Proceedings of 6th International Fruit Fly Symposium 6–10 May 2002, Stellenbosch, South Africa pp. 105–110 Response of female Medfly, Ceratitis capitata (Diptera: Tephritidae), to olfactory stimuli from various host plants in a wind tunnel Serge Quilici* & Thuy Nguyen Ngoc CIRAD Réunion, Pôle de Protection des Plantes, 7 Chemin de l’IRAT, 97410 Saint-Pierre (France) The Medfly, Ceratitis capitata (Wiedemann), is a highly polyphagous pest of economic importance on Réunion Island as in many other parts of the world. Using a flight tunnel, a study was conducted on the response of C. capitata females to volatile compounds emitted by different host plants. Results confirm that the odours of host fruit play an important role in the female host-plant location behaviour. The response varied depending on fruit species: guava (Psidium guayava L.), citrus (Citrus spp.), coffee (Coffea arabica L.), chilli (Capsicum frutescens L.), Indian almond (Terminalia catappa L.) and Spanish cherry (Mimusops elengi L.). Host plant leaves also induced a weaker response and a limited attractiveness of non-host fruit odour was also shown. INTRODUCTION Kairomones from plant origin have been shown to play a significant role in attracting females of various tephritid species during host-plant location process (Prokopy & Roitberg 1989; Fletcher & Prokopy 1991). This perception of, or attraction to, plant volatiles is well-known in oligophagous tephritid species but also in polyphagous ones such as Bactrocera tryoni (Froggatt) (Eisemann & Rice 1992), the Caribbean fruit fly, Anastrepha suspensa (Loew) (Nigg et al. 1994), the oriental fruit fly, Bactrocera dorsalis Hendel (Cornelius et al. 2000) or the Mediterranean fruit fly (Medfly), Ceratitis (Ceratitis) capitata (Wiedemann) (Light et al. 1988; Light et al. 1992; Prokopy & Vargas 1996; Katsoyannos et al. 1997; Jang 1997; Warthen et al. 1997). Light et al. (1992), using electroantennography, showed that the Medfly female antennae could perceive the volatile constituents of nectarine. In field cages, Prokopy & Vargas (1996) showed a strong attractiveness for females of ripe intact or crushed coffee fruit, compared with five lowerranking host fruits (guava, banana, tangerine, papaya and avocado) and three non-host fruits. Previous studies have also shown that wind tunnels constitute a suitable tool for analysing the response of fruit flies to these semiochemicals (Fein et al. 1982; Jang & Light 1991; Jang et al. 1997). This field of research proves useful for a better understanding of the insect–plant relationship in tephritid species of economic importance and may lead to the identification of promising new female attractants for monitoring or control. On Réunion Island, C. capitata is a tephritid of economic importance for fruit crops, particularly in the lowlands on the leeward western side of *To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: serge.quilici@cirad.fr the island, which has a drier climate favourable to this species (Etienne 1982). This study aimed at looking for host fruits highly attractive to Medfly females, using a wind tunnel. The first objective was to specify the methodology for studying the response of Medfly female to olfactory stimuli in a wind tunnel with respect to the influence of wind speed on female response. Once adequate parameters were defined, we compared the oriented flight of Medfly females in response to olfactory stimuli from various host or non-host plants, in no-choice or choice situations. Our purpose was to assess the relative attractiveness of previously studied host fruits, such as coffee or guava, and of some common tropical host fruits present on Réunion Island, such as Indian almond (Terminalia catappa L.) (Combretaceae) or Spanish cherry (Mimusops elengi L.) (Sapotaceae). In addition, we also examined whether ripe host fruits were preferred to unripe ones, and if host-plant leaves presented any attractiveness for females. MATERIALS AND METHODS The strain of C.capitata used in these experiments was started from samples of infested fruit collected on Réunion Island and larvae were subsequently reared on artificial diet in the laboratory