Journal of Comparative Physiology A J Comp Physiol A (1989) 164:787-796 Sensory, .~.-. ~.~,%" and 9 Springer-Verlag 1989 U V vision: a bird's eye view of feathers* Dietrich Burkhardt Institut ffir Zoologic, Universitfit Regensburg, D-8400 Regensburg, Federal Republic of Germany Accepted November 9, 1988 Summary. The spectral reflectance of feathers was measured in the range between 310 and 730 nm by means of a diode array spectrometer. In many feathers an ultraviolet (UV) reflection adds to the reflection in the visible range which causes their coloration as seen by man. The UV reflectance is related to the presence of pigments in feathers and to the arrangement of structures which influence the light reflecting properties. Feathers giving strong UV reflection are called type A, without UV reflection type B, and giving weak to medium UV reflection type A/B. On the assumption that birds are tetrachromates, colour vision in birds and their possible chromaticity diagrams are discussed. If red, green, blue and UV are primary colours, three secondary colours are present in the daylight spectrum: yellow, blue-green, and violet-ultraviolet. Three more secondary hues may originate from mixing spectral lights: purple (red and violet), 'bird's purple' (red and UV), and 'green purple' (green and UV). Some feathers with doublebanded reflectance curves will produce hues which are not present in the daylight spectrum. Introduction Schiemenz (1924) was the first to discover ultraviolet (UV) vision in a vertebrate, the minnow, yet evidence for UV vision in birds was presented much later (Huth and Burkhardt 1972; Wright 1972). Since these first accounts on hummingbirds and pigeons, UV sensitivity records have been collected for more than 30 bird species. 10 of the 16 families involved belonging to the Passeriformes (compare Chen and Goldsmith 1986). However, wavelength discrimination curves including the UV * Dedicated to Johann Schwartzkopff on his 70th birthday are rare (pigeon: Emmerton and Delius 1980; hummingbirds: Goldsmith et al. 1981), most studies focusing on retinal mechanisms. At the receptor level the situation seems to be complex: several types of cones and oil droplets of various colours determine the spectral response. UV vision could be mediated by retinal mechanisms with a peak sensitivity at 400 nm (Graf and Norren 1974; Norren 1975), or a peak sensitivity at 370 nm (Chert et al. 1984). Sensitivity peaks within the range visible to man occur at around 420, 450, 480, 510 and 570-600 nm, depending on the species investigated (see Chen and Goldsmith 1986; Jane and Bowmaker 1988). In training experiments with the Pekin nightingale (or red-billed Leiothrix) we have just established four sensitivity peaks located at 380, 470, 530 and 620 nm, the sensitivity being nearly five times higher in the UV than in the visible range (Burkhardt and Maier 1989). From these results it seems likely that colour vision in birds is at least tetrachromatic, yet nothing is known about the chromaticity diagram including the UV in any bird. Only few papers deal with the biological significance of colour vision including the UV in birds (reviews: Burkhardt 1983, 1988; Bowmaker, in press). UV vision might play a role in homing (Kreithen and Eisner 1978) and migration, in foraging for food (Burkhardt 1982), and in recognition of plumage colours (Burkhardt 1983). In contrast, in insects the significance of the UV in visually guided behaviour has received a great deal of attention (see Daumer 1958; Silberglied 1979; Meyer-Rochow and Eguchi 1983; Schwind 1985; Wehner 1987; Menzel 1987). Since we know that in birds coloration plays an important role in intraand interspecific behaviour, it is surprising that next to nothing is known about the reflectance properties of bird's feathers in the UV range, the 788 available data being restricted to the spectral range visible to man (see Voitkevich 1966; Dorst 1974; Burrt 1986; Durrer 1986). Our first attempt to fill this gap was to photograph some 'bird-relevant' objects in both the UV and visible ranges. This approach is useful in the field (Lutz 1924; Daumer 1958; Eisner et al. 1969; Burkhardt 1982), but yields satisfactory results only if the filters applied can be matched to the absorption properties of the visual system. Therefore, a better approach is to record the spectral reflectance of an object under question with a photometer. The goal of the present study was to take advantage of recent developments in photometers (Gerlinger and Schlemmer 1988) which have made such measurements rapid and simple. I present here spectral reflection data including data from the UV range for a variety of feathers. Material and methods Single feathers of various birds, found in the field or in aviaries, were collected. Some feathers were taken from birds