Psychol. Inj. and Law DOI 10.1007/s12207-013-9153-z Psychological Injuries and Legal Decision Making in Civil Cases: What We Know and What We Do not Know Jonathan P. Vallano Received: 12 October 2012 / Accepted: 17 April 2013 # Springer Science+Business Media New York 2013 Abstract Civil plaintiffs frequently seek monetary compensation for their psychological injuries. Despite the increased study of psychological injury within the legal system, there has been little empirical examination of how psychological injury evidence impacts legal decisions. To illuminate what is known (and not known) on this topic, this paper reviews legal and empirical research regarding how legal decision makers (judges and jurors) perceive and use psychological injury evidence to render civil judgments. A few themes emerged from this review: (1) Courts generally devalue psychological injury, often making it difficult for plaintiffs to pursue and succeed on these claims; (2) these difficulties are a likely byproduct of legal decision-makers’ misperceptions of mental illness; and (3) despite a recent surge in empirical research on how jurors perceive and use psychological injury evidence to render civil decisions, many unexplored areas remain. Specific recommendations for legal policy and suggestions for future research are highlighted throughout this review. Keywords Psychological injury . Mental health . Negligence . Civil law . Juror decision making Introduction Civil plaintiffs routinely seek redress for their psychological injuries. Within a legal setting, psychological injury has been defined as “mental harm, suffering, or injury caused Author Note Jonathan P. Vallano is an Assistant Professor of Psychology at the University of Pittsburgh at Greensburg. J. P. Vallano (*) University of Pittsburgh at Greensburg, 150 Finoli Drive, Greensburg, PA 15601, USA e-mail: jov15@pitt.edu to a person that may be a worsening of a pre-existing condition and that negatively impacts functional activity… leading to claims in court for damages” (Young, 2008, p. 78). The impact of psychological injury is far from trivial, both on society and its victims. Regarding the impact on society, Avraham (2006) estimated that 50 % of civil jury awards involve pain and suffering. Regarding the impact on its victims, tortious conduct can produce mild, “gardenvariety” psychological symptoms (e.g., embarrassment and humiliation; Kovera & Cass, 2002) or, in many cases, severe psychological disorders (e.g., anxiety and depressive disorders; Kane & Dvoskin, 2011). Civil plaintiffs most commonly seek recovery for these injuries when they are concomitant with their physical injuries and less commonly in civil cases that primarily result in psychological injury (e.g., intentional infliction of emotional distress, loss of consortium, sexual harassment; Poser, Bornstein & McGorty, 2003). Clinical and forensic psychologists have long assessed the legitimacy and impact of psychological injuries on civil plaintiffs (Foote & Goodman-Delahunty, 2005; Prins, Verhaak, Bensing, & van der Meer, 2008). As a result, mental health professionals often provide expert testimony regarding the nature, severity, and impact of psychological injuries, if any, on the plaintiff’s overall functioning (Greenberg & Wheeler, 2004). But how are these subjective psychological injuries treated and perceived by legal decision makers (attorneys, judges, and jurors), particularly when compared to the more objective physical injuries? How do these perceptions affect legal decisions? Surprisingly little attention has been given to these issues (see O’Donohue & Bowers, 2006). This article will provide an overview of the relevant legal and empirical research about what we know (and do not know) regarding how legal decision makers perceive and use psychological injury evidence in civil cases. This article will also highlight debated topics and provide suggestions for research to advance our Psychol. Inj. and Law understanding of how psychological injury evidence is used in the courtroom. As a caveat, it should be noted that the present paper is not about how forensic psychologists assess and diagnose psychological injuries, such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), chronic pain, and mild traumatic brain injury. Tests related to assessments of these conditions and whether they are validly presented are mentioned at times [e.g., the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI-2; Butcher et al., 2001), the Structured Interview of Reported Symptoms (Rogers, Bagby & Dickens, 1992), and the Symptom Validity Test (Slick, Hopp, Strauss & Thompson, 1997)], but within the broader context of how these tests help evaluate the plaintiff’s overall psychological injury claim, and how these injuries are conveyed to legal decision makers. Given the adversarial nature of tort and related actions, the plaintiff’s mental health testimony rarely goes unchallenged. The act of seeking recovery for psychological injury typically places this allegation “in controversy,” which subjects the injury claim to scrutiny under Rule 35 of the Federal Rules of Civil Procedure or its state equivalent (Kovera & Cass, 2002).Under Rule 35, the defense can request an independent medical examination (IME) to challenge the plaintiff’s psychological injury claim. The opposing mental health expert will likely question whether the defendant caused the plaintiff’s injuries and/or attack the legitimacy and severity of the plaintiff’s injury (Finch, Guthrie & Henderson, 2008). Civil Courts’ Treatment of Psychological Injury Claims Psychological Injury Claims in Civil Trials: An Overview To initiate a lawsuit, a civil plaintiff must have suffered some form of injury due to the defendant’s conduct. To succeed at trial, a plaintiff must establish that the defendant’s action or failure to act caused the plaintiff’s injuries (Kane & Dvoskin, 2011; Keeton & Prosser, 1984). Upon finding the defendant legally responsible (or liable), a jury may award compensatory damages, the function of which is to help return plaintiffs to their pre-injury state (Greene & Bornstein, 2003). Injured plaintiffs can receive compensation for their economic damages (e.g., pecuniary losses—medical bills, lost wages) or noneconomic damages (e.g., injuries not easily translated into a monetary value—pain and suffering, loss of consortium, loss of enjoyment of life; Poser et al., 2003). Psychological injury primarily falls within the category of noneconomic damages because their intangible nature makes it difficult to determine a precise dollar amount (Zavos, 2009). At trial, psychological injury evidence is typically presented by plaintiffs themselves and/or their treating mental health professional (Smith, 2007). The treating professional may serve as a fact witness, but should not serve as an expert witness because he/she cannot provide independent data. The expert should be retained solely to conduct an evaluation and offer opinions based on a thorough, multisource, evaluation. The mental health professional provides expert testimony regarding the nature and severity of the plaintiff’s injury and further addresses the possibility of malingering (Vincent, Lemond & Inman, 2008). The expert also addresses the projected course and prognosis of the plaintiff’s injuries, including an opinion on causation. That is, the expert testifies to whether the plaintiff’s injuries were a direct result of the defendant’s conduct (Foote & GoodmanDelahunty, 2005; Goodman-Delahunty & Foote, 2009). Civil plaintiffs face many legal obstacles to obtaining a full recovery for their psychological injuries. Arguably the biggest obstacle that plaintiffs encounter is the courts’ treatment of psychological injury claims. Not only do courts make it difficult for plaintiffs to initiate these claims, but the defense is often given wide latitude to challenge these claims at trial (see Smith, 2007). The following sections discuss these obstacles in more detail. Obstacle 1: The Physical Injury Requirement One of the earliest restrictions on the plaintiff’s ability to initiate a psychological injury claim involves barring recovery in the absence of physical injury (often referred to as the impact rule; Rhee, 2004; Smith, 2007). For many years, under general tort law, plaintiffs could seek recovery for their psychological injuries (a) only if they suffered physical injury and/or (b) only if the psychological injury is concomitant with their physical injury (see Bornstein & Schwartz, 2009). As an illustration of this prevailing notion, pain and suffering injuries are often referred to as “parasitic damages” (Zavos, 2009). Although most jurisdictions have replaced the strict physical injury requirement with the more lenient “zone of danger” rule (Huffaker, 2001), a minority of jurisdictions have retained this requirement (e.g., Georgia). Even under the zone of danger rule or a derivation thereof (see Dillon v. Legg, 1968), a reasonable possibility of physical harm must still exist (see Wal-Mart Stores, Inc. v. Bowers, 1999). Thus, courts sometimes retain a general emphasis on physical injury before permitting recovery for pain and suffering. That being said, plaintiffs can obtain compensation for tortious conduct that produces purely psychological injury [e.g., intentional infliction of emotional distress (IIED), civil rights claims (Cass, Levett & Kovera, 2009; Kircher, 2007)]. However, these claims are not without restrictions. Psychol. Inj. and Law For example, an IIED plaintiff can recover for their psychological injuries only when the defendant causes severe emotional distress (see Bornstein & Schwartz, 2009; Hyatt v. Trans World Airlines, 1997). Whether the plaintiff’s injury constitutes severe distress falls under the courts’ discretion. This is problematic for plaintiffs whose injuries the courts deem insufficiently severe [e.g., emotional distress resulting from a false HIV test (Johnson v. Methodist Hospital, 2006)] and for plaintiffs who manifest only minor injuries due to effective