for more than 50 generations. Adults are reared at 25 ± 3°C and 70 ± 15% RH, in large perspex cages (50 × 50 × 60 cm), with two egg-laying devices placed at the top of the cage. Each device consists of a red plastic funnel punctured with numerous small holes enabling the females to deposit their eggs into it. Flies are provided with water and food consisting of cane sugar and enzymatic yeast hydrolysate (ICN Biochemicals, U.S.A.). Eggs are collected daily and placed on an artificial diet for larval development (Etienne 1982). At the end of 106 Proceedings of the 6th International Fruit Fly Symposium larval development, the plates containing larvae are placed in large boxes containing a layer of sand, where pupation occurs. After pupation, the sand is sifted in order to collect the pupae. According to their date of emergence, adult flies of both sexes used were grouped daily in cubical cages (30 × 30 × 30 cm) in order to constitute cohorts of known age; only females were used in this study. For all experiments, females were used when 10 days old, an age when they had reached their sexual maturity. They were naive, that is they had neither previous contact with any host plant, nor egg-laying experience before the experiment. The experiments were conducted in a plastic (Lexan) wind tunnel (l = 255 cm; b = 80 cm; h = 45 cm) placed in a room were temperature was maintained at 25 ± 1°C. The airflow was brought from the outside into the tunnel by a ventilator, then extracted to the outside. The airspeed was measured using a telescopic anemometer equipped with a thermic sensor (Model 8330, Velocicheck, TSI). A set of cold light neons, placed 50 cm above the tunnel, was equipped with a regulator providing a light intensity which varied from 0 to 5400 Lux. Light intensity was measured with a luxmeter (Lux Meter LX-101, Bioblock Scientific). For all experiments, females were individually tested . Each female was placed in a small glass tube (5 × 1.5 cm) which was carefully placed down the airflow at a distance of 1.2 m from the odour source(s). In all experiments, in addition to the odour, we used one or two red plastic spheres (diameter 7 cm) placed at the upwind extremity of the tunnel 2 cm above the tunnel ceiling. For the study of female response to olfactory stimuli in a choice situation, the spheres were punctured with small holes to allow a good flow of the volatiles, and the odour was sent inside the sphere. The observation began when the female reached the opening of the tube and ended after a period of two minutes, or when the female landed on the sphere. In each experiment, 25 females were tested in a series of five for each treatment. Each female was used only once. Preliminary methodological study For methodological studies the source of odour consisted of 150 g of ripe guava (Psidium guayava L.) fruits cut in slices and placed in a metallic grid box situated behind the filter, at the upwind side of the tunnel. The response of females to the same olfactory stimulus (guava odour) was com- pared at the airspeed of 0, 20, 30 and 40 cm/sec. Then the response of females to the same stimulus was compared at three different periods of the day: morning (08:00–10:00), middle of the day (12:00–14:00) and afternoon (16:00–18:00). Finally the response of females to guava odour was compared under four light intensities: 300, 600, 1200 and 2400 Lux, at an airspeed of 20 cm/sec, the experiment being conducted in the middle of the day (10:00–12:00). Female response to olfactory stimuli Based on the preliminary methodological study, the conditions chosen for all subsequent experiments were: airspeed – 20 cm/sec; light intensity – 600 Lux, period of the day–10:00–12:00. For all experiments, three replicates of 25 flies, individually tested, were carried out for each treatment. No-choice situation. To study the response to olfactory stimuli in a no-choice situation, the flies were exposed to two series of ripe host or non-host fruits odours. The fruits were placed in a metallic grid box situated behind the filter at the upwind side of the tunnel. In a first series of experiments, the odour source was composed of ripe fruits