killed by accidents, some from mounted specimens in the department's collection. Measuring was done without previous cleaning, although sometimes the barbs or barbules had to be rearranged. Reflectance was recorded with a single-beam Zeiss MCS 230 photometer unit in 1 nm steps and a resolution of 2.5 nm. The light source was a Zeiss CLX 111 Xenon unit, and the reference of 100% reflectance was a MS 20 ceramics white standard. The feathers were placed on black velvet in a Zeiss G K 111 goniometer; the angle of light incidence was 45 ~ and the reflectance was measured at an angle of 0 ~ or 25 ~ The measuring light spot had a diameter of about 2 ram. A pair of UV-transparent achromatic lenses and light guides connected the goniometer with the photometer and its light source; the convergence and divergence angles of the light beams were 25 ~. For reflectance bands due to interference this arrangement might slightly increase the half-width of reflectance bands and cause a minor shift of the peak wavelength. The recorded reflectance values were probably below the actual reflectance values of the feathers. Two effects contribute to this. (1) The reflectance values decrease if a feather is not placed exactly in the focal plane. (2) If the barbs and barbules do not cover the measuring spot entirely, part of the incident light is absorbed by the black velvet background. However, neither effect distorts the shape of the spectral reflectance curves or their maxima, minima and slopes. Some preliminary measurements of U V reflectance were obtained by means of photography, using a calibrated grey scale placed next to the object Schott filters (3 mm U G 1 plus 2 m m BG 38) were used as a U V filter. The transmission peak of this filter combination was 65% at 365 nm; transmission below 320 nm and above 400 nm was less than 1%o. Data obtained with this technique are marked with ~ in the Tables. The examined feather samples are classified according to their reflectance properties: type A, reflectance in both the visible and the U V range; type B, reflectance in the visible range only; type A/B, reflectance in the visible range combined with weak to medium reflectance in the U V range. This classification might be helpful at present, yet it should be emphasized that all intermediate types of U V reflection may occur, and that D. Burkhardt: UV-vision: a bird's eye view of feathers there is variation between various spots of the same feather, between feathers of the same bird, and between comparable feathers of specimens of the same species. Munsell notations of the feathers were taken by comparing feathers with samples in the Munsell book of color (1976) in diffuse daylight. Results Black, white and grey feathers All black feathers investigated as yet, whether velvety or shiny, have shown no detectable reflectance, either throughout the spectral range visible to man or in the UV (Table 1). Two examples of reflectance types found in white feathers are given in Fig. I. White of the type A is represented by a feather of the snowy owl: the reflectance is high both in the visible range and in the UV. In type A/ B white, represented by the reflectance of a white pigeon feather in Fig. 1, reflectance is high in the visible range but declines below 400 nm. Samples of type A white and A/B white feathers are listed in Table 1. So far, no type B white has been found. Reflectance curves of grey feathers are lower than those of white feathers, but otherwise similar: either the reflectance curve is flat throughout the visible and the UV (type A grey) or there is a drop below 400 nm (type A/B grey). An example of the type A grey is the grey parrot, while belly feathers of the robin are of type A/B grey. Red, chestnut and brownfeathers In red feathers type A as well as type B reflectance was found. For example, type A red is present in (~ 60 R 45 .,, ,...,.. ............................................................................. 30 15 I 0 3O0 400 I i I i 500 ~ 600 i I (nm) 700 Fig. 1. Spectral reflectances of white feathers. Continuous curve: wing secondary of a snowy owl, Nyctea scandiaca, type white A, high U V reflectance. Dotted curve: wing primary of a pigeon, Colurnba livia, type A/B white, low U V reflectance. Abscissa: wavelength in nm; ordinate: reflectance (R) in per cent as related to the white standard. The same conventions arc used in Figs. 2-10 throughout 789 D. Burkhardt: UV-vision: a bird's eye view of feathers Table 1. Black, white and grey feathers Colour Origin of feather Species Remarks Munsell notation (hue/vahie/chroma) Black, no UV Raven, wing primary Rook, wing primary Magpie, wing primary Blackbird, breast, juvenile Great hornbill, wing primary Snowy owl, breast Bullfinch, belly White wagtail, belly Herring gull, breast Naked-throated bellbird Grey Parrot, undertail