, the application of critical thinking skills is both a complex task and developmental process. The first component of Halpern’s model is the recognition and implementation of critical thinking as a skill. Within a musical context, this invites the teacher to act as a guide in order for proper interpretation, analyzation and execution in the practice and performance application of the musical experience. Lisk (2006) 5 described music as intelligence in action and that performing in an instrumental ensemble requires a multisensory approach combined with a perceptive decision-making process. Making music is then a problem-solving, in-the-moment exercise. Students, however, must be taught not only how to apply critical thinking skills but to recognize where they become a necessary and vital component of music. Between the ages of 5–12, when the children are developing this musical intelligence repertoire, the children are functioning within the concrete operational period, which would allow for optimal operant conditioning. The concrete operational period is where the child is gaining the ability to experience and understand multiple perspectives coupled with a structured, guided and appropriate stimuli and response behavior. Therefore, students can be taught to think critically within a musical environment. Moog (1984) described music as a multisensory learning experience that involves the temporal phenomena of acoustic, motoric, and other. The last classification of other refers to the outside conditions that may be experienced during a musical experience such as color, temperature and pain. This creates a crossroads in that music becomes both an individual and ensemble task each requiring in its own specific skill set. The ensemble setting allows for the interaction of students with others either developing or having established musical abilities. The latter would allow for the application of Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development theory, which occurs when people are learning from their interaction with someone that has a more advanced ability than theirs (Sternberg, 2003). The second step in the Halpern structural model states that the instruction and practice of critical thinking skills are necessary for their transfer. Lisk (1996) stated that 6 the musician must have an active involvement in the musical decisions of the ensemble, therefore allowing the individual to create meaning in the musical performance. This active involvement needs to include both the intrapersonal and interpersonal development of the musician in order to facilitate a consonance within the ensemble. In this scenario, the music teacher or director becomes the vehicle behind the transfer of critical thinking skills, through his use of guided practice on a routine and structured basis. This directly leads to the third component of transferring this structured facilitation to new contexts. In the setting of a music ensemble rehearsal, a new piece of music allows for a reexamination of skill sets that previously have been previously acquired and the transfer of said skill sets to the new context. Music because of its inherent nature of patterns and structure allows for a combination of both applying known skills as well as the learning of new ones. The last component introduced by Halpern is metacognition, which is the reflection upon one’s thinking process. This process, which is sometimes referred to as thinking, about thinking is the evaluative stage in the critical thinking process. This evaluation provides for an examination of one’s actions in retrospect and, therefore, the possibility of providing alternatives or response to similar stimuli in future situations or behaviors. These cognitive connections between music and other skills sets have been recognized as a contributory factor to an individual as a whole. Because of the multisensory nature of music, the development of musical skills has been shown to have an impact on a student both academically and personally. 7 Statement of the Problem The research problem is to what extent does instrumental music education have an impact critical thinking skills. The purpose of this study is to demonstrate the impact of instrumental music education on critical thinking skills of school-age students. Purpose of the Study The purpose of this research was to explore the relationship between critical thinking and instrumental music education. “If creative thinking is just everyday problem solving, then there should be general principles that can be applied across domains of knowledge” (Halpern, 1996, p. 372). Problem solving can be described as the difference between what has happened and what one wanted to happen. The solving aspect occurs when one takes corrective action in order to meet objectives. The Global Development Research Center (2008) outlined the following sequential steps for problem solving: • Problem definition • Problem analysis • Generating possible solutions • Analyzing the solutions • Selecting the best solution(s) • Planning the next course of action (next steps) This research was done through analyzing testing of both instrumental music participants and nonparticipants through the course of their school years (8th and 11th grades). This study was able to examine the differences between students and the amount of music exposure they have had based on the number of years. According to Breakwell, 8 Hammond, Fife-Schaw, and Smith (2006), this would constitute a longitudinal design, whereas data are being collected from the same sample over a period of time with all data being collected retrospectively. Research Questions The main research question examined the relationship between instrumental music education instruction in Grades 8 and 11 and critical thinking skills on the Pennsylvania System of School Assessment (PSSA; sections RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1). PSSA Operational Definitions • Reading B: Interpretation and analysis of text • Mathematics C.1 (geometry): Analyze characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes • Mathematics D.2 (algebraic concepts): Analyze mathematical situations using numbers, symbols, words, tables and/or graphs • Mathematics E.1 (data analysis and probability): Interpret and analyze data by formulating answers or questions These selected areas not only provide the appropriate prerequisite of critical thinking skills and demonstrate problem-solving ability but also remain consistent data throughout the grade levels of the assessment (8th and 11th). Research Question 1 How does the number of years (8th and 11th) that a student is involved in music education provide any statistical difference in the development of critical thinking skills as assessed by the PSSA (cumulative score per grade level)? 9 Research Question 2 How do the Instrumental music students’ mean scores on sections (RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1) of the PSSA assessment compare to Noninstrumental music students from 8th to 11th grade. Research Question 3 Utilizing the means of the individual PSSA scoring (sections RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1), what is the relationship between the following: • Instrumental group scores versus Noninstrumental group scores from 8th–11th grades Significance of the Study The significance of this topic relates to all of music education. In the times of cutbacks and the so-called return to the basics, music education for its own sake has placed music on the endangered list. Bridging this gap in literature would allow music educators to demonstrate and show the importance of music education to all students. This study examined the development of critical thinking skills over a period of time. The students’ scores will be available from their 8th- and 11th-grade years. This means that the research can quantitatively demonstrate the effect of music education of a period of time. Psychologically, it allowed for a greater understanding of how thought, adaptation and developmental processes are acquired by an individual. It allows for the congruent developmental processes and the ways in which they can be affected by outside influences. 10 In addition, this study engaged the active learning process and its own effectiveness. Active learning focuses the responsibility of learning on the learner by allowing the learner to engage in the processing of information in an interactive environment where the teacher uses activities that promote student engagement through problem-based, collaborative and cooperative learning (Prince, 2004). McManus (2001) stated that much of the theory of active learning is the difference between a teachercentered paradigm and a learner-centered paradigm. The teacher-centered paradigm is much the traditional approach of higher education. The teacher passes information by lecture to the student, who acts as an empty vessel. The learner-centered paradigm focuses on the absorption and application of material by the students. The differences between these two philosophies of learning are as basic as those of learning themselves. The teacher-centered paradigm is structured on the premise that the teacher has mastered the material and is going to pass this knowledge along to the student. Conversely, the student-centered paradigm recognizes that the student has already accumulated knowledge and that by processing the information dynamically, new information and knowledge structures will be informed. Perhaps more importantly, these paradigms point to significant differences between the relationships of instructors and students. In the teacher-centered paradigm, the instructor is the center of learning. However, there is limited or no interaction between student and teacher. In the learner-centered paradigm, the relationship and interaction between teacher and student is the cornerstone of transferring the material from information to knowledge. The teacher-centered paradigm and learner-centered paradigm can also be referred to as the difference between passive 11 and active learning, since passive learning is mostly accomplished through verbal lectures where the student is simply a recipient of the information (McManus, 2001). The influence of instrumental music education on the development of the cognitive process allows for greater understanding of the ability of instrumental music to influence learning. Because of the availability of the students’ scores from their 8th- and 11th-grade years, the research can quantitatively demonstrate the effect of music education of a period of time. Critical thinkers are evaluating new situations, looking for complexity and ambiguity making connections, speculating, searching for evidence, looking for connections between particular situation and prior knowledge and experience (Halpern, 1996). In addition, this research allows for a greater understanding of how thought, adaptation and developmental processes are acquired by an individual. It also allows for the congruent developmental processes and how they can be affected by outside influences such as music. Schellenberg (2006) examined 6- to 11-year-old children who each varied in amount of musical training. The baseline IQ was established by administering the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC–III) as well as other areas of intellectual functioning such as grades in school and standardized tests of academic achievement. The sample was comprised of 72 boys and 75 girls (N = 147) ages 6–11 recruited from a middle class suburb of Toronto, Canada. The predictor variables were measured using a questionnaire that was administered to the parents about their child’s history with private music lessons. The criterion variables consisted of measures of intelligence, which were assessed using the WISC–III, academic ability, which was assessed using the Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement (K–TEA), and 12 social adjustment, which was measured using the Parent Rating Scale of Behavioral Assessment System for Children (BASC). The principal analyses, consisting of correlations between the main predictor variable and criterion variables demonstrated that music lessons were positively correlated with both academic achievement and IQ but not social adjustment. The outcome was that the duration of music lessons has a small but positive correlation to measures of intelligence. The second study examined the effects of long term music lessons on intellectual abilities and more specifically if these had lasting effects even after the music lessons had ended. The participants of the second study were undergraduates at a suburban Canadian university with the range in age being between 16–25 with more than half taking private music lessons N = 84 for an average of 7.8 years. The students were surveyed based on a questionnaire where the students were paid to participate in the 2-hour survey. The criterion variables in the second study consisted of intelligence and academic achievement, which was measured using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS– III), and an additional subtest was Object Assemble, which was administered to measure spatial–temporal ability (Schellenberg, 2006). The results indicated that taking music lessons regularly was correlated positively with IQ especially in the areas of perceptual organization and working memory. The results of the Schellenberg (2006) study indicated that there was a positive correlation between childhood lessons and IQ, and that the correlation would have an impact into early adulthood. Perhaps the more important conclusion in this study is that there is a direct causal effect between the duration of musical training and the predictor of 13 better intellectual functioning. The benefit of this study to the efforts of music education was enhanced because the effects of music lessons on intelligence could not be discounted by parents’ education or family income. As a result of the previous findings, intelligence is an adaptable and modifiable skill that can be enhanced over a period of time. This, in turn, directly relates to the ability of instrumental music education to create cognitive links through the conceptual development of child and adolescent mind. As presented in the Schellenberg (2006) study, the single factor of instrumental lessons could account for the substantial proportion of the variance of the tests. Therefore, the summation could be made that music lessons were the determining predictor valuable that altered the test scores. Critical thinking, just like that of intelligence, personality and academic achievement is a skill that can be either taught or enhanced over a period of time. Therefore, instrumental music education can have a similar longitudinal impact to a student’s critical thinking skills. Halpern (1997) stated that critical thinking skills can be taught effectively through the transfer of training where thinking skills can be applied to a vast array of contexts. When people think critically, they are evaluating the outcomes of their thinking processes, which could involve solving problems, making inferences and evaluating decisions. In music education, critical thinking skills are applied where the paradigm shifts from teacher-centered to student-centered learning. The musicians use these skills to evaluate their performances in the context of technique, intonation, stylistic interpretation, notation and ensemble (Reimer, 2002). 14 Definition of Terms Critical thinking/problem solving. Problem-solving assessments are administered as part of the PSSA with the testing occurring in 3rd, 5th, 8th, and 11th grades. The PSSA uses open-ended questions to assess student’s problem-solving ability. This assessment uses open-ended questions in both the reading and mathematics portion of the assessment. The PSSA provides a separate score for each individual to their respective schools. Instrumental music education. The student will have participated in the following grade levels and therefore demonstrate formalized experience in music education (instrumental music lessons and band). Fifth grade will have 1 year of instrumental music experience, 8th grade will have 4 years, and the 11th graders will have 7 years of experience in music education. Problem solving and critical thinking. According to Skinner (2005), the determinant for how one thinks critically must be based and trained in operant conditioning. Therefore, the abilities required to be a critical thinker are trainable through a sequenced stimuli and response generative conditioning. Skinner referred to this action of decision making as the behavior of deciding with the primary reason being to escape from indecision (Skinner, 2005). Skinner stated that problem solving in its most basic sense is simply satisfying a need, such as hunger. He said, “Problem-solving may be defined as any behavior which, through the manipulation of variables, makes the appearance of a solution more probable” (Skinner, 2005, p. 247). To any problem, there are a multitude of responses. Some of these responses are based on past situations, and the same response may work with this given stimuli. However, there are times when an 15 individual encounters a new situation that would require a new response. This new response could occur by random selection and solve the problem by accident. There are times, however, when a new response is required and it is not based on random selection. Skinner refers to this occurrence as a trial and error performance where the organism learns how to try. According to Skinner, the control of the stimuli is the key in individual problem solving; therefore, the stimuli is controlling the response. The arrangement of the stimuli response variables in a syllogistic manner becomes, by its own nature, problem solving. One of the techniques of problem solving is the self-probe, which is the systematic review of tentative solutions. In some cases, this self-probe is merely an act of repetition. Skinner associated this skill of how to think with the acts of deprivation, in that individuals control their environments in order to manipulate their ability to find solutions. The origin of an idea is formulated in terms of a response. However, Skinner stated that it is actually the manipulation of variables (stimuli) that triggers the response. Even though it may seem as if this has been triggered subconsciously or as a sudden idea, this is as a result of encountering a similar situation that shares some of the same properties as the current situation. A truly original idea is that which “results from manipulations of variables which have not followed a rigid formula and in which the ideas have other sources of strength” (Skinner, 2005, p. 254). This is where the separation of an original idea and novel idea takes place in that a novel idea is one whose structure does not involve an original idea. However, it does utilize past experience and a defined behavioral process. This difference may appear to be subtle, but it becomes the basis of an evolution of ideas. The seemingly original or groundbreaking ideas are structured 16 upon what has already occurred, and they are merely another step in the process of thinking and invention. Because of this identification of structure in the origin of ideas, it then becomes possible, in educational pedagogy, to teach people to think critically through their ability to influence the stimuli and response process. Halpern (1996) stated that critical thinking involves using a particular set of skills that is useful within a given set of circumstances, usually when solving a problem. This skill set can be derived from a knowledge structure or a schema that has been obtained from a variety of sources including social, environmental and educational factors. The result is that an individual is forming inferences into logical patterns that provide the person with a defined evaluation component of the thought process. The objective of all critical thinking is to obtain a desired outcome for a given circumstance. Nondirected or automatic thinking, according to Halpern, is a result of routine, and while it may be goal directed, it involves little to no conscious effort. In order to solve a set of problems, the application of critical thinking must occur in a process that is systematic in order for learners to apply successful outcomes to the given situations. While serendipitous moments may occur, in order for the learners to sustain a rate of successful solutions, there must be a systematic thought process applied as to what the potential outcomes would be—a mental trial and error that leads to a successful conclusion. 