See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232999554 Assessing Gibb's Supportive and Defensive Communication Climate: An Examination of Measurement and Construct Validity Article in Communication Research Reports · February 2011 DOI: 10.1080/08824096.2011.541360 CITATIONS READS 12 2,554 3 authors: G. L. Forward Kathleen Czech Point Loma Nazarene University San Diego State University 8 PUBLICATIONS 67 CITATIONS 5 PUBLICATIONS 50 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Carmen Lee University of Southern California 4 PUBLICATIONS 42 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Thesis on healthy eating View project All content following this page was uploaded by Kathleen Czech on 07 July 2016. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. SEE PROFILE Communication Research Reports Vol. 28, No. 1, January–March 2011, pp. 1–15 Assessing Gibb’s Supportive and Defensive Communication Climate: An Examination of Measurement and Construct Validity G. L. Forward, Kathleen Czech, & Carmen M. Lee This project investigated the utility of Gibb’s (1961) theory of supportive and defensive communication as operationalized by Costigan and Schmeidler (1984) in their survey instrument. As part of a larger project, 202 randomly selected faculty members completed a survey assessing the supportive and defensive communication behaviors of their current department chair. These data were then examined to determine the internal reliability and consistency of the instrument, as well as underlying dimensional stability. The results of this analysis indicate the presence of significant multicollinearity and a 4-factor, rather than a 12-factor, solution as originally hypothesized by Gibb. The article concludes with suggestions for reconceptualizing and interpreting the communication climate construct as a function of two underlying dimensions, including one supportive and one defensive behavior focused on task and one supportive and one defensive behavior focused on interpersonal relationships. Keywords: Communication Climate; Defensive Communication; Measurement; Supportive Communication; Validity G. L. Forward (PhD, The Ohio State University, 1994) is a professor in the Department of Communication and Theatre at Point Loma Nazarene University. Kathleen Czech (EdD, University of San Diego, 2007) is a professor in the Department of Communication and Theatre at Point Loma Nazarene University. Carmen M. Lee (PhD, University of California–Santa Barbara, 2007) is an assistant professor in the School of Communication at San Diego State University. The dataset utilized in this project was part of Kathleen Czech’s dissertation. An earlier version of this manuscript was presented to the Group Communication Division at the National Communication Association Convention, November 22, 2008, San Diego, CA. Correspondence: G. L. Forward, Department of Communication and Theatre, Point Loma Nazarene University, 3900 Lomaland Dr., San Diego, CA 92106; E-mail: glforward@pointloma.edu ISSN 0882-4096 (print)/ISSN 1746-4099 (online) # 2011 Eastern Communication Association DOI: 10.1080/08824096.2011.541360 2 G. L. Forward et al. Gibb’s (1961) summary of his long-term observations about small group interaction has achieved iconic status. Despite the fact that Gibb’s eight-page, 1961 Journal of Communication article does not have a single citation, his theory of supportive and defensive communication is both ubiquitous and enduring. The supportive– defensive communication construct has been applied to the study of persuasion (Eadie, 1982; Winer & Majors, 1981), family therapy (Kingstone & Endler, 1997), education (Myers 1995; Myers & Rocca, 2001), cultural diversity (Schauber, 2001), and organizational effectiveness (Cross, 1978; Larsen & Folgero, 1993). In addition, the theory remains popular with communication textbook authors, despite minimal empirical research supporting the construct as originally explicated by Gibb. Nearly 50 years after its initial introduction, many small group, interpersonal, and communication theory texts mention some or all of Gibb’s notions about supportive and defensive communication as it relates to communication climate. In this article, we briefly summarize Gibb’s (1961) theory and define the 12 categories of supportive and defensive communication behaviors that he described. The specific contribution of this article, however, is the empirical examination of the reliability and underlying dimensionality of the supportive–defensive communication