of either guava (Psidium guayava L.), orange (Citrus sinensis (L.), chilli (Capsicum frutescens L.), or pumpkin (Cucurbita pepo L.).For the control (clean air) no fruit was placed in the odour compartment. In a second series of experiments the females were offered another series of ripe fruit odours: Spanish cherry (Mimusops elengi L.), Indian almond (Terminalia catappa L.), coffee (Coffea arabica L.), pumpkin (Cucurbita pepo L.) or no odour (control). The weight of the fruit sample used for the different treatments was 150 g for Indian almond, Spanish cherry, guava, orange, pumpkin or coffee, and 40 g for chilli. To allow for a better volatile emission, small fruits (Indian almond, Spanish cherry, chilli) were punctured with a needle while bigger fruits (guava, orange, pumpkin) were cut into slices.mm Choice situation. For this series of experiments the odour sources were situated outside the tunnel, in two plastic bags with entry and exit holes for airflow. The entries were connected by a silicone pipe to a vacuum pump of variable flow injecting clean air at a speed of l l/min, while the exits were connected with the tunnel by silicone pipes. Once they have passed above the odour sources, the airflows charged with volatiles are then sent inside the two red spheres placed within the tunnel.mmm In a first series of experiments, ripe fruits of Quilici & Nguyen Ngoc: Response of female Medfly to olfactory stimuli in a wind tunnel 107 Fig. 1. Response of Ceratitis capitata females to volatiles of ripe guava fruit at different wind speeds (n = 25). Figures followed by the same letter do not differ significantly (Fisher test, P = 0.05). various host plants were compared in a two-choice situation: orange vs clementine, guava vs chilli, guava vs Spanish cherry, guava vs coffee, guava vs Indian almond and guava vs orange. The response to unripe vs ripe fruits was compared in a second series of experiments, successively for clementine, chilli, Spanish cherry, coffee and Indian almond. Finally, on a third series of experiments, the response of females to different plant organs was evaluated for Indian almond then for coffee (ripe fruit vs control, leaf vs control and ripe fruit vs leaf ). Data analysis For the different experiments, analysis were carried out to compare the percentages of females reaching the sphere. A Chi-square test with Yates’ correction or an exact Fisher test (when females showed a weak response) with P = 0.05 were performed using the software S-plus 2000. RESULTS Preliminary methodological study The percentage of females reaching the sphere was higher at a windspeed of 20 cm/sec., though the only significant difference was between 20 and 40 cm/sec (Fig. 1). As a result of this preliminary test, an air speed of 20 cm/sec. was chosen for all subsequent experiments. The latter were conducted in the midday period (10:00–12:00), using a light intensity of 600 Lux. Female response to olfactory stimuli For all experiments, the response of females did not differ between the three replicates (Chi-square tests), so all three were analysed together. No-choice situation In the first series of experiments, the response of females to fruit odours was significantly higher Fig. 2. Response of Ceratitis capitata females to volatiles of ripe fruits from different host or non-host plant (n = 3). Figures followed by the same letter do not differ significantly (Chi-square test, P = 0.05). 108 Proceedings of the 6th International Fruit Fly Symposium Fig. 3. Response of Ceratitis capitata females to volatiles of ripe fruits from different host or non-host plants (n = 3). Figures followed by the same letter do not differ significantly (Chi-square test, P = 0.05). than the response to the control. The percentage of females flying upwind or reaching the target sphere was significantly higher when the odour source consisted of ripe chillis or guava fruits than when ripe pumpkin fruits were used (Fig. 2). The response to ripe orange fruit odour, significantly lower than that to ripe chilli fruit, was not significantly different to ripe pumpkin or guava fruits. In the second series of experiments, ripe fruits of Indian