coverts Egyptian vulture, wing Pigeon, wing primary Griffon vulture, wing primary Heron, breast Mute swan, wing primary Great hornbill, wing secondaries Herring gull, wing Wheatear, back Nuthatch, back, wing Grey parrot Robin, belly Pigeon, wing primary Corvus corax Shiny" N/l/- Corvus frugilus Shiny a N/1 / -- Pica pica Shiny, phot Turdus merula Velvety, phot Buceros bicornis Shiny, phot Nyctea scandiaca Velvety Pyrrhula pyrrhula Shiny, phot Motacilla alba Shiny, phot Larus argentatus Shiny, phot Procnias nudicollis Psittaeus erithacus Phot Velvety, phot Neophron perenopterus Phot Columba livia Shiny b Gyps vulvus Shiny Ardea cmerea Velvety Cygnus olor Shiny Buceros bicornis Shiny, phot Larus argentatus Phot Oenanthe oenanthe Phot Sitta europaea Phot Psittacus erithacus Erithacus rubecula Phot Columba Iivia Shiny, phot u White A, high UV White A/B, low UV Grey A, high UV Grey A/B, low UV N/9.5/- N/9.5/- N/9.5/- N/4.25/-- " u v vision demonstrated for the genus b UV vision demonstrated for the species the scarlet ibis (Fig. 2); these feathers have a towpass filter p r o p e r t y in the visible range, the reflectance rising steeply above 580 nm, and there is an additional reflectance b a n d in the U V at a r o u n d 350 nm. These feathers are listed in Table 2. Type B red is represented by red tail feathers o f the great spotted w o o d p e c k e r (Fig. 2): While the rise o f reflectance a r o u n d 600 n m is similar to the rise in the type A red, the s e c o n d a r y reflectance p e a k in the U V is missing. Quite different is the type B reflectance curve o f chestnut-cotoured feathers, such as in the taiI o f the black redstart (Fig. 2). T h e r e is n o reflectance t h r o u g h o u t the U V and the short wavelengths in the visible range. A b o v e 500 n m the reflectance rises, first gradually, and then, in the region above 550 nm, nearly linearly. Chestnut feathers are listed together with type A red and type B red in Table 2. M e d i u m and d a r k b r o w n feathers exhibit reflection curves similar to the chestnut feathers, yet in general the reflectance is lower. A p a r t f r o m a 790 D. Burkhardt: UV-vision: a bird's eye view of feathers 25 50 (%) E%] 40 - 20 R R ~ " 30 15 20 10 10 0 I 300 I 400 I I I I I I 500 ), 600 [nrn] 700 0 I 300 Fig. 2. Spectral reflectances of red feathers. Upper, continuous curve: wing primary of the scarlet ibis Edocimus ruber, type A red, high UV reflectance, peaking at 370 nm. Middle, dotted curve : tail feather of the great spotted woodpecker Dendrocopus major, type B red, nearly no UV reflectance. Lower, solid curve: tail feather of a female black redstart, Phoenicurus rubecula, chestnut, type B, no UV reflectance rise of reflectance above 600 nm, the reflectance of whitish- and greyish-brown feathers resembles that of grey feathers of type A or A/B. An example of greyish-brown of type A is given in Fig. 3. Yellow feathers Most of the yellow feathers investigated have a reflectance curve similar to type A red feathers, namely a reflectance band in the UV below I I 400 I I I I I 500 h 600 ~m) 700 Fig. 3. Greyish-brown wing primary of a goshawk Accipiter gentilis. Type A greyish-brown, UV reflectance high 350 nm, almost no reflectance in the short range of visible wavelengths, and a sharp rise of reflectance to a high level throughout the long wavelength range. The rise occurs between 450 and 550 nm, thus causing hues ranging from greenishyellow, through vivid yellow, to orange. Representative samples are yellow parts of feathers of a budgerigar and a white-fronted amazone (Figs. 4, 9). This type A yellow in the Psitticaformes resembles that of Passeriformes, yellow feathers of which may have a considerable UV reflectance as was shown mostly by filter photography. Obviously, these passeriform feathers are also type A yellow. Yellow-orange feathers of the red-billed Leiothrix Table 2, Red, chestnut and brown feathers Colour Origin of feather Species Red A, high UV Scarlet ibis, wing primary Greater flamingo, wing primary Grey parrot, red tail feather Chestnut-fronted macaw, forehead White-fronted amazone, wing secondary Bullfinch, male, breast Scarlet ara Great spotted woodpecker, tail Robin, throat Black redstart Pheasant, tail Goldfinch, back Ruddy shelduck Edocimus ruber Red B, no UV Chestnut, no UV Brown (similar to chestnut), no UV a UV vision demonstrated for the genus Remarks MunselI notation (hue/value/chroma) 7.5 R/5/6 Phoenicopterus ruber Phot Psittacus erithacus Phot 7.5 R/5/10 Ara severa Amazona albifrons Pyrrhula pyrrhula 7.5 R/8.5/12 Phot Ara macao Dendrocopusmajor 8.75 R/5/12 8.75 R/4/M Erythacus rubecula 7.5 YR/5/8 Phoenicurus ochruros Phasianus ochruros 7.5 YR/5/6 7.5 YR/5/6 Carduelis carduelis Phot a Casarcaferruginea Shiny 7.5 YR/5/6 D. Burkhardt: UV-vision: a bird's eye view of feathers 791 