coping mechanisms (see Schneider, Swan & Fitzgerald, 1997). Similar constraints exist when seeking recovery in employment discrimination cases. Although the plaintiff need not suffer injury (physical or psychological) to initiate a harassment claim (Harris v. Forklift Systems, 1993), compensatory damage awards are capped at $900,000 in some jurisdictions (see the Civil Rights Act of 1991). As a result, plaintiffs who experience severe psychological injury are often unable to receive compensation for the full extent of their injuries. Other torts similarly employ caps on compensatory damage awards (e.g., medical malpractice; Hyman, Black, Silver & Sage, 2009). Often, these caps generally apply only to mental (but not physical) injuries (Zavos, 2009). Moreover, employers (under the Americans with Disabilities Act) and insurance companies attempt to confine recovery to bodily (physical) injury, with many courts obliging (see Hebert v. Webre, 2008; Lopez v. Geico Insurance Co., 2012). The existence of the physical injury requirement not only makes recovery difficult but also implies that physical and psychological injuries are fundamentally different. In fact, the oft-cited rationale for the physical injury requirement is that the existence of objective physical injury lends credibility to subjective psychological injury claims (DePianto, 2012). Because physical injuries are often substantiated via standard medical equipment (X-rays, brain scans; Rhee, 2004), by comparison, psychological injuries appear difficult to verify. However, partly because of their overlap in legal cases and partly due to subjective elements in both, the concepts of “physical” and “psychological” injury remain ill-defined, and no clear guidelines exist to classify an injury as physical or psychological (Koch, O’Neil & Douglas, 2005). This difficulty in classification may stem from the polytrauma or the multiple injuries plaintiffs might experience, and therefore, the high inter-relatedness between these injuries (Duckworth & Iezzi, 2010). In fact, many injuries have interacting physical and psychological origins (e.g., pain—Bornstein & Schwartz, 2009; Finch, 2005). Moreover, recent neuro-scientific research suggests that these injuries may have more similarities than differences; for example, increasingly both physical and psychological injury can be objectively verified via functional magnetic resonance imaging (Beery, 2004; see Eggen & Laury, 2011, for a review). Legal and Empirical Recommendations Given that strict empirical evidence for the physical/psychological injury distinction is lacking, especially in cases of polytrauma and comorbidity, courts should consider whether they should retain the physical injury requirement. Although courts have long been skeptical of psychological injury claims due to the belief that they open the door to frivolous lawsuits (Smith, 2007), research suggests that psychologists are increasingly capable of assessing psychological injuries such as PTSD and whether they are malingered (Kane & Dvoskin, 2011). As a practical matter, the absence of available guidelines to classify an injury as physical or psychological in cases involving polytrauma or comorbidity makes it difficult for courts to determine whether the physical injury requirement has been satisfied. Plaintiffs who suffer an injury of both physical and psychological origin may not be adequately compensated if erroneously classified as purely psychological. For example, certain diagnoses such as PTSD have physical and psychological components, and courts must decide whether the physical injury requirement is met for such disorders (see Ware v. ANW, 2008). This requirement may also produce unintended consequences. Because this requirement forces courts to distinguish physical from psychological injury, the act of bifurcation may make jurors more likely to perceive pain and suffering as insignificant “add on” or bonus damages (Engel, 2010). For these reasons, many legal scholars (e.g., DePianto, 2012; Gray, 2009) recommend the abolition of this requirement. Whether the physical injury requirement is maintained should be informed by empirical research. Although physical and psychological injuries are ostensibly different, little is known regarding the dimensions on which they differ. For example, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric American Psychiatric Association, 2000) includes under pain disorder three types associated with a general medical condition, psychological factors, or both. More importantly, little is known about what legal decision makers perceive to be the differences between physical and psychological injuries. Given the issues about the distinction between physical and psychological injury, it appears that research is needed concerning how judges classify injuries as physical and psychological to determine whether the physical injury requirement has been satisfied. Programmatic research should be additionally undertaken, for example, on how jurors perceive the physical injury requirement, and the differences between the plaintiff’s physical and psychological injuries. This research will provide empirical data to inform whether courts should maintain or eliminate the physical injury requirement, as well as common legal distinctions between Psychol. Inj. and Law physical and psychological injury (e.g., economic and noneconomic damage awards, respectively). Obstacle 2: Challenging a Psychological Injury Claim Another obstacle to a full plaintiff recovery is the extensive latitude given to the defense when challenging the plaintiff’s psychological injury claim (Beery, 2004; Smith, 2007). In most civil cases, the plaintiff’s psychological injuries are subject to scrutiny if the courts deem the injuries “in controversy.” If the injuries are considered in controversy, the defense can request an IME to challenge the nature and the extent of the plaintiff’s injuries. In practice, the mere presence of psychological injury typically places the allegation in controversy. However, there are several exceptions to this general rule. In some instances, mild psychological injury that does not involve expert testimony regarding a specific psychological disorder (e.g., humiliation, embarrassment) rarely places the injury in controversy (Foote & GoodmanDelahunty, 2005). In other instances, these injuries are considered to be within the knowledge base of the jury, so that expert testimony is not required. The knowledge that severe mental distress places a psychological injury allegation in controversy leads to a catch22 for the plaintiff. If the plaintiff attempts recovery for severe emotional distress, testimony by an opposing psychological expert may (a) reduce the perceived severity of the injury or (b) greatly harm the plaintiff’s credibility. If the plaintiff does not attempt to mitigate the damages, the probability of undercompensation becomes a near certainty. Either option may effectively contribute to vertical inequity (Greene, Downey, & Goodman-Delahunty 1999), where plaintiffs with severe injuries receive the same level of compensation as plaintiffs with mild injuries. That is, the presence of an opposing expert may reduce the likelihood that these plaintiffs will obtain compensation that reflects the severity of their injury. Legal and Empirical Recommendations The severity of the plaintiff’s injuries often dictates whether plaintiffs can pursue recovery for their psychological injuries and whether the defense can challenge these claims. Judges must serve as “gatekeeper” (see Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, 509 U.S. 579, 1993) and evaluate mental health information to assess whether the injury is mild or severe, and thus in controversy (Redding & Murrie, 2010). Despite its importance, we know little about how judges perceive the plaintiff’s psychological injury claim and how they determine whether an injury is severe enough to place the injury in controversy. Because most judges receive little (if any) formal training about scientific evidence, particularly training on mental health issues (see Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, 509 U.S. 579, 1993—often referred to as “Daubert II”); Redding & Murrie, 2010), whether judges should evaluate evidence of psychological injury severity to make these determinations demands further examination. Given that judges currently evaluate psychological injury evidence, future research should examine how judges perceive psychological injuries (and their severity) and the resulting effects on legal decisions. Although the defense commonly challenges the severity of the plaintiff’s alleged injuries via cross-examination and opposing expert testimony, we know little regarding the impact of these techniques on jurors’ legal decisions. How does the mere act of challenging the plaintiff’s injury claim affect liability and damage decisions? Through what modality is this challenge most effective (cross-examination vs. opposing expert testimony), and which type of legal challenge is most effective (e.g., challenging the plaintiff’s credibility and/or whether the defendant caused the injury)? Do these challenges have differential effects on mild and severe injury? That is, perhaps these challenges contribute to vertical inequity by reducing jurors’ damage awards, but only for those plaintiffs alleging severe psychological injury. Obstacle 3: The Presence and Admissibility of Pre-existing Psychological Injury When a plaintiff alleges psychological injury at trial, the defense commonly challenges these allegations by introducing pre-existing psychological injury (PEI) evidence. It is likely that many civil plaintiffs suffer from PEI, for nearly 50 % of the population meets diagnostic criteria for a psychological disorder during their lifetime (Moffitt et al., 2010). Although damaging to a case, PEI does not preclude plaintiffs from recovering for their psychological injuries (see the “eggshell skull” rule—you take plaintiffs as they are; Levy & Rosenberg, 2003). As