Uploaded by jessie.poquerusse

10.1007 s41252-020-00180-9

advertisement
Mindfulness for Autism
Jessie Poquérusse, Francesco Pagnini &
Ellen J. Langer
Advances in Neurodevelopmental
Disorders
Multidisciplinary Research and Practice
Across the Lifespan
ISSN 2366-7532
Adv Neurodev Disord
DOI 10.1007/s41252-020-00180-9
1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by Springer
Nature Switzerland AG. This e-offprint is
for personal use only and shall not be selfarchived in electronic repositories. If you wish
to self-archive your article, please use the
accepted manuscript version for posting on
your own website. You may further deposit
the accepted manuscript version in any
repository, provided it is only made publicly
available 12 months after official publication
or later and provided acknowledgement is
given to the original source of publication
and a link is inserted to the published article
on Springer's website. The link must be
accompanied by the following text: "The final
publication is available at link.springer.com”.
1 23
Author's personal copy
Advances in Neurodevelopmental Disorders
https://doi.org/10.1007/s41252-020-00180-9
ORIGINAL PAPER
Mindfulness for Autism
Jessie Poquérusse 1 & Francesco Pagnini 1,2 & Ellen J. Langer 1
# Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2020
Abstract
Objectives An important complement to molecular treatments, psychological interventions for autism spectrum disorder (ASD)
are efficient and sustainable. The Western-influenced mindfulness concept is one such particularly powerful psychological
construct with widespread benefits spanning health, education, and business spheres. By alleviating the difficulties inherent to
certain rigid patterns of cognition and behavior, cognitively fluidifying mindfulness-based interventions anchored in training
one’s attention to variability have emerged as promising for improving the well-being of individuals with ASD. The purpose of
this paper is to review the evidence supporting these benefits and point to their potential underlying mechanisms.
Methods In this conceptual paper, we first review the broad evidence of the benefits of a range of Western-influenced mindfulness interventions for individuals with ASD, after which we deconstruct their underlying intra-individual processes and effects,
and, finally, frame these psychological mechanisms in light of current core cognitive theories of ASD.
Results This paper highlights the established importance and continued promise of Western-influenced mindfulness training in
understanding and improving the psychological health and overall well-being of individuals with ASD. As such, we hope to lay
the foundation for avenues of future research on mindfulness-inspired interventions aimed at non-invasively improving the lives
of individuals and families affected by ASD.
Conclusions This work provides a brief overview bridging Western-influenced mindfulness and cognitive theories of ASD. In so
doing, it sheds light on this ripe intersection highly amenable to therapeutic intervention.
Keywords Mindfulness . Autism spectrum disorder . Theory of mind . Central coherence . Executive functioning
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a disorder of
neurodevelopment clinically defined in the Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders version V (American
Psychiatric Association 2013) by difficulties with social interaction and communication along with restrictive and repetitive
behaviors. Unconfounded by increased diagnostic acuity,
ASD has been rising in diagnostic prevalence, with estimates
reaching 1 in 59 children affected in the United States alone
(Baio et al. 2018). In addition to the individual challenges it
presents, ASD represents a global medical and socioeconomic
challenge (Buescher et al. 2014; Lavelle et al. 2014). While
multifactorial, ASD’s etiology is largely genetic, with
* Jessie Poquérusse
jessie.poquerusse@gmail.com
1
Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA,
USA
2
Department of Psychology, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore,
Milan, Italy
conservative heritability estimates consistently surpassing
50% (Gaugler et al. 2014), and disruptions are evident in
processes spanning a number of genetically, epigenetically,
and environmentally guided brain developmental stages
(Courchesne et al. 2018; Poquérusse and Luikart 2017). At a
psychological level, a number of developmental differences
have been identified, including challenges with emotional understanding and regulation (Poquérusse et al. 2018), theory of
mind and social cognition (Baron-Cohen et al. 1985), executive functioning (Hill 2004), weak central coherence (Jolliffe
and Baron-Cohen 1999a, 1999b, 2001), and delayed expressive language development (Gernsbacher et al. 2016). To address these interdependent etiologies, substantial work has and
is being done at all scales of neuroscience and psychology to
develop treatments for ASD, from patient-centric genetic engineering and high throughput screenings for small molecule
drug discovery (Sung et al. 2014) to the development of specialized communication devices (Alzrayer et al. 2014;
Martínez-Santiago et al. 2018; Tang et al. 2013), social skills
training protocols (Choque Olsson et al. 2016; Kerr et al.
2002), anti-anxiolytic social stories (Chung and Chen 2017;
Author's personal copy
Adv Neurodev Disord
Marr and Nackley 2010), and a variety of cognitive and behavioral therapies, both patient- (Cooper et al. 2018; Kincade
2009; Larsson 2013) and caretaker-centric (Brookman-Frazee
et al. 2006).
Mindfulness
Among psychological therapies, mindfulness-based interventions have been recently emerging as a promising tool for
individuals with ASD to reshape the mind’s architecture as a
complement to molecular biological therapeutic efforts. While
first introduced as a concept into social psychology over three
decades ago, mindfulness can be distilled into two main nonindependent definitions. Eastern-rooted philosophies traditionally conceptualize mindfulness as a moment-to-moment,
nonjudgmental awareness of one’s present experience (KabatZinn 1982, 1990). In contrast, Western-influenced concepts,
born of Langer’s original social psychological definition, simply define the essence of mindfulness as a heightened attention to variability and openness to novelty (Langer 1989;
Langer and Piper 1987).
