J Child Fam Stud (2018) 27:26–33 DOI 10.1007/s10826-017-0866-6 ORIGINAL PAPER The Effects of Sociocultural Pressures and Exercise Frequency on the Body Esteem of Adolescent Girls in Korea Sukkyung You 1 ● Kyulee Shin2 Eui Kyung Kim3 ● Published online: 15 September 2017 © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2017 Abstract Sociocultural pressures to be thin and exercise frequency have been reportedly related to female adolescents’ body esteem in previous research. Using 490 female middle school students in Korea, this study examined the mediating role of thin-ideal internalization on the relationships of sociocultural pressure (i.e., parental, peer, and media) and exercise frequency to body esteem. The results indicated that parental pressure only had a direct relationship with body esteem, but no indirect relationship. Media pressure, peer pressure, and exercise frequency were indirectly, but not directly, related to body esteem through thinideal internalization. The implications and future directions of research are discussed. Keywords Thin body idealization Sociocultural factors Body esteem Exercise Adolescents ● ● ● ● * Kyulee Shin kyuleeshin@anyang.ac.kr Sukkyung You skyou@hufs.ac.kr Eui Kyung Kim ekim7@ncsu.edu 1 College of Education, Hankuk University of Foreign Studies, 270 Imun-dong, Dongdaemun-Gu, Seoul 130-791, Korea 2 Anyang University, College of Liberal Arts, 22, Samdeok-ro 37beon-gil, Manan-gu, Anyang-si, Gyeonggi-do 430-714, Korea 3 Department of Psychology, North Carolina State University, Raleigh, NC 27695, USA Introduction Adolescence is a transitional period from childhood to adulthood and thus induces diverse developmental as well as adaptive difficulties (Zimmermann and Iwanski 2014). Psychologically, adolescents struggle to establish their identity (Luyckx et al. 2013) and tend to develop an irrational fear of being criticized or negatively evaluated by others (Beesdo et al. 2007). Studies frequently report that female adolescents are especially vulnerable to their sociocultural surroundings with respect to their body esteem (e.g., Shroff and Thompson 2006). According to the sociocultural framework, poor body esteem is a result of the increasing sociocultural pressure on women to have a thin body (Cafriet al. 2005; Jung and Forbes 2006). This sociocultural pressure concerning the thin-ideal is communicated to women by messengers such as media channels (e.g., TV and magazines) and significant others (e.g., family and peers; Anschutzet al. 2008). Specifically, when women, especially adolescents who are still vulnerable to others’ evaluation of them, are exposed to the sociocultural pressure to be thin, they take the unrealistic body shape as an ideal and feel dissatisfied with their own body. Thus, the sociocultural framework suggests that exposure to and awareness of sociocultural pressure, as well as the internalization of this thin-ideal, are related to disturbances in body image (Fitzsimmons-Craft et al. 2012). Body esteem is a term widely used to refer to an individual’s emotions toward his or her own body (Franzoi and Shields 1984). Adolescent girls have been socialized to believe that appearance is a critical factor for evaluation by self and others (Slater and Tiggemann 2015). However, the onset of puberty involves bodily changes and often moves female adolescents further from societal standards of ideal female body shape (McCabe and Ricciardelli 2001), and J Child Fam Stud (2018) 27:26–33 thus, low body esteem has become an expected part of puberty (Kateret al. 2002). Similar to Western female adolescents, Korean female adolescents reportedly have negative attitudes and feelings toward their bodies (Jung and Forbes 2006; Junget al. 2009). A previous study comparing US and Korean middle school students found that Korean girls reported greater body dissatisfaction than US girls (Jung et al. 2009). Additionally, Korean youths with a low level of body dissatisfaction were likely to exhibit a low level of self-esteem and high level of depressive symptoms (You et al. 2016). In order to understand the paths to healthy body esteem, previous body image studies examined and identified media-, parental-, and peer-pressure as primary sociocultural factors influencing the development of body esteem among female adolescents and undergraduates (Hardit and Hannum 2012; Keery et al. 2004b; Shroff and Thompson 2006). The relationship between media pressure and body esteem has been well supported in previous research (Lawler and Nixon 2011). For example, when Korean elementary, high school, and college students watched TV shows emphasizing the thin ideal, they reported higher levels of awareness of the gap between the ideal body and their own bodies and lower levels of body esteem than those who did not watch the shows (Jang and Kim 2006). However, few studies have examined how various sociocultural factors including media, parental, and peer pressures are associated with the body image of Korean female adolescents. A recent study by Shin, You and Kim (2016) investigated the differential effects among Korean college females and found that parent and media pressures, but not peer pressure, had direct relationships with body dissatisfaction; media pressure