American Academy of Political and Social Science Nature and Nurture: Genetic and Environmental Influences on Behavior Author(s): Robert Plomin and Kathryn Asbury Source: The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, Vol. 600, The Use and Usefulness of the Social Sciences: Achievements, Disappointments, and Promise (Jul., 2005), pp. 86-98 Published by: Sage Publications, Inc. in association with the American Academy of Political and Social Science Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/25046112 Accessed: 11-02-2020 18:17 UTC REFERENCES Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article: https://www.jstor.org/stable/25046112?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at https://about.jstor.org/terms American Academy of Political and Social Science, Sage Publications, Inc. are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms The appropriate conjunction between the words natu and nurture is not versus but and. There is increasi acceptance of the evidence for substantial genetic inf ence on many behavioral traits, but the same resear also provides the best available evidence for the impo tance of environmental influence and important clu about how the environment works. Because much developmental action is at the interface between genes and environment, genetic research needs to incorporate Nature and Nurture: measures of the environment, and environmental research will be enhanced by collecting DNA. Keywords: genetics; nature vs. nurture; twin studies; behavioral traits; DNA Genetic and Environmental Influences on Behavior After thetheAmerican AmericansixAcademy hundredofAcademy Political volumes and of of The Political Annals and of Social Science with hardly a mention of genetics, it is time to consider genetic as well as environmental influences on behavior. The theme of this review is that both genetics and environment, and the interplay between them, contribute importantly to the development of individ- ual differences in behaviors including mental By ROBERT PLOMIN and KATHRYN ASBURY health and cognition. Quantitative genetic research - exemplified by the twin design that compares identical twins (monozygotic, MZ) and fraternal twins (dizygotic, DZ) - has gone beyond merely demonstrating the importance of genetic influence (heritability) to investigat- ing more sophisticated issues such as developmental change and continuity, heterogeneity and comorbidity, and the interplay between genes and environment. Robert Plomin is MRC Research Professor of Behavioral Genetics at the Institute of Psychiatry in London, where he is deputy director of the Social, Genetic and Devel- opmental Psychiatry (SGDP) Centre. The goal of the SGDP Centre is to bring together genetic and environ- mental research strategies to investigate behavioral development, a theme that characterizes his research. Kathryn Asbury is a postdoctoral researcher at the SGDP Centre whose research focuses on the developmental interface between genes and environment. DOI: 10.1177/0002716205277184 86 ANNALS, AAPSS, 600, July 2005 This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms NATURE Most AND NURTURE exciting is 87 the floo the specific DNA sequenc ioral disorders such as m as personality. called most QTLs in a The latter quantitative trait important implicatio of small effect size "QTL set" that can environmental risk indices such as socioeconomic status or education are used. Unlike quantitative genetic research that requires unique samples such as twins, molecular genetic research only requires DNA, which can be obtained painlessly and inexpensively using cheek swabs. We look forward to exciting advances in understanding the interplay between genes and environment once DNA is incorporated into social science research. Our hope for this article is that it facilitates this integration. In the first part of the article, we describe quantitative genetic research, emphasizing what it has taught us about the way the environment works to affect behav- ioral development. The second part considers the future of genetic research, which lies with DNA. The brevity of this article does not permit us to discuss back- ground issues (such as the focus on individual differences rather than population means), methodological issues (such as the twin method or methods for finding QTL associations), or to document thoroughly the research that supports our conclusions (for details, see Plomin et al. 2001). The History of Nature and Nurture The application of genetic research to human problems has experienced spectacular highs and lows over the decades since its inception in 1865. The study of nature and nurture in the development of behavioral traits began quietly with work from Francis Galton (1865). Pace and interest increased only gradually until, in 1924, the first twin and adoption studies in developmental psychology were published (Merriman 1924; Theis 1924). Events little more than a decade later compli- cated the course