17 Assumptions and Limitations PSSA The PSSA is a statistical measuring instrument that is utilized throughout the state of Pennsylvania. Because the assessment is utilized throughout the entire state of Pennsylvania, it is assumed to be a valid measuring instrument. Only one school system is being analyzed throughout this study. Therefore, the research should be replicable in all other scenarios where instrumental music education is present within the school system, especially Grades 5, 8, and 11. The limitations of this study are mostly based in the inexperience this researcher has conducting studies. The procedure is extremely important to establishing and maintaining both its internal and external validity. The validity of the state wide assessment is maintained by the Department of Education for the Commonwealth of Pennsylvania as well as the school district through regimented testing procedures and evaluation. The validity of the data collection will be established through a checks and balance system as well as an alpha numeric coding system. The PSSA is a statewide assessment that has been closely monitored by the Commonwealth of Pennsylvania in order to provide for an accurate coefficient alpha as well as decision consistency with the latter demonstrating great importance when it comes to standardized testing. Both the coefficient alpha and decision consistency demonstrated consistency across both of the true score measures and pseudodecision tables with scores ranging from .60 to the mid-.80s. These scores increased as the level of the test increased with the highest scores ranging above .90. 18 The PSSA assessment demonstrated strong internal and external relationships between the tests’ components, which were evidenced in the high correlation between subject area strands. An independent study commissioned by the Pennsylvania Board of Education also evidenced strong content and correlation validity when examining the statistical relationships of the PSSA assessment including convergent and discriminant validity with validity being increased when test scores are consistent. • Sampling approach and procedures (including recruitment, informing, and consenting). o If consent is needed, it could add additional time and complications to the study itself. • Procedures for assignment to groups. • Continuity, anonymity and accuracy of assigning the groups are the keys to a successful study. • Procedures for data collection, including determining the validity and reliability of measures. • Checking and assuring the accuracy of the t test. • Procedures for data analysis. Sample Limitations • Students who have an interest in instrumental music may already have a stronger sense of problem solving than those who do not want to get involved. • Students may already be involved in instrumental music but just not in a public school setting (i.e., private lessons). There is no direct way to access this information. 19 • Those students choosing music may already excel in mathematics and reading, therefore giving them an advantage to implement critical thinking skills. • Socioeconomic status may have a direct impact on those students who choose to study an instrument; therefore, sample population limitation may be inherent to the sample. • The students’ scores between the 5th-grade sample in both reading and math already displayed significant differences in the baseline measurements. The assumption can be made that these differences may be attributed to other influences that include IQ and socioeconomic status (SES). Expected Findings The main research question examined the relationship between instrumental music education instruction in Grades 8 and 11 and critical thinking skills on the PSSA assessment (sections RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1). This question led to the hypothesis that there is a correlation between critical thinking and instrumental music education. The findings of the present study should remain consistent with that of the reviewed literature in that there is a positive correlation between instrumental music and critical thinking. The first research subquestion examined whether there would be correlation between the number of years that a student was involved in instrumental music when compared to those students who were not. The expected finding was that a student would demonstrate an increase in PSSA scores when compared to those students who were not involved in music. 20 The second subquestion examined the mean cumulative scores of the PSSA subsections to demonstrate a statistical difference between the two samples. Because of the literature that showed higher scores on standardized assessments, this study predicted that there would be a significant statistically difference between the two sample populations. The third researched subquestion analyzed the statistical variance between each of the four subsections. The analysis of variance (ANOVA) is expected to illustrate that a student who participates in instrumental music education will outperform a student who does not take instrumental music. 21 CHAPTER 2. LITERATURE REVIEW The sweeping federal mandate of the No Child Left Behind Act (NCLB, 2001) has left public school systems scrambling to decrease achievement gaps and meet the benchmarks in the areas of English and Mathematics. NCLB was enacted in 2001 and was formed to promote and highlight the four major aspects of the educational system. NCLB will create accountability through assessments where the federal, state, and local governments can use the data in order to make informed judgments about their educational system and to monitor the progress of said educational system. The data will then be disaggregated in a manner by which areas of race, gender, economic, and disadvantaged students can be analyzed. NCLB was created to increase the school flexibility and to empower the school districts by giving them greater control over federal dollars, allowing them to transfer up to 50% of federal funds without prior approval. NCLB was designed to expand the options for disadvantaged students. Those students in failing schools would be able to transfer to better achieving schools, increased access to programs, and through the availability of charter school programs. An increased emphasis on reading would be taught to these students through the availability of funds that coincide with the President’s Reading First Program and also a focus on strengthening teacher quality. NCLB was also created to promote immigrant and bilingual education programs in order to increase English proficiency. 22 Over the past 10 years with the enactment of NCLB (2001), the focus of education has been to close the achievement gap between school populations and in doing so arts education has begun a steady decline (Rabkin & Redmond, 2006). Arts education is considered by many to be a nonessential part of an educational curriculum and in some minds; it should only exist for entertainment or leisure activities, therefore providing stigmas that are hard to overcome. However, the arts have long stimulated active learning and have been credited for creating behaviors that provide opportunities for critical thinking and self-awareness (Hamblen, 1997). In 1998, Eisner proposed a three-tier system in order to place into perspective the outcomes of an arts education. The first outcome examines whether or not the subject matter is being taught. The idea is that the teaching concept must be directly related to the material at hand. The second tier is that the student is able to recognize and comprehend the aesthetic experiences taking place. The final tier is the ancillary effects of the arts education, such as promoting creative behaviors, critical thinking and self-awareness (Hamblen, 1997). These ancillary effects have become the focus of many studies examining the relationship between the arts and other academic areas or the arts and other areas of development, such as IQ. Music and Academic Achievement There is a multitude of research available on the effect of music education on academic achievement. Babo (2004) analyzed the relationship between students’ participation in music education and their academic performance, and the research set controls of IQ, SES, and gender. Babo utilized a pool of 548 middle school students. Of 23 that number, a total of 93 students participated in instrumental music. Students were selected randomly and anonymously by class lists that were generated by the guidance counselors. Middle School 1 consisted of N = 40 selected students with 14 males and 26 females and Middle School 2 consisted of N = 53 students with 21 males and 32 females. Eighty-five Noninstrumental students were selected in a similar manner in order to establish a relationship between the two groups. The two groups totaled a data pool of N = 178 middle school students. The study indicated that there was a strong relationship between a student’s achievement in the language arts and their participation in instrumental music education. While the regression models clearly indicated that IQ had the strongest influence on test scores, instrumental music education also contributed highly to the overall variance of the mathematics total score. Further analysis, when controlling for gender and socioeconomic status, demonstrated that instrumental music influenced both the language arts and mathematical scores. The influence of the IQ variable could not be discounted in standardized testing and is an area that needs to be explored more fully to complete the integrity of the study. An additional area that needs to be scrutinized is that the study utilized two different middle schools and did not account for differences in instruction or teacher quality, all of which could have an impact on the overall results and efforts of the research. The study’s overall goal was to strengthen the case that the arts and more specifically music, contribute to the overall academic achievement of students (Babo, 2004). The construct that instrumental music education can have an influence on the students’ performance ability in mathematics and language arts is demonstrated clearly in this study. However, 24 what the study does not account for is how the students were affected cognitively. The idea that the scores were higher could simply be ancillary to the actual cause for the increase in test scores. While the previous study indicated that the students increased their test scores in the language arts and when controlling for SES and gender increased their mathematical scores, this alteration in cognitive ability seems to continue as the student ages. In addition, the construct that instrumental music has a long lasting affect on academic achievement is further enhanced by a study at Whitworth University. Strauch (2009) conducted research that indicates college freshmen, who have taken band in high school, not only have higher grade point averages (GPAs) while coming in to college, but they maintain those higher averages throughout their college career. Strauch examined the N = 537 students of the 2007–2008 incoming freshman class at Whitworth University, of which 103 (19.2%) had played in band through high school. The students who participated in band not only had a higher GPA but also higher Scholastic Aptitude Test (SAT) scores on both the verbal and math portions of the test (Olson, 2009). The increase in SAT scores, which are administered in the students’ high school years, concurs with the Babo (2004) study, which displayed increases in the same assessed areas. The theory that the arts can influence other academic areas was examined by Moga, Burger, Hetland, and Winner (2000), who performed a meta-analysis of the relationship between academic achievement and arts education Moga et al. reviewed 188 reports and found three areas where causal links proved to be reliable. The first found a medium sized causal relationship between listening and a temporary improvement in spatial–temporal reasoning. In educational terms, these 26 reports provided little useful 25 information because it was not found why the connection exists. However, it does point to the existence of a link between the psychological and possible neurological connections that are created between music and the brain. This, in turn, demonstrates that music has a cognitive affect on how the brain functions and also the ability to create changes in the way nonmusical information is processed. The second finding, which was based on 19 reports, demonstrated that there is a large causal relationship between learning to play music and spatial reasoning. This effect has greater applicability in educational scenarios because the effect was reported equally among both general and at risk populations. It was shown that 69 out of every 100 students between the ages of 3 and 12 displayed an increase in spatial reasoning skills (Moga et al., 2000). This causal relationship becomes a greater contributor to the way information is processed. If music can have an impact on the cognitive process of spatial reasoning, then perhaps there are other areas of temporal functioning that stand to benefit from participation in music. However, depending on how material is taught and further utilized, spatial reasoning could provide limited educational benefits to the overall curriculum. The last study, which examined 80 reports, displayed a causal connection between drama and verbal skills, in which the transfer of verbal skills increased in students who enacted texts compared to those who did not enact texts. The evidence also demonstrated an increase in the students’ ability to read new texts. The impact upon education is that students can benefit from a direct transfer of information, allowing them to format new information more effectively. The connection between the arts and other academic areas is not by any means a new topic, and it has been linked throughout the centuries. Conventional wisdom would 26 suggest that music and mathematics are interrelated subjects simply by the use of mathematical computations in both the rhythmical and theoretical aspects of music. Vaughn (2000) stated that if music enhances spatial–temporal reasoning, then it would stand to reason that music would contribute to the area of mathematics that utilizes said reasoning. Vaughn reported on three meta-analyses that examined the relationship between music and mathematics. The first of these meta-analyses was performed using 20 correlational studies that used the SAT as their assessment. The total sample size was N = 5,788,132 and ranged in years from 1950–1999. The results indicated that there was a modest positive correlation between the study of music and mathematical achievement. However, other factors could not be ruled out; therefore, causation could not be completely affirmed. The next meta-analysis was performed using 6 experimental studies in which the students received instruction on either instrumental or vocal training for a period of at least 4 months. Total sample size for the experimental study was N = 357 and the publications ranged from 1957–1999. Results appeared to be inconclusive except for the study that included spatial–temporal reasoning, which showed a positive effect by the interjection of music into the student’s curriculum. The last of the meta-analyses were experimental studies that examined the use of background music to enhance mathematical performance. The N = 1,652 participants listened to music that was considered to be soothing, such as classical, instrumental music and Muzak, which was contradicted with music that was considered to be distracting, such as rock and rap. The results indicated a very weak hypothesis with the integration of background music only having a small positive effect. Therefore, the assumption could be made that it is the interactive and developmental properties of music that allows the cognitive process to be 27 enhanced and even transformed over a period of time. The possibility of the transformation of other cognitive skills then becomes a distinct and viable possibility. Music and Personality Exposure to instrumental music, in and of itself, may not always demonstrate academic success, but it may allude to the ability to influence other areas of an individual’s personality. Costa-Giomi (2004) studied the effects of 3 years of piano instruction on both academic performance and self-esteem. The research noted that the students who studied piano showed no significant difference in academic achievement from their peers who did not, but they did show a difference in self-esteem. This study utilized N = 117 fourth-grade children attending a public school in Montreal, Canada. The 63 children in the experimental group received piano instruction for 3 years and an acoustic piano for home use. For the control group (N = 54), the students received no formal music instruction. The students were monitored throughout their 3 years of formal instruction in the categories of self-esteem, academic achievement, cognitive abilities, musical abilities and motor proficiency. The students showed increases in self-esteem and their school grades but not in the standardized testing for either mathematics or language arts. This study demonstrates that the influence of music may go beyond that of traditional academic areas in that the student could be gaining attributes that affect all areas of the cognitive structures. The area of adolescent development is one that is continuous and assists in developing one’s thought process. Music has long been used to influence the emotional states of a human being. Approaching the phenomenon through an inductive theory construction, group interviews 28 were conducted of N = 8 adolescents that were subsidized by follow up forms (Saarikallio & Erkkila, 2007). All the material collected was analyzed using the constructive grounded theory method. The constructivist grounded theory, which was developed by Charmaz, was described by Creswell (2007) as an approach “that includes emphasizing diverse local worlds, multiple realities, and the complexities of particular worlds, views, and actions” (p. 65). Charmaz focused on the relationship between researcher and participants and pays particular attention to this interaction; therefore, the researcher’s viewpoint becomes a critical part of the constructivist theory. Instead of playing a distant part to the research and theory, the researcher injects viewpoints and an understandable and coherent form of writing. While grounded theory is based in reality or, as Charmaz stated, multiple realities, it is designed to gain a better understanding and possibly the ability to control a given situation. While much music consumption in adolescents is based in affective behaviors, the understanding of the psychological functions is conceptually diverse and theoretically unstructured (Saarikallio & Erkkila, 2007). This study was designed in order to assist in theoretically structuring the mood regulation aspects of music. Adolescents were chosen to be the focal point of this study because of their large consumption of music and the importance that music plays in their lives. These teens were chosen by the use of purposeful sampling and then separated into two age groups, 14 and 17. Each group consisted of two boys and two girls. In the first of two interview sessions, the teens were asked to bring along one musical selection of personal importance. The teens were asked to discuss the meanings 29 of the recording and then completed a follow up form after each activity. The form consisted of three parts: describe the musical situation, affective behaviors measuring energy and pleasantness level, and reflect on the experience. The second interview session was used to focus more deeply on mood relationships and changes (Saarikallio & Erkkila, 2007). Saarikallio and Erkkila (2007) used axial coding to define categories and then establish links between the separate levels and categories. The end result was a model that displayed the main categories, regulatory strategies, regulated elements of mood, musical activities, and outside influences. This study used a relatively small purposeful sampling group of eight participants who were interviewed by groups. To increase the depth and validity of the research and results, a larger sampling group should have been utilized as well as both group and individual interviews. The larger sampling group could have added adolescents who have demonstrated troubled behavior as well as students who have exhibited normal behaviors. The data would be collected in multiple field visits, analyzed and then the process repeated. Then the formulation of a focal point centers the study and allows for the establishment of a given theory. Also, the adolescents were allowed to choose a recording that had a distinct personal meaning to them. To enhance the integrity of the study, it would have been important to include the introduction of separate pieces of music to evoke an affective behavior. Perhaps at this point, the researchers could have exposed themselves to the same music, written down their affective behavior, and then compared them to the adolescents’ reactions. The researchers concluded that this is a relatively 30 unexplored phenomenon and should be examined more in depth to determine reliability and consistency. The questions in these studies demonstrate that music has a variety of effects on learners such has how proper music instruction can assist in developing either academic or social skill sets. However, students who develop these skills in a music program do so developmentally over an extended period of time; therefore, the focus of the study needs to be on the developmental side rather than that of the postdevelopment. Of the previous studies referenced, there are two main areas that still need to be examined. The first is the question of how does the arts compare to other activities, and the second of is exploring the differences over an extended period of time. An example of this longitudinal type of study was presented by Shropshire (2007). Students’ Preliminary Scholastic Aptitude Test (PSAT) scores were examined for differences between students who participated in music, athletics, and both programs. The results showed the students who were involved in music outperformed those who participated in athletics with no appreciable difference in those who participated in both and those who participated in music only. According to Arasi (2006), the extra musical benefits far outweigh the musical influences when a person becomes an adult, meaning that the lifelong effects of music on a person outlives the influences of the music itself. Her study qualitatively examined the affects of a high school music program on three students and cited the benefits of lifelong skills such as critical thinking, problem solving, and self-confidence. While the study examined the phenomenon of ancillary musical influences, it only examined three adults’ perceptions of what skills they had developed through high school chorus. Because of the 31 qualitative nature of the study, the individuals’ perceptions were not compared or contrasted with others who did not have the same experiences. Therefore, the contribution of this study is primarily based from a personal perspective and in doing so, it provides an internal light on an individual’s perspective on how he has been influenced by music. While this in and of itself can be limiting, it also demonstrates how a person views himself in relationship to the rest of society, and how this relationship has been influenced by music. Critical Thinking Moga et al. (2000) stated that perhaps the focus should shift from the requirement of the arts to make a transfer of skills to other subject areas such as math and science to examining how the arts cultivate transfer. While the arts are a vitally important aspect of culture, the notion that they contribute to other areas of the learning comes naturally because of the inclusive nature of the arts as a whole. The integral nature of the arts allows for the performer to become an active problem solver and, therefore, provides a direct impact upon the overall outcome. This idea incorporates the motivational aspect of the arts; the individual has an influence on the end result, thus evolving into a learnercentered design. This learner-centered design was referenced by psychologist David Perkins when he stated that any subject can transfer thinking skills, but the arts are a particular vehicle for this because of their ability to develop contextually through their engagement, ability to sustain attention and the encouragement of rich connections (as cited in Moga et al., 2000). This strategy for transfer is featured in the principles of learning-centered design. 32 In 1968, Gagne suggested that instruction should take place in a hierarchical sequence allowing the learner to benefit from a bottom-up processing method wherein the most elemental parts are taught first. This method would be constructed by using the parts-to-whole organizing principal allowing for the higher order skills to be taught after the basics have been learned (Van Patten, Chao, & Reigeluth, 1986). In 1977, Gagne identified eight internal phases that support the learning sequence process: (a) activating motivation, (b) informing the learner of the objective, (c) directing attention, (d) stimulating recall, (e) providing learning guidance, (f) enhancing retention, (g) promoting the transfer of learning, and (h) eliciting performance and providing feedback. Mawhinney, Frusciante, Aaron, and Liu (2002) identified five design principles that should be applied to establish a learning-centered program. The first is to develop a student knowledge base, which is developed in music by formulating small groups of homogenous ability and instruments. This allows for the students to progress at a level that is appropriate and developmentally sound. In this early stage of development, students will learn the basics of their instrument, as well as musical terminology and historical contexts. During this period the students are working on the first two steps, remember and understanding, of the Anderson and Krathwohl (2001) revised taxonomy. The second design principle outlined by Mawhinney et al. (2002) is to increase student motivation. Students who begins instruments will become frustrated within the first few months. In order to increase the motivation of the students, they should be encouraged to practice toward goals that are challenging and appropriate. To further encourage the efforts of the students, they should be allowed to explore new rhythms, notes and dynamics expressions and they should begin to create their own melodic 33 structures. In order to complete the activity of exploration and creation, the students will need to engage the third design principle of student strategic processing. During the design stage, the students will need to access their critical and creative thinking as well as problem-solving skills. Lisk (2006) described music as intelligence in action and that performing in an instrumental ensemble requires a multisensory approach combined with a perceptive decision-making process. This makes music in itself a problem-solving inthe-moment exercise. Students, however, must be taught to recognize not only how to apply critical thinking skills but where they become a necessary and vital component of music. Students’ work should be showcased and demonstrated both to their peers and to their parents. Not only will this provide intrinsic motivation for the students, but it allows the students to analyze their work and the work of others. As students demonstrate their playing and creating ability, individual differences will occur in the students’ development. This critical fourth stage now shifts the attentional process to the relationship between the teacher and student. The teacher’s role, at this stage, is to bridge the developmental gap between the students with sound pedagogical technique. To assist in bridging this gap, Turner (1999) suggested the creation of centers, such as rhythm reading areas, so that students can work on problem areas at their own pace and achieve musical success. These centers contribute to the discovery learning method much like that of the Montessori method, which relies on collaborative learning and problem solving to drive the learning process. The fifth design principle is creating learning contexts, where the social context of learning is controlled in order to increase student achievement. Mawhinney et al. (2002) stated that student achievement can be increased by establishing learning based 34 environment of collaboration, cultural understanding, and technology. In instrumental music lessons, there are a multitude of areas that can be addressed. Music in and of itself is rich in cultural heritage and can be used to represent the cultural backgrounds of the students. The idea of collaboration can be addressed through peer on peer assessments where the students can utilize their analyzing, evaluating and problem-solving techniques. Lisk (1996) described that the musician must have an active involvement in the musical decisions of the ensemble, therefore allowing the individual to create meaning in the musical performance. In order to assist the students in building their knowledge base as well as developing creatively, music writing and processing programs can be used where the students can create their own written compositions. These compositions can then be played by other students allowing them to experience each other’s work. Turner (1999) stated that this student-centered design allows for students to contribute their unique musical ideas to the lesson without relying on the assessment of the teacher. In addition, it allows the students opportunities to demonstrate their musical knowledge and understanding (Turner, 1999). By using the five design principles suggested by Mawhinney et al. (2002), instrumental music education can move from a teacher-centered to a student-centered design. The teacher is still needed to act as knowledgeable instructor guiding expectation levels and working toward a level of mastery (Suzuki). At the same time, students should be allowed to explore music through both their creative and critical thinking skills sets, which stress the inherent motivation that is the quest for knowledge (Montessori). Activities themselves must be developed that are both age and developmentally appropriate so that the students are ensured success (Piaget). These design principles 35 coupled with a learning-centered curriculum change the paradigm of the education system on the most basic of premises that learning is student-centered. The learning-centered curriculum is a strategic student-oriented plan designed to be advantageous for the students both in form of interaction and retention. Before implementing such a curriculum, a school entity needs to take into account the following areas: learning context, planning, assessment, and programming strategies (Hubball, Gold, Mighty, & Britnell, 2007). Once these strategies have been implemented, the school system needs to evaluate each stage of the students’ development processes to ensure readiness, difficulty of subject matter and motivational strategies. In curriculum, evolution is a self-evident and discovery process that allows for cognitive modifiability, which in turn grants flexibility to both the students and the teachers. Perhaps, then, the effect of an arts infused program transcends the outcome of arts itself. These intangible lessons can then be generated into other aspects of thinking such as critical thinking. According to Halpern (1996), critical thinkers are constantly analyzing new situations, searching for complexity and ambiguity, then taking that information and comparing it to prior knowledge and experience. Halpern (1996) stated that critical thinking involves using a particular set of skills that is useful within a given set of circumstances, usually when solving a problem. This skill set can be derived from a knowledge structure or schemata that have been obtained from a variety of sources including social, environmental and educational factors. The result is that an individual is forming inferences into logical patterns that provide the person with a defined evaluation component of the thought process. The objective of all critical thinking is to obtain a desired outcome for a given circumstance. Nondirected or 36 automatic thinking, according to Halpern, is a result of routine, and while it may be goal directed, it involves little to no conscious effort. Gagne (1980) believed that the initial components of learning must be in place in order for higher order complex learning skills, like problem solving, to take place. That traditional education is not designed to address these complex skills; therefore, they must be taught by less traditional methods (Derry & Murphy, 1986). Moga et al. (2000) examined whether the arts engendered creative thinking, which involves problem solving, inquiry, and open-ended thinking. Their meta-analyses, the first of which was based on seven correlational studies, found that there was a modest association between studying the arts and performance on creative measures. In three of the studies, however, the students self-selected the arts. Therefore, it could be stated that the creative thinkers were naturally drawn to that subject matter. The second metaanalyses focused on experimental studies that examined the arts and the effects on verbal creativity. The effect size in this case was not significant as demonstrated by both the t test and Stouffer’s Z. The last of the meta-analyses examined experimental studies that focused on the effects of arts education on figural creativity. The results indicated that a causal relationship could be established between arts education and figural creativity. However, the same results indicate that this does not hold true for verbal or conceptual creativity. The idea that arts can influence creative thought on any level indicates that there is an effect on the cognitive process. The construct that creative thinking can be enhanced directly aligns itself with the thought process of critical thinking. If it were not for creative thinking, the critical thinking process would have no grounds on which to be based. 37 Halpern (1997) stated that “when we think critically, we are evaluating the outcomes of our thought processes” (p. 4). The evolution of this human thinking process has come to be known as cognitive process instruction, and the goal is to understand how knowledge, cognitive processes and mechanisms can improve how people think (Halpern, 1997). Music instruction becomes but one facet in this process of instruction, but because of its didactic nature, it allows for the cognitive responses to flow through a multisensory experience, evoking both educational and aesthetic experiences. Music Education and Intelligence Music education, by itself, is not the only contributing factor to an individual’s ability to function, form creative and constructive thoughts. Intelligence can be generalized in terms of how one learns from experience and adapts to one’s surroundings. Measurements of intelligence can be traced to two sources: one, psychophysical intelligence (sensory and physical/motor skills) and two, judgmental abilities (Sternberg, 2003). Because of intelligences’ implicit meanings, the word intelligence can more aptly be defined by its context or connotation rather than the explicit meaning of its assessment. Francis Galton (1822–1911) believed that intelligence was based in psychophysical abilities. These abilities included weight discrimination, pitch discrepancy, strength, and other sensitivities (Sternberg, 2003). Alfred Binet (1857–1911) believed that intelligence was based in judgment and was comprised of three different parts: discretion, adaptation, and criticism (Sternberg, 2003). Binet used this intelligence discretion to determine people who should be 38 considered mentally retarded. He later used these theories to establish mental age in conjunction with physical age. William Stern (1912) was the first person to develop the ability to compare intelligence through relative means rather than by physical age. This was done through the establishment of an intelligence quotient (IQ), which calculates the ratio between mental and chronological age (Sternberg, 2003). The result being that when the mental age exceeds the actual age the resulting score will be over 100 and the inverse will result in a score below 100. The next evolutionary development in intelligence is the focus on the structure of intelligence and the applicability of factor analysis, which separates intelligence into various abilities and factors. The man closely associated with factor analysis is Charles Spearman (1863–1945). He concluded that intelligence can be separated into its various constructs but the labeling of a “g” factor or general factor or “mental energy” was what he felt as the most important (Sternberg, 2003). A more contemporary viewpoint of intelligence would lie in the theory of multiple intelligences by Gardner. His original theory included seven different areas of intelligence: Linguistic, Logical–mathematical, Musical, Spatial, Bodily–kinesthetic, Interpersonal, and Intrapersonal (Guignon, 2004). His latest addition to the seven intelligences is the naturalist, or the ability to classify plants, minerals, and animals. These eight intelligences could still be broken down into the respective psychophysical and judgmental abilities. The importance of these intelligences lies in close alignment with what is being viewed as important either by society or another criterion for assessment. 39 The idea that music is an intelligence is, in and of itself, a revolution in thought because of its mandate that it acts in symbiosis, with the rest of the being including cognitive structures. All the evidence displayed contributes to an individual’s awareness of the formation of the thought process and to the overall outcomes of intelligence. Perhaps the ability of music to influence not only affects the cognitive functioning of the individual but also personality and moods of the individuals. Reimer (2002) stated that Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences removes the construct that music is a talent with the connotation that some people are gifted with it and others are not, but instead, that music manifests itself in the cognitive operation process. She stated that “music is a way to know the world—to create and share meaning in the world—and to function effectively in this mode of cognition, one’s musical intelligence must be developed” (p. 77). Schellenberg (2006) examined 6- to 11-year-old children who each varied in amount of musical training. The baseline IQ was established by administering the WISC– III as well as assessing other areas of intellectual functioning such as grades in school and standardized tests of academic achievement. The sample was comprised of N = 147 (72 boys and 75 girls) ages 6–11 recruited from a middle class suburb of Toronto, Canada. The predictor variables were measured using a questionnaire that was administered to parents about their child’s history with private music lessons. The criterion variables consisted of measures of intelligence, which were assessed using the WISC–III, academic ability, which was assessed using the K–TEA, and social adjustment, which was measured using the Parent Rating Scale of BASC. 40 The principal analyses, consisting of correlations between the main predictor variable and criterion variables, demonstrated that music lessons were positively correlated with both academic achievement and IQ, but not social adjustment. The outcome was that the duration of music lessons has a small but positive correlation to measures of intelligence. The second study examined the effects of long term music lessons on intellectual abilities and more specifically, if these had lasting effects even after the music lessons had ended. The participants of the second study were undergraduates at a suburban Canadian university with the range in age being between 16 and 25. More than half had taken private music lessons (N = 84) for an average of 7.8 years. The students were surveyed based on a questionnaire where the students were paid to participate in the 2hour survey. The criterion variables in Study 2 consisted of intelligence and academic achievement, which was measured using the WAIS–III, and an additional subtest was Object Assemble was administered to measure spatial–temporal ability (Schellenberg, 2006). The results indicated that taking music lessons regularly was correlated positively with IQ especially in the areas of perceptual organization and working memory. The results of the Schellenberg (2006) study indicated that there was a positive correlation between childhood lessons and IQ with this impacting lasting into early adulthood. Perhaps the more important conclusion in this study is that there is a direct causal effect between the duration of musical training and the predictor of better intellectual functioning. The benefit of this study to the efforts of music education was enhanced, because the effects of music lessons on intelligence could not be discounted by parents’ education or family income. 