construct, as operationalized by Costigan and Schmeidler (1984). Finally, we critique Gibb’s theory based on our empirical findings and suggest two underlying dimensions that more elegantly and accurately capture the global dynamics inherent in Gibb’s theory while retaining the utility of many of the specific communication behaviors in the original model. Literature Review Jack Gibb (1961) was one of the first scholars to attempt to identify specific communication behaviors that contributed to the development of an overarching communication climate. In an eight-year study of small, task-oriented groups, Gibb identified specific communication patterns that tended to increase or decrease defensiveness between people. These communication patterns are represented in most contemporary small-group communication texts. Defensive Versus Supportive Communication A defensive communication climate is one in which an individual feels threatened or anxious when in communication with others (Gibb, 1961). A defensive conversation may appear normal outwardly while inwardly the person is investing considerable mental energy in defending him or herself. According to Gibb, as a person becomes more defensive, they become less able to accurately perceive the motives, values, and emotions of the sender. Major themes in a defensive communication climate include critical, judgmental attitudes that overshadow working conditions, inflexibility and dogmatism, autocratic management, manipulation, little personal support, and pervasive feelings of inadequacy (Myers & Rocca, 2001). Conversely, major communication themes in supportive environments foreground encouragement and understanding. Supportive communication is clear and Communication Research Reports 3 accurate with information that is freely shared. Member opinions are accepted, and individuals are encouraged to achieve their goals. Most importantly, accusation and blame are minimized. Supportive communication that is accepting, nonjudgmental, empathic, and does not make assumptions about the other person’s motives is a necessary part of interpersonal effectiveness (Myers & Rocca, 2001). Behavioral Categories Gibb (1961) delineated 12 categories of behaviors that tend to create either a supportive or defensive communication climate. The categories are arrayed in six pairs linking one supportive behavior with its’ conceptual opposite defensive behavior. These six pairs of climate factors are contrasted in specific communication behaviors explicated in the following (see Table 1). Evaluation versus description. Evaluation consists of communication behaviors that engage in judgmental and accusatory language. Evaluation is often marked by ‘‘you language’’ that places blame on the other person. Communication that is descriptive, in contrast, tends to arouse a minimum of uneasiness. Language in which the listener perceives a need for information and a genuine desire to understand another’s view is descriptive. Descriptive communication is marked by the use of ‘‘I language’’ that places responsibility on the sender of the message (Gibb, 1961). Control versus problem orientation. Language that is used to control the listener evokes defensiveness. Control is often marked by implicit attempts to be manipulative. The speaker may view the listener as ignorant, uniformed, unwise, or possessing of inappropriate attitudes. Problem orientation, however, uses language that is not overtly persuasive or controlling, but rather focuses on a desire for collaboration. The sender utilizes language that seeks a mutual definition of the problem. The speaker thus implies that there is no predetermined solution, attitude, or method to impose and they are open to finding the best solution (Gibb, 1961). Strategy versus spontaneity. Strategy is a communication behavior that implies hidden motives and deceit. When a sender is perceived as engaging in strategic communication involving ambiguous and multiple motivations, the receiver becomes Table 1 Behavioral Characteristics of Supportive and Defensive Communication Climates Defensive climates 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. Evaluation (EVL) Control (CON) Strategy (STR) Neutrality (NEU) Superiority (SUP) Certainty (CER) Supportive climates 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. Description (DES) Problem Orientation (PRB) Spontaneity (SPO) Empathy (EMP) Equality (EQU) Provisionalism (PRV) 4 G. L. Forward et al. defensive. Gibb (1961) suggested that the antidote is communication that is