R Religion Vania Rolon Brunel University London, London, UK Synonyms Belief; Faith; Religiosity; Worship Definition A system of faith and worship typically composed of animism of inanimate objects and sometimes of a superhuman controlling power that may or may not be a personal God or gods and with its own set of moral injunctions. Introduction Despite the wide array of both past and present cultures and the incredible variety among them, religion seems to be part of our evolutionary heritage (Schmitt and Fuller 2015; Wilson 2002). Granted, some religions are monotheistic whereas others are polytheistic, and some religions, although not all, believe in a high god creator of the universe. Nevertheless, despite this apparent variability, religions do share some near universal features. Explaining the existence of these beliefs has been a goal of the interdisciplinary field known as the cognitive science of religion, a field that encompasses both the evolutionary psychology and evolutionary anthropology of religion. The major question regarding the field is whether religion itself is a mechanism selected by natural selection to solve a particular adaptive problem, or if it is merely a by-product of a lion of different psychological mechanisms such as agency detection. This section will explore the main theories regarding the evolutionary origins of religion. It is important to bear in mind that specifics of different religions will not be addressed. Instead, only near universal features of religion will be discussed. Near Universal Features of Religion When one hears the word “religion,” it is instinctive to think about Christianity, Islam, Judaism, Hinduism, or other of the most well-known religions. These are known as world religions, and they actually have more in common than meets the eye. Most cultures and their respective religions share a common narrative about the origins of humanity, invoke supernatural agents, signal in-group loyalty through organized public affirmation of religious beliefs, ascribe animistic agency to nonhuman, mysterious sources, and distinguish between morally sacred and morally profane thoughts, beliefs, and actions (Schmitt and Fuller 2015). Nevertheless, it seems not all of these near universal characteristics evolved # Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018 T. K. Shackelford, V. A. Weekes-Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_342-1 2 simultaneously. We do not know if some of these are sufficiently ancient for biological evolution to have shaped them for a particular purpose. In other words, some religion-related adaptations may have evolved before the emergence of organized religion as we know it, whereas others are modern inventions (Kirkpatrick 1999). For instance, Peoples et al. (2016) found that animism was the oldest trait of religion across a wide sample of hunter-gatherer societies. Other traits such as belief in the afterlife followed by shamanism emerged shortly after and were still present in most societies. Ancestor worship, which appeared later, was no longer found in many of the societies studied. Finally, belief in high gods was the most recent and least common characteristic, suggesting that these were absent in early humans. This emergence of religious characteristics over different periods of time makes it even harder to distinguish whether some aspects of religion are indeed adaptations selected through natural selection to solve a particular problem, or if they are by-products of other psychological mechanisms. Given that the debate is still ongoing, this section will describe both stands on religion, that which argues religious belief is an adaptation and that which argues it is a by-product. Religion as an Adaptation: Society as Organismic The main theory arguing for religion as an adaptation views religion as organismic (Wilson 2002). Religious believers often compare their communities to a single organism that can work similar to individuals in the harmony and cooperation of their parts. Simply put, religious groups may work like social insect colonies. Organisms are a product of natural selection that acquires properties that enable them to survive and reproduce through variation. If religious groups are organismic in this sense, particular groups may have evolved because their properties proved more adaptive than the properties from other groups. For instance, imagine a flock of birds. Selection at the level of the gene would involve birds Religion varying in the degree to which they alert other birds of approaching predators. An individual bird does not gain anything by alerting other birds of danger, unless the surrounding birds are kin and share genes with the caller. Instead, a hypervigilant bird may be detracted from feeding by spending more time scanning the horizon, and by drawing attention from predators. As a result, these callers are less likely to survive and reproduce than non-caller birds. Generations later, the remaining birds are the descendants of non-callers. An organismic theory, on the other hand, would argue that a group composed of mostly non-caller birds could face possible extinction and that natural selection would favor groups of callers