had stronger relationships with body dissatisfaction than parental pressure. Although exposure to the sociocultural pressure to be thin is harmful to an individual’s body esteem, the degree to which people internalize these pressures as their own beliefs, values, and ideals can be more harmful (Dittmar 2005). Based on this idea, recent studies suggested the mediating effects of the internalization of the thin-ideal on the relationship between sociocultural pressures and body esteem. Korean female high school students tend to internalize the social standard of ideal beauty presented by the mass media (Goh and Jang 2003; Kim and Park 2008). When Korean adolescent girls internalize the media pressure to be thin, they are more likely to display higher levels of body dissatisfaction (Lee and Park 2008; Park et al. 2016). Some researchers have argued that society’s thin ideal portrayed by the media might become more problematic if it were reinforced by more immediate social contacts such as parents and peers (Dunkley et al. 2001; Lawler and Nixon 2011). 27 In addition to identifying the risk factors for body dissatisfaction, such as sociocultural pressures, there have been efforts to identify potential protective factors that may be positively associated with body esteem among adolescents. Piran (2002) suggested one way that female adolescents might learn to appreciate their bodies is to participate in physical activities connecting the mind and body. Physical activity may help girls and women focus more on what a body can do rather than its appearance, which can in turn help them resist the internalization of the society’s ideal body image (Fredrickson and Roberts 1997). Sports and physical activity may also act as an empowering tool that encourages females to challenge themselves, learn their physical abilities, and improve their body image (Krane et al. 2001). Following these arguments, Slater and Tiggemann (2006) examined and found that physical activity was a strong predictor of the thin-ideal internalization and body esteem. Ha et al. (2011), using a Korean high school student sample, also found that students who participated in exercise more frequently showed higher levels of satisfaction with their bodies. Using a Korean female adolescent sample, the current study examined the mediating effects of the internalization of the thin-ideal on the relationship between sociocultural pressures and body esteem. Based on the idea that sociocultural pressures are likely to have detrimental effects on body esteem when female adolescents internalize the thinideal communicated by sociocultural pressures, we hypothesized that the internalization of the thin-ideal would mediate the relationship between sociocultural pressures and body esteem. Following the findings which suggest that media pressure is a stronger predictor of lower body esteem (Shin et al. 2016), we expected that media pressure would have the strongest association with the internalization of the thin-ideal and body esteem, followed by parent pressure and peer pressure. Method Participants Female middle school students (Grades 7–9 in the US), whose parents provided consent for them to participate in the study, were asked to fill out the survey. Among the total of 514 enrolled students, there were 24 students who declined to participate in the survey; their incomplete surveys were excluded from the data. Of the surveys distributed, those from 490 students (95.33%) were completed and used for this study. The age range was from 12 to 15 years (M = 13.86, SD = 0.98). 28 J Child Fam Stud (2018) 27:26–33 Procedure Exercise frequency The students completed the paper-and-pencil questionnaire in the homeroom classroom under homeroom teacher supervision. The teacher read aloud the instructions provided by the researchers and was available to clarify or answer questions. The students were told not to write their names on the questionnaire for the sake of anonymity. The questionnaire took approximately 20–30 min to complete. The students were asked to answer how many times a week they engaged in any exercise causing an increase in their heart rate during their leisure time based on a scale ranging from 1 to 3: 1 = Not at all (0–2 times a week), 2 = Sometimes (3–5 times a week), and 3 = Frequently (6–7 times a week). Data Analyses Measures Body esteem The Korean Overall Body Esteem Scale (KOBES; Gim and Cha 2006) was used to measure body esteem. The KOBES’s reliability and validity with Korean female undergraduate samples were found to be in the acceptable range in previous studies (e.g., Cronbach’s α > 0.70; Lee and Choo 2015). The scale consisted of 9 items (e.g., I like what I see when I look in the mirror) that the respondents rated based on a scale ranging from “never true for me” (1) to “always true for me” (5). For the student sample, the internal consistency of the scale was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = 0.85). Internalization The Sociocultural Attitudes Toward Appearance Scale-3 (Thompson et al. 2004) was translated into Korean and used to assess internalization. The SATAQ-3 was found to have high convergent validity and internal consistency among female adolescent samples across countries including the