41 As a result of the previous findings, intelligence is an adaptable and modifiable skill that can be enhanced over a period of time. This, in turn, directly relates to the ability of instrumental music education to create cognitive links through the conceptual development of child and adolescent mind. As presented in the Schellenberg (2006) study, the single factor of instrumental lessons could account for the substantial proportion of the variance of the tests. Therefore, the summation could be made that music lessons were the determining predictor valuable that altered the test scores. Critical thinking, just like that of intelligence, personality and academic achievement is a skill that can be either taught or enhanced over a period of time. Therefore, instrumental music education can have a similar longitudinal impact to a student’s critical thinking skills. Halpern (1997) stated that critical thinking skills can be taught effectively through the transfer of training where thinking skills can be applied to a vast array of contexts. When students think critically, they are evaluating the outcomes of their thinking processes, which could involve solving problems, making inferences and evaluating decisions. In music education, critical thinking skills are applied where the paradigm shifts from teacher-centered to student-centered learning. Musicians use these skills to evaluate their performances in the context of technique, intonation, stylistic interpretation, notation and ensemble (Reimer, 2002). 42 CHAPTER 3. METHODOLOGY Purpose of the Study The purpose of this research is to explore the correlation between critical thinking and instrumental music education. Instrumental education traditionally uses a hierarchical structure in order to facilitate structured and sequenced learning through vertical skills transfer. Gagne (1980) stated that in a typical education higher functioning intellectual skills are rarely taught and often do not benefit from direct instruction in how to exhibit these specific cognitive processes. Instrumental music education uses problem-solving and critical thinking skills as an inherit part of the instructional and learning method. However, the transfer of this skill set to other subject areas must occur through a vertical and lateral transfer of the learning process. The transfer of problem skills, in this study, must occur from that of instrumental music education to the areas of reading and mathematics. This research was done through analyzing testing of both instrumental music participants and nonparticipants of the course of their school years (8th and 11th grades). In addition, the study analyzed group scores (math and reading) from their 5th, 8th and 11th grades. This study was able to examine the differences between students and the amount of music exposure they have had based on the number of years of instrumental music education. According to Breakwell et al. (2006), this would constitute a 43 longitudinal design, whereby data are being collected from the same sample over a period of time. The importance of this study relates directly to understanding the executive intellectual skills that instrumental music education may assist in developing and also that these skills are transferable to other academic areas. By bridging the gap in literature, music educators would be able to demonstrate the importance of music education to all students, teachers and administrators. Previous studies have covered the effect of instrumental music on academic achievement, intelligence and personality. Conceptually, the construct that instrumental music can influence the cognitive development process will provide great value to and significance to the existing body of literature. Research Design Prebeginning Building administrators were contacted in order to ensure cooperation and availability of both sample size and information provided by the Pennsylvania Department of Education (PADOE). Initial Phase Students for Group A, B, C will be selected based on the following criteria: grade level (5th, 8th, 11th) and their participation in instrumental music. Students for Group A1, B1, and C1 will be selected based on the following criteria: grade level (5th, 8th, 11th) and nonparticipation in music. Once the stratified sample is formed, the same sample will be continuous throughout the research. Group A (5th grade) will only be used for the scaled score of overall math and reading assessment. 44 Final Phase The critical thinking and problem-solving assessments are administered as part of the PSSA with the testing occurring in 3rd, 5th, 8th and 11th grades Results were analyzed, compared and correlated from grade to grade, years of participation in instrumental music, and between Group B and Group C using archival data. Instrumental music education begins in the 5th grade and given the grading years of the PSSA, this allows for a pretest (5th) and posttest design (8th & 11th). This will allow the research to demonstrate the scores of both Noninstrumental and Instrumental, which will be analyzed for statistical differences between studies and later for variance. The scores from the PSSA tests were then used to analyze the difference between Instrumental and Noninstrumental students to determine if there any relationship between the variables of critical thinking and instrumental music education. Quantitative: Correlational Research Design Correlational studies provide the first step in determining the relationship between two variables, and therefore, they allow for the connection or lack thereof to be determined between the criterion and predictor variables. The research question, effects of instrumental music upon critical thinking skills, is an example of a correlational study utilizing retrospective and archival data. The correlation coefficient will allow for a measure of the degree or strength of this relationship between instrumental music education and critical thinking (Howell, 2008). Further, a bivariate correlation approach will allow for the demonstration of degree and direction of the relationship (Howell, 2008). The results from a correlation study will be demonstrated as either a positive, negative or null hypothesis. This type of 45 study is subject to errors because of the influence of additional variables. In the case of this research study, the variables were controlled by the use of stratified sampling within the specified criteria. The cross-lagged panel correlation method could be used to assist in controlling the validity of the results. This method allows the researcher to examine the issue over different periods of time (Howell, 2008). Utilizing a bivariate analysis, which allows for the simultaneous examination of two variables (instrumental music education and critical thinking skills), allowed the researcher to demonstrate whether or not a relationship exists between the dependent and criterion variables. Once a relationship is established, the researcher was able to make a prediction through regression of the dependent variable (critical thinking). The two primary constructs are critical thinking (dependent variable) and instrumental music education (independent variable). The psychological basis for investigation is whether or not instrumental music education can affect a student’s critical thinking skills. Problem Solving The PSSA uses open-ended questions to assess students’ problem-solving ability. This assessment uses open-ended questions in both the reading and mathematics portion of the assessment. The PSSA (PADOE, 2005) provides a separate score for the openended questions to each of the schools. Critical Thinking Skills Halpern (1996) stated that critical thinking involves using a particular set of skills that is useful within a given set of circumstances, usually when solving a problem. This skill set can be derived from a knowledge structure or schemata that has been obtained 46 from a variety of sources, including social, environmental and educational factors. The result is that an individual is forming inferences into logical patterns that provide the person with a defined evaluation component of the thought process. The objective of all critical thinking is to obtain a desired outcome for a given circumstance. Nondirected or automatic thinking, according to Halpern, is a result of routine and while it may be goal directed, it involves little to no conscious effort. Critical thinking allows the person to subjectively analyze objective material. In 1998, Halpern proposed a four component model for the transfer of critical thinking skills. The first component is attitude, which she describes as the recognition of critical thinking as a skill and then the implementation of such by the individual. Next is the instruction and practice of critical thinking as a skill, in that a person will recognize the benefit of the utilization of critical thinking skills. The third is the ability to transfer the elements of problem and the application of them to a new contextual situation. The last component is the use of metacognition to enhance and facilitate the process of critical thinking. Even though the PSSA uses the terminology of critical thinking skills, the assessment scores the open-ended questions as problem-solving skills. Therefore, the dependent variable for this research project will be critical thinking skills. According to the definition provided in section 3.3, the attributes of critical thinking are present in problem solving. However, problem solving can be taught and utilized in a sequential systematic way (Halpern, 1998). 47 Instrumental Music Education In this research design, instrumental music education is defined as a student who has participated in formalized instrumental music instruction in either the woodwind or brass family. Instrumental music education, in the Tunkhannock Area School District, begins in the 5th grade. In this research project, the student will have the following levels of experience or background in music education: 5th grade will have 1 year of instrumental music experience, 8th grade will have 4 years, and the 11th grade student will have 7 years of experience in music education. Target Population and Participant Selection Stratified Samples Students for Group A, B, C will be selected based on the following criteria: grade level (5th, 8th, 11th) and their participation in instrumental music. Groups B and C will involve the same students as group A; however, the students will now be in the 8th (B) and 11th (C) grades. Students for Group A1, B1, and C1 will be selected based on the following criteria: grade level (5th, 8th, 11th) and nonparticipation in music. Groups B1 and C1 will involve the same students as group A1; however, the students will now be in the 8th (B1) and 11th (C1) grades. Once the stratified sample is formed, the same sample will be continuous throughout the research. The critical thinking assessments are administered as part of the PSSA with the testing occurring in 3rd, 5th, 8th, and 11th grades. Results will be analyzed, compared and correlated from grade to grade, years of participation in instrumental music, and between Group A and Group A1. 48 Instrumental Music Students Students will be chosen based on their continue participation in music from 5th– 11th grade. If a student has discontinued instrumental music before the 11th grade, the student will then not be used in the Instrumental music sample. Noninstrumental Music Students Students for this sample will never have participated in instrumental music education within the school system. Leedy and Ormrod (2005) recommended that when the population size is around 500, 50% of the population size should be sampled. Each grade level is estimated at around 200 students, which gives a population size of about 100 students. Therefore, a population size of 100 students (50 Instrumental and 50 Noninstrumental) will be chosen. In the case that 50 Instrumental students are not available for a particular grade, the sample will be adjusted to accommodate, for example, using the graduating class of 2008 and 2009, thus yielding a larger sample size. By using multiple years, the power of the study would increase as well as the validity and integrity of the study. For a sample size of N = 50, a power analysis indicated that power was .9565 and the critical F = 4.11. The effect size was estimated at p = .5 with the alpha being .05. Stratified sampling is utilized because this study is examining three separate strata of the given student population. These three strata are as follows: 5th, 8th, and 11th grade. This sampling method will allow this researcher to examine the three grade levels, which in turn will allow for a direct comparison of the problem-solving and critical skill development of these students. In addition, since this is a longitudinal study, the same students’ development can then be examined over the three grade levels. 49 According to Howell (2008), the Type I error is defined as rejecting the null hypothesis when it is true, and the Type II error is failing to reject the null hypothesis when it is false. The manipulation of rejection levels can also be helpful when there are many samples or there are several stages to a testing process. Controlling Type I and II errors allows the researcher to control the conditions by eliminating specific errors in order to control the variables. This will then stabilize the results of the test. For example, in drug testing, a high rejection level may be set increasing, the Type I errors and decreasing the Type II errors. The second stage of the testing process might have a very low rejection level, increasing the number of Type II errors and decreasing the number of Type I errors. However, since the researcher controlled the first stage of the process, the results will display few Type II errors in the end research (Howell, 2008). Procedures Prebeginning Building administrators will be contacted in order to insure cooperation and availability of both sample size and information provided by the PADOE. Sample Identification Phase Students for Group A, B, C will be selected based on the following criteria: grade level (5th, 8th, 11th) and their participation in instrumental music. Students for Group A1, B1 and C1 will be selected based on the following criteria: grade level (5th, 8th, 11th) and nonparticipation in music. Once the stratified sample is formed, the same sample will be continuous throughout the research. 50 Initial Phase The critical thinking and problem-solving assessments are administered as part of the PSSA with the testing occurring in 3rd, 5th, 8th, and 11th grades. Final Phase Results will be analyzed, compared and correlated from grade to grade, years of participation in instrumental music, and between Group A and Group B using archival data. Ethical Considerations Right to Privacy Permission was gained in advance from all parties and the participants’ information is kept confidential. All students are assigned numbers as part of the PSSA assessment. These numbers will be utilized in order to protect the students’ anonymity and the students’ identity will remain confidential. This service of confidentiality will be provided by the school district and the researcher will remain separate from this knowledge. Honesty According to Leedy and Ormrod (2005), “Researchers must report their findings in a complete and honest fashion without misrepresenting what they have done or intentionally misleading others about the nature of their findings” (p. 102). The research must follow the guidelines provided by the American Psychological Association (2002) Code of Ethics in regards to plagiarism, reporting of research, and publication credit. 51 All students take the PSSA assessment as part of their state requirement. All the data from said assessment is collected by the state and disaggregated to the individual school systems both as large group assessments and individual scores for students. The students, as part of the assessment, are assigned individual numbers by the state. Therefore, the school system can provide a list of numbers to the researcher of the target populations. This stratified information will then be placed into SPSS in order to generate a random sample. Once the individual students have been selected, a random number generator will be used to assign a random number to the PSSA assigned number. This allows for a two stage process of protection and will not allow people who have access to the PSSA assigned number the ability to track the individual student. In addition to this random number assignment, the researcher will be prohibited from receiving the names of the students that correspond to the assigned PSSA number. This will assist in removing any bias on the part of the researcher and insuring anonymity. Instruments Problem-solving assessments are administered as part of the PSSA with the testing occurring in 3rd, 5th, 8th, and 11th grades. The PSSA uses open-ended questions to assess students’ problem-solving ability. This assessment uses open-ended questions in both the reading and mathematics portion of the assessment. The PSSA provides individual student scores for each of the subsets listed as follows. 52 • Reading B: Interpretation and analysis of text • Mathematics C.1 (geometry): Analyze characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes • Mathematics D.2 (algebraic concepts): Analyze mathematical situations using numbers, symbols, words, tables and/or graphs • Mathematics E.1 (data analysis and probability): Interpret and analyze data by formulating answers or questions These selected areas not only provide the appropriate prerequisite of critical thinking skills and demonstrate problem-solving ability, but they also remain consistent data throughout the grade levels of the assessment (3rd, 5th, 8th, and 11th). Problem Solving and Critical Thinking According to Skinner (2005), the determinant for how one thinks critically must be based and trained in operant conditioning. Therefore, the abilities required to be a critical thinker are trainable through a sequenced stimuli and response generative conditioning. Skinner referred to this action of decision making as the behavior of deciding, with the primary reason being to escape from indecision. Skinner stated that problem solving in its most basic sense is simply satisfying a need, such as hunger. He said, “Problem-solving may be defined as any behavior which, through the manipulation of variables, makes the appearance of a solution more probable” (p. 247). To any problem, there are a multitude of responses. Some of these responses are based on past situations, when the same response may work with this given stimuli. However, there are times when an individual encounters a new situation that would require a new response. This new response could occur by random selection and solve the problem by accident. 