Spatial Workspace Symmetry Affects Generalization of Sensorimotor Learning Jens Tiggelbeck & Jochen Müsseler Institute of Psychology Department of Work and Cognitive Psychology RWTH Aachen University, Germany This Poster Rotated visual trajectories Introduction Introduction: Sensorimotor adaptation and its subsequent generalization is often conceptualized as formation and updating of internal models aimed at compensating for the perceived incongruence between proximal and distal action effects (Wolpert, et al., 1995). Recent findings by Wang and Müsseler (2012) suggest an influence of action space symmetry as a determining factor: parallelism seems to promote parallel while symmetry results in equally symmetric intramanual transfer. Research Questions: Do the symmetry’s reference frame (egocentric vs. allocentric) and associated congruence between proximal (motor) and distal (visual) action spaces influence adaptation to and transfer of rotational visuomotor transformations? Department Website Adapted motor trajectories Target CCW (-30°) CW (+30°) Left starting position Right starting position Fig. 2: General experimental setup and apparatus for all reported experiments. Fig. 1: Action space including visuomotor rotation and required motor adaptation. General method: Participants had to slide a cursor from one of two starting positions (balanced across subjects) towards a target (cf. Fig. 1) by performing short targeted flicking motions (open loop) via a digitizer tablet and stylus. Each participant had to adapt to one of two possible visuomotor transformations: cursor trajectories were either visually rotated by 30° clockwise (CW) or counterclockwise (CCW). Intramanual transfer in the form of aftereffects was assessed at the untrained starting position without visual feedback. Results: Generalization of adapFig. 3: Congruent proximal (bottom: tation at the untrained starting locadigitizer tablet) and distal (top: monitor) tion follows a symmetric pattern action spaces resulting in an overlap of (significant interaction between egocentric and allocentric symmetry. starting position and transformation). However, overall reduced generalization is observed when outward compensation is required, i.e. away from the egocentric midline. Ex. 2: Allocentric Symmetry After symmetric transfer could be established under egocentric symmetry conditions, visual and motor spaces are rotated 90° clockwise. Thus, symmetry within the allocentric but not the egocentric reference frame is retained. While adaptation should still be achieved, diminished or even parallel generalization is expected to occur. 25 20 Angular Deviation in Degree Visual and motor action spaces are in alignment concerning allocentric and egocentric symmetry. Based on the results obtained by Wang and Müsseler (2012) symmetrical transfer is expected, i.e. learned outward and inward compensations should be mirrored at the action space’s midline during transfer trials. a Left x CW (outward compensation required = +°) Right x CCW (outward compensation required = +°) Left x CCW (inward compensation required = -°) Right x CW (inward compensation required = -°) b CW CCW Left x CW Right x CCW Left x CCW Right x CW 15 10 5 0 -5 -10 -15 -20 -25 -5 -1 6 12 18 24 30 36 42 48 54 60 Timeline / Baseline (negative numbers) and Experimental (positive numbers) Blocks Left Right Starting Position in Adaptation Blocks Fig. 4a: Adaptation and generalization performance across the entire experimental session. Each data point represents the mean of five consecutive trials. Continuous lines refer to adaptation blocks with visual feedback. Dotted lines denote transfer blocks without visual feedback. Positive angular values represent trajectories aimed away from the triangle’s central axis of symmetry, negative values refer to deviations towards the triangle’s midline. Fig. 4b: Mean angular deviation across all transfer trials in each of the four experimental conditions. Error bars represent averaged intrapersonal standard deviations of aiming errors. The gray dashed line refers to a clockwise (CW), the dashed and dotted line to a counterclockwise (CCW) transformation. a 25 20 Angular Deviation in Degree Ex. 1: Egocentric Symmetry Left x CW (outward compensation required = +°) Right x CCW (outward compensation required = +°) Left x CCW (inward