The Journal of Experimental Education ISSN: 0022-0973 (Print) 1940-0683 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vjxe20 School Belonging, Ethnic Identity, and Psychological Adjustment Among Ethnic Minority College Students Praveena Gummadam, Laura D. Pittman & Micah Ioffe To cite this article: Praveena Gummadam, Laura D. Pittman & Micah Ioffe (2016) School Belonging, Ethnic Identity, and Psychological Adjustment Among Ethnic Minority College Students, The Journal of Experimental Education, 84:2, 289-306, DOI: 10.1080/00220973.2015.1048844 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00220973.2015.1048844 Published online: 02 Sep 2015. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 4332 View related articles View Crossmark data Citing articles: 27 View citing articles Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=vjxe20 THE JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL EDUCATION, 84(2), 289–306, 2016 C Taylor & Francis Group, LLC Copyright ISSN: 0022-0973 print/1940-0683 online DOI: 10.1080/00220973.2015.1048844 MOTIVATIONS AND SOCIAL PROCESSES School Belonging, Ethnic Identity, and Psychological Adjustment Among Ethnic Minority College Students Praveena Gummadam, Laura D. Pittman, and Micah Ioffe Northern Illinois University This study considers how the psychological adjustment of ethnic minority college students may be linked to a sense of school belonging and ethnic identity, two constructs related to individuals feeling like they belong to a larger group. Using self-reports from 311 undergraduates from ethnic minority backgrounds, school belonging was found to be negatively associated with depressive symptoms, and positively associated with perceived self-worth, scholastic competence, and social acceptance, while ethnic identity was only found to be positively associated with self-worth. Furthermore, the interaction between ethnic identity and school belonging was significantly associated with self-worth, suggesting that in the absence of a sense of belonging at school, stronger ethnic identity was linked to higher self-worth. College students reported the lowest levels of self-worth when they were neither connected to their college nor connected to their ethnic group. Keywords school belonging, ethnic identity, college students, self-worth, self-concept, depressive symptoms, ethnic groups OVER THE LAST few decades, college campuses have become more diverse, with 36% of college students being from minority backgrounds in 2010 (Synder & Dillow, 2013). This growth in ethnic diversity among undergraduate students is expected to continue in the decade ahead (Hussar & Bailey, 2011). Yet students from minority backgrounds struggle in college, reporting worse psychological adjustment than Caucasian students (Smith, Chesin, & Jeglic, 2014). Multiple theories point to the importance of individuals feeling like they belong to a larger group in order to enhance their psychological well-being (e.g., sociometer theory: Leary & Baumeister, 2000). Among college students from minority backgrounds, two factors related to belonging to a group, a sense of school belonging and ethnic identity, have been linked to better psychological adjustment (e.g., Phinney & Alipuria, 1990; Pittman & Richmond, 2007); however, to date, these constructs Address correspondence to Dr. Laura D. Pittman, Department of Psychology, Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL 60115, USA. E-mail: lpittman@niu.edu 290 GUMMADAM, PITTMAN, AND IOFFE have not been considered as independent variables simultaneously. This study examined how these two constructs are associated, individually and collectively, to ethnic minority college students’ psychological adjustment. School Belonging Growing evidence suggests that a sense of school belonging may be an important factor contributing to the psychological adjustment of college students. While likely linked to other constructs related to the school environment (e.g., teacher support, school affiliation; for a discussion of this issue see Barber & Schluterman, 2008), school belonging goes beyond these constructs. Goodenow (1993) defined school belonging as “the extent to which students feel personally accepted, respected, and included by others in the school environment” (p. 80). This sense of belonging is more than specific relationships with individuals in the school; it assesses the broader sense of feeling connected to the larger school community. Much of the research examining school belonging has focused on its links to educational outcomes, such as school engagement, academic motivation, and academic achievement (e.g., Cham, Hughes, West, & Im, 2014; Eccles & Roeser, 2003). Yet the positive associations between school belonging and psychological adjustment have also been well documented (e.g., Resnick et al. 1997). Specifically, a stronger sense of school belonging has been associated with lower levels of depression (e.g., Anderman, 2002; Shochet & Smith, 2014), increased self-esteem (e.g., Galliher, Rostosky, & Hughes, 2004), and increased academic self-efficacy (e.g., Roeser, Midgley, & Urdan, 1996). Although the majority of the research on school belonging has been conducted with middle school and high school students, some studies have considered a “sense of belonging” at the college level (e.g., Hausmann, Schofield, & Woods, 2007). Like research with younger ages, most of this research has considered either factors predicting a sense of belonging at college (e.g., Hurtado & Carter, 1997; Wilson & Gore, 2013) or the links between sense of college belonging and academic-related outcomes such as grades, academic engagement, persistence, or retention (e.g., Faircloth & Hamm, 2005; Hausmann et al., 2007; Zumbrunn, McKim, Buhs, & Hawley, 2014). However, a smaller literature has considered the links between a sense of school belonging at college and psychological outcomes (e.g., Freeman, Anderman, & Jenson, 2007; Pittman & Richmond, 2007). For example, among college students, a sense of school belonging, both at the classroom and at the broader school level, has been found to be positively associated with aspects of the self, including academic self-efficacy, scholastic competence, social acceptance, and global self-worth (Freeman et al., 2007; Pittman & Richmond, 2007, 2008; Zumbrunn et al., 2014). Similarly, among graduate students, a sense of connection with their department was associated with academic self-concept (Curtin, Stewart, & Ostrove, 2012). School belonging among college students has also been linked with lower levels of depression and loneliness (Hagerty, Williams, Coyne, & Early, 1996; Mounts, 2004; Pittman & Richmond, 2007, 2008). Further, a sense of belonging in college was found to mediate the association between social class and academic and social adjustment in college, such that lower social class predicted lower levels of school belonging, which in turn predicted lower academic and social adjustment (Ostrove & Long, 2007). Therefore, school belonging seems to be relevant in predicting the psychological adjustment of college students, and may be important to consider when looking specifically at ethnic minority college students. SCHOOL BELONGING AND ETHNIC IDENTITY 291 Considerable research has examined how the sense of school belonging is linked to academic outcomes and retention among minority college students (e.g., Hurtado & Carter, 1997). In fact, interventions aimed at enhancing African American college students’ sense of school belonging have been associated with higher grade point averages (GPAs) over time (Shook & Clay, 2012; Walton & Cohen, 2011); however, these interventions did not influence Caucasian college students’ GPAs. While one of these interventions also found prospective links to improved physical health and self-reported happiness (Walton & Cohen), few have considered how school belonging is associated with psychological adjustment among ethnic minority college students. For adolescents in middle or high school, school belonging has been negatively associated with