C Psychology in the Schools, Vol. 54(7), 2017 View this article online at wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/pits 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. DOI: 10.1002/pits.22024 RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN PSYCHOLOGICAL WELLBEING OF THAI COLLEGE STUDENTS, GOAL ORIENTATIONS, AND GENDER JOHN J. SOSIK The Pennsylvania State University JAE UK CHUN Korea University RAVINDER KOUL The Pennsylvania State University This paper examined the direct and interaction effects of students’ learning and performanceavoidance goal orientations on their psychological wellbeing and a moderating role of students’ gender in these relationships. Using 564 self-reports of freshman college students in a Thai university, we found students’ psychological wellbeing to be positively related to their learning goal orientation and negatively related to their performance-avoidance goal orientation. Additionally, the negative relationship between students’ performance-avoidance goal orientation and psychological wellbeing was stronger for men than women. Lastly, differences in students’ psychological wellbeing between men and women became more pronounced with increases in learning goal orientation for students with low levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation, but not for students with high levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation. These findings were obtained after C 2017 Wiley Periodicals, controlling for students’ grade point average and academic program. Inc. Over the last decade, interest in students’ wellbeing has grown due to increased rates of suicide, depression, anxiety, bullying, and aggressive behavior in schools (Gruttadaro & Crudo, 2012). One aspect of wellbeing that can be influenced by the climate and nature of learning in schools is psychological wellbeing (PWB), the degree to which one possesses personal meaning and self-realization and lives in accordance with one’s true self (Ryff, 1989). PWB, also referred to as eudaimonic wellbeing, represents one of two aspects of wellbeing (Ryan & Deci, 2001). The second component is subjective wellbeing (SWB) or hedonistic wellbeing which is one’s degree of happiness, manifested in greater positive affect, less negative affect, and greater life satisfaction (Ryan & Deci, 2001). Factor analytic studies (e.g., Linley, Maltby, Wood, Osborne, & Hurling, 2009) have shown PWB and SWB to be highly related but distinct constructs. PWB is associated with the successful achievement of personal goals of development (e.g., knowledge, skill, and ability acquisition and mastery) and overcoming challenges in life (Ryff, 1989). In school contexts, students’ achievement goals involving learning have been identified as important personal influences on PWB as they often reflect the kind of self-conceptions students hold as prescribed by social norms, culture, and teachers (Czopp, Lasane, Sweigard, Bradshaw, & Hammer, 1998). To assess teaching effectiveness, educational and positive psychology researchers are now encouraging schools to focus on students’ self-concept, achievement goals, PWB, and character strengths (Farrington et al., 2012). This recent trend assumes that students’ “character is built not through lectures or direct instruction from teachers but through the experience of persevering as students confront challenging academic work” (Tough, 2016, p. 66). Many schools communicate strong expectations that academic diligence, perseverance, and effort are admired The authors wish to thank Kaushik Krishnaswamy Kumar and three anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. Correspondence to: John J. Sosik, School of Graduate and Professional Studies, The Pennsylvania State University, Malvern, PA 19355. E-mail: jjs20@psu.edu 703 704 Sosik et al. as acceptable student values that define the school’s culture and/or reflect national interests and values. Students are expected to express these interests and values in their behavior so that their self-concept is consistent with these cultural expectations (Tough, 2016). Students are rewarded by teachers for strong academic effort, but must also conform to expectations of society and their peers to fit in by exhibiting average effort, as long as they are not seen as “being dumb” (Grabill et al., 2005). Relevant to notions of academic performance, achievement goals can be classified as two types: (1) learning goal orientation that values self-improvement and intrinsic motivation by striving to master new knowledge, skills and abilities, and (2) performance-avoidance orientation that values avoiding evaluations of incompetence when compared to others (Dweck, 2006). Such orientations have been theorized to enhance wellbeing only when one’s goals are in accord with the interests and values of the self-concept (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). Self-concept is a primary source of selfconcordance because it represents beliefs about oneself in private and public settings (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). As such, students endowed with a high learning goal orientation are likely to possess a self-concept in which academic progress and task involvement are highly salient and strive to attain goals of self-improvement and intrinsic interest. In contrast, students with a high-performance avoidance orientation are likely to possess a self-concept in which ego-comparative desires to avoid evaluations of incompetence and ego-driven managing of positive impressions for others are highly salient (Payne, Youngcourt, & Beaubien, 2007). The self-conceptions students form stemming from their achievement goal orientation are not only culturally determined, but are also influenced by gender. Culture provides students with society’s expectations for what to value, the identity images to portray, and how to behave (Hofstede & Hofstede, 2005). This study’s Thai cultural context provides norms of collectivism (loyalty to the group), femininity (caring for others), and reinforcing of traditions (Hofstede & Hofstede, 2005). Traditionally, men tend to portray images of autonomy and competence, whereas women tend to portray images showing concern for group harmony and personal growth of self and others (Czopp et al., 1998). However, prior research has not adequately presented and tested theoretical explanations of the relationships between achievement goals and PWB in terms of self-concordance theory (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999) and cultural and gender norms. Self-concordance may be particularly relevant in school contexts where conforming to social expectations yet attempting to be selfexpressive are common but may differ by gender. Nor has prior research examined interactive goal patterns, above and beyond main effects, where students’ achievement goal orientations reflecting self-conceptions interact with students’ gender to influence their PWB. Accordingly, this study attempted to fill these gaps in the literature by examining PWB with a focus on two different self-conceptions students possess with their achievement goal orientation (self-improvement vs. ego enhancement) and the moderating role of