over groups of non-callers. In this case, suppose there are two different flocks. The first flock is the one described above, in which the new generation is mostly composed of selfish birds that will not alert others of predators. The second flock is formed of mostly callers, with a few noncallers. When danger comes, the first group will have few or no birds alerting. Even if there are a few callers, chances are predators will get these birds first since raising an alarm makes them an easy, conspicuous target. At one point, the group will be out of callers to raise alarms, and the unsuspecting birds will become another animal’s meals and go extinct. The second flock, however, has better odds at surviving as a group. Because most birds are callers, some will die when alerting others, but some will also survive and further reproduce. The “caller” genes will be passed on to future generations, and the flock will likely live on. But how do flocks of callers and non-caller birds compare to humans and religion? First and foremost, it is important to remember that humans are social animals. Our ancestors would not have lived long had they not realized that cooperation was a reliable method to survive. To support this idea, Wilson (2002) mentions how current huntergatherer societies are strong units of selection and egalitarianism. Meat is usually shared among all members of the community, with the best hunter and his family getting no more meat than others. Additionally, it appears many hunter-gatherer Religion societies do not have leaders, aside from those who have earned the respect of other members by being models of good conduct and advise. Such egalitarian utopia seems counterintuitive to the concept of the selfish gene, yet even members in these societies are likely to have selfish impulses. The reason why they do not act on these impulses is because they are effectively controlled by other members of the group, a form of guarded egalitarianism known as reverse dominance. In many animal groups, the strongest individual is usually capable of dominating any rivals, but in hunter-gatherer tribes, any individual who attempts to dominate the rest is likely to encounter combined resistance. Even the strongest individual would be no match for the collective strength of the group and could instead be punished for these dominance attempts. Examples of reverse domination range from gossip and ridicule to ostracism and assassination. With such costly consequences, individuals are better off by simply cooperating and following the group’s social norms. Mechanisms that promote the welfare of the group as whole without demanding extreme sacrifice from its members would further reinforce adherence to social norms. It is here where religion and superstitious beliefs come into place. Wilson (2002) describes the case of the Chewong, a hunter-gatherer tribe that resides in the rainforest of the Malay Peninsula and that functions on egalitarianism like many other hunter-gatherer societies. Equal distribution of food and resources is governed by a system of superstitions known as punen in which not satisfying an urgent desire results in calamity. Actions that may result in a state of punen include not inviting someone to share food brought back from the hunt, and not sharing gathered goods when these were obtained from far away regions or when these are scarce. Belief in the punen system conveys great advantages, and it is probably not by mere coincidence that the actions that may result in calamity have to do with food sharing. If a gatherer had to go very far to obtain a resource, then such resource must be shared with everyone, whereas if the resource is close to the settlement, individuals are allowed to keep whatever amounts they 3 gather on their own. Such rules make perfect sense from an evolutionary perspective; forcing others to share resources that can easily be acquired would not convey any advantages because anyone would be able to gather these anyway, whereas sharing resources that were hard to obtain increases the chances of survival for those individuals who would have never been able to acquire these goods by themselves. Additionally, if all members truly believe that calamity will befall those who do not share, then they are more likely to obey any injunctions imposed by the group. Overall, fear in a higher, punishing force or entity ensures compliance and obedience. Groups with such altruistic beliefs like the Chewong would have been more likely to cooperate and aid each other than groups with selfish beliefs, and because cooperation in a hostile environment is essential for survival, tribes with selfish systems would have disappeared. In this sense, like a beehive that works as one single organism because all its parts work together to ensure survival, religion serves as an adaptation that allows humans to function as a coherent whole. Religion as a By-product: From Hyperactive Agency Detection to Mating and Attachment Though an interesting theory, Wilson’s (2002) proposal of viewing society as organismic and religion as an adaptation that allows groups to function as a whole is not an idea that goes unchallenged. Selection at the level of the gene rather