almond (Terminalia catappa) and Spanish cherry (Mimusops elengi) induced a significantly higher response than ripe pumpkin fruits (Fig. 3). The response to Indian almond fruits was significantly higher than that to coffee fruits, but not significantly different than the response to Spanish cherry fruits. Ripe coffee fruits induced an intermediate response between the other host fruits and the non-host fruit. Choice situation Females placed in a choice situation in the presence of odours of unripe or ripe fruits of Clementine or Spanish cherry showed a significantly stronger response to ripe fruits (Fig. 4). For all other fruits tested, the responses of females to unripe or ripe fruit were not significantly different. Females showed no significant difference in their response to ripe fruits of guava vs orange, chilli, coffee and Spanish cherry respectively. Similarly, Fig. 4. Choice of landing site by Ceratits capitata females between two red spheres emitting odour of unripe or ripe fruits (n = 3). Figures followed by the same letter do not differ significantly (Chi-square test, P = 0.05). Quilici & Nguyen Ngoc: Response of female Medfly to olfactory stimuli in a wind tunnel 109 Fig. 5. Choice of landing site by Ceratitis capitata females between two red spheres emitting odours of guava or Indian almond ripe fruits (n = 3). Figures followed by the same letter do not differ significantly (Chi-square test, P = 0.05). for citrus fruits, the response of females to ripe fruits of Clementine or orange was not significantly different. By contrast, ripe Indian almond fruit was more attractive than ripe guava fruit (Fig. 5). In a choice situation, females showed a significantly higher response to ripe fruit or to Indian almond or coffee leaves, compared with the controls. When females were simultaneously offered ripe fruit and leaves in a choice situation, they showed a higher response to fruit in the case of Indian almond, while the difference was not significant in the case of coffee (Fig. 6). DISCUSSION Our results confirm that the odour of various host fruits shows a strong attractiveness for female Medfly. Interestingly, though coffee has been shown to contain volatiles attractive to the female Medfly and is presumed to be an ancestral host of Medfly in the Americas (Prokopy & Vargas 1996), we could show that, in a choice situation, the female Medfly is more attracted to ripe fruits of Indian almond than to ripe coffee berries. In future studies, it would be worth investigating the chemical compounds present in the odour of Indian almond ripe fruits. Though the response of female to ripe fruits of Spanish cherry is not significantly greater than to ripe coffee berries, this host fruit also shows a good attractiveness and it would also be worth analysing its volatiles. In the conditions of Réunion, fruits of both Indian almond and Spanish cherry are heavily attacked by C. capitata. The former are also heavily attacked by the Natal fruit fly, Ceratitis rosa (Karsch), which is less common on Spanish cherry fruits. Ripe fruits of Clementine or Spanish cherry were Fig. 6. Choice of landing site by Ceratitis capitata females between two red spheres emitting odours of coffee plant organs, or no odour (n = 3). Figures followed by the same letter do not differ significantly (Chi-square test, P = 0,.05). more attractive than unripe fruits of the same species. Though Prokopy & Vargas (1996) showed that the odour of ripe or near-ripe coffee fruit was significantly more attractive than the odour of unripe fruit, we failed to find such a difference between these two types of fruits in our experimental conditions, which may be due to the phenological stage of the fruits used. In the case of Indian almond, though ripe fruit attracted more females, the difference was not significant. Interestingly, the tendency was inverse in the case of chilli fruit. The tests in a choice situation showed that, compared to a control, the leaves of both coffee and Indian almond were attractive for female Medfly. This attractiveness of leaves seems higher in the case of coffee (for which the attractiveness of leaves is not different from that of ripe fruit) than in the case of Indian almond. CONCLUSION This work enabled us to specify the methodology for wind tunnel assays for studying the response of female Medfly to olfactory stimuli. 