25 5O (%) 20 R 15 10 5 - 0 300 I 400 I I 500 I )~ I 600 I i [nm] 700 0 I 300 Fig. 4. Spectral reflectance of a yellowish-white feather of a budgerigar Melopsittacus undulatus (dotted line), type A yellow, UV reflectance high. While the yellow feather reflects the long wavelength range, for comparison the second (continuous) curve gives the reflectance of a blue feather of the same bird with a reflectance band in the short wavelength range I I 400 I I I I I 500 ~ 600 (am) 700 Fig. 5. Spectral reflectance of the velvety-green part of a feather of a white-fronted amazone Amazona albifrons. Type A green, high UV reflectance Table 3. Yellow feathers Colour Origin of feather Species Yellow A, high UV Rose-ringed parakeet, breast White-fronted amazone, wing secondary Budgerigar Greenfinch, wing primary Waxwing, tail Blue-headed wagtail, breast Great hornbill, neck Psittacula eupatria 10 Y/7/8 Amazona albifrons 5 Y/8_5/~2 Yellow B, no UV Remarks Melopsittacus undulatus Carduelis chloris Phot, mat& Bombycilla garrulus Phot Motacillaflava Phot Buceros bicornis Phot Munsell notation (hue/value/chroma) 2.5 GY/9/4. a UV vision demonstrated for the genus lack a UV reflection, as do some pale yellow and grey-yellow feathers (shown by filter photography in the great hornbill). Thus it seems justified to subdivide yellow feathers into a type A yellow and a type B yellow (Table 3). Greenfeathers To date, four types of reflectance curves have been found in green feathers. Green of the type A was found in some green feathers of parrots, an example being the white-fronted amazone (Fig. 5). The reflectance curve partly resembles that of the type A yellow: there is an UV reflectance band below 350 nm, and the reflectance is low in short wavelengths .of the visible range and rises steeply above 500 rim, peaking at about 540 nm. However, in the region between 550 and 600 nm the reflectance declines. Green of types A/B and B seems to be rare, yet feathers of the greenfinch's back, olive coloured, are lacking reflectance in both the UV and adjacent short visible wavelength ranges. A gradual rise above 450 to 500 nm leads to a flattened peak at around 560 nln and a decline with longer wavelengths. While dull or velvety green feathers (types A and B) have a fairly wide spectral range of reflectance, iridescent green feathers either have only a single and sharp reflectance peak in the green region (type B interference green) or there may be secondary peaks in the UV to violet region (type A interference green). An example of strong additional UV to violet reflection is the iridescent green part of some feathers of the Indian peafowl (Fig. 6), while using filter-photography it was found that the iridescent green feathers of the quetzal (Pharomachrus mocino) and a hummingbird 792 D. Burkhardt: UV-vision: a bird's eye view of feathers 25 75 (%) (%) 20 60 R R 15 45 30 5 .. 9 0 ".-.......... I I 400 300 I 15 "... ............. ..."" ..., ...... I 500 I ~ I 600 I (nm) I 700 Fig. 6. Spectral reflectances of iridescent green parts of feathers of the Indian peafowl Pavo cristatus. In different parts of the plumage iridescent green colours can be found with strong or weak additional reflectance in the short wavelength region. Continuous curve: type A interference green, secondary peaks at 410 and 330 nm. Dotted curve: type B interference green, hardly any reflectance in the U V 0 I 300 I 400 I I 500 I ~ I 600 I I (nm) 700 Fig. 8. Spectral reflectance of a shiny violet feather found in the Malay rainforest, probably from the Asian fairy bluebird Irena puella 50 40 R 50 30 (%) :.'"..'~...i'i~:.,,.:.................................. 4O R 30 10 20 0 .:-! I 300 10 I 0 300 I 400 ] I I I 500 X 600 I I (nm) 700 Fig. 7. Spectral reflectance of the velvety-blue parts of a feather of the jay Garrulus glandarius, type A blue (Lesbia sparganura) have weak or no UV reflectance. Blue and violet feathers An example of a bright blue although velvety feather is found in the jay (Garrulus glandarius). The blue parts of these feathers exhibit a broad reflectance band spanning the UV to green region of the spectrum with the peak at 380 nm (Fig. 7). The reflectance of feathers from the blue-and-yellow macaw (Ara ararauna) is similar, while blue feathers of the budgerigar (Melopsittacus undulatus) show a wider reflectance band with a plateau between 350 and 450 nm, declining above and below these wavelengths (Fig. 4). A shiny blue-violet feather found in the Malay rain forest is probably from an Asian fairy bluebird (Irena puella). With I 400 I I I l 500 k 600 I I (nm) 700 Fig. 9. Spectral reflectances of different parts of a feather of the white-fronted amazone Amazona albifrons. Continuous curve: red part near the base of the feather, type A red. Dotted curve: yellow part, middle of the feather, type A yellow. Densely