Prebiotic intake reduces the waking cortisol response and alters emotional bias in healthy volunteers Kristin Schmidt1, Philip Cowen1, Catherine J Harmer1, George Tzortzis2, Philip WJ Burnet1 Background Compelling evidence links the enteric gut microbiota and brain function, which has led to renewed efforts to identify strains that confer a beneficial effect upon the host (probiotics; 1, 2). Rodent studies implicate the microbiome as essential to early-life programming of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis and show reductions in anxiety phenotypes and corticosterone3,4. Brain-area dependent effects5 exerted via multiple mechanisms of action such as the immune system, vagus nerve, and serotonergic pathways hint at the complexity of the microbiome’s downstream effects. In humans, preliminary results support the ability of gut microbiota to affect cortisol levels6,7 and neural correlates of emotional processing8. This has led to increased research on probiotics’ ability to manipulate neural and endocrine targets in stress-related disorders such as irritable bowel disease, depression and anxiety. However, there may be significant scope to improve manipulations through the use of prebiotics, non-digestible dietary oligosaccharides that can support the sustained proliferation of multiple probiotic strains. Our study extends findings to effects of two strains of prebiotics on behavioural processing of emotional information and the HPA axis. Methods & Results Participants Forty-five healthy volunteers (22 males) received one of two prebiotics (fructooligosaccharides [FOS] or Bimuno-galactooligosaccharides [BGOS]) or a placebo at 5.5g per day for 3 weeks. Cortisol The salivary cortisol awakening response (CAR) was sampled before and after prebiotic/placebo administration to assess HPA axis activity. CAR was confirmed by increased levels 15, 30, 45, and 60 minutes after waking (F(2.41, 98.63)=58.61, p<0.001, follow-up comparisons all p<0.001]). Salivary cortisol did not differ significantly between groups at baseline but was significantly lower following B-GOS compared with placebo (day x group x time ANOVA, day x group interaction [F(2,41)=3.61, p<0.05], follow-up effects of group at baseline [F(2,41)=1.08, n.s.] and post-prebiotics [F(2,41)=4.20,p<0.05; Placebo vs. GOS p<0.05, all others n.s.]). Attentional dot-probe Analysis of behavioural data revealed decreased attentional vigilance to negative versus positive information in a dot-probe task after B-GOS compared to placebo (group x emotion x masking condition [F(2,41)=3.14, p=0.05], follow-up valence x group interaction in the masked [F(2,41)=.85, n.s.] and unmasked [F(2,41)=4.29, p<0.05, Placebo vs. GOS p<0.05, all others n.s.] conditions). Cortisol awakening response before and after 21 days of prebiotic supplement ingestion *p<.05 * * * Attentional dot-probe paradigm • 60 negative and 60 positive words were paired with neutral words and presented on a computer screen. • Unmasked word pairs were presented for 500ms, masked word pairs for 17ms after which a mask was displayed for 483ms. • Words / masks were replaced by a probe (either * or **) in the location of one of the preceding stimuli and participants were asked to respond (indicating the number of stars by pressing a key) as quickly and accurately as possible. • Attentional vigilance scores were calculated for each participant by subtracting the reaction time from trials when probes appeared in the same position as the emotional word (congruent trials) from trials when probes appeared in the opposite position to the emotional word (incongruent trials). Attentional vigilance as measured by the dot-probe paradigm after prebiotic administration Conclusions Our findings support a role of gut microbiota in affective information processing and the regulation of the HPA axis. These effects were limited to the BGOS prebiotic and were not found after administration of a FOS prebiotic. The study implicates that: The manipulation of gut microbiota with B-GOS alters HPA axis responsivity in healthy volunteers. This extends findings of HPA axis manipulations with probiotics in humans and rodents to the use of prebiotics (nutrients that aid the growth of intrinsic gut microbiota). B-GOS modifies emotional biases, as it was shown to increase positive versus negative vigilance compared to placebo. Attentional vigilance is key to threat-related information processing and can be modified by anxiolytics9,10. While the precise mechanisms of action of prebiotics are still relatively unknown, they are known to include the immune system, which may explain effects on both the HPA axis (via the adrenal glands) and attentional vigilance (e.g. via the hypothalamus). However, other relevant pathways linking the gut microbiota and brain include the vagus nerve and serotonergic system 11. References 1) Forsythe, P., & Kunze, W.A. (2013). Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences, 70, 55-69. 2) Rhee, S.H., Pothoulakis, C., & Mayer, E.A. (2009). Nat Rev Gastroenterol Hepatol, 6, 306-314. 3) Dinan & Cryan, 2012; Dinan, T. G., & Cryan, J. F. (2012). Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(9), 1369–78. 4) Gareau, M.G., Jury, J., & Perdue, M.H. (2007). Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol., 293, G198-203. 5) Bravo, J. a, Forsythe, P., Chew, M. V, Escaravage, E., Savignac, H. M., Dinan, T. G., Bienenstock, J., et al. (2011). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(38), 16050–5. 6) Rao, A. V., Bested, A. C., Beaulne, T. M., Katzman, M. A., Iorio, C., Berardi, J. M., & Logan, A. C. (2009). Gut pathogens, 1(1), 6. 7) Messaoudi, M., Lalonde, R., Violle, N., Javelot, H., Desor, D., Nejdi, A., Bisson, J.