LEARNERS’ KNOWLEDGE: COMMON FRAMEWORKS AND CONCEPTUAL SITES Philip Anding Faculty of Cognitive Sciences & Human Development Universiti Malaysia Sarawak 94300 Kota Samarahan, Sarawak, Malaysia Abstract This study investigates learners’ knowledge of d.c. circuits and considers students’ knowledge in the conceptual-procedural and qualitativequantitative continua. Knowledge in short-term memory is accessible indirectly to a researcher because a researcher interprets meaning from data that represent knowledge in the brain. Verbal data are coded by interpreting the meaning of ‘action’ concepts used by learners when they performed cognitive tasks. A specific meaning of an ‘action’ concept represents a piece or a group of pieces of knowledge in short-term memory and is referred to as a facet of knowledge. Facets of knowledge that are about the same concept formed a category. Categories are categorized further into major categories whereby a major category is about a superordinate concept. A major category reflects different dimensions of learners’ knowledge of a super-ordinate concept. Common frameworks are constructed by linking categories along similar dimensions and a core concept is discovered from the common frameworks. This study suggests a core concept as a ‘conceptual site’ for constructing teaching-learning interventions of d.c. circuits. 1. Introduction Piaget and Vygotsky saw learning as an active process whereby learners construct their own knowledge. According to constructivist’s views, the construction of knowledge in the brain occurs in piecemeal involving a series of steps of limited size and it is a process which takes time (Taber 2006). ‘Building blocks’ of knowledge are constructed to form a knowledge construct. A learner’s knowledge is unique because a learner constructs her own knowledge. Learners not only construct their own knowledge, but they come to science classrooms with already existing knowledge of many physical phenomena which has consequences for learning (Driver & Bell 1986; Driver & Easley 1978; Driver & Erickson 1983; Gilbert, Osborne & Fensham 1982; Gilbert & Watts 1983; Osborne & Wittrock 1983; Pope & Gilbert 1983; Taber 2006). 2. Background Physics is a difficult subject for most students or learners. Learners often developed alternative conceptions from everyday physical and linguistic experience (Driver & Erickson 1983). Physics is difficult because it involves qualitative-quantitative representations, also referred to as physical and mathematical knowledge representations. Learners tend to memorize equations, apply purely quantitative treatment of physical phenomena and often lack deeper understanding of the equations that are used to produce the numerical answers (Ploetzner et al. 1990). 2.1 Re-presentations of Knowledge Knowledge in long-term memory is re-presented as representation concepts and propositions in short-term memory. The processing of of knowledge in short-term memory may be considered as the ‘tracing’ of memory (Seung 1982) and is limited to 7±2 chunks (Miller 1956). A ‘memory trace’ could be in the form of a learner’s verbalisations, written statements, calculations, sketches and so forth. Ericsson and Simon (1982) are of the view that think-aloud protocols closely represent a learner’s knowledge because verbalisations and the processing of knowledge are assumed to involve the same cognitive process. It is possible to access indirectly a learner’s knowledge by interpreting the meaning of a learner’s verbalisations, written statements, calculations, sketches and so forth. 2.2 Physics Knowledge Concepts in physics and everyday usage may have the same names, but the concepts often have different meaning. Chi (1992) mentioned three basic ontological categories of knowledge which are ‘matter or material substances’, ‘events or processes’ and ‘abstractions’, and acknowledged that there may be other ontological categories as well. Concepts based on everyday situations belong to the so called ‘matter-based’ or ‘material-based’ category whereas physics concepts are ‘events-based’ or ‘processes-based’ category (Chi 1992). Knowledge in the ‘matter-based’ or ‘material-based’ category does not operate in the same manner as knowledge in the ‘processed-based or events-based’ category (Chi 1992). Solomon (1993) considered knowledge in terms of two distinct ‘domains’, the ‘symbolic’ and ‘life-world’ domains. The two ‘domains’ represent two largely distinct, non-interacting ‘regions’ of conceptual structure characterised by distinct modes of cognition, different genesis or origin and different mode of operation (Solomon 1993; Taber 2006). 2.3 Knowledge Representations Scientific models, propositions, definitions and formulae are expressed as qualitative-quantitative knowledge (Eylon & Ganiel 1990; Heyworth 1999; Licht 1991; Ploetzner & VanLehn 1996). Some researchers prefer to use the terms physical and mathematical knowledge to imply qualitativequantitative knowledge representations (Carey & Spelke 1994; Greca & Moreira 2002; Nersessian 1999; Lee & Law 2001; Sabella 2000). Qualitative knowledge representation expresses semantic meanings and relationships of terms and entities (Greca & Moreira 2002), and does not facilitate the determination of exact results. Quantitative knowledge representation relies on mathematical formalisms and allows the determination of exact results (Greca & Moreira 2002; Ploetzner & VanLehn 1996). It is not possible to separate learners’ knowledge in a clear-cut manner into qualitative and quantitative knowledge (Ploetzner & VanLehn 1996; Ploetzner et al. 1990). Qualitative and quantitative knowledge refers to the ends of the qualitative-quantitative continuum (Ploetzner et al. 1990; Ploetzner & VanLehn 1996; White & Frederiksen 1990). Learners operate at different levels along the qualitativequantitative continuum. 