Eastern-based mindfulness has long been known to be
physically and psychologically beneficial to individuals with
ASD. Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy, which teaches
individuals to be present in the moment in a nonjudgmental
and accepting way (Kabat-Zinn 1990; Williams et al. 2008),
increases well-being in addition to reducing anxiety, depression, rumination, somatization, distrust, and interpersonal sensitivity in individuals with ASD (Kiep et al. 2015; Spek et al.
2013). Similar mindfulness-based trainings, including in the
form of joint mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) and
cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) programs, have been
shown to reduce anxiety, depression, and aggression in individuals with ASD (Cachia 2017; Sizoo and Kuiper 2017),
while programs for individuals with ASD, directly focused
on enhancing and directing attention, increasing awareness
of bodily sensations, feelings and thoughts, and increasing
self-control, improve social communication and emotional
and behavioral functioning (Ridderinkhof et al. 2018). In addition to patient-centric interventions, similar mindfulnessbased interventions may benefit individuals with ASD by
training their social environment as well to be more mindful
(Singh et al. 2014).
In contrast to the Eastern-influenced concept, Westerninfluenced concepts, born of the original social psychological
definition, simply define mindfulness as a heightened attention to variability and openness to novelty, a mindset rooted in
and fostering a creative, divergent, and non-algorithmic mode
of thinking (Langer 1989). It is well established that this approach is robustly embedded in and promotes (1) a greater
awareness of and sensitivity to one’s environment, alongside
a state of heightened present involvement, engagement, and
wakefulness; (2) an openness to new information and increased awareness of context and multiple perspectives, such
as those relevant to problem-solving and interpersonal harmony; and (3) a resulting increased ability to create new categories for structuring perception, including a loosening of certain
stereotypes and archetypes. Health benefits include stress reduction, decreased addiction, and increased longevity and
well-being; work-related benefits include sharpened cognitive
acuity, creativity, productivity, and performance, and social
benefits range from enhanced emotional awareness to greater
relationship satisfaction (Langer and Moldoveanu 2000).
Such Western-influenced mindfulness training is particularly
promising for individuals with ASD. Indeed, the less timeand logistically intensive Western mindfulness trainings—
not necessarily contingent on an institution or planned
schedule—are relatively simple in implementation.
Hereinafter, we will refer to Langer’s Western-influenced
construct of mindfulness as simply ‘mindfulness’, while integrating spheres of overlap with the Eastern-influenced construct of mindfulness, assessing how it fits into the broader
scheme of and provides therapeutic potential targeted to core
cognitive theories of ASD.
Mindfulness and ASD
A contributing factor to anxiety and a cognitive hallmark of
ASD is a certain rigidity of thought and behavior, which, in
the Western-influenced construct, is the reverse of mindfulness (Langer 1989), and can be loosened through mindfulness
training. Without probing the causal or consequential nature
of this rigidity of thought and behavior in ASD, training individuals to be more mindful by paying closer attention to environmental nuance, in general, allows them to grow aware of
and desensitized to its constant state of flux and intrinsic ambiguity (Langer 2005). Alongside this, realizing that verbal,
perceptual, and cognitive limits are human-generated, and
thereby malleable, helps individuals live in a more relaxed,
dynamic state with respect to their perceptions and actions. A
specific example, and one highly relevant to our medical
times, is that of medical labels. While ubiquitous and logistically useful, such biased labels should neither be perceived as
fixed nor identity-defining (Davis 1979). Despite the computational necessity for the human mind to navigate the world by
dynamically creating categories and other mental and conceptual shortcuts, mindfulness training as defined by deliberate
attention to variability—including the intrinsic variability and
nuance of linguistic labels—fluidifies such categories in a way
that may end up being less stigmatizing and self-fulfilling for
individuals with ASD. It is important to note here that, even if
the rigidity of ASD is a self-protective, disengaging reaction
to the ubiquitous mindlessness of others, mindfulness training
may enable help individuals stay in tune to subtle differences
Author's personal copy
Adv Neurodev Disord
and improve their moment-to-moment integration of the
mindful/mindless cues of their social entourage. The ideal
outcome is a heightened ability to person, time, and
domain—specifically to fine-tune such thought and behavioral patterns.
Linked to this cognitive and behavioral rigidity, three core
interrelated and evolving cognitive theories characteristic of
ASD have emerged (Rajendran and Mitchell 2007). These
include deficits in (1) theory of mind (ToM), or the ability to
infer the mental state of others; (2) central coherence, or the
preferential focus on a stimuli’s global whole rather than local
parts; and (3) executive functioning, or the ability to plan
ahead, make decisions, and inhibit impulsive behaviors.