with all evidence, PEI must meet admissibility requirements before it reaches the jury. To be admissible, PEI must be more probative (relevant to helping juries decide whether a statement is more likely than not true) than prejudicial (the possibility that PEI will excessively and negatively affect a jury; see Beery, 2004). The plaintiff’s PEI is often considered generally probative to jurors’ credibility assessments and specifically probative to legal causation (Smith, 2007). When evaluating credibility, jurors can consider whether the plaintiff is intentionally blaming the defendant for distress that pre-dated the defendant’s conduct. When evaluating legal causation, jurors can consider whether PEI provides a plausible alternative explanation for the plaintiff’s alleged injuries. That is, whether the plaintiff is intentionally concealing the contribution of his or her PEI to their current injury allegation or whether an otherwise Psychol. Inj. and Law honest plaintiff is incorrectly attributing a spike in symptomatology to the defendant’s conduct. Due to its possible relevance, courts routinely admit testimony about psychological PEI (see Beery, 2004; Smith, 2007). Legal and Empirical Recommendations Despite the prevalence of PEI in civil cases, courts often undertake little (if any) analysis regarding the probative and prejudicial nature of PEI. Courts may avoid this inquiry because (a) they lack specific knowledge on PEI and how it may affect the jury and/or (b) because no available guidelines exist to assist their decisions. A potential consequence is that courts may unknowingly admit irrelevant and prejudicial PEI. Of course, relevancy guidelines are only necessary if cases exist where PEI is irrelevant to a current injury claim. Consider a negligence case where the plaintiff currently alleges PTSD, but suffered from a pre-existing psychological injury of another type in the distant past. Ten years ago, the plaintiff was diagnosed with major depressive disorder. Through a combination of successful treatment and/or the natural course of this disorder, the plaintiff had become asymptomatic at the time that the negligent conduct occurred. In this example, the preponderance of the evidence dictates that the PEI is unrelated to the plaintiff’s current injury claim. Under the assumption that PEI can be either relevant or irrelevant to the plaintiff’s current injury claim, psychologists should generate empirically supported guidelines to assist judges when evaluating the probative nature of PEI. When evaluating the probative nature of PEI, particularly in regards to legal causation, the following factors should be considered: (a) symptom overlap between the PEI and the current injury allegation, (b) the date of PEI onset, (c) the course and duration of the PEI, and (d) its amenability to treatment. If the PEI and current injury allegation share many disorders and symptoms in common, the PEI is relevant by providing a plausible alternative explanation for the plaintiff’s injuries. Providing judges with co-morbidity rates may assist in this determination; PEI that is highly comorbid with the alleged injury (e.g., panic disorder and agoraphobia) is more relevant than PEI that is not commonly associated with the alleged injury (e.g., generalized anxiety disorder and anti-social personality disorder; National Comorbidity Survey Replication—Kessler et al., 2003). The date of onset is also useful when determining the relevance of PEI. Because many disorders can be effectively managed (see Angermeyer & Matschinger, 2003) and become frequently asymptomatic (e.g., mood disorders; American Psychiatric Association, 2000), long-standing PEI’s are often less relevant than those diagnosed in closer proximity to the defendant’s conduct. The course, duration, and treatability of the disorder are similarly relevant, for each speaks to whether the plaintiff experienced these symptoms right before the defendant’s alleged conduct occurred. Disorders that are generally more acute and amenable to treatment (e.g., anxiety disorders) are less likely to provide alternative explanations for the plaintiff’s current mental health status. In this regard, the plaintiff’s demonstration or desire to mitigate PEI’s in mental health history must be considered before making this determination (e.g., whether the plaintiff sought and obtained successful treatment for his or her disorder). Little (if any) research has examined the prejudicial impact of psychological PEI. The paucity of research on this topic may be due to the lack of clarity regarding what constitutes prejudicial evidence. One way to operationalize and measure prejudicial impact is to examine whether the presence of PEI evidence is more heavily weighed by jurors when compared to other trial evidence (see DePianto, 2012). Thus, PEI may be prejudicial if its presence overwhelms the impact of all other exculpatory evidence by guaranteeing a not liable verdict. Future research should manipulate the presence (and absence) of psychological PEI to determine its relative and overall impact on case outcomes when compared to other case evidence. In certain research designs, multiple regression analyses can help determine whether PEI is overly prejudicial by comparing R2 and R2 change to other evidence, including the amount of variance in legal judgments accounted for by PEI. If PEI strongly shifts the balance in favor of the defendant when compared to other evidence, its presence may be considered unduly prejudicial, necessitating stricter admissibility guidelines. Legal Decision Makers’ Perceptions of Psychological Injury The above review illustrates how the law often places civil plaintiffs at a disadvantage when initiating psychological injury claims. Perhaps, an even bigger obstacle encountered by civil plaintiffs are legal decision makers’ perceptions of psychological injury. In fact, the very legal requirements that place plaintiffs at a disadvantage are a likely by-product of legal decision makers’ (a) lack of knowledge about mental health issues and, as a result, (b) misperceptions about psychological injury. These misperceptions are not surprising, for generally, legal officials receive no specific training on mental health issues (see Redding & Murrie, 2010). The following sections address what we know (and do not know) regarding legal officials’ knowledge of mental health issues, and the common misperceptions held within the legal community regarding psychological injury evidence. Attorneys’ Perceptions of Psychological Injury Evidence Plaintiffs’ attorneys play a key role in the initiation and resolution of a psychological injury claim. In fact, attorneys Psychol. Inj. and Law are usually the first legal professionals to formally assess the merits of the plaintiff’s psychological injury claim. In these cases, plaintiffs’ attorneys must decide whether to take the case and then whether to pursue settlement or trial. Upon choosing to pursue the claim at trial, attorneys must decide whether and how much to emphasize the plaintiff’s psychological injuries (see Beery, 2004). These decisions may strongly hinge on two factors: (a) attorneys’ personal beliefs regarding psychological injury and (b) their predictions regarding how jurors will perceive their client’s injuries. Based on these factors, strong cases (those most likely to be successful and produce a large damage award) are likely to be prioritized over weak cases. However, research indicates that attorneys’ ability to predict case outcomes is limited—attorneys are often inaccurate and overconfident regarding their ability to successfully resolve their cases (Goodman-Delahunty, Hartwig, Granhag & Loftus, 2010; Loftus & Wagenaar, 1988). This research suggests that attorneys may be equally poor predictors of how a jury will perceive psychological injury at trial. Although plaintiffs attorneys’ perceptions of psychological injury likely play an integral role in case dispositions, little (if any) research has examined their perceptions of these injuries, or of how they believe jurors will perceive these injuries. Specifically, how do plaintiffs’ attorneys initially judge the nature, severity, and credibility of their client’s psychological injuries? How do these perceptions and their beliefs about how jurors will perceive psychological injury affect their case decisions? That is, are attorneys less likely to accept a civil case primarily involving psychological injury? What strategies do they utilize to increase compensatory damage awards, and are these strategies effective? Future research should provide attorneys with different types and severity levels of psychological injury to initially examine attorneys’ own perceptions of these injuries, and attorneys’ perceptions of how jurors will perceive these injuries. This research should further examine how attorneys’ perceptions affect their dispositional decisions. Results will illuminate whether attorneys devalue psychological injury claims and whether this devaluation is due to attorneys’ beliefs, or their beliefs about jurors’ likely reactions to this evidence at trial. Judges’ and Jurors’ General Perceptions of Psychological Injury Evidence Once an attorney pursues recovery for his or her client’s injuries, triers of fact (the judge or jury) are the final arbiters of the case outcome. Other than personal experience, judges and jurors often have little knowledge about psychological disorders (see Redding & Murrie, 2010). The knowledge that they do have may also be erroneous, placing another roadblock between plaintiffs and their deserved compensation. The following sections examine what judges and jurors most likely know about psychological injury. Lay Knowledge of Mental Health Issues Research indicates that lay knowledge of mental illness (often referred to as mental health literacy) is often less than to be desired (Haslam & Giosan, 2002; Lauber, Nordt & Wulf, 2005; Lauber, Nordt, Falcato & Wulf, 2003). For example, adults have difficulty labeling psychological injuries as