European Journal of Pain 11 (2007) 153–163 www.EuropeanJournalPain.com The pain self-efficacy questionnaire: Taking pain into account Michael K. Nicholas * Pain Management and Research Centre, University of Sydney at Royal North Shore Hospital, St. Leonards, Sydney, NSW 2065, Australia Received 19 July 2005; received in revised form 12 December 2005; accepted 14 December 2005 Available online 30 January 2006 Abstract Self-efficacy beliefs in people with chronic pain have been assessed either by reference to confidence in ability to perform specific tasks or to confidence in performing more generalised constructs like coping with pain. Both approaches reflect aspects of the original conceptualisations of self-efficacy and both have proved useful, but it is noteworthy that confidence in performing activities in the context of pain is rarely addressed. An important element in the original formulations of self-efficacy referred to persistence in the face of obstacles and aversive experiences. In this context, self-efficacy beliefs for people experiencing chronic pain might be expected to incorporate not just the expectation that a person could perform a particular behaviour or task, but also their confidence in being able to do it despite their pain. This aspect of the self-efficacy construct has been included in a measure for people with chronic pain, the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire (PSEQ). The accumulated evidence from a number of published studies and a confirmatory analysis with a large cohort of heterogeneous chronic pain patients attending a pain management program provide support for the PSEQ’s original psychometric properties developed with a sample of chronic low back pain patients. The importance of taking the context of pain into account in the assessment of self-efficacy beliefs in pain populations and the ways in which this measure can be used to improve the assessment of people experiencing chronic pain, before and after treatment, are examined. Ó 2005 European Federation of Chapters of the International Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Chronic pain; Self-efficacy; Disability; Coping; Pain management 1. Introduction Bandura (1977) proposed that ‘‘efficacy expectations determine how much effort people will expend and how long they will persist in the face of obstacles and aversive experiences’’ (p. 194). In the study of pain, efficacy expectations (or ‘self-efficacy’ beliefs) have been used to explain a range of behaviours and aspects of pain experience (e.g., Jensen et al., 1991; Lackner and Carosella, 1993; Anderson et al., 1995; Arnstein et al., 1999; Pincus and Morley, 2002; Turk, 2002a; Altmaier et al., 1993; Asghari and Nicholas, 2001; Rudy et al., 2003; Keefe et al., 2004). In experimentally induced * Tel.: +612 9926 7318; fax: +612 9926 6548. E-mail address: miken@med.usyd.edu.au. pain, stronger confidence in ability to tolerate pain has been found to predict actual tolerance, regardless of whether cognitive or pharmacological agents were employed to control pain (Bandura et al., 1987). In patients with chronic osteoarthritic knee pain, those with high self-efficacy for controlling arthritis pain have been found to have higher pain thresholds and tolerance for experimentally induced thermal pain than those whose self-efficacy was low (Keefe et al., 1997). Similarly, in patients with chronic pain, confidence in ability to perform specified activities has been correlated with the subsequent performance of those activities (Council et al., 1988). Bandura (1989) went beyond the idea that self-efficacy beliefs related only to specific behaviours and argued that people could hold efficacy beliefs about their 1090-3801/$32 Ó 2005 European Federation of Chapters of the International Association for the Study of Pain. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.12.008 154 M.K. Nicholas / European Journal of Pain 11 (2007) 153–163 ability to cope in the face of adversity. In this context he referred to strong self-efficacy beliefs as a ‘‘resilient selfbelief system’’ whereby ‘‘people who believe they can exercise control over potential threats do not conjure up apprehensive cognitions and, hence, are not perturbed by them’’ (p. 419). In support of this argument Bandura cited a path analysis study that demonstrated links between coping efficacy beliefs, avoidance behaviours and anxiety (Ozer and Bandura, 1990). The idea that self-efficacy beliefs may be related not only to specific behaviours, but also to broader constructs, like coping, has been reflected in many self-efficacy scales (e.g., Lackner and Carosella, 1993; Anderson et al., 1995; Edwards et al., 2000). However, the relationship between self-efficacy beliefs and coping may require further study in the light of the ongoing debate over the construct of ‘coping’ in the context of pain (e.g., Eccleston, 1995; Turner et al., 2000). There is a good case for distinguishing between the effect (coping) with the actions involved in coping. To date, this distinction has received relatively little attention in studies of coping self-efficacy and pain. While generally supportive of the thesis that self-efficacy beliefs held by people in chronic pain are related to their behaviour, the published findings on self-efficacy beliefs also reveal considerable variability in both the measures used to assess self-efficacy beliefs and in the relationships with behaviours and other aspects of pain experience. For example, some follow Bandura’s own methods and use single-item scales that focus on particular behaviours or tasks (e.g., Dolce et al., 1986; Rudy et al., 2003), while others have used more general measures with multiple items whose scores are summed to measure more generalised constructs like ‘ability to cope with pain’ (e.g., Lackner and Carosella, 1993; Anderson et al., 1995; Edwards et al., 2000; McCracken and Eccleston, 2003). Some scales have included a number of subscales or subcategories of self-efficacy. For example, Lackner et al. (1996) described a modification of Lorig et al.’s (1989) arthritis self-efficacy scale that has three self-efficacy subscales – confidence in performing a certain task, the ability to tolerate pain and to control pain. In addition, some researchers have reported that selfefficacy beliefs are predictive of physical function (e.g., Lackner et al., 1996), while others have found that improved physical functioning was not associated with increased self-efficacy, at least initially (Altmaier et al., 1993). Similarly, Dolce et al. (1986) found self-efficacy beliefs were enhanced in some chronic pain patients who increased their level of exercise, but not in all. Gibson and Strong (1996) reported that one measure of selfefficacy was a better predictor of assessed work capacity than another. It is