110 Proceedings of the 6th International Fruit Fly Symposium Our results confirm that odours from host fruits probably play a major role in the host-plant location behaviour of the females in this highly polyphagous species. Among the different host or non-host plants tested, a strong attractiveness of the ripe fruits of Indian almond, and to a lesser degree of Spanish cherry was proven. This study provides elements for future research aiming at identifying volatiles potentially attractive for the female Medfly, and that could be used to improve monitoring or bio-technical control methods against this economically important pest. Given the very large host range of Medfly, it would be worth investigating in future studies the response of female Medfly to other poorly known tropical host fruits that are recorded as being heavily attacked by the fly in some countries. REFERENCES CORNELIUS, M.L., DUAN, J.J. & MESSING, R. 2000. Volatile host fruit odors as attractants for the Oriental fruit fly (Diptera: Tephritidae). Journal of Economic Entomology 93(1): 93–100. EISEMANN, C.H. & RICE, M.J. 1992. Attractants for the gravid Queensland fruit fly Dacus tryoni. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 62: 125–130. ETIENNE, J. 1982. Étude systématique, faunistique et écologique des Téphritides de la Réunion. Thèse Ecole Pratique des Hautes Études, Paris. FEIN, B.L., REISSIG, W.H. & ROELOFS, W.L. 1982. Identification of apple volatiles attractive to the apple maggot, Rhagoletis pomonella. Journal of Chemical Ecology 8(12): 1473–1487. FLETCHER, B.S. & PROKOPY, R.J. 1991. Host location and oviposition in tephritid fruit flies. In: Bailey, W.J. & Ridsdill-Smith, J.(Eds) Reproductive Behavior of Insects. Individual and Populations. Chapman and Hall, London. JANG, E.B. 1997. Development of attractants for female fruit flies in Hawaii. In: Allwood, A.J. & Drew, R.A.I. (Eds) Management of Fruit Flies in the Pacific. 115–116. ACIAR Proceedings No. 76, Canberra. JANG, E.B. & LIGHT, D.M. 1991. Behavioral responses of female oriental fruit flies to the odor of papayas at three ripeness stages in a laboratory flight tunnel (Diptera: Tephritidae). Journal of Insect Behaviour 4: 751–762. JANG, E.B., CARVALHO, L.A. & STARCK, J.D. 1997. Attraction of female oriental fruit fly, Bactrocera dorsalis, to volatile semiochemicals from leaves and extracts of a non-host plant, panax (Polyscias guilfoylei) in laboratory and olfactometer assays. Journal of Chemical Ecology 23: 1389–1401. KATSOYANNOS,B.I.,KOULOUSSIS,N.A.& PAPADOPOULOS, N.T. 1997. Response of Ceratitis capitata to citrus chemicals under semi-natural conditions. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 82: 181–188. LIGHT, D.M., JANG, E.B. & DICKENS, J.C. 1988. Electroantennogram responses of the Mediterranean fruit fly, Ceratitis capitata, to a spectrum of plant volatiles. Journal of Chemical Ecology 14: 159–180. LIGHT, D.M., JANG, E.B. & FLATH, R.A. 1992. Electroantennogram responses of the Mediterranean fruit fly, Ceratitis capitata, to the volatile constituents of nectarines. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 63: 13–26. NIGG, H.N., MALLORY, L.L., SIMPSON, S.E., CALLAHAM, S.B., TOTH, J.P., FRASER, S., KLIM, M., NAGY, S., NATION, J.L. & ATTAWAY, J.A. 1994. Caribbean fruit fly, Anastrepha suspensa (Loew), attraction to host fruit and host kairomones. Journal of Chemical Ecology 20: 727– 743.mmm PROKOPY, R.J. & ROITBERG, B.D. 1989. Fruit fly foraging behavior. In: Robinson, A.S. & Hooper, G. (Eds) Fruit Flies: Their Biology, Natural Enemies and Control. World Crop Pests, Vol. 3B. Elsevier, Amsterdam: 293–306. PROKOPY, R.J. & VARGAS, R.L. 1996. Attraction of Ceratitis capitata flies to odor of coffee fruit. Journal of Chemical Ecology 22(4): 655–666. WARTHEN, J.D., LEE, C.J., JANG, E.B., LANCE, D.R. & McINNIS, D.O. 1997. Volatile potential attractants from ripe coffee fruit for female Mediterranean fruit fly. Journal of Chemical Ecology 23: 1891–1900.