of progress. The slow but steady growth of genetic science was stopped abruptly by a world sickened by Nazi war crimes and all that was associated with them. The Nazi regime s abuse of genetics was high-profile and terrifying to a world in mourning, a world whose way of life had been threatened. The emergence of behaviorism around the same time (Watson 1930) proved a further road- block in the development of genetic science, achieving a huge impact on the behavioral sciences with its ostensibly comforting theory of an environmental paradigm based on the assumption that we are what we learn. Even from this poor midcentury soil, however, molecular genetic research managed to flourish, bursting onto the scientific stage in a Nobel Prize-winning blaze of glory with the discovery of the structure of DNA less than a decade after the end of World War II (Watson and Crick 1953). The genetic code was cracked in 1966, This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms t 88 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY the four-letter alphabet (G, A, T, C) of DNA code for the twenty amino acids that are crowning glory of the century and the begin Human Genome Project s working draft of th DNA in the human genome, nucleotide bases t case of DNA (International Human Genome et al. 2001). The slow but steady growth o was stopped abruptly by a w by Nazi war crimes and all associated with them. During this time, genetic research on hum building a case for the importance of genetics book on behavioral genetics was only publis research with nonhuman animals (Fuller a nearly every area of behavior that has been s shown strong genetic influence (Plomin e sistently shown heritable influence in man research such as mental illness, personality, c drug use and abuse. Some areas showing st surprising, such as self-esteem, interests, a This research has led to growing acceptance ronment in the etiology of individual differe in the growing number of genetics papers in funded research grants. The public also acce F or example, a recent poll found that more t reported genetics as being at least as importan ness, personality, learning difficulties, and in coming). Before the pendulum of fashion shift important to emphasize that this same geneti evidence for the importance of the environm ents and teachers have it about right: genet about half of the variance. Consider schizophrenia. Until the 1960s, sch ronmental in origin, with theories putting th This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms NATURE for AND the NURTURE fact that 89 schizop could run in families f adoption studies succe twins are much more sim twins are genetically ide are only 50 percent simi phrenic, the chances are DZ twins, the chances schizophrenia is just as phrenic parents at birt ents, providing efforts to dramatic identify schizophrenia 2003). som (Craddo Back in the 1960s, when schizophrenia was thought to be caused environmentally, it was important to emphasize the evidence for genetic influence, such as the concordance of 45 percent for identical twins. Now that genetic influence is widely recognized, it is important to emphasize that identical twins are only 45 percent concordant for schizophrenia, which means that in more than half of the cases, these pairs of genetic clones are discordant for schizophrenia. This discordance cannot be explained genetically - it must be due to environmental factors. Note that the word environment in genetic research really means nongenetic , a much broader definition of environment than is usually encountered in the behavioral sciences. That is, environment denotes all nonheritable factors including possible biological events such as prenatal and postnatal illnesses, not just psychosocial factors. Nonetheless, the point is that genetics often explains half of the variance of behavioral traits, but this means that the environment explains the other half. In addition to providing strong evidence for the importance of environmental influence, two of the most important findings from genetic research involve nurture rather than nature: nonshared environment and what has been called the nature of nurture. Nonshared environment. Recent research shows that the environment works very differently than we previously supposed. Theories of socialization have gener- ally assumed that aspects of the environment such as socioeconomic status or parental divorce will, for better or worse, make children growing up in the same family similar to each other. Environmental research in genetically sensitive designs has consistently found a different pattern, namely, that the environments that affect behavioral development work by making children in the same family dif- ferent (Plomin and Daniels 1987). We know this, for example, because genetically unrelated children growing up in the same adoptive family scarcely resemble each other for personality, psychopathology, and cognitive abilities after adolescence. Siblings are often