unclear if the variable findings are due to the measures used to assess self-efficacy beliefs or to problems with the actual construct of self-efficacy in relation to pain. Somewhat surprisingly, most published measures of self-efficacy beliefs used in pain samples do not explicitly ask the patient to take their pain into account when describing their confidence in performing specific tasks (e.g., Lorig et al., 1989; Jensen et al., 1991; Lackner and Carosella, 1993; Anderson et al., 1995; Rudy et al., 2003). It is arguable that it is one thing to feel able to perform some task and another thing to feel able to do it while in pain. The significance of this point is that lasting relief from chronic pain is rarely achieved by current treatments (Turk, 2002b). Accordingly, most people with chronic pain are confronted with the challenge of leading as normal a life as possible despite their pain. By overlooking the context (i.e., the presence of pain) in which a person is being asked to estimate their confidence in performing an activity, it would also seem to make the task somewhat artificial and create difficulties in the interpretation of their responses. An analogous point has been described in relation to patients with generalised social phobia where the context or social framework of information on outcomes was found to influence patients’ anxiety predictions (Alden et al., 2004). One attempt to assess self-efficacy beliefs in the context of pain was described by Altmaier et al. (1993). But in contrast to the common finding from other selfefficacy scales, Altmaier et al. did not find a relationship between strengthened self-efficacy beliefs and increased physical functioning. Whether this was due to the nature of the scale itself or some aspect of taking pain into account was not clear. Examination of the scale’s items does reveal possible problems in its general utility. For instance, many of the items refer to specific activities that may not be relevant to all individuals or groups (e.g., shoveling snow; driving the car; raking leaves; working on a house repair; riding a bicycle). It is possible that a measure that does not refer to such specific activities might have broader applicability and test the importance of including the context of pain more effectively. Nicholas (1989) described another self-efficacy scale (the Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire; PSEQ) for people in chronic pain that also asks the respondents to take pain into account when rating their self-efficacy beliefs. Unlike the scale used by Altmaier et al. (1993), the activities referred to in the PSEQ are more general (e.g., paid/ unpaid work; social activities), although specific examples are given as a guide. This was intended to make the measure applicable to a broad range of respondents. The PSEQ has now been used in a number of different clinical settings and in different countries (e.g., Nicholas et al., 1992; Williams et al., 1993, 1996, 1999; Frost et al., 1993; Estlander et al., 1994; Ralphs et al., 1994; Coughlan et al., 1995; Gibson and Strong, 1996; Watson et al., 1997; Cohen et al., 2000; Asghari and Nicholas, 2001; Ayer and Tyson, 2001; Strong et al., 2002; Adams and Williams, 2003; Dehghani et al., 2004). To date, however, a full account of the PSEQ and its psychometric M.K. Nicholas / European Journal of Pain 11 (2007) 153–163 properties has not been published. This paper is intended to examine this body of work as it relates to the psychometric properties and uses of the PSEQ, as well as the contribution it could make to exploring the application of the construct of self-efficacy to understanding the impact of persisting pain, especially in relation to other constructs such as fear-avoidance and pain acceptance. 2. Method 2.1. Development of the scale and initial psychometric evaluation study 2.1.1. Item development Ten items were selected to reflect a wide variety of classes of activities and tasks, with indicative examples, commonly reported as problematic by patients with chronic pain (see Appendix A for a list of items). Some items were derived from existing scales (e.g., Pain Beliefs Questionnaire: Gottlieb, 1984) and modified to fit the requirements of the new scale. Other items were derived from the author’s experience in working with chronic pain patients. Most importantly, all items include mention of performing the activities despite their pain (e.g., ‘‘I can do most of the household chores (e.g., tidying-up, washing dishes), despite the pain’’; ‘‘I can gradually increase my activity level, despite the pain’’). The items were tested for comprehension by patients attending the pain clinic and modified until all items were considered comprehensible. Respondents are asked to rate how confident they are that they can do each of the 10 activities or tasks at present despite the pain they are experiencing. Each item is rated by selecting a number on a 7-point scale, where 0 equals ‘‘not at all confident’’ and 6 equals ‘‘completely confident’’. A total score is calculated by summing the scores for each of the 10 items, yielding a maximum possible score of 60. Higher scores reflect stronger self-efficacy beliefs. Once an acceptable version was derived, more formal assessment of its properties began when the PSEQ was included in a battery of scales for use with a sample of chronic low back pain patients selected for a randomised trial of cognitive-behavioural pain management (see Nicholas et al., 1992). 2.1.2. Original Westmead subjects The original (Westmead) sample on which the PSEQ was tested comprised a consecutive series of 103 patients with chronic low back pain attending a pain management program at Westmead Hospital in Sydney (Australia), a large city teaching hospital. The 103 subjects consisted of an initial series (of 18 patients) who completed only the PSEQ, and a subsequent series of 85 patients (who completed the PSEQ as part of a battery of measures – see below). After multidisciplinary pain clinic assessment 155 all 103 subjects had been accepted into the program on the basis that they met the criteria of a history of chronic (i.e., more than 6-months) low-back pain; were not considered suitable for further invasive treatments; were aged between 18 and 60 years; had no compensation claim due for settlement within 12-months; were able to read and speak English; and were willing to participate in a research-based treatment program. The study was approved by the hospital’s ethics committee and conducted from May 1986 to February 1988. The mean age of the 103 subjects selected (50 men, 53 women) was 42.0 years (range = 18–60). The median duration of low back pain was 36 months (interquartile range: 24–120 months). Forty-four (42.3%) patients had had one or more back surgeries. One hundred and one (97%) had had some type of physiotherapy. Thirty-five (33.7%) had had one or more nerve blocks. Most patients reported having tried a range of medications and 94 (90.4%) were taking one or more pain-related medications at the pretreatment assessment. Seventeen (16.3%) were employed and 71 (68.3%) were claiming compensation insurance payments. Twenty-six (25%) had had more than three years of secondary education. At the time of initial assessment all patients were continuing to seek treatment for their pain conditions. 