First, ToM, or the ability to represent and understand
others’ mental states, such as goals, emotions, and beliefs
(Bauminger-Zviely et al. 2013; Bauminger and Kasari
1999), is compromised in ASD and affects individuals’
capacities, as desired, to behave in socially normative ways
(Baron-Cohen 1997; Baron-Cohen et al. 1985). Specific
evidence has suggested that individuals with ASD, while
not universal, may lack a fully representational, and thus
delayed, ToM, rather than a fully absent one, as represented by their greater difficulty with secondary ToM than
primary ToM (Baron-Cohen 1989). Consistently, ToM capacities, conceptualized as a cognitive module developmentally linked to verbal mental capacity, may develop
differentially across individuals with ASD according to a
spectrum (Baron-Cohen et al. 1999). Evidence of deficits
in ToM stems mainly from deficits seen in the Strange
Stories test, in which individuals with ASD have difficulty
explaining the potential motives behind peoples’ strange
behaviors (Happé 1994), and, more generally, have difficulty using context-appropriate mental state terms (Jolliffe
and Baron-Cohen 1999a, 1999b), as well as the Reading
the Mind in the Eyes Task, in which individuals with ASD
have difficulty interpreting the mental states of others
based on images of their eyes (Baron-Cohen et al. 1997),
which was since reproduced with vocalizations
(Rutherford et al. 2002). Mindfulness training, by sensitizing to context and perspective, may help individuals with
ASD more readily tune in to contextual cues and understand others’ perspectives. Specifically, it may increase
ToM by training attention to the subtle variations in story
details, eye and facial expressions, and vocalization characteristics that represent shifts in emotions and mental
states. In addition, and more broadly, viewed as a variation
of mindlessness, such ToM deficits may be vastly improved through such increased social cognitive flexibility
training (Bock et al. 2015). This is supported by studies
showing that formal ToM training, in the form of structured sessions focused on listening to others, assessing social situations and recognizing others’ intentions and emotions while focusing on such concepts as perception,
limitations, and fantasy versus reality—established corollaries of mindfulness training of varying degrees
removed—significantly improved ASD individuals’ ToM
(Begeer et al. 2011).
Second, individuals with ASD exhibit weak central coherence (WCC), or a cognitive style, while heterogeneous (López
et al. 2008), Whivh prioritizes attention to acute detail rather
than the global whole (Frith and Happé 1994; Happe 2005).
The first evidence of WCC emerged from studies demonstrating that individuals with ASD were better able than
neurotypical individuals to locate a small target shape in a
drawing of a larger shape composed of potentially confusing
lines, such as in the Embedded Figures Task (Jolliffe and
Baron-Cohen 1997; Shah and Frith 1983). Since WCC has
been demonstrated in various studies and potentially causally
linked to the cognitively complex ability to handle hierarchical relations between global and local levels (reduced hierarchization), one of the many potential explanations is the reduced ability of ASD individuals to generalize, i.e., generalize
newly learned behaviors to a novel environment due to their
narrower concepts and sharper, more delineated boundaries
(reduced generalization) (de Marchena et al. 2016). Such differences may reflect issues related to the control of the spread
of visual attention, predictions of which are that (1) individuals with ASD have a visual attention which is spatially overly
focused and (2) individuals with ASD have difficulty broadening their attention. Such WCC may be improved through
mindfulness training by helping individuals not only see
boundaries as fluid and constantly changing (‘boundary extension’), but also by shifting back and forth more readily
between widening and narrowing their attention. Rather than
automatically paying excessive attention to detail, participants
are trained to shift perspectives, including that of the “big
picture”, in addition to theirs—thus building the ability to
see through a centrally coherent lens in addition to the default.
As a corollary, participants trained in such mindfulness techniques view both internal and external experiences as passing
events in a wider field of awareness—thereby building central
coherence.
Third, individuals with ASD exhibit deficits in executive
functioning (EF), an umbrella term for the domain-general
cognitive functions of working memory, planning ahead, behavioral inhibition, and initiating, sustaining and shifting attention, alongside the associated flexibility of thought and
action (Demetriou et al. 2018; Hill 2004; Landa and
Goldberg 2005). Among many, deficits in EF manifest themselves as difficulty inhibiting the interfering influence of one
input modality on another (Adams and Jarrold 2012), deficits
in tests of intentionality, to which the cognitively complex
ability to handle multiple embedded rules are inherent
(Poljac and Bekkering 2012), and deficits in attention flexibility inherent to executive memory and metacognition (Wallace
et al. 2016). The Western-influenced construct of mindfulness
Author's personal copy
Adv Neurodev Disord
specifically encourages attention shifting flexibility. This capacity to promote multiple perspectives and shift attentional
foci may as such naturally improve both EF and cognitive
performance, in particular in individuals with a diagnosed
attention problem, often comorbid with ASD (Carson et al.
2001; Stevens et al. 2016). Indeed, mindfulness-based programs improve EF (van de Weijer-Bergsma et al. 2012;
Zylowska et al. 2008) in ways that are strongly applicable to
and promising for individuals with ASD.
Furthermore, mindfulness training designed to enhance attention to variability may be extended to one’s internal environment. A core feature of ASD is the difficulty affected individuals experience with regard to interpreting, regulating,
and expressing and coping with their emotions (Mazefsky
2015; Mazefsky and White 2014; Poquérusse et al. 2018).