spontaneous. Spontaneity is defined as straightforwardness, directness, and honesty. Spontaneity consists of communication that is consistent, genuine, and transparent (Rothwell, 2007). Neutrality versus empathy. Neutrality in speech occurs when a speaker indicates a lack of concern for the welfare of the listener. Most people desire to be valued as persons and to be seen worthy of concern and affection. Communication that exhibits low affect and little warmth or caring is often seen as rejection (Gibb, 1961). This indifference is countered with empathy. Empathy is thinking and feeling what you perceive another to be thinking and feeling (Rothwell, 2007). Communication that conveys empathy contains messages that indicate that the speaker indentifies with the listener’s problems, shares their feelings, and accepts emotional reactions at face value. Superiority versus equality. When a person communicates to another that they feel superior by virtue of a position, power, wealth, intellectual ability, or physical characteristics, they arouse defensiveness. A receiver of this type of communication is likely to react by not hearing the message, forgetting, competing with the sender, or by becoming jealous of them (Gibb, 1961). Equality recognizes that whatever the differences in our abilities, talents, or intellect, that in order to produce encouragement and productivity, one should treat people with respect and politeness and as equals (Rothwell, 2007). Certainty versus provisionalism. Certainty is defined as dogmatic, single-minded behavior combined with an unwillingness to compromise. The dogmatic individual is seen as needing to be right and wanting to win the argument rather than solve the problem. Provisionalsim reduces defensiveness by allowing a provisional attitude, a willingness to investigate issues rather than taking sides, and demonstrating openness to new possibilities (Gibb, 1961). Summary Although Gibb’s (1961) theory of supportive and defensive communication is enduring, ubiquitous, and conceptually appealing, it has received little elaboration or empirical support, despite its iconic status. In large measure, the paucity of empirical data is due to the fact that Gibb himself never published a survey instrument that would enable other researchers to explore, verify, or refine his theory. In an effort to address this reality, we intend to provide additional information concerning the internal consistency and dimensionality of the Costigan and Schmeidler (1984) operationalization of Gibb’s 12 supportive and defensive communication behaviors. Method Participants The participants in this study (N ¼ 202) were randomly selected faculty members who teach at institutions affiliated with the Council of Independent Colleges Communication Research Reports 5 1 (CIC), headquartered in Washington, DC. The sample was solicited as part of a larger research project evaluating department chair leadership (Czech & Forward, 2010). After securing a CIC membership directory, we consulted a table of random numbers to identify a starting point and selected every k ¼ 27th university until we had a list of 26 schools. Each department in those schools was grouped into one of four broad academic domains (humanities, professional studies, social sciences, and physical sciences). One department from each group was randomly selected; and all full-time faculty members, excluding the chair, were contacted by mail and asked to evaluate their department chairs’ communication behaviors. This procedure resulted in N ¼ 202 usable surveys evaluating 104 different department chairs. This represents a 48% response rate, which is generally acceptable for mailed surveys (Dillman, 2000), and is especially robust given the many requests for assistance received by faculty members (Baruch, 1999). The 202 participants in this study ranged in age from 27 to 82 years, with a mean age of 50.3 (SD ¼ 10.9). Fifty-three percent (n ¼ 106) were women, and the sample was predominately White (83.2%; n ¼ 168). These participants had been at their current institution for an average of 10.6 years (SD ¼ 9.5) and had been involved in higher education for 16.7 years (SD ¼ 11.3) total. Procedure and Instrumentation The Communication Climate Inventory (CCI) operationalizes Gibb’s (1961) initial 12 factors assessing supportive and defensive communication behaviors within organizational workgroups (Larsen & Folgero, 1993). Thirty-six questions are presented in a Likert-type format scaled from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always) (see