2.1.3. Psychometric evaluation The reliability and validity of the PSEQ were initially examined using data gathered at the pre-treatment assessments for the pain management program. Reliability was assessed by examination of internal consistency (Cronbach’s a coefficient) and stability over time (test–retest analysis with Pearson correlations and analysis of change). Validity was assessed through analysis of the PSEQ’s factor structure (principal components factor analysis) and by examination of the PSEQ’s relationships with validated measures of constructs that would be expected to have different types of relationship with self-efficacy. 2.2. Confirmatory study Data on the PSEQ were obtained from a sample of 1306 heterogeneous chronic pain patients at pre-admission to a 3-week cognitive behavioural pain management program at another tertiary referral pain centre (RNSH) in Sydney. The sample represents a consecutive series of patients admitted to the program over a period of 8 years, from November 1994 to October 2002. Admission criteria were similar to those of the Westmead program, with the exception that at RNSH there were no age limits and the program was part of regular treatment rather than a primary research project. In order to complete the measures the subjects had to be able to read English. In addition to completing the PSEQ, these patients also completed a battery of other 156 M.K. Nicholas / European Journal of Pain 11 (2007) 153–163 2.2.1. RNSH sample characteristics The 1306 heterogeneous chronic pain patients had a mean age of 41 years (range: 13–88); male/female ratio of 47/53%; their work status was: full-time or part-time (20%), unemployed due to pain (62%); their median duration of pain was 36-months (lower and upper quartiles: 23–72 months); their mean pain severity score (on the 0–6 pain scale of the Multidimensional Pain Inventory: Kerns et al., 1985) was 4.4 (SD: 1.8; median: 4.3; upper and lower quartiles: 5.0 and 3.7); their main pain sites were: more than two main sites: 32%; low-back: 43%. Medication for pain was being used by 86.5%, and the mean score on the modified Roland and Morris (1983) Disability Questionnaire (Asghari and Nicholas, 2001) was 13.2 (SD: 5.2). antidepressants, and sedatives/hypnotics, yielding a scoring range of 0–5. Tendency to focus on somatic symptoms was assessed by the Modified Somatic Perception Questionnaire (MSPQ) (Main, 1983). Range is 0–39, with a higher score indicating greater somatic focussing. Impact of pain on daily life was assessed by the Sickness Impact Profile-Self (SIP-S) (Bergner et al., 1981). In this study, the SIP was interviewer administered and rather than ‘health status’, the subjects were asked to relate each item to the impact of their back pain (e.g., Turner, 1982). Only the total score was used and this was expressed as a percentage, with a higher score indicated greater disability due to pain. A spouse/significant other version of the SIP (SIP-O) was also used to provide their perspective on the impact of pain on the subject’s daily life (e.g., Turner, 1982). In the confirmatory study, instead of the SIP, impact of pain on daily life was assessed by the modified Roland and Morris Disability Questionnaire (see Asghari and Nicholas, 2001), in which the word ‘pain’ was substituted for ‘back’. 2.3. Other measures (also administered at initial assessment) 3. Results self-report measures as part of their pre-admission assessment. The Northern Sydney Area Health’s Human Research Ethics Committee gave approval for the use of these (de-identified) data for research. These instruments were intended to capture the typical multidimensional features of chronic pain and included: Average Weekly Pain – derived from the Pain Rating Chart (Budzynski et al., 1973) on a 0–5 scale. Pain qualities were measured by the McGill Pain Questionnaire (MPQ) (Melzack, 1975), but only the dimensions of Sensation (range: 0–42), Affect (0–14) and Evaluation (0–5) were scored. Severity of depressive symptoms was measured by the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al., 1961). Severity of current anxiety was assessed by the state version of the State-Trait Anxiety Scale (STAI) (Spielberger et al., 1970). Current use of pain coping strategies was assessed by the Coping Strategies Questionnaire (CSQ) (Rosenstiel and Keefe, 1983). This includes seven subscales (6-items each) and two further items for rating control over pain and ability to decrease pain. Each subscale is scored out of 36. Beliefs about pain that could promote disability and psychological distress were assessed by the Pain Beliefs Questionnaire (PBQ) (Gottlieb, 1984). Examples of unhelpful statements include: ‘‘I have to lie down when I am in pain’’ and ‘‘I think I am harming my body when I have pain’’. For scoring purposes in this study seven items that were judged by the author to reflect coping or more adaptive beliefs were reverse-scored (e.g., ‘‘For the most part, I live a normal life’’). Thus, a higher score reflects more unhelpful beliefs (range was 0–129). Medication use was assessed by summing the number of classes of medication subjects reported taking for pain or pain-related problems (e.g., sleep, depression) (Turner et al., 1982). Classes included: narcotic analgesics, nonnarcotic analgesics, non-steroidal anti-inflammatories, 3.1. Initial psychometric evaluation with the Westmead sample As can be seen from the mean scores in Table 1, the Westmead sample had moderately severe depressive and anxiety symptoms (e.g., the mean depression score was just above the BDI cut-off score, of 18, for depression used by Kerns and Haythornthwaite (1988) in their study of depression in chronic pain patients). Mean pain levels were moderate (5.6/10), and interference in normal daily activities due to pain was quite marked (e.g., the means score on both versions of the SIP were substantially worse than those reported in the study on chronic low back pain patients by Turner and Clancy (1988)). The median score on the PSEQ was 26.5 (lower and upper quartiles: 15 and 34). 3.1.1. Reliability Reliability was established in a number of ways. 3.1.2. Internal consistency The measure of internal consistency of items, Cronbach’s a coefficient, was calculated as 0.92. This value is very high and indicates the instrument has excellent internal consistency (Nunnally and Bernstein, 1984). 