severe and diagnosable disorders (e.g., depression; Lauber et al., 2005) and have difficulty describing the general symptomatology underlying many commonly diagnosed disorders (e.g., mood and personality disorders; Prins et al., 2008). Upon giving adults vignettes describing symptoms of schizophrenia and depression, Lauber et al. (2003) found that adults correctly identified the depression vignette as a depressive disorder 40 % of the time, and correctly identified the schizophrenia vignette as schizophrenia 70 % of the time. Similarly, Angermeyer and Matschinger (2003) observed that participants were more likely to identify schizophrenia as a mental disorder than depression. A troubling implication of these findings is that laypeople not only have difficulty identifying common disorders adduced by civil plaintiffs (e.g., mood disorders) but also devalue their severity. Reflecting on this issue, Jorm, Christensen and Griffiths (2005, p. 52) commented that there is a “general lack of exposure to depression leading to an underestimation of its seriousness and the necessity for intervention.” Based upon the common misconceptions of mental illness in the general population, it is not surprising that legal decision makers subscribe to similar misperceptions about psychological injury evidence. But perhaps, the largest overarching misperception endorsed within the legal system is that physical injuries are fundamentally more damaging than psychological injuries (Gray, 2009). This assumption underlies the legal distinctions between these injuries that make it easier for plaintiffs to recover for physical than psychological injuries (Bornstein & Schwartz, 2009). In fact, the latest draft of the Restatement of Torts (3rd; 2007 American Law Institute, 2010) endorses this belief and outlines reasons why the legal distinction between physical and emotional (psychological) injury should remain. These reasons can be categorized as follows—emotional injuries are (a) not objectively verifiable, (b) easily fabricated, and (c) not severe. The following sections address the merits of these assumptions. Misperception 1: Psychological Injuries Are not Objectively Verifiable Some courts appear to hold the false belief that the mercurial nature of psychological injury does not permit objective Psychol. Inj. and Law verification (Smith, 2007). As such, when compared to physical injuries, psychological injuries are often defined by the courts as subjective and intangible (Leonard v. John Crane, 142 Cal. Rptr. 3d 700, 2012; People v. House, 61 AD3d 700, 2009). Unlike physical injuries, which are often amenable to direct and observable evidence (e.g., X-rays, brain scans), despite the accuracy that might be obtained through psychological assessment, no known method exists to definitively substantiate a purely psychological injury. Thus, psychological assessments use not only objective tests but also heavily rely on clinical judgment of the plaintiff’s self-reported symptoms (Kane & Dvoskin, 2011). However, especially when used inappropriately, self-report data and clinical judgment are fraught with a host of biases that can lower diagnostic accuracy (Borum, Otto & Golding, 1993; Kane & Dvoskin, 2011). Despite these challenges, it is unfair to portray psychological injuries as completely subjective. Although physical injuries may be more easily observable than psychological injuries, this depends upon what is meant by “observable.” Just as one can view a broken arm, one can view depression by the manifestation of overt behavior (e.g., psychomotor retardation, weight loss, lack of interest). In addition, there are many tools at psychologists’ disposal to verify the existence of an underlying psychological malady and whether their symptoms can be attributed to malingering (Kane & Dvoskin, 2011). Instead of solely relying on clinical judgment, competent mental health experts utilize a battery of reliable and valid psychological tests to support initial diagnoses (see Groth-Marnat, 2003). Although these tests rely on self-report, they include respondent validity indicators (e.g., the MMPI-2; Graham, 2000). In addition, competent psychologists utilize other means to enhance diagnostic reliability (i.e., symptom validity testing, review of records, and behavioral observation). It should also be considered that the process of confirming the existence of physical injury is not as objective as it appears. The term “objective” refers to evidence “not influenced by personal feelings or opinions” (Merriam-Webster’s collegiate dictionary, 11th edn, 2012). Under this definition, the clinical judgment utilized to assess the nature and extent of physical injuries opens the door for subjectivity and bias (Crumlish & Kelly, 2009; Ransohoff & Feinstein, 1978). Although many medical tests aid diagnostic decisions, physicians must interpret test results based upon their training and experience. In addition, many physical injuries are subjective in that they do not appear on an X-ray or brain scan (e.g., pain, fibromyalgia, and certain bone fractures. Furthermore, as mentioned, it is often difficult (and in some cases, impossible) to determine whether the claimed injury has a physical or psychological origin (Finch, 2005). These complexities make both physical and mental injuries difficult to distinguish and thus difficult to verify via objective means. The emphasis on whether psychological injuries are actually objectively verifiable detracts from the more important issue of whether jurors perceive these injuries as objectively verifiable. Future studies should examine what components make an injury objectively verifiable and whether psychological injuries are generally perceived by jurors as less objectively verifiable than physical injuries. Specifically, researchers should utilize various civil scenarios that could plausibly result in either physical or psychological injury, manipulate the plaintiff’s alleged injury (physical vs. psychological), and hold the consequences to the plaintiff’s overall functioning constant. If it is found that jurors also perceive psychological injuries as subjective, future studies should examine what evidence (e.g., psychological testing, clear rules on how to use clinical judgment) can best enhance its perceived objectiveness and standing as a legitimate and recoverable injury. Misperception 2: Psychological Injuries are Easily Fabricated Due to the belief that psychological injuries are difficult (if not impossible) to verify, courts approach these injuries with great skepticism (Smith, 2007). In the present climate of tort reform, courts have been wary of allowing recovery for psychological injuries, as they believe this would enable a profundity of civil lawsuits (Eskin v. Bartee, 262 S.W.3d 727, 2008). Research suggests that jurors share this skepticism and question plaintiffs attempting to recover for these ostensibly subjective injuries (Hans & Vodino, 2007; O’Donohue & Bowers, 2006; O’Donohue & O’Hare, 1997). For example, Vallano, Winter, and Charman (recently submitted for publication) found that mock jurors were especially skeptical of psychological injuries that were (a) extremely severe and (b) exceeded their expectations for reasonable injuries. An important matter to consider is that there are numerous motivations for plaintiffs to fabricate psychological injury (including the obvious monetary incentive; O’Donohue & Bowers, 2006), and there are often few (if any) penalties for making such a claim. Because these injuries are difficult to observe and heavily reliant on self-report, psychological injuries might appear easy to feign. Even more, given their lack of expertise, many plaintiffs’ attorneys’ may not identify malingering due to an unconditional trust in their client, while others are not knowledgeable enough about psychological tests to assess the plaintiff’s credibility. In contrast, psychologists can resort to many methods to assess whether a plaintiff is lying or exaggerating about their psychological symptoms. In forensic settings, evaluators frequently utilize tests with sound psychometric properties, some of which are specifically designed to detect malingering (e.g., MMPI-2; Test of Memory Malingering; Tombaugh Psychol. Inj. and Law 2006, and Validity Indicator Profile; Frederick, 1997). The MMPI-2, one of the most commonly utilized psychological tests in forensic settings (Kane & Dvoskin, 2011), contains empirically supported validity scales, the F family of scales and other scales that detect response biases (Graham, 2000). In addition, medical evaluators often use clinical judgment and not objective measures to determine physical symptoms such as pain, tingling, numbness, and blurry vision. Therefore, relative to psychological symptoms, some physical symptoms might be just as easily fabricated, as they also rely on self-report and clinical judgment. This paper argues that the sweeping generalization that psychological injuries are more easily fabricated than physical injuries would seem inaccurate. This oversimplification can lead to the unintended consequence of courts’ unfairly scrutinizing a psychological injury claim to avoid frivolous lawsuits. In reality, nearly any injury can be fabricated, and any lawsuit can be frivolous. Because techniques exist to provide reasonable certainty regarding the existence of psychological injury and because physical symptoms might not be as objective as they seem, psychological injuries should not be devalued in court, making it difficult for plaintiffs to obtain adequate recovery for their injuries. Misperception 3: Psychological Injuries Are not Severe If stakeholders in the legal system endorse the previously discussed misconceptions, they would be led to conclude that psychological injuries are not severe. However, of the general population who meet criteria for a diagnosable disorder within their lifetime, a high percentage of these symptoms can be classified as severe (Kessler et al., 2003). In fact, psychological injuries can be equally, if not more