-F., et al. (2011). British journal of nutrition, 105(5), 755–64. 8) Tillisch, K., Labus, J., Kilpatrick, L., Jiang, Z., Stains, J., Ebrat, B., Guyonnet, D., et al. (2013). Gastroenterology, 144(7), 1394–1401. 9) Browning, M., Reid, C., Cowen, P. J., Goodwin, G. M., & Harmer, C. J. (2007). Journal of psychopharmacology, 21(7), 684– 90. 10) Murphy, S. E., Downham, C., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J. (2008). Psychopharmacology, 199(4), 503–13. 11) Clarke, G., Grenham, S., Scully, P., Fitzgerald, P., Moloney, R. D., Shanahan, F., Dinan, T. G., et al. (2013). Molecular psychiatry, 18(6), 666-73. This poster is financially supported by the BBSRC and Clasado Ltd, and an MRC Studentship (KS). We thank Steven Errington for his help with data collection. Copyright © 2014 Kristin Schmidt kristin.schmidt@psych.ox.ac.uk 1Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Warneford Hospital, Neurosciences Building, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK 2Clasado Research Services Ltd, Reading, RG6 6BZ, UK P.1.e.003 Prebiotic intake reduces the waking cortisol response and alters emotional bias in healthy volunteers K. Schmidt 1, P. Cowen 1, C.J. Harmer 1, G. Tzortzis 2, P.W.J. Burnet 1 1University of Oxforda Department of Psychiatrya Oxforda United Kingdom 2Clasado Research Services Ltda Clasado Ltda Readinga United Kingdom Background: There is now compelling evidence for a link between gut microbiota and brain function in both rodents and humans [1]. A recent fMRI study extended these findings to show that ingestion of probiotics astrains of gut bacteria that confer a beneficial effect on their host) can modulate the neural correlates of emotional processing [2], and abnormalities in the processing of emotional information are strongly linked to psychiatric disorders such as anxiety and depression. Further, the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal aHPA) axis, often dysregulated in patients with aand those at risk of) depression and anxiety, impacts affective and memory processing and has strong bidirectional links with the gut microbiome [3]. The present study explored the effects of prebiotics – nutrients that proliferate several intrinsic gut microbiota in the host – on HPA axis activity and emotional processing in healthy volunteers. Methods: Forty-five healthy volunteers a22 males) received one of two prebiotics afructooligosaccharides [FOS] or Bimuno-galactooligosaccharides [B-GOS]) or a placebo for 3 weeks. The salivary cortisol awakening response aCAR) was sampled before and after prebiotic/placebo administration to assess HPA axis activity. On the final day of treatment participants completed a computerised task battery assessing the processing of emotionally salient information. Results: The salivary CAR did not differ significantly between groups at baseline but was significantly lower after B-GOS compared with placebo aday × group × time ANOVA, day × group interaction [Fa2,41)=3.61, p<0.05] followed-up with group × time ANOVAs [main effects of group: baseline: Fa2,41)=1.08, n.s., post: Fa2,41)=4.20, p<0.05; Placebo vs. GOS p<0.05, all others p>0.40]). Analysis of behavioural data revealed decreased attentional vigilance to negative versus positive information in a dot-probe task after B-GOS compared to placebo agroup × emotion × masking condition [Fa2,41)=3.14, p=0.05] follow-up ANOVAs [valence × group interaction: masked: Fa2,41)=0.85, n.s.; unmasked: Fa2,41)=4.29, p<0.05, Placebo vs. GOS p<0.05, all others n.s.]). Conclusions: Our study demonstrates that the manipulation of gut microbiota with B-GOS alters HPA axis responsivity in healthy volunteers, which is in keeping with the effects of probiotics. In rodents, the gut microbiome has an important role in regulating the HPA axis early in life, however, as of yet much less is known from human populations. B-GOS was also shown to increase the processing of positive versus negative attentional vigilance compared to placebo, which supports findings that the modulation of gut microbiota with probiotics has subjective anxiolytic effects since attentional vigilance is believed to play a key role in anxiety and its modulation by anxiolytics. No effects were found after administration of a FOS prebiotic. Overall, novel evidence supports the notion that manipulations of gut microbiota show widespread effects on central functioning and behavioural phenotypes via the HPA axis, as well as the vagus nerve, serotonergic system, and immune system [1]. While the exact mechanisms of action of pre- and probiotics in healthy and clinical populations and their relation to the processing of emotional information are still relatively poorly defined, our study supports their role in affective information processing and the regulation of the HPA axis. 1. Cryan, J.F., Dinan, T.G., 2012. Mind-altering microorganisms: the impact of the gut microbiota on brain and behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13, 701–712. 2. Tilisch, K., Labus, J., Kilpatrick, L., Jiang, Z., Stains, J., Ebrat, B., Guyonnet, D., Legrain-Raspaud, S., Trotin, B., Naliboff, B., Mayer, E.A., 2013. Consumption of fermented milk product with probiotic modulates brain activity. Gastroenterology 144a7), 1394–1401. 3. Dinan, T.G., Cryan, J.F. 2012. Regulation of the stress response by the gut microbiota: implications for psychoneuroendocrinology. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37a9), 1369–1378. Disclosure statement: This poster is financially supported by the BBSRC and Clasado Ltd, and an MRC Studentship aKS). Citation: Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2014;24aSuppl 2):S191 Keywords Psychoneuroendocrinology Anxiety disorders Stress