than within-group selection is a major key idea in evolutionary studies. Furthermore, one must consider other possible explanations and assess which one can do a better job at explaining the wider array of phenomena that religion has to offer. As Buss (2002) states, “Explanations for some religious phenomena, such as rituals and rites, may fail to account for other phenomena such as piousness or prayer.” In order for a religion-specific mechanism to have evolved, it would have had to solve a particular adaptive problem faced by our ancestors, yet religion 4 encompasses such a diverse amalgamation of beliefs and behaviors that is highly unlikely that it is the product of a single adaptation. Additionally, as discussed previously, many features of religion are too recent to have been shaped by natural selection and thus work as an adaptation. Finally, any hypothesis on the evolutionary origins of religion should not assume the existence of yet other unexplained mechanisms. An argument that religion evolved to ameliorate fear of death begs the question as to why humans would be built to even fear death in the first place (Kirkpatrick 1999). Finally, as Tooby and Cosmides (1992) state, our cognition resembles a confederation of hundreds of functionally dedicated computers, each designed to solve one particular problem faced during our evolutionary history. There is not one single general purpose computer capable of solving several adaptive problems at once. It is unlikely that religion is the computer in charge of creating cooperative behaviors among social groups, solving humans’ fear of death, monitoring sexual conduct and reducing promiscuity, preventing immoral actions, and so forth. Instead, the idea that religion is a by-product resulting from numerous domainspecific psychological mechanisms is perhaps more plausible and could help explain the myriad near universals that were described at the beginning of this section. Because our minds evolved to process the world in ways that are adaptive rather than truthful, our cognition is prone to the occasional systematic error. For instance, it seems humans come equipped with mechanisms for categorizing and reasoning about the natural world (Kirkpatrick 1999). These include reliably distinguishing animate from inanimate objects, invoking different principles of inference such as causal reasoning, and ascribing goals, desires, and motives to people and objects displaying certain patterns of motion. Sometimes, these mechanisms may be misapplied beyond their intended domains. For instance, humans tend to have what is known as a hyperactive agency detection device. This device allows us to assume agency in inanimate objects, which may have been adaptive during our Pleistocene years, as confusing a rattling branch Religion for a snake and avoiding it did not present costs to survival, whereas ignoring a rattling sound could lead to death if such sound did, in fact, turn out to be a snake. It is generally safer to mistake an inanimate object for an animate one, yet when these mechanisms are taken a step too far, an individual may ascribe agency and even anthropomorphize inanimate objects. As mentioned previously Peoples et al. (2016) found that animism or attributing a soul to plants, inanimate objects and natural phenomena was the oldest and most common religious characteristic found across a wide sample of current hunter-gatherer societies. These findings suggest humans are particularly prone to assign some form of life or essence to objects. Social exchange and kin-related mechanisms are also manifested in religious beliefs and practices. Ancestor worship, reciprocal altruism, promotion of social cohesion and cooperation, and subordination of individuals’ self-interest to that of the group are other common characteristics of religions (Kirkpatrick 1999). Worshipping dead ancestors reflects kin altruism, as the living attempt to help the dead in the afterlife by burying artifacts and possessions while asking for help and guidance with their own worldly affairs. Attempts to enforce altruism do not end there, as many religions promote prosocial behavior beyond blood ties through the widespread use of kinship terms such as calling other members “brothers and sisters” or calling a priest “father.” Promoting deities that monitor cooperation and altruism and punish those who do not abide by these rules may also be an encouraging method for promoting prosocial behavior. Additionally, altruistic and kin mechanisms can be observed not only in how people interact with each other and with their ancestors, but also in how they interact with the supernatural forces that they believe control nature, be it a high god or other entity. If a deity is perceived as kin, then attachment mechanisms may become activated. Perceiving God as a fatherly figure or as a caretaker plays on the attachment system by which the caregiver is sought as a haven of safety, particularly during times of danger. If a deity is not perceived as kin, interaction with it should be Religion guided by the same principles of reciprocal altruism. Such seems to be the case given practices in which individuals offer deities different forms of offerings (e.g., impressive and massive monuments, works of art, and a variety of sacrifices) with the intention of gaining favors (e.g., good harvests, military victory, a place in heaven) from these high-status forces. In general, supernatural beings across religions tend to resemble humans, albeit with some “super” characteristics added. Deities may be ubiquitous or have the power to control some aspect of nature, but they otherwise display the full range of standard human emotions, desires, and motives, which is why interactions between humans and deities operate under the same psychological mechanisms as interaction amongst humans. One final mechanism worth mentioning that seems to come into play in religion relates to mating and controlling others’ sexual behavior. As Buss (2002) points out, religious doctrines frequently target mating and sexual behavior. When it comes to sociosexuality, religion has tried to regulate aspects such as when conception should take place, the age at which intercourse should begin, how many sexual partners an individual is allowed to have, the moral standing of premarital sex, and so on (Schmitt and Fuller 2015). According to the reproductive religiosity model (Weeden et al. 2008), heightened religiosity is often adaptively used by religious groups to discourage sexual permissiveness. Discouraging unrestricted sex is beneficial for males in that it decreases the chances of being cuckolded, and it is also beneficial for females in that it helps ensure their husbands will not abandon their long-term commitments to themselves and their offspring. Several studies suggest a link between religiosity and measures of permissive sexuality. For instance, Strassman et al. (2012) found that religions that were stricter in forcing females to signal menstruation and had higher levels of mate guarding had higher chances of paternity certainty. Another aspect of religion as a device for mating is that religious leaders are often males who hold power and resources, which are important for females when looking for mates. Buss 5 (2002) explains that religious leaders use their power to gain preferential access to fertile women. In native Greenland, for example, a woman would be considered fortunate if the Angekokk or prophet offered his sexual caresses. Similarly, the religious leaders among the Tachtadshys in Lycia could have sexual intercourse with any women they fancied during the yearly religious assemblies, and virgins among the Zikris were believed to be cleansed if they had intercourse with the high priest. These examples show how religious leaders had better access to females than the average man, which in turn increased their reproductive success. Conclusion The evolutionary origins of religion are still unknown, and religion is composed of such a plethora of phenomena that it is hard to separate one aspect from the other. Although it is tempting to believe religion is an adaptation, it seems to be involved in one too many aspects of the human psyche to have evolved to solve a particular adaptive problem. Additionally, several aspects of religion seem to have evolved across time, with some practices such as burying the dead emerging before others such as belief in high gods or injunctions on moral sexual behavior. If religion did evolve as an adaptation, it could be possible that later components (e.g., belief in a punishing deity to ensure compliance to norms imposed by the group) were added to it because humans realized the value in them. Cross-References ▶ Joseph Henrich on Religion ▶ Religion and Morality ▶ Religion and Religious Beliefs ▶ Religion as a By-product ▶ Religion as an Adaptation 6 References Buss, D. M. (2002). Sex, marriage, and religion: What adaptive problems do religious phenomena solve? Psychological Inquiry, 13(3), 201–203. Kirkpatrick, L. A. (1999). Toward an evolutionary psychology of religion and personality. Journal of Personality, 67(6), 921–952. Peoples, H. C., Duda, P., & Marlowe, F. W. (2016). Huntergatherers and the origins of religion. Human Nature, 27(3), 261–282. Schmitt, D. P., & Fuller, R. C. (2015). On the varieties of sexual experience: Cross-cultural links between religiosity and human mating strategies. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 7(4), 314. Strassmann, B. I., Kurapati, N. T., Hug, B. F., Burke, E. E., Gillespie, B. W., Karafet, T. M., & Hammer, M. F. Religion (2012). Religion as a means to assure paternity. PNAS Proceedings Of The National Academy Of Sciences Of The United States Of America, 109(25), 9781–9785. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1110442109. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2000). Toward Mapping the Evolved Functional Organization of Mind and Brain. In Gazzaniga, M. (Ed), The New Cognitive Neurosciences, 1167–1178. MIT Press, Cambridge Weeden, J., Cohen, A. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (2008). Religious attendance as reproductive support. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(5), 327–334. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.03.004. Wilson, D. S. (2002). Darwin’s cathedral: Evolution, religion, and the nature of society. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.