similar, but their similarity is rooted in their genes rather than in the environment they share. Environment is hugely important to human development, but genetic research has shown beyond doubt that the most effective envi- This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms 90 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY ronmental influences are those that ope different, not similar. These environm because they are not shared by children g So why are children growing up in the s almost two decades has attempted to ide ronment, exploring differential parentin ences in relation to a wide range of beh with many environments showing signifi and Dunn 2001; Turkheimer and Waldron find specific nonshared environmental variance, it should be emphasized that non the environment works to influence beha on the family environment, it seems reas ily, with peers and individual life event sources of nonshared environment (Har contributes to nonshared environment in experiences, or the subtle interplay of over time, small differences in experienc come. Identifying the factors responsible of the big challenges for the future of th The nature of nurture. Dozens of twi genetic factors substantially influence me ments such as parenting, stress, or soc called "the nature of nurture" (Plomin can this be true, given that environmen environments can be considered as extend ences between individuals as they select, m ence of the world. In quantitative gene type-environment (GE) correlation bec genetic propensities and exposures to e Given that environmental measures as well as behavioral measures show genetic influence, it is reasonable to ask whether associations between environ mental and behavioral measures are mediated genetically Multivariate genetic analysis (discussed later) can be used to analyze genetic and environmental contributions to the correlation between environmental and behavioral measures. Genetic factors can mediate the correlation to the extent that the environment rep- resents a direct response to genetically influenced characteristics. For example, differences in parenting could be the genetic result of child psychopathology, rather than its cause. A general guideline from multivariate genetic research of this sort is that genetic factors tend to be responsible for about half of the phenotypic correlation between measures of the environment and measures of behavior. For this reason, environmental measures cannot be assumed to be truly environmental. A far-reaching implication of this research supports a shift from thinking about passive models of how the environment affects individuals toward models that rec- This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms NATURE AND NURTURE 91 ognize the active role we structing in memory - o In quantitative genetics action, which refers to dif relation, which denotes psychopathology, a part diathesis-stress Wachs, strate and in detect in which Plomin quantitative an overall h fort gene effect DeFries, and Fulker 1988 GE interaction have been measured environments coming). One example es functional polymorphism moderate the effect of ch (Caspi et al. 2002). In short, genetic resear about the environment ment and the role of gen mechanisms can be predi the context of geneticall of genes in experience a research strategies are b importance heritability multivariate of analyses Developmental one ing s nature estimates change development these of stages, does teen-year-olds. responsible For for h a no althoug genetic effects at one same genes could have be ar age dif exam the fact for individuals who late "schizophrenia genes" are that these genes operate t only express their hallucin opment One of enables the such more hig striking ability, often called intell influence increases steadil (McGue et al. 1993). This i ronmental factors This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms becom 92 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY ing the life course. It is not known why increases during development. It is possib development, but it is also possible that t latter hypothesis receives support from age change and continuity that suggests t for genetic influence throughout deve how can genetic influence increase? On ment correlation , which, as mentioned genetic propensities and exposure to ex may occur because small genetic differen life, creating environments that are corr [Ejnvironments can be con phenotypes, reflecting gen between individuals as they construct their own experi Multivañate heterogeneity and comor which genetic research is going beyond h which, as mentioned earlier, focuses on th rather than the variance of each trait con to which genetic factors that affect one genetic research in psychopathology su often differ greatly from traditional dia as anxiety or depression. For example, genes are largely responsible for anxie Recent research suggests that this genetic broader in that genetic influences psychopathology: internalizing