lower levels of depressive symptoms among Hispanic Americans, gifted African Americans and Hispanic Americans, and refugees from Somalia (Kia-Keating & Ellis, 2007; Maurizi, Caballo, Epstein-Ngo, & Cortina, 2013; Mueller & Haines, 2012). In addition, school belonging has been positively linked to perceived academic competence among African American, Asian American, and Hispanic American high school students (Faircloth & Hamm, 2005). Interestingly, Mounts (2004) found no mean level differences in school belonging across four different ethnic groups in a college sample; however, school belonging was found to be a stronger mediator between campus racial climate and psychological adjustment (i.e., symptoms of depression and anxiety) for African American as compared to Caucasian college students. In summary, while evidence suggests a sense of school belonging is an important influence on psychological outcomes during middle school and high school, more research is needed to examine these associations during college. Ethnic Identity Ethnic identity is another factor that has been linked to psychological adjustment among ethnic minority adolescents and college students (e.g., Phinney & Alpuria, 1990; Umaña-Taylor et al., 2014). Phinney (1996) defines ethnic identity as “an enduring, fundamental aspect of the self that includes a sense of membership in an ethnic group and the attitudes and feelings associated with that membership” (p. 222). Specifically, those with a stronger identity have been found to have a clearer commitment and sense of belonging to their group (Roberts et al., 1999). Ethnic identity is a dynamic and complex construct involving exploration and commitment that develops over time (Phinney, 1989) and is a part of a person’s inner feelings, daily experiences, and interpersonal interactions (Gaylord-Harden, Ragsdale, Mandera, Richards, & Petersen, 2007). During adolescence, identity formation is a central developmental task (Erikson, 1968), and the development of one’s ethnic identity is considered particularly important among ethnic minority youth (Phinney, 1989). Multiple theories posit that adolescents begin to formulate their ethnic or racial identity by exploring and learning about their own group (e.g., Cross, 1994; Helms, 1990). Recent work has found that ethnic identity continues to develop during emerging adulthood, with most of this research focused on college students (e.g., Umaña-Taylor et al., 2014). Previous research has found that ethnic minority college students who have a stronger ethnic identity have more positive self-esteem, more self-confidence, and better overall psychological adjustment (Phinney & Alipuria, 1990; St. Louis & Liem, 2005). Conversely, those who have a less well-defined ethnic identity report more symptoms of anxiety and depression (Brittian, Umaña-Taylor, Lee et al., 2013; Yip, Seaton, & Sellers, 2006). Findings have been consistent across studies focused on specific ethnic groups, including Hispanic Americans (e.g., Cronin, 292 GUMMADAM, PITTMAN, AND IOFFE Levin, Branscombe, van Laar, & Tropp, 2012), Asian Americans (e.g., Lee, Yun, Yoo, & Nelson, 2010; Tran & Lee, 2010), and African Americans (e.g., Lorenzo-Hernández & Ouellette, 1998). In fact, a meta-analysis of 184 studies revealed a moderate association between ethnic identity and the well-being of ethnic minority individuals (r = .17), although stronger effect sizes were found with measures of positive well-being (i.e., self-worth) as compared to maladaptive functioning (i.e., depressive symptoms; Smith & Silva, 2011). Furthermore, the association appeared to be stronger among individuals under 40 years of age, and no significant differences emerged based on race, gender, or socioeconomic status (Smith & Silva, 2011). Thus, it appears that individuals from a variety of ethnic minority groups in the United States benefit from having a stronger ethnic identity. School Belonging and Ethnic Identity School belonging and ethnic identity have been linked to both positive and negative psychological outcomes in adolescent and college student samples. Yet only one study was identified where both constructs were considered. Specifically, Brown and Chu (2012) reported a positive association between ethnic identity and school belonging among Mexican immigrant children in elementary school, but in this study school belonging was considered a dependent, not independent, variable. We argue that both of these constructs include the underlying concept of belonging to a larger group, which has been discussed by a number of theories as important for individuals’ well-being. According to the belongingness hypothesis (Baumeister & Leary, 1995), humans have a need for belonging and social attachments, and when people feel that they belong, they feel more positive about themselves. While Baumeister and Leary (1995) focused on specific relationships, they acknowledged that the sense of belonging can also develop based on involvement with a larger group (e.g., a political movement). Similarly, sociometer theory (Leary & Baumeister, 2000) suggests that individuals are motivated to enhance feelings of inclusion and decrease feelings of rejection from groups in order to maintain their self-esteem. Therefore, those who feel excluded or do not have a sense of belonging to a group are likely to experience lower levels of self-esteem and more negative affect. As stated earlier, individuals with a stronger ethnic identity have a clearer commitment and stronger sense of belonging to their ethnic group (Phinney & Ong, 2007; Roberts et al., 1999), while those with a stronger sense of school belonging feel more connected to their school (Goodenow, 1993). Accordingly, ethnic minority college students can experience a sense of belonging to a larger group through at least two ways: feeling connected to their college or feeling connected to their ethnic group. Consideration of how these two factors function both independently and collectively provides new information in understanding college students’ psychological adjustment. It may be that those with a stronger sense of belonging to both these groups have particularly enhanced psychological adjustment. However, based on both the belongingness hypothesis (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) and sociometer theory (Leary & Baumeister, 2000), a lack of connection to either their college or their ethnic group likely leads to lower self-worth and negative psychological outcomes. Consequently, considering the combined influence of these constructs on ethnic minority college students’ psychological adjustment is warranted. SCHOOL BELONGING AND ETHNIC IDENTITY 293 Current Study This study examined how school belonging and ethnic identity, both individually and collectively, were associated with psychological adjustment among ethnic minority college students. While both of these constructs have been operationalized as being related to belonging to a larger group (Goodenow, 1993; Phinney & Ong, 2007), they have not been considered as independent variables simultaneously. Walton and Cohen (2011) suggested that members of minority groups may be especially prone to feeling unsure about belonging to mainstream institutions like colleges, making it particular relevant to study. In addition, the ethnic identity of ethnic minority college students may be more salient on college campuses, where students are often experiencing more diversity than in their high schools (Locks, Hurtado, Bowman, & Oseguera, 2008). Theoretically, this more diverse context may make ethnic identity more important than when students are living among others with a similar ethnic background (Brittian, Umaña-Taylor, & Derlan, 2013). Hence, there may be two pathways that enhance the psychological adjustment of ethnic minority college students, and exploring how they function together is needed. Given the call to include both positive and negative outcomes when considering minority populations (e.g., Garcı́a Coll et al., 1996), psychological adjustment was conceptualized as including