debilitating, than physical injuries (Blumberg, 2009; Rhee, 2004). As an illustration, DePianto (2012) found that adults’ ratings of their overall subjective well-being were most heavily impacted by their emotional (and not physical) well-being. This is likely because some psychological injuries are not easily amenable to treatment due to their pervasive and/or inflexible nature (e.g., personality disorders, severe polytrauma). Even worse, there is a heavy stigma associated with psychological injury (King et al., 2007). The general public often classifies individuals as weak if they are unable to cope with their distress, especially if they seek professional help (Eisenberg, Downs, Golberstein & Zivin, 2009). This mental distress can be exacerbated by the general skepticism regarding these hidden injuries. For these reasons, individuals with mental illness have difficulty seeking the proper treatment for their disorder, prolonging their suffering (Schomerus & Angermeyer, 2008). Are physical injuries actually more severe than psychological injuries? It is undoubtedly difficult (and likely impossible) to establish whether physical and psychological injuries differ in actual severity, given that these perceptions are largely subjective. Whether physical injuries are actually more severe than psychological injuries diverts from the more important question: Do legal decision makers generally perceive psychological injuries as not very severe, compared to their physical counterparts? This question is important because it may provide the best explanation for why physical and psychological injuries are treated differently by the courts and why some psychological injury claims may be generally devalued by legal decision makers. Future research should employ the previously discussed experimental methodology to allow for causal inferences about the relative perceived severity of physical and psychological injuries (by manipulating injury type and measuring perceptions of injury severity). Because courts and legal guidelines (e.g., the Restatement of Torts) implicitly (and often explicitly) express their distrust of psychological injury, future research should examine whether jurors share these views. One reason for the underappreciation of mental distress in court may be that inadequate tools exist to measure and convey injury severity to legal decision makers. Experts often use psychological testing to assess the plaintiff’s injuries, and many psychological tests that aid in diagnostic decisions contain some type of built-in indicators of injury severity, with a greater number of symptoms indicating injury severity (e.g., the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory—Spielberger, 1983; Beck Depression Inventory— Beck, Steer & Brown, 1996). The DSM-IV-TR has individual guidelines to classify specific disorders as mild, moderate, or severe, and also contains a global assessment of functioning (GAF) scale to rate the client’s overall daily functioning on a 1–100 scale. As indicated by its name, the GAF scale primarily assesses injury severity by the level of impairment in daily functioning. Unfortunately, these crude assessments are unable to fully assess and convey the extent of the plaintiff’s psychological injury, given that injury severity is likely more complex and multidimensional than indicated by the number of total symptoms or functional impairment (Kane & Dvoskin, 2011). Future research should examine the ability of these approaches to fully demonstrate to the court the extent of the plaintiff’s injuries and how triers of fact perceive these approaches to injury severity. Jurors’ Specific Perceptions of Psychological Injury Evidence in Civil Trials The above review examined how judges and jurors generally treat and perceive psychological injury. This review now narrows the focus to how jurors likely perceive psychological injury evidence in the context of civil trials. The following section summarizes the available literature relevant to these topics and addresses the factors that likely Psychol. Inj. and Law affect how jurors perceive psychological injury evidence in the courtroom. Jurors’ perceptions of the plaintiff’s injuries are likely based on their general notions of psychological injury. These preconceived notions, often termed schemas (Alba & Hasher, 1983), consist of jurors’ general beliefs about psychological injury, including what injuries typically result in certain civil cases. To date, most research has examined the content of jurors’ physical injury schemas, finding that jurors associate certain physical injuries with certain types of tortious conduct (Hart, Wissler & Saks, 1997; Hart, Evans, Wissler, Feehan, & Saks, 1997). For example, Hart et al. (1997) found that jurors have well-developed physical injury schemas for slip and fall and car accident cases; jurors believed these cases typically resulted in broken bones and whiplash, respectively. These jurors exhibited less-developed schemas for product liability and medical malpractice cases, for which they had difficulty reporting typical injuries. Unfortunately, there is scant research examining legal decision makers’ psychological injury schemas. The few indirect investigations on this issue have found that jurors generally exhibit underdeveloped psychological injury schemas (Hart et al., 1997; Popovich, Jolton, Mastrangelo & Everton, 1995). Popovich et al. (1995) discovered that even though sexual harassment almost always produces psychological injury, only 25 % of participants’ harassment scripts mentioned that the victim would suffer an “emotional response.” Even more striking, Hart et al. (1997) found that in four negligence cases, mental distress was present in 1 % of participants’ injury schemas. Although it is tempting to conclude that psychological injury schemas are either nonexistent or underdeveloped (particularly compared to physical injuries), the “jury is still out” on this issue. Future research should directly examine the content of psychological and physical injury schemas in various civil cases. Because the aforementioned research asks participants to generally report their schemas in civil cases, future research should specifically ask participants to visualize and record the typical psychological (and physical) injuries they expect to result in civil cases. In the process, these studies should examine how jurors’ attitudes and experience with mental illness affect the content of their psychological injury schemas (see Hart et al., 1997). Although schemas undoubtedly influence how jurors generally perceive psychological injury, how do jurors perceive actual testimony regarding the plaintiff’s psychological injuries at trial? Unfortunately, most research has examined jurors’ perceptions of the plaintiff’s physical injuries (Hart et al., 1997; Wissler, Evans, Hart, Morry & Saks, 1997). For example, Wissler et al. (1997) manipulated whether an expert described mild or severe physical injury and measured mock jurors’ perceptions of the plaintiff’s injuries on nine dimensions: how visible the injury is, the amount of physical pain, duration of physical pain, amount of mental suffering, duration of mental suffering, severity of disability, severity of disfigurement, overall severity of injury, and the effect of the injury on the plaintiff’s everyday life. Plaintiffs with severe injuries received higher ratings on five of the nine injury dimensions than plaintiffs with mild injuries (visibility, disability, the amount of mental suffering, overall severity, and the effect on the plaintiff’s everyday life). The fact that participants’ ratings on these nine dimensions were not perfectly correlated supports the notion that physical injury severity is a multidimensional construct. Unfortunately, there has been relatively little study regarding the dimensions that comprise psychological injury, as well as jurors’ perceptions of these dimensions. However, the relevant research on physical injury strongly suggests that psychological injury severity is also a multidimensional construct. Recent research by Vallano and colleagues examined jurors’ perceptions of psychological injury severity in sexual harassment cases (Winter & Vallano, 2013; Vallano, Winter, & Charman, manuscript submitted for publication). After manipulating whether expert testimony described mild or severe psychological injury, Winter and Vallano (manuscript submitted for publication) used the psychological injury questionnaire (PIQ) to assess participants’ perceptions of psychological injury severity. This research indicated that, consistent with physical injury research, mock jurors perceived differences in severity and rated the injury descriptions differently on several, but not all, of the included dimensions. The PIQ is the first known attempt to specifically assess the dimensions that comprise psychological injury severity and what dimensions distinguish mild from severe injury. Although some dimensions from the physical injury severity of Wissler et al. (1997) were incorporated in the PIQ (e.g., amount and duration of pain, visibility, and the effect on the plaintiff’s everyday functioning), other dimensions were not incorporated because they may not be as readily applicable to psychological injury (e.g., the amount of disability, disfigurement). Thus, additional dimensions were added to more adequately examine the unique and multidimensional nature of psychological injury, such as frequency of symptoms, permanency of the injury, whether the injury is perceived as treatable, and whether the victim can cope with the injury on his or her own. Perceived frequency of the plaintiff’s symptoms should be incorporated, because this dimension is employed by the DSM-IV-TR to assess injury severity. The perceived permanency of the psychological injury should also be incorporated (i.e., acute vs. chronic), given that this dimension is included on other injury severity measures (e.g., the National Association of Insurance Commissioners—see Cohen & Hughes, 2007) and