598200 research-article2015 EABXXX10.1177/0013916515598200Environment and BehaviorHipp et al. Article The Relationship Between Perceived Greenness and Perceived Restorativeness of University Campuses and Student-Reported Quality of Life Environment and Behavior 2016, Vol. 48(10) 1292­–1308 © 2015 SAGE Publications Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0013916515598200 eab.sagepub.com J. Aaron Hipp1, Gowri Betrabet Gulwadi2, Susana Alves3, and Sonia Sequeira4 Abstract University students are exposed to many stressors, necessitating opportunities for restoration. Research has indicated that actual experiences in nearby green spaces are associated with restorative psychological and physiological health benefits. However, the perception of greenness and restorativeness of environments might also impact health outcomes. Can green campus spaces provide restorative potential to university students? Do students perceive the greenness and restorative benefits? To explore these questions, students at three universities (convenience sample) were surveyed with items on perceived greenness of campus, perceived restorativeness of campus, and the World Health Organization Qualityof-Life Scale. Results indicate that those with higher perceived campus greenness report greater quality of life, a pathway significantly and partially 1North Carolina State University, Raleigh, NC, USA of Northern Iowa, Cedar Falls, USA 3Okan University, İstanbul, Turkey 4Washington University in St. Louis, MO, USA 2University Corresponding Author: J. Aaron Hipp, Department of Parks, Recreation, and Tourism Management, North Carolina State University, 5124 Jordan Hall, Campus Box 8004, Raleigh, NC 27695, USA. Email: jahipp@ncsu.edu Hipp et al. 1293 mediated by perceived campus restorativeness. Future research should help identify effective ways in which university green spaces can be developed as health resources for students. Keywords university, campus, green space, quality of life, perceived restorativeness, students Introduction Proximity and access to green spaces are associated with perceived psychological restoration and enhanced health outcomes (Korpela, Ylen, Tyrväinen, & Silvennoinen, 2010; Stigsdotter & Grahn, 2011; van den Berg, Maas, Verheij, & Groenewegen, 2010). Restoration, or restorativeness, refers to the personal psychological restoration following demands to one’s attention and faculties. An environment is restorative if it enables psychological or physiological restoration from depleted attention and faculties. The perception of green space has a positive effect on perceived stress and psychological restoration. For example, Sugiyama, Leslie, Giles-Corti, and Owen (2008) have shown that perceived neighborhood greenness is associated with perceived physical and mental health. Young adults who attend universities for the first prolonged period of time away from home have high potential for stress and negative health impacts and hence high need for restoration (Atri, Sharma, & Cottrell, 2006; Dyson & Renk, 2006; Rawson, Bloomer, & Kendall, 1994). Many university campuses have green spaces integrated into their grounds, including grassy fields and turf, manicured flower beds, trees and wooded areas, and other landscaped spaces available to the student body. The present study explores whether there are associations between students’ perceptions of campus greenness, campus restorativeness, and quality of life (QOL). We hypothesize that perceived campus greenness is related to student perceived QOL and that this relationship is mediated by perceived campus restorativeness. College enrollment in the United States was 21 million in Fall 2010 and projected to increase by 15% through Fall 2020 (National Center for Education Statistics, 2015). Many university students (on average 18 years of age as first-year students) leave their home environment for the first time and do so for a prolonged period to pursue their higher education degree. This change in environment, combined with academic, social, and financial pressures, can be stress-producing for “emerging adults” (Arnett, 2000; Dyson & Renk, 2006) and impact the physical health and wellness of students (Rawson 1294 Environment and Behavior 48(10) Perceived Greenness Direct pathway: β = .21, p<.001 Mediated pathway: β = .13, p = .03 Student Quality of Life β = .21, p<.001 β = .50, p<.001 Perceived Restoraveness Figure 1. Tested pathways. et al., 1994). Specifically, loneliness, loss of social status, and unfamiliarity with customs of new places/land can cause stress (Atri et al., 2006). Higher levels of change-related stress can cause increased depressive symptoms (Dyson & Renk, 2006). As student success is heavily dependent on academic performance (e.g., grade point average), the university years are a vulnerable time for these emerging adults who are facing many new experiences while trying to maintain their health. There are many dimensions of wellness; not all are behavior-based (Adams, Bezner, Drabbs, Zambarano, & Steinhardt, 2000). The built environment of a university campus, which the student interacts with daily, may serve as a health-promoting resource but