problem phobia; and externalizing problems that in der, and drug abuse (Kendler et al. 200 Broad effects of genes have also been the differences between cognitive abili abilities, the same genes largely affect all Genetic overlap is also substantial acro reading, and mathematics disability, leadi ing disabilities (Plomin and Kovas forth This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms NATURE AND NURTURE overlap among 93 cognitive ab understanding the brain and Spinath 2002). Sometimes multivariate comorbidity. An example o social sciences concerns behavior. Antisocial beha genetic influence and, un influence. However, anti personality (Viding et traits al. is highly forthcomin that the genetic core of ea sonality (Viding, Frick, an iors characteristic of aut deficits in both social an reveals marked genetic h related and traits Plomin nonsocial as assessed by forthcoming traits indicative The results of these mul not only for diagnosis but immediate implication is t for molecular genetic res responsible for genetic inf unemotional personality hood suggests that this sh than the broader phenoty DNA The future of genetic research on behavior lies in molecular genetic st DNA that will eventually identify specific DNA variants responsible for the spread influence of genes in behavioral development. Identifying these DNA ants will make it possible to address questions such as those raised above gene-environment, developmental, and multivariate mechanisms - wit greater precision and power. As compared to quantitative genetic studies of and adoptees, molecular genetics will have a far greater practical impact on b ioral research because molecular genetic research does not require special tions such as twins or adoptees. DNA can be easily and inexpensively o (from cheek swabs rather than blood, for about $10 per individual), and gen of a DNA marker is also inexpensive (about 100 per individual). Moreover called gene chips (microarrays) are available that can genotype hundreds sands of genes for an individual in three days (Butcher et al. 2004). This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms 94 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY Finding genes associated with complex tr genes already identified is easy and ine dimension to behavioral research (Plomi area of dementia in later life will be p research. The only known risk factor f (LOAD) is a gene, apolipoprotein E (APO form of the gene called allele 4 increases association between APOE allele 4 and (Corder et al. 1993), much research on d APOE to ascertain whether results differ for individuals with and without this genetic risk factor (e.g., Laurin et al. 2004; Mukamal et al. 2003; Podewils et al. 2005). Genotyping APOE will also become routine in clinics if this genetic risk factor is found to predict differential response to interventions or treatments. Many large-scale behavioral studies are currently obtaining DNA on their samples in anticipation of the time when genes are identified that are relevant to their area of interest. There is nothing to be gained by sticking our heads in the sand and pretending that genetic differences do not exist. It should be noted that DNA variation has a unique causal status in explaining behavior. When behavior is correlated with anything else, the old adage applies that correlation does not imply causation. For example, parenting is correlated with children's behavioral outcomes, but this does not necessarily mean that the parenting causes the outcome. Genetic research has shown that parenting behavior in part reflects genetic influences on children's behavior. When it comes to interpreting correlations between biology and behavior, such correlations are often mistakenly interpreted as if biology causes behavior. For example, correlations between neurotransmitter physiology and behavior or between neuroimaging indices of brain activation and behavior are often interpreted as if brain differences cause behavioral differences. However, these correlations do not nec- essarily imply causation because behavioral differences can cause brain differ- ences. In contrast, in the case of correlations between DNA variants and behavior, the behavior of individuals does not change their genome. The DNA sequence itself does not change. For this reason, correlations between DNA differences and behavioral differences can be interpreted causally: DNA differences cause the behavioral differences. This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms NATURE There AND is NURTURE no single 95 huma in a thousand of DNA bas the population. Most life but medical and behaviora genome that are respons been ide genes th APOE gene that is a risk successful for the thousan single gene is necessary gene disorders have beha 282 of these disorders inc Restifo 2004). However, su of .0001 or less. The DNA ble for the heritability of usually called complex tr genes as well as by mult schizophrenia, affective ism, and hyperactivity