has been shown to strongly predict civil damage awards (see Wissler, Psychol. Inj. and Law Kuehn, & Saks, 2000). Further dimensions include perceived treatability, or how successful medication or therapy can treat psychological disorders. Hart et al. (1997) discovered that the likelihood of successful treatment (termed “correctability”) was one of the primary dimensions underlying perceptions of physical injury severity. Although not yet studied, treatability perceptions likely influence compensatory damage awards. For instance, although research generally finds a positive relationship between injury severity and damage awards (see Robbennolt, 2000), plaintiffs suffering from severe, irreparable harm (e.g., paralysis) may receive less compensation than plaintiffs suffering from severe but repairable injuries (e.g., skin grafts for third degree burns). Furthermore, the perceived ability of the plaintiff to cope with their disorder may affect overall perceptions of psychological injury severity, as some disorders (e.g., depression) may be considered less severe because the general population believes they do not require the assistance of others (Schomerus & Angermeyer, 2008). The Effect of Psychological Injury Evidence on Legal Decisions It is important to understand legal decision makers’ perceptions of psychological injury evidence because these perceptions will affect their legal judgments. The following sections address how psychological injury should affect legal judgments in civil cases and summarizes the scant research related to how psychological injury actually affects jurors’ legal judgments. Liability Determinations How Psychological Injury Evidence Should Affect Jurors’ Liability Decisions In civil cases, jurors must determine whether the defendant’s conduct caused the plaintiff’s injuries. Thus, the nature and extent of the plaintiff’s psychological injury is ostensibly irrelevant to this determination and should exert little (if any) influence on jurors’ liability decisions (Greene, Johns & Smith, 2001). However, the plaintiff’s psychological injuries are often incorporated within jurors’ evaluations of the plaintiff’s credibility, as well as whether the defendant caused the plaintiff’s injuries (Smith, 2007). For example, a jury that is skeptical of the plaintiff’s injury claim may critique the overall credibility of the plaintiff’s claim. In addition, a jury that believes that the plaintiff’s injuries are too severe, or perhaps too delayed, may surmise that the defendant did not cause the injury. How Psychological Injury Evidence Actually Affects Jurors’ Liability Decisions The bulk of research on this topic has examined the effect of physical injury severity on civil judgments. Generally speaking, more severe physical injuries increase findings of liability (e.g., Greene et al., 2001; Robbennolt, 2000; Woody, 2008; but see Cather, Greene & Durham, 1996). One explanation for this positive relationship is defensive attribution theory (Shaver, 1970). Defensive attribution theory posits that more severe injuries evoke emotions such as sympathy (Bright & Goodman-Delahunty, 2010) and fear (Feigenson, Park, & Salovey, 1997, 2001) over the concern that jurors may themselves be the victim of similar negligent conduct, which jurors attempt to reduce by punishing the defendant. However, the existence of this positive relationship may further depend upon perceptions of the defendant’s conduct (Greene et al., 2001), whether jurors are able to award compensatory damages (Bornstein, 1998), and whether the alleged injuries typically result from the negligent conduct (Hart et al., 1997). Another proposed explanation for this positive relationship is the hindsight bias (Hastie, Schkade & Payne, 1999), where knowledge of more severe injuries induces a retrospective analysis that the defendant’s conduct must have been similarly severe. Although few studies have experimentally manipulated the severity of the plaintiff’s psychological injury, recent research indicates that this evidence similarly impacts liability judgments. For example, Winter and Vallano (2013) provided participants with a sexual harassment case summary and manipulated expert testimony regarding the severity of the complainant’s psychological injuries (minimal, moderate, or severe). Results indicated that more severe psychological injuries increased findings of liability, but only for male participants. Vallano et al. (2013) used similar methodology and manipulated participants’ expectations for psychological injury severity (expecting only mild injury vs. expecting mild or severe injury) in a sexual harassment case, and subsequently altered the actual injury severity adduced by the plaintiff (minimal, moderate, or severe). Results indicated that more severe psychological injury allegations increased findings of liability, but only when the injuries fell within the range of participants’ expectations. For participants expecting mild injury, injury allegations that increased from mild to moderate severity decreased liability judgments. For participants expecting mild to severe injury, injury allegations that increased from mild to moderate severity increased liability judgments. Contrary to legal instructions, the aforementioned studies establish that psychological injury severity affects mock jurors’ liability judgments. Future research should attempt to replicate these findings with different cases (e.g., negligence) in a more ecologically valid context (e.g., trial Psychol. Inj. and Law simulations with deliberating juries). Because this research is in its infancy, future studies should investigate additional mediating and moderating variables (e.g., perceptions of credibility, and whether the defendant’s conduct was a proximate cause of the plaintiff’s injuries). Moreover, future research should examine theoretical frameworks to explain the social–cognitive dynamics underlying jurors’ liability decisions in cases involving psychological injury. Compensatory Damage Determinations How Psychological Injury Evidence Should Affect Compensatory Damage Decisions Unlike liability judgments, psychological injury severity should affect jurors’ compensatory damage decisions, and more specifically, their noneconomic damage awards. After all, the general purpose of compensatory damages is to make plaintiffs whole by returning them to their preinjury state (Greene, Johns & Bowman, 1999). As a result, compensatory damage awards should increase in proportion to the severity of a plaintiff’s injuries. Moreover, psychological injury severity should account for a large amount of variance in juror’s damage awards, for legal instructions instruct jurors to heavily consider injury severity when awarding compensatory damages (Wissler et al., 1997). How Psychological Injury Evidence Actually Affects Compensatory Damage Decisions Despite the widespread concern that compensatory damage awards are unpredictable and excessive (Vidmar & Rice, 1992), plaintiffs often have low success rates at trial and most receive minimal compensatory damage awards (Civil Justice Survey of State Courts, 2001). The concern of excessive damage awards may be due to the belief that jurors consider factors other than the severity of the plaintiff’s injuries. However, research indicates that jurors appropriately consider physical injury severity, which is strongly and positively related to compensatory damage awards (e.g., Greene et al., 2001; Robbennolt, 2000; Wissler et al., 2000). Diamond and Salerno (2013) further suggest that the use of more specific and sensitive measures may indicate that injury severity accounts for even more of the variance in noneconomic damage awards. Specifically, prior research has employed the National Association of Insurance Commissioners’ severity of injury scale, which places injury severity on a 1–9 scale, where 1=emotional injury only (e.g., fright), and 9 = death (Cohen & Hughes, 2007; Wissler, Hart & Saks, 1999). Thus, the development of more sensitive injury severity measures will help researchers more accurately portray the impact of injury severity on legal decisions. Similarly, the perceived severity of the plaintiff’s psychological injuries strongly impacts overall compensatory damage awards (e.g., see generally Diamond & Salerno, 2013; Wissler et al., 1997). Despite manipulating physical injury severity, Wissler et al. (1997) found that mock jurors’ perceptions of mental suffering were the single greatest predictor of pain and suffering awards. The only known research manipulating psychological injury severity also supports this notion, demonstrating that psychological injury severity is strongly and positively related to compensatory damage decisions (Vallano et al. 2013; Winter & Vallano, 2013). In addition, this research further examined the role of perceived complainant credibility, an oft-unstudied variable in this literature. Results indicated that perceived complainant credibility mediated the positive relationship between psychological injury severity and compensatory damages. That is, extreme injuries were perceived as less credible than mild injuries, resulting in a reduction of the overall compensatory damage award. There are many areas for future research regarding the effects of psychological injury on compensatory damage awards. Most importantly, researchers and practitioners would be greatly benefitted by a theoretical model to explain how jurors calculate monetary awards for psychological injuries. That is, how do jurors’ translate psychological injuries into monetary awards, and what factors affect this translation? As researchers have long lamented the complexity of translating psychological injuries into dollar amounts (Hans & Vodino, 2007; Vidmar & Rice, 1992) and the lack of legal guidance to assist jurors when attaching monetary values to psychological injury (Wissler et al., 1999), this issue is receiving increased attention (see Avraham, 2006; Hans & Reyna, 2011). For example, Hans and Reyna (2011) laudably employ a theoretically