as such has been understudied. The everyday physical environment that surrounds the students may provide health-supporting interactions that if identified and maintained could enhance student campus experiences. Research on stress-reducing experiences in natural settings has shown that access to green spaces helps facilitate psychological restoration and reduces perceived stress (Korpela & Hartig, 1996). Physical attributes such as greenness and natural content in these settings are associated with perceived and actual restoration (Hipp & Ogunseitan, 2011; Jorgensen & Gobster, 2010; Kaplan, 2001; Ulrich et al., 1991). Exposure and access to green spaces and neighborhood vegetation have positive relationships with physical activity (Coombes, Jones, & Hillsdon, 2010; Giles-Corti et al., 2005), social support (Felsten, 2009; Kuo, Bacaicoa, & Sullivan, 1998; Kuo, Sullivan, Coley, & Brunson, 1998; Maas, van Dillen, Verheij, & Groenewegen, 2009), attention span and ability to focus (Arbogast, Kane, Kirwan, & Hertel, 2009), and stress mitigation (Fan, Das, & Chen, 2011). Specifically, and of relevance to Hipp et al. 1295 this study, is the association between natural green settings and greater perceived restorativeness (Kaplan, 2001). Most universities offer some kind of green space to their often multicultural and diverse students. As of 2014, more than 225 campuses were listed as a “Tree Campus USA” by the U.S. Arbor Day Foundation indicating that universities invest time and money, and place much pride in maintaining a green campus (http://www.arborday.org/programs/treecampususa/campuses. cfm). Demanding and heavy-class schedules, coursework, and extracurricular jobs of university students may result in time mostly spent indoors without convenient and frequent access to green space on campus, even if such spaces exist. However, the perception of greenness may be associated with the perception that the surroundings offer restorative experiences. For example, having a tree-lined walking trail on campus might enhance a student’s perception of greenness and the perceived restorativeness of the campus. Although many studies on restorative environments use student participants, few have addressed university-specific green spaces (McFarland, Waliczek, & Zajicek, 2008; Ogunseitan, 2005). On a campus, green spaces form a part of a student’s holistic experience (Kuh, 2011). The impact of campus green space has been studied with a few undergraduate populations. McFarland and colleagues (2008) showed a link between frequency of using campus green space (e.g., walking to class) and self-reported affective and cognitive QOL. The content and greenness of the view from indoors on campus can influence the perceived restorativeness of indoor settings (Felsten, 2009). At a U.K. university, researchers found that perception and use of campus green spaces were associated with perceived quality and design of the green space (Speake, Edmondson, & Nawaz, 2013). Also, a recent U.S. photovoice study found that undergraduate students preferred natural areas or natural areas with some built aspects around their campus over other types of campus spaces (Seitz, Reese, Strack, Frantz, & West, 2013). In a nonstudent population, green space serves as a buffer against the negative effects of stressful life events (van den Berg et al., 2010). Research suggests that vulnerable populations are more likely to seek out green restorative environments for resilience (Tidball, 2012); thus, a restorative environment could be considered a health resource (Hartig, 2007) for college students experiencing stress. There is a clear need for restorative and quality green spaces for students (Banning, Clemons, McKelfresh, & Gibbs, 2010). Provided this background, we hypothesized a significant relationship between perceived greenness of campus environments and self-reported QOL, but that this relationship is mediated by perceived restorativeness of the campus environment. 1296 Environment and Behavior 48(10) Method Location Three universities (convenience sample) with green campus spaces were selected for the present study to provide some degree of generalizability and because each provided sufficient green space for exploring perceived greenness. Two universities are located in separate states in the Midwestern United States, approximately 350 miles apart on a north–south plane. University A, established in 1900, hosted the online survey and data collection for all three university data collection procedures. University A (14,067 full- and parttime undergraduate and graduate students) is a Tree Campus USA with a large urban campus located in a major U.S. city. To be designated a Tree Campus, universities must meet five criteria including maintaining a tree advisory committee, care plan, dedicated budget, and service learning projects (http://www.arborday.org/programs/treecampususa/). The green spaces are a mix of tree-lined alleys, large campus quadrangles (quads) with maintained lawns, and manicured and maintained perennial plantings along building edges. University B, established in 1876 (13,200 students), features a large campus in a medium-sized city. The