Mindfulness training may help individuals identify emotions
by teaching them to be more in tune to the inner changes that
characterize an emotion, whether physical, such as the
blushing cheeks or racing heart of an activated sympathetic
nervous system, or cognitive, such as arising thought patterns
that may exacerbate negative emotions. This in turns allows
individuals to (1) better identify, label, process, and express
arising emotions, while (2) better understanding the triggers
that precipitate emotional reactivity. In addition, since difficulties in ASD are frequently caused or expedited by sensory
overload, which is often acute and overwhelming (Atkinson
2015; Belek 2018), mindfulness training may teach individuals to listen and accordingly attend to sensory symptoms at
still subtle and manageable intensity levels (Watford and
Stafford 2015). More generally, attention to the constant flux
of experience through mindfulness may allow individuals to
realize that as new roles in life are adopted and external circumstances change, moods and emotions change
accordingly—the active noticing and distinguishing of which
helps reduce emotional reactivity, increasing overall wellbeing (Singh et al. 2011).
Building on the above, mindfulness may just as powerfully
benefit individuals with ASD indirectly through the deliberate
actions they are trained and encouraged to engage in, for
which a state of baseline mindfulness is not only a prerequisite, but also a feedforward effect. For example, many forms
of therapy that span music, arts, and sports engage individuals
in a way that reduces stress and increases overall well-being
(Fogleman 2012). These activities require individuals to be
present, focused, active, in tune to kinesthetic, visual or musical variability, and in tune to different lenses, such as the
different perspectives and messages conveyed through art.
Music therapy for example brings individuals into the present
moment by training and cultivating their attention to variability of pitch, tone, and rhythm, among others; in addition, it
helps with emotional atonement by allowing for emotional
expression through musical rhythms which match and reflect
emotions (Gebauer et al. 2014; Tan and Khetrapal 2016).
Such mindfulness-fostering effects are broad ranging, and
similarly inherent to visual, artistic, or kinesthetic forms of
expressive therapy.
Finally, mindfulness training may help individuals with
ASD by being applied to their social environment. On a personal level, mindfulness training helps by improving the wellbeing of caretakers themselves, with obvious and direct repercussions on individuals with ASD (Keenan-Mount et al.
2016). Children with ASD whose parents have high levels
of mindfulness experience lower levels of behavior problems,
stress, and depressive symptoms (Beer et al. 2013). Following
a mindful parenting training, ASD children had fewer episodes of aggressions, non-compliance, and self-injury; the
training also improved parent-children interactions and parents’ satisfaction with their parenting skills (Singh et al.
2006, 2014). Similar mindfulness training of relatives and
caretakers, having been shown to slow disease progression
and improve psychological well-being in response to a broad
range of neurological challenges, is far-reaching (Haller et al.
2017; Pagnini et al. 2015).
On a practical level, caretakers who are trained to be mindful pay more attention to nuances in the individuals with ASD
with whom they work. This enables them to more readily
recognize, target, and attend to patterns in their patients, helping implement subtle but high impact changes. In addition,
free of normative social constructs and able to see boundaries
as flexible and dynamic, mindful caretakers may more readily
generate non-traditional and sustainable solutions for ASD.
These range from deliberately choosing medical language to
promoting social integration and finding creative ways of incorporating sensory integration therapy (Grandin and Panek
2013). Indeed, by seeing past the limitations, imprecision, and
questionability of medical language (Davis 1979), caretakers
shift from thinking in absolutes to thinking in terms of possibilities and potential. For example, in settings spanning the
clinic to the classroom (Langer 1997), conditional language
may be used to empower all individuals, aged and with ASD
alike, to exceed normative expectations (Pagnini et al. 2016).
Globally, mindfulness training defined as increased attention
to variability, by fluidly considering individuals in light of the
multidimensional bell curves of human-centric traits rather
than strictly within the scope of hard threshold-defined clinical
constructs, both logistically and philosophically improves our
approach to ASD while promoting neurodiversity.
Future Research Directions
Mindfulness training of individuals with ASD and their caretakers is a powerful and promising way of increasing the wellbeing of individuals with ASD. Indeed, we believe that the
synoptic perspective provided by this conceptual paper lays
forth an under-explored focus area, bridging and highlighting
Author's personal copy
Adv Neurodev Disord
the promise of Western-influenced mindfulness and cognitive
theories of ASD. Integrating mindfulness training into ASD
therapies not only treats the non-algorithmic mind in addition
to the biological brain, but also provides a rapid, cost-effective, and sustainable complement to ongoingly developed molecular treatments. This said, future research is warranted in
order to create the most effective ASD-centric mindfulness
programs and understand how it can be more seamlessly implemented across a diversity of clinical and non-clinical settings, incorporated into or alongside other therapeutic
interventions.
Of particular salience are the following emerging
questions:
What are the precise mechanisms underlying the effects of
mindfulness training on individuals with ASD, in terms of the
core features of ToM, central coherence, and EF, but also
other more or less related aspects of ASD such as emotion
regulation and sensory hypersensitivity? How can the benefits
of cognitive flexibility-inducing mindfulness training best be
reconciled with the characteristic need for structure and rigidity inherent to ASD?
In light of the strong heterogeneity of ASD and its defining
features and presentations, how do mindfulness interventions
affect syndromic vs non-syndromic autism, in addition to the
different ‘autisms’ in the form of ASD cognitive subtypes
(Cantio et al. 2016) and/or endophenotypes (Constantino
2018)? For example, are there certain genetic backgrounds
that might be differentially responsive to mindfulness interventions? Do mindfulness interventions affect males and females with ASD to the same extent, and in the same manner?