students’ gender. This study contributes to the literature by demonstrating how students’ goal orientations interact with gender to influence their PWB, which is often challenged by social, economic, and cultural expectations. It also examines PWB as a performance measure beyond grade point average (GPA) as called for by prior educational researchers (e.g., Farrington et al., 2012). T HEORETICAL BACKGROUND AND H YPOTHESES Sheldon and Elliot’s (1999) self-concordance model provides the general theoretical framework for this study. According to this model, when individuals pursue and achieve goals that are consistent with their enduring interests and values that align with contextual norms, they experience autonomy, competence, and relatedness associated with increased wellbeing. In line with this model, Brunstein, Schultheiss, and Grassmann (1998) pointed out that “what particular type of goal a person adopts Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits Psychological Wellbeing of Thai College Students 705 from a class of functionally equivalent goals (e.g., achievement-oriented goals) . . . depends on the person’s self-concept and self-related wishes as well as the affordances and demands inherent in his or her social environment” (p. 495), such as the current study’s Thai cultural context. This line of research suggests that students are motivated to possess a self-conception that is consistent with their desired goal (e.g., learning or performance-avoidance goal orientations). However, students select goals that may or may not reflect their true interests or values stemming from their authentic self. When students select goals that are in accord with their authentic self and cultural norms, such concordance may activate a self-referent, intrinsic motivation, and task involvement, thus enhancing PWB (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). In contrast, students may choose goals dictated by others (e.g., teachers, other students, cultural norms) that are not in accord with their authentic self. Such dissonance may activate an external referent, extrinsic motivation, and egocomparative desires, thus decreasing PWB (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). This dissonance occurs as the self-concordance standard is not met, resulting in diminished PWB (Keyes & Ryff, 2000). Building upon this theoretical framework, we examined the role learning goal orientation, performance-avoidance goal orientation, and gender play in influencing students’ PWB, while controlling for their academic program and previous semester GPA. PWB represents self-assessments of positive psychological functioning on six highly correlated elements: (1) sense of self-acceptance of one’s self and past life, (2) positive relations with others through love and friendship, (3) autonomy in self-determination, independence, and self-regulation, (4) environmental mastery in choosing or creating situations that embed the self and others, (5) sense of a strong purpose-in-life that provides clear comprehension of one’s personal meaning, sense of direction, and major life goals, and (6) personal growth in knowledge, skills, abilities, and morality necessary to become a self-actualized individual (Ryff, 1989). Several literature reviews and meta-analyses (Elliot & Murayama, 2008; Payne et al., 2007; Ryan & Deci, 2001; Ryff, 2013) suggest that learning goal orientation, performance-avoidance goal orientation, and gender are antecedents of PWB. Learning goal orientation reflects a growth mindset predisposition that assumes that ability can be developed through an inward focus on understanding, mastering of tasks, and valuing self-improvement, whereas performance-avoidance goal orientation reflects a fixed mindset predisposition that assumes that ability is set and not changeable so one must satisfy ego-driven desires aimed at not appearing stupid or inferior in comparison with others (Dweck, 2006). Learning goal orientation was chosen because it reflects self-conceptions of task involvement and self-improvement that can validate the self-enhancement standard that one is becoming better through temporal comparisons of the self. Performance-avoidance goal orientation was chosen because it reflects self-conceptions of ego involvement in tasks and ego-comparative desires that can challenge the self-consistency standard through inauthentic choices of goals dictated by others that do not reflect the values and interests of the self well (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). Theorists have also presented alternative formulations of goal orientations as trichotomous (learning, performance-avoidance, and performance-approach), 2 × 2 models (learning approach, learning avoidance, performance approach, and performance avoidance), and 2 × 3 models (task approach, task avoidance, self-approach, self-avoidance, other approach, and other avoidance) (cf. Payne et al., 2007). We choose Dweck’s (2006) dichotomous account of learning versus performanceavoidance goal orientations because they are parsimonious representations of task-involved versus ego-involved self-conceptions identified as most relevant in school contexts (Kaplan & Maehr, 1999). Prior research suggests that the effects of these two achievement goal orientations on PWB are likely to be contingent on personal attributes such as student gender (Perez, 2012), contextual characteristics such as academic program (Ryff, 2013), and knowledge of performance results as reflected in GPA (Wigtil & Henriques, 2015), which we controlled for in this study. Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits 706 Sosik et al. Main Effects of Achievement Goal Orientations on PWB Students who possess self-conceptions of self-improvement as to conform to school and cultural norms and expectations can do so by engaging in behaviors that are motivated by a learning goal orientation. In their quest to acquire new skills and improve their competence, students seek feedback to evaluate and improve their performance. They act upon this feedback to improve their skills and gain new knowledge. They view feedback on their mistakes as opportunities to learn from others, and not as threats to their ego based on inferior performance compared to others (Dweck, 2006). Such attitudes and behaviors reflect a growth mindset (Dweck, 2006) that can enhance several elements of PWB. Mastery of new skills and acquisition of knowledge in school may provide students with evidence of personal growth, and confirmation that they are successfully mastering and adapting to their school environment. The progress students make while learning may also serve as proof of their self-determination and autonomy, and provide them with an educational purpose in life. Given research demonstrating positive associations between perceived improvement and personal growth and self-acceptance (Keyes & Ryff, 2000), positive validation of self-improvement is likely to be satisfying and therefore promote student PWB. Prior empirical research supports the proposed positive relationship between learning goal orientation and PWB. Kaplan and Maehr (1999) found that students with goals focused on learning tasks reported higher levels