green spaces include a prairie preserve, walking and hiking trails, clusters of trees close to campus buildings, treelined streets, and open green space. The third University (C) established in 1907 (1,685 students) is located in a metropolitan city in Scotland. This was the smallest university and most urban campus of the three. Green spaces here included tree-lined lawn quads, abutting a large city park. Measures Following rigorous pilot testing and cognitive interviewing with students at University A, an online questionnaire and its paper version were created. The questionnaire covered demographic characteristics (gender and age), current ZIP and postal codes, whether the student lived on or off campus, current student academic status (perceived grade point average and year of study), and number of enrolled credits. The questionnaire included the World Health Organization’s Quality of Life Brief survey (Skevington, Lotfy, & O’Connell, 2004) with 26 items pertaining to physical (e.g., How well are you able to get around?), environmental (e.g., To what extent do you have the opportunity for leisure activities; How healthy is your physical environment?), psychological (e.g., How well are you able to concentrate?), and social (e.g., How satisfied are you with the support you get from your friends?) domains of QOL. Also included was Sugiyama et al.’s (2008) 11-item Perceived Greenness Scale. Hipp et al. 1297 Students completed this scale for their campus environment. Example items from the scale include the following: “My campus environment includes: (1) Many street trees; (2) Sports fields; and (3) Grassed areas.” Answers were on a 5-point Likert-type scale from strongly disagree to strongly agree. Finally, respondents were asked to complete the 26-item Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS; Hartig, Korpela, Evans, & Garling, 1997) for their university environment with items pertaining to being away, fascination, extent, and compatibility. An example item from the scale was “Spending time here gives me a break from my day-to-day routine.” Answers were on a 7-point Likerttype scale from 0 = not at all to 6 = very much. Procedure The online questionnaire was administered in two academic semesters (at the end of Spring 2011 and Fall 2011) after approval by the Institutional Review Board at each university. The sample procedure was a modified snowball sample. Specific student groups and student Listservs were targeted and asked to send the survey link via email to their members. To ensure that only students answered the survey, an email address with the suffix .edu specific to the three universities had to be entered to gain access to the survey. Survey respondents at each university were entered in a drawing for a US$50 iTunes gift card. Due to a low response rate, University B also provided hard copies of the survey during Fall 2011. Respondent demographics did not statistically differ between the two semesters and survey procedures at University B. Data Analysis The following methods of data analyses were conducted to answer the research questions. A bivariate correlation analysis was run for the following variables: PRS, perceived greenness, and each of the QOL domains (physical, psychological, social, and environmental). The second step was to test mediation models. We adjusted all regression models for gender (male/female), age (continuous), university (A/B/C), undergraduate versus graduate student status, and living on or off campus. A linear regression analysis between perceived greenness and QOL and each of its constructs (physical, social, and psychological well-being) was tested. A regression between perceived greenness and perceived restorativeness and its constructs (being away, fascination, coherence, and compatibility) was completed to find the relationship between perceived greenness and perceived restorativeness. Then a regression was tested between perceived restorativeness and 1298 Environment and Behavior 48(10) Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations of Perceived Greenness, Quality of Life, and Perceived Restorativeness by University and With Aggregate Data. University A (n = 305-308) Perceived Greenness WHOQOL-BREF Total Physical Psychological Social Environmental PRS Total Being away Fascination Coherence Compatibility University B University C Aggregate (n = 84-85) (n = 45-46) (N = 437-441) 2.94 (0.51) 2.98 (0.60) 1.87 (0.53) 2.84 (0.62) 3.70 (0.46) 16.0 (2.12) 14.8 (2.37) 18.2 (5.52) 17.3 (3.70) 3.98 (0.47) 16.4 (2.37) 15.7 (2.21) 19.3 (5.28) 18.9 (3.76) 3.40 (0.40) 15.0 (3.06) 13.4 (2.45) 13.9 (5.62) 13.9 (3.23) 3.72 (0.48) 16.0 (2.30) 14.8 (2.41) 17.9 (5.64) 17.2 (3.87) 3.64 (0.94) 2.83 (1.53) 3.57 (1.19) 4.49 (1.16) 3.78 (1.18) 3.76 (0.90) 2.93 (1.32) 3.53 (1.24) 4.46 (1.02) 4.12 (1.09) 2.95 (0.96) 2.42 (1.61) 2.29 (1.33) 4.69 (1.15) 3.08 (1.00) 3.59 (0.96) 2.80 (1.50) 3.43 (1.27) 4.50 (1.13) 3.77 (1.17) Note. PRS = Perceived Restorativeness Scale; WHOQOL-BREF = World Health Organization Quality of Life-Brief. its constructs and QOL and its constructs. Finally, Sobel tests for statistically significant indirect effects, or mediation, were