2.4 Descriptors of Knowledge Driver and Easley’s (1978) described young learners’ knowledge of physical phenomenon in terms of alternative conceptual ‘frameworks’ that exist in the private domain of the mind. Other researchers such as Gilbert and Watts’ (1983) considered young learners’ alternative conceptual ‘frameworks’ to be in the public domain. Gilbert, Osborne and Fensham (1982), Driver and Erickson (1983), Gilbert and Watts (1983), Osborne and Wittrock (1983) appear to consider that knowledge forms conceptual structures in the brain and that it is possible to a certain extend to model learners’ conceptual structures. There is often little consistency on the descriptors of knowledge that are used to represent learners’ knowledge. Gilbert and Watts (1983) suggest conceptions, categories and frameworks as three levels of descriptions. However, it may be necessary to consider not just conceptions but also the range of ideas, reasoning, explanation, calculations, sketches and so forth, that learners used when they consider physical phenomena. Gilbert and Watts (1983) and Watts, Gilbert and Pope (1982) described an alternative ‘framework’ as ‘a composite picture based upon ideas shared by a number of pupils’, ‘generalised non-individual descriptions’ and ‘thematic interpretations of data, stylised, mild caricatures of the responses made by students’. Central to a ‘framework’ is a concept and there maybe different concepts associated with learners’ manifold ‘frameworks’ (Harrison & Treagust 2000; Osborne & Wittrock 1983; Redfors & Niedderer 2002; Taber 2000). The ‘framework’ which is use first is likely to represent the knowledge which is stronger in a learner’s cognitive domain (Redfors & Niedderer 2002). However, some researchers questioned the findings about manifold or multiple ‘frameworks’ especially with regard to the adequacy of the researchers’ descriptions of learners’ conceptions because manifold or multiple ‘frameworks’ suggest that learners’ ideas are not theory-like or structured (Claxton 1993; Kuiper 1994; Pope & Denicolo 1996). 2.5 Structured or ‘In-Pieces’ Rather than a tightly connected, logically organised structure, people have ‘knowledge-in-pieces’ which are loosely connected ideas about the world; ideas that are diverse, often incoherent and relatively independent of each other (di Sessa 1996; di Sessa, Gillespie & Esterly 2004). diSessa (1996) calls these ideas ‘p-prims’. They are phenomenological because ‘p-prims’ are responses to experienced and observed phenomena. They are primitive in the sense that they generally are self-evident and need no explanation; they simply happen (Karlgren & Ramberg 1995). The ‘knowledge-in-pieces’ and ‘knowledge constructs’ views appear to be contradictory views regarding the coordination of learners’ knowledge in the brain. It is again more beneficial to consider a learner’s knowledge along a ‘knowledge-in-pieces’ and ‘knowledge constructs’ continuum because it may well be that learners’ knowledge is ‘structured’ or partly in ‘pieces’ depending on a learner’s level of expertise. 3.0 Concept Area: D.C. Circuits Learners often have difficulties learning about d.c. circuits (Eylon & Ganiel 1990; Saxena 1992; Shipstone, von Rhöneck, Jung, Kärrqvist, Dupin, Johsua & Licht 1988; Tsai 2003). Children’s ideas and conceptions of battery-bulb circuits are often represented as conceptual models of current (Borges & Gilbert 1999; Osborne, Black, Smith & Meadows 1991; Shipstone 1984). Conceptual models are often too simple, limited and do not represent in a holistic manner learners’ knowledge of physical phenomena. Conceptual models of ‘battery-and-bulb’ circuits do not reflect the type of d.c. circuits that older learners study in their course. According to Osborne (1981) and Shipstone (1984), learners’ general understanding of current flow in d.c. circuits improves with age and with physics teaching in classrooms, but other studies such as Härtel (1982) argued that even after extensive teaching learners still fail to grasp some of the basic characteristics of electrical circuits. This shows that some ideas learners have of d.c. circuits are easily challenged through teaching while others persist and resist change (). 