US, Australia, and India (i.e., Cronbach’s α range = .83–.92; Keery et al. 2004a). Seven items were used to represent the incorporation of media images and messages of attractiveness into one’s own self-identity (e.g., Reading magazines makes me want to change my appearance). For the sample, the internal consistency of the scale was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = 0.75). Sociocultural influence To measure sociocultural influence, 43 items (i.e., 13 for peer, 20 for parental, and 10 for media influence) of the Tripartite Influence Scale-Revised (Keery et al. 2004a, 2004b) were used. This scale was translated into Korean. This scale has been reported to have good reliability with Korean adolescent samples (Cronbach’s α range = 0.85~0.90; Song and Lee 2009; Wee 2015; Kim et al. 2015). For the sample, the internal consistency of the scale was acceptable (Cronbach’s α = 0.89, 0.79, and 0.77 for peer, parent, and media pressure, respectively). Structural equation modeling (SEM) was used to assess the hypothesized structural relationships between the latent variables. SEM was selected because it has the flexibility to test relationships among latent variables of interest (Kline 1998). This is one of the advantages of using SEM over traditional methods such as ANOVA and regression analysis, in that it allows researchers to quantify measurement errors in a separate term, thus isolating the “true” effects (McDonald and Ho 2002). In stage one, a confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to test the measurement model for the latent variables. In the second stage, a structural model (see Fig. 1) tested the hypotheses in the present study. For SEM analysis, we used the questionnaire items as measured variables to represent the latent variables in the model shown in Fig. 1. Two mediational models were tested to compare and derive the best model. The model fit was assessed based on several criteria: the non-normed fit index (NNFI; Bentler and Bonett 1980), the comparative fit index (CFI; Bentler 1990), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; Steiger and Lind 1980). Values less than 0.08 for the RMSEA and values close to 0.95 for the NNFI and CFI were used to determine a good-fitting model (Hu and Bentler 1999). All analyses were conducted using AMOS 18 (Arbuckle 2003). To test the significance of the mediating effects, we used maximum likelihood bootstrapping with a 95% confidence interval (Shrout and Bolger 2002). This method utilizes repeated sampling from the data set and estimates the indirect effect in each re-sampled data set. Table 1 shows the correlations, means, and standard deviations for the variables. Results To assess the plausibility of the hypothesis about the relationship between social pressure and body esteem being mediated by students’ thin-ideal internalization, two mediational models were tested. The initial structural model reflecting partial mediation was specified with both direct paths from social pressure to body esteem and indirect paths through a mediator (i.e., internalization). The second J Child Fam Stud (2018) 27:26–33 29 Fig. 1 Final model estimation with standardized coefficients. Note. ***p < 0.000, **p < 0.001, *p < 0.05; significant path coefficients are shown in bold line Table 1 Correlations and descriptive statistics for study variables 1 1. Peer influence 2 3 4 5 6 1 2. Parent influence 0.151** 1 3. Media influence 0.238*** 0.187** 4. Exercise frequency 0.049 0.113* 0.186** 1 5. Internalization 0.322** 0.206** 0.583** 0.017 1 6. Body esteem −0.149** −0.240** −0.260** 0.047 −0.388** 1 Mean 3.518 1.917 2.456 1.69 2.744 3.269 SD 1.014 0.956 0.930 0.652 0.931 0.717 Skewness −0.539 1.146 0.196 0.410 −0.029 0.009 Kurtosis −0.078 0.813 −0.560 −0.727 −0.430 −0.023 1 ***p < 0.000; **p < 0.001; *p < 0.05 structural model represented the full mediational model without direct effects of social pressure on body esteem. The results for the initial structural model indicate that both models provided a good fit to the data: the partial mediational model (χ2(91) = 254.6; CFI = 0.966; NNFI = 0.960; RMSEA = 0.060) and the full mediational model (χ2(95) = 265.595; CFI = 0.944; NNFI = 0.920; RMSEA = 0.061). A chi-square difference test was conducted to determine the better-fitting model. The χ2 difference and the difference in the degree of freedom between the full mediation model and the partial mediation model were employed in this model selection. The chi-square difference test provided support for the partial meditational model and, therefore, it was selected as the final theoretical model. The fit of the final model was deemed acceptable in terms of the three fit indices. Figure 1 shows the standardized parameter estimates for this model. Preacher and Hayes’s (2008) nonparametric bootstrapping analyses were conducted to estimate the standard errors for the resulting sampling distribution. These standard errors were used to compute 95% CIs for each of the path parameters. According to the bootstrap results (Table 2), two dimensions of social pressure (peer and media) had significant indirect relationships with body esteem through the mediating variable (i.e., thin-ideal internalization), but no direct