completed with perceived greenness as the independent variable and QOL and constructs as dependent variables (Sobel, 1982). The Sobel test (t statistic and p value) statistically compares the indirect effect of the mediator (perceived restorativeness and its constructs) against the null hypothesis of no effect. Results Across the two semesters, there were 439 respondents (308, 69.8% from University A; 85, 19.3% from University B; and 46, 10.4% from University C). Online Appendix Table 1 provides basic demographics and residential information regarding the respondents. Undergraduates represented 55.6% of the sample, the average age was 23.6 years, and 78.2% of respondents were female. The respondents differed across the three university campuses as did their environments (see Table 1); thus, the home university was controlled in regression and mediation analyses. For instance, University C had the oldest average respondents and University A had the respondents with highest perceived academic standing and provided a sample with more graduate 1299 Hipp et al. Table 2. Regression Results. QOL Female −0.05 Age −0.10 University B (University 0.19** A comparison) University C −0.07 Undergraduate 0.06 Live on campus 0.01 Perceived green 0.21*** n 427 .15 Adjusted R2 Physical Psychological Social Environmental QOL QOL QOL QOL −0.04 −0.13* 0.08 −0.07 −0.02 0.14* 0.07 −0.14* −0.02 −0.07 −0.11 0.02 −0.04 −0.05 −0.10 0.13* 425 .04 −0.08 −0.01 −0.01 0.18** 425 .07 −0.12 0.13 0.02 0.18* 287 .12 −0.17* 0.18* 0.01 0.24*** 332 .20 Note. The effect of perceived greenness on student quality of life (standardized beta coefficients). QOL = quality of life. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. students. University C respondents were more likely to have children in the home, compared with both universities located in the United States, and University C respondents had significantly more roommates than University A, but not University B. Overall students at Universities A and B perceived that their universities offered relatively restorative environments (Table 1). The average PRS score for University B was 3.76 (SD = 0.90; range between 0, “Not at all Restorative” and 6, “Completely Restorative”; 3 equates to “Neither little nor much” restorativeness and 4, “Rather much” restorativeness), and 3.64 (0.94) for University A. University C’s PRS score was 2.95 (0.96). Without adjustment, the key variables of interest were significantly correlated. Perceived greenness was correlated with QOL (r = .30, p < .001) and perceived restorativeness (r = .53, p < .001). QOL was significantly correlated with perceived restorativeness (r = .30, p < .001). Regression results between perceived greenness of campus and student QOL can be found in Table 2. After adjusting for demographics found to be significantly associated with perceived greenness and/or QOL (gender, age, university, undergraduate/graduate student, and living on or off campus), perceived greenness was significantly associated with student QOL. The beta coefficient and full model (adjusted R2) were highest for environmental QOL (e.g., “How healthy is your physical environment?”) and lowest for physical QOL (e.g., “How satisfied are you with your capacity for work?”). Table 3 provides the regression results between perceived campus 1300 Environment and Behavior 48(10) Table 3. Regression Results. QOL Female −0.05 Age −0.09 University B (University 0.20*** A comparison) University C −0.14** Undergraduate 0.04 Live on campus 0.01 PRS 0.21*** n 427 .16 Adjusted R2 Physical Psychological QOL QOL Social QOL −0.04 −0.13* 0.08 −0.06 −0.01 0.15** 0.09* 0.02 −0.04 −0.09 −0.06 −0.11 0.10 425 .03 −0.13* −0.11* −0.02 0.06 −0.03 0.01 0.18*** 0.70*** 425 287 .08 .54 Environmental QOL −0.07 −0.11* −0.09 −0.22*** 0.04 −0.01 0.66*** 332 .55 Note. The effect of perceived restorativeness (PRS) on student quality of life (standardized beta coefficients). QOL = quality of life; PRS = Perceived Restorativeness Scale. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. environment restorativeness and student QOL. Perceived campus environment restorativeness was significantly associated with student QOL with social and environmental QOL most associated with perceived restorativeness (ΔR2 = .44 and .39, respectively). The third regression tested (Table 4), for purposes of mediation analysis, was the association of perceived campus greenness with perceived campus restorativeness. Perceived campus greenness was significantly associated with perceived campus restorativeness. Perceived campus greenness was highly associated with three of the four restorativeness constructs—being away, fascination, and compatibility (ΔR2 > .11), but not with coherence. Sobel test results (Table 5) indicate that the relationship between perceived campus environment greenness and student QOL is significantly mediated by perceived campus restorativeness. Within perceived restorativeness constructs, being away, fascination, and compatibility significantly mediated the relationship between perceived greenness and social and environmental QOL. Discussion Students with higher perceived campus greenness were more likely