53 There are times, however, when a new response is required and it is not based on random selection. Skinner referred to this occurrence as a trial and error performance where the organism learns how to try. According to Skinner, the control of the stimuli is the key in individual problem solving; therefore, the stimuli is controlling the response. The arrangement of the stimuli response variables in a syllogistic manner becomes, by its own nature, problem solving. One of the techniques of problem solving is the self-probe, which is the systematic review of tentative solutions. In some cases, this self-probe is merely an act of repetition. Skinner associated this skill of how to think with the acts of deprivation, in that individuals control their environments in order to manipulate their ability to find a solution. The origin of an idea is formulated in terms of a response; however, Skinner stated that it is actually the manipulation of variables (stimuli) that triggers the response. Even though it may seem as if this has been triggered subconsciously or as a sudden idea, this is as a result of encountering a similar situation that shares some of the same properties as the current situation. A truly original idea is that which “results from manipulations of variables which have not followed a rigid formula and in which the ideas have other sources of strength” (Skinner, 2005, p. 254). This is where the separation of an original idea and novel idea takes place, in that a novel idea is one whose structure does not involve an original idea; however, it does utilize past experience and a defined behavioral process. This difference may appear to be subtle. However, it becomes the basis of an evolution of ideas. Seemingly original or groundbreaking ideas are structured upon what has already occurred and are merely another step in the process of thinking and invention. Because of this identification of structure in the origin of ideas, it then 54 becomes possible, in educational pedagogy, to teach people to think critically through their ability to influence the stimuli and response process. Halpern (1996) stated that critical thinking involves using a particular set of skills that is useful within a given set of circumstances, usually when solving a problem. This skill set can be derived from a knowledge structure or schemata that has been obtained from a variety of sources, including social, environmental and educational factors. The result is that an individual is forming inferences into logical patterns that provide the person with a defined evaluation component of the thought process. The objective of all critical thinking is to obtain a desired outcome for a given circumstance. Nondirected or automatic thinking, according to Halpern, is a result of routine and while it may be goal directed it involves little to no conscious effort. Operational Definition: Instrumental Music Education The student will have participated in the following grade levels and therefore demonstrate formalized experience in music education (instrumental music lessons and band). Fifth grade will have 1 year of instrumental music experience, 8th grade will have 4 years, and the 11th grade student will have 7 years of experience in music education. Instrumental music education begins in the 5th grade, and given the grading years of the PSSA, this allows for a pretest (5th) and posttest design (8th and 11th). This will allow the research to demonstrate the scores of both Noninstrumental and Instrumental, which will be analyzed for statistical differences between studies and later for variance. 55 Research Questions and Hypotheses Main Research Question To what extent does instrumental music education instruction in Grades 8–11 have an impact on critical thinking skills on the PSSA assessment (sections RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1)? HA: Instrumental music education has a positive impact on critical thinking skills. H0: Music education has no impact on critical thinking skills. Subquestions Research Question 1: How does the number of years (8th and 11th) that a student is involved in music education provide any statistical difference in the development of critical thinking skills as assessed by the PSSA (cumulative score per grade level)? • Analysis and comparison of the Instrumental and Noninstrumental sample is completed at 8th grade and then the same for 11th grade. HA1: There will be a positive correlation between those students who have participated in music education and critical thinking skills. The students’ critical thinking skills will increase with the number of years of participation. H01: There will be no change in the students’ critical thinking skills. Research Question 2: How do the Instrumental music students’ mean scores on sections (RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1) of the PSSA assessment compare to Noninstrumental music students from 8th to 11th grade? • Correlation coefficient will be utilized to examine the variance between grade levels (8th to 11th) for both samples. A comparison of the sum of means of from the individual subset of critical thinking scores (RB, MC1, MD2, ME1) 56 from the 8th- and 11th-grade sample populations. The means from all four subset scores will be added and then compared between samples. HA2: There will be a positive statistical difference between Instrumental and Noninstrumental music students at all three grade levels. H02: There will be no statistical difference between Instrumental and Noninstrumental music students. Research Question 3: Utilizing the means of the individual PSSA scoring (sections RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1), what is the relationship between the following: • Instrumental group scores versus Noninstrumental group scores from 8th–11th grades This will demonstrate change over a period of time in each of the subset test scores. HA3: There will be a positive statistical difference between grade level and Instrumental and Noninstrumental music group scores. H03: There will be no statistical difference between grade level and group scores. Data Analysis Main Research Question To what extent does instrumental music education instruction in Grades 8–11 have an impact on critical thinking skills on the PSSA assessment (sections RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1)? 57 Statistical Analysis: One-Way ANOVA Howell (2008) defined an ANOVA as a statistical technique for testing for differences between the means of several groups. Because of the utilization of only one independent variable, the one-way ANOVA would be used to analyze the statistical difference between the means of the groups. Subquestions Research Question 1: How does the number of years (8th and 11th) that a student is involved in music education provide any statistical difference in the development of critical thinking skills as assessed by the PSSA (cumulative score per grade level)? • Analysis and comparison of the Instrumental and Noninstrumental sample at 8th grade and then the same for 11th grade. Statistical Analysis: Independent-Samples t Test Independent-samples t test will be used to compare the sum of means from the individual subset scores (RB, MC1, MD2, ME1) from the 8th- and 11th-grade sample populations. The means from all four subset scores will be added and then compared between samples. Research Question 2: How do the Instrumental music students’ scores compare to Noninstrumental music students at all three grade levels? • Correlation coefficient will be utilized to examine the variance between grade levels (8th to 11th) for the samples. 58 Statistical Analysis: One-Way ANOVA Because of the utilization of only one independent variable, the one-way ANOVA would be used to analyze the statistical difference between the means of the groups (Howell, 2008). Research Question 3: Utilizing the PSSA scoring (sections RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1), what is the relationship between the following: • Instrumental group scores versus Noninstrumental group scores • Grade level and group scores o This will demonstrate change over a period of time in each of the subset test scores. Statistical Analysis: Repeated Measures ANOVA According to Howell (2008), a repeated measures design is where the participant receives all levels of the independent variables. The repeated-measures ANOVA would allow for the same subject to be followed throughout the study, which allows for a statistical analysis of their progress over a period of time. Expected Findings The main research question examined the relationship between instrumental music education instruction in Grades 8 and 11 and critical thinking skills on the PSSA assessment (sections RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1). This question led to the hypothesis that there is a correlation between critical thinking and instrumental music education. The findings of the present study should remain consistent with that of the reviewed literature in that there is a positive correlation between instrumental music and critical thinking. A 59 significant statistical difference is expected between the Noninstrumental and Instrumental sample in regards to the subsections of Reading and Mathematics. The first research subquestion examined if there would be correlation between the number of years that a student was involved in instrumental music when compared to those students who were not. The students who participated in an additional 3 years of instrumental music should demonstrate a significant statistical difference when compared to the sample that did not participate in instrumental music. The second subquestion examined the mean cumulative scores of the PSSA subsections to demonstrate a statistical difference between the two samples. Because of the literature that showed higher scores on standardized assessments, this study predicted that there would be a significant statistical difference between the two sample populations. The third researched subquestion analyzed the statistically variance between each of the four subsections. The ANOVA is expected to illustrate that a student who participates in instrumental music education will outperform a student who does not take instrumental music. 60 CHAPTER 4. DATA COLLECTION AND ANALYSIS Introduction This research study seeks to examine the relationship between instrumental music education and critical thinking skills in Grades 8 and 11 as assessed by the Pennsylvania System of School Assessment. The subsets that were examined included Reading (B): Interpretation and Analysis of Fictional and Nonfictional Text, which assesses the academic standards 1.1 Learning to read independently, 1.2 Reading critically in all content areas and 1.3 Reading, analyzing and interpreting literature (PADOE, 2009– 2010). In mathematics, sections C.1: Geometry—Analyze characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes, D.2 Algebraic concepts—Analyze mathematical situations using numbers, symbols, words, tables and/or graphs and E.1 Data analysis and probability— Interpret and analyze data by formulating answers or questions. Description of the Stratified Sample The researcher identified the potential sample based on the students’ participation in instrumental music in Grades 5–11 or their nonparticipation in music. The participant must have retained residency in the school district allowing them to take part in the PSSA assessment as well as to participate in the instrumental music program. Once this potential sample was determined, a representative from the school district selected the 61 sample and then mailed the consent forms to students who were over 18 years of age and permission forms to the parents of students who were under 18. The forms were completed and mailed back to a third party prohibiting the researcher from knowing who received forms. In addition this allowed the researcher, as well as the school, to remain neutral in the selection process. The sample forms were then returned to the researcher. The research examined a sample of (N = 100) students (50 Noninstrumental music students and 50 music students) from two graduated classes of a high school. There were 28 males and 22 females in each of the participant groups. By using multiple years, the power of the study would increase as well as the validity and integrity of the study. For a sample size of 50, a power analysis indicated that power was .9565 and the critical F = 4.11. The effect size was estimated at .5 with p = .05. Stratified sampling was utilized because this study examined three separate strata of the given student population. These three strata were as follows: 5th, 8th, and 11th grade. This sampling method allowed the researcher to examine the three grade levels, thus allowing for a direct comparison of the problem solving and critical skill development of these students. Since this is a longitudinal study, the same students’ development was examined over the three grade levels. The 5th-, 8th-, and 11th-grade reading and math mean raw scores for the given samples are indicated in Figure 1. Series 1 is representative of the Noninstrumental group and Series 2 is representative of the Instrumental group. As show in Figure 1, the Noninstrumental sample remains consistently below the Instrumental music sample in both the reading and math categories in all three grade levels. The math raw scores are higher than that of reading in both samples in all three measured grade levels. 62 Figure 1. Mean raw scores of both samples (5th, 8th, and 11th). Figure 2 represents the percentage of total correct scores for reading and Figure 3 represents percentage total correct scores for reading with both samples being measured at all three grade levels. When evaluating the percentage correct, both samples indicated higher reading scores than math in all three years with the Instrumental music sample consistently achieving a higher percentage correct. The highest scores for both samples were achieved in 8th grade with the Noninstrumental sample scoring a 79.89% correct in reading and 68.48% correct in math while the Instrumental sample scored an 86.85% in reading and 81.70% in math. Research Questions The main research question examined the relationship between instrumental music education instruction in Grades 8 and 11 and critical thinking skills on the PSSA assessment (sections RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1). 63 Figure 2. Percentage of correct reading answers (5th, 8th, and 11th). Figure 3. Percentage of correct math answers (8th and 11th). Interpretation of Descriptives The (NI) is representative of the Noninstrumental sample while the (I) is representative of the Instrumental sample. N = 50 in each group and is comprised of students from two graduated high school classes. 64 Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations (8th Grade) Subsection M SD R8.B Noninstrumental Instrumental 17.24 19.16 3.952 2.965 M8.C.1—Geometry Noninstrumental Instrumental 4.02 4.56 1.813 1.728 M8.D.2—Algebra Noninstrumental Instrumental 7.14 8.98 2.634 1.348 M8.E.1—Data Analysis Noninstrumental Instrumental 2.98 3.28 .845 .757 R8.B—8th-grade reading. The means of both the Noninstrumental (M = 17.24) and Instrumental sample (M = 19.16) indicated that not only is the score of the Instrumental sample higher but there is less of an SD = 2.965 when compared to the Noninstrumental sample (SD = 3.952). This is reinforced when the minimum and maximum scores are taken into account of both samples indicating that the scores for the Instrumental sample are more tightly grouped than that of the Noninstrumental sample. The tighter grouping of scores demonstrates that the Instrumental sample is scoring collectively higher than that of the Noninstrumental sample. M.8.C.1. 8th-grade mathematics C.1: geometry. The 8th-grade Mathematics C.1 descriptives indicate a difference between Noninstrumental and Instrumental means of .54 and the SD = 1.813, 1.728, respectively. This limited variance could be due to the small number of questions assessed. 65 M.8.D.2. 8th-grade mathematics D.2 Algebraic concepts. The differences between the mean of the Noninstrumental group (M = 7.14) and the Instrumental group (M = 8.98) and standard deviation indicates a closer scoring of the Instrumental sample than that of the Noninstrumental sample. M.8.E.1. 8th-grade mathematics E.1 Data analysis and probability. As indicated with the previous tests, the difference between the means of the Noninstrumental group and Instrumental group remains significant with the Noninstrumental sample scoring M = 2.98 and the Instrumental sample scoring M = 3.28. In Levene’s test of homogeneity of variances, the null hypothesis is that the variances between groups are relatively equal. Since, as indicated in Table 2, the sig. value is p > .05. Here, all of the variances are more than the significance, which is p <.05, the assumption of equal variances appears. Therefore, in the cases of R8.B, M8.C.1 and M8.E.1, equal variances can be assumed and in the case of M8.D.2 equal variances cannot be assumed. The subsets that were examined included Reading (B): Interpretation and Analysis of Fictional and Nonfictional Text, which assesses the academic standards 1.1 Learning to read independently, 1.2 Reading critically in all content areas and 1.3 Reading, analyzing and interpreting literature (PADOE, 2009–2010). In mathematics, those examined section were C.1: Geometry—Analyze characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes, D.2 Algebraic concepts—Analyze mathematical situations using numbers, symbols, words, tables and/or graphs and E.1 Data analysis and probability— Interpret and analyze data by formulating answers or questions. 