Leah Moyer 12/11/19 PSYC 202 COLL Paper 1 In the article I chose, the issue addressed by the study explored the relationship between attachment style in children and the art they produced of themselves and their families. The study’s main goal was to confirm that analyzing “family drawings” as a measure of security/insecurity in children is a valid indicative instrument of attachment style. The experimenters asked the children to create a “drawing of a family”, i.e. a portrait of themselves and their attachment figures, which is how most interpreted the question. They reasoned that certain stylistic elements, content, level of detail, and more in their art may correlate with such attachment that they also tested alongside the drawing prompt. The researchers divided 117 kids between the ages of 6 and 10 (living in Milan, Italy of similar socioeconomic backgrounds) into 3 groups of 39 children each, homogenously organized by their attachment styles: secure, anxious-ambivalent insecure, and avoidant insecure (there were not enough children displaying “disorganized attachment” to include this subgroup). To determine which attachment style the children possessed, they employed the standardized Separation Anxiety Test (SAT). I thought the use of a more modern test was rather clever, as the researchers describe employing a significantly shorter study that was created in the past by psychologists to efficiently determine children’s attachment styles. The method is much more of an accessible experiment, using pictographic images depicting scenarios rather than reenacting the scenario with each child and their parent, which certainly saves a good deal of time, effort, and emotional vulnerability on the part of the subjects. The way the children’s subsequent drawings were then analyzed empirically was based on “the coding system of Kaplan and Main”, further expounded upon by “Fury, Carlson, and Sroufe”. Three levels of coding were applied to the drawings. The first measured “graphic, formal, and content” aspects of the drawings involving an “ad hoc grid comprising 35 nominally scored items grouped into five categories.” The second analysis evaluated the drawings based on “eight 5-point global rating scales.” Finally, the third category rated “overall characteristics of the representation” (referencing the presence of bizarre content, overall creativity, and care taken in the drawing’s execution). The overall analysis indeed confirmed there were significant differences between the drawings created by children with different attachment styles, and similarities between the children that subsequently shared attachment styles. The basis upon how they judged the drawings was to me, fascinating. The graphics included in the article expound upon what specific criteria was used to sort the work symbolically. Something very interesting is that a tipoff a child had secure attachment was their attention to gender roles. It was stated that securely attached children showed “well-individuated, gender- differentiated” persons, and that “mother figures were depicted as feminine.” On the other hand, insecurely attached kids “were more likely to draw poorly individuated and poorly gender-differentiated figures” (as well as other markers such as omitted body parts, inappropriate space or lack of space between figures, and less attention to detail). Something that around half of people learn, either consciously or unconsciously as they grow older, is that perceptions of gender, sex, race, etc. as it pertains to the individual are not so clear cut. Oftentimes notions of gender in particular is redundant, nonsensical, and more often than not a symbolic gesture than anything pertaining to a universal standard/truth. So why is it that when children are younger is it almost necessary, and in fact healthy for kids to set these simplistic stereotypes? My guess is that these symbols of people, of self and other, are necessary to early development as they help a child adjust comfortably to the world around them. Without these certain basic identifiers, no child would be able to feel psychologically “secure”. The experimental methodology of interpreting the inner workings of one’s mind as translated to paper certainly reminds me of certain methods of psychoanalysis we studied in class. Freud’s ideas on the unconscious as well as the Rorschach test seem particularly relevant, although these theories have been more often than not applied to adults. The significant difference that can be made here is adults are generally warier about the way they are seen, the things they create, and of course are more aware of the ways in which their external output could be interpreted by others as compared to kids. Thus, the simple yet versatile idea of turning children’s drawings (devoid of self-conscious ulterior motive) into data on their inner psychology is brilliant. Since they confirmed there is indeed a correlation that can be made between the two, the conclusion that an analysis of a family drawing done by a child can be used to facilitate early intervention at signs of attachment/adjustment issues in childhood is valid. That being said, this study is a correlational study rather than a “true experiment”. In a true experiment the researcher perfectly manipulates one variable to measure its effect on another, but since this study has a variable that would be unethical to manipulate (a child’s form of attachment based on the way their parent/s raise them), correlational is the only way this can be studied. The experiment was helpful nonetheless even if the science could be seen as minorly subjective based on the uniform nature of the population studied and the methods used. Even so, the strong correlation is significant to psychology and particularly childcare; if the warning signs of an insecure attachment can be recognized earlier on by psychologists and even parents, then there’s the possibility they do not become bigger problems to the child’s quality of life later in adulthood. References: Procaccia, R., Veronese, G., & Castiglioni, M. (2014). The impact of attachment style on the family drawings of school-aged children. The Open Psychology Journal, 7, Article 917. https://doi-org.proxy.wm.edu/10.2174/1874350101407010009