to selfreport a higher QOL; however, this relationship was significantly mediated by perceived restorativeness. This mediation suggests that perceiving the campus environment as restorative is a significant step in the relationship 1301 Hipp et al. Table 4. Regression Results. Being away PRS Fascination Coherence Compatibility Female 0.01 −0.05 Age −0.04 0.07 University B (University −0.06 −0.04 A comparison) University C 0.02 0.12 Undergraduate 0.22*** 0.14* Live on campus 0.05 0.12 Perceived greenness 0.50*** 0.42*** n 427 427 .33 .20 Adjusted R2 0.02 −0.07 −0.09 0.07 −0.06 −0.05 −0.01 −0.07 −0.02 −0.09 0.13 0.02 0.43*** 426 .28 0.13 0.08 −0.09 0.18** 425 .03 −0.01 0.27*** 0.05 0.39*** 425 .30 Note. The effect of perceived green on perceived restorativeness (PRS; standardized beta coefficients). PRS = Perceived Restorativeness Scale. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Table 5. Sobel Mediation Analyses (t Statistic). Dependent variable QOL Mediating variable PRS Being away Fascination Coherence Compatibility 3.46*** 1.36 0.90 1.48 4.96*** Physical QOL Psychological QOL Social QOL Environmental QOL 0.74 0.57 0.28 1.80 1.76 2.23* 0.80 0.54 1.31 3.46*** 10.13*** 6.05*** 6.83*** 0.23 9.36*** 10.01*** 5.95*** 8.57*** 1.67 8.63*** Note. Summary of 25 separate mediation analyses with significant indirect effects noted. Perceived greenness is the independent variable (see Figure 1). For example, perceived restorativeness mediated the relationship between perceived greenness and student quality of life. Analyses adjusted for the following variables: gender, age, university, undergraduate/graduate student, living on campus/not. QOL = quality of life; PRS = Perceived Restorativeness Scale. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. between perceiving the campus as green and rating a higher QOL. That is, it is perhaps not perceived greenness that leads to greater QOL, but that perceived greenness offers opportunities for restoration that lead directly to improved QOL. 1302 Environment and Behavior 48(10) Multiple studies have shown that green environments are associated with QOL (Chiesura, 2004; Stigsdotter et al., 2010), green environments are more restorative than urban or less green environments (Carrus et al., 2015; Honold, Lakes, Beyer, & van der Meer, 2016; Tyrväinen et al., 2014), and restorative environments have health benefits including QOL (HansenKetchum & Halpenny, 2010; Hartig & Evans, 1993). Here, we hypothesized that, and our data support, the pathway between perceived greenness and QOL is significantly mediated, or in part explained, by perceived restorativeness. This is an important finding, especially with a student population on a campus environment as students are highly vulnerable to attention fatigue and stress (Atri et al., 2006; Dyson & Renk, 2006; Rawson et al., 1994). Although with middle school students and not university students, a quasiexperimental study by Kelz, Evans, and Röderer (2015) showed that an improved schoolyard (more greenery, hedges, and potted plants) increased both perceived restorativeness and self-reported psychological well-being. Our correlational study did not examine an increase in greenness, rather examined how students perceive existing greenness. Our findings indicate that existing and available green campus areas, when perceived as green by students, have the potential to promote perceived psychological restorativeness that in turn has the potential to increase the QOL. The physical health domain of the World Health Organization Quality of Life Scale was not significantly associated with perceived restorativeness and was not significantly mediated by perceived restorativeness along the pathway between perceived greenness to QOL. This domain includes questions such as “To what extent do you feel that physical pain prevents you from doing what you need to do?”; “How much do you need any medical treatment to function in your daily life?”; and “How well are you able to get around?” Given we were interested in psychological restoration, it is unsurprising that a physical health scale with questions on physical function and ability was found unrelated to perceived restorativeness of the environment. The remaining three domains—psychological, social, and environmental QOL—were each significantly associated with perceived restorativeness. Items from these three domains inquire about positive feelings, negative feelings, self-esteem, social support, feelings of safety, information, leisure opportunities, and health of the physical environment (Skevington et al., 2004). Stress-reducing environments (green, restorative campus) signal opportunities for leisure activities and social contact and thus contribute to QOL. Perceiving a green and restorative environment should be associated with the specific subdomains tested here, including positive feelings, negative feelings, perceived social support, opportunities and participation in recreation and leisure activities, and environmental quality. Hipp et al. 1303 The mean perceived restorativeness scores of campuses in this study are similar to the mean perceived restorativeness scores of natural