3.1.3. Test–retest reliability This was tested on a different sample of a mixed group (n = 145) of chronic pain patients treated at the Royal North Shore Hospital in Sydney in 1995–96 (see Asghari and Nicholas, 2001, for a description of the full sample). For comparison purposes, this sample had a M.K. Nicholas / European Journal of Pain 11 (2007) 153–163 3.2. Validity Table 1 Westmead sample of chronic low back pain patients Measure Average pain (1–5 scale) MPQ: Sensory MPQ: Affective MPQ: Evaluative Modified Somatic Perception Questionnaire (MSPQ) State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (State) Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) Pain Beliefs Questionnaire (PBQ) CSQ: Divert attention (0–36) CSQ: Reinterpret (0–36) CSQ: Catastrophise (0–36) CSQ: Ignore (0–36) CSQ: Pray/hope (0–36) CSQ: Coping statements (0–36) CSQ: Increase behaviour (0–36) CSQ: Control pain (0–6) CSQ: Decrease pain (0–6) Sickness impact Profile-Self (0–100.3) Sickness impact Profile-Other (0–100.3) Medications used (Total no. of types) PSEQ (0–60) N Mean score 157 SD 85 70 70 70 70 2.8 18.8 5.9 3.2 12.8 0.77 6.55 3.29 1.46 7.58 70 85 70 85 85 85 85 85 85 85 85 85 85 51.1 19.2 57.5 15.6 7.0 16.9 13.8 17.8 20.5 17.4 2.6 2.0 33.3 13.55 9.71 20.69 8.94 6.82 8.39 6.90 9.52 6.81 6.58 1.22 1.19 12.27 71 26.1 13.72 85 1.9 1.02 103 25.8 12.4 Means and standard deviations of dependent variables at pretreatment (not all subjects completed all measures, depending on stage of the overall research program). MPQ: McGill Pain Questionnaire; CSQ: Coping Strategies Questionnaire; PSEQ: Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire. mean age of 50.3 years (SD: 12.8); mean pain duration of 9.9 years (SD: 8.9); mean pain level (0–10 scale) of 6.12 (SD: 2.3); and mean depression severity (BDI) was 14.9 (SD: 8.1). The test–retest period was from initial assessment to 3months later. During this time all patients received some form of ongoing treatment (mainly medication), but reported no change in mean disability or pain. The test– retest correlation (r) from baseline to 3-months was 0.73; (p < 0.001). The mean scores for the two occasions were 26.7 (SD: 12.5) and 26.9 (SD: 12.6), respectively (i.e., no significant change). Interestingly, similar findings were reported by Williams et al. (1996) with a waiting-list control group of mixed chronic pain patients (n = 31) tested 12-weeks apart. In that study, where patients (mean age: 51.1 years, SD: 10.7; mean pain duration: 7.2 years, SD: 6.6; mean BDI: 16.6, SD: 6.5; mean pain severity (0–100): 67.9, SD: 22.3) continued with whatever treatments their doctors had prescribed, the mean PSEQ score at baseline was 26.3 (SD: 10.8) and after 12 weeks it was 26.7 (SD: 6.2), again no significant change was found (and no change in pain or disability either). These findings would suggest that the PSEQ has a high degree of reliability, both internally and across a period of at least 3-months under conditions of no change in either pain or disability. Validity was assessed through analysis of the PSEQ’s factor structure and by examination of the PSEQ’s relationships with validated measures. 3.2.1. Factorial structure A principal components factor analysis with orthogonal rotation was conducted on the full (Westmead) sample in order to investigate the factorial structure of the scale. A one-factor solution resulted from the imposition of the eigen-value-greater-than-one criterion, accounting for 58.6% of the total variance. All items had factor loadings above 0.64 (item 7) and these are reported in Table 2. Corrected item–total correlations (which excluded the relevant item from the total for each correlation) are also presented in Table 2. These correlations varied from 0.67 (item 7) to 0.84 (items 9 and 10). Although item 7 (to do with coping with pain without medication) had the lowest factor loading (and lowest item–total correlation), the loading of 0.64 was still moderately high and the content of the item does have strong construct validity. For example, Ralphs et al. (1994) demonstrated that this item was significantly correlated with mean morphine dose in chronic pain patients attending an inpatient program. As a result, item 7 was retained. 3.2.2. Correlational studies Although there is no ‘gold standard’ measure of selfefficacy against which the PSEQ could be compared, self-efficacy theory would predict a strong relationship between the PSEQ and measures of activity. Given the generalised nature of the PSEQ score, it would be expected that PSEQ scores would correlate strongly (but negatively) with the total score on the SIP. Similarly, it would be expected that PSEQ scores would be correlated with other pain-related activities, such as use of pain-related medication and pain-coping strategies, as measured by the CSQ. In the case of medication usage, it would be expected that the PSEQ scores would correlate negatively with higher medication usage (as Table 2 Corrected item–total correlations, item means and standard deviations, and factor loadings for PSEQ (N = 103, Westmead chronic low back pain sample) Item no. Item–total correlation Mean SD Factor loading 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 0.70 0.72 0.71 0.83 0.74 0.79 0.67 0.79 0.84 0.84 2.7 3.3 2.8 3.0 3.0 2.3 1.9 2.2 2.3 2.5 1.5 1.6 1.7 1.5 1.7 1.6 1.9 1.7 1.6 1.6 0.703 0.722 0.703 0.834 0.735 0.791 0.643 0.795 0.852 0.848 158 M.K. Nicholas / European Journal of Pain 11 (2007) 153–163 their use could be considered to reflect lack of confidence in ability to function due to pain). In the case of the strategies sampled by the CSQ, a positive correlation would be expected with those strategies thought to reflect active approaches (e.g., coping-self statements; and increase behaviour) and perceived ability to control pain, but negative correlations would be expected with those strategies thought to reflect more passive strategies (e.g., catastrophising and praying/hoping) (e.g., Brown and Nicassio, 1987). It would also be expected that the PSEQ would be correlated strongly with the PBQ. While the PBQ is a more broadly based instrument than the PSEQ, it incorporates elements of self-efficacy beliefs. But given the PBQ’s emphasis on non-adaptive beliefs a negative correlation with the PSEQ would be expected. Given the inclusion of ‘despite pain’ in each item of the PSEQ, correlations between the PSEQ and pain (average pain ratings and MPQ subscales) and other somatic perceptions (MSPQ) would be expected to be negative. Pearson product–moment correlations between the PSEQ and the other assessment measures were examined (see Table 3). Due to the large number of intercorrelations strict significance criteria were imposed. Accordingly, only correlations of r > 0.40 and p < 0.001 were considered significant. As expected, significant negative correlations were obtained between the PSEQ and total number of medications used, impact of pain on daily life (SIP-Self- and Sig-other-rated), mood (BDI, STAI), and unhelpful coping strategies and beliefs (catastrophising subscale of the CSQ, and PBQ). Also as expected, significant positive correlations were obtained between the PSEQ and active coping strategies measured (ignore pain, coping self-statements, increase behaviour and control pain subscales of the CSQ). In contrast, no significant correlations were found between the PSEQ and the measures of pain and somatic focussing (average pain ratings, MPQ subscales, or MSPQ), but all were in the negative direction, as expected. 