The experience of pain is a subjective sensation, controlled by multiple factors (Trout, 2004). Pain affects the majority of the population, daily. Therefore, research into pain is important in order to understand the pain process and how to treat pain effectively. Pain is a complex, social problem that has evolved and adapted significantly over time. Prior to 1965, a simple neurophysiologic model of pain predominated. All physical pain was thought to emanate from activation of specific pain receptors in the periphery, initiating pain impulses through a spinal pathway to the brain (Rosenzweig, Leiman, Breedlove, 1999). This model was a simple sensory stimulus-response model. Psychological contributions to pain were not recognised. Significant theories such as The Gate Control Theory (GCT) (1965) and Neuromatrix Pain Theory (NPT) (1999) are used to understand and explain pain. This essay will discuss the biological process of pain and how psychology and biology can be incorporated to describe how pain. Pain is a psychological response produced by the body to indicate injury or illness. Research is able to explain the process of pain in terms of biology. From a biological perspective, it is known that beneath the skin in the periphery, where muscles and tissue are located, are nociceptors (pain sensory cells). When noxious stimulus occurs (injury), chemicals are released to stimulate nociceptors. Depending on the type of injury of pain, a different type of nociceptor will be released – the alpha delta which are small cells that produce fast well localised pain and the C fibre which produce slow poorly localised pain such as burning or throbbing. These nociceptors propagate the information about the pain to the spinal cord through the dorsal sensory fibre (located at the back of the spinal cord) to pass information into the spinal cord and to the brain. Chemicals reach the brain via the spinothalamic pathway towards the thalamus; this is known as the relay station. Following this, the second order neuron releases chemicals that pass pain information to the third order neuron which locates the pain to the part of the somatosensory cortex that correlates with the position of the injury. This process is known as the ascending pathway (Kalat, 2009). The gate control theory recognized the perception of pain is inherently more complex than simple receiving and recording. The gate control theory forced medical and biological sciences to accept the brain as an active system that filters, selects and modulates inputs (Melzack, 1999). This was a major advance in recognising the contribution of the brain not only to the ultimate perception of pain, but also to the nature of pain itself (trout, 2004). Implicit in GCT was the idea that a gate was either opened or closed at the level of the spinal cord (i.e., impulse transmission was either facilitated or inhibited at that level). If impulse transmission is sufficiently inhibited at the level of the spinal cord, then perception of pain (which occurs in the brain) is blocked. The number of fibres entering the spinal cord can affect the intensity of pain that we feel. An example of this is rubbing an area that has been bumped, triggering more nociceptor and essentially blocking the gate allowing only some nociceptors to get through, potentially dampening the intensity of the bump or fall. This theory fails to account for the perception of pain in the absence of sensory stimuli (such as occurs with phantom limb pain), and it fails to account for the perception of the body as a unity (Melzack, 2001). However, other processes can allow the gate to open and close such as emotion and attention (Melzack and Wall, 1965). These processes may have an influence because they travel through the same location of the gate system (Hagbarth and Kerr, 1954). Cognitive, sensory and emotional factors that could open the gate and increase pain intensity include physical inactivity, worrying about pain, having no distractions, depression, anxiety, anger and stress. Factors that can close the gate include meditation, distractions, high physical fitness, stress management and positive attitude (Deardorff, 2003). However, GCT still leaves unanswered questions regarding chronic pain, sex difference and the effects of previous pain experience. NPT retains some key aspects of the gate theory of pain, but it offers a more comprehensive framework for under-standing the subjectivity of pain. It recognizes the importance of both ascending and descending inputs to the conscious experience of pain and includes additional inputs that were not part of the gate theory, such as the important contributions of memory and past experiences (Melzack, 2001). NPT suggests we are all born with a genetically determined neural network in our brain known as the body-self neuromatrix (BSN). The BSN creates the perceptions we have of our bodies and our sense of self and can generate chronic pain through processing inputs. Cognitive, sensory and affective inputs such as memories, past experiences, emotions and sensory inputs travel into the neuromatrix and cause an output such as a stress response, a voluntary or involuntary physical movement and a cognitive evaluation. Over time and through life experiences, the body-self neuromatrix can change and therefore the way in which an individual processes different types of pain can alter. Prior experience with intense pain can be a positive influence if managed in a way that elicited a feeling of accomplishment, or this past experience can cause increased anxiety when anticipated a second time. Eg., women who experience little pain in a prior pregnancy may be more confident because the previous experience with pain was manageable (Hodnett,2002). The BSN is also influenced by cultural values and therefore explains as to why individuals from different cultural backgrounds view pain differently. NPT explains that even without nociceptive stimuli, pain can occur. The main advantage of this model is its ability to explain how sensory, behavioural, visual and nociceptive components can come together and influence one another. However, the model fails to explain sex difference in pain in that females report more pain than men (Unruh, 1996). To conclude, researchers have found increasing evidence that psychology plays a part in the pain process. GCT changed the idea of pain and found evidence psychological thoughts, emotions and ideas can impact the biological process of pain by influencing the ‘gate’ mechanism that either opens or closes depending on the nature of these factors. Furthermore, NPT analysed processes in the brain that can create pain without the input of a painful stimuli and showed that memories, previous experiences and emotions can cause the feeling of pain. Highlighting, clear evidence that shows both biology and psychology play a major part in the pain process.