environments (e.g., lakes, mountains, and so on; Tenngart-Ivarsson & Hagerhall, 2008). There are positive outcomes of attending green campuses. Perceived greenness is significantly associated with perceived restorativeness and social and environmental well-being. University campuses spend a lot of money on greening and maintaining the campus to recruit new students. Although many universities provide and maintain green and natural campus environments that students, staff, and faculty have direct and easy access to, where is the green located? Is it mainly in the “frontyard” where newly recruited students arrive, or along campus tour routes, or are they integrated into the overall campus design where they can be seen and experienced from students’ study, work, and rest spaces? The campus becomes a home for at least 2 to 4 years for students who are in the midst of making a stressful life transition. Understanding student perceptions of greenness and their varied responses to available campus green space will assist future researchers and designers in understanding, managing, and highlighting green spaces on university campuses as a health resource for a diverse student body. For instance, Lau and Yang (2009) showed how concepts from the design of healing gardens could be successfully applied to a compact campus design in Hong Kong. The findings in this study indicate that availability of green campus space enhances the QOL by enhancing the perception of greenness and impacting opportunities for restoration. Hence, this study makes a specific conceptual contribution by considering the university campus environment as a health resource for its students. The results will be useful to environmental psychologists who study the processes of stress and restoration, to architects and designers who prioritize and apply design features in green campus spaces, and to the campus community who can find ways to educate students regarding their green health resource. Limitations In this study, students from three campuses in three different places (with differences in campus greenness and vegetation) were surveyed. The sample sizes and response rate varied across the campuses. Although the results are encouraging and point to future branches of this research, they cannot be generalized. With larger sample sizes, and more control on variables, future studies can adopt a sharper lens to examine associations between greenness and health outcomes. For example, in this study, students self-reported their QOL and 1304 Environment and Behavior 48(10) restoration; additional studies could explore other health measures such as number of times they visited the student health center. There are also limitations associated with three self-reported scales: perceived greenness, perceived restorativeness, and self-reported QOL. It is possible that those students with greater affect self-reported higher scores on each of the three scales and those with lower affect self-reported lower scores. Beyond the 11-item perceived greenness scale, students did not have to identify or specify green areas on campus. We are unable to nominally separate the undergraduate student whose dormitory room looks over a grassy field and whose commute to the majority of classes crosses a quad from an undergraduate student whose dormitory faces another building and commutes along a trafficked road. We believe such lifespaces result in the range of perceived greenness, perceived restorativeness, and QOL scores across campuses, but beyond controlling for university and living on/off campus, we cannot assume the potential exposures beyond what is self-reported. Finally, as with all cross-sectional studies, correlations and significant regression models do not make for causal pathways. Conclusion This study examined links between student perception of campus greenness, campus restorativeness, and QOL indicators. Future studies can build on these findings and explore the impact of specific design attributes and affordances on campus for groups of students with different restorative needs. For example, design students work longer hours in studio spaces using a variety of media (e.g., drawings, computer graphics, models) when compared with nondesign students working on a study paper. Are their restorative needs the same or different? Would outdoor classrooms and instructional spaces work to preserve health of students in either case? Would creating more orchestrated and spontaneous opportunities for actually experiencing green spaces (e.g., walking the trails or spending some time on campus green) within a typical college day encourage more healthy behaviors among students? With this study, we open rich possibilities for future research and its application. Acknowledgments The authors would like to think Kenneth Chiang, MPH, for his assistance with survey development and implementation. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Hipp et al. 1305 Funding The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. References Adams, T. B., Bezner, J. R., Drabbs, M. E., Zambarano, R. J., & Steinhardt, M. A. (2000). Conceptualization and measurement of the spiritual and psychological dimensions of wellness in a college population. Journal of American College Health, 48(4), 165-173. doi:10.1080/07448480009595692 Arbogast, K., Kane, B., Kirwan, J., & Hertel, B. (2009). Vegetation and outdoor recess time at elementary schools: What are the connections? Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29, 450-456. Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. The American Psychologist, 55, 469-480. Atri, A., Sharma, M., & Cottrell, R. (2006). Role of social support, hardiness, and acculturation as predictors of mental health among international students of Asian Indian origin. International Quarterly of Community Health Education, 27, 59-73. doi:10.2190/IQ.27.1.e Banning, J. H., Clemons, S., McKelfresh, D., & Gibbs, R. W. (2010). Special places for students: Third place and restorative place. College Student Journal, 44(4), 906-912. Carrus, G., Scopelliti, M., Lafortezza, R., Colangelo, G., Ferrini, F., Salbitano, F., . . .Sanesi, G. (2015). Go greener, feel better? The positive effects of biodiversity on the well-being of individuals visiting urban and peri-urban green areas. Landscape and Urban Planning, 134, 221-228. Retrieved from http://dx.doi. org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2014.10.022 Chiesura, A. (2004). The role of urban parks for the sustainable city. Landscape and Urban Planning, 68, 129-138. doi:10.1016/j.landurbplan.2003.08.003 Coombes, E., Jones, A. P., & Hillsdon, M. (2010). The relationship of physical activity and overweight to objectively measured green space accessibility and use. Social Science & Medicine, 70, 816-822. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.11.020 Dyson, R., & Renk, K. (2006). Freshmen adaptation to university life: Depressive symptoms, stress, and coping. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 1231-1244. doi:10.1002/jclp.20295 Fan, Y., Das, K. V., & Chen, Q. (2011). Neighborhood green, social support, physical activity, and stress: Assessing the cumulative impact. Health & Place, 17, 1202-1211. Felsten, G. (2009). Where to take a study break on the college campus: An attention restoration theory perspective. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29, 160-167. Giles-Corti, B., Broomhall, M. H., Knuiman, M., Collins, C., Douglas, K., Ng, K., . . .Donovan, R. J. (2005). Increasing walking: How important is distance to, attractiveness, and size of public open space? American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 28(2 Suppl. 2), 169-176. 1306 Environment and Behavior 48(10) Hansen-Ketchum, P. A., & Halpenny, E. A. (2010). Engaging with nature to promote health: Bridging research silos to examine the evidence. Health Promotion International, 26, 100-108. doi:10.1093/heapro/daq053 Hartig, T. (2007). Three steps to understanding restorative environments as health resources. In C. W. Thompson & P. Travlou (Eds.), Open space: People space (pp.163-180). London, England: Taylor & Francis. Hartig, T., & Evans, G. W. (1993). Psychological foundations of nature experience. In T. Garling & R. G. Golledge (Eds.), Behavior and Environment: Psychological and Geographical Approaches (pp. 427-457). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Hartig, T., Korpela, K. M., Evans, G. W., & Garling, T. (1997). A measure of restorative quality in environments. Scandinavian Housing & Planning Research, 14, 175-194. Hipp, J. A., & Ogunseitan, O. A. (2011). Effect of environmental conditions on perceived psychological restorativeness of coastal parks. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 31, 421-429. Honold, J., Lakes, T., Beyer, R., & van der Meer, E. (2016). Restoration in urban spaces: Nature views from home, greenways, and public parks. Environment and Behavior, 48, 796-825. Jorgensen, A., & Gobster, P. H. (2010). Shades of green: Measuring the ecology of urban green space in the context of human health and well-being. Nature and Culture, 5, 338-363. Kaplan, R. (2001). The nature of the view from home: Psychological benefits. Environment and Behavior, 33, 507-542. Kelz, C., Evans, G. W., & Röderer, K. (2015). The restorative effects of redesigning the schoolyard: A multi-methodological, quasi-experimental study in rural Austrian middle schools. Environment and Behavior, 47, 119-139. doi:10.1177/0013916513510528 Korpela, K. M., & Hartig, T. (1996). Restorative qualities of favorite places. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 16, 221-233. Korpela, K. M., Ylen, M., Tyrväinen, L., & Silvennoinen, H. (2010). Favorite green, waterside and urban environments, restorative experiences and perceived health in Finland. Health Promotion International, 25, 200-209. doi:10.1093/heapro/ daq007 Kuh, G. D. (2009). Understanding campus environments. In G. S. McClellan & J. Stringer (Eds.), The handbook of student affairs administration (pp. 59-80) San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Kuo, F. E., Bacaicoa, M., & Sullivan, W. C. (1998). Transforming inner-city landscapes: Trees, sense of safety, and preference. Environment and Behavior, 30, 28-59. Kuo, F. E., Sullivan, W. C., Coley, R. L., & Brunson, L. (1998). Fertile ground for community: Inner-city neighborhood common spaces. American Journal of Community Psychology, 26, 823-851. Lau, S. S. Y., & Yang, F. (2009). Introducing healing gardens into a compact university campus: Design natural space to create healthy and sustainable campuses. Landscape Research, 34, 55-81. doi:10.1080/01426390801981720 Hipp et al. 1307 Maas, J., van Dillen, S. M., Verheij, R. A., & Groenewegen, P. P. (2009). Social contacts as a possible mechanism behind the relation between green space and health. Health Place, 15, 586-595. doi:10.1016/j.healthplace.2008.09.006 McFarland, A. L., Waliczek, T. M., & Zajicek, J. M. (2008). The relationship between student use of campus green spaces and perceptions of quality of life. HortTechnology, 18, 232-238. National Center for Education Statistics. (2015). Fast Facts. Washington, DC: Institute for Education Sciences. Retrieved from https://nces.ed.gov/fastfacts/ display.asp?id=98 Ogunseitan, O. A. (2005). Topophilia and the quality of life. Environmental Health Perspectives, 113(2), 143-148. Rawson, H. E., Bloomer, K., & Kendall, A. (1994). Stress, anxiety, depression, and physical illness in college students. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 155, 321-330. doi:10.1080/00221325.1994.9914782 Seitz, C. M., Reese, R. F., Strack, R. W., Frantz, S., & West, B. (2013). Identifying and improving green spaces on a college campus: A photovoice study. Ecopsychology, 6, 98-108. doi:10.1089/eco.2013.0103 Skevington, S. M., Lotfy, M., & O’Connell, K. A. (2004). The World Health Organization’s WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment: Psychometric properties and results of the international field trial. A report from the WHOQOL Group. Quality of Life Research, 13, 299-310. Sobel, M. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociological Methodology, 13, 290-312. Speake, J., Edmondson, S., & Nawaz, H. (2013). Everyday encounters with nature: Students’ perceptions and use of university campus green spaces. Human Geographies: Journal of Studies and Research in Human Geography, 7(1), 21-31. Stigsdotter, U. K., Ekholm, O., Schipperijn, J., Toftager, M., Kamper-Jørgensen, F., & Randrup, T. B. (2010). Health promoting outdoor environments—Associations between green space, and health, health-related quality of life and stress based on a Danish national representative survey. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health, 38, 411-417. doi:10.1177/1403494810367468 Stigsdotter, U. K., & Grahn, P. (2011). Stressed individuals’ preferences for activities and environmental characteristics in green spaces. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 10, 295-304. Sugiyama, T., Leslie, E., Giles-Corti, B., & Owen, N. (2008). Association of neighbourhood greenness with physical and mental health: Do walking, social coherence and local social interaction explain relationships? Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health, 62, e9. Tenngart-Ivarsson, C., & Hagerhall, C. M. (2008). The perceived restorativeness of gardens—Assessing the restorativeness of a mixed built and natural scene type. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 7, 107-118. Tidball, K. G. (2012). Urgent biophilia: Human-nature interactions and biological attractions in disaster resilience. Ecology and Society, 17(2), 5. 1308 Environment and Behavior 48(10) Tyrväinen, L., Ojala, A., Korpela, K., Lanki, T., Tsunetsugu, Y., & Kagawa, T. (2014). The influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: A field experiment. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 38(1), 1-9. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.12.005 Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. F., Losito, B. D., Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., & Zelson, M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 11, 201-230. van den Berg, A. E., Maas, J., Verheij, R. A., & Groenewegen, P. P. (2010). Green space as a buffer between stressful life events and health. Social Science & Medicine, 70, 1203-1210. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.01.002 Author Biographies J. Aaron Hipp is an associate professor of Community Health and Sustainability in the Department of Parks, Recreation, and Tourism Management at North Carolina State University. His research examines the role the built environment plays in community health. Gowri Betrabet Gulwadi is an associate professor of Interior Design in the School of Applied Human Sciences at the University of Northern Iowa. She has research and teaching interests in designs for health including the effect of lighting and green space on well-being. Susana Alves is an environmental psychologist and assistant professor at Okan University in Turkey, formerly at Edinburgh College of Arts and the OPENspace Research Centre. Her research examines how landscapes can be used to promote people’s health and psychological well-being. Sonia Sequeira is a graduate of the Brown School, Washington University, with a dual degree in public health and social work. Her primary interests are in addressing health disparities through environmental and policy interventions.