In terms of the defining features of ASD, what, if any,
might be the intra- and intercellular genetic, epigenetic, brain
anatomical and functional, and physiological correlates of the
benefits of ASD (e.g., on amygdala functioning, sensory sensitivity, or circadian rhythms, among others)? How could this
inform the discovery of biomarkers reflective and/or predictive of the benefits of mindfulness on ASD?
How can mindfulness programs be best developed to train
the multiple layers of actors within an individual with ASD’s
social environment—from family members and caretakers to
policy makers and law enforcement professionals—in order to
best interact with, engage, and care for individuals with ASD?
For example, how might mindfulness training increase the
accuracy of diagnoses (minimizing both false negatives and
false positives)? Is there an ideal time period during which to
implement mindfulness interventions to maximize their benefits (either through an acute intervention and/or with the idea
of developing sustainable habits)?
In light of the rising evidence of the broader autism phenotype (Gerdts and Bernier 2011; Green et al. 2020), how might
such mindfulness training affect the very cognitive faculties
such as ToM, central coherence, and EF among individuals
with ASD’s social entourage, genetically related and not, in
such a way as to improve their own well-being as well as that
of individuals with ASD?
Moving beyond ASD to the broader landscape of
neurodevelopmental disabilities as a whole, how might such
mindfulness interventions benefit neuropsychiatric disorders
of related genetic and psychological/cognitive etiology, including, for example, schizophrenia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (Gudmundsson et al. 2019)? How might
this shed light on their distinct and overlapping etiologies and
inform ways to enable such affected individuals to live better?
Given that the concerted effort of clinical specialists, families themselves and broad societal players are key to understanding and improving the well-being of individuals with
ASD, we believe that improving individual and caretaker
mindfulness training, in addition to further research on underlying mechanisms, will be crucial to the collaborative initiative to better address the ASD challenge of the twenty-first
century.
Authors’ Contributions JP conceived of the article and wrote the paper in
collaboration with FP and EL.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that the research was conducted
in the absence of any commercial or financial relationship that could be
construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Adams, N. C., & Jarrold, C. (2012). Inhibition in autism: children with
autism have difficulty inhibiting irrelevant distractors but not prepotent responses. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
42(6), 1052–1063. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1345-3.
Alzrayer, N., Banda, D. R., & Koul, R. K. (2014). Use of iPad/iPods with
individuals with autism and other developmental disabilities: a
meta-analysis of communication interventions. Review Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 1(3), 179–191. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s40489-014-0018-5.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical
manual of mental disorders, 5th edition. American Psychiatric
Association Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.
9780890425596.744053.
Atkinson, S. (2015). Autism and sensory overload. Learning Disability
Practice, 18(7), 15. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00421-6.
Baio, J., Wiggins, L., Christensen, D. L., Maenner, M. J., Daniels, J.,
Warren, Z., Kurzius-Spencer, M., Zahorodny, W., Rosenberg, C.,
White, T., Durkin, M., Imm, P., Nikolaou, L., Yeargin-Allsopp, M.,
Lee, L., Harrington, R., Lopez, M., Fitzgerald, R., Hewitt, A., et al.
(2018). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children
aged 8 years — autism and developmental disabilities monitoring
network, 11 sites, United States, 2014. Morbidity and Mortality
Weekly Report Surveillance Summaries, 67(6), 1–23. https://doi.
org/10.15585/mmwr.ss6706a1.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: a case of
specific developmental delay. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 30(2), 285–297. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.
1989.tb00241.x.
Author's personal copy
Adv Neurodev Disord
Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory
of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child
have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46. https://doi.org/
10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8.
Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997).
Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high
functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 38(7),
813–822.
Baron-Cohen, S., O’Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R., & Plaisted, K.
(1999). Recognition of faux pas by normally developing children
and children with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(5), 407–418.
Bauminger, N., & Kasari, C. (1999). Brief report: theory of mind in highfunctioning children with autism. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 29(1), 81–86. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:
1025974701090.
Bauminger-Zviely, N., Eden, S., Zancanaro, M., Weiss, P. L., & Gal, E.
(2013). Increasing social engagement in children with highfunctioning autism spectrum disorder using collaborative technologies in the school environment. Autism, 17(3), 317–339. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1362361312472989.
Beer, M., Ward, L., & Moar, K. (2013). The relationship between mindful parenting and distress in parents of children with an autism spectrum disorder. Mindfulness, 4(2), 102–112. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s12671-012-0192-4.
Begeer, S., Gevers, C., Clifford, P., Verhoeve, M., Kat, K., Hoddenbach,
E., & Boer, F. (2011). Theory of mind training in children with
autism: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 997–1006. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s10803-010-1121-9.
Belek, B. (2018). Articulating sensory sensitivity: from bodies with autism to autistic bodies. Medical Anthropology, 38(1), 30–43. https://
doi.org/10.1080/01459740.2018.1460750.
Bock, A. M., Gallaway, K. C., & Hund, A. M. (2015). Specifying links
between executive functioning and theory of mind during middle
childhood: cognitive flexibility predicts social understanding.
Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(3), 509–521. https://
doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2014.888350.
Brookman-Frazee, L., Stahmer, A., Baker-Ericzén, M. J., & Tsai, K.
(2006). Parenting interventions for children with autism spectrum
and disruptive behavior disorders: opportunities for cross-fertilization. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 9(3–4), 181–
200. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-006-0010-4.
Buescher, A. V. S., Cidav, Z., Knapp, M., & Mandell, D. S. (2014). Costs
of autism spectrum disorders in the United Kingdom and the United
States. JAMA Pediatrics, 168(8), 721. https://doi.org/10.1001/
jamapediatrics.2014.210.
Cachia, R. L. (2017). Mindfulness and autism spectrum disorder. In M.
Fitzgerald & J. Yip (Eds.), Autism: paradigms, recent research and
clinical applications (pp. 253–268). London: IntechOpen.
Cantio, C., Jepsen, J. R. M., Madsen, G. F., Bilenberg, N., & White, S. J.
(2016). Exploring ‘the autisms’ at a cognitive level. Autism
Research, 9(12), 1328–1339. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1630.
Carson, S., Shih, M., & Langer, E. (2001). Sit still and pay attention?
Journal of Adult Development, 8(3), 183–188. https://doi.org/10.
1023/A:1009594324594.
Choque Olsson, N., Rautio, D., Asztalos, J., Stoetzer, U., & Bölte, S.
(2016). Social skills group training in high-functioning autism: a
qualitative responder study. Autism, 20(8), 995–1010. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1362361315621885.
Chung, C. H., & Chen, C. H. (2017). Augmented reality based social
stories training system for promoting the social skills of children
with autism. In M. Soares, T. Z. Ahram, & C. Falcao (Eds.),
Advances in ergonomics modeling, usability (pp. 495–505).
Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
Constantino, J. N. (2018). Deconstructing autism: from unitary syndrome
to contributory developmental endophenotypes. International
Review of Psychiatry, 30(1), 18–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/
09540261.2018.1433133.
Cooper, K., Loades, M. E., & Russell, A. (2018). Adapting psychological
therapies for autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 45,
43–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2017.11.002.
Courchesne, E., Pramparo, T., Gazestani, V. H., Lombardo, M. V.,
Pierce, K., & Lewis, N. E. (2018). The ASD living biology: From
cell proliferation to clinical phenotype. Molecular Psychiatry, 24(1),
88–107. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0056-y.
Davis, D. A. (1979). What’s in a name? A Bayesian rethinking of attributional biases in clinical judgment. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 47(6), 1109–1114. https://doi.org/10.1037/
0022-006X.47.6.1109.
de Marchena, A. B., Eigsti, I., & Yerys, B. E. (2016). Brief report: generalization weaknesses in verbally fluent children and adolescents
with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 45(10), 3370–3376. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s10803-015-2478-6.
Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Quintana, D. S., Naismith, S. L., Song, Y.
J. C., Pye, J. E., Hickie, I., & Guastella, A. J. (2018). Autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of executive function. Molecular
Psychiatry, 23(5), 1198–1204. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.75.
Fogleman, C. D. (2012). Therapies for children with autism spectrum
disorders. American Family Physician, 85(9), 878–879. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s10803-012-1452-9.
Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: beyond "theory of mind".
Cognition, 50(1–3), 115–132.
Gaugler, T., Klei, L., Sanders, S. J., Bodea, C. A., Goldberg, A. P., Lee,
A. B., Mahajan, M., Manaa, D., Pawitan, Y., Reichert, J., Ripke, S.,
Sandin, S., Sklar, P., Svantesson, O., Reichenberg, A., Hultman, C.,
Devlin, B., Roder, K., & Buxbaum, J. D. (2014). Most genetic risk
for autism resides with common variation. Nature Genetics, 46(8),
881–885. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng.3039.
Gebauer, L., Skewes, J., Westphael, G., Heaton, P., & Vuust, P. (2014).
Intact brain processing of musical emotions in autism spectrum disorder, but more cognitive load and arousal in happy vs. sad music.
Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8, 192. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.
2014.00192.
Gerdts, J., & Bernier, R. (2011). The broader autism phenotype and its
implications on the etiology and treatment of autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research and Treatment, 545901, 1–19. https://doi.
org/10.1155/2011/545901.
Gernsbacher, M. A., Morson, E. M., & Grace, E. J. (2016). Language
development in autism. In G. Hickock & S. L. Small (Eds.),
Neurobiology of language (pp. 879–886). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00070-5.
Grandin, T., & Panek, R. (2013). The autistic brain: thinking across the
spectrum. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
Green, C. C., Brown, N. J., Yap, V. M. Z., Scheffer, I. E., & Wilson, S. J.
(2020). Cognitive processes predicting advanced theory of mind in
the broader autism phenotype. Autism Research, 13(6), 921–934.
https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2209.
Gudmundsson, O. O., Walters, G. B., Ingason, A., Johansson, S., Zayats,
T., Athanasiu, L., Sonderby, I., Gustafsson, O., Nawaz, M., Jonsson,
G., Jonsson, L., Knappskog, P., Ingvarsdottir, E., Davidsdottir, K.,
Djurovic, S., Knudsen, G., Askeland, R., Haraldsdottir, G.,
Baldursson, G., et al. (2019). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder
shares copy number variant risk with schizophrenia and autism spectrum disorder. Translational Psychiatry, 9(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/
10.1038/s41398-019-0599-y.