of PWB than those with ego-involved goals focused on maintaining an image of competence. Turashvili and Japaridze (2012) found a positive relationship between student task-oriented (learning) coping strategies and PWB. Tuominen-Soini, Salmela-Aro, and Niemivirta (2008) reported positive relationships between students’ self-improvement and growth goals and SWB. Student achievement aspirations and positive attitudes toward learning have been linked with increased SWB (Gilman & Huebner, 2006). Taken together, these arguments and empirical results suggest: Hypothesis 1: Students’ learning goal orientation is positively associated with their PWB. Students who have a performance-avoidance goal orientation may pursue goals and display behaviors meant to project an image of intelligence to conform to school and cultural norms and expectations. Such an orientation describes individuals who strive to avoid looking incompetent or less able than their peers by cultivating an appearance of effortless achievement (Payne et al., 2007). Students with this orientation evade developmental feedback regarding their performance because they view such feedback and the mistakes they make as validation of their inferiority to others and as threats to their ego (Dweck, 2006). To avoid the disapproval of (in)competence and negative judgments about it, such students worry about their traits, knowledge, skills, and abilities and how inadequate they might be. Rather than working hard to address their developmental needs, they think of ways to appear competent to their teachers and fellow students. However, the feigning of inauthentic images to satisfy external demands introjected by others is likely to reduce selfconcordance and associated wellbeing (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). Such attitudes and behaviors reflect a fixed mindset (Dweck, 2006) that can diminish several elements of PWB. Social anxiety arises when individuals feel pressured to make feigned impressions of competence, satisfy ego-comparative desires, and believe that they will not be able to do so (Payne et al., 2007). When students believe that their abilities are fixed, their imperfections may become more salient aspects of their self-concept (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999), thereby detracting from their capacity to feel good about themselves and accept themselves as they are. Avoiding situations where feedback is provided and social interaction is required may add to student stress and anxiety, thereby reducing motivation to engage in positive relationships with others. The choice of goals that do not represent their true values and interests, but those imposed by external social or peer Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits Psychological Wellbeing of Thai College Students 707 pressure, is likely to diminish students’ sense of self-determination and autonomy as well as personal growth. Given research demonstrating negative associations between perceived decline/inferiority and personal growth and self-acceptance (Keyes & Ryff, 2000), such negations to self-perceptions of competence are likely to be dissatisfying and anxiety-provoking threats to the ego, and therefore diminish student PWB. Prior empirical research supports the proposed negative relationship between performance avoidance goal orientation and PWB. Kaplan and Maehr (1999) found that students with goals focused on ego protection/enhancement reported lower levels of PWB than those with goals focused on task-oriented learning of new skills. Mackinnon and Sherry (2012) found that first-year university students who avoided academic goals due to perfectionistic concerns to be associated with low levels of SWB. Tuominen-Soini, Salmela-Aro, and Niemivirta (2008) reported positive relationships between students’ tendencies to avoid feedback and social adjustment problems and stress. Turashvili and Japaridze (2012) found a positive relationship between students’ avoidance coping strategies and depression. Thus: Hypothesis 2: Students’ performance-avoidance orientation is negatively associated with their PWB. Interaction Effect of Achievement Goal Orientations and Gender on PWB The relationship between performance-avoidance goal orientation and PWB may depend on students’ gender. College men tend to be extrinsically motivated by real and apparent academic success, independence, and being perceived as “cool and hyper-masculine” (Grabill et al., 2005). Pursuing such appearances may be motivated by self-conceptions of being academically competent. Prior educational studies have shown men to possess higher levels of ego-oriented academic motivation (Anderson & Dixon, 2009) and performance-avoidance goal orientation (D’Lima, Winsler, & Kitsantas, 2014) than women. Moreover, of the six elements of PWB, autonomy may be most consistent with masculine self-conceptions of independence and success associated with performance-goal orientations (Dweck, 2006). Prior studies on PWB have shown men to possess higher levels of autonomy than women (e.g., Perez, 2012; Ryff, 2013). In contrast, college women tend to be intrinsically motivated by learning/mastery/task goals and more concerned with the development of self and others than men (Anderson & Dixon, 2009; D’Lima et al., 2014; Grabill et al., 2005). These goals are consistent with feminine values of improving quality of life and enjoying one’s role in it that characterize the culture of Thailand (Hofstede & Hofstede, 2005). Performance-avoidance goal orientation involves extrinsically motivated feigning images of competence (a motive at odds with Thai cultural values) and its associated self-discord, whereas learning goal orientation involves intrinsically motivated pursuits of self-improvement while not standing out from the crowd (a motive consistent with Thai cultural values) and its associated self-concordance and wellbeing (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). As such, performance-avoidance goal orientation is expected to diminish PWB to a greater extent for men than women. The level of students’ learning goal orientation may influence the extent to which PWB differences between students possessing high and low levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation will be larger for men than women. Learning and performance goal orientations are orthogonal; students can possess high (low) levels of both orientations (Elliot & Murayama, 2008). As such, learning goal orientation may provide greater salience of self-conceptions of self-improvement and task involvement for students who possess lower levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation. By emphasizing self-conceptions of personal growth and purpose in performing educational tasks, increased learning goal orientation may turn student attention away from fixed mindset attitudes of helplessness in their academic abilities (Dweck, 2006). Such self-conceptions of improvement may Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits 708 Sosik et al. validate the standard of self-enhancement and make less salient any self-conceptions of perceived decline/inferiority associated with performance-avoidance goal orientation that