3.3. Confirmatory study with RNSH sample (n = 1306) The mean PSEQ score was 23 (SD: 12.7) and the median was 22 (lower and upper quartile scores: 14 and 33). As with the Westmead sample, the scores were normally distributed, but the mean and median scores were slightly lower than those in the original sample. The mean PSEQ scores from the RNSH sample are close to those reported by Williams et al. (1993) (d = 24.1; SD: 11.4) with another heterogeneous chronic pain sample at pre-admisison to a 4-week inpatient pain management program in London. Internal reliability (Cronbach a correlation) was calculated at 0.93. This finding accords closely with that of the Westmead sample and figures cited in two pub- Table 3 Correlations between PSEQ and other measures at Pre-treatment (Westmead sample, all with chronic low back pain, n = 103) Measure Average pain McGill Pain Questionnaire Sensation Affect Evaluative MSPQ STAI-S BDI PBQ CSQ Divert attention Reinterpret Catastrophis Ignore Pray/hope Coping statements Increase behaviour Control pain Decrease pain SIP-Self SIP-Other Total number of medications used Correlation with PSEQ 0.17 NS 0.31 0.36 0.30 0.36 0.49 0.59 0.74 NS NS NS NS p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 0.13 0.24 0.55 0.46 0.10 0.48 0.45 0.56 0.25 0.60 0.48 0.45 NS NS p < 0.001 p < 0.001 NS p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 NS p < 0.001 p < 0.001 p < 0.001 Due to the large number of intercorrelations, strict criteria were set for significance: r > 0.4 and p < 0.001. MSPQ: Modified Somatic Perception Questionnaire; STAI-S: State version of State-Trait Anxiety Inventory; BDI: Beck Depression Inventory; PBQ: Pain Beliefs Questionnaire; CSQ: Coping Strategies Questionnaire; SIP-Self: Self-report version of Sickness Impact Profile; SIP-Other: Significant-other report version of Sickness Impact Profile; PSEQ: Pain Self-Efficacy Questionnaire. lished studies, one with chronic low back pain clients where the PSEQ’s Cronbach’s a was assessed as 0.94 (Gibson and Strong, 1996). The other study (Asghari and Nicholas, 2001), with a heterogeneous group of chronic pain patients attending a pain clinic for initial assessment, yielded a Cronbach a of 0.92. Consistent with the high Cronbach a value, examination of (corrected) item–total correlations (which excluded the relevant item from the total for each correlation) revealed these were also mostly high and ranged between 0.5 (for item 7) and 0.8. Apart from item 7, dealing with confidence in coping without medication, all other items, had an item–total correlation of 0.7 or greater. The factorial structure of the PSEQ was also confirmed with the RNSH sample of 1306 heterogeneous chronic pain patients. Principal components analysis yielded a single factor solution (Eigen value > 1) that accounted for 60.9% of variance in scores. A subset (n = 130, comprising a consecutive series of patients who attended the program over a 2-year period) of the RNSH sample was followed-up 3–4 years following their attendance at the pain management program (Beeston, 2001). Comparison of mean PSEQ scores (by t tests) at each occasion according to use/non-use of medication for pain revealed significant differences in M.K. Nicholas / European Journal of Pain 11 (2007) 153–163 159 Table 4 Comparison of mean PSEQ (SD) scores from a sample of heterogeneous chronic pain patients attending a pain management program at RNSH according to whether they were taking medication or not at pre-treatment, post-treatment and 3–4 year follow-up (n = 130) Medication status Occasions Pre-treatment Taking Not taking Significance Post-treatment F/U (3–4 years) N PSEQ (SD) N PSEQ (SD) N PSEQ (SD) 114 16 19.3 (10.8) 26 (10.4) P < 0.021 28 102 32 (12.6) 38.6 (12.8) P < 0.015 43 77 26.2 (13) 36.3 (13.6) P < 0.001 PSEQ scores between the two groups at each stage (see Table 4). Those not taking medication for pain reported significantly higher self-efficacy levels at each stage (at pre-treatment, t = 2.34, df = 128, p < 0.021; at posttreatment, t = 2.47, df = 128, p < 0.015; at follow-up, t = 3.97, df = 118, p < 0.001). Overall, there is close correspondence between the psychometric properties of the PSEQ with this large heterogeneous sample of chronic pain patients and the (relatively small) original Westmead sample of low back pain patients. 4. Discussion This paper sought to examine a measure designed to assess self-efficacy beliefs in people with chronic pain. Unlike most published measures of self-efficacy beliefs in people with chronic pain, the PSEQ explicitly asks respondents to take their pain into account. The results obtained from a number of studies reveal that the PSEQ has strong psychometric properties. A high degree of reliability was reflected in high internal consistency and high stability across time (up to 3months) under conditions of pain-focussed treatment and no change in pain or disability (Asghari and Nicholas, 2001; Williams et al., 1996). The PSEQ’s validity was reflected in high correlations (in expected directions) with measures of pain-related disability and different coping strategies. Importantly also, a number of studies have found scores on the PSEQ to be related to a range of complex behaviours amongst different chronic pain samples. These inlcude dropout from a pain management program (Coughlan et al., 1995), pain behaviours (Asghari and Nicholas, 2001), as well as work status (Cohen et al., 2000; Adams and Williams, 2003), medication use (Ralphs et al., 1994; Beeston, 2001), and interference in daily activities observed by the patients’ significant-others (Nicholas et al., 1992). Strong et al. (2002) also found the PSEQ to be a better predictor of functional outcomes than the Pain Stages of Change Questionnaire (PSOCQ; Kerns et al., 1997) after a pain management program, providing further support for the PSEQ’s construct validity. The evidence of the PSEQ’s sensitivity to change under conditions when it would be expected to change also provides support for its construct validity (Murphy and Davidshofer, 1988). Nicholas et al. (1992) reported