dotted curve: green part, near the tip, type A green. Note that the reflectance of the green part is the lowest above 620 nm and highest below 400 nm as compared with the other curves its peak around 360 nm, the reflectance shows a steep decline towards 500 nm and then declines more gradually towards the red end of the spectrum (Fig. 8). Obviously, the strongest reflectance is in the UV and the feather of this bird might be called UV-violet rather than blue-violet. From filter photography it was deduced that the violet patches on the budgerigars's head have a reflectance curve similar to that shown for the UV-violet feather of Fig. 8. Multicoloured feathers Many feathers have striking multicoloured patterns. An example is a yellow, green and red spotted feather of a white-fronted amazone (Fig. 9). The red, yellow, and green are all of the respective A types, that is, they have a considerable amount D. B u r k h a r d t : UV-vision : a bird's eye view of feathers 25 20 R 15 10 0 I 300 I 400 I I I I 500 X 600 I I (nm) 700 Fig. 10. Spectral reflectances of the eye of a male Indian peafowl tail feather. C o n t i n u o u s curve : bluish-green ring, type A interference green. D o t t e d curve: blue ring a r o u n d the centre, no secondary peak in the UV, type B interference blue. Densely dotted curve, outer bronze-colouring ring, secondary peaks at 530, 450 a n d 350 n m of UV reflectance. In the visible range, however, the spectral reflectance curves differ significantly between these colours: the red and the yellow feather spots have plateau reflectance curves with cutoff slopes at different wavelengths (red, 600 nm; yellow, 500 nm). The green spots have a similar cutoff slope to the yellow ones but have a stronger reflectance, i.e. a marked peak, between 500 and 550 nm and a weaker reflectance in the long wavelength range compared with both yellow and red spots. In Fig. 10 various parts around the eye of a male Indian peafowl tail feather have been studied. There were between one and three reflectance bands. Depending on colour, peaks occurred from the red part of the spectrum down into the UV region. For example, in the bronze-coloured ring a major peak was found at 630 nm, a second peak at 520 nm, a third at 450 nm, and finally a slight rise below 390 nm. The brilliant blue region had a single reflectance peak at 470 nm, and the green part had reflectance peaks at 550 and 400 nm and a minor band below 330 nm. Finally, the dark violet core showed a weak reflectance band at 390 nm and a slight rise of reflectance below 350 nm. Discussion Origin of plumage colours The spectral reflectances of the samples showed considerable differences in the UV range. A few remarks on the origin of plumage colours may be useful for understanding the possible mechanisms causing different UV reflectances in various feathers. 793 While in feathers of the A type the reflectance between 300 and 400 nm was fairly high, type B and type A/B feathers either showed no reflectance in the UV or a marked decline below 400 nm. It seems reasonable to relate the lack or presence of UV reflectance chiefly to the melanin content of the feathers. Light absorbance by melanin pigments is known to increase progressively with the reciprocal of the wavelength down to 300 nm (Needham 1974). Melanin-caused colorations range from pale yellow and chestnut, to brown and black. In many feathers the melanin content changes towards the base (Dorst 1974). Melanincontaining feathers are not expected to reflect much UV. In contrast to the melanin pigments, some carotenoids do not absorb UV. They absorb strongly in the blue part of the spectrum and have a steep decline of absorbance in the range between 500 and 600 nm (Vetter et al. 1971). Such carotenoids are frequently found in bird feathers (Voitkevich 1966; Durrer 1986), and also in the oil droplets of bird retina (see Leibmann and Granda 1975; Goldsmith et al. 1984; Jane and Bowmaker 1988). The type A feathers of red, yellow and green colour probably contain this kind of carotenoid. Feathers with spongious keratin containing irregularly spaced air-filled cavities will reflect all wavelengths and hence appear white. These feathers would comprise the type A white. The presence of a small amount of melanin in these feathers could provide the basis for the type A/B white. The origin of the blue colour of many feathers could be very similar. As in white feathers, light is scattered by the outer keratin layers. But in contrast to the situation in white feathers, deep layers of melanin will absorb the farther penetrating long wavelength radiation. Thus the stray light will become blue due to the Tyndall effect. Therefore, it is not surprising that in blue and violet feathers an additional reflectance is present in the UV, as the Tyndall effect becomes stronger at shorter wavelengths. (For another model of colour production in blue barbs, due to interference, see Dyck 1971.) Only rarely are green feathers coloured by pigment, in most cases the green colour originating from the combination of a yellow pigment and the structural blue described above. If the yellow pigment is of the carotenoid type a high UV reflectance would not be surprising, consistent with type A green. If the pigments contain melanin, UV will be absorbed, probably resulting in green of types A/B or B, depending on the amount and distribution of melanin. 