fail to meet both the standards of self-enhancement and self-consistency. Students with a high level of learning goal orientation are less likely to experience ego-involvement concerning their view of success and the anxiety that comes with it (Payne et al., 2007), which are potential hindrances to self-conceptions of personal growth, purpose in life, autonomy, and environmental mastery that reflect PWB. In contrast, students, especially men, possessing lower levels of learning goal orientation may be less task involved, and more ego involved in choosing to define success and differentiating the self from others through social comparisons and competition (Anderson & Dixon, 2009), despite this view being in discord with Thai cultural values of harmony and concern for others (Lerdpornkulrat, Koul, & Sujivorakul, 2012). Moreover, self-conceptions of perceived decline/inferiority associated with performance-avoidance goal orientation may exert a “double dose” negative effect on PWB due to failure to meet the self-enhancement and self-consistency standards that the self uses to examine its condition (Keyes & Ryff, 2000). These self-conceptions may come from unfavorable social comparisons with other students and exacerbate perceived differences in academic performance ability with them. Self-differentiation individuates students from their peers (Schmidt, 2005). Students’ self-conceptions of individuation may promote competition rather than cooperation, and anxiety from being seen as different from one’s peers in the collectivistic Thai culture that values conformity to group norms and interpersonal harmony. Such self-conceptions may enhance the negative relationship between students’ performance-avoidance goal orientation and PWB, especially for male students who value independence and autonomy more than female students. Thus, we explored for a three-way interaction among learning and performance-avoidance goal orientations, and students’ gender: Research Question 1: (a) Will PWB differences between students possessing low and high levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation be smaller for women than men? (b) Will the magnitude of these differences be greater for students possessing a higher level of learning goal orientation? M ETHOD Sample and Procedure Participants in our all-volunteer sample were enrolled as freshman-year undergraduate degree students in six different fields of study at a university located north of Bangkok, Thailand. Five hundred and seventy-five students were invited to participate, and more than 98% of the responses to the survey were complete and included in our analysis. The final sample consisted of 564 students, 40.6% males and 59.4% females. Participants’ age ranged from 17 to 19. Engineering students (35.1%) were studying civil, electrical, or mechanical engineering. Fine arts students (17.9%) were studying visual and performing arts and computer graphics. Education students (17.6%) were pursuing education degrees with concentrations in language, math, computer, production engineering, or electrical engineering. Economics students (15.4%) were studying functioning of markets, firms, and financial organizations for employment in business, nonprofit, finance, and government organizations. Nursing students (14.0%) were pursuing a degree with professional qualifications for employment within the health-care sector. Participants completed a paper-and-pencil survey that assessed gender, PWB, learning goal orientation, performance-avoidance goal orientation, and academic program. Participants were informed that responses to the survey would be confidential and anonymous, and that their survey Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits Psychological Wellbeing of Thai College Students 709 completion represented assumed consent. All surveys were distributed and returned in blank envelopes. It was at the discretion of the prospective participant to complete the survey or place a blank or partially completed survey into the provided envelope. All survey items were written in Thai. Following Brislin’s (1980) translation-back-translation procedure, two bilinguals in English and Thai conducted two-way translations of the survey items. Participants were debriefed following data analysis. Measures We used variables determined at different points in time to tap into the process inherent in the hypotheses and reduce the concern for common source bias (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, & Podsakoff, 2012). Specifically, students’ biological gender was fixed at birth, GPA determined at the end of the first semester of the students’ freshman year from school records, and the remaining variables assessed 5 months later at the end of the second semester of the students’ freshman year. Unless otherwise indicated, every survey item was measured on a five-point response scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Gender. Students’ biological gender (sex) was coded as 0 for men and 1 for women. Goal Orientation. The Thai language goal orientation measures used in this study were based on a Finnish to English translation of an instrument that was developed and validated by Niemivirta (1998). The scales used in this study were tested and validated in prior studies conducted in Thailand (e.g., Koul, Clariana, Jitgarun, & Songsriwittaya, 2009; Lerdpornkulrat et al., 2012). Learning goal orientation was measured with five items (α = .83; sample item: “I study in order to learn and to understand new ideas”), and performance-avoidance goal orientation was measured with four items (α = .85; sample item: “In class, the fear of looking ignorant always motivates me”). Psychological Wellbeing. We used a reduced 41-item adaptation of Ryff’s (1989) 42-item version of the Scales of PWB translated into Thai to assess students’ PWB. Reviews of this scale highlight its construct validity and wide use in both Western and Eastern contexts (cf. Ryan & Deci, 2001; Ryff, 2013) including Thailand (e.g., Hengudomsub, Koedbangkham, & Kangchai, 2007; Lohapan & Ussahawanitchakit, 2016). We pretested the Thai version of the PWB scale with a group of nearly 100 students for a content adequacy assessment that provided support for construct validity as it allows the deletion of items that may be conceptually inconsistent. The item reading “In general, I feel I am in charge of the situation in which I live” was dropped because it caused interpretation issues for students during pretest screenings of the survey items for understanding accuracy. The remaining 41 items were collapsed into six parcels representing their theoretical dimension of PWB (Ryff, 1989; Ryff & Keyes, 1995): self-acceptance (seven items; sample item: “When I look at the story of my life, I am pleased with how things have turned out”), positive relations with others (seven items; sample item: “People would describe me as a giving person, willing to share my time with others”), autonomy (seven items; sample item: “I judge myself by what I think is important, not by the values of what others think is important”), environmental mastery (six items; sample item: “I am quite good at managing the many responsibilities of my daily