the presence of more positive perceptions of the self and fewer depressive symptoms. Since college students focus on both academic and social pursuits (e.g., Arnett, 2004), aspects of the self-system considered in this study were scholastic competence (i.e., how individuals perceive themselves in the academic domain) and social acceptance (i.e., how individuals perceive themselves in the social domain). In addition, the study assessed students’ perceptions of selfworth, which is thought to be influential in how individuals interact with their environment (e.g., Harter, 2012). Finally, self-reported depressive symptoms were assessed, as previous research has frequently used lower levels of depressive symptoms as an indicator of psychological adjustment (e.g., Brody, Yu, Chen, Miller, Kogan, & Beach, 2013; Chen, Benet-Martinez, Wu, Lam, & Bond, 2012). Considering these four constructs, this study adds to our understanding of how both school belonging and ethnic identity may be important to the psychological adjustment of ethnic minority college students. METHOD Participants and Procedure In total, 322 college students completed questionnaires during either the fall or spring semester at a large, public Midwestern university, receiving credit for their introductory psychology course. Eleven international students were excluded from the sample, leading to a final sample size of 311 (56% female; age M = 19.33 years; SD = 2.25 years). On average participants had been enrolled at this university for 1.75 semesters (SD = 1.62), with 55% being freshmen. Most students (59%) reported their family as being financially comfortable or having more than enough money, yet others reported their families as only having enough money for basic necessities (27%), living under meager conditions (12%), or experiencing extreme financial hardships (2%). Participants were recruited through an online listing of all possible studies available to students enrolled in Introduction to Psychology. On this listing, participants were informed that only those 294 GUMMADAM, PITTMAN, AND IOFFE “who identify as being an ethnic minority” were eligible. The final sample included 154 African Americans, 47 Asian Americans, 64 Hispanic Americans, and 46 who identified as a part of another ethnic minority (e.g., mixed heritage, Serbian, or Polish). The majority of the sample (55%) identified themselves as third-generation Americans, with 37% and 8% identifying as first- and second-generation Americans, respectively. It should be noted that approximately 30% of the undergraduate population at this university were ethnic minorities. Participants completed paper-and-pencil questionnaires in small-group sessions that lasted approximately 45 minutes. They were informed that the purpose of the study was to understand factors related to psychological adjustment among ethnic minority college students. All procedures were approved by the university’s internal review board. After reviewing and signing the consent form, participants filled out demographic information and specific questionnaires described in the following. Measures Demographic Information Questions were asked to gather information regarding participant gender, ethnicity, age, and generational status in the United States, as well as family socioeconomic status and parental education level. To gather information on ethnicity, participants were asked to complete the following open-ended item based on Phinney and Ong (2007): “In terms of ethnic group, I .” In addition, a closed-ended item asked them to select which consider myself to be one of seven ethnic groups best fit their ethnicity, where one group was “mixed” and one group was “other.” Responses between the open-ended and closed-ended items were compared and were found to be consistent. Participants were classified as first generation if they were the only person in their family born in the United States or if the individual and his or her parents and grandparents were born in another country. Participants were classified as second generation if at least one parent or grandparent and the individual were born in the United States. Individuals were classified as third generation if at least one grandparent, one parent, and the individual were born in the United States. Since many students are not aware of their family’s income, they were asked to report their family’s standard of living using the following categories: 1 = has extreme financial hardships/is living in poverty; 2 = is living under meager conditions; 3 = has enough money for the basics; 4 = is comfortable; and 5 = has more than enough money. School Belonging The 18-item revised Psychological Sense of School Membership (PSSM; Goodenow, 1993) measure was used to assess school belonging among participants. The PSSM was originally devised to assess school belonging in high school students, but has been adapted to be used with college students related to the university setting (Pittman & Richmond, 2007). For example, a question was changed to “most professors at this university are interested in me” instead of the original question, which was “most teachers at this school are interested in me.” It uses a 5-point rating scale format with responses ranging from not at all true (1) to completely true (5). Both the original and the college student revision of the PSSM have been found to have adequate internal consistency and were linked to individual and school characteristics in expected ways (Goodenow, SCHOOL BELONGING AND ETHNIC IDENTITY 295 1993; Pittman & Richmond, 2007). In this sample, the PSSM had good internal consistency (α = .89 for total sample; α = .87–.89 across ethnic groups). While recent research suggests that school belonging as measured by the PSSM is a multidimensional construct, across studies there is not consistency in the factor structure supported (e.g., Ye & Wallace, 2014; You, Ritchey, Furlong, Shochet, & Boman, 2011). Thus, like most research using the PSSM (e.g., Cham et al., 2014; Shochet & Smith, 2014), this study continued to conceptualize school belonging as a single dimension in our analyses. Ethnic Identity The Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure-Revised (MEIM-R; Phinney & Ong, 2007) was used to measure the strength of ethnic identity among participants. The MEIM-R is a self-report scale that consists of six items. There are an additional two items that are used to obtain demographic information regarding ethnic group, for a total of eight items. This measure is a revision of the original Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (Phinney, 1992) and is designed to better reflect the two core factors of exploration and commitment believed to be integral to ethnic identity (Phinney & Ong, 2007). Items are answered using a 5-point response format with choices ranging from strongly agree (1) to strongly disagree (5). When research focuses on the overall strength of ethnic identity, as was done in this study, Phinney and Ong (2007) suggest using the single overall score, which is calculated by taking the mean of the items. Higher scores indicate a stronger ethnic identity. The internal consistency of the MEIM-R has been shown to be acceptable in previous research (Phinney & Ong, 2007) and in this sample (α = .88 for total sample; α = .84–.92 across ethnic groups). Self-Competence and Self-Worth Three subscales from the Self-Perception Profile for College Students (Neemann & Harter, 1986) were used to measure three separate aspects of self-competence among participants. Specifically, the four-item social acceptance and scholastic competence subscales and the six-item global self-worth subscale were used. For each item participants were asked which type of student they are most like (e.g., “Some students are not satisfied with their social skills BUT other students think their social skills are just fine”). Once participants chose which type of student they are most like, they were asked to choose a response option of either “Sort of true for me” or “Really true for me.” This question format was used by the authors in order to reduce the tendency of respondents to give socially desirable answers. Each subscale has half of the items negatively worded. Composite scores were calculated by taking the average of all items from each subscale, after reversing appropriate items. Higher scores indicate a greater sense of competence and self-worth (Neemann & Harter, 1986). The internal consistencies of these scales were acceptable in this sample for each subscale: social acceptance (α = .84 for total sample; α = .81–.91 across ethnic groups), scholastic competence (α = .76 for total sample; α = .73–.82 across ethnic groups), and global self-worth (α = .84 for total sample; α = .81–.88 across ethnic groups). 