Haller, C. S., Bosma, C. M., Kapur, K., Zafonte, R., & Langer, E. J.
(2017). Mindful creativity matters: trajectories of reported
Author's personal copy
Adv Neurodev Disord
functioning after severe traumatic brain injury as a function of mindful creativity in patients’ relatives: a multilevel analysis. Quality of
Life Research, 26(4), 893–902. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-0161416-1.
Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(2), 129–154. https://doi.
org/10.1007/BF02172093.
Happe, F. (2005). The weak central coherence account of autism. In F. R.
Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism
and pervasive developmental disorders (Vol. 1, 3rd ed.). Hoboken:
Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470939345.ch24.
Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 8(1), 26–32.
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism and
Asperger syndrome faster than normal on the embedded figures test?
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines,
38(5), 527–534.
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999a). A test of central coherence theory: linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or
Asperger syndrome: is local coherence impaired? Cognition, 71(2),
149–185.
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999b). The strange stories test: a replication with high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(5),
395–406. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023082928366.
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). A test of central coherence theory:
can adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome integrate fragments of an object? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 6(3),
193–216. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546800042000124.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for
chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General
Hospital Psychiatry, 4(1), 33–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/01638343(82)90026-3.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: using the wisdom of your
body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Delacorte
Press.
Keenan-Mount, R., Albrecht, N. J., & Waters, L. (2016). Mindfulnessbased approaches for young people with autism spectrum disorder
and their caregivers: do these approaches hold benefits for teachers?
Australian Journal of Teacher Education, 41(6), 68–86. https://doi.
org/10.14221/ajte.2016v41n6.5.
Kerr, S. J., Neale, H. R., & Cobb, S. V. G. (2002). Virtual environments
for social skills training. Assets ‘02: Proceedings of the fifth international ACM conference on Assistive technologies. 104-110.
https://doi.org/10.1145/638249.638269.
Kiep, M., Spek, A. A., & Hoeben, L. (2015). Mindfulness-based therapy
in adults with an autism spectrum disorder: do treatment effects last?
Mindfulness, 6(3), 637–644. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-0140299-x.
Kincade, S. R. (2009). CBT and autism spectrum disorders: a comprehensive literature review.[Unpublished master's thesis]. University
of Lethbridge.
Landa, R. J., & Goldberg, M. C. (2005). Language, social, and executive
functions in high functioning autism: a continuum of performance.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(5), 557–573.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0001-1.
Langer, E. J. (1989). Mindfulness. Cambridge: Da Capo Press.
Langer, E. J. (1997). The power of mindful learning. Jackson: Ingram
Publisher Services US.
Langer, E. J. (2005). On becoming an artist: reinventing yourself through
mindful creativity Ballantine Books.
Langer, E. J., & Moldoveanu, M. (2000). The construct of mindfulness.
Journal of Social Issues, 56(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/00224537.00148.
Langer, E. J., & Piper, A. I. (1987). The prevention of mindlessness.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(2), 280–287.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.2.280.
Larsson, E. V. (2013). Is applied behavior analysis (ABA) and early
intensive behavioral intervention (EIBI) an effective treatment for
autism? A cumulative review of impartial reports. Autism, 27, 168–
179. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2007-2362.
Lavelle, T. A., Weinstein, M. C., Newhouse, J. P., Munir, K., Kuhlthau,
K. A., & Prosser, L. A. (2014). Economic burden of childhood
autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 133(3), e520–e529. https://
doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-0763.
López, B., Leekam, S. R., & Arts, G. R. J. (2008). How central is central
coherence? Autism, 12(2), 159–171. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1362361307086662.
Marr, D., & Nackley, V. (2010). Using social stories & sensory stories in
autism intervention. OT Practice, 15(10), 17–20.
Martínez-Santiago, F., Montejo-Ráez, A., & García-Cumbreras, M.
(2018). Pictogram tablet: a speech generating device focused on
language learning. Interacting with Computers, 30(2), 116–132.
https://doi.org/10.1093/iwc/iwx022.
Mazefsky, C. A. (2015). Emotion regulation and emotional distress in
autism spectrum disorder: foundations and considerations for future
research. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(11),
3405–3408. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2602-7.
Mazefsky, C. A., & White, S. W. (2014). Emotion regulation. Concepts
& practice in autism spectrum disorder. Child and Adolescent
Psychiatric Clinics of North America., 23(1), 15–24. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.chc.2013.07.002.
Pagnini, F., Phillips, D., Bosma, C. M., Reece, A., Langer, E., & Langer,
E. (2015). Mindfulness, physical impairment and psychological
well-being in people with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis.
Psychology & Health, 30(5), 503–517. https://doi.org/10.1080/
08870446.2014.982652.
Pagnini, F., Bercovitz, K., & Langer, E. (2016). Perceived control and
mindfulness: Implications for clinical practice. Journal of
Psychotherapy Integration, 26(2), 91–102. https://doi.org/10.1037/
int0000035.
Poljac, E., & Bekkering, H. (2012). A review of intentional and cognitive
control in autism. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 436. https://doi.org/
10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00436.