the Appendix). The original wording of each question was altered to reflect the department chair and faculty member relationship as the specific context of the interaction. The perceived level of supportive–defensive communication was assessed with questions like the following: ‘‘My chair treats me with respect’’ (supportive), and ‘‘My chair criticizes my work in the presence of others’’ (defensive). The scores of the first 18 questions indicate the degree to which the faculty’s relationship with their department chair reflects the six dimensions of a supportive communication climate. The next 18 questions indicate the degree to which the faculty’s relationship with their department chair reflects the six dimensions of a defensive communication climate. The inventory can be used to measure an organization’s total communication environment or the climate of individual work relationships (Costigan & Schmeidler, 1984). In this study, we utilized the CCI to assess the individual communication climate between a department chair and individual faculty members. Descriptive summaries including means, standard deviations, skewness, and kurtosis for each item comprising Costigan and Schmeidler’s (1984) CCI are presented in Table 2. We assessed the normality of the data by examining skewness 6 G. L. Forward et al. Table 2 Original Communication Climate Inventory (Means, Standard Deviations, Skewness, and Kurtosis) Item Supportive climate items DES 1 DES 2 DES 3 PRB 1 PRB 2 PRB 3 PRV 1 PRV 2 PRV 3 EMP 1 EMP 2 EMP 3 EQU 1 EQU 2 EQU 3 SPO 1 SPO 2 SPO 3 Defensive climate items EVL 1 EVL 2 EVL 3 SUP 1 SUP 2 SUP 3 CER 1 CER 2 CER 3 NEU 1 NEU 2 NEU 3 CON 1 CON 2 CON 3 STR 1 STR 2 STR 3 M SD Skewness Kurtosis 3.90 3.95 3.73 4.01 4.41 4.02 4.43 4.41 4.08 3.89 4.18 4.09 4.40 4.17 4.36 3.94 4.12 4.15 1.09 1.10 1.09 0.98 1.04 1.01 0.88 0.89 1.01 1.15 1.06 1.09 1.00 1.12 1.07 1.15 1.11 1.10 0.87 0.82 0.70 0.88 1.98 1.15 1.76 1.70 1.02 0.96 1.50 1.39 1.82 1.47 1.86 0.95 1.29 1.39 0.13 0.39 0.33 0.03 3.27 0.86 2.84 2.74 0.33 0.10 1.83 1.49 2.63 1.39 2.73 0.05 0.85 1.15 1.53 2.58 1.47 1.40 2.39 1.85 1.85 1.73 1.86 3.87 2.77 3.82 2.16 1.71 2.26 1.92 2.18 1.61 0.91 1.19 0.88 0.88 1.25 0.99 1.06 1.06 1.13 1.06 1.04 1.02 1.05 0.94 1.16 1.21 1.20 0.98 2.03 0.24 2.17 2.61 0.55 1.31 1.28 1.48 1.36 0.87 0.00 0.78 0.63 1.65 0.70 1.31 0.84 1.84 3.94 0.99 4.47 6.62 0.71 1.43 1.00 1.40 1.04 0.23 0.67 0.29 0.46 2.79 0.48 0.67 0.29 3.00 Note. DES ¼ description; PRB ¼ problem orientation; PRV ¼ provisionalism; EMP ¼ empathy; EQU ¼ equality; SPO ¼ spontaneity; EVL ¼ evaluation; SUP ¼ superiority; CER ¼ certainty; NUE ¼ neutrality; CON ¼ control; STR ¼ strategy. Communication Research Reports 7 and kurtosis (Kline, 2005). Results revealed that these data were not normally distributed. The normal range for skewness is between 1.00 and þ1.00; however, these data were skewed with scores ranging from 1.98 to 2.61. In addition, the kurtosis scores ranged from 0.99 to 6.62, with many scores outside of the normal range (i.e., 3.00 to þ3.00). As a result, we performed log transformations of the skewed data to approximate normal distributions. Table 3 presents the means, standard deviations, skewness, and kurtosis of the CCI items after log transformation. Results Internal Consistency We have included two estimates of internal consistency for each supportive and defensive subscale. As can be seen in Table 4, the coefficient alphas for each subscale are generally robust. Keyton (2001) suggested that an alpha of .70 is a generally accepted standard for communication research. All of the communication subscales, except neutrality (a ¼ .65), met or exceeded this standard. This initial assessment of internal consistency suggests that the subscales might have utility as parsimonious, stand-alone measures of these constructs. Second, a correlation matrix was obtained to explore the possibility of collinearity among the subscales (see Table 4). Monge (1980) suggested that Pearson rs > .70 may be indicative of multicollinearity. Again, with the exception of neutrality, the resultant correlations are high across the board, and a great many exceed the suggested .70 standard, indicating that the variables may be too closely related and that some items may essentially be measuring the same things. To further test for this possibility, we ran an additional procedure that provides tolerance and variation inflation factor (VIF) diagnostics. Tolerance values significantly below 1 indicate collinearly, whereas values close to 1 indicate little or no overlap between the variables assessed. Likewise, VIF values of 1 indicate little or no association between the variables, whereas VIF values >2 reveal unacceptably high correlations (Blaikie, 2003). The diagnostics in Table 5 suggest that there is a significant problem of multicollinearity among the variables in this dataset. We explore these findings in more detail through factor analysis. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) of the CCI Subsequent to data transformation, we submitted the 36-items of the CCI to a second-order CFA using Amos 16.0 (Arbuckle, 2004). We used maximum likelihood estimation to test the 12-factor model. To assess the overall fit of this model, we examined the following indexes: chi-square likelihood ratio (v2), the ratio of chi-square to degrees of freedom (v2=df), the comparative fit index (CFI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). The model would be deemed adequate if: (a) the CFI had a value of .90 or higher; (b) the RMSEA had a value of .08 or lower; and (c) the ratio of v2=df had a value 8 G. L. Forward et al. Table 3 Final Communication Climate Inventory (Means, Standard Deviations, Skewness, and Kurtosis) Item Supportive climate items DES 1 DES 2 DES 3 PRB 1 PRB 2 PRB 3 PRV 1 PRV 2 PRV 3 EMP 1 EMP 2 EMP 3 EQU 1 EQU 2 EQU 3 SPO 1 SPO 2 SPO 3 Defensive climate items EVL 1 EVL 2 EVL 3 SUP 1 SUP 2 SUP 3 CER 1 CER 2 CER 3 NEU 1 NEU 2 NEU 3 CON 1 CON 2 CON 3 Str1 Str2 Str3 M SD Skewness Kurtosis 3.90 3.95 3.73 4.01 0.14 0.25 0.14 0.15 0.23 3.89 0.20 0.22 0.14 0.20 0.15 3.93 0.21 0.20 1.09 1.10 1.09 0.98 0.21 0.21 0.20 0.20 0.22 1.15 0.22 0.22 0.21 0.23 0.22 1.15 0.23 0.23 0.87 0.82 0.70 0.88 1.26 0.25 1.00 0.93 0.32 0.96 0.64 0.49 1.15 0.72 1.15 0.95 0.58 0.65 0.13 0.39 0.33 0.03 0.38 0.89 0.20 0.38 1.17 0.10 0.69 0.81 0.02 0.68 0.07 0.05 0.94 0.79 0.13 2.58 0.11 0.09 2.39 0.21 0.21 0.17 0.20 3.87 2.77 3.82 2.16 0.81 2.26 0.21 2.17 0.15 0.20 1.19 0.19 0.19 1.25 0.21 0.22 0.22 0.23 1.06 1.04 1.02 1.05 0.20 1.16 0.24 1.20 0.21 1.22 0.24 1.43 1.85 0.55 0.43 0.55 0.84 0.66 0.87 0.00 0.78 0.63 0.69 0.70 0.66 0.84 1.08 0.36 0.99 0.87 2.32 0.71 0.89 0.96 0.66 0.87 0.23 0.67 0.29 0.46 0.54 0.48 0.89 0.29 0.08 Note. DES ¼ description; PRB ¼ problem orientation; PRV ¼ provisionalism; EMP ¼ empathy; EQU ¼ equality; SPO ¼ spontaneity; EVL ¼ evaluation; SUP ¼ superiority; CER ¼ certainty; NUE ¼ neutrality; CON ¼ control; STR ¼ strategy. Communication Research Reports 9 Table 4 Correlation Matrix Variable 1. Description 2. Problem orientation 3. Provisionalism 4. Empathy 5. Equality 6. Spontaneity 7. Superiority 8. Evaluation 9. Certainty 10. Neutrality 11. Control 12. Strategy 1 2 3 4 5 6 .87 .71 .62 .67 .67 .67 .56 .59 .71 .40 .66 .71 .77 .73 .84 .75 .75 .54 .63 .65 .64 .61 .74 .88 .79 .79 .74 .65 .70 .68 .44 .67 .73 .91 .85 .83 .61 .71 .70 .59 .63 .79 .91 .85 .74 .81 .70 .46 .73 .81 .93 .63 .73 .72 .49 .69 .86 7 8 9 10 11 12 .71 .73 .73 .75 .67 .90 .39 .40 .44 .65 .78 .68 .76 .45 .82 .69 .75 .78 .46 .76 .90 Note. N ¼ 202. All correlations are statistically significant at p < .01. The italicized values on the diagonal are the Cronbach’s alphas’ for each subscale. below 3 (see Kline, 2005; Marsh, Balla, & McDonald, 1988; Quintana & Maxwell, 1999). The results of these calculations indicate that the proposed model does not fit the data well, v2(581, N ¼ 202) ¼ 1,440.71, p < .001; v2=df ¼ 2.48, CFI ¼ .87, RMSEA ¼ .09 (see Figure 1). Despite the inadequacy of the model, it should be noted that all of the standardized path coefficients were significant, except for the NEU 2 Table 5 Collinearity Diagnostics for Communication Behaviors Collinearity statistics Communication behavior Supportive Description Problem orientation Provisionalism Empathy Equality Spontaneity Defensive Superiority Evaluation Certainty Neutrality Control Strategy Tolerance Variation inflation factor .44 .25 .31 .16 .20 .22 2.30 4.10 3.22 6.37 5.02 4.61 .29 .35 .28 .74 .28 .27 3.48 2.89 3.60 1.35 3.57 3.78 10 G. L. Forward et al. Figure 1 Second-Order Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Original Communication Climate Inventory. item (b ¼ 0.13, p ¼ .11). Each subscale and the items that are expected to load on those subscales, according to Gibb (1961), are detailed in Figure 1. Given the lack of fit of the original CCI data, we conducted a principal component analysis with promax rotation on the 36-items to assess dimensionality. The analysis produced a highly satisfactory Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy (¼ .95) resulting in a five-factor solution accounting for 69.83% of the total variance (see Table 6). However, the fifth factor contained only a single neutrality (NEU 2) item. Since factors with a single loading should be considered volatile and unreliable, we have excluded it from any additional analysis (Mertler & Vannatta, 2005). The remaining four factors have been labeled and arranged in a 2 2 matrix indicating the types of questions that loaded on each factor (see Table 7). The first factor, which we called ‘‘collaboration,’’ involves how one interpersonally approaches others and includes provisionalism, equality, spontaneity, and empathy. In addition, we note that strategy, originally hypothesized by Gibb (1961) to be a disconfirming behavior, has almost identical loadings on both of the positively valenced factors. The second factor involving a combination of control, certainty, and superiority was labeled ‘‘authoritarianism.’’ The third factor, which we labeled ‘‘descriptive orientation,’’ explains how one approaches the issue under discussion and includes all three description items and problem orientation. Again, several items have cross-loadings that exceed .40 and complicate interpretation (see Table 6). The last factor, which involves a combination of neutrality and interpersonal indifference, we labeled ‘‘manipulation.’’ Communication Research Reports 11 Table 6 Final Communication Climate Inventory Factor Structure Variable Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4 Factor 5 EQU 3 EMP 2 EQU 1 SPO 3 SPO 1 PRB 2 STR 3 EMP 3 SPO 2 PRV 1 SUP 1 EVL 3 PRV 2 EQU 2 STR 2 STR 1 EMP 1 EVL 1 PRV 3 CER 3 CON 3 SUP 3 CER 2 SUP 2 CON 2 CON 1 EVL 2 DES 3 DES 1 DES 2 PRB 1 CER 1 NEU 1 NEU 3 PRB 3 NUE 2 .877 .876 .874 .861 .838 .838 .834 .832 .824 .821 .819 .814 .791 .786 .769 .768 .723 .715 .694 .660 .538 .487 .679 .501 .722 .618 .345 .616 .530 .703 .670 .639 .409 .450 .517 .176 .511 .515 .520 .510 .613 .467 .594 .482 .473 .522 .535 .532 .557 .617 .602 .683 .364 .625 .529 .826 .824 .790 .751 .733 .723 .703 .660 .471 .447 .565 .558 .712 .392 .309 .145 .238 .601 .703 .586 .634 .697 .594 .709 .658 .647 .529 .673 .644 .473 .563 .716 .763 .619 .447 .584 .693 .525 .488 .723 .259 .599 .701 .379 .904 .839 .787 .787 .738 .350 .397 .559 .167 .553 .685 .445 .562 .529 .657 .453 .719 .549 .450 .359 .376 .391 .455 .581 .447 .713 .381 .581 .489 .356 .270 .478 .196 .435 .408 .266 .452 .391 .447 .610 .507 .804 .767 .728 .042 .021 .079 .051 .016 .036 .082 .040 .101 .035 .195 .155 .160 .264 .026 .005 .039 .232 .031 .338 .106 .019 .044 .141 .035 .111 .006 .167 .042 .060 .059 .085 .085 .050 .115 .033 .842 Note. Values in bold font represent primary factor loadings. DES ¼ description; PRB ¼ problem orientation; PRV ¼ provisionalism; EMP ¼ empathy; EQU ¼ equality; SPO ¼ spontaneity; EVL ¼ evaluation; SUP ¼ superiority; CER ¼ certainty; NUE ¼ neutrality; CON ¼ control; STR ¼ strategy. 12 G. L. Forward et al. Table 7 Underlying Dimensions of Supportive and Defensive Communication Climates Orientation Task People Supportive Collaboration including provisionalism, equality, spontaneity, and empathy Descriptive including description and problem orientation Defensive Authoritarian including control, certainty, and superiority Manipulation including strategy and neutrality Discussion Gibb’s (1961) theory of supportive and defensive communication has endured because it is relatively parsimonious, intuitively appealing, and generally consistent with the personal experiences of many. It is clear, however, that the construct as currently articulated and measured would benefit from additional investigation. Our analysis of the Costigan and Schmeidler (1984) operationalization suggests the following issues that need to be addressed. Dimensionality The empirical evidence seems not to support the notion of 12 discrete communication behaviors. Examination of the correlation matrix reveals high levels of collinearity, suggesting that some of the variables may essentially be measuring the same thing. Further, examination of the CFA suggests that supportive and defensive communication can each be better explained by examining the two underlying dynamics. Table 7 illustrates these dynamics. These underlying dimensions of supportive and defensive communication are congruent with earlier studies that concluded leadership can best be understood by evaluating one’s orientation toward people and orientation toward task (Hackman & Johnson, 2009). The factors extracted in this