296 GUMMADAM, PITTMAN, AND IOFFE TABLE 1 Descriptive Statistics of Measures for Total Sample and Ethnic Subgroup (N = 311) Measure Total (N = 311) Means (SD) Asian (n = 47) Mean (SD) Hispanic (n = 64) Mean (SD) African American (n = 154) Means (SD) Other (n = 46) Means (SD) F Ratio Family standard of living Generational status School belonging Ethnic identity Depression Global self-worth Scholastic competence Social acceptance 3.46(0.83) 2.19(0.94) 3.71(0.59) 3.90(0.79) 15.91(8.93) 3.24(0.61) 2.86(0.67) 3.12(0.78) 3.82(0.67) 1.11(0.43) 3.64(0.56) 3.69(0.87) 17.30(8.72) 3.00(0.63) 2.66(0.67) 2.98(0.84) 3.48(0.80) 1.48(0.76) 3.84(0.57) 3.82(0.87) 14.16(9.41) 3.30(0.60) 2.98(0.64) 3.14(0.72) 3.29(0.88) 2.86(0.50) 3.66(0.62) 4.04(0.69) 16.68(9.07) 3.32(0.58) 2.90(0.68) 3.12(0.81) 3.59(0.75) 2.04(0.89) 3.75(0.55) 3.77(0.81) 14.39(7.61) 3.15(0.64) 2.76(0.61) 3.22(0.65) 5.61∗∗ 134.40∗∗∗ 1.58 3.43∗ 2.04 3.96∗∗ 2.69∗ 0.76 ∗p < .05, ∗∗ p < .01. Depressive Symptoms The 20-item Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) was used to assess depressive symptoms. Participants indicate how often they have experienced, in the past week, each of 20 symptoms, using a 4-point rating scale format with choices ranging from 0 and 3 (i.e., 0 = rarely or none of the time, less than 1 day to 3 = most or all of the time, 5–7 days). The measure is scored by adding all of the responses, after reversing positive items. The range of the overall score is between 0 and 60, with higher scores indicating a greater presence of depressive symptoms (Radloff, 1991). The CES-D has been found to have good internal consistency in previous research with a college population (Radloff, 1991) and in this sample (α = .86 for the total sample; α = .82–.88 across ethnic groups). The validity of the CESD has been supported through expected positive correlations with other measures of depressive symptoms (Radloff, 1977; Skorikov & Vandervoort, 2003), as well as the ability to discriminate between a psychiatric inpatient sample and a nonclinical sample (Radloff, 1977). RESULTS Preliminary Analyses Descriptive statistics of the independent and dependent variables for the total sample and each ethnic group are presented on Table 1. One-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted to determine if there were significant differences in the variables by ethnicity. No significant differences were found between ethnic groups on school belonging, depressive symptoms, scholastic competence, or social acceptance. However, there were significant differences between ethnic groups in ethnic identity, global self-worth, family standard of living, and generation status. Post hoc Bonferroni comparisons indicated that African Americans, on average, reported a stronger ethnic identity and a lower family standard of living than Asian Americans. In addition, Asian Americans had significantly lower global self-worth than either African Americans or Hispanic Americans. Finally, the generational status of participants was significantly different for each SCHOOL BELONGING AND ETHNIC IDENTITY 297 TABLE 2 Bivariate Correlations of Variables (N = 311) School belonging Ethnic identity Depressive symptoms Global self-worth Scholastic competence Social acceptance ∗p School belonging Ethnic identity Depressive symptoms Global self-worth Scholastic competence Social acceptance 1.00 .19∗∗∗ −.52∗∗∗ .43∗∗∗ .35∗∗∗ .39∗∗∗ 1.00 −.11 .21∗∗∗ .18∗∗ .08 1.00 −.49∗∗∗ −.39∗∗∗ −.35∗∗∗ 1.00 .56∗∗∗ .53∗∗∗ 1.00 .22∗∗∗ 1.00 < .05, ∗∗ p < .01, ∗∗∗ p < .001. pairwise comparison between ethnic groups, where African Americans had the highest generational status (i.e., their families had been in this country the longest), followed by those who reported being from an ethnic group categorized as “other,” followed by Hispanic Americans and then Asian Americans. Preliminary analyses were run to determine whether there were associations between demographic and dependent variables. Significant correlations were found between dependent variables and both students’ family standard of living and generational status. Specifically, the family standard of living was significantly correlated with depressive symptoms and scholastic competence (r = -.26, p < .001, and r = .17, p = .002, respectively)1. In addition, generational status was positively correlated with global self-worth (r = .12, p = .05). However, age, semester at the university, and year in college (i.e., freshman, sophomore, etc.) were not significantly associated with any of the dependent variables. In addition, based on independent-samples t-tests, using a two-tailed test of significance, no gender differences were found on any of the dependent variables. Based on these analyses, the following variables were added as controls in the remaining analyses: family standard of living, generational status, and ethnicity. Ethnicity was dummy coded, with the African American ethnic group serving as the comparison group because it had the greatest number of participants (n = 154). Correlations among the dependent variables were examined to determine whether they were measuring distinct constructs (see Table 2). The three self-competence scales were positively correlated with one another, as would be expected given that they are subscales of the same measure. However, these correlations were only moderate, suggesting that they are measuring related but distinct constructs. Additionally, as expected, depressive symptoms were negatively correlated with the measures of self-competence. School belonging and ethnic identity were positively correlated, but at a relatively low level. School belonging was also significantly associated with all the dependent variables, such that better outcomes were found when students reported a 1Analyses were run to determine if dichotomizing the family standard of living variable into low-income versus not would alter the findings. While this variable was found to similarly predict depressive symptoms and scholastic competence, when used in the regressions as a control variable it accounted for less of the variance than the 5-point scale. Based on these analyses, the 5-point scale was utilized in these analyses as it was the more conservative approach to testing the associations of interest. 