life”), purpose in life (seven items; sample item: “I have a sense of direction and purpose in life”), and personal growth (seven items; sample item: “I have the sense that I have developed a lot as a person over time”). The combining of these subscales into one composite scale was consistent with results of Linley et al.’s (2009) examination of the factor structure of PWB measures. Cronbach’s alpha for this scale was .90. Control Variables. Because context can influence PWB (Ryff, 2013), academic program type was included as a control variable to partial out its effect on PWB. Given that knowledge of results of Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits 710 Sosik et al. Table 1 Results of Measurement Model Comparisons Model Three-factor model (PWB / LGO / PAO) Two-factor model I (PWB and LGO / PAO) Two-factor model II (PWB and PAO / LGO) Two-factor model III (PWB / LGO and PAO) One-factor model χ 2 (df) CFI TLI RMSEA ࢞χ 2 (࢞df) 380.09 (87) 1,061.73 (89) 943.67 (89) 1,499.52 (89) 1,656.57 (90) .93 .76 .79 .65 .61 .92 .72 .75 .59 .53 .07 .14 .13 .17 .18 − ** 681.64 (2) ** 563.58 (2) ** 1,194.43 (2) ** 1,276.48 (3) Note. Chi-square difference for each model reflects its deviation from the three-factor model. PWB = students’ psychological wellbeing; LGO = learning goal orientation; PAO = performance-avoidance goal orientation; CFI = comparative fit index; TLI = Tucker-Lewis index; RMSEA = root mean squared error of approximation. ** p < .01. academic performance may be associated with students’ PWB (e.g., Turashvili & Japaridze, 2012), GPA at the end of the first semester of their freshman year was obtained from school records and used as an additional control variable. We did not control for academic year because this variable was constant (freshman year) nor student age because the host university did not provide age data for each student. R ESULTS Preliminary Analyses Confirmatory Factor Analyses. A series of confirmatory factor analyses (CFAs) were conducted to address the issues of common method variance and discriminant validity of the study measures. We first examined the fit indices of the three-factor model (PWB, learning goal orientation, and performance-avoidance goal orientation) where the items or parcels of items were set to load on their respective factors. Model fit of this three-factor model was adequate (χ 2 = 380.09, df = 87, CFI = .93, TLI = .92, RMSEA = .07), and all factor loadings were significant, ranging from .57 to .83 for PWB, .67 to .75 for learning goal orientation, and .69 to .83 for performance-avoidance goal orientation (Hu & Bentler, 1999). As presented in Table 1, a series of chi-square difference tests revealed that the three-factor model fit the data significantly better than other alternative models. Taken together, these results provide evidence of the construct validity of the study measures and minimize concern for common method bias. Descriptive Statistics. Means, standard deviations, reliabilities, and correlations of the study variables are presented in Table 2. A review of the correlations indicates that students’ PWB was positively related to their learning goal orientation (r = .45, p < .01) and negatively related to their performance-avoidance goal orientation (r = −.52, p < .01), providing initial support for Hypotheses 1 and 2. Results also indicated that students’ GPA was positively related to their learning goal orientation (r = .13, p < .01) and PWB (r = .24, p < .01), and negatively related to their performance-avoidance goal orientation (r = −.15, p < .01). Women reported significantly higher levels of PWB (r = .25, p < .01) and significantly lower levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation (r = −.25, p < .01) than men. Hypothesis Tests We tested our hypotheses with hierarchical regression analysis using one-tailed tests. Before conducting the analysis, we mean centered all continuous variables used as a component of the Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits Psychological Wellbeing of Thai College Students 711 Table 2 Means, Standard Deviations, Intercorrelations, and Alphas of Variables Variables M SD 1 2 1. GPA 3.00 .58 − ** 2. Engineering dummy .35 .48 −.45 ** 3. Fine arts dummy .18 .38 .21 ** 4. Education dummy .18 .38 .53 * 5. Economics dummy .15 .36 .09 ** 6. Gender .59 .49 .19 ** 7. Learning goal orientation 4.04 .62 .13 ** 8. Performance-avoidance goal orientation 2.88 .95 −.15 ** 9. Psychological wellbeing 140.80 16.73 .24 − ** −.34 ** −.34 ** −.32 ** .19 * −.08 .07 ** −.24 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 − ** −.22 − ** ** −.20 −.20 − * .01 .10 .04 − ** ** −.06 .24 −.11 .08 (.83) ** ** ** .15 −.17 .01 −.25 −.05 (.85) * ** ** ** ** −.09 .27 −.03 .25 .45 −.52 (.90) Note. N = 564 students. Values in parentheses along the diagonal are Cronbach’s alphas. Academic programs coded as a series of dummy variables: engineering, fine arts, education, and economics, with nursing as the comparison group. Gender was coded 0 = men, 1 = women. GPA = grade point average from prior semester. * p < .05; ** p < .01. Table 3 Results of Hierarchical Regression Analysis Outcome: PWB Predictors Constant GPA Engineering dummy Fine arts dummy Education dummy Economics dummy LGO PAO Gender LGO × PAO LGO × gender PAO × gender LGO × PAO × gender ࢞R2 Model 1 Model 2 ** 149.76 (1.85) ** 6.11 (1.55) ** −12.15 (2.07) ** −13.65 (2.55) −3.26 (2.86) ** −10.84 (2.54) .15 ** Model 3 ** 146.30 (1.85) * 2.91 (1.23) ** −8.19 (1.73) ** −6.11 (2.08) −3.51 (2.28) ** −5.21 (2.03) ** 11.61 (1.22) ** −9.83 (.85) .51 (1.16) .39 (.90) −1.81 (1.67) ** 3.11 (1.12) .35 ** ** 146.10 (1.85) * 2.86 (1.23) ** −8.03 (1.73) ** −5.86 (2.08) −3.24 (2.28) ** −5.25 (2.02) ** 11.66 (1.21) ** −9.45 (.87) .64 (1.16) −1.14 (1.18) −1.43 (1.67) 2.20 (1.20) * 3.59 (1.80) * .01 Note. N = 564 students. Unstandardized estimates with standard errors in parentheses are reported. PWB = students’ psychological wellbeing; GPA = grade point average from prior semester; LGO = learning goal orientation; PAO = performance avoidance goal orientation. Academic programs coded as a series of dummy variables: engineering, fine arts, education, and economics, with nursing as the comparison group. Gender was coded 0 = men, 1 = women. * p < .05; ** p < .01. interaction terms to make the results more interpretable (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003). Table 3 presents the regression results of the direct and interaction effects of students’ learning goal orientation, performance-avoidance goal orientation, and gender on their PWB. Regression model 1 in Table 3 indicates the effects of the control variables, students’ gender and academic program represented as dummy variables, on students’ PWB, adding unique variance (R2 = .15, ࢞R2 = .15, p Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits 712 Sosik et al. 165 160 Psychological Wellbeing 155 150 145 Men 140 Women 135 130 125 120 Low PAO High PAO FIGURE 1. Moderating effect of gender on the relationship between performance avoidance goal orientation and psychological wellbeing. PAO = performance-avoidance goal orientation. < .01). Regression model 2 in Table 3 indicates the direct and two-way interaction