relationship. Specifically, higher levels of peer and media pressure were positively associated with the internalization of the thin-ideal and, in turn, the internalization of the thin-ideal was negatively associated with body esteem. Contrarily, parental pressure and body esteem only had a negative direct relationship, but no indirect relationship. These findings partially supported our hypothesis that the internalization of the thin-ideal would mediate the relationship between sociocultural 30 J Child Fam Stud (2018) 27:26–33 Table 2 Results for indirect effect in final model Indirect effect Unstandardized coefficient Standard error Standardized coefficient 95% C.I. (bootstrap with bias correction) PEI → IN → BE −0.031** 0.021 0.091 (0.009, 0.095) PAI → IN → BE −0.011 0.011 0.031 (−0.001, 0.056) MI → IN → BE −0.104** 0.060 0.321 (0.032, 0.265) EF → IN → BE 0.020* 0.016 −0.042 (−0.076, −0.002) PEI peer influence, PAI parents influence, MI media influence, IN Internalization, EF exercise frequency, BE body esteem *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01 pressures and body esteem. It was also hypothesized that media pressure would have the strongest indirect relationship with body esteem through the internalization of the thin-ideal, followed by parental pressure and peer pressure. However, this hypothesis was also partially supported, as our study found that media pressure had the strongest indirect relationship, followed by peer pressure, but not parental pressure. Furthermore, the internalization of the thin-ideal was also found to mediate the relationship between exercise frequency and body esteem. A higher exercise frequency was significantly associated with lower thin-ideal internalization and, in turn, was associated with higher body esteem. These findings were consistent with our hypothesis that the internalization of the thin-ideal would mediate the relationship between exercise frequency and body esteem. Discussion This study examined the mediating effects of the internalization of a skinny body ideal on the relationship of sociocultural pressures (i.e., peer, parental, and media pressure) and exercise frequency with body esteem. According to the results, peer pressure and media pressure only had significant indirect relationships with body esteem through internalization. That is, female students who felt pressure from peers and media outlets on their body image tended to show higher levels of internalization of the society’s thin ideal and, in turn, were more likely to develop negative attitudes and feelings toward their physical appearance. These findings are partially consistent with previous research; Shin et al. (2016) reported that parent and media pressures, but not peer pressure, were significantly associated with body esteem among Korean female undergraduates. These inconsistent findings suggest that peer pressure is more highly associated with adolescents’ evaluation of their bodies, as adolescent girls tend to place great emphasis on the appearance-related judgments of their peers (Helfert and Warschburger 2013; Shroff and Thompson 2006), but that it becomes less important among female undergraduates. Among the significant effects, media pressure had the strongest indirect relationship with body esteem through internalization, followed by peer pressure and exercise frequency. These results support the findings of previous body esteem studies which reported that the internalization of the thin-ideal mediated the relationship between media influences and body dissatisfaction (Keery et al. 2004a, 2004b; Rodgers et al. 2015). Finally, exercise frequency also only had an indirect relationship with body esteem through thin-ideal internalization. Exercise frequency was negatively associated with the internalization of the thin-ideal and, in turn, was positively associated with body esteem. It is possible that when adolescent girls exercise frequently, they may realize how unrealistic and unattainable society’s thin ideal is, and evaluate their body more positively using more realistic standards of beauty. These findings may provide meaningful information on the development of body esteem programs. For example, providing female adolescents with psychological education and training about the unrealistic thin-ideal portrayed by the current society and ways to develop a healthy and realistic body image and esteem, instead of trying to decrease their exposure to media and peer pressures. Contrarily, the internalization of the thin-ideal did not have any mediating effects on the relationship between parental pressure and body esteem; parental pressure only had a direct relationship with body esteem. Specifically, the perceived level of parental pressure to fit the society’s standard was not significantly associated with the internalization of the thin-ideal, but was significantly associated with negative evaluation of their bodies. This result was inconsistent with the findings of Keery et al. (2004a, 2004b) that suggested the internalization of the thin-ideal mediated the relationship between parental influence and body dissatisfaction. These results suggest that Korean female adolescents are less likely to internalize the unrealistic thin ideal communicated by their