significant improvements in PSEQ, SIP (spouse-rated), medication-use, and CSQ in low back pain patients after cognitive-behavioural treatment (that included exercises) compared to a control condition. Similarly, Williams et al. (1993) reported that patients attending an inpatient pain management program achieved significant improvements on measures of mood, medication use, disability, specific activities (e.g., stairs climbed) and the PSEQ, but not on pain severity ratings. The PSEQ’s validity was also supported by a high correlation with another, more activity-specific measure of self-efficacy beliefs (Self-Efficacy Scale; SES; Kaivanto et al., 1995) (Gibson and Strong, 1996). Significantly, in the light of Bandura’s proposition about performance in the face of adversity, the PSEQ was more strongly associated with perceived work capacity in injured workers with chronic pain than the SES, which did not incorporate the presence of pain as a context. This finding could be seen as supportive of the importance of taking into account the context (i.e., pain) in which the tasks or functions were to be performed when assessing self-efficacy beliefs, rather than just the task itself – similar to findings in the social anxiety literature (Alden et al., 2004). The finding that correlations between the PSEQ and pain experience (pain severity and MPQ subscales), and other somatic perceptions (MSPQ), were generally quite low suggests that while pain experience is reflected in self-efficacy beliefs (in an inverse relationship), other factors contribute to self-efficacy beliefs as well – as argued originally by Bandura and by more recent psychological accounts about adjustment to pain (e.g., Turk, 2002b; Keefe et al., 2004). Treatment studies that have shown improvements in self-efficacy (PSEQ) and disability, but not pain severity, would also be consistent with this view (Nicholas et al., 1992; Williams et al., 1993, 1996). Bandura’s self-efficacy theory would predict that a generalised measure of self-efficacy as a resilient selfbelief system in the face of difficulties would change in the light of personal achievements (in performance), observation of others performing relevant behaviours, and verbal persuasion. All three elements could be said to exist in (group) cognitive-behavioural pain management programs. As already noted, such 160 M.K. Nicholas / European Journal of Pain 11 (2007) 153–163 changes in self-efficacy beliefs measured by the PSEQ have been repeatedly found in a number of cognitive-behavioural treatment studies. These findings also suggest that the lack of correspondence between selfefficacy beliefs and functional activities reported by Altmaier et al. (1993) may well have been due to their measure’s item content rather than more fundamental questions about the relationship between self-efficacy beliefs and activities when the presence of pain is taken into account. It might also be expected that self-efficacy for performing tasks despite pain would have an inverse relationship to fears about pain and possible injury. This relationship has been reported in two studies (Watson et al., 1997; Ayer and Tyson, 2001). Interestingly, Ayer and Tyson found that the PSEQ accounted for a greater proportion of the variance in disability scores (in patients with chronic low back pain) than fear-avoidance beliefs assessed by the Fear-Avoidance Beliefs Questionnaire (FABQ; Waddell et al., 1993). As both self-efficacy and fear-avoidance beliefs have been shown to be associated with disability in chronic pain patients it would be important to explore the nature of these relationships. One possible scenario is that a person in chronic pain could be fearful of an activity they expected to aggravate their pain, but whether or not they performed the activity might depend on how confident they were in being able to do it despite the pain. It is also likely that pain self-efficacy beliefs would interact with acceptance of pain (e.g., McCracken and Eccleston, 2003). For example, it might be predicted that even if a person accepted the chronic nature of their pain, but their pain self-efficacy was low, the chances of their performing an activity that could aggravate their pain would be low. On the other hand, it might also be predicted that if pain acceptance was low and pain self-efficacy was high, the peson might be more inclined to seek pain-relieving treatment despite having few functional limitations (they might say ‘‘I just don’t want to live in pain’’). These predictions also remain to be tested. Apart from theoretical considerations, the findings presented here provide support for the use of the PSEQ in clinical settings as both a screening instrument to determine patients’ beliefs about a fundamental aspect of pain management (i.e., confidence in performing normal activities despite pain) and in evaluating outcomes after treatment. As a screening instrument, it can provide an indication of their receptivity to an intervention (like a pain program) that did not offer significant pain relief. A very low score (e.g., <17, from Coughlan et al., 1995) could be interpreted as the patient believing pain relief was necessary before s/he could become more active. In this case, these beliefs would need to be addressed prior to such a person starting a pain management program. On the other hand, a high PSEQ score at initial assessment (say, over 40, from Williams et al., 1996; Cohen et al., 2000) would appear incongruent with someone needing a pain management program. This should prompt further examination of the reasons for being there. The finding that high PSEQ scores following pain management programs are commonly strongly associated with clinically significant functional gains provides a potentially useful guage for evaluating outcomes in chronic pain patients. In this context the PSEQ score may provide an indication of the likely maintenance of any behavioural changes or even likelihood of resumption of work given the presence of pain. Thus, scores of around 40 post-treatment (as found in injured workers who had returned to work by Cohen et al. (2000), Adams and Williams (2003); and in patients who generally maintained their treatment gains at 6- and 12-month followups: Williams et al., 1993, 1996) might be considered to have reached a type of threshold where maintenance of gains or return to work was reasonably likely. On the other hand, scores around 30 post-treatment might suggest that things were more in the balance and less likely to be predictive of return to work or maintenance of treatment gains (e.g., Coughlan et al., 1995). Bandura’s self-efficacy theory would predict that those patients who make behavioural changes in a pain management treatment but do not report a sufficient increase their level of pain self-efficacy would be at risk of relapse