Poquérusse, J., & Luikart, B. W. (2017). A neurodevelopmental perspective for autism-associated gene function. OBM Neurobiology, 1(2),
004. https://doi.org/10.21926/obm.neurobiol.1702004.
Poquérusse, J., Pastore, L., Dellantonio, S., & Esposito, G. (2018).
Alexithymia and ASD: a complex relationship. Frontiers in
Psychology, 9, 1196. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPSYG.2018.01196.
Rajendran, G., & Mitchell, P. (2007). Cognitive theories of autism.
Developmental Review, 27(2), 224–260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
dr.2007.02.001.
Ridderinkhof, A., de Bruin, E. I., van den Driesschen, S., & Bögels, S. M.
(2018). Attention in children with autism spectrum disorder and the
effects of a mindfulness-based program. Journal of Attention
Disorders, 24(5), 681–692. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1087054718797428.
Rutherford, M. D., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2002). Reading
the mind in the voice: a study with normal adults and adults with
Asperger syndrome and high functioning autism. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 32(3), 189–194. https://doi.org/10.
1023/A:1015497629971.
Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: a
research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and
Allied Disciplines, 24(4), 613–620. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14697610.1983.tb00137.x.
Author's personal copy
Adv Neurodev Disord
Singh, N. N., Lancioni, G. E., Winton, A. S. W., Fisher, B. C., Wahler, R.
G., McAleavey, K., et al. (2006). Mindful parenting decreases aggression, noncompliance, and self-injury in children with autism.
Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 14(3), 169–177.
https://doi.org/10.1177/10634266060140030401.
Singh, N. N., Lancioni, G. E., Manikam, R., Winton, A. S. W., Singh, A.
N. A., Singh, J., & Singh, A. D. A. (2011). A mindfulness-based
strategy for self-management of aggressive behavior in adolescents
with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(3), 1153–
1158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.12.012.
Singh, N. N., Lancioni, G. E., Winton, A. S. W., Karazsia, B. T., Myers,
R. E., Latham, L. L., & Singh, J. (2014). Mindfulness-based positive
behavior support (MBPBS) for mothers of adolescents with autism
spectrum disorder: effects on adolescents’ behavior and parental
stress. Mindfulness, 5(6), 646–657. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671014-0321-3.
Sizoo, B. B., & Kuiper, E. (2017). Cognitive behavioural therapy and
mindfulness based stress reduction may be equally effective in reducing anxiety and depression in adults with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 64, 47–55. https://
doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.03.004.
Spek, A. A., van Ham, N. C., & Nyklíček, I. (2013). Mindfulness-based
therapy in adults with an autism spectrum disorder: a randomized
controlled trial. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(1), 246–
253. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2012.08.009.
Stevens, T., Peng, L., & Barnard-Brak, L. (2016). The comorbidity of
ADHD in children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 31, 11–18. https://doi.
org/10.1016/J.RASD.2016.07.003.
Sung, M., Chin, C. H., Lim, C. G., Liew, H. S. A., Lim, C. S., Kashala, E.,
& Weng, S.-J. (2014). What’s in the pipeline? Drugs in development
for autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychiatric Disease and
Treatment, 10, 371–381. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S39516.
Tan, M., & Khetrapal, N. (2016). Music inspired framework for
remediating emotional deficits in autism. Procedia Computer
Science, 88, 469–474. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.procs.2016.07.466.
Tang, H.-H., Jheng, C.-M., Chen, M. Y., Chien, M.-E., Lin, N.-M., &
Chen, M. Y. (2013, March 12-16). iCAN: a tablet-based pedagogical system for improving the user experience of children with autism in the learning process. Paper presented at the International
Conference on Orange Technologies (ICOT), Tainan, Taiwan.
https://doi.org/10.1109/ICOT.2013.6521186.
van de Weijer-Bergsma, E., Formsma, A. R., de Bruin, E. I., & Bögels, S.
M. (2012). The effectiveness of mindfulness training on behavioral
problems and attentional functioning in adolescents with ADHD.
Journal of Child and Family Studies, 21(5), 775–787. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s10826-011-9531-7.
Wallace, G. L., Kenworthy, L., Pugliese, C. E., Popal, H. S., White, E. I.,
Brodsky, E., & Martin, A. (2016). Real-world executive functions in
adults with autism spectrum disorder: profiles of impairment and
associations with adaptive functioning and co-morbid anxiety and
depression. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(3),
1071–1083. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2655-7.
Watford, T. S., & Stafford, J. (2015). The impact of mindfulness on
emotion dysregulation and psychophysiological reactivity under
emotional provocation. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory,
Research, and Practice, 2(1), 90–109. https://doi.org/10.1037/
cns0000039.
Williams, J. M. G., Russell, I., & Russell, D. (2008). Mindfulness-based
cognitive therapy: further issues in current evidence and future research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(3), 524–
529. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.76.3.524.
Zylowska, L., Ackerman, D. L., Yang, M. H., Futrell, J. L., Horton, N. L.,
Hale, T. S., Pataki, C., & Smalley, S. L. (2008). Mindfulness meditation training in adults and adolescents with ADHD. Journal of
Attention Disorders, 11(6), 737–746. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1087054707308502.
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Download