parents than their US counterparts. However, although parental pressure was not significantly associated with the internalization of the thin-ideal, it was directly associated with body esteem. Specifically, Korean adolescent girls who experienced parental pressure to be thin tended to have low body esteem, regardless of their J Child Fam Stud (2018) 27:26–33 level of thin-ideal internalization, providing support for the findings of Salafia and Gondoli (2011)’s study that parents play an important role in the development of body dissatisfaction among adolescent girls in the UK. These results suggest that an adolescent girl’s low body esteem may lead her parents to encourage her to lose weight, or that parental pressure to be thin may cause their adolescent girl not to like her body. Thus, in addition to providing female adolescents with programs for developing positive body esteem, it might be critical for future research to evaluate and better understand the close relationship between parental pressure to be thin and adolescent girls’ body esteem. Strengths, Limitations, and Directions for Future Research Findings of this study can contribute to the current literature by providing a more comprehensive understanding of the roles parents, peers, and media have on the body image formation specifically among Korean female adolescents. Another novel contribution of this study is that in contrast to most previous studies that identified exercise frequency as an outcome of body esteem and thin-ideal internalization (e.g., Vartanian and Novak 2011), the current study tested a conceptual model hypothesizing participation in physical activity would be related to decreased internalization of the thin-ideal, and in turn would be related to increased body esteem. Findings of the current study, thus, will specifically provide empirical evidence to identify and foster not only sociocultural, but also physical factors that may provide an opportunity for female youths to develop and enhance positive body esteem. Particularly, a better understanding of the relationship of exercise behaviors to thin-ideal internalization and body esteem may provide meaningful information for the development of effective body esteem programs. For example, the current study’s results may suggest the inclusion of physical activities in the intervention programs to reduce the internalization of unrealistic body ideal posed by the society and foster positive body esteem by focusing more on body actions than on body appearance. Despite its meaningful implications for future research, this study has some limitations. First, this cross-sectional study predicts relationships between relevant variables, but does not suggest causal relationships. Future research should take a longitudinal approach to address causality between the variables. Second, previous studies have reported that females are more prone to responding in a socially desirable way (e.g., Chung and Monroe 2003; Krumpal 2013), suggesting that our female adolescent participants might not always have been honest when answering the survey questions. In order to address this limitation, future research should make use of multiple 31 sources of information instead of only relying on a selfreport survey. Finally, the current study did not take into account the different sources of peer (same sex vs. opposite sex) and parental pressure (paternal vs. maternal). These sources of peer and parent pressure may have differential effects on body esteem among female adolescents and, therefore, future research should consider differentiating between these sources in order to better understand the effects of sociocultural pressure on the internalization of the thin-ideal and body esteem. Acknowledgements This work was partially supported by the Hankuk University of Foreign Studies Research Fund. Author Contributions SY: designed and executed the study, analyzed the data, and wrote the paper. KS: collaborated with the design and wrote the paper. EK: collaborated with the design and writing/ editing of the paper. Compliance with Ethical Standards Conflict of Interest ing interests. The authors declare that they have no compet- Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. References Anschutz, D. J., Engels, R. C. M. E., & Van Strien, T. (2008). Susceptibility for thin ideal media and eating styles. Body Image, 5, 70–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.06.008. Arbuckle, J. L. (2003). AMOS user’s guide. Chicago, IL: SmallWaters Corporation. Beesdo, K., Bittner, A., Pine, D. S., Stein, M. B., Hofler, M., Lieb, R., & Wittchen, H. (2007). Incidence of social anxiety disorder and the consistent risk for secondary depression in the first three decades of life. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 903–912. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.64.8.903. Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 238–246. Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588–606. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.88.3. 588. Cafri, G., Yamamiya, Y., Brannick, M., & Thompson, J. K. (2005). The influence of sociocultural factors on body image: A meta‐ analysis. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 12, 421–433. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy.bpi053. Chung, J., & Monroe, G. S. (2003). Exploring social desirability bias. Journal of Business Ethics, 4, 291–302. https://doi.org/10.1023/a: 1023648703356. 32 Cusumano, D. L., & Thompson, J. K. (1997). Body image and body shape ideals in magazines: Exposure, awareness, and internalization. Sex Roles, 37, 701–721. https://doi.org/10.1007/ BF02936336. Dittmar, H. (2005). Vulnerability factors and processes linking sociocultural pressures and body dissatisfaction: An introduction to the second special issue on body image. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 1081–1087. Dunkley, T. L., Wertheim, E. H., & Paxton, S. J. (2001). Examination of a model of multiple sociocultural influences on adolescent girls’ body dissatisfaction and dietary restraint. Adolescence, 36 (142), 265–279. Fitzsimmons-Craft, E. E., Harney, M. B., Koehler, L. G., Danzi, L. E., Riddell, M. K., & Bardone-Cone, A. M. (2012). Explaining the relation between thin ideal internalization and body dissatisfaction among college women: The roles of social comparison and body surveillance. Body Image, 9, 43–49. https://doi.org/10.1016. j.bodyim.2011.09.002. Franzoi, S. L., & Shields, S. A. (1984). The body esteem scale: Multidimensional structure and sex differences in a college population. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(2), 173–178. Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. (1997). Objectification theory: Towards understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173–206. Gim, W.-S., & Cha, J.-H. (2006). Body-related values and consumer behavior: An exploratory study for scale development. Korean Journal of Consumer and Advertising Psychology, 7(3), 455–485. Goh, E., & Jang, N. (2003). Suggestions for fashion marketing strategy based on a study of adolescents’ body image and clothing behavior by the age and gender. Journal of the Korean Home Economics Association, 41, 13–26. Ha, J. Y., Cho, H. I., & So, Y. H. (2011). The relationship among body image, physical attractiveness, self-esteem, hopelessness depression and suicidal ideation according to exercise participation in high school students. Korean Society of Sport and Leisure Studies, 41, 843–858. Hardit, S. K., & Hannum, J. W. (2012). Attachment, the tripartite influence model, and the development of body dissatisfaction. Body Image, 9, 469–475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2012. 06.003. Helfert, S., & Warschburger, P. (2013). The face of appearance-related social pressure: Gender, age and body mass variations in peer and parental pressure during adolescence. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 7, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1186/ 1753-2000-7-16. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118. Jang, H.-S., & Kim, T.-R. (2006). The effect of mass media on body perception and state esteem of body image. Korea Institute for Youth Development, 17(2), 57–83. Jung, J., & Forbes, G. B. (2006). Multidimensional assessment of body dissatisfaction and disordered eating in Korean and US college women: A comparative study. Sex Roles, 55, 39–50. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-006-9058-3. Jung, J., Forbes, G. B., & Lee, Y. (2009). Body dissatisfaction and disordered eating among early adolescents from Korea and the US. Sex Roles, 61, 42–54. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-0099609-5. Kater, K. J., Rohwer, J., & Londre, K. (2002). Evaluation of an upper elementary school program to prevent body image, eating, and weight concerns. Journal of School Health, 72(5), 199–204. Keery, H., Shroff, H., Thompson, J. K., Wertheim, E., & Smolak, L. (2004a). The sociocultural internalization of appearance questionnaire - adolescents (SIAQ-A): Psychometric analysis and J Child Fam Stud (2018) 27:26–33 normative data for three countries. Eating and Weight Disorders, 9(1), 56–61. Keery, H., van, dB., & Thompson, J. K. (2004b). An evaluation of the tripartite influence model of body dissatisfaction and eating disturbance with adolescent girls. Body Image, 1, 237–251. https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.03.001. Kim, N.-H., Kim, K. H., & Kim, S.-H. (2015). The influence of sociocultural pressure of being slim on restrained eating attitude and eating behavior: The effect of body dissatisfaction and mindfulness. The Korean Journal of Health Psychology, 20(3), 527–546. Kim, J.-M., & Park, K.-M. (2008). The effects of eating disorder belief and sociocultural attitudes towards appearance on female high school students’ eating attitude. Journal of Korean Management Association, 26(3), 65–77. Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. NY: Guilford. Krane, V., Stiles-Shipley, J. A., Waldron, J., & Michalenok, J. (2001). Relationships among body satisfaction, social physique anxiety, and eating behaviors in female athletes and exercisers. Journal of Sport Behavior, 24, 247–264. Krumpal, I. (2013). Determinants of social desirability bias in sensitive surveys: A literature review. Quality & Quantity, 47, 2025–2047. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11135-011-9640-9. Lawler, M., & Nixon, E. (2011). Body dissatisfaction among adolescent boys and girls: The effects of body mass, peer appearance culture and internalization of appearance ideals. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(1), 59–71. Lee, H. K., & Choo, H. J. (2015). Daily outfit satisfaction: The effects of self and others’ evaluation on satisfaction with what I wear today. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 39(3), 261–268. Lee, S.-Y., & Park, Y. S. (2008). Relationship of sociocultural attitude towards appearance, body esteem, eating attitude, depression, and suicidal ideation among Korean female adolescents. The Korean Society of Stress Medicine, 16(4), 325–333. Luyckx, K., Klimstra, T. A., Duriez, B., Van Petegem, S., & Beyers, W. (2013). Personal identity processes from adolescence through the late 20s: Age trends, functionality, and depressive symptoms. Social Development, 22, 701–721. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode. 12027. McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2001). The structure of the perceived sociocultural influences on body image and body change questionnaire. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 8(1), 19–41. McDonald, R. P., & Ho, M.-H. (2002). Principles and practice in reporting structural equation analysis. Psychological Methods, 7, 64–82. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.64. Park, I. P., Yoo, J.-M., & Kim, C.-S. (2016). The effect of overexposure in the mass media’s to emphasize the appearance on body images and self-esteem of female adolescents. Korean Journal of Youth Studies, 23(7), 167–187. Piran, N. (2002). Embodiment: A mosaic of inquiries in the area of body weight and shape preoccupation. In S. M. Abbey (ed.), Ways of knowing in and through the body: Diverse perspectives on embodiment (pp. 211–2014). Welland, Ontario: Soliel. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879. Rodgers, R. F., McLean, S. A., & Paxton, S. J. (2015). Longitudinal relationships among internalization of the media ideal, peer social comparison, and body dissatisfaction: Implications for the tripartite influence model. Developmental Psychology, 51, 706–713. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000013. J Child Fam Stud (2018) 27:26–33 Salafia, E. B., & Gondoli, D. M. (2011). A 4-year longitudinal investigation of the processes by which parents and peers influence the development of early adolescent girls’ bulimic symptoms. The Journal Of Early Adolescence, 31, 390–414. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0272431610366248. Shin, K., You, S., & Kim, E. (2016). Sociocultural pressure, internalization, BMI, exercise, and body dissatisfaction in Korean female college students. Journal of Health Psychology, 1359105316634450. Shroff, H., & Thompson, J. K. (2006). The tripartite influence model of body image and eating disturbance: A replication with adolescent girls. Body Image, 3, 17–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. bodyim.2005.10.004. Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommendations. Psychological Methods, 7(4), 422–445. Slater, A., & Tiggemann, M. (2006). The contribution of physical activity and media use during childhood and adolescence to adult women's body image. Journal of Health Psychology, 11(4), 553–565. Slater, A., & Tiggemann, M. (2015). Media exposure, extracurricular activities, and appearance-related comments as predictors of female adolescents’ self-objectification. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 39(3), 375–389. Song, K. J., & Lee, M. S. (2009). The effects of sociocultural attitude toward appearance on perceptual, attitudinal body images and 33 clothing behavior. Journal of the Korean Home Economics Association, 47(2), 97–110. Steiger, J.H., & Lind, J.C. (1980, May). Statistically based tests for the number of common factors. In the annual meeting of the Psychometric Society, Iowa City, IA (Vol. 758). Thompson, J. K., van, dB., Roehrig, M., Guarda, A. S., & Heinberg, L. J. (2004). The sociocultural attitudes towards appearance scale-3 (SATAQ-3): Development and validation. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35, 293–304. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.10257. Vartanian, L. R., & Novak, S. A. (2011). Internalized societal attitudes moderate the impact of weight stigma on avoidance of exercise. Obesity, 19, 757–762. Wee, E. (2015). A study on differences in body comparison, body satisfaction, and appearance management behaviors according to social cultural attitudes toward appearance among adolescents in Gwangju and Jeonnam province - by sex and age. Korean Home Economics Education Association, 27(1), 13–29. You, S., Shin, K., & Kim, A.Y. (2016). Body image, self-esteem, and depression in Korean adolescents. Child Indicators Research, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-016-9385-z. Zimmermann, P., & Iwanski, A. (2014). Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood age differences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental variations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 182–194. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025413515405. Journal of Child & Family Studies is a copyright of Springer, 2018. All Rights Reserved.