298 GUMMADAM, PITTMAN, AND IOFFE greater sense of school belonging. However, ethnic identity was only positively correlated with global self-worth and scholastic competence. Hierarchical Regressions Hierarchical ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions were run predicting each of the four dependent variables (i.e., depressive symptoms, scholastic competence, global self-worth, and social acceptance). The demographic control variables of ethnicity, family standard of living, and generational status were entered into the first step of each regression. In Step 2, school belonging and ethnic identity were entered into the equation simultaneously. In Step 3, the interaction term between school belonging and ethnic identity, which was created by multiplying the two centered variables together, was entered. Significant interaction terms were probed using methods suggested by Aiken and West (1991). As shown in Table 3, Step 1 of the regression found a similar pattern of findings related to demographic variables. That is, a better family standard of living was associated with better psychological adjustment across all four dependent variables. In addition, generational status was positively associated with social acceptance. Finally, in comparison to African Americans, Asian Americans reported lower levels of self-worth and scholastic competence. In addition, students from other ethnicities reported lower levels of scholastic competence compared to African American students. In Step 2, school belonging was strongly associated with all four adjustment outcomes explored, including a negative association with depressive symptoms and positive associations with global self-worth, scholastic competence, and social acceptance. In comparison, having a stronger ethnic identity was positively associated with only global self-worth. This suggests that, compared to ethnic identity, a sense of school belonging is more strongly linked to adjustment among ethnic minority college students. In Step 3, a significant interaction was found between school belonging and ethnic identity when predicting global self-worth, but not the other dependent variables. Specifically, as shown in Figure 1, when school belonging was stronger, ethnic identity was not significantly associated with self-worth (slope = 0.01, t(309) = 0.13, p > .05), but when school belonging was lower, ethnic identity was positively associated with greater global self-worth (slope = 0.18, t(309) = 5.60, p < .001). Framed another way, when a student had neither a stronger sense of ethnic identity nor a stronger sense of school belonging, the student were found to have the lowest levels of self-worth. DISCUSSION This study suggests that a sense of school belonging at the university level is associated with the psychological adjustment of ethnic minority college students. In contrast, ethnic identity was not strongly associated with psychological adjustment, especially when considered at the same time as a sense of school belonging. However, in the absence of a strong sense of school belonging, ethnic identity was linked with self-worth. This suggests that a strong feeling of belonging to one’s own ethnic group may protect minority students who feel disconnected from their college. In addition, it supports the idea, based on previous theory (Baumeister & Leary, 299 B SE B Depressive symptoms β .25∗∗∗ .01∗ .18∗∗∗ .05∗∗ R2 SE B .04 .13∗ β .40∗∗∗ .11∗ −0.14 (0.06) −.11∗ 0.41 (0.05) 0.08 (0.04) −0.01 (0.12) −.01 −0.33 (0.14) −.20∗ −0.18 (0.11) −.10 0.02 (0.06) 0.10 (0.04) B Global self-worth .17∗∗∗ .00 .11∗∗∗ SE B β .30∗∗∗ .10 −0.01 (0.07) −.01 0.34 (0.06) 0.08 (0.05) −0.05 (0.13) −.03 −0.45 (0.15) −.24∗∗ −0.26 (0.12) −.14∗ −0.07 (0.06) −.10 0.17 (0.05) .21∗∗∗ B Scholastic competence .07∗∗∗ R2 ∗p Note. Gen status = generational status; Std liv. = family standard of living; SB = school belonging; EI = ethnic identity. <.05, ∗∗ p <.01, ∗∗∗ p <.001. Step 1 .09∗∗∗ Gen status 0.23 (0.79) .02 Std. liv. −2.90 (0.60) −.27∗∗∗ Ethnic group (control group = African American) Hispanic −1.64 (1.67) −.07 Asian 2.56 (2.00) .10 Other −1.24 (1.58) −.05 Step 2 .22∗∗∗ SB −7.18 (0.75) −.48∗∗∗ EI −0.14 (0.58) −.01 Step 3 .01 SB × EI 1.58 (0.88) .09 Total R2 .32∗∗∗ R2 .18∗∗∗ .01 .14∗∗∗ .04∗ R2 TABLE 3 OLS Regression of Mental Health Outcomes by School Belonging and Ethnic Identity SE B −0.13 (0.08) 0.49 (0.07) 0.03 (0.06) 0.22 (0.15) 0.08 (0.18) 0.20 (0.14) 0.16 (0.07) 0.12 (0.05) B Social acceptance −.08 .37∗∗∗ .03 .11 .04 .09 .20∗ .13∗ β 300 GUMMADAM, PITTMAN, AND IOFFE FIGURE 1 Interaction between school belonging and ethnic identity predicting global self-worth. 1995; Leary & Baumeister, 2000), that not feeling connected to any group would be linked to worse psychological adjustment. This study adds to the growing literature suggesting that a sense of school belonging is an important factor in the psychological adjustment of college students (e.g., Freeman et al., 2007; Pittman & Richmond, 2008). Further, it extends this research by demonstrating these associations among ethnic minority college students. While previous research has demonstrated a sense of school belonging is linked to academic outcomes among minority college students (e.g., Hurtado & Carter, 1997; Walton & Cohen, 2011), research examining its links to psychological adjustment has been limited (but see Mounts, 2004). In this study, associations were found between school belonging and multiple measures of psychological adjustment, including aspects of the self system (i.e., scholastic competence, social acceptance, and global self-worth) and depressive symptoms. School belonging may enhance psychological adjustment in multiple ways. For example, by feeling like they belong at college, students may feel more comfortable approaching professors, leading them to fare better academically and to have more positive perceptions of their scholastic competence (Zumbrunn et al., 2014). Additionally, students who have a stronger sense of school belonging have been found to have more positive peer relationships (e.g., Hamm & Faircloth, 2005), which has been linked to more positive self-perceptions of social acceptance (Parker, Rubin, Erath, Wojslawowicz, & Buskirk, 2006). Further, college students who feel like they belong to their college have been found to experience less loneliness (e.g., Mounts, 2004), which has been linked to fewer depressive symptoms and improved feelings of self-worth (Vanhalst et al., 2012). Future research could explore more explicitly the possible mechanisms through which school belonging is associated with psychological adjustment. In addition, testing how interventions targeting school belonging (e.g., Walton & Cohen, 2011) may impact indicators of SCHOOL BELONGING AND ETHNIC IDENTITY 301 psychological adjustment is warranted, especially among ethnic minority college students (e.g., Smith et al., 2014). Compared to school belonging, ethnic identity was less strongly associated with psychological adjustment in ethnic minority college students in this study. Specifically, while bivariate correlations indicated ethnic identity was associated with both scholastic competence and global self-worth, only a weak association was found with self-worth, after controlling for demographic characteristics and school belonging. The lack of stronger support for a link between ethnic identity and psychological adjustment was surprising based on previous research (e.g., Brittian, Umaña-Taylor, Lee et al., 2013; Yip et al., 2006). However, this pattern of findings is consistent with some previous research where ethnic identity has been more strongly linked to positive outcomes (e.g., self-worth) as compared to negative outcomes (e.g., depressive symptoms; Silva & Smith, 2011). Yet across all four outcomes, including self-worth, school belonging was the stronger predictor. Thus, this study suggests that school belonging is more important to ethnic minority students’ psychological adjustment than feeling a sense of belonging to one’s ethnic group. Considering that college students are often living away from home for the first time and embedded within a new community, a sense of school belonging may be of particular importance. Given that the majority of the sample were freshmen students, it may be that over time the importance of school belonging decreases as students become more acclimated to school life. In addition, the importance of both school belonging and ethnic identity could vary by context. Benner and Graham (2007) found that the ethnic composition of a high school, especially in comparison to the composition of their middle school, was associated over time with adolescents’ sense of school belonging. Similarly, the ethnic composition of the college campus has been found to moderate the links between ethnic identity and psychological adjustment, such that it has stronger associations when less diversity is present (Brittian, Umaña-Taylor, & Derlan, 2013). Therefore, the strength of the associations between ethnic identity and psychological outcomes may vary based on the level of diversity on the university campus. However, as the significant