66 Table 2. Test of Homogeneity of Variances (8th Grade) Subsection Levene statistic R8.B p 3.195 .077 M8.C.1 .018 .894 M8.D.2 20.083 .000 M8.E.1 .017 .895 One-Way ANOVA Results The statistical analysis that was to compare these sections was a one-way ANOVA. Howell (2008) defined an ANOVA as a statistical technique for testing for differences between the means of several groups. Because of the examination of only one independent variable, the one-way ANOVA was used to analyze the statistical difference between the means of the 8th-grade Instrumental and Noninstrumental groups and then the 11th-grade Instrumental and Noninstrumental groups. The data in Table 3 represent the results of the one-way ANOVA, which was used to examine the means between the two 8th-grade samples. Reading: R8.B: 8th grade. The ANOVA in Table 3 indicates the Mean Square Between (MSB) as 92.160 and the Mean Square Within (MSW) groups as 1195.840. When the MSB is divided by the MSW, the result is the F(1, 98) = 7.553, which has a p = .007. Because the F > 1, the null hypothesis can be rejected and the independent variable does have an effect on the dependent variable. The degrees of freedom (1) is determined 67 by the formula of N (total number of observations) – 1 and indicates the number of degrees between the two sources of variations (Howell, 2008). In the case of Reading (8th-grade section B), there is enough of difference between the two groups to indicate that the Instrumental group has outperformed the Noninstrumental group in 8th grade in terms of the 8th-grade reading assessment. Table 3. Between-Subjects ANOVA (8th Grade) Subsection df MS F p R8.B 1 92.160 7.553 .007 M8.C.1 1 7.290 2.325 .131 M8.D.2 1 84.640 19.335 .000 M8.E.1 1 2.250 3.497 .064 Mathematics: M.8.C.1. This section of geometry assesses the ability to analyze characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes. The ANOVA indicates the MSB as 7.290 and the MSW groups as 307.300. The result is the F(1, 98) = 2.325 p =.131. The F > 1; however, the null hypothesis cannot be rejected because p is > .05. Mathematics: M.8.D.2. The third test used the one-way ANOVA to examine the 8th-grade Mathematics section D.2 Algebraic concepts—Analyze mathematical situations using numbers, symbols, words, tables and/or graphs. The F(1,98) = 19.335, p = .000 indicating that there is a large difference in the scores between these two samples, 68 This indicates that there is a strong difference between these two groups in their ability to analyze mathematical situations. Mathematics: M.8.E.1. The last section examined as part of this test segment was the 8th-grade Mathematics section E.1 Data analysis and probability—Interpret and analyze data by formulating answers or questions. As indicated with the previous tests, the difference between the means of the Noninstrumental group and Instrumental group remains significant with the F(1, 98) = 3.497, p = .064. Even though the null hypothesis cannot be rejected in this subsection, the F statistic and significance warrants further investigation. The results of the one-way ANOVA indicated that the students in the Instrumental group outperformed those in the Noninstrumental group in both Reading B and M.D.2. However, subsections M.C.1 and M.E.1 have proven not to be statistically different. After 3 years of musical instruction from 5th to 8th grade, the differences in Reading B and Mathematics M.D.2 subsections that analyze critical thinking are readily apparent. Interpretation of Descriptives (11th Grade) Table 4 is representative of the 11th-grade sample, which is the same sample as used in the 8th-grade one-way ANOVA. R.11.B. The descriptives indicate a higher mean (M = –20.98) in the Instrumental sample than in the Noninstrumental sample (M = 17.30). In the Instrumental sample, the minimum score of 12 and maximum of 33 display higher values than that of the Noninstrumental sample, which is represented with a minimum score of 5 and a maximum of 28. This indicates that the both the lower scoring and higher scoring 69 students of the Instrumental sample are outscoring those students of the Noninstrumental sample. Table 4. Descriptives (11th Grade) Subsection M SD Min. Max. Noninstrumental Instrumental 17.30 20.98 5.956 5.427 5 12 28 33 M11.C.1 Noninstrumental Instrumental 4.84 5.90 1.910 1.644 1 2 9 9 M11.D.2 Noninstrumental Instrumental 8.76 11.04 2.952 2.321 3 5 14 14 M11.E.1 Noninstrumental Instrumental 1.36 1.70 .631 .505 0 0 2 2 R11.B M11.C.1. The second test examined was the 11th-grade mathematics section C.1. The means indicate a difference of 1.06 between the Noninstrumental and Instrumental sample. The maximum score in this test is that of 9 and the minimum that was scored was a 1. M11.D.2. The third section examined was the 11th-grade mathematics section D.2. The means display a significant difference of M = 8.76 for the Noninstrumental sample and M = 11.04 for the Instrumental sample. In this section, both samples indicated a maximum score of 14. 70 M.11.E.1. The last section examined was the 11th-grade mathematics section E.1. This section demonstrates a small variance between the means; however, there were only two questions in this section representing a minimum of zero and a maximum of two. The descriptives are very similar to that of the 8th grade in that the minimum scores of the Instrumental sample remained consistently higher than those of the Noninstrumental sample, with the maximum scores either remaining the same or higher in the Reading category for the Instrumental grouping. Table 5. Test of Homogeneity of Variances (11th Grade) Subsection Levene statistic p R11.B 0.517 0.474 M11.C.1 2.244 0.137 M11.D.2 3.958 0.049 M11.E.1 6.175 0.015 The Levene statistic indicates that neither R11.B nor M11.C.1 rejects the null hypothesis. In this case, the means did not differ enough in order to reject the null hypothesis. However, when the Brown–Forsythe and the Welch test (Table 6) was also run, the null hypothesis could rejected in all cases. The Brown–Forsythe test utilizes the median rather than the mean of the sample, and the differences between the two samples 71 can be compared, accounting for variances between the groups adding an additional measure of assurance for the rejection of the null hypothesis. Table 6. Robust Tests of Equality of Means (11th Grade) Subsection F p R11.B Welch Brown–Forsythe 10.429 10.429 .002 .002 M11.C.1 Welch Brown–Forsythe 8.845 8.845 .004 .004 M11.D.2 Welch Brown–Forsythe 18.430 18.430 .000 .000 M11.E.1 Welch Brown–Forsythe 8.845 8.845 .004 .004 Figure 4 lists the percentages of the subset questions that each sample answered correctly. The difference between the two samples, with the Instrumental sample remaining the highest, maintains an average of 9.8% in 8th grade and 14.05% in 11th grade. This indicates that not only did the Instrumental music sample outscore the Noninstrumental in both 8th and 11th grades, but they also increased that score by 11th grade. 72 Figure 4. Percentage of correct answers for both samples (5th, 8th, and 11th). One-Way ANOVA Results The data in Table 7 represent the results of the one-way ANOVA, which was utilized to test the means between the two 11th-grade samples when there is only one independent variable. Reading: R11.B: 11th grade. The ANOVA in Table 7 indicates the MSB = 338.560 and the MSW = 3181.480. When the MSB is divided by the MSW, the result is the F(1, 98) = 10.429, p = .002, which indicates a statistically significant difference between the two groups for the reading subsection. 73 Table 7. Between-Groups ANOVA (11th Grade) Subsection SS df MS F p 338.560 1 338.560 10.429 .002 M11.C.1 28.090 1 28.090 8.845 .004 M11.D.2 129.960 1 129.960 18.430 .000 M11.E.1 2.890 1 2.890 8.845 .004 R11.B Mathematics: M.11.C.1. This section of geometry assesses the ability to analyze characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes. The ANOVA indicates the MSB = 28.090 and the MSW = 311.220. The result is the F(1, 98) = 8.845, p = .004. This test also indicates a significant difference between the Instrumental and Noninstrumental sample, which demonstrates that the students who participated in Instrumental music strongly outperformed those students in the Noninstrumental group. This is significant, because this was one of the two subsets, whereas in 8th grade the null hypothesis could not be rejected and displayed a nonsignificant difference. Mathematics: M.11.D.2. The 11th-grade Mathematics section D.2 is used to assess algebraic concepts and analyze mathematical situations using numbers, symbols, words, tables and/or graphs. The F(1, 98) = 18.430, p = .000, indicating that there is a large difference in the scores between these two samples. Mathematics: M.11.E.1. The last section examined as part of this test segment was the 11th-grade Mathematics E.1 assess data analysis and probability by being able to 74 interpret and analyze data by formulating answers or questions. As indicated with the previous tests, the difference between the means of the Noninstrumental group and Instrumental group remains significant with the F(1, 98) = 8.845, p = .004. This rejects the null hypothesis that could not be done in the 8th-grade test making this an area of improvement for the 11th-grade Instrumental music students. Statistically, this means that the performance of the Instrumental group has increased from 8th grade to 11th grade validating that the students are experiencing the effects of the prolonged exposure to instrumental music education. The results of the one-way ANOVA indicate significant increases in the three subsections of Reading (RB), Mathematics (C.1) and Mathematics (E.1). In subsection Mathematics (D.2), there was a significant difference indicated between the two groups in both 8th grade and again in 11th grade. However, the F(1, 98) = 19.335 for 8th grade and F(1, 98) = 18.430 for 11th grade, which indicates a slight decrease in the F value. The relationship between instrumental music education and critical thinking skills as assessed by the PSSA subsections (RB, MC1, MD2 and ME1) was shown to be significant in R8B and M8D2 and then again in all four subsections in 11th grade. Of note is that in 3 out of the 4 subsections analyzed, the Instrumental music sample displayed significant increases over that of the Noninstrumental music sample when comparing 8th to 11th grade. Research Question 1 How does the number of years (8th and 11th) that a student is involved in music education provide any statistical difference in the development of critical thinking skills as assessed by the PSSA (cumulative score per grade level)? 75 The means of all four subset scores for 8th grade have been combined in Table 8 and the result indicates that the totals for the Noninstrumental group (M = 31.38) and the Instrumental (M = 35.98) demonstrate a variance of 4.6. Table 8. Group Means and Standard Deviations (8th Grade; N = 50) Group M SD Noninstrumental 31.38 7.453 Instrumental 35.98 4.922 As represented in Table 9, the Levene’s test for equality of variances (p = .005) demonstrates that the two samples do differ significantly from each other. Therefore, equal variances cannot be assumed, and the groups, while equal in sample size are statistically different from one another. Table 9. Independent-Samples t Test for Means of Samples (8th Grade) Levene’s test Equal variances not assumed F p 8.080 .005 95% confidence interval t test t df p MD SE –3.642 84.909 .000 –4.600 1.263 76 Lower Upper –7.111 –2.089 An independent-samples t test (Table 10) was used to compare the sum of means of the individual subset scores (RB, MC1, MD2, ME1) for the 8th- and 11th-grade sample populations. The means from all four subset scores were added and then compared between samples. Table 10. Means and Standard Deviations (11th Grade; N = 50) Group M SD Noninstrumental 32.26 9.998 Instrumental 39.62 8.408 An independent-samples t test was used statistically to assess whether or not two groups are different from each by the comparison of means. In this scenario, the means of both the Instrumental and Noninstrumental group are compared. The t statistic, as listed in Table 9, when equal variances are not assumed, is t(84.90) = –3.642, with the twotailed test at p = .000 and the mean difference of –4.600. The t statistic is calculated by taking the sample Noninstrumental mean (M = 31.38) minus the sample Instrumental mean (M = 35.98) divided by the standard error difference (1.263). The result of t(84.90) = –3.642 determines that the difference between the sample mean and the hypothesized mean is statistically significant. In this scenario, both the lower and upper confidence levels indicate the same values. The confidence interval level between two means in this independent-samples t test is based on the 95% level, but it can also be used to represent 77 other values such as 90 or 99%. The lower level limit is a (CI = –7.111) and the upper level limit is (CI = –2.089), which indicates a relatively small value range. The small confidence level is indicative of the relatively small difference between the means of the sample. In this test, the samples provide a point of comparison to the 11th-grade means t test, therefore allowing for the differences between the samples to be analyzed and providing data for group comparison. The independent-samples t test was used again to analyze the total of the means of the subset scores of the 11th-grade students. As indicated in Table 10, the Noninstrumental group M = 32.26 and the Instrumental group M = 39.62, with the variance of 7.36 between the two samples. In Table 11 the Levene’s test for the equality of variances indicated a nonsignificance of p = .166, which is > .05. The null is not rejected, meaning that equal variances can be assumed, and therefore, assumption has been met, which could not be done in the 8th-grade sample. Table 11. Independent-Samples t Test for Means of Samples (11th Grade) Levene’s test Equal variances assumed 95% confidence interval t test F p t df p MD SE 1.948 .166 –3.984 98 .000 –7.360 1.847 78 Lower Upper –11.026 –3.694 The confidence level lower is (CI = –11.027) and the upper level is (CI = –.3693), which is larger than that of the 8th-grade sample. The results indicated that t(98) = – 3.984, p = .000, which means that the two samples are statistically different. The Instrumental music sample indicates a higher mean than that of the Noninstrumental music sample, with the difference increasing from 4.6 in 8th grade to 7.36 in 11th grade. Therefore, in terms of the research question, the students who were exposed to an additional 3 years of instrumental music education showed an increase of cumulative scores on the PSSA assessment in 11th grade. Research Question 2 How do the Instrumental music students’ mean scores on sections (RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1) of the PSSA assessment compare to Noninstrumental music students from 8th to 11th grade? The correlation coefficient was utilized to examine the variance between 8th and 11th-grade levels for both samples. Because of the utilization of only one independent variable, the repeated-measures ANOVA was used to analyze the statistical difference between the means of the groups (Howell, 2008). In Table 12, the cumulative subset score means (RB, MC1, MD2 and ME1) of Noninstrumental and Instrumental groups are displayed in the descriptive statistics, indicating the variance between the two groups as well as the increase in variance from 8th grade (4.6) and 11th grade (7.36). In Figure 5, both samples that estimated marginal means are represented in 8th grade and then again in 11th grade, displaying the increase for the Instrumental music sample. This result indicates that the three additional years of 79 instrumental music education related to the increasing mean cumulative score on the analyzed subsections of the PSSA assessment. Table 12. Means and Standard Deviations for Combined Samples Grade Group M SD Noninstrumental Instrumental 31.38 35.98 7.453 4.922 Combined 11 Noninstrumental 32.26 9.998 39.62 8.408 Combined 8 Instrumental Figure 5. Means of both samples (8th to 11th grade). 80 Sphericity refers to the equality of variances between different levels in a repeated-measures ANOVA. In Table 13, the measurements such as the Greenhouse– Geisser, Huynh–Feldt and lower bound assist in the corrections of the sphericity measurement (Howell, 2008). Since the significance level on all four tests is > .05, the null hypothesis can be rejected. Therefore, sphericity remains intact, and assumption is met. Table 13. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Tests of Within-Subjects Effects Source Groups df MS F p np2 Sphericity assumed 1 255.380 15.369 .000 .136 Greenhouse–Geisser 1 255.380 15.369 .000 .136 Huynh–Feldt 1 255.380 15.369 .000 .136 98 16.616 Error (Groups) Sphericity assumed In Table 14, the tests of within-subjects contrasts display an F(1, 98) = 5.731, p = .019 and a np2 = .055, displaying that there is a strong difference between the two sample means. In Table 15, the combined 8th-grade score indicated a p = .005, whereas equal variances cannot be assumed. However, in the combined 11th-grade score, equal variances can be assumed and the assumption has been met. 81 Table 14. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Tests of WithinSubjects Contrasts Source Groups 1 Error (Groups) F p np 2 5.731 .019 .136 df 98 (16.616) Table 15. Levene’s Test of Equality of Error Variances (Combined) Grade F df p Combined 8 8.080 1 .005 Combined 11 1.948 1 .166 In Table 16, the tests of between-subjects effects indicated the F(1, 98) = 16.466, p = .000. Therefore, there was a significant different between the two groups, which indicates a significant statistical difference between the two samples. The difference is further displayed in Figures 6 and 7, where the grouping of the sample for the Instrumental music indicates both higher scores and higher frequencies for the combined scores in 8th and 11th grades when the Instrumental sample is compared to the Noninstrumental sample. 82 Table 16. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Between Subjects (Group Cumulative Means) df Group 1 Error 98 a F p np2 Observed powera 16.466 .000 .144 .980 (108.589) Computed using alpha = .05 Repeated-measures ANOVA. The results of this repeated-measures ANOVA (Table 16) indicate that the Instrumental sample has showed statistical increases over that of the Noninstrumental sample when the means of sections (RB, MC1, MD2, and ME1) are added and compared. The results of this test indicate that the students who participated in instrumental music education are likely to have increased test scores on the PSSA when compared to students who did not participate in instrumental music education. Research Question 3 Utilizing the means of the individual PSSA scoring (sections RB, C.1, D.2, and E.1), what is the relationship between the following: • Instrumental group scores versus Noninstrumental group scores from 8th–11th grades The skewness of the Instrumental sample, as represented in Table 17, displays a shift from .396 in 8th grade to –.026 in 11th grade. This indicates a shift in the scores of the sample size when the mean and median remain the same. The Noninstrumental 83 sample indicates a skewness in the 8th grade of .110 and 11th grade .167, which is indicative of the distribution shifting to the right of being asymmetrical. The negative kurtosis, which indicates a smaller peak around the mean, decreases in both samples. However, the decrease is greater in that of the Instrumental sample from –.446 to –1.187 when compared to that of the Noninstrumental sample –.757 to –1.109. These descriptives indicate a shift in the paradigm from 8th to 11th grade in displaying higher overall results in the Instrumental group scores. Table 17. Skewness and Kurtosis of 8th and 11th Grades (N = 50) Group Skewness Kurtosis Noninstrumental 8th 11th 0.11 0.167 –0.757 –1.109 Instrumental 8th 11th 0.396 –0.026 –0.446 –1.187 The descriptive statistics in Table 18 indicate that the Noninstrumental sample has a M = 17.24 for Reading B (8th grade) and the Instrumental sample has a M = 19.16. For 11th grade, Noninstrumental sample displays a score of M = 17.30 and the Instrumental sample M = 20.98. Levene’s test is a test for assumption of equality of variances. This indicates that the p = .077 in 8th grade and then p = .474 in 11th grade indicating that the null hypothesis cannot be rejected. Therefore, variances are equal and assumption is met. 84 Table 18. Means and Standard Deviations (Reading) Group M SD RB8 Noninstrumental Instrumental 17.24 19.16 3.952 2.965 RB11 Noninstrumental 17.30 5.956 Instrumental 20.98 5.427 Table 19. Levene’s Test of Equality of Error Variances (Reading) Group F p RB8 3.195 .077 RB11 .517 .474 Interpretation of the repeated-measures ANOVA (Table 20) for the Reading B assessment indicates that there is a significant difference between the two variances from 8th to 11th grade from the Noninstrumental sample to the Instrumental sample. The tests of between-subjects effects indicates F(1, 98) = 10.885, p = .001 and the np2 = .100. This information is further supported in Table 21 of the Reading B means, which displays the difference between the two sample groups. The percentage of variance from 8th–11th grade for the Noninstrumental sample is (–13.89) and for the Instrumental sample is (–10.11). While the mean has increased for both samples, this is a direct result 85 of the increase in the number of questions assessed from a total of 26 (8th grade) to 33 (11th grade). The Instrumental sample decreased less from 73.69% to 63.58%, in percentage correct from 8th grade to 11th grade, than the Noninstrumental sample from 66.31 to 52.42. Table 20. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Tests of Between-Subjects Reading df Group 1 Error 98 a F p np2 Observed powera 10.885 .001 .100 .904 (36.012) Computed using alpha = .05 Table 21. Means, Percentage Correct, and Maximum Score (Reading) Reading M % correct Max. score Noninstrumental 8th grade 11th grade 17.24 17.3 66.31 52.42 26 33 Instrumental 8th grade 11th grade 19.16 20.98 73.69 63.58 26 33 The second repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted on Math section C.1. The descriptive statistics, in Table 22, indicate an increased mean for both groups from 8th 86 grade to 11th grade, with the Instrumental sample displaying a larger increase of 1.06 compared to that of the Noninstrumental sample of .54. This is a statistically strong indicator that the Instrumental sample out performed the Noninstrumental sample. Table 22. Means and Standard Deviations (M.C.1) Group M SD Noninstrumental Instrumental 4.02 4.56 1.813 1.728 MC.1.11 Noninstrumental 4.84 1.910 5.90 1.644 MC.1.8 Instrumental Levene’s test of equality, Table 23, does not reject the null hypothesis in that the p > .05, meaning that equal variances can be assumed However, it does indicate a significant difference between the two testing years from p = .894 (8th grade) to p = .137 (11th grade). Levene’s test utilizes the mean and because of the limited number of questions, the difference between the standard deviations are limited and have not met assumption. However, the statistical differences noted are still worthy of examining due to the differences in significance between the 8th- and 11th-grade samples. Because of the small number of questions in this category, the percentage of questions correct (Table 24) becomes an area of clarity in that the difference between the samples demonstrates a decrease in the Noninstrumental sample of 3.65% to an increase in the Instrumental sample of .42%. While the Noninstrumental sample decreased, the 87 Instrumental sample increased in percentage of questions correct. In Table 25, the tests of between-subjects effects indicates F(1, 98) = 6.627, p =.012 and np2 = .063 with an observed power of .722. These results indicate that there is a limited statistically significant difference between the two samples. Table 23. Levene’s Test of Equality of Error Variances (M.C.1) Group F P MC.1.8 .018 .894 MC.1.11 2.244 .137 Table 24. Means and Percentage Correct (M.C.1) M.C.1 M % correct Noninstrumental 8th grade 11th grade 4.02 4.84 57.43 53.78 Instrumental 8th grade 11th grade 4.56 5.9 65.14 65.56 Max. score 88 Table 25. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Test of Between-Subjects M.C.1 df Group 1 Error 98 a F p np2 Observed powera 6.627 .012 .063 .722 (4.829) Computed using alpha = .05 The third section examined was the Mathematics section D.2. The descriptive statistics in Table 26 indicate that the standard deviation increased in both samples. However, this increase is most evident in the Instrumental sample when comparing the 8th grade (1.348) to the 11th grade (2.321). The standard deviation indicates that the relative number correct in each sample is now statistically closer grouped when comparing 8th- to 11th-grade samples. This could indicate that the difference between the groups has narrowed between the two samples. Table 26. Means and Standard Deviations (M.D.2) Group M SD Noninstrumental Instrumental 7.14 8.98 2.634 1.348 MD.2.11 Noninstrumental 8.76 2.952 11.04 2.321 MD.2.8 Instrumental 89 The tests of within-subjects contrasts, in Table 27, indicate the F(1, 98) = .940, p = .335, showing no statistically significant difference within the two samples. There is a statistical difference between the means of the two samples; however, there is not a significant difference between the variance of the two samples. Table 27. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Tests of Within-Subjects Contrasts (M.D.2) df Group 1 Error 98 a F p np2 Observed powera .940 .335 .010 .160 (2.574) Computed using alpha = .05 The tests of between-subjects effects, in Table 28, indicate F(1, 98) = 23.963, p = .000, np2 = .196, which demonstrates that there is a strong difference between the two groups. Table 28. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Tests of Between-Subjects M.D.2 df Group 1 Error 98 a F p np2 Observed powera 23.963 .000 .196 .998 (8.854) Computed using alpha = .05 90 As indicated in Table 29, the numerical difference between the two samples increased when comparing 8th to 11th grade, which is primarily because of the increased maximum score from 11 to 14, in 8th and 11th grade, respectively. This demonstrates that the even though the Instrumental sample outperformed the Noninstrumental sample in this category, there was no statistical difference between the samples. Table 29. Means, Percentage Correct, and Maximum Score (M.D.2) M.D.2 Noninstrumental 8th grade 11th grade Instrumental 8th grade 11th grade % correct Max. score 7.14 8.76 64.91 62.57 11 14 8.98 11.04 81.64 78.86 11 14 M The Mathematics section E.1 (Table 30) deals with data analysis and probability, and in the last section, that was examined using the repeated-measures ANOVA. The standard deviation, as listed in Table 30, is of interest in this test because of the variance between both samples. The Noninstrumental sample demonstrates a decrease from SD = .845 to SD = .631, which is a difference of .214. The Instrumental sample demonstrates a decrease from SD = .757 to SD = .505, which is a difference of .250. Because of the small number of questions in each section 8th and 11th, 4 and 2, respectively, this is a 91 significant difference, indicating that there is a strong increase in the Instrumental sample from 8th grade to 11th grade. Table 30. Means and Standard Deviations (M.E.1) Group M SD Noninstrumental Instrumental 2.98 3.28 .845 .757 ME.1.11 Noninstrumental 1.36 .631 1.70 .505 ME.1.8 Instrumental In Table 31, Levene’s test of equality of error variances indicates that equal variances can be assumed for 8th grade; however, it cannot for 11th grade. This is also reflected by the smaller standard deviation in the 8th-grade sample when compared to the 11th grade. Therefore, for 8th grade the assumptions are met, but they are not met for 11th grade. This could be a result of the small number of questions that were asked as part of the assessment. In Table 32, the test between subjects has an F(1, 98) = 9.819, p = .002, np2 = .091 indicating a statistically significant difference between the variances of the two groups. The differences in the percentage correct between the two samples indicate a decrease in the Noninstrumental sample from 74.5% to 68%, which is a decrease of 6.5% and the Instrumental sample indicates an increase from 82% to 85%, which is an increase of 3%. This indicates that as the Noninstrumental music sample decreased from M8.E.1 to 92 M11.E1, the Instrumental music sample increased its average score, indicating a numerical shift in the sample populations. Table 31. Levene’s Test of Equality of Error Variances (M.E.1) Group F P ME.1.8 .017 .895 ME.1.11 6.175 .015 Table 32. Repeated-Measures ANOVA Tests of Between-Subjects M.E.1 df Group 1 Error 98 a F p np2 Observed powera 9.819 .002 .091 .873 (.521) Computed using alpha = .05 The last research question set out to examine the relationship between the 8th – to 11th-grade Noninstrumental and Instrumental sample. All of the repeated-measures ANOVA indicated that the Instrumental sample displayed statistically significant differences in all four areas, with strongest subsections being M.D.2 and Reading B. 93 Conclusion The main research question examined the relationship between instrumental music education instruction in Grades 8–11 and critical thinking skills on the PSSA assessment (sections RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1). The subsets that were examined included Reading (B): Interpretation and Analysis of Fictional and Nonfictional Text, which assesses the academic standards 1.1 Learning to read independently, 1.2 Reading critically in all content areas and 1.3 Reading, analyzing and interpreting literature (PADOE, 2009–2010). In mathematics, sections C.1: Geometry—Analyze characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes, D.2 Algebraic concepts—Analyze mathematical situations using numbers, symbols, words, tables and/or graphs and E.1 Data analysis and probability—Interpret and analyze data by formulating answers or questions. While the Noninstrumental sample scores were always lower than that of the Instrumental sample, the results of the one-way ANOVA indicated significant differences in the 8th-grade subsections of Reading B and M.D.2. The null hypothesis could not be rejected in M8.C.1 as well as M8.E.1. However, in 11th grade, the results of the one-way ANOVA rejected the null hypothesis in all four subsections and a significant increases were noted in subsections MC.1 in 8th grade F(1, 98) = 2.325 to 11th grade F(1, 98) = 8.845 and M.E.1 in 8th grade F(1, 98) = 3.497 to 11th grade F(1, 98) = 8.845. Research Question 1 How do the number of years (8th and 11th) that a student is involved in music education provide any statistical difference in the development of critical thinking skills as assessed by the PSSA (cumulative score per grade level)? The independent-samples t test indicated a significant difference between samples and between grade levels. In 8th 94 grade, the Noninstrumental sample had a combined M = 31.38 and the Instrumental sample had a combined M = 35.98, which is a difference of 4.6. In 11th grade, the Noninstrumental sample had a combined score of M = 32.26 and the Instrumental sample had a combined score of M = 39.62, which is a difference of 7.36. This increase indicates that not only did the Instrumental sample out score those in the Noninstrumental sample, there scores increased over that 3-year period. Research Question 2 How do the Instrumental music students’ mean scores on sections (RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1) of the PSSA assessment compare to Noninstrumental music students’ mean scores from 8th to 11th grade? The Noninstrumental sample demonstrated an increase from 31.38 in 8th grade to 32.26 in 11th grade, which is an increase of .88. The Instrumental sample demonstrated an increase from 35.98 in 8th grade to 39.62 in 11th grade, which is an increase of 3.64. The results of the repeated-measures ANOVA enforced the analysis of the descriptive statistics in that the tests of between-subjects effects the F(1, 98) = 16.466, p = .000, with an observed power of .980 with p = .05. The Instrumental sample increased its score at four times the rate of the Noninstrumental sample indicating a significant ability to comprehend the material being assessed and this ability increased from 8th grade to 11th grade. Research Question 3 Utilizing the means of the individual PSSA scoring (sections RB, C.1, D.2, and E.1), what is the relationship between the following: • Instrumental group scores versus Noninstrumental group scores from 8th–11th grades 95 The subsets that were examined included Reading (B): Interpretation and Analysis of Fictional and Nonfictional Text, which assesses the academic standards 1.1 Learning to read independently, 1.2 Reading critically in all content areas and 1.3 Reading, analyzing and interpreting literature (PADOE, 2009–2010). In mathematics, it assesses sections C.1: Geometry—Analyze characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes, D.2 Algebraic concepts—Analyze mathematical situations using numbers, symbols, words, tables and/or graphs and E.1 Data analysis and probability—Interpret and analyze data by formulating answers or questions. The repeated-measures ANOVA indicated that variances in the subsections RB, MC.1 and ME.1 were significant with the last two demonstrating increases in both MC.1 and ME.1. There was a variance in percentage of M.D.2 in the Noninstrumental sample of 2.78 compared to the Instrumental sample at 2.34. Even though the Instrumental sample percentage and mean scores were higher, the variance was not significant between the two samples. Therefore, the Instrumental music students demonstrated increases in Reading (RB) and Math scores (MC.1 and ME.1), utilizing the characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes as well and interpreting and analyzing data when compared to the Noninstrumental sample over the course of 8th to 11th grade. The Instrumental music sample consistently outscored the Noninstrumental music sample when comparing the Reading B, Mathematics M.C.1. and M.E.1 subsections of the PSSA assessment. The only section that did not display a significant statistical difference was M.D.2, which assesses the students’ ability to analyze mathematical situations using numbers, symbols, words, tables, and/or figures. 96 CHAPTER 5. RESULTS, CONCLUSIONS, AND RECOMMENDATIONS Introduction This study sought to identify whether or not there is a relationship between critical thinking skills and instrumental music education as measured by the PSSA in Grades 8–11. The research examined the results of specific subsections of the reading and math PSSA assessment, which were selected based on their incorporation of critical thinking assessments. The subsets that were examined included Reading (B): Interpretation and Analysis of Fictional and Nonfictional Text, which assesses the academic standards 1.1 Learning to read independently, 1.2 Reading critically in all content areas and 1.3 Reading, analyzing and interpreting literature (PADOE, 2009– 2010). It also included Mathematics sections C.1: Geometry—Analyze characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes, D.2 Algebraic concepts—Analyze mathematical situations using numbers, symbols, words, tables and/or graphs and E.1 Data analysis and probability—Interpret and analyze data by formulating answers or questions. The goal of this study was to explore the possible relationship between instrumental music education and critical thinking as assessed by the PSSA. In instrumental music, the student is the center of an active learning paradigm, meaning that the student is expected to actively participate and engage critical thinking sills. In this particular school system, the Instrumental student sample had begun instrumental lessons 97 in the 5th grade with participation continuing through 11th grade. The sample was assessed in 8th grade and again in 11th grade. Summary of the Results The main research question examined the relationship between instrumental music education instruction in Grades 8 and 11 and critical thinking skills on the PSSA assessment (sections RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1)? While the Noninstrumental sample scores were always lower than that of Instrumental sample, significant statistical differences were noted in the 8th-grade subsections of Reading B and M.D.2. Reading (B): Interpretation and Analysis of Fictional and Nonfictional Text, which assesses the academic standards 1.1 Learning to read independently, 1.2 Reading critically in all content areas and 1.3 Reading, analyzing and interpreting literature and Mathematics D.2., and the Algebraic concepts—Analyze mathematical situations using numbers, symbols, words, tables and/or graphs. The one-way ANOVA, 8th grade, subsections Reading B. and M.D.2 indicated a significant difference between the Instrumental sample and Noninstrumental sample. In 11th grade, the remaining two subsections that had not shown a significant statistical difference in 8th grade now displayed significant differences in 11th grade, and these were Mathematics C.1: Geometry—Analyze characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes and Mathematics E.1 Data analysis and probability—Interpret and analyze data by formulating answers or questions. This increase in scoring between the Noninstrumental and Instrumental sample, from 8th to 11th grade, correlates with the research at Whitworth University. Strauch (2009) conducted a study that indicates college freshmen, who have taken band in high 98 school, not only have higher GPAs while coming in to college, but they maintain those higher averages throughout their college career. Strauch examined the 537 students of the 2007–2008 incoming freshman class at Whitworth University, of which 103 (19.2%) had played in band through high school. The students who participated in band, at the high school level, not only had a higher GPA coming into college but also higher SAT scores on both the verbal and math portions of the test (Olson, 2009). The results of the research question posed by this study indicated that not only were students able to maintain their 8th-grade scores in 11th grade, but they were able to outperform their classmates in each of the four subsections. Schellenberg (2006) examined 6- to 11-year-old children who each varied in amount of musical training. The baseline IQ was established by administering the WISC– III as well as other areas of intellectual functioning such as grades in school and standardized tests of academic achievement. The sample was comprised of N = 147 (72 boys and 75 girls) ages 6–11 recruited from a middle class suburb of Toronto, Canada. The predictor variables were measured using a questionnaire that was administered to parents about their child’s history with private music lessons. The criterion variables consisted of measures of intelligence, which was assessed using the WISC–III, academic ability, which was assessed using the K–TEA, and social adjustment, which was measured using the Parent Rating Scale of BASC. The principal analyses consisted of correlations between the main predictor variable and criterion variables, which demonstrated that music lessons were positively correlated with both academic achievement and IQ, but not social adjustment. The results indicated that the duration of music lessons had a small but positive correlation to measures of intelligence as 99 measured by academic achievement (Schellenberg, 2006). This current study reinforces the finding that music can affect intelligence over a period of time, which is directly associative to the ability of instrumental music education to impact test scores that quantitatively measure the depth of knowledge (DOK) in specific content areas. In this study, students were exposed to 3 years of instrumental music education, which includes both group lessons as well as large group rehearsals. The differences between the two samples in the Reading B, Mathematics Sections M.C.1 (Geometry) and M.D.2 (Algebraic Concepts) are of particular interest in this research question, because they displayed increased differences between the two samples. The Mathematics sections both deal with symbols. The possibility exists that because music is an art based in symbols, the students have simply become practiced at their use. Additionally, students are experiencing increased development in their ability to process information that is conceptually abstract; therefore, the possibility exists that music can assist with that formation of the ability to process abstract information. In addition, C.1 analyzes the ability of students to interpret differences between two and three dimensional shapes that could be related to the active learning process and the compare and contrast methods that are utilized in instrumental music rehearsals. Research Question 1 How does the number of years (8th and 11th) that a student is involved in music education provide any statistical difference in the development of critical thinking skills as assessed by the PSSA (cumulative score per grade level)? The comparison of the sum of means of the subset scores indicated a difference between samples and between grade levels. In 8th grade, the difference between the means of the Noninstrumental and the 100 Instrumental sample was 4.6. In 11th grade, the difference between means of The Noninstrumental sample and The Instrumental sample was significant indicating that the students who were involved in music education for three additional years outscored their Noninstrumental music counterparts, which is an indicator that the students in the Instrumental sample are increasing their cumulative scores at a higher rate than that of the Noninstrumental sample. The theory that the arts can influence other academic areas was examined by Moga et al. (2000), who performed a meta-analysis of the relationship between academic achievement and arts education. Moga et al. reviewed 188 reports and found three areas where causal links proved to be reliable, one of which was based on 19 reports, demonstrating that there is a large causal relationship between learning to play music and spatial reasoning. This effect has greater applicability in educational scenarios, because the effect was reported equally among both general and at risk populations. It was shown that 69 out of every 100 students who participated in instrumental music between the ages of 3 and 12 displayed an increase in spatial reasoning skills (Moga et al., 2000). These findings are related directly to this current study’s findings, whereas the students’ scores showed an increase in spatial reasoning skills in the subsection M.C.1— Geometry (two- and three-dimensional shapes). Therefore, after an additional 3 years of exposure to music education, from 8th to 11th grade, students’ scores indicated rapid gains in the M.C.1. subsection. Research Question 2 How do the Instrumental music students’ mean scores on sections (RB, C.1, D.2 and E.1) of the PSSA compare to Noninstrumental music students from 8th to 11th grade? The Noninstrumental sample demonstrated an increase in 8th grade of .88, and the 101 Instrumental sample demonstrated an increase in 11th grade of 3.64. The combined means of these subset scores allows for a concise look at the differences between the two groups. At this juncture in the research, it is evident that the scores have increased; however, the variance and amount of increase in differences is the keystone to this study. The results of the repeated measures ANOVA indicated that the Instrumental music sample scores increased at four times the rate of the Noninstrumental music sample from 8th to 11th grade. Schellenberg (2006) examined the effects of long term music lessons on intellectual abilities and, more specifically, if these had lasting effects even after the music lessons had ended. The participants of this study were undergraduates at a suburban Canadian university with the range in age being 16–25. More than half had been taking private music lessons (N = 84) for an average of 7.8 years. The students were surveyed based on a questionnaire where the students were paid to participate in the 2hour survey. The criterion variables in this study consisted of intelligence and academic achievement, which was measured using the WAIS–III. An additional subtest, object assemble was administered to measure spatial–temporal ability (Schellenberg, 2006). The results indicated that taking music lessons regularly was correlated positively with IQ, especially in the areas of perceptual organization and working memory. Therefore, if instrumental music lessons can be correlated positively with perceptual organization and working memory, students might see an increase in scores of the equivalent Mathematics and Reading portions of the subsets of the PSSA examination because of the use of working memory. 