794 Finally, coloration of feathers may be caused by interference of light on structural layers oriented in parallel at a quarter wavelength's distance. Interference colours may range from violet to red depending on which wavelengths match the structural distances. Single-banded interference colours did occur in the samples as well as colours with side bands, findings which are consistent with the results of Durrer and Villiger (1966). In green feathers of iridescent appearance feathers both with and without additional UV to violet side bands were found and they were classified accordingly as types A or B interference green. Quite independent of which hue is being considered, the colour of a feather as seen by man might be caused either by a comparatively small reflectance band in our visible range [for red see the scarlet ibis (Fig. 2); for green see the Indian peafowl (Fig. 6); and for blue-violet see the Asian fairy bluebird (Fig. 8)] or by a rather wide reflectance band with a depression in a narrow waveband. For example, the yellow feather of the budgerigar (Fig. 4) appears yellow to the human eye as the complementary colour (blue) is absorbed. Likewise, the budgerigar's blue feathers absorb the orange part of the spectrum. The different types of wavelength-dependent reflectances imply that Munsell notations do not give appropriate information about spectral properties of feathers. This is even more obvious when the UV reflectance, which is not seen by man, is taken in account, because in birds, especially those with high UV sensitivity, it will considerably influence the perception of hues. Tetrachromatic vision and plumage colours as they may appear to birds In most birds investigated so far at least four receptor types have been found (see Jane and Bowmaker 1988). This raises the question of how the colour space in birds might be organized. For several reasons it seems unlikely that a chromaticity diagram can be constructed as a quadrangle in a plane with the four receptor-peak regions placed at its corners. Such a configuration would imply that white in the diagram's center could be reached by an unlimited number of complementary wavelengths at opposite locations on the quadrangle's sides. This would also imply that each of two pairs of receptors not adjacent in the spectrum could be stimulated appropriately to cause a perception of white. Any location within the chromaticity diagram could be reached by stimulating three out D. Burkhardt: UV-vision: a bird's eye view of feathers / P/ / /'~. L B \ R V \ G BG Juv .... \i B uv "'" 7.,.", : ", i ~ ! ~176 ""'-.. \ UV B Fig. 11. Schematic drawings of different types of chromaticity diagram. Thick continuous lines: regions of primary colours; broken lines: intermediate spectral colours; dotted lines: colours exclusively evoked by mixtures of two different regions in the spectrum. R, red; Y, yellow; G, green; BG, blue-green; B, blue; UV, ultraviolet; P, purple; UB, intermediate colour between UV and blue; UG, intermediate colour resulting from adding UV and green spectral lights; UR, intermediate colour resulting from adding UV and red light. Upper left: chromaticity diagram of human colour vision. Any point within the colour triangle may be reached by adding the three primaries, as indicated by arrows. Upper and lower right: hypothetical chromaticity quadrangle for tetrachromatic vision including UV as a primary colour. As can be seen by comparison of the diagrams, any point within the chromaticity diagram can be reached by adding different triplets of primaries. Lower left: hypothetical chromaticity tetrahedron. In contrast to the chromaticity quadrangle, not one dotted line is possible, but three intermediate colours can be found which are not included in the spectrum. Any point within the tetrahedron can be reached by adding four primaries of four receptors, that is four different combinations of three receptors (Fig. 11 right). Hence the relation between the wavelength content of a stimulus and the perceived colour would be even more ambiguous than in trichromatic vision. Therefore, I favour a chromaticity diagram constructed in three-dimensional space, similar to that proposed for fish (Neumeyer 1988). The four receptors would be