interaction in the current study showed, when students were not strongly connected to their school, ethnic identity was associated with self-worth. Using longitudinal designs, future research should test whether there are two avenues available to enhance feelings of self-worth among students from ethnic minority backgrounds: increasing their connection to the broader campus community, and enhancing the strength of their ethnic identity. In order to promote psychological adjustment among students, especially ethnic minority students, college administrators may want to take two sets of actions. First, they could add components to their new student orientation and/or advising programs that would enhance a sense of school belonging. Second, they may want to add courses or campus events that target increasing the knowledge and understanding of diverse populations. Given that college students are typically still developing their ethnic identity (Umaña-Taylor et al., 2014), such initiatives would likely stimulate students to explore their ethnic heritage, which is related to developing a secure and positive ethnic identity (Phinney, 2006). Taking steps to enhance both school belonging and ethnic identity would likely help more students than either alone, as having a stronger ethnic identity appears to be important when ethnic minority students do not feel like they belong at the university. While school belonging and ethnic identity are two ways in which college students may feel a sense of belonging to a broader community or group, they could have a sense of belonging to multiple other groups not assessed in this study (e.g., religious or political organizations, sororities/fraternities). Similar to how ethnic identity was linked to enhanced self-worth when 302 GUMMADAM, PITTMAN, AND IOFFE school belonging was lower, students who feel a sense of belonging to these other groups may be well adjusted, even when they do not feel connected to the university. In addition, the importance of the quality of relationships with specific individuals would be important to consider and may have an even more important influence on psychological adjustment than school belonging (e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Therefore, more research is needed considering the interplay between school belonging and other ways in which college students may feel a sense of belonging. This research was exploratory in nature and had multiple limitations that could be addressed in future research. The cross-sectional nature of this study makes it impossible to infer causality between the predictors and adjustment outcomes. In fact, it may be that those college students who have more scholastic competence, self-worth, or social acceptance or fewer depressive symptoms are better able to connect with individuals at their university, enhancing their sense of school belonging. Similarly, those with more positive psychological adjustment may more readily explore and actively strengthen their ethnic identity. Longitudinal studies, where changes in psychological adjustment, school belonging, and ethnic identity could be assessed over time, would be helpful to disentangle the direction of the effect. Furthermore, collecting data at the same time point across all participants would help control variation in students’ psychological adjustment that may be associated with confounding factors (e.g., presence of midterms, winter blues). As suggested earlier, an experimental design would help in testing the influence of school belonging and ethnic identity over time. Another weakness that could be addressed in future research is the reliance on self-report data for all aspects of functioning. Additional measures, such as actual academic performance, could be obtained. In addition, while the measures selected for this study have been widely used, the response options and subsequent composite scores are ordinal in nature. In using these scores in correlational and regression analyses, an assumption was made that these variables were linear. Further, while the current study assessed both ethnic identity and school belonging globally, some have found specific factors underlying these constructs. Using the MEIM, ethnic identity has been found to tap into two underlying components: (a) affirmation and belonging, and (b) exploration and active involvement (e.g., Roberts et al., 1999). Similarly, school belonging has been found to have an underlying factor structure including three components: caring relationships, acceptance, and rejection (You et al., 2011). Studies examining these dimensions of ethnic identity and school belonging may identify specific associations that are linked to the psychological adjustment of college students, providing information about how best to target interventions. Future research may also investigate whether these associations are only true for specific ethnic/racial backgrounds. Given the sample sizes, analyses in this study collapsed across all minority groups and only controlled for ethnic group. Exploration of whether these findings are consistent across multiple ethnic groups as well as within specific subgroups (e.g., Mexican Americans, Chinese Americans) would be informative. Finally, prior research has often found a prospective link between academic achievement and a sense of school belonging among young adolescents (e.g., Anderman, 2003; but see Hurtado and Carter [1997], where this link was not supported among Hispanic college students). For that reason, future studies should assess and control for current and past academic achievement (e.g., ACT test scores, GPA) when examining the associations between school belonging and psychological adjustment. Despite these limitations, this study highlights the importance of minority college students’ feelings of belonging, especially to the university. Striving for enhanced psychological adjustment among ethnic minority college students is itself a worthy goal; however, in doing so, these SCHOOL BELONGING AND ETHNIC IDENTITY 303 students may also have increased academic success (e.g., Richardson, Abraham, & Bond, 2012). Establishing intervention programs early in freshman year that enhance a sense of school belonging (e.g., Walton & Cohen, 2011) may start ethnic minority college students on a positive pathway leading to better psychological adjustment and, in turn, better academic outcomes and eventual graduation. Overall, this study suggests the continued need to consider a sense of school belonging among ethnic minority college students, not only because of its ties to academic outcomes and retention in college (e.g., Hausmann et al., 2007), but to help improve students’ psychological adjustment. AUTHOR NOTES Praveena Gummadam, MA, received her graduate training in clinical psychology at Northern Illinois University. Laura D. Pittman, Ph.D., is Associate Professor of Clinical Psychology and Director of Clinical Training at Northern Illinois University. She takes a risk and resilience approach to understand how family processes and broader contextual factors (e.g., schools, culture) influence children’s and adolescents’ socioemotional and cognitive functioning. Micah Ioffe, MA, is a doctoral candidate in the clinical psychology program at Northern Illinois University. Her research interests focus on the influence of family dynamics and relationships on children’s and adolescents’ psychological functioning. ACKNOWLEDGMENT This research was conducted as part of the first author’s master’s thesis. REFERENCES Aiken, L., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Anderman, E. M. (2002). School effects on psychological outcomes during adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 795–809. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.94.4.795 Anderman, L. H. (2003). Academic and social perceptions as predictors of change in middle school students’ sense of school belonging. Journal of Experimental Education, 72, 5–22. doi:10.1080/00220970309600877 Arnett, J. (2004). Emerging adulthood: The winding road from the late teens through the twenties. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Barber, B. K., & Schluterman, J. M. (2008). Connectedness in the lives of children and adolescents: A call for greater conceptual clarity. Journal of Adolescent Health, 43, 209–216. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.01.012 Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.117.3.497 Benner, A. D., & Graham, S. (2007). Navigating the transition to multi-ethnic urban high schools: Changing ethnic congruence and adolescents’ school-related affect. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17, 207–220. doi:10.1111/j.15327795.2007.00519.x Brittian, A. S., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., & Derlan, C. L. (2013). An examination of biracial college youths’ family ethnic socialization, ethnic identity, and adjustment: Do self-identification labels and university context matter? Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 19, 177–189. doi:10.1037/a0029438. Brittian, A. S., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Lee, R. M., Zamboanga, B. L., Kim, S. Y., Weisskirch, R. S., . . . & Caraway, S. J. (2013). The moderating role of centrality on associations between ethnic identity affirmation and ethnic minority college students’ mental health. Journal of American College Health, 61, 133–140. doi:10.1080/07448481.2013.773904 304 GUMMADAM, PITTMAN, AND IOFFE Brody, G. H., Yu, T., Chen, E., Miller, G. E., Kogan, S. M., & Beach, S. R. H. (2013). Is resilience only skin deep? Rural African Americans’ preadolescent socioeconomic status-related risk and competence and age 19 psychological adjustment and allostatic load. Psychological Science, 24, 1285–1296. doi:10.1177/0956797612471954 Brown, C. S., & Chu, H. (2012). Discrimination, ethnic identity, and academic outcomes of Mexican immigrant children: The importance of school context. Child Development, 83, 1477–1485. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01786.x Cham, H., Hughes, J. N., West, S. G., & Im, M. H. (2014). Assessment of adolescents’ motivation for educational attainment. Psychological Assessment, 26, 642–659. doi:10/1037/a0036213 Chen, S. X., Benet-Martinez, V., Wu, W. C. H., Lam, B. C., & Bond, M. H. (2012). The role of dialectical self and bicultural identity integration in psychological adjustment. Journal of Personality, 81, 61–75. doi:10.1111/j.14676494.2012.00791.x Cokley, K., McClain, S., Enciso, A., & Martinez, M. (2013). An examination of the impact of minority status stress and impostor feelings on the mental health of diverse ethnic minority college students. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development, 41, 82–95. doi:10.1002/j.2161-1912.2013.00029.x Cronin, T. J., Levin, S., Branscombe, N. R., van Laar, C., & Tropp, L. R. (2012). Ethnic identification in response to perceived discrimination protects well-being and promotes activism: A longitudinal study of Latino college students. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 15, 393–407. doi:10.1177/1368430211427171 Cross, W. E. (1994). Nigresence theory: Historical and explanatory notes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 44, 119–1123. doi:10.1006/jvbe.1994.1008 Curtin, N., Stewart, A. J., & Ostrove, J. M. (2013). Fostering academic self-concept: Advisor support and sense of belonging among international and domestic graduate students. American Educational Research Journal, 50, 108–1137. doi:10.3102/0002831212446662 Eccles, J. S., & Roeser, R. W. (2003). Schools as developmental contexts. In G. R. Adams & M. D. Berzonsky (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence (pp. 129–148). Malden, MA: Blackwell. Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity, youth, and crisis. New York, NY: W. W. Norton. Faircloth, B. S., & Hamm, J. V. (2005). Sense of belonging among high school students representing four ethnic groups. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 34, 293–309. doi:10.1007/s10964-005-5752-7 Freeman, T. M., Anderman, L. H., & Jenson, J. M. (2007). Sense of belonging in college freshman at the classroom and campus levels. Journal of Experimental Education, 75, 203–220. doi:10.3200/JEXE.75.3.203-220 Galliher, R. V., Rostosky, S. S., & Hughes, H. K. (2004). School belonging, self-esteem, and depressive symptoms in adolescents: An examination of sex, sexual attraction, and urbanicity. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33, 235–245. doi:10.1023/B:JOYO.0000025322.11510.9d Garcı́a Coll, C., Lamberty, G., Jenkins, R., McAdoo, H. P., Crnic, K., Wasik, B. H., & Vázquez Garcı́a, H. (1996). An integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Child Development, 67, 1891–1914. Gaylord-Harden, N. K., Ragsdale B. L., Mandera, J., Richards, M. H., & Petersen, A. C. (2007). Perceived support and internalizing symptoms in African American adolescents: Self-esteem and ethnic identity as mediators. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 77–88. doi:10.1007/s10964-006-9115-9 Goodenow, C. (1993). The psychological sense of school membership among adolescents: Scale development and educational correlates. Psychology in the Schools, 30, 79–90. doi:10.1002/1520-6807(199301)30:1<79::AIDPITS2310300113>3.0.CO;2-X Hagerty, B. M., Williams, R. A., Coyne, J. C., & Early, M. R. (1996). Sense of belonging and indicators of social and psychological functioning. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 10, 235–244. Hamm, J. V., & Faircloth, B. S. (2005). The role of friendship in adolescents’ sense of school belonging. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 107, 61–78. doi:10.1002/cd.121 Harter, S. (2012). The construction of the self: Developmental and sociocultural foundations (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Hausmann, L. R., Schofield, J. W., & Woods, R. L. (2007). Sense of belonging as a predictor of intentions to persist among African American and White first-year college students. Research in Higher Education, 48, 803–839. doi:10.1007/s11162-007-9052-9 Helms, J. E. (1990). Toward a model of White racial identity development. In J.E. Helms (Ed.), Black and White racial identity. Theory, research, and practice (pp. 50–66). Westport, CT: Praeger. Hurtado, S., & Carter, D. F. (1997). Effects of college transition and perceptions of the campus racial climate on Latino college students’ sense of belonging. Sociology of Education, 70, 324–345. SCHOOL BELONGING AND ETHNIC IDENTITY 305 Hussar, W. J., & Bailey, T. M. (2011). Projections of education statistics to 2019 (NCES 2011-017). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved from http://nces.ed.gov/pubs2011/2011017.pdf Kia-Keating, M., & Ellis, B. H. (2007). Belonging and connection to school in resettlement: Young refugees, school belonging and psychosocial adjustment. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 12, 29–43. doi:10.1177/1359104507071052 Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of self-esteem: Sociometer theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 1–62. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(00)80003-9. Lee, R. M., Yun, A. B., Yoo, H. C., & Nelson, K. P. (2010). Comparing the ethnic identity and well-being of adopted Korean Americans with immigrant/US-born Korean Americans and Korean international students. Adoption Quarterly, 13, 2–17. doi:10.1080/10926751003704408 Locks, A. M., Hurtado, S., Bowman, N. A., & Oseguera, L. (2008). Extending notions of campus climate and diversity to students’ transition to college. Review of Higher Education, 31, 257–285. doi:10.1353/rhe.2008.0011 Lorenzo-Hernández, J., & Ouellette, S. C. (1998). Ethnic identity, self-esteem, and values in Dominicans, Puerto Ricans, and African Americans. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 2007–2024. doi:10.1111/j.15591816.1998.tb01358.x Maurizi, L. K., Caballo, R., Epstein-Ngo, Q., & Cortina, K. S. (2013). Does neighborhood belonging matter? Examining school and neighborhood belonging as protective factors for Latino adolescents. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 83, 323–334. doi:10.1111/ajop.12017 Mounts, N. S. (2004). Contributions of parenting and campus climate to freshmen adjustment in a multiethnic sample. Journal of Adolescent Research, 19, 468–491. doi:10.1177/0743558403258862 Mueller, C., & Haines, R. T. (2012). Adolescent perceptions of family connectedness and school belonging: Links with self-concept and depressive symptoms among gifted African American and Hispanic youth. Gifted Children, 5(2), article 3. Retrieved from http://docs.lib.purdue.edu/giftedchildren/vol5/iss2/3 Neemann, J., & Harter, S. (1986). Manual for the Self-Perception Profile for College Students. Denver, CO: University of Denver. Ostrove, J. M., & Long, S. M. (2007). Social class and belonging: Implications for college adjustment. Review of Higher Education, 30, 363–389. doi:10.1353/rhe.2007.0028 Parker, J. G., Rubin, K. H., Erath, S. A., Wojslawowicz, J. C., & Buskirk, A. A. (2006). Peer relationships, child development, and adjustment: A developmental psychopathology perspective. In D. Cicchetti, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, Volume 1: Theory and method (2nd ed., pp. 419–493). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. Phinney, J. S. (1989) Stages of ethnic identity development in minority group adolescents. Journal of Early Adolescence, 9, 34–39. doi:10.1177/0272431689091004 Phinney, J. S. (1992). The Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure: A new scale for use with diverse groups. Journal of Adolescent Research, 7, 156–176. doi:10.1177/074355489272003 Phinney, J. S. (1996). When we talk about American ethnic groups, what do we mean? American Psychologist, 51, 918–927. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.9.918 Phinney, J. S. (2006). Ethnic identity exploration in emerging adulthood. In J. J. Arnett & J. L. Tanner (Eds.), Emerging adults in America: Coming of age in the 21st century (pp. 117–134). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Phinney, J. S., & Alipuria, L. L. (1990). Ethnic identity in college students from four ethnic groups. Journal of Adolescence, 13, 171–183. doi:10.1016/0140-1971(90)90006-S Phinney, J. S., & Ong, A. D. (2007). Conceptualization and measurement of ethnic identity: Current status and future directions. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, 237–281. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.54.3.271 Pittman, L. D., & Richmond, A. (2007). Academic and psychological functioning in late adolescence: The importance of school belonging. Journal of Experimental Education, 75, 270–290. doi:10.3200/JEXE.75.4.270-292 Pittman, L. D., & Richmond, A. R. (2008). University belonging, friendship quality, and psychological adjustment during the transition to college. Journal of Experimental Education, 76, 343–361. doi:10.3200/JEXE.76.4.343-362 Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. doi:10.1177/014662167700100306 Radloff, L. S. (1991). The use of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20, 149–166. doi:10.1007/BF01537606 306 GUMMADAM, PITTMAN, AND IOFFE Resnick, M. D., Bearman, P. S., Blum, R. W., Bauman, K. E., Harris, K. M., & Jones, J. (1997). Protecting adolescents from harm. Findings from the National Longitudinal Study on Adolescent Health. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278, 823–832. Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: A systemic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 353–387. doi:0.1037/a0026838 Roberts, R. E., Phinney, J. S., Masse, L. C., Chen, Y. R., Roberts, C. R., & Romero, A. (1999). The structure of ethnic identity of young adolescents from diverse ethnocultural groups. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19, 301–322. doi:10.1177/0272431699019003001 Roeser, R. W., Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. C. (1996). Perceptions of the school psychological environment and early adolescents’ psychological and behavioral functioning in school: The mediating role of goals and belonging. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 408–422. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.88.3.408 Shochet, I. M., & Smith, C. L. (2014). A prospective study investigating the links among classroom environment, school connectedness, and depressive symptoms in adolescents. Psychology in the Schools, 51, 480–492. doi:10.1002/pits.21759 Shook, N. J., & Clay, R. (2012). Interracial roommate relationships: A mechanism for promoting sense of belonging at university and academic performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 1168–1172. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2012.05.005 Skorikov, V. B. & Vandervoort, D.J. (2003). Relationships between the underlying constructs of the Beck Depression Inventory and the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 63, 319–335. doi:10.1177/0013164402251035 Smith, K. M., Chesin, M. S., & Jeglic, E. L. (2014). Minority college student mental health: Does majority status matter? Implications for college counseling services. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development, 42, 77–92. doi:10.1002/j.2161-1912.2014.00046.x Smith, T., & Silva, L. (2011). Ethnic identity and personal well-being of people of color: A meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 58, 42–60. doi:10.1037/a0021528 Snyder, T. D., & Dillow, S. A. (2013). Digest of education statistics 2012 (NCES 2014-015). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved from http://nces.ed.gov/pubs2014/2014015.pdf St. Louis, G. R., & Liem, J. H. (2005). Ego identity, ethnic identity, and the psychosocial well-being of ethnic minority and majority college students. Identity, 5, 227–246. doi:10.1207/s1532706xid0503 1 Tran, A. G., & Lee, R. M. (2010). Perceived ethnic–racial socialization, ethnic identity, and social competence among Asian American late adolescents. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16, 169. doi:10.1037/a0016400 Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Quintana, S. M., Lee, R. M., Cross, W. E., Rivas-Drake, D., Schwartz, S. J., . . . Ethnic and Racial Identity in the 21st Century Study Group. (2014). Ethnic and racial identity during adolescence and into young adulthood: An integrated conceptualization. Child Development, 85, 21–39. doi:10.1111/cdev.12196 Vanhalst, J., Klimstra, T. A., Luyckx, K., Scholte, R. H. J., Engels, R. C. M. E., & Goossens, L. (2012). The interplay of loneliness and depressive symptoms across adolescence: Exploring the role of personality traits. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41, 776–787. doi:10.1007/s10964-011-9726-7 Walton, G. M., & Cohen, G. L. (2011). A brief social-belonging intervention improves academic and health outcomes of minority students. Science, 331, 1447–1451. doi:10.1126/science.1198364 Wilson, S., & Gore, J. (2013). An attachment model of university connectedness. Journal of Experimental Education, 81, 178–198. doi:10.1080/00220973.2012.699902 Yeager, D. S., & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81, 267–301. doi:10.3102/0034654311405999 Ye, F., & Wallace, T. L. (2014). Psychological Sense of School Membership Scale: Method effects associated with negatively worded items. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 32, 202–215. doi:10.1177/0734282913504816 Yip, T., Seaton, E. K., & Sellers, R.M. (2006). African American racial identity across the lifespan: Identity status, identity content and depressive symptoms. Child Development, 77, 1504–1517. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00950.x You, S., Ritchey, K., Furlong, M., Shochet, I. M., & Boman, P. (2011). Examination of the latent structure of the Psychological Sense of School Membership Scale. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 29, 225–237. doi:10.1177/0734282910379968 Zumbrunn, S., McKim, C., Buhs, E., & Hawley, L.R. (2014). Support, belonging, motivation and engagement in the college classroom: A mixed method study. Instructional Science, 42, 661–684. doi:10.1007/s11251-014-9310-0