based cognitive approach to how jurors translate pain and suffering into compensatory damage awards. Specifically, Hans and Reyna propose that jurors first make a “gist” judgment to assess injury severity, and then fit the perceived severity within a numerical range of compensation. Much more research is needed to adequately test the components of this theory, as well as examining possible social and motivational explanations for noneconomic damage awards. For instance, the empathy–altruism hypothesis (Batson et al., 1991) may be applicable to jurors’ noneconomic damage decisions. That is, jurors who experience empathy for the plaintiff’s injuries may provide more compensation than jurors who do not experience empathy. However, jurors’ motivation to help the plaintiff may depend on their ability to help; that is, their perceptions of whether money can return plaintiffs to their pre-injury state or offset the damage done to the plaintiff to some degree. Psychol. Inj. and Law Summary and Conclusions The importance and interest in the study of psychological injury within the legal system is growing, as evidenced by the recent creation of the journal Psychological Injury and Law in 2008. Although voluminous amounts of research have examined the prevalence and impact of psychological disorders, much less research has specifically examined how legal decision makers perceive psychological injury evidence, and how this evidence impacts legal judgments. This legal and empirical review strongly suggests that psychological injury claims are often mistakenly devalued by courts, legal officials, and jurors alike. This underappreciation and asymmetric difficulty to obtain fair recovery for valid psychological injury claims compared to physical injury is most likely due to erroneous perceptions about the causes, consequences, and legitimacy of these injuries. Although legal decision makers may perceive psychological injury claims as lacking objectivity, severity, and credibility, these beliefs have little empirical support, and psychologists increasingly have the means to attribute malingering, feigning, and other response biases by objective tests and other established methods (Kane & Dvoskin, 2011). More research is needed to examine whether these perceptions about the lack of validity of psychological injury claims also exist for jurors. There are many gaps in the literature regarding the perceptions of psychological injury, and the resulting effects on legal decisions. Additional research should further examine jurors’ preconceived notions of psychological injury and work towards establishing a reliable and valid measure to assess jurors’ perceptions of psychological injury severity. This research would help unravel the dimensions that comprise and distinguish mild from severe psychological injury for triers of fact and laypeople, which will inform expert testimony and judicial decision-making. Although recent research suggests that psychological injury evidence affects liability and damage decisions, more research is needed to replicate these findings, especially in more ecologically valid contexts. Future applied studies should examine other factors that affect how jurors perceive psychological injury, whereas future theoretical studies should establish a framework to connect these variables. Continued investigation of these topics will help improve knowledge and understanding of psychological injury in legal officials, triers of fact, and other relevant stakeholders to further increase its legitimacy and importance within the legal community, and the ability of psychologists to correctly diagnose its presence, absence, or malingered presentation in court. References Alba, J. W., & Hasher, L. (1983). Is memory schematic? Psychological Bulletin, 93, 203–231. American Law Institute. (2010). Restatement of the law (third), torts: Liability for physical and emotional harm (Vol. 1). Philadelphia: American Law Institute. American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR (4th ed., text rev.). Washington: Author. Angermeyer, M. C., & Matschinger, H. (2003). Public beliefs about schizophrenia and depression: Similarities and differences. Social Psychiatry Epidemiology, 38, 526–534. doi:10.1007/s00127-0030676-6 Avraham, R. (2006). Putting a price on pain and suffering damages: A critique of the current approaches and a preliminary proposal for change. Northwestern University Law Review, 100, 87–120. Batson, C. D., Batson, J. G., Slingsby, J. K., Harrell, K. L., Peekna, H. M., & Todd, R. M. (1991). Empathic joy and the empathy–altruism hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 413–426. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.413 Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation. Beery, B. T. (2004). My doctor made me crazy: Can a medical malpractice plaintiff allege psychological damages without making credibility the issue? Thomas M. Cooley Law Review, 27, 321– 351. Blumberg, J. N. (2009). Pleading panic: Pure emotional damages as “sickness or disease” for bodily injury claims. Missouri Law Review, 76, 867–894. Bornstein, B. H. (1998). From compassion to compensation: The effect of injury severity on mock jurors’ liability judgments. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1477–1502. Bornstein, B. H., & Schwartz, S. L. (2009). Injured body, injured mind: Dealing with damages for psychological harm. The Jury Expert, March 2009. Borum, R., Otto, R., & Golding, S. (1993). Improving clinical judgment and decision-making in forensic evaluation. The Journal of Psychiatry and Law, 21, 35–76. Bright, D. A., & Goodman-Delahunty, J. (2010). Mock juror decision making in a civil negligence trial: The impact of gruesome evidence, injury severity, and information processing route. Psychiatry, Psychology, and Law, 1, 1–21. Butcher, J. N., Graham, J. R., Ben-Porath, Y. S., Tellegen, A., Dahlstrom, W. G., & Kaemmer, G. (2001). Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2: Manual for administration and scoring (2nd ed.). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Cass, S. A., Levett, L. M., & Kovera, M. B. (2009). The effects of harassment severity and organizational behavior on damage awards. Behavioral Sciences and the Law. doi:10.1002/bsl.886 Cather, C., Greene, E., & Durham, R. (1996). Plaintiff injury and defendant reprehensibility: Implications for compensatory and punitive damage awards. Law and Human Behavior, 20, 189– 206. Civil Justice Survey of State Courts. (2001). United States Department of Justice. Office of Justice Programs. Ann Arbor: Bureau of Justice Statistics, 10.3886/ICPSR03957.v3. Civil Rights Act of 1991. Pub. L. No. 102–106, U.S.C. § 1981. Cohen, T. H., & Hughes, K. A. (2007). Medical malpractice claims in seven states, 2000–2004. Washington: Bureau of Justice Statistics, US Department of Justice. Crumlish, N., & Kelly, B. D. (2009). How psychiatrists think. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment, 15, 72–79. Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, 509 U.S. 579 (1993). DePianto, D. (2012). The hedonic impact of “stand-alone” emotional harms: An analysis of survey data. Law and Psychology Review, 36, 115–145. Diamond, S. S., & Salerno, J. M. (2013). Empirical analysis of juries in tort cases. Research Handbook on the Economics of Torts, in press. Psychol. Inj. and Law Dillon v. Legg, 68 Cal 2d. 728 (1968). Duckworth, M. P., & Iezzi, T. (2010). Physical injuries, pain, and psychological trauma: Pathways to disability. Psychological Injury and Law, 3, 241–253. doi:10.1007/s12207-010-9086-8 Eggen, J. M., & Laury, E. J. (2011). Toward a neuroscience model of tort law: How functional neuroimaging will transform tort doctrine. Columbia Science and Technology Law Review, 1, 1–92. Eisenberg, D., Downs, M. F., Golberstein, E., & Zivin, K. (2009). Stigma and mental health seeking among college students. Medical Care Research and Review, 1, 1–20. doi:10.1177/1077558709335173 Engel, D. M. (2010). Lumping as default in tort cases: The cultural interpretation of injury. Loyola of Los Angeles Law Review, 44, 33–68. Eskin v. Bartee, 262 S.W.3d 727 (2008). Feigenson, N., Park, J., & Salovey, P. (1997). Effect of blameworthiness and outcome severity on attributions of responsibility and damage awards in comparative negligence cases. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 597–616. Feigenson, N., Park, J., & Salovey, P. (2001). The role of emotions in comparative negligence judgments. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31, 576–603. Finch, M. (2005). Law and the problem of pain. University of Cincinnati Law Review, 74, 285–327. Finch, M., Guthrie, C., & Henderson, C. (2008). Expert testimony on psychological injury: Procedural and evidentiary issues. Psychological Injury and Law, 1, 20–35. Foote, W. E., & Goodman-Delahunty, J. G. (2005). Evaluating sexual harassment: Psychological, social, and legal considerations in forensic examinations. Washington: American Psychological Association. Frederick, R. I. (1997). Validity indicator profile manual. Minnetonka: NCS Assessments. Goodman-Delahunty, J., & Foote, W. E. (2009). Forensic evaluations enhance scientific theory: Assessing causation of harm. Pragmatic Case Studies in Psychotherapy, 5, 38–52. Goodman-Delahunty, J., Hartwig, M., Granhag, P. A., & Loftus, E. F. (2010). Insightful or wishful: Lawyers’ ability to predict case outcomes. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 16, 133–157. Graham, J. R. (2000). MMPI-2: Assessing personality and psychopathology (3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Gray, O. S. (2009). Commentary. Wake Forest Law Review, 3, 1193–1195. Greenberg, S. A., & Wheeler, J. G. (2004). Forensic psychological examinations in personal injury cases: Empirical haves and have nots. Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 4, 80–95. doi:10.1300/J158v04n01_06 Greene, E., & Bornstein, B. H. (2003). Determining damages: The psychology of jury awards. Washington: American Psychological Association. Greene, E., Downey, C., & Goodman-Delahunty, J. (1999). Juror decisions about damages in employment discrimination cases. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 17, 107–121. doi:10.1002/ (SICI)1099-0798(199901/03) Greene, E., Johns, M., & Bowman, J. (1999). The effects of injury severity on jury negligence decisions. Law and Human Behavior, 23, 675–693. Greene, E., Johns, M., & Smith, A. (2001). The effects of defendant conduct on jury damage awards. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 228–237. Groth-Marnat, G. (2003). Handbook of psychological assessment (4th ed.). Hoboken: Wiley. Hans, V. P., & Reyna, V. F. (2011). To dollars from sense: Qualitative to quantitative translation in jury damage awards. Legal Studies Research Paper Series. Ithaca: Cornell Law School. Hans, V. P., & Vodino, N. (2007). After the crash: Citizens’ perceptions of connective-tissue injury lawsuits. Cornell Law Faculty Publications. http://scholarship.law.cornell.edu/lsrp_papers/84 Harris v. Forklift Systems, Inc., 510 U.S. 17 (1993). Hart, A. J., Evans, D. L., Wissler, R. L., Feehan, J. W., & Saks, M. J. (1997). Injuries, prior beliefs, and damage awards. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 15, 63–82. Hart, A. J., Wissler, R. L., & Saks, M. J. (1997). Multidimensional perceptions of illness and injury. Current Research in Social Psychology, 2, 14–22. Haslam, N., & Giosan, C. (2002). The lay concept of “mental disorder” among American undergraduates. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 479–485. Hastie, R., Schkade, D. A., & Payne, J. W. (1999). Juror judgments in civil cases: Hindsight effects on judgments of liability for punitive damages. Law and Human Behavior, 23, 445–470. Hebert v. Webre, 982 S 2d. 770 (2008). Huffaker, M. L. (2001). Recovery for infliction of emotional distress: A comment on the mental anguish accompanying such a claim in Alabama. Alabama Law Review, 52, 1003–1027. Hyatt v. Trans World Airlines, Inc., 943 S.W.2d 292 (Mo.App. E.D.1997). Hyman, D. A., Black, B., Silver, C., & Sage, W. M. (2009). Estimating the effect of damages caps in medical malpractice cases: Evidence from Texas. Journal of Legal Analysis, 1, 355–409. Johnson v. Methodist Hospital, No. 01-05-01068-CV (2006) Jorm, A. F., Christensen, M., & Griffiths, K. M. (2005). Public beliefs about causes and risk factors for mental disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 40, 764–767. doi:10.1007/s00127-005-0940-z Kane, A. W., & Dvoskin, J. A. (2011). Evaluation for personal injury claims. New York: Oxford University Press. Keeton, P., & Prosser, W. L. (1984). Prosser and Keeton on the law of torts, 5th edn. St. Paul: West Publishing Kessler, R. C., Bergland, P., Demlar, O., Jin, R., Koretz, D., Merikangas, K. R., Rush, A. J., Walters, E. E., & Wang, P. S. (2003). The epidemiology of major depressive disorder. Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Journal of the American Medical Association, 289, 3095–3105. King, M., Dinos, S., Shaw, J., Watson, R., Stevens, S., Passatti, F., Weich, S., & Serfaty, M. (2007). The stigma scale: Development of a standardized measure of the stigma of mental illness. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 190, 248–254. Kircher, J. J. (2007). The four faces of tort law: Liability for emotional harm. Marquette Law Review, 90, 789–920. Koch, W.J., O’Neil, M., & Douglas, K.S. (2005). Empirical limits for the forensic assessment of PTSD litigants. Law and Human Behavior, 29, 121–149. Kovera, M. B., & Cass, S. A. (2002). Compelled mental health examinations, liability decisions, and damage awards in sexual harassment cases. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 8, 96–114. doi:10.1037//1076-8971.8.1.96 Lauber, C., Nordt, C., Falcato, L., & Wulf, R. (2003). Do people recognize mental illness? Factors influencing mental health literacy. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 253, 248–251. doi:10.1007/s00406-003-0439-0 Lauber, C., Nordt, C., & Wulf, R. (2005). Lay beliefs about treatment for people with mental illness and their implications for antistigma scales. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 50, 745–752. Leonard v. John Crane, 142 Cal. Rptr. 3d 700 (2012). Levy, M. I., & Rosenberg, S. E. (2003). The eggshell plaintiff revisited: Causation of mental damages in civil litigation. Mental and Physical Law Disability Law Reporter, 27, 183–185. Loftus, E. F., & Wagenaar, W. A. (1988). Lawyers’ predictions of success. Jurimetrics Journal, 28, 437–453. Lopez v. Geico Insurance Co., Dist. Ct. NM (2012). Merriam-Webster’s collegiate dictionary, 11th edn. (2012). Springfield: Merriam-Webster. Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Kokaua, J., Milne, B. J., Polanczyk, G., & Poulton, R. (2010). How common are common mental disorders? Evidence that lifetime prevalence rates are doubled by Psychol. Inj. and Law prospective versus retrospective ascertainment. Psychological Medicine, 40, 899–909. doi:10.1017/S0033291709991036 O’Donohue, W., & Bowers, A. H. (2006). Pathways to false allegations of sexual harassment. Journal of Investigative Psychology and Offender Profiling, 3, 47–74. doi:10.1002/jip.43 O’Donohue, W., & O’Hare, E. (1997). The credibility of sexual abuse allegations: Child sexual abuse, adult rape, and sexual harassment. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 19, 273–279. People v. House, 61 AD3d 700 (2009). Popovich, P. M., Jolton, J. A., Mastrangelo, P. M., & Everton, W. J. (1995). Sexual harassment scripts: A means to understanding a phenomenon. Sex Roles, 32, 315–334. doi:10.1007/BF01544600 Poser, S., Bornstein, B. H., & McGorty, E. K. (2003). Measuring damages for lost enjoyment of life: The view from the bench and the jury box. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 53–68. Prins, M. A., Verhaak, P. F. M., Bensing, J. M., & van der Meer, K. (2008). Health beliefs and perceived need for mental health care of anxiety and depression: The patients’ perspective explored. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1038–1058. Ransohoff, D. F., & Feinstein, A. R. (1978). Problems of spectrum and bias in evaluating the efficacy of diagnostic tests. New England Journal of Medicine, 299, 926–930. doi:10.1056/NEJM197810262991705 Redding, R. E., & Murrie, D. C. (2010). Judicial decision-making about forensic mental health evidence. Special Topics in Forensic Practice, 26, 683–707. Rhee, R. J. (2004). A principled solution for negligent infliction of emotional distress claims. Arizona State Law Journal, 36, 805–884. Robbennolt, J. K. (2000). Outcome severity and judgments of “responsibility”: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 2575–2609. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02451.x Rogers, R., Bagby, R. M., & Dickens, S. E. (1992). Structured Interview of reported symptoms. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources. Schneider, K. T., Swan, S., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (1997). Job-related and psychological effects of sexual harassment in the workplace: Empirical evidence from two organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 401–415. Schomerus, G., & Angermeyer, M. C. (2008). Stigma and its impact on help-seeking in mental disorders: What do we know? Epidemiologia e Psichiatria Sociale, 17, 31–37. Shaver, K. G. (1970). Defensive attribution: Effects on severity and relevance on the responsibility assigned for an accident. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 14, 101–113. doi: 10.1037/ h0028777 Slick, D. J., Hopp, G., Strauss, E., & Thompson, G. B. (1997). Victoria symptom validity test: Professional manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources. Smith, D. (2007). The disordered and discredited plaintiff: Psychiatric evidence in civil litigation. Cardozo Law Review, 31, 750–822. Spielberger, C. D. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y). Palo Alto: Mind Garden. Tombaugh, T. N. (1996). TOMM: The Test of Memory Malingering Manual. North Tonawanda: Multi-Health Systems. Vallano, J. P., Winter, R. J., & Charman, S. D. (2013). Is this injury reasonable? Do psychological injury expectations affect mock jurors’ legal decisions in a sexual harassment case? Psychiatry, Psychology, & Law. doi:10.1080/13218719.2012.744626 Vidmar, N., & Rice, J. J. (1992). Assessments of noneconomic damage awards in medical negligence: A comparison of jurors with legal professionals. Iowa Law Review, 78, 883–911. Vincent, J. P., Lemond, S. A., & Inman, T. (2008). Evaluating claims of emotional damages in civil litigation: Not all mental health plaintiffs are created equal. Houston: Houston Lawyer. Wal-Mart Stores, Inc. v. Bowers, 752 So. 2d 1201 (1999). Ware v. ANW, 180 P.3d 610 (2008). Winter, R.J., & Vallano, J. P. (2013). The effect of psychological injury on sexual harassment determinations. Psychological Injury and Law, 5, 208–220. Wissler, R. L., Evans, D. L., Hart, A. J., Morry, M. M., & Saks, M. J. (1997). Explaining “pain and suffering” awards: The role of injury characteristics and fault attributions. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 181–207. Wissler, R. L., Hart, A. J., & Saks, M. J. (1999). Decision making About general damages: A comparison of jurors, judges, and lawyers. Michigan Law Review, 98, 751–826. Wissler, R. L., Kuehn, P. F., & Saks, M. J. (2000). Instructing jurors on general damagesin personal injury cases: Problems and responsibilities. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 6, 712–742. Woody, W. D. (2008). The influence of liability information, severity of injury, and attitudes towards vengeance on damage awards. Psychological Reports, 102, 239–251. Young, G. (2008). Psychological injury and law: Defining a field. Psychological Injury and Law, 1, 78–93. doi:10.1007/s12207008-9008-1 Zavos, H. (2009). Monetary damages for nonmonetary losses: An integrated answer to the problem of the meaning, function, and calculation of noneconomic damages. Loyola of Los Angeles Law Review, 43, 193–272.