or drop-out. Coughlan et al.’s (1995) findings could be seen as consistent with that. Council et al. (1988) also described this phenomenon with a different self-efficacy scale. While this issue needs further investigation, it would clearly be a potentially useful means of identifying possible ‘at risk’ cases at the end of a pain management treatment who could be offered further help as a means of preventing possible drop-out or relapse. To conclude, the findings obtained with the PSEQ across a number of studies with different pain populations provide support for the idea originally espoused by Bandura (1989) that it is useful to conceptualise self-efficacy as a reflection of a ‘resilient self-belief system’ in the face of obstacles. By specifying the nature of the obstacles to be faced (i.e., pain) the PSEQ may provide more clinically useful information than simply asking someone about their confidence in performing an activity in isolation. The PSEQ psychometric strengths provide support for its use in both clinical and research settings (Jensen, 2003). Acknowledgements I acknowledge the assistance of the following people with aspects of the development of this paper: Amanda Williams, Mohsen Dehghani, Ali Asghari, Toby Newton-John and the anonymous reviewers. M.K. Nicholas / European Journal of Pain 11 (2007) 153–163 Appendix A PAIN S-E QUESTIONNAIRE (PSEQ) M.K.Nicholas, 1989 NAME: __________________________________________ DATE: __________________ Please rate how confident you are that you can do the following things at present, despite the pain. To indicate your answer circle one of the numbers on the scale under each item, where 0 = not at all confident and 6 = completely confident. For example: 0 Not at all Confident 1 2 3 4 5 6 Completely confident Remember, this questionnaire is not asking whether of not you have been doing these things, but rather how confident you are that you can do them at present, despite the pain. 1. 2. 3. 4. I can enjoy things, despite the pain. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Completely Confident confident I can do most of the household chores (e.g., tidying-up, washing dishes, etc.), despite the pain. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Completely Confident confident I can socialise with my friends or family members as often as I used to do, despite the pain. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Completely Confident confident I can cope with my pain in most situations. 0 Not at all Confident 5. 6. 7. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Completely confident I can do some form of work, despite the pain. (“work” includes housework, paid and unpaid work). 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Completely Confident confident I can still do many of the things I enjoy doing, such as hobbies or leisure activity, despite pain. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Completely Confident confident I can cope with my pain without medication. 0 1 2 3 4 Not at all Confident 5 6 Completely confident 8. I can still accomplish most of my goals in life, despite the pain. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Not at all Completely Confident confident 9. I can live a normal lifestyle, despite the pain. 0 1 2 3 4 Not at all Confident 10. 5 6 Completely confident I can gradually become more active, despite the pain. 0 Not at all Confident 1 2 3 4 5 6 Completely confident 161 162 M.K. Nicholas / European Journal of Pain 11 (2007) 153–163 References Adams JH, Williams ACdeC. What affects return to work for graduates of a pain management program with chronic upper limb pain? J Occ Rehab 2003;13:91–106. Alden LE, Mellings TMB, Laposa JM. Framing social information and generalized social phobia. Behav Res Ther 2004;42:585–600. Altmaier EM, Russel DW, Kao CF, Lehmann TR, Weinstein JN. Role of self-efficacy in rehabilitation outcome among chronic low back pain patients. J Counsel Psychol 1993;40:335–9. Anderson KO, Dowds BN, Pelletz RE, Edwards WT, PeetersAsdourian C. Development and initial validation of a scale to measure self-efficacy beliefs in patients with chronic pain. Pain 1995;63:77–84. Arnstein P, Caudill M, Mandle C, Norris A, Beasley R. Self-efficacy as a mediator of the relationship between pain intensity, disability and depression in chronic pain patients. Pain 1999;80:483–91. Asghari A, Nicholas MK. Pain self-efficacy beliefs and pain behaviour: a prospective study. Pain 2001;94:85–100. Ayer M, Tyson GA. The role of self-efficacy and fear-avoidance beliefs in the prediction of disability. Aust Psychol 2001;36:250–3. Bandura A. Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioural change. Psychol Rev 1977;84:191–215. Bandura A. Perceived self-efficacy in the exercise of personal agency. The Psychologist: Bul Brit Psychol Soc 1989;10:411–24. Bandura A, O’Leary A, Taylor CB, Gauthier J, Gossard D. Perceived self-efficacy and pain control: opioid and nonopioid mechanisms. J Pers Soc Psychol 1987;53:563–71. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiat 1961;4:561–71. Beeston L. Long term follow-up of the cognitive-behavioural pain management programme, ADAPT. Unpublished MSc (Med; Pain Management) thesis, University of Sydney; 2001. Bergner M, Bobbitt RA, Carter WB, Gilson BS. The Sickness Impact Profile: development and final revision of a health status measure. Med Care 1981;19:787–805. Brown GK, Nicassio PM. Development of a questionnaire for the assessment of active and passive coping strategies in chronic pain patients. Pain 1987;31:53–64. Budzynski T, Stoyva J, Adler LS, Mullaney DJ. EMG biofeedback and tension headache: a controlled outcome study. Psychosom Med 1973;35:484–96. Cohen M, Nicholas MK, Blanch A. Medical assessment and management of work-related low back or neck-arm pain: more questions than answers. J Occ Health Safety, Aust New Zealand 2000;16:307–17. Coughlan GM, Ridout KL, Williams ACdeC, Richardson PH. Attrition from a pain management program. Brit J Clin Psychol 1995;34:471–9. Council JR, Ahern DK, Follick MJ, Kline CL. Expectancies and functional impairment in chronic low back pain patients. Pain 1988;33:323–32. Dehghani M, Sharpe L, Nicholas MK. Modification of attentional biases in chronic pain patients: a preliminary study. Eur J Pain 2004;8:585–94. Dolce JJ, Crocker MF, Moletteire C, Doleys DM. Exercise quotas, anticipatory outcome and self-efficacy expectancies in chronic pain: a preliminary report. Pain 1986;24:365–72. Eccleston C. The attentional control of pain: methodological and theoretical concerns. Pain 1995;63(1):3–10. Edwards R, Telfair J, Cecil H, Lenoci J. Reliability and validity of a self-efficacy instrument specific to sickle cell disease. Behav Res Ther 2000;38:951–63. Estlander A-M, Vanharanta H, Moneta GB, Kaivanto K. Anthropmetric variables, self-efficacy