represented by the edges of a tetrahedron. This implies the presence of three regions of intermediate colours in the daylight spectrum (yellow, blue-green - both visible to us -, and a mixture of UV and blue-violet, visible to birds). Furthermore, while in man's chromaticity diagram there is only one intermediate colour which does not occur in the daylight spectrum, namely purple, in tetrachromatic vision there would be three intermediate colours which are not present in the day- D. Burkhardt : UV-vision: a bird's eye view of feathers light's s p e c t r u m , namely, m i x t u r e s o f red a n d blue (purple), o f green a n d U V , a n d o f red a n d U V (Fig. 11). I n spite o f the speculative n a t u r e o f this hypothesis s o m e conclusions a b o u t the a p p e a r a n c e o f colours can be d r a w n f r o m o u r m e a s u r e m e n t s . Different feathers t h a t a p p e a r white to h u m a n s will differ in c o l o u r for a bird with U V vision, depending on their U V reflectance. Thus, a b s o r p t i o n o f U V (types A/B a n d B white) will cause the feather to l o o k c o l o u r e d to the bird, since the U V p a r t o f the daylight s p e c t r u m is filtered out. G r e y feathers a b s o r b i n g U V will h a v e the s a m e hue, as only the three receptors in the r a n g e visible to m a n are stimulated. O n the o t h e r h a n d , while white feathers reflecting the whole s p e c t r u m except the U V were found, no b l a c k feathers a b s o r b i n g all wavelengths except U V were seen. But it should n o t be ruled out t h a t there m i g h t be shiny b l a c k feathers with a reflectance b a n d in the U V , due to interference. So far, in the present samples o f d a r k feathers reflecting in the s h o r t w a v e l e n g t h range, only U V reflectance c o m b i n e d with reflectance in the ( h u m a n ) violet a n d blue r a n g e has been found. Red, orange, yellow, green a n d iridescent green feathers each occur in two types, with a n d w i t h o u t additional U V reflection. Therefore, c o m b i n a t i o n s o f red a n d U V as well as o f green a n d U V occur. These d o u b l e - b a n d reflectances were m e n t i o n e d a b o v e as p r o b a b l y giving rise to hues which are n o t present in the daylight spectrum. A c o m b i n a tion o f red a n d blue reflectance as the third type o f i n t e r m e d i a t e colour was present in a p a r t o f the I n d i a n p e a f o w l feather. Thus, all o f the three s e c o n d a r y c o l o u r types not present in the daylight s p e c t r u m actually do o c c u r in the p l u m a g e o f birds (the red a n d blue c o m b i n a t i o n is also frequently f o u n d in colours o f fruits). It seems extremely unlikely that such feathers should be highly u n s a t u r ated for birds. This m a k e s m e believe t h a t the chromaticity d i a g r a m is o r g a n i z e d as a t e t r a h e d r o n rather t h a n as a q u a n d r a n g l e in a plane. T o investigate the biological significance o f bird colours including the U V a p p e a r s to be a challenge. W e s h o u l d realize t h a t different feathers t h a t l o o k fairly similar to m a n m a y l o o k very different to a bird a c c o r d i n g to w h e a t h e r they are o f the A, A/B, or B type, which are m a r k e d by different U V reflectances n o t visible for m a n . F o r example, snow reflects U V ; a s n o w y owl w o u l d be b a d l y m a t c h e d to s n o w unless its p l u m a g e belonged to the A type o f white (with high U V reflectance), which it actually does. U V p a t t e r n s are invisible to m a m m a l p r e d a t o r s , while they could be conspicu o u s for U V sensitive raptors. Thus, the questions 795 o f inter- a n d intraspecific colour signals, o f c a m o u flage, a n d o f a p o s e m a t i c c o l o r a t i o n should be reinvestigated in this context (see B u r k h a r d t 1988). F u r t h e r m o r e , s o m e birds m i g h t be sexually dim o r p h i c in U V patterns, similar to s o m e butterflies. A p p e a r a n t l y , this r e p o r t raises m a n y m o r e questions t h a n it is able to answer. Acknowledgements. The study was supported by the DFG with funds from the SFB 4, E 6. The Vogelpark Walsrode (D-3030, FRG) kindly supplied feathers and allowed birds to be photographed. The Carl Zeiss Company (D-7082, Oberkochen, FRG) generously gave access to the CMS 230 diode array photometer. E. Finger assisted during measurements and evaluation of data, and B. Kramer improved the English. I am grateful to Ingrid de la Motte for her invaluable help. References Bowmaker JK (in press) Avian colour vision and the environment. In: Proceedings of the 19th International Ornithological Congress, Ottawa Burkhardt D (1982) Birds, berries and UV. A note on some consequences of UV vision in birds. Naturwissenschaffen 69 : 153-157 Burkhardt D (1983) Wavelength perception and colour vision. In: Cosens D J, Vince-Price D (eds) The biology of photoreception. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 371397 (Society for Experimental Biology Symposia 36) Burkhardt D (1988) Die Welt mit anderen Augen. Wie Insekten und V6gel die Welt und ihre Farben sehen. Aus Forschung und Medizin, Schering AG Berlin Burkhardt D, Maier E (1989) The spectral sensitivity of a passerine bird is highest in the UV. Naturwissenschaften 76 : 8283 Bum EH jr (1986) An analysis of physical, and optical aspects of avian coloration with emphasis on wood-warblers. American Ornithologists' Union, Washington DC, pp 1-126 (Ornithological Monographs no 38) Chen D-M, Goldsmith TH (1986) Four spectral classes of cone in the retinas of birds. J Comp Physiol A 159:473-479 Chen D-M, Coltins JS, Goldsmith TH (1984) The ultraviolet receptor of bird retinas. Science 225 : 337-340 Daumer K (1958) Blumenfarben, wie sie die Bienen sehen. Z Vergl Physiol 41:49-110 Dorst J (1974) The life of birds, vol I: The colours of birds. Weidenfeld and Nicolson, London, pp 48-59 Durrer H (1986) Colouration. In: Bereiter-Hahn J, Matoltsy AG, Richards KS (eds) Biology of the integument, vol 2, Chap V, The skin of birds, part 12. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York, pp 23%247 Durrer H, Villiger B (1966) Schillerfarben der Trogoniden. Eine elektronenmikroskopische Untersuchung. J Ornithol 107:1-26 Dyck J (1971) Structure and colour-production of the blue barbs of Agopornis roseicollis and Cotinga maynana. Z Zellforsch 115 : 17-29 Eisner T, Silberglied RE, Aneshansley D, Carrel JE, Howland HC (1969) Ultraviolet video-viewing: the television camera as an insect eye. Science 166:1172-1174 Emmerton J, Delius JD (1980) Wavelength discrimination in the visible and ultraviolet spectrum by pigeons. J Comp Physiol 141 : 47-52 Gerlinger H, Schlemmer H (1988) Simultanspektrometer fiir die Produktkontrolle. GIT Fachz Lab 32:167-170 Goldsmith TH, Collins JS, Perlman DL (1981) A wavelength 796 discrimination function for the hummingbird Archilochus alexandri. J Comp Physiol 143:103-110 Goldsmith TH, Collins JS, Licht S (1984) The cone oil droplets of avian retinas. Vision Res 24:1661-1671 Graf V, Norren DV (1974) A blue sensitive mechanism in the pigeon retina: "~m.x400 rim. Vision Res 14:1203-1209 Huth H-H, Burkhardt D (1972) Der spektrale Sehbereich eines Violettohr-Kolibris. Naturwissenschaften 59:650 Jane SD, Bowmaker JK (1988) Tetrachromatic colour vision in the duck (Anas platyrhynchos L.): microspectrophotometry of visual pigments and oil droplets. J Comp Physiol A 162:225-235 Kreithen ML, Eisner T (1978) Ultraviolet light detection by the homing pigeon. Nature 272:347-348 Leibmann PA, Granda AM (1975) Super dense carotenoid spectra resolved in single cone oil droplets. Nature 253 : 370-372 Lutz FE (1924) Apparantly non-selective characters and combination of characters, including a study of ultraviolet in relation to the flower-visiting habits of insects. Ann NY Acad Sci 29:181-283 Menzel R (1987) Farbensehen blfitenbesuchender Insekten. Internationales Bfiro der Kernforschnngsanlage, Jfilich Meyer-Rochow VB, Eguchi E (1983) Flfigelfarben, wie sic die Falter sehen - a study of UV and other colour patterns in Lepidoptera. Ann Zool Jpn 56:85-99 Note added in proof. Black feathers with a reflectance band in the UV have been found recently in the swallow (Hirundo domestica) and the blue-necked ranger (Tangara cyanicollis) by E. Finger (Diploma thesis, Regensburg, 1989). D. Burkhardt: UV-vision: a bird's eye view of feathers Munselt Book of Color (1976) Munsell Color Corporation, Baltimore Needham AE (1974) The significance of zoochromes. Springer, Berlin Heidelberg New York Neumeyer Ch (1988) Das Farbensehen des Goldfisches. Eine verhaltensphysiologische Analyse. Thieme, Stuttgart Norren DV (1975) Research note. Two short wavelength sensitive cone systems in pigeon, chicken and daw. Vision Res 15:1164-1166 Schiemenz F (1924) Ober den Farbensinn der Fische. Z Vergl Physiol 1:175-220 Schwind R (1985) Sehen unter und fiber Wasser, Sehen von Wasser. Das Sehsystem eines Wasserinsekts. Naturwissenschaften 72: 343-352 Silberglied RE (1979) Communication in the ultraviolet. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 10:373-398 Vetter W, Englert G, Rigassi N, Schwieter U (1971) Spectroscopic methods. In: Isler O (ed) Carotenoids, IV. Birkh/iuser, Basel, pp 189-266 Voitkevich AA (1966) The feathers and plumage of birds. Sidgwick and Jackson, London, pp 20-39 Wehner R (1987) ' Matched filters' - neural models of the external world. J Comp Physiol A 161:511-531 Wright AA (1972) The influence of ultraviolet radiation on the pigeon's colour discrimination. J Exp Anal Behav 17 : 325-337