ar genes have been identified complex traits in behavi more difficult than antic many more genes of muc McGuffin 2003). ants have already find some of the The most these ful far-reaching genes are portion of schizophrenia, identified the varia research w tices of our schools, our c cal diagnoses, individualiz diction of problems that Behavioral science will b postgenomic era in which how these genes work. Su to the bottom-up strategy fied by its DNA sequence cells and then cell system other end of the continu integrationist top-down l organism rather than a re single the molecule effects of in a sing specific ge relate with experience (P This top-down behavior to distinguish it from This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms th 96 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY phrase has become synonymous with b that behavioral genomic research is like diagnosis, and intervention, and eventu genes as early-warning systems. Bottom gene-behavior pathways will eventually tion is that DNA will serve as an integr including the behavioral sciences. Nature and Nurture As discussed earlier, the importance of genes as well as environment in the etiology of individual differences in behavior is increasingly accepted in science as well as in society. We predict that this trend will accelerate in the postgenomic era as specific genes are found that are responsible for the heritability of behavioral disor- ders and dimensions. However, as is the case with most advances in science, these new findings also raise new problems for medicine, parenting, education, employment, law, and insurance, as well as larger philosophical issues such as human dignity, free will, and moral responsibility. These issues have been addressed recently in a report by the Nuffield Council on Bioethics (2002). The Nuffield report has three parts. The first explains the historical and scientific background to behavioral genetic research, which is also the topic of a book by Steven Pinker called The Blank Slate (2002). The second part reviews the findings of behavioral genetics, coming to similar conclusions to those described in this article. The third part is most relevant here: it examines the ethical, legal, and policy implications of this research. With the postgenomic era of DNA, the report is especially concerned about the need to monitor and regulate DNA tests and interventions and the possi- bilities for enhancing capabilities that could lead to even greater inequalities in society. A general sense of unease about genetics comes from a feeling that genetic differences contradict equality: are not all men (and women) created equal? Although this was a self-evident truth to the signers of the American Declaration of Independence, the founding fathers of the United States were not so naïve as to think that all people are created identical. The essence of a democracy is that all people should have legal equality despite their individual differences, regardless of the environmental or genetic origins or those differences. Decisions, both good and bad, can be made with or without knowledge, but we believe firmly that better decisions can be made with knowledge than without. There is nothing to be gained by sticking our heads in the sand and pretending that genetic differences do not exist. The basic message of behavioral genetics is that each of us is an individual. Recognition of, and respect for, individual differences is essential to the ethic of individual worth. Proper attention to individual needs, including provision of the environmental circumstances that will optimize the development of each person, is a Utopian ideal and no more attainable than other utopias. Nevertheless, we can approach this ideal more closely if we recognize, rather than ignore, individuality. This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms NATURE AND NURTURE 97 References Asbury, and K., Bouchard ence: D. T. Wachs, nonverbal T. EPD J., Butcher, L. ples to type Caspi, in J. K. E. DNA QTLs cycle on for T. of E. A. In In M. L. L micr comple violence T. J Moffitt, E. Moderation H., Lykken Meaburn, Sugden, depression: Corder, E. McClay, the A., T. Plom early Baltimore: pooled detect A., D. R. in revisited. 5-43. M., Genotyping Caspi, Jr., theory Lubinski, and abilities in Moffitt by a Saunders, po W J. L. Haines, and M. A. Pericak V Alzheimer's disease in late onset Craddock, N., Dissecting Fuller, J. Galton, Harlaar, sis of and L., F. M. and 1865. N., DNA C. O'Donovan, psychosis. L. W. Journal R. Heredity Butcher, markers o Thompso E. talent Meabu associated w Psychiatry. Harris, Inlow, J. J. R. 1998. K., and The L. L. nurture as Restifo. 2 166:835-81. International Human Genome Sequencing Consortium. 2001. Initial sequencing and analysis of the human genome. Nature 409:860-921. Kendler, K. S., M. C. Neale, C. A. Prescott, R. C. Kessler, A. C. Heath, L. A. Corley, and L. J. Eaves. 