beliefs, and pain and disability ratings on the isokinetic performance of low back pain patients. Spine 1994;19:941–7. Frost H, Klaber Moffett J, Moser J, Fairbank J. Evaluation of a fitness programme for patients with chronic low back pain. Brit Med J 1993;310:151–4. Gibson L, Strong J. The reliability and validity of a measure of perceived functional capacity for work in chronic back pain. J Occ Rehab 1996;6:159–75. Gottlieb BS. Development of the Pain Belief Questionnaire: a preliminary report. In Paper presented at the Association for the Advancement of Behavioural Therapy, Philadelphia, PA; 1984. Jensen MP. Questionnaire validation: a brief guide for readers of the research literature. Clin J Pain 2003;19:345–52. Jensen MP, Turner JA, Romano JM. Self-efficacy and outcome expectancy relationship to chronic pain, coping strategies and adjustment. Pain 1991;44:263–9. Kaivanto K, Estlander A-M, Moneta GB, Vanharanta H. Isokinetic performance in low back pain patients: The predictive power of the Self-Efficacy Scale. J Occ Rehab 1995;5:87–99. Keefe FJ, Lefebvre JC, Maixner W, Salley AN, Caldwell DS. Selfefficacy for arthritis pain: relationship to perception of thermal laboratory pain stimuli. Arthritis Care Res 1997;10(3):177–84. Keefe FJ, Rumble ME, Scipio CD, Giordano LA, Perri LM. Psychological aspects of persistent pain: current state of the science. J Pain 2004;5(4):195–211. Kerns RD, Haythornthwaite J. Depression among chronic pain patients: cognitive-behavioral analysis and effect on rehabilitation outcome. J Consult Clin Psychol 1988;56:870–6. Kerns RD, Turk DC, Rudy TE. The West Haven Yale Multidimensional Pain Inventory (WHYMPI). Pain 1985;23:345–56. Kerns RD, Rosenberg R, Jamison RN, Caudill MA, Haythwornthwaite J. Readiness to adopt a self-management approach to chronic pain: the pain stages of change questionnaire. Pain 1997;72:227–34. Lackner JM, Carosella AM. The relative influence of perceived pain control, anxiety and functional self-efficacy on spinal function among patients with chronic low back pain. Spine 1993;24:2254–60. Lackner JM, Carosella A, Feuerstein M. Pain expectancies, pain, and functional self-efficacy expectancies as determinants of disability in patients with chronic low back disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol 1996;64:212–20. Lorig K, Chastain RL, Shoor EUS, Holman HR. Development and evaluation of a scale to measure perceived self-efficacy in people with arthritis. Arthrit Rheumat 1989;32:37–44. Main CJ. The modified somatic perception questionnaire. J Psychosom Res 1983;27:503–14. McCracken LM, Eccleston C. Coping or acceptance: what to do about chronic pain? Pain 2003;105:197–204. Melzack R. The McGill Pain Questionnaire: major properties and scoring methods. Pain 1975;1:277–99. Murphy KR, Davidshofer CO. Psychological testing: principles and applications. London: Prentice-Hall; 1988. Nicholas MK. Self-efficacy and chronic pain. In Paper presented at the annual conference of the British Psychological Society, St. Andrews, Scotland; 1989. Nicholas MK, Wilson PH, Goyen J. Comparison of cognitive behavioural group treatment and an alternative non-psychological treatment for chronic low back pain. Pain 1992;48:339–47. Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH. Psychometric theory. 3rd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1984. Ozer EM, Bandura A. Mechanisms governing empowerment effects: a self-efficacy analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol 1990;58(3):472–86. Pincus T, Morley S. Cognitive appraisal. In: Linton SJ, editor. New avenues for the prevention of chronic musculoskeletal pain and disability. Pain research and clinical management, vol. 12. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2002. p. 123–42. Ralphs JR, Williams ACdeC, Richardson PH, Pither CE, Nicholas MK. Opiate reduction in chronic pain patients: a comparison of M.K. Nicholas / European Journal of Pain 11 (2007) 153–163 patient-controlled reduction and staff controlled cocktail methods. Pain 1994;56:279–88. Roland M, Morris S. A study of the natural history of back pain. Part I: development of a reliable and sensitive measure of disability in low-back pain. Spine 1983;8:141–4. Rosenstiel AK, Keefe FJ. The use of coping strategies in chronic low back pain patients: relationship to patient characteristics and current adjustment. Pain 1983;17:33–44. Rudy TE, Lieber SJ, Boston JR, Gourley LM, Baysal E. Psychosocial predictors of physical performance in disabled individuals with chronic pain. Clin J Pain 2003;19:18–30. Spielberger C, Gorsuch R, Luschere N. Manual for the State Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto (CA): Consulting Psychology Press; 1970. Strong J, Westbury K, Smith G, McKenzie I, Ryan W. Treatment outcome in individuals with chronic pain: is the Pain Stages of Change Questionnaire (PSOCQ) a useful tool? Pain 2002;97: 65–73. Turk DC. A diathesis-stress model of chronic pain and disability following traumatic injury. Pain Res Manage 2002;7:9–19. Turk DC. Clinical effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of treatments for patients with chronic pain. Clin J Pain 2002;18:355–65. Turner JA. Comparison of group progressive-relaxation training and cognitive-behavioural group therapy for chronic low back pain. J Consult Clin Psychol 1982;50:757–65. Turner JA, Calsyn DA, Fordyce WE, Ready LB. Drug utilization patterns in chronic pain patients. Pain 1982;12:357–63. 163 Turner JA, Clancy S. Comparison of operant behavioural and cognitive-behavioural group treatment for chronic low back pain. J Consult Clin Psychol 1988;56:261–6. Turner JA, Jensen MP, Romano JM. Do beliefs, coping, and catastrophizing independently predict functioning in patients with chronic pain? Pain 2000;85:115–25. Waddell G, Newton M, Henderson I, Sommerville D, Main CJ. A fear-avoidance beliefs questionnire (FABQ) and the role of fearavoidance beliefs in chronic low back pain and disability. Pain 1993;52:157–68. Watson PJ, Booker CK, Main CJ. Evidence for the role of psychological factors in abnormal paraspinal activity in patients with chronic low back pain. J Musculoskel Pain 1997;5:41–56. Williams ACdeC, Nicholas MK, Richardson PH, Pither CE, Justins DM, Chamberlain JH. Evaluation of a cognitive behavioural programme for rehabilitating patients with chronic pain. Brit J Gen Pract 1993;43:513–8. Williams ACdeC, Richardson PH, Nicholas MK, Pither CE, Harding VR, Ralphs JA. Inpatient versus outpatient pain management: results of a randomised controlled trial. Pain 1996;66:13–22. Williams ACdeC, Nicholas MK, Richardson PH, Pither CE, Fernandes J. Does randomisation affect the generality of findings from a controlled trial? The effects of patient preference versus randomisation on inpatient versus outpatient chronic pain management. Pain 1999;83:57–65.