compensation required = -°) Right x CW (inward compensation required = -°) b CW CCW Left x CW Right x CCW Left x CCW Right x CW 15 10 5 0 -5 -10 -15 -20 -25 -5 -1 6 12 18 24 30 36 42 48 54 60 Left Right Results: The data suggests greatly Timeline / Baseline (negative numbers) and Experimental (positive numbers) Blocks Starting Position in Adaptation Blocks reduced but qualitatively still observable symmetrical generalization Fig. 6a: Adaptation and generalization performance across the entire experimental session (see Fig. 4a for further explanations). (interaction only by trend between Fig. 6b: Mean angular deviation across all transfer trials in each of the four experimental conditions (see Fig 4b for further explanations). starting position and transformation). Note that the angular deviations from the unaltered ideal trajectories do not significantly differ from zero in any of the four experimental conditions. Fig. 5: Congruent proximal (bottom: digitizer tablet) and distal (top: monitor) action spaces. Allocentric but not egocentric symmetry is maintained. a Ex. 3: Incongruence Results: Data suggests a lack of transfer (no a significant interaction between starting position and transformation and no aiming errors significantly larger than zero). Angular Deviation in Degree 20 Following the reduced symmetric transfer in the allocentric condition, the influence of an incongruency in orientation between the visual and motor action spaces is assessed. Due to the increased uncertainty of the overall spatial properties of the action space, a lack of systematic generalization is hypothesized. Fig. 7: Incongruent proximal (bottom right: digitizer tablet) and distal (top: monitor) action spaces retain allocentric and egocentric symmetry but do no longer overlap. 25 Left x CW (outward compensation required = +°) Right x CCW (outward compensation required = +°) Left x CCW (inward compensation required = -°) Right x CW (inward compensation required = -°) b CW CCW Left x CW Right x CCW Left x CCW Right x CW 15 10 5 0 -5 -10 -15 -20 -25 -5 -1 6 12 18 24 30 36 42 48 54 Timeline / Baseline (negative numbers) and Experimental (positive numbers) Blocks 60 Left Right Starting Position in Adaptation Blocks Fig. 8a: Adaptation and generalization performance across the entire experimental session (see Fig. 4a for further explanations). Fig. 8b: Mean angular deviation across all transfer trials in each of the four experimental conditions (see Fig. 4b for further explanations). Conclusion Aiming accuracy following an adaptation to a visuomotor rotation of 30° CW or CCW is comparable (no significant differences in means and standard deviations of aiming accuracy) across all three experiments. However, transfer performance is clearly influenced by manipulations of the underlying symmetry’s reference frame as (Ex. 1 vs. Ex. 2). These results are in line with the findings of Wang and Müsseler (2012) and Taylor and Ivry (2013). However, egocentric action space symmetry does not seem to be the only influencing factor. Congruency between action spaces might also play a vital role (Ex. 3). Possible explanations pertain to different neuronal substrates for egocentic and allocentric motor representations (Chen, et al., 2014) and to increased ambiguity of spatial action space properties leading to a conservative choice of the applicable internal model during transfer trials (Wang and Müsseler, 2012). Contact: Jens Tiggelbeck, jens.tiggelbeck@psych.rwth-aachen.de, Institute of Psychology, RWTH Aachen University, Jägerstr. 17-19, D-52066 Aachen, Germany Telephone: +49 (0)241 / 80-96553 http://www.psych.rwth-aachen.de Literature: Chen, Y., Monaco, S., Byrne, P., Yan, X., Henriques, D. Y. P., & Crawford, J. D. (2014). Allocentric versus egocentric representation of remembered reach targets in human cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 34(37), 12515–12526. Taylor, J. A., & Ivry, R. B. (2013). Context-dependent generalization. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 171. Wang, L., & Müsseler, J. (2012). Generalization of visuomotor adaptation depends on the spatial characteristic of visual workspace. Experimental Brain Research, 223(3), 353–365. Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269(5232), 1880–1882. This research was funded by a grant from the DFG (Mu 1298/9)