study advance that assertion by suggesting that orientation toward people exists on a continuum from collaboration to manipulation. In addition, orientation toward the task exists on a continuum from descriptive to authoritative. The specific communication behaviors that contribute to these four components were identified in the results section and can be further explored by looking at the individual questions and CFA (see the Appendix and Figure 1). Conclusion We contend that the notion of supportive and defensive communication is a powerful and useful one. Nonetheless, the empirical evidence presented here suggests potential problems with the underlying dimensionality of the primary instrument created to reflect the 12 behaviors originally hypothesized by Gibb (1961; see also Costigan & Schmeidler, 1984). However, rather than rejecting Gibb, we believe a reconceptualized model and subsequent instrument development could be useful Communication Research Reports 13 and worthwhile. A newly refined instrument should capitalize on a more nuanced understanding of the two underlying dimensions of communication climate as suggested in Table 7. The strength of the current measurement tool is in identifying specific behaviors that contribute to the two supportive (collaboration and description) and two defensive (manipulation and authoritarianism) communication climate dimensions. Finally, we urge caution in interpreting these results due to the unique nature of our sample. As full-time faculty members, our respondents are not representative of all types of employers or jobs and may not be representative of faculty in state-supported schools. Future research should endeavor to replicate these findings with other populations in other organizational contexts. In addition, researchers could begin their work with the proposed conceptual framework and refine a measurement tool that captures these global dynamics while simultaneously identifying specific behaviors that contribute to these interpersonal relational impressions. Note [1] Educational researchers have suggested that private and public universities may differ in significant ways due to governmental oversight and funding (Forward, Czech, & Allen, 2007). To control for this potentially confounding variable, we limited our sample to faculty at private, 4-year universities comprising the 544 member institutions belonging to the Council of Independent Colleges. References Arbuckle, J. L. (2004). Amos 16.0. Chicago: SPSS. Baruch, Y. (1999). Response rate in academic studies: A comparative analysis. Human Relations, 52, 421–438. Blaikie, N. (2003). Analyzing quantitative data. London, England: Sage. Costigan, J. L., & Schmeidler, M. A. (1984). Exploring supportive and defensive communication climates. In J. W. Pfeiffer & L. D. Goodstein (Eds.), The 1984 handbook for group facilitators (pp. 112–114). San Diego, CA: University Associates. Cross, G. (1978). How to overcome defensive communication. Personnel Journal, 3, 441–456. Czech, K., & Forward, G. L. (2010). Leader communication: Faculty perceptions of the department chair. Communication Quarterly, 58, 431–457. Dillman, D. (2000). Mail and Internet surveys: The tailored design method. New York, NY: Wiley. Eadie, W. F. (1982). Defensive communication revisited: A critical examination of Gibb’s theory. Southern Speech Communication Journal, 47, 163–177. Forward, G. L., Czech, K., & Allen, P. A. (2007). Leadership, communication and religiosity in higher education administration: Distinctions that make a difference. Journal of Communication and Religion, 30, 153–185. Gibb, J. (1961). Defensive communication. Journal of Communication, 11, 141–148. Hackman, M., & Johnson, C. (2009). Leadership: A communication perspective (5th ed.). Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland. 14 G. L. Forward et al. Keyton, J. (2001). Communication research: Asking questions, finding answers. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Kingstone, J. C., & Endler, N. S. (1997). Supportive vs. defensive communications in depression: An assessment of Coynes model. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 29, 44–53. Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practices of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford. Larsen, S., & Folgero, I. S. (1993). Supportive and defensive communication. International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management, 5(3), 22–25. Marsh, H. W., Balla, J. W., & McDonald, R. P. (1988). Goodness-of-fit indices in confirmatory factor analysis: Effects of sample size. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 391–411. Mertler, C. A., & Vannatta, R. A. (2005). Advanced and multivariate statistical methods: Practical application and interpretation. Glendale, CA: Pyrczak. Monge, P. R. (1980). Multivariate multiple regression. In P. R. Monge & C. N. Capella (Eds.), Multivariate techniques in human communication research (pp. 13–56). New York, NY: Academic. Myers, S. (1995). Student perceptions of teacher affinity-seeking and classroom climate. Communication Research Reports, 12, 192–199. Myers, S., & Rocca, K. A. (2001). Perceived instructor argumentativeness and verbal aggressiveness in the college classroom: Effects on student perceptions of climate, apprehension, and state motivation. Western Journal of Communication, 65, 113–137. Quintana, S. M., & Maxwell, S. E. (1999). Implications of recent developments in structural equation modeling for counseling psychology. Counseling Psychologist, 27, 485–527. Rothwell, J. D. (2007). In mixed company: Small group communication (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. Schauber, A. (2001). Effecting extension organizational change toward cultural diversity: A conceptual framework. Journal of Extension, 39(3), 1–7. Winer, S., & Majors, R. E. (1981). A research note on supportive and defensive communication: An empirical study of three verbal interpersonal communication variables. Communication Quarterly, 29, 166–172. Appendix Exploring Supportive and Defensive Communication Climates DES 1 My chair tries to describe situation fairly without labeling them as good or bad. DES 2 My chair presents his or her feelings and perceptions without implying that a similar response is expected from me. DES 3 My chair attempts to explain situations clearly and without personal bias. PRB 1 My chair defines problems so that they can be understood but does not insist that others agree. PRB 2 I feel free to talk to my chair. PRB 3 My chair defines problems and makes his or her faculty aware of them. PRV 1 My chair allows me as much creativity as possible in my job. PRV 2 My chair allows flexibility on the job. PRV 3 My chair is willing to try new ideas and to accept other points of view. EMP 1 My chair understands the problems that I encounter in my job. EMP 2 My chair respects my feelings and values. EMP 3 My chair listens to my problems with interest. Communication Research Reports 15 EQU 1 My chair does not try to make me feel inferior. EQU 2 My chair participates in meetings with faculty without projecting his or her higher status or power. EQU 3 My chair treats me with respect. SPO 1 My chair does not have hidden motives in dealing with me. SPO 2 I feel that I can be honest and straightforward with my chair. SPO 3 I feel that I can express my opinions and ideas honestly to my chair. EVL 1 My chair criticizes my work without allowing me to explain. EVL 2 My chair judges the actions of his or her faculty members. EVL 3 My chair criticizes my work in the presence of others. SUP 1 My chair tries to make me feel inadequate. SUP 2 My chair makes it clear that he or she is in chair SUP 3 My chair believes that if a job is to be done right, he or she must oversee it or do it. CER 1 My chair cannot admit that he or she makes mistakes. CER 2 My chair is dogmatic; it is useless for me to voice an opposing point of view CER 3 My chair thinks that he or she is always right. NEU 1 My chair is not interested in faculty personal problems. NEU 2 My chair becomes involved in faculty conflicts. NEU 3 My chair offers moral support during a personal crisis. CON 1 My chair tries to change other people’s attitudes and behaviors to suit his or her own. CON 2 My chair believes that he or she must control how I do my work. CON 3 My chair needs to be in charge of the situation. STR 1 My chair tries to manipulate faculty to get what he or she wants or to make himself or herself look good. STR 2 I have to be careful when talking to my chair so that I will not be misinterpreted. STR 3 My chair twists and distorts what I say when I speak what is really on my mind. Note. DES ¼ description; PRB ¼ problem orientation; PRV ¼ provisionalism; EMP ¼ empathy; EQU ¼ equality; SPO ¼ spontaneity; EVL ¼ evaluation; SUP ¼ superiority; CER ¼ certainty; NEU ¼ neutrality; CON ¼ control; STR ¼ strategy. Copyright of Communication Research Reports is the property of Eastern Communication Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. View publication stats