effects of students’ learning goal orientation, performance-avoidance orientation, and gender on students’ PWB after controlling for students’ gender and academic program represented as dummy variables, adding unique variance (R2 = .50, ࢞R2 = .35, p < .01). Regression model 3 in Table 3 indicates the direct, two-way, and three-way interaction effects of students’ learning goal orientation, performanceavoidance orientation, and gender on students’ PWB after controlling for students’ gender and academic program represented as dummy variables, adding unique variance (R2 = .51, ࢞R2 = .01, p < .05). As regression model 3 in Table 3 shows, students’ learning goal orientation was positively related to their PWB (b = 11.66, p < .01) after controlling for students’ GPA and academic program and all two-way and three-way interactions, thus supporting Hypothesis 1. Regression model 3 in Table 3 also shows that students’ performance-avoidance goal orientation was negatively related to their PWB (b = −9.45, p < .01) after controlling for students’ GPA and academic program and all two-way and three-way interactions, thus supporting Hypothesis 2. Regarding Research Question 1a, we examined whether PWB differences between students possessing low and high levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation would be greater for men than women. Regression model 2 in Table 3 shows that the interaction of students’ performanceavoidance orientation and gender on their PWB was positive and significant (b = 3.11, p < .01). To probe the interaction patterns, we plotted two simple slopes at one standard deviation above and below the mean values of performance-avoidance goal orientation for men and women. As shown in Figure 1, the difference in PWB for students possessing low and high levels of performanceavoidance goal orientation was more pronounced for men than for women as evidenced by the steeper slope. Regarding Research Question 1b, we examined whether the magnitude of these differences would be greater for students possessing a higher level of learning goal orientation. Specifically, we examined whether the positive relationship between students’ learning goal orientations and their PWB would be more pronounced for men than women with lower levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation but not for students with higher levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation. Regression model 3 in Table 3 shows that the interaction of students’ learning goal orientation, performance-avoidance orientation, and gender on their PWB was positive and significant (b = 3.59, p < .05). Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits Psychological Wellbeing of Thai College Students 713 165 160 Psychological Wellbeing 155 150 (1) High PAO, Women 145 (2) High PAO, Men 140 (3) Low PAO, Women 135 (4) Low PAO, Men 130 125 120 Low LGO High LGO FIGURE 2. Moderating effect of performance avoidance goal orientation and gender on the relationship between learning goal orientation and psychological wellbeing. LGO = learning goal orientation; PAO = performance-avoidance goal orientation. To probe the interaction patterns, we plotted four simple slopes at one standard deviation above and below the mean values of learning and performance-avoidance goal orientations for men and women. As shown in Figure 2, the linear relationship between students’ learning goal orientation and PWB was plotted for women with a high level of performance-avoidance goal orientation (Slope 1), men with a high level of performance-avoidance goal orientation (Slope 2), women with a low level of performance-avoidance goal orientation (Slope 3), and men with a low level of performanceavoidance goal orientation (Slope 4). Figure 2 shows the positive relationship between students’ learning goal orientation and PWB was more pronounced for men with lower levels of performanceavoidance goal orientation (Slope 4) than for women with lower levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation (Slope 3); slope difference test t = −2.15, p < .05 (Dawson, 2014). No other pairs of slopes were significantly different from each other. D ISCUSSION Establishing contexts that positively shape students’ learning mindsets to promote academic performance, development, and PWB have been goals of researchers and educators for the past 25 years (Dweck, 2006; Ryff, 2013; Tough, 2016). By pursuing learning and/or performance-oriented achievement goals, students choose the way they define success in the classroom to establish and maintain their social status among peers and to validate their self-conceptions (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). Despite such research and application objectives, little attention has been given to how PWB may be influenced by the direct and interaction effects of the achievement goals that college men and women pursue to create a desired self-conception in their interactions with teachers and fellow students (Payne et al., 2007). To begin to address this issue systematically, we replicated prior research demonstrating a positive relationship between students’ learning goal orientation and PWB, and a negative relationship between their performance-avoidance goal orientation and PWB. Our findings confirm the superiority of task-involved goals over ego-involved goals, and extend previous research conducted in the United States and Finland (e.g., Kaplan & Maehr, 1999; Tuominen-Soini et al., 2008) to Asian contexts (i.e., Thailand). The Thai culture places great importance on the feminine value of liking Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits 714 Sosik et al. what one is doing (e.g., learning from tasks) as opposed to the masculine value of wanting to be perceived as being the best (e.g., satisfying the ego) compared to the U.S. culture that is far more masculine, favoring competitiveness and assertiveness (Hofstede & Hofstede, 2005). Consistent with this cultural profile, results shown in Table 2 indicate that men were associated with higher levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation satisfying ego-comparative desires to avoid perceptions of incompetence than women. However, this masculine goal avoidance tendency comes at the expense of men’s self-ratings of their PWB. Taken together, these results extend the wellbeing literature that has primarily assessed affect-based forms of SWB as an outcome of achievement goal orientation to PWB in the Thai culture. Beyond replicating prior research and extending it to the Thai culture, a major aim of this study was to extend the achievement goal orientation and wellbeing literatures by demonstrating the moderating role of student gender on the interaction between students’ learning and performanceavoidance goal orientations. The data indicated that the negative relationship between students’ performance-avoidance goal orientation and PWB was stronger for men than women. Gender differences have been associated with the kind of learning orientations that students possess (e.g., Czopp et al., 1998; Dweck, 2006). Past research, however, has been inconclusive regarding how pursuing different goal orientations might relate to variation in PWB depending upon gender. This study supports the notion that pursuit of performance-avoidance goals in the classroom, while striving to maintain a masculine image of being competent and “cool” (Czopp et al., 1998), may be detrimental to men’s PWB. The effort required to project such an inauthentic self-image can be psychologically draining and diminish wellbeing (Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). Furthermore, such a self-image is not consistent with the feminine Thai cultural values of being less competitive and aggressive (Hofstede & Hofstede, 2005) and makes salient a self-image that fails to meet the self-regulatory standards of self-enhancement and self-consistency (Keyes & Ryff, 2000). Because feigning images of competence and competiveness can evoke social anxiety (Payne et al., 2007) and be inconsistent with the PWB elements of positive relationships with others and self-acceptance (Ryff, 2013), the cost of engaging in self-enhancement looms large for men who pursue performance-avoidance goals. This study also elucidated how learning and performance-avoidance orientations interact with students’ gender to influence PWB. In exploring these interactive goal patterns with gender, above and beyond their main effects, we found that gender differences in students’ PWB became more pronounced with increases in learning goal orientation for students with lower levels of performanceavoidance goal orientation, but not for students with higher levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation. An inspection of Figure 2 indicates the relationship between students’ learning goal orientation and PWB increased at a higher rate for men compared to women possessing a low level of performance-avoidance goal orientation (Slope 4 vs. Slope 3). This interaction effect did not emerge for men and women possessing a high level of performance-avoidance goal orientation (Slope 1 vs. Slope 2). The three-way interaction depicted in Figure 2 and indicated in Table 3 suggests that high levels of performance-avoidance goal orientation can be quite detrimental to PWB as it seems to limit the beneficial effects of learning goal orientation on PWB, especially for men who employ such avoidance coping strategies. Such findings are not surprising, as they converge with prior research (Peterson & Seligman, 2004; Ryan & Deci, 2001), which suggests that the pursuit of goals not integrated into the self is not only stressful and detrimental to positive affect, life satisfaction, and happiness, but also fails to meet standards of self-enhancement and self-consistency (Keyes & Ryff, 2000; Sheldon & Elliot, 1999). Such pursuit of goals lacking self-concordance also detracts from learning strategies that support of personal growth and environmental mastery, two elements of PWB that may be essential in educational contexts (Dweck, 2006). One practical implication of these results is that pursuing ego-involved image enhancement goals through a performance-avoidance goal orientation may constitute a significant detriment Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits Psychological Wellbeing of Thai College Students 715 to students’ cognitive assessments of their optimal psychological functioning and meeting the challenges of everyday life, especially for men (Grabill et al., 2005). This implication is not only relevant in more feminine cultures such as Thailand, but also in more masculine cultures such as the United States and Germany where societies are driven by competition, achievement, and success (Hofstede & Hofstede, 2005). We encourage schools to develop interventions that advocate growth mindsets focused on learning goal orientation, and warn against the dangers of fixed mindsets focused on performance-avoidance goal orientation and its negative relationship with PWB. Second, results demonstrating the interactive goal patterns with students’ gender suggest the need for university administrators to consider policies that appreciate the diversity of learning styles and preferences for task versus ego involvement in demonstrating competence set within cultural norms and expectations (Grabill et al., 2005). Wellbeing profiles of those students potentially at risk of succumbing to perceived incompetence could be evaluated, so that personalized action plans can then be designed to help move them toward self-improvement and mastery goals, and enhance their sense of wellbeing. Several study limitations should be noted. First, data were collected from university students in Thailand. Although collecting data on PWB in a collectivistic and feminine culture like Thailand is novel, it may have limited considerations of PWB to the more socially acceptable elements of PWB such as maintaining positive relations with others and achieving personal growth by meeting obligations within the community (Lerdpornkulrat et al., 2012). Future cross-cultural research should examine PWB and the achievement goal orientations used in the current study in both individualistic and collectivistic societies, and in more masculine societies. Moreover, freshman college students may not have the requisite life experiences to best assess all elements of PWB, so longitudinal data collection from older adult professionals and across a range of industrial domains is needed. Second, data were collected at one point in time, which precludes causal conclusions from being drawn and raises the potential for common method bias (Podsakoff et al., 2012). Although results of our CFA allayed such concerns and achievement goal orientations are trait-like constructs typically determined at earlier points in the lifespan (Elliot & Murayama, 2008), the potential exists for inflated relationships between these independent variables and PWB. Future research might use multisource ratings of student achievement goal orientation collected from teachers and students to avoid this potential issue. A third limitation concerns our contrast between learning and performance-avoidance goal orientations. We choose performance-avoidance goal orientation to operationalize the ego-involved self-conception because it best reflects motives to look better than one is concerning one’s abilities (Payne et al., 2007). Future research might consider performance-approach goal motivation, aimed at projecting an image of proving one’s competence to gain favorable judgments about it, to provide a more positive learning mindset for students. A related fourth limitation concerns our measurement of self-perceptions of achievement goal orientations rather than actual self-presentations of behaviors as perceived by observers such as teachers and other students. Collection of ratings from independent observer sources can be compared with self-ratings to assess level of agreement in future research. In conclusion, this study replicated results showing direct or main effects of two typical types of achievement goal orientations on students’ PWB reflecting self-conceptions about how their view of academic success either enhances or attenuates PWB. It also extended the achievement goal orientation and wellbeing literatures by demonstrating the moderating role of students’ gender on the interaction between their learning and performance-avoidance goal orientations, in response to calls for the examination of interactive goal patterns and demographics characteristics (e.g., Payne et al., 2007). These contributions are important practically as well because educators are responsible for the development of students’ knowledge, skills, and abilities and wellbeing, thereby making it Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits 716 Sosik et al. necessary to create curricula that consider students’ mindsets toward learning and learning goals associated with their gender. R EFERENCES Anderson, D. M., & Dixon, A. W. (2009). Winning isn’t everything: Goal orientation and gender differences in university leisure-skills classes. Recreational Sports Journal, 33, 54–64. Brislin, R. W. (1980). Translation and content analysis of oral and written material. In H. C. Triandis & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 389–444). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grassmann, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 494–508. Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erblaum. Czopp, A. M., Lasane, T. P., Sweigard, P. N., Bradshaw, S. D., & Hammer, E. D. (1998). Masculine styles of self-presentation in the classroom: Perceptions of Joe Cool. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 13, 281–294. Dawson, J. F. (2014). Moderation in management research: What, why, when, and how. Journal of Business and Psychology, 29, 1–19. D’Lima, G. M., Winsler, A., & Kitsantas, A. (2014). Ethnic and gender differences in first-year college students’ goal orientation, self-efficacy, and extrinsic and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Educational Research, 107(5), 341–356. Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York: Random House. Elliot, A. J., & Murayama, K. (2008). On the measurement of achievement goals: Critique, illustration, and application. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 613–628. Farrington, C. A., Roderick, M., Allensworth, E., Nagaoka, J., Keyes, T. S., Johnson, D. W., & Beechum, N. O. (2012). Teaching adolescents to become learners: The role of noncognitive factors in shaping school performance: A critical literature review. Chicago: University of Chicago Consortium on Chicago School Research. Gilman, R., & Huebner, E. S. (2006). Characteristics of adolescents who report very high life satisfaction. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35(3), 311–319. Grabill, K., Lasane, T. P., Povitsky, W. T., Saxe, P., Mungro, G. B., Phelps, L. M., & Straub, J. (2005). Gender and study behavior: How social perception, social norm adherence, and structured academic behavior are predicted by gender. North American Journal of Psychology, 7, 7–24. Gruttadaro, D., & Crudo, D. (2012). College students speak: A survey report on mental health. Arlington, VA: National Alliance on Mental Health. Hengudomsub, P., Koedbangkham, J., & Kangchai, W. (2007). Physical health and psychological well-being in Thai older adults: Social comparison as a mediator. Journal of Science, Technology, and Humanities, 5(1–2), 43–55. Hofstede, G., & Hofstede, G. J. (2005). Cultures and organizations: Software of the mind (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. Kaplan, A., & Maehr, M. L. (1999). Achievement goals and student wellbeing. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24, 330–358. Keyes, C. L. M., & Ryff, C. D. (2000). Subjective change and mental health: A self-concept theory. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63(3), 264–279. Koul, R., Clariana, R. B., Jitgarun, K., & Songsriwittaya, A. (2009). The influence of achievement goal orientation on plagiarism. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 506–512. Lerdpornkulrat, T., Koul, R., & Sujivorakul, C. (2012). Influence of ability beliefs and motivational orientation on the self-efficacy of high school science students in Thailand. Australian Journal of Education, 56(2), 163–181. Linley, P. A., Maltby, J., Wood, A. M., Osborne, G., & Hurling, R. (2009). Measuring happiness: The higher order factor structure of subjective and psychological well-being measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(8), 878–884. Lohapan, N., & Ussahawanitchakit, P. (2016). Psychological well-being and job success: An empirical research of tax auditors in Thailand. Business and Management Review, 7(5), 95–104. Mackinnon, S. P., & Sherry, S. B. (2012). Perfectionistic self-presentation mediates the relationship between perfectionistic concerns and subjective wellbeing: A three-wave longitudinal study. Personality and Individual Differences, 53, 22–28. Niemivirta, M. (1998). Individual differences in motivational and cognitive factors affecting self-regulated learning—A pattern-oriented approach. In P. Nenninger, R. S. Jäger, A. Frey, & M. Woznitza (Eds.), Advances in motivation (pp. 23–42). Landau, DE: Verlad Empirische Pädagogik. Payne, S. C., Youngcourt, S. S., & Beaubien, J. M. (2007). A meta-analytic examination of the goal orientation nomological net. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(1), 128–150. Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits Psychological Wellbeing of Thai College Students 717 Perez, J. A. (2012). Gender difference in psychological well-being among Filipino college student samples. International Journal of Humanities and Social Science, 2(13), 84–93. Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and classification. New York: Oxford/American Psychological Association. Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2012). Sources of method bias in social science research and recommendations on how to control it. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 539–569. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166. Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of Psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6), 1069–1081. Ryff, C. D. (2013). Psychological well-being revisited: Advances in the science and practice of eudaimonia. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 83, 10–28. Ryff, C. D., & Keyes, C. L. M. (1995). The structure of psychological wellbeing revisited. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(4), 719–727. Schmidt, M. A. (2005). Individuation: Finding oneself in analysis-taking risks and making sacrifices. Journal of Analytical Psychology, 50(5), 595–616. Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction, and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(3), 482–497. Tough, P. (2016). How kids really succeed. Atlantic, 317(5), 56–66. Tuominen-Soini, H., Salmela-Aro, K., & Niemivirta, M. (2008). Achievement goal orientations and subjective wellbeing: A person-centered analysis. Learning and Instruction, 18, 251–266. Turashvili, T., & Japaridze, M. (2012). Psychological wellbeing and its relation to academic performance of students in a Georgian context. Problems of Education in the 21st Century, 49, 73–80. Wigtil, C. J., & Henriques, G. R. (2015). The relationship between intelligence and psychological well-being in incoming college students. Psychological Wellbeing, 5(4), 1–19. Psychology in the Schools DOI: 10.1002/pits