102 The Instrumental Music students, at this point in time, could be benefiting from their original higher PSSA, self-esteem, and direct instruction. Johnson and Memmott (2006) noted that students in both high quality and deficient music programs had higher standardized test scores than those that participated in no instrumental music programs, although students in high quality music programs outscored those in deficient music programs. Research Question 3 Utilizing the means of the individual PSSA scoring (sections RB, C.1, D.2, and E.1), what is the relationship between the following: The variances in the subsections RB, MC.1, M.D.2 and ME.1 were significant with M.D.2 Algebraic Concepts and the Reading B Section showing the greatest increase in score variance. The students’ increases in standardized test subsections were congruent with those of Shropshire (2007), who examined the students’ PSAT scores for differences between students who participated in music, athletics, and both programs. The results showed that the students who were involved in music outperformed those who participated in athletics with no appreciable difference for those who participated in both and those who participated in music only. This score variance between the Noninstrumental and Instrumental samples could be as a result of several environmental, social, and even developmental influences, which are limitations of this current study. Research that has accounted for these factors through regression models still indicates that IQ has the strongest influence on standardized test scores. Babo (2004) analyzed the relationship between a students’ participation in music education and their academic performance. He set controls of IQ, SES, and gender. The 103 study indicated that there was a strong relationship between students’ achievements in the language arts and their participation in instrumental music education. While the regression models clearly indicated that IQ has the strongest influence on test scores, instrumental music education also contributed highly to the overall variance of the mathematics total score. Further analysis, when controlling for gender and socioeconomic status, demonstrated that instrumental music influenced both language arts and mathematical scores (Babo, 2004). The study by Babo indicated, just as this research, that instrumental music has an impact on both language and mathematics. By examining the developmental process of critical thinking, this longitudinal, quantitative, correlational study sought to examine the relationship between instrumental music education and critical thinking as an adaptive and fluid learning dynamic. The importance of this study relays that instrumental music can be related to the cognitive process in a positive and contributory method demonstrating that active learning is an effective method to develop critical thinking skills. Discussion of the Results The construct of what this study measured can assist in explaining that instrumental music asserts itself in developing the person through its active learning style and through ability to allow people progress through the learning process. The PSSA Technical Report states that critical thinking and problem solving is a process and not content. Therefore, the assumption can be made that this process is applicable in specific testing areas. These areas are identified by the technical report as open-ended questions, which are rated by their DOK. 104 The subsections of the test were then selected based on their level of DOK as well as their identification as areas that would require critical thinking or problem solving as a process through open-ended or multiple choice questions. This research validates other research that is already in existence by demonstrating that instrumental music can relates to a person’s cognitive development over a period of time. These results were those expected by the researcher given the experience and background of the researcher. Limitations Because the sample was only collected from one school, the ability to generalize results over a larger population may be limited due to the ability of the sample to be influenced and affected by the limitations of population. These influences are, but not limited to, the ability of an individual teacher to have an impact on the cognitive processes of the student in either positive or negative outcomes. Those students choosing music may already excel in mathematics and reading; therefore, this gives them an advantage to implement critical thinking skills. Socioeconomic status may have a direct impact on those students who choose to study an instrument. Therefore, the sample population limitation may be inherent to the sample. This population limitation could be directly related to the cost of an instrument, because a specific socioeconomic group may not be able to afford the purchase of an instrument. Fitzpatrick (2006) noted that even Instrumental music students with low SES outperformed higher SES Noninstrumental music students in all subject areas by 9th grade as assessed by the Ohio Proficiency Test. However, in this study, there was no account given to SES factors of either sample because the cost of either purchase or rental of an instrument may limit those that choose 105 to participate in instrumental music. Students who have an interest in instrumental music may already have a stronger sense of problem solving than those who do not want to get involved in music. The PSSA is a statewide assessment that is continually monitored for external and internal validity; however, students place their own importance on these exams. Those students who work to achieve and do so through intrinsic motivation would outscore those students who are not motivated. With the assumption being that sample size would have negated the issue, this study did not attempt to control, the influence of motivation on students’ test scores. The possibility remains that students who seek music might also be beneficiaries of higher levels of self-esteem and self-efficacy, which are developed through performance opportunities, leading toward a greater internal drive than those students who have not. In addition, the individual subsection information was not available for 5th grade. However, the overall scores for Math and Reading were available. The difference in the baseline scores of the 5th grade sample may be attributed to other variables such as IQ and SES, which were not accounted for in the study. To account for this in future studies, the addition years of score availability might prove itself to be quite valuable in order to establish levels of variance as well given several more years to the longitudinal nature of this study. The ability to establish a baseline score in 5th grade would allow a researcher to track progress over 6 years with data being collected in 5th, 8th, and 11th grade. This additional dataset could prove to be a valuable tool for later analysis both in terms of generic PSSA categories of math and reading and the specific subsets that were measured in this study. 106 Recommendations for Further Research There are several factors that may be considered for future study in order to include or exclude critical thinking as a true correlational variable in this research. The students because of their increased musical training might have developed stronger math and reading skills across the board and this study has simply focused on particular subsections rather than the larger picture of academic achievement. The subsets were chosen based on their ability to measure the critical thinking process as indicated by the DOK rating assigned by the PSSA in their technical analysis as well as the focus on both analysis and open-ended questions. Additional grade levels. Examining the 5th-grade scores in relationship to the 8th grade and then the 11th grade would assist in developing a pattern of cognitive development as well as mitigating the influence of an individual teacher. Sample. The study should be expanded to other school districts in order to eliminate environmental factors such as individual teachers, socioeconomic status, and race. This would give additional credence to the construct that instrumental music has influenced these outcomes rather than other influential factors. Including additional schools would be beneficial to broaden the ability of the research to be quantified across instrumental music without being limited to one particular regional area. Gender would also have been interesting to include in the results, which in turn would have required a larger sample size. The reason of interest in this case would have been to demonstrate whether or not critical thinking skills develop differently over the course of time in regards to gender. 107 Conclusion The theory that music affects people is by far not new. However, efforts to study its effects on cognitive development is in the advent stage as analysis of long term cognitive developmental differences. This study sought to find a correlation between critical thinking and instrumental music education. The results showed that the students in the Instrumental music sample outperformed a randomized sample of their peers on subsections Reading B and Mathematics C.1, D.2 and E.1. These subsections of the PSSA assessment were chosen based on their existence in both 8th and 11th grades, the commitment to use open-ended questions and their strength on their DOK rating. The results from these subsections may not be unique. However, the study revealed that this body of students who studied instrumental music in Grades 5–11 developed cognitive growth to outscore their classmates on the same assessments and at a faster pace with increased indicators in M.C.1: Geometry—Analyze characteristics of two and three dimensional shapes and M.E.1: Data analysis and probability—Interpret and analyze data by formulating answers or questions. Students are influenced through their participation in music to outperform their Noninstrumental music peers. The analysis provided gives credence to school systems and music programs in their support of instrumental music education in that not only these students may achieve at a higher level than their Noninstrumental music counterparts. Prior research has demonstrated that music can be related to increased scores in language arts and mathematics, and this current study serves as an extension of that work by examining the relationship between instrumental music education and critical thinking skills by using PSSA subsections in both reading and mathematics. At a time when school district and states struggle to balance budgets, this 108 information could prove to be a critical and deciding factor in the overall importance of music education to a student’s success. 109 REFERENCES American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical principles of psychologists & code of conduct. Retrieved from http://www.psychology.iastate.edu/faculty/dvogel /Ethics.htm American Suzuki Center. (n.d.). Bird’s eye view of the Suzuki talent education method. Retrieved from http://www.uwsp.edu/cofac/suzuki/SuzukiMethod.htm Anderson, L. W., & Krathwohl, D. R. (2001). A taxonomy for learning, teaching, and assessing: A revision of Bloom’s taxonomy of educational objectives. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Arasi, M. (2006). Adult reflections on a high school choral music program: Perceptions of meaning and lifelong influence. Doctoral dissertation, Georgia State University, Atlanta. Babo, G. D. (2004). The relationship between instrumental music participation and standardized assessment achievement of middle school students. Research Studies in Education, 22(1), 14–27. Breakwell, G. M., Hammond, S., Fife-Schaw, C., & Smith, J. (Eds.). (2006). Research methods in psychology (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Cooney, W., Cross, C., & Trunk, B. (1993). From Plato to Piaget: The greatest educational theorists from across the centuries and around the world. Lanham, MD: University Press of America. Costa-Giomi, E. (2004). Effects of three years of piano instruction on children’s academic achievement, school performance and self-esteem. Psychology of Music, 32(2), 139–152. Creswell, J. (2007). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five traditions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Derry, S. J., & Murphy, D. A. (1986). Designing systems that train learning ability: From theory to practice. Review of Educational Research, 56, 1–39. Eisner, E. (1998). Does experience in the arts boost academic achievement? Art Education, 51(1), 7–15. 110 Fitzpatrick, K. (2006). The effect of instrumental music participation and socioeconomic status on Ohio fourth-, sixth-, and ninth-grade proficiency test performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 54(1), 73–84. Gagne, R. M. (1968). Learning hierarchies. Educational Psychologist, 6, 3–6. Gagne, R. M. (1977). The conditions of learning and theory of instruction. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Gagne, R. M. (1980). Learnable aspects of problem solving. Educational Psychologist, 15, 84–92. Global Development Research Center. (2008). The problem solving process. Retrieved from http://www.gdrc.org/decision/problem-solve.html Guignon, A. (2004). Multiple intelligences: A theory for everyone. Education World. Retrieved from http://www.educationworld.com/a_curr/curr054.shtml Hake, R. (1998). Interactive engagement versus traditional methods: A six-thousand student survey of mechanics test data for introductory physic courses. American Journal of Physics, 66(1), 64–74. Halpern, D. F. (1996). Thought and knowledge: An introduction to critical thinking. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Halpern, D. F. (1997). Critical thinking across the curriculum: A brief edition of thought and knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains: Dispositions, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 53(4), 449–455. Hamblen, K. A. (1997). Theories and research that support art instruction for instrumental outcomes. Arts Education Policy Review, 98(3), 27. Howell, D. C. (2008). Fundamental statistics for the behavioral sciences (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth. Hubball, H., Gold, N., Mighty, J., & Britnell, J. (2007, Winter). Supporting the implementation of externally generated learning outcomes and learning-centered curriculum development: An integrated framework. New Directions for Teaching and Learning, 112, 93–105. 111 Johnson, C., & Memmott, J. (2006). Examination of relationships between participation in school music programs of differing quality and standardized test results. Journal of Research in Music Education, 54(4), 293–307. Leedy, P. D., & Ormrod, J. E. (2005). Practical research: Planning and design. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Lisk, E. (1996). The creative director: Intangibles of music performance. Galesville, MD: Meredith. Lisk, E. (2006). The creative director: Conductor, teacher, leader. Galesville, MD: Meredith. Mawhinney, H., Frusciante, A., Aaron, P., & Liu, Y. (2002). Design principles for learning-centered schools: Building effective strategies for addressing the achievement gap. Retrieved from http://www.education.umd.edu/EDPA/papers /mawhinney.pdf McManus, D. (2001). The two paradigms of education and the peer review of teaching. NAGT Journal of Geoscience Education, 49(6), 423–434. Moga, E., Burger, K., Hetland, L., & Winner, E. (2000). Does studying the arts engender creative thinking? Evidence for near but not far transfer. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34(3–4), 91–104. Moog, H. (1984). Specific aspects of musical learning: A contribution towards a learning theory of music. Psychology of Music, 12(2), 106–118. Olson, C. (2009). Music and academic success go together at Whitworth. Teaching Music, 16(6), 20. Pennsylvania Department of Education. (2005). Pennsylvania system of school assessment. Retrieved from http://www.pde.state.p.us/a_and_t/lib/a_and_t/2005 -06MathematicsAssessmentHandbook.pdf Pennsylvania Department of Education. (2009–2010). Pennsylvania system of school assessment. Retrieved from http://www.portal.state.pa.us/portal/server.pt/gateway /PTARGS_0_123031_890907_0_0_18/PSSA_Assessment_Handbook_2009 -2010.pdf Prince, M. (2004). Does active learning work? A review of the research. Journal of Engineering Education, 93(3), 223–231. Rabkin, N., & Redmond, R. (2006). The arts make a difference. Educational Leadership, 63(5), 60–64. 112 Reimer, B. (2002). A philosophy of music education: Advancing the vision (3rd ed.). Boston, MA: Prentice Hall. Saarikallio, S., & Erkkila, J. (2007). The role of music in adolescents’ mood regulation. Psychology of Music, 35(1), 88–109. Schellenberg, E. G. (2006). The long-term positive associations between music lessons and IQ. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 457–468. Shropshire, W. (2007). Differences in student academic achievement between students who participate in music programs and students who participate in athletic programs. Retrieved from Dissertations & Theses @ Capella University. (Publication No. AAT 3291461) Skinner, B. F. (2005). Science and human behavior. Cambridge, MA: B. F. Skinner Foundation. Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Cognitive psychology (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson. Strauch, R. (2009). Music and academic success go together at Whitworth. Teaching Music, 16(6), 20. Teachout, D. J. (2007). Understanding the ties that bind and the possibilities for change. Arts Education Policy Review, 108(6), 19. Turner, M. (1999). Child-centered learning and music programs. Music Educators Journal, 86(1), 30–33, 51. Van Patten, J., Chao, C. I., & Reigeluth, C. M. (1986). A review of strategies for sequencing and synthesizing instruction. Review of Educational Research, 56(4), 437–471. Vaughn, K. (2000). Music and mathematics: Modest support for the oft-claimed relationship. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 34, 149–166. 113