4.0 Approach to investigate Learners’ Knowledge The research paradigm of this study is post-structural and employs the approach of grounded theory. ‘Slices of data’ include think-aloud data, interviews, verbal data generated from a d.c. circuit-cards task and small group discussions. Data analysis involves interpretation of meaning of ‘action concepts’ and interpretation at a thematic level. The various meanings of ‘action’ concepts are referred to as facets of knowledge because they represent little ‘faces’ of the various sides of learners’ knowledge. A little ‘face’ or ‘facet’ of knowledge refers to a specific meaning that is associated to an ‘action’ concept. Pope and Denicolo (1986) considered facets of knowledge as the range of ideas, ‘theories’, reasoning and operations of learners. A ‘facet’ is a little ‘face’ or ‘side’ of a many-sided thing. In this study, conceptions, ideas, reasoning, explanations and mental operations are collectively termed as ‘facets’ of knowledge. Facets of knowledge that are about the same concept are categorised as a category of facets of knowledge. The categories are categorised further into major categories whereby a major category is about a super-ordinate concept of d.c. circuits. The members of a major category may differ dimensionally. Categories that relate along the same dimension are connected to form a common framework based on ‘thematic interpretations of data’ (Gilbert & Watts, 1983). A major category that exhibits dimensional differences between its members links the common frameworks and may be considered a core category or concept which is central to all the common frameworks (Glaser & Strauss 1999; Strauss & Corbin 1998; Strauss 1987). A core concept is common to all the common frameworks, but is associated with different meaning in each framework and represents the different ways students consider a concept. A core category is a ‘conceptual site’ that forms the basis of a theoretical scheme or model of learners’ knowledge of d.c. circuits because a core category may serve as a conceptual site where ‘bridges’ may be built across different common frameworks. 5.0 Research Objectives The research objectives of this study are to: (i) Uncover facets of knowledge; (ii) Generate categories of facets of knowledge; (iii) Generate major categories of facets of knowledge; (iv) Construct common frameworks; and (v) Discover a core concept that is central to learners’ knowledge of d.c. circuits. 6.0 Results Open coding through microanalysis of data uncovered 125 facets of knowledge of d.c. circuits. The 125 facets are categorised and 24 categories of facets of knowledge are generated. The categories are categorised further into nine major categories. The categories are connected along dimensions and resulted in the construction of three common frameworks. These common frameworks are referred to as (a) ‘Connection-Consumption’ Framework, (b) ‘V=IR Dominant’ Framework and (c) ‘Relationship-Ratio-V=IR’ Framework. In the ‘Connection-Consumption’ Framework, learners determine ammeter and voltmeter readings based on the physical connections of the components of d.c. circuits and a resistor is a component which consumes current and voltage. In the ‘V=IR Dominant’ Framework, learners determine ammeter and voltmeter readings based on purely the manipulation and calculation of the V=IR equation or representation. Resistance is a number that fits into the V=IR equation and a resistor may or may not consume current or voltage. In the ‘Relationship-Ratio-V=IR’ Framework, students determine current and voltage based on the relationship between variables, ratio of the resistances and application of the V=IR equation. The ‘resistorresistance’ concept is identified as the core concept of d.c. circuits. 7.0 Discussion The common frameworks and core concept informed teachers of learners’ knowledge of d.c. circuits. Learners’ ideas and way of thinking can be identified from the common frameworks. Modelling learners’ knowledge helps create awareness amongst teachers of the nature of learners’ knowledge such as awareness of the difficulties in communicating scientific ideas and reasoning with learners (Watts 1983). It is possible to teach science more effectively if learners’ existing ideas are taken into account (Driver & Easley 1978; Gilbert, Osborne & Fensham 1982; Driver & Erickson 1983; Gilbert & Watts 1983; Osborne & Wittrock 1983). The most important factor that influenced learning is what learners already know (Ausubel 2000, 1963; Taber 2006). Teaching needs to be constructed and negotiated by considering learners’ knowledge (Larochelle and Bednarz 1998; Osborne, Bell and Gilbert (1983). Informing teaching requires developing theory and presenting generalisations that could guide science teachers (Taber 2006). This study constructed three common frameworks that could be used to inform teachers on learners’ knowledge of d.c. circuits. The common frameworks constructed in this study are consistent with Chi’s ideas on ontological categories. The discovery of a core concept is important to teaching because a core concept represents the various ways learners understand d.c. circuits and it is useful as an ‘organiser’ for designing lessons and teaching in the classroom. Selected References Borges, A.T., & Gilbert, J.K. (1999). Mental models of electricity. International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 21, No. 1, 95-117. Chi, M.T.H. (1992). Conceptual change within and across ontological categories: examples from learning and discovery in science. In Giere, R.N. (editor), Cognitive models in science (pp 129-186). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Cohen, R., Eylon, B., & Ganiel, U. (1983). Potential difference and current in simple electric circuits: a study of students’ concepts. American Journal of Physics, 51(5), 407-412. diSessa, A.A., Gillespie, N.M., & Esterly, J.B. (2004). Coherence versus fragmentation in the development of the concept of force. Cognitive Science, 28, 843-900. diSessa, A.A., & Sherin, B.L. (1998). What changes in conceptual change. International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 20, No. 10, 1155-1191. Driver, R., Leach, J., Millar, R., & Scott, P. (1996). Young People’s Images of Science. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press. Driver, R. (1983). The Pupil as Scientist? Milton Keynes, UK: The Open University Press. Driver, R. (1981). Pupils’ alternative frameworks in science. European Journal of Science Education, Vol. 3, No. 1, 93-101. Ericsson, K.A., & Simon, H.A. (1993). Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports As Data (Revised Edition). Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press. Eylon, B-S., & Ganiel, U. (1990). Macro-micro relationships: the missing link between electrostatics and electrodynamics in students’ reasoning. International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 12, No. 1, 79-94. Gilbert, J.K., & Watts, D.M. (1983). Concepts, misconceptions and alternative conceptions: changing perspectives in science education. Studies in Science Education, 10, 61-98. Gilbert, J.K., & Zylbersztajn, A. (1985). A conceptual framework for science education: the case study of force and movement. European Journal of Science Education, Vol. 7, No. 2, 107-120. Glaser, B. (2002). Conceptualization: on theory and theorizing using grounded theory. International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 1 (2). Article 3. Retrieved 27/07/2005 from http://www.ualberta.ca/~ijqm/ Glaser, B.G., & Strauss, A.L. (1967). The Discovery of Grounded Theory: Strategies for Qualitative Research. London,UK: Weidenfeld and Nicolson. Greca, I.M., & Moreira, M.A. (2000). Mental models, conceptual models, and modelling1. International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 22, No. 1, 1-11. Gunstone, R., & Watts, M. (1985). Force and Motion. In Rosalind Driver, Edith Guesne and Andrée Tiberghien. (editors). (1985). Children’s Ideas in Science. Milton Keynes, UK: Open University Press. Heyworth, R.M. (1999). Procedural and conceptual knowledge of expert and novice students for the solving of a basic problem in chemistry. International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 21, No. 2, 195-211. Osborne, J., Black, P., Smith, M., & Meadows, J. (1991). Primary SPACE (Science Processes And Concept Exploration) Project Research Report: Electricity. UK: Liverpool University Press. Ploetzner, R., Spada, H., Stumpf, M., & Opwis, K. (1990). Learning qualitative and quantitative reasoning in a microworld for elastic impacts. European Journal of Psychology of Education, Vol. V, No. 4, 501-516. Ploetzner, R. (1995). The construction and coordination of complementary problem representations in physics. Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 6, 203-238. Pope, M., & Denicolo, P. (2001). Transformative Education: Personal Construct Approaches to Practice and Research. London: Whurr Publishers Ltd. Redfors, A., & Ryder, J. (2001). University physics students' use of models in explanations of phenomena involving interaction between metals and electromagnetic radiation. International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 23, No. 12, 1283-l301. Saxena, A.B. (1992). An attempt to remove misconceptions related to electricity. International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 14, No. 2, 157-162. Seung, T.K. (1982). Structuralism and Hermeneutics. New York: Columbia University Press. Shipstone, D.M., Rhöneck, C.v., Jung, W., Kärrqvist, C., Dupin, J.-J., Johsua, S., & Licht, P. (1988). A study of students’ understanding of electricity in five European countries. International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 10, No. 3, 303-316. Slotta, J.D., Chi, M.T.H., Joram, E. (1995). Assessing students’ misclassifications of physics concepts: an ontological basis for conceptual change. Cognition and Instruction, 13(3), 373-400. Solomon, J. (1984). Prompts, cues and discrimination: the utilization of two separate knowledge systems. European Journal of Science Education, Vol. 6, No. 3, 277-284. Stocklmayer, S.M., & Treagust, D.F. (1996). Images of electricity: how do novices and experts model electric current* International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 18, No. 2, 163-178. Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of Qualitative Research: Techniques and Processes for Developing Grounded Theory. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications. Taber, K.S. (2006). Beyond Constructivism: the Progressive Research Programme into Learning Science. Studies in Science Education, 42; pp. 125-184. Treagust, D.F., & Harrison, A.G. (2000). In search of explanatory frameworks: an analysis of Richard Feynman's lecture 'Atoms in motion'. International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 22, No. 11, 1157-1170. Tsai, C-C. (2003). Using a conflict map as an instructional tool to change student alternative conceptions in simple series electric-circuits. International Journal of Science Education, Vol. 25, No. 3, 307-327. White, B.Y., & Frederiksen, J.R. (l990). Causal model progressions as a foundation for intelligent learning environments. Artificial Intelligence, 42, 99-157.