1996. Childhood parental loss and alcoholism in women: A casual analysis using twin-family design. Psychological Medicine 26:79-95. Kendler, K. S., C. A. Prescott, J. Myers, and M. C. Neale. 2003. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Archives of General Psychiatry 60:929-37. Laurin, D., K. H. Masaki, D. J. Foley, L. R. White, and L. J. Launer. 2004. Midlife dietary intake of antioxidants and risk of late-life incident dementia: The Honolulu- Asia Aging Study. American Journal of Epide- miology 159:959-67. McGue M., T. J. Bouchard Jr., W. G. Iacono, and D. T. Lykken. 1993. Behavioral genetics of cognitive ability: A life-span perspective. In Nature, nurture, and psychology , ed. R. Plomin and G. E. McClearn, 59-76. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Merriman, C. 1924. The intellectual resemblance of twins. Psychological Monographs 33:1-58. Mukamal, K. J., L. H. Kuller, A. L. Fitzpatrick, W. T. Longstreth Jr., M. A. Mittleman, and D. S. Siscovick. 2003. Prospective study of alcohol consumption and risk of dementia in older adults. JAMA 289: 1405-13. Nuffield Council on Bioethics. 2002. Genetics and human behaviour: The ethical context London: Nuffield Council on Bioethics. Petrill, S. A. 1997. Molarity versus modularity of cognitive functioning? A behavioral genetic perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science 6:96-99. Pinker, S. 2002. The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Penguin. Plomin, R. 1994. Genetics and experience: The interplay between nature and nurture. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Plomin, R., K. Asbury, and J. Dunn. 2001. Why are children in the same family so different? Nonshared envi- ronment a decade later. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 46:225-33. This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms 98 THE ANNALS OF THE AMERICAN ACADEMY Plomin, R., and C. S. Bergeman. 1991. The nature of nur sures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14:373-427. Plomin, R., and D. Daniels. 1987. Why are children in the same family so different from each other? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 10:1-16. Plomin, R., J. C. DeFries, I. W. Craig, and P. McGuffin. 2003a. Behavioral genetics in the postgenomic era. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. . 2003b. Behavioral genomics. In Behavioral genetics in the postgenomic era, ed. R. Plomin, J. C. DeFries, I. W Craig, and P. McGuffin, 531-40. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Plomin, R., J. C. DeFries, and D. W. Fulker. 1988. Nature and nurture during infancy and early childhood. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Plomin, R., J. C. DeFries, G. E. McClearn, and P. McGuffin. 2001. Behavioral genetics. 4th ed. New York: Worth Publishers. Plomin, R., and Y. Kovas. Forthcoming. Generalist genes and learning disabilities. Psychological Bulletin. Plomin, R., and P. McGuffin. 2003. Psychopathology in the postgenomic era. Annual Review of Psychology 54:205-28. Plomin, R., M. J. Owen, and P. McGuffin. 1994. The genetic basis of complex human behaviors. Science 264:1733-39. Plomin, R., and F. M. Spinath. 2002. Genetics and general cognitive ability (g). Trends in Cognitive Sci 6:169-76. Podewils, L. J., E. Guallar, L. H. Kuller, L. P. Fried, O. L. Lopez, M. Carlson, andC. G. Lyketsos. 2005. Physi- cal activity, APOE genotype, and dementia risk: Findings from the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study. American Journal of Epidemiology 161:639-51. Ronald, A., F. Happé, and R. Plomin. Forthcoming. The genetic relationship between individual differences in social and nonsocial behaviors characteristic of autism. Developmental Science. Theis, S. V. S. 1924. How foster children turn out. Publication no. 165. New York: State Charities Aid Association. Turkheimer, E., and M. Waldron. 2000. Nonshared environment: A theoretical, methodological, and quanti- tative review. Psychological Bulletin 126:78-108. Venter, J. C., et al. 2001. The sequence of the human genome. Science 291:1304-51. Viding, E., R. ]. R. Blair, T. E. Moffitt, and R. Plomin. Forthcoming. Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. Viding, E., P. ]. Frick, and R. Plomin. N.d. Genetic influences on the relationship between callousunemotional traits and conduct problems in 7-year-old twins. Manuscript submitted for publication. Wahlsten, D. 1990. Insensitivity of the analysis of variance to heredity-environment interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13:109-61. Walker, S. O., and R. Plomin. Forthcoming. The nature-nurture question: Teachers' perceptions of how genes and the environment influence educationally relevant behavior Educational Psychology. Watson, J. B. 1930. Behaviorism. New York: Norton. Watson, J. D., and F. H. C. Crick. 1953. Genetical implications of the structure of deoxyribonucleic acid. Nature 171:964-67. This content downloaded from 130.111.28.95 on Tue, 11 Feb 2020 18:17:04 UTC All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms