Impact of White Matter Changes on Clinical Manifestation of Alzheimer’s Disease A Quantitative Study Nobutsugu Hirono, MD; Hajime Kitagaki, MD; Hiroaki Kazui, MD; Mamoru Hashimoto, MD; Etsuro Mori, MD Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 2, 2016 Background and Purpose—There have been conflicting results involving the clinical significance of white matter changes in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD). We studied the association between the volume of white matter hyperintensities (WMHs) on T2-weighted images and cognitive, neurological, and neuropsychiatric symptoms. Methods—The subjects were 76 AD patients who had WMHs but no obvious cerebrovascular diseases. We quantified the volume of WMHs by using fast-fluid–attenuated inversion recovery images and whole brain atrophy by using 3D spoiled gradient-echo images. Effects of WMHs and brain atrophy on dementia severity, cognitive function, neuropsychiatric disturbances, and neurological findings were examined. Results—Whole brain atrophy was significantly associated with dementia severity and cognitive disturbances, as well as with grasp reflex and some kinds of neuropsychiatric disturbances. After we controlled for the effects of brain atrophy, duration of symptoms, and demographic factors, we found that WMH volume was not associated with global cognitive disturbances or dementia severity but was significantly associated with urinary incontinence, grasp reflex, and aberrant motor behaviors. Brain atrophy and WMH volume were not significantly correlated either before or after controlling for age, sex, education, and duration of symptoms. WMH volume was associated with hypertension, but brain atrophy was not positively correlated with any vascular risk factors. Conclusions—Our results support the hypothesis that WMHs in AD patients are superimposed phenomena of vascular origin. WMHs contribute to specific neurological and neuropsychiatric manifestations but not to global cognitive impairment, which is more closely associated with brain atrophy. (Stroke. 2000;31:2182-2188.) Key Words: Alzheimer disease 䡲 atrophy 䡲 neurological deficits 䡲 risk factors 䡲 white matter W hite matter (WM) changes are often found in patients with vascular dementia, especially of small vessel/ subcortical subtypes, including Binswanger’s disease, and are generally considered to be a consequence of chronic ischemia associated with microangiopathy.1,2 However, it is also true that the presence of WM changes is not exclusively linked to vascular dementia, because WM changes on neuroimagings are also common in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD).3–10 Presently, there is a lack of agreement on the effect of WM changes on the development of dementia. Some studies have shown a significant relationship between WM changes and certain cognitive functions and/or dementia severity,11–17 whereas others4,10,18 –32 have failed to find any such relationships in patients with AD. Different study populations and the heterogeneity of WM changes may explain the inconsistency among these studies. Selection of AD patients without vascular risk factors and/or neurological signs may result in excluding severe WM changes and ignoring the clinical significance of WM changes. Small subcortical infarcts frequently accompanied by WM changes,1,2 which do not necessarily exclude the diagnosis of AD by the clinical diagnostic criteria, obscure the independent effects of WM changes by affecting the strategic sites causing dementia.24 In some studies, periventricular changes in the form of caps or smooth halos were included, with irregular confluent changes in the deep and periventricular WM. The former changes are apparently of nonischemic origin, whereas the latter changes represent ischemic tissue damage.33,34 In addition, the neuroimaging technique used is an important factor. MRI is more sensitive to WM changes than is CT.35–38 Although WM changes have been semiquantified with a visual rating scale in most studies, none of these scales have been validated, and the concordance of different scales is insufficient.39 Although MRI quantification of WM changes is desirable, a manual outlining of WM changes12,25 Received May 5, 2000; final revision received June 16, 2000; accepted June 16, 2000. From the Divisions of Clinical Neurosciences (N.H., H. Kazui, M.H., E.M.) and Neuroimaging Research (H. Kitagaki), Hyogo Institute for Aging Brain and Cognitive Disorders, Himeji, Japan. Correspondence to Dr Nobutsugu Hirono, Division of Clinical Neuroscience, Hyogo Institute for Aging Brain and Cognitive Disorders, 520 Saisho-ko, Himeji 670-0981, Japan. E-mail hirono@hiabcd.go.jp © 2000 American Heart Association, Inc. Stroke is available at http://www.strokeaha.org 2182 Hirono et al Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 2, 2016 is essentially dependent on visual inspection that may be affected by an arbitrary gray scale used for display and filming. Computer-based thresholding methods for voxel intensities40,41 are preferable. Finally, the effect of brain atrophy has rarely been considered,16,29 although some studies4,13,22 have analyzed the cerebrospinal fluid space. Because diffuse brain atrophy, which is a main gross pathological feature of AD, is an index of neuronal and synaptic loss, brain atrophy should also be taken into consideration in analyzing the impact of WM changes on cognitive function. In the present study, we examined a purely selected cohort of patients with AD who had WM changes but no obvious cerebrovascular diseases so as to determine the effect of WM changes on cognitive, neurological, and neuropsychiatric symptoms. We quantified the volume of WM hyperintensities (WMHs) and brain atrophy on MRI by means of computerbased techniques. We also tested the hypothesis that WM changes are associated with vascular risk factors but not with brain atrophy. Subjects and Methods The present study was conducted at the Hyogo Institute for Aging Brain and Cognitive Disorders (HI-ABCD), a research-oriented hospital for dementia Subjects All procedures of the present study strictly followed the 1993 Clinical Study Guidelines of the Ethics Committee of HI-ABCD and were approved by the Internal Review Board. After a complete description of all procedures of the present study, written informed consent was obtained from patients or their relatives. On the basis of the following inclusion/exclusion criteria, 76 AD patients were selected from a consecutive series of 391 patients with dementia who were given a short-term admission for examination to the HI-ABCD infirmary between April 1997 and March 1999. All patients were examined by both neurologists and psychiatrists with standardized medical history inquiries, neurological examinations, routine laboratory tests, electroencephalography, magnetic resonance (MR) images of the brain, and MR angiography of the head and neck. The inclusion criteria were those identified by (1) the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition, revised for dementia42; (2) the presence of WM changes, which were defined as irregular periventricular, early confluent deep, or confluent deep WMHs on T2-weighted MRI according to Fazekas and colleagues34,43 and our previous report44; and (3) the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke, Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association, for AD45 when disregarding WM changes. Although 234 patients fulfilled the inclusion criteria, 158 patients were excluded from the present study in accordance with the exclusion criteria. Excluded were patients (1) with medical illnesses possibly causing cognitive impairment or WM lesions, including demyelinating diseases, thyroid diseases, vitamin deficiencies, and malignant diseases with or without antineoplastic agents (n⫽23); (2) with focal brain lesions, including lacunar infarcts and hematoma (n⫽84); (3) with complication of developmental abnormalities, mental diseases, substance abuse, or significant neurological antecedents, such as brain trauma, brain tumor, epilepsy, and inflammatory disease (n⫽21); (4) with evidence of severe intracranial or cervical arterial occlusive lesions on MR angiography (n⫽1); and (5) whose informed consent was not obtained (n⫽29). The subjects consisted of 64 women and 12 men; the mean⫾SD age at examination was 75.6⫾7.1 years, and the mean educational attainment was 8.8⫾2.0 years. The mean duration of symptoms, determined through an interview with the primary caregiver and defined as the time between the first appearance of symptoms of Quantitative Study of WM Hyperintensities 2183 sufficient severity to interfere with social or occupational functioning and the admission,46 was 30.8⫾19.4 months. The functional severity was very mild in 7 patients, mild in 41 patients, moderate in 22 patients, and severe in 6 patients, as determined by the Clinical Dementia Rating Scale (CDR).47 No patient had a history of stroke. Assessment of Vascular Risk Factors and Neurological Disturbances Hypertension, diabetes mellitus, lipid disorder, smoking habit, drinking habit, and cardiac diseases were evaluated as vascular risk factors. Hypertension was judged as present when either a systolic pressure of ⬎160 mm Hg or a diastolic pressure of ⬎95 mm Hg was demonstrated on repeated examinations or when a history of treatment for hypertension was present. Diagnosis of diabetes mellitus was made when the fasting blood glucose level was ⬎7.770 mmol/L (140 mg/dL) or when a history of treatment for diabetes mellitus was present. Lipid disorder was judged as present when laboratory examination of the serum at presentation showed a total cholesterol level of ⬎5.698 mmol/L (220 mg/dL), a triglyceride level of ⬎1.695 mmol/L (150 mg/dL), or an HDL cholesterol level of ⬍1.036 mmol/L (40 mg/dL) or when a history of treatment was present. Smoking habit was defined as ⱖ1 cigarette/d for ⱖ1 year, and drinking habit was defined as ⱖ30 mL ethanol equivalent per day for ⱖ1 year sometime in life. Cardiac diseases were assumed to be present whenever there was a known history or clinical demonstration of any kind of heart disease, including myocardial infarction, angina pectoris, and arrhythmia. A careful neurological examination was given to document the presence or absence of hemiparesis, sensory loss, visual field defects, postural instability (gait disturbance and/or pulsion), pyramidal signs (hyperreflexia, spasticity, and/or extensor plantar responses), extrapyramidal signs (resting tremor, bradykinesia, and/or rigidity), pseudobulbar palsy, ataxia, grasp reflexes, and urinary incontinence. No attempt was made to grade the severity of these risk factors or neurological abnormalities. Assessment of Cognitive Function and Neuropsychiatric Status We assessed the cognitive function of the patients with the MiniMental State Examination,48 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale– Revised,49 and Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale–Cognitive Part.50 The 10-word list recall subtest of the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale was also analyzed separately. The patients’ behavioral changes were assessed semiquantitatively during an interview with the caregiver by using the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI).51 In the NPI, the following 10 behavioral changes in dementia were rated on the basis of the condition of the patients in the previous month before the interview: delusions, hallucinations, depression (dysphoria), anxiety, agitation and aggression, disinhibition, euphoria, irritability and lability, apathy, and aberrant motor activity. According to the criterion-based rating scheme, the severity of each manifestation was classified into 4 grades (from 0 to 3), and the frequency of each manifestation was classified into 5 grades (from 0 to 4). The NPI score (severity⫻frequency) was calculated for each manifestation (range of possible scores 0 to 12). All clinical measures were taken with the investigators blinded to the inclusion of subjects in the present study. MR Acquisition MR was performed on a 1.5-T superconducting magnet (Signa Advantage, General Electric Medical Systems). Axial double-echo fast-spin echo T2-weighted images (3000/105/2 [repetition time/ effective echo time/excitations]), spin-echo T1-weighted images (550/15/2), and fast-fluid–attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) images (9002/147/2200/1 [repetition time/effective echo time/inversion time/excitations]) were obtained for 14 locations parallel to the anteroposterior commissure plane with a section thickness of 5 mm and intersection gap of 2.5 mm covering the area from the base of the cerebellum to the vertex. In all acquisitions, the field of view was 200⫻200 mm, and the matrix size was 256⫻256. All scans were 2184 Stroke September 2000 Measurement of volume of WMH areas. Original images (top) and processed images (bottom). The volume of WMH areas was obtained by automatic count of the number of voxels of values higher than the threshold (shown in blue) within the regions of interest determined by a manually driven mouse cursor (white line). Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 2, 2016 reviewed by one neuroradiologist without knowledge of the patients’ clinical data. Lacunar infarcts were specified as lesions with diameters of ⱕ15 mm with (1) hyperintensity on T2-weighted images, (2) distinct hypointensity on T1-weighted images, and (3) hyperintensity with central hypointensity on FLAIR images. By use of these criteria, lacunar infarcts can be distinguished from the état cribré or punctuate hyperintensity form of WMHs.52 For measurements of whole brain volume (WBV) and total intracranial volume (TIV), we also obtained coronal, 3D, spoiled gradient-echo images (11.1/2/2 [repetition time/effective echo time/excitations]). The field of view was 220⫻220 mm, the matrix size was 256⫻256, contiguous sections were 124⫻1.5 mm, and the flip angle was 20°. The images were generated perpendicular to the anteroposterior commissural plane, which covers the whole calvarium. Measurement of Volume of WMHs We used FLAIR images for quantification of WMH volume. FLAIR is a heavily T2-weighted inversion-recovery technique that nulls fluid, such as cerebroventricular fluid. By use of this technique, subtle periventricular WM lesions can be easily recognized, and their extents can be assessed on a background of cerebrospinal fluid and normal WM. This technique has been reported to be useful for examining WM diseases, such as multiple sclerosis, cerebral infarction, and leukoaraiosis.53,54 The MR data sets of all images were directly transmitted to a personal computer (Power Macintosh 8100/80, Apple) from the MR unit and analyzed by means of the public-domain National Institutes of Health Image version 1.61 program (written by Wayne Rasband and available from the Internet by anonymous ftp from zippy.nimh.nih.gov or on floppy disk from NTIS, 5285 Port Royal Rd, Springfield, VA 22161, part No. PB93-504868) with residential macro programs developed in our institution. To fit a limitation of the software (8-bit voxel value), 12 bits of MR voxel data were converted to 8 bits at a scale factor of 0.5 by the minimum (voxel value 0) and maximum (voxel value 510) levels. Basically, we used a semiautomatic segmentation technique through intensity thresholding, thereby avoiding the observer’s bias. The segmentation thresholding for WMHs was a priori determined to be 3.5 SDs in voxel intensity levels of the normal WM. The outline of WMHs with the surrounding normal WM, gray matter, and cerebroventricular fluid was first traced with a manually driven mouse cursor (Figure). The volume of WMHs was obtained by automatically counting the number of voxels that showed signal intensities higher than the threshold within this outline and then by multiplying the number by the voxel size [(200/256)2⫻7.5⫽ 4.58 mm3]. Because some normal gray matter demonstrated signal intensities higher than the threshold, we carefully excluded these high–signal-intensity gray matter structures as far as possible. Measurements were performed by another investigator blinded to the clinical information. The test-retest reliability for this method was examined with 20 patients, and a high intraclass correlation coefficient was obtained (r⫽0.969). Measurement of WBV and TIV The detailed MRI procedure for obtaining WBV and TIV is described elsewhere.55 In brief, the data sets of all spoiled gradientecho images were directly transmitted to a graphic workstation (INDIGO II HighImpact, Silicon Graphics) from the MRI unit and were analyzed with 3D MRI-analyzing software developed by Yamato et al.56 The software makes use of a combination of a gray-scale algorithm (a 3D expansion of the region-growing method), an edge-detection algorithm (a 3D expansion of Sobel filtering), and some a priori knowledge. With this software, the whole brain was segmented by detecting the boundary between the cerebrospinal fluid and gray matter, and the calvarium was extracted by detecting the outer surface of the dura mater. WBV and TIV were calculated by multiplying the number of voxels in the extracted regions by the voxel size. The caudal end of the whole brain and calvarium was manually set at the plane intersecting the occipitoatloid junction, which is the only supervised operation required. The appropriateness of the extraction of the whole brain and calvarium was assessed by 2 reviewers who were blinded to the clinical data and who examined on-site 3D reconstruction displays of elective view points and 2D slice images of selected sections. The reliability and validity of this method have been established and are described elsewhere.55,57 The measurements were performed by the same neuroradiologist who reviewed the MR images. To adjust for premorbid brain volume variability, WBV was normalized by dividing it by TIV. A smaller normalized WBV (nWBV) indicates a greater brain atrophy. Statistical Analysis We used nonparametric statistics because many variables were not normally distributed. Computation was performed with the SAS program package, release 6.12 (Statistical Analysis Systems). The relationship between vascular risk factors and WMH volume was examined by using partial Spearman rank correlation coefficients, where the effects of age, sex, education, duration of symptoms, and brain atrophy (nWBV) were controlled. Similarly, the relationship between vascular risk factors and brain atrophy (nWBV) were examined while controlling the effects of the confounding variables and WMH volume. The effects of WMH volume and nWBV on Hirono et al TABLE 1. Risk Factors, Neurological Signs, and Scores of Cognitive Tests and NPI Frequency, n (%) Mean⫾SD Score Score 30 (39.5) Diabetes MMSE 19.1⫾4.3 7 (9.2) ADAS total 23.8⫾9.7 Lipid disorder 35 (46.1) ADAS recall 3.5⫾1.3 Smoking 12 (15.8) WAIS VIQ 78.4⫾11.5 Alcohol Cardiac disease 12 (15.8) WAIS PIQ 10 (13.2) 75.7⫾13.5 WAIS FIQ 75.4⫾12.1 Neurological disturbances: NPI scores Pyramidal sign 12 (15.8) Delusions 2.5⫾3.5 Extrapyramidal sign 10 (13.2) Hallucinations 0.2⫾0.9 7 (9.2) Agitation 2.2⫾3.5 19 (25) Grasp reflex Postural instability Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 2, 2016 Dysphoria 1.2⫾2.5 Pseudobulbar palsy 1 (1.3) Anxiety 1.1⫾2.5 Urinary incontinence 18 (23.7) Euphoria 0.1⫾0.5 Apathy 4.6⫾3.1 Disinhibition 1.4⫾3 Irritability 1.8⫾3.3 Aberrant motor behavior 2.4⫾2.4 MMSE indicates Mini-Mental State Examination; ADAS, Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale–Cognitive Part; WAIS, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale– Revised; VIQ, Verbal Intelligence Quotient; PIQ, Performance Intelligence Quotient; and FIQ, Full Scale Intelligence Quotient. neurological signs, cognitive functions, CDR, and the NPI scores were also tested by using partial Spearman rank correlation coefficients; age, sex, duration of symptoms, and education level were entered into the models. The effect of nWBV or WMH volume was also controlled in each analysis. For all analyses, the statistical ␣ level was set at 0.05. Results The mean⫾SD WMH volume was 38.4⫾23.3 cm3. The mean WBV was 1009⫾96 cm3, TIV was 1394⫾103 cm3, and nWBV was 0.725⫾0.048. No significant correlation was noted between the volume of WMHs and nWBV before (r⫽⫺0.002, P⫽0.99) or after (r⫽0.12, P⫽0.31) controlling for the effects of age, sex, education, and duration of symptoms. The frequencies of risk factors and neurological disturbances and the mean scores of the cognitive tests and TABLE 2. Association of Volume of WMHs and nWBV With Demographic Factors WMH Volume rs nWBV P rs P Age 0.20 0.088 ⫺0.34 ⬍0.001 Sex (female/male) 0.13 0.28 ⫺0.12 0.30 ⫺0.05 0.66 ⫺0.06 0.59 0.11 0.34 ⫺0.23 0.052 Education Duration 2185 TABLE 3. Association of Volume of WMHs and nWBV With Risk Factors Risk factors: cognitive test scores Hypertension Quantitative Study of WM Hyperintensities Values are P values or partial Spearman rank correlation coefficients (rs) after controlling for effects of the remaining demographic factors and either nWBV or volume of WMHs. WMH Volume nWBV Risk Factors rs P rs P Hypertension 0.26 0.029 ⫺0.07 0.55 Diabetes 0.12 0.32 0.12 0.31 Lipid disorder 0.092 0.44 0.09 0.44 Smoking ⫺0.029 0.81 0.24 0.048 Alcohol ⫺0.15 0.22 0.04 0.76 Cardiac disease ⫺0.063 0.60 ⫺0.11 0.35 Values are P values or partial Spearman rank correlation coefficients (rs) after controlling for effects of age, sex, education, duration of symptoms, and either nWBV or volume of WMHs. NPI are summarized in Table 1. None of the patients showed hemiparesis, sensory loss, ataxia, or visual field defects. Because pseudobulbar palsy was seen in only 1 patient, no further analysis of this sign was undertaken. There was a highly significant negative correlation between age and nWBV even after controlling for the effects of the confounding variables and volume of WMHs (Table 2). Although the relationship between volume of WMHs and age was significant in a univariate analysis, it did not remain significant after controlling for the effect of the confounding variables and nWBV. Similarly, the relationship between nWBV and the duration of symptoms was significant only before controlling for the effects of the confounding variables and volume of WMHs. Table 3 describes partial Spearman rank correlation coefficients between the volume of WMHs and the risk factors and between nWBV and the risk factors. WMH volume was positively correlated with hypertension. nWBV was positively correlated with smoking (nWBV was larger in smokers). Table 4 summarizes the partial Spearman rank correlation coefficients of volume of WMHs and nWBV with neurological disturbances, CDR, cognitive test scores, and NPI scores after controlling for the effects of the confounding variables. The volume of WMHs was significantly correlated with incontinence and grasp reflex and with the NPI aberrant motor behavior scores but not with cognitive test scores or CDR. On the other hand, nWBV was significantly correlated with all cognitive test scores and CDR, with grasp reflex, and with NPI disinhibition and aberrant motor behavior scores. Discussion WM changes, when defined as irregular periventricular hyperintensities, early confluent deep WMHs, and confluent deep WMHs, had a significant relationship with hypertension, as expected. An association between WM changes and high blood pressure or hypertension and/or the other vascular risk factors has been demonstrated in a number of clinical studies,7,13,18,44 and the relationship between these WM changes and ischemic vascular changes has been documented in pathological studies.33,34 On the other hand, we found no significant correlation between brain atrophy and vascular risk factors. Furthermore, brain atrophy was not correlated with WMH volume either with or without controlling for age, sex, education, and the duration of symptoms. Using quanti- 2186 Stroke September 2000 TABLE 4. Association of volume of WMHs and nWBV With Neurological, Cognitive, and Neurobehavioral Symptoms WMH Volume rs P nWBV rs P Neurological findings Pyramidal sign ⫺0.005 0.97 0.19 Extrapyramidal sign ⫺0.20 0.10 ⫺0.21 0.077 0.11 Incontinence 0.26 0.027 ⫺0.16 0.20 Grasp 0.32 0.007 ⫺0.27 0.025 ⫺0.019 0.88 ⫺0.15 0.20 Postural instability Cognitive tests 0.022 0.86 0.42 ⬍0.001 ⫺0.001 0.99 ⫺0.45 ⬍0.001 ADAS recall 0.11 0.35 0.30 0.011 WAIS VIQ 0.025 0.84 0.35 0.003 MMSE ADAS Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 2, 2016 WAIS PIQ ⫺0.11 0.38 0.48 ⬍0.001 WAIS FIQ ⫺0.039 0.75 0.47 ⬍0.001 0.11 0.36 ⫺0.26 0.028 0.42 CDR NPI scores Delusion ⫺0.005 0.97 ⫺0.10 Hallucination ⫺0.069 0.57 0.05 0.69 Aggression 0.11 0.37 ⫺0.05 0.68 Depression 0.032 0.79 ⫺0.13 0.29 Anxiety 0.16 0.18 ⫺0.17 0.17 Euphoria 0.063 0.60 ⫺0.09 0.45 Apathy 0.14 0.23 ⫺0.17 0.17 Disinhibition 0.14 0.25 ⫺0.25 0.036 Irritability 0.056 0.64 ⫺0.14 0.26 Aberrant motor behavior 0.28 0.019 ⫺0.27 0.023 Values are P values or partial Spearman rank correlation coefficients (rs) after controlling for effects of age, sex, education, duration of symptoms, and either nWBV or volume of WMHs. tative MRI, DeCarli et al29 also demonstrated no significant differences either in brain or cerebrospinal fluid volumes between AD patients with and without WMHs. These findings suggest that WM changes and brain atrophy were independent phenomena. Recent studies have demonstrated that AD itself is not involved in forming WMHs.20,58,59 Although WMHs are sometimes reported as being significantly more frequent in AD patients than in control subjects, the increase is attributed to mild periventricular changes of probably nonischemic origin, including caps, halos, and thin lining.5,7,9,10 Moreover, Scheltens et al6 reported that compared with WMH in control subjects, WMH was more intense in patients with senile onset AD but not in patients with presenile onset AD and suggested that additional microvascular factors are involved in elderly patients with senile onset AD. Together with those findings in the recent studies, our results suggests that WM changes in AD patients, when they are defined as irregular periventricular or confluent deep WMHs, are superimposed phenomena of ischemic origin. It is also interesting that our results demonstrated that a smoking habit had a modest but significant protective effect against brain atrophy. Smoking has been reported to prevent AD,60 and our findings might support this hypothesis. In the present study, WMH volume was not correlated with the severity of dementia, global cognitive impairment, or memory impairment. This finding is compatible with the findings of our previous study,61 in which we demonstrated that WMHs were associated with decreased cerebral blood flow but not with decreased oxygen metabolism in patients with AD. Brain atrophy, but not reduced cerebral blood flow, was significantly associated with cognitive impairments. These findings suggest that the cognitive impairment in our patients is not attributable to WM changes but to brain atrophy, although WM changes are reported to impair some cognitive functions that were not evaluated in the present study.11 WMHs associated with more severe small-vessel diseases might affect cognitive functions in patients with AD. Snowdon et al62 reported that in subjects with pathological evidence of AD, those lacunar infarcts in the basal ganglia, thalamus, or deep white matter had poorer cognitive function and a higher prevalence of dementia than those without infarcts. However, even in patients who were diagnosed as having vascular dementia, the association between WM changes and global cognitive impairment is unconvincing.10,11,22,25,63– 66 Although Binswanger’s disease reportedly causes dementia without a cortical degenerative process, the pathological features of this disorder include not only WM changes but also lacunar infarcts in the basal ganglia and thalamus.1,2 Coexistent lacunar infarcts may affect the strategic sites, causing dementia.67 Inzitari et al68 pointed out that a strong association between WM changes and dementia was an epiphenomenon that could be explained by a history of stroke. In a longitudinal study of patients with lacunar infarcts, Loeb et al69 found that the development of dementia was significantly associated with cerebral atrophy and new focal cerebrovascular episodes but not with WM changes. These findings, together with those in the present study, suggest that cognitive impairment, both in AD and vascular dementia, is not principally attributable to WM changes. On the other hand, the present study clearly demonstrated that WM changes and brain atrophy were independently associated with certain neurological and neurobehavioral signs. Urinary incontinence, grasp reflexes, and aberrant motor behaviors were significantly correlated with WMH volume even after controlling for brain atrophy, although the latter 2 were also correlated with brain atrophy after controlling for WMH volume. Urinary incontinence is considered to be one of the central clinical features of Binswanger’s disease,1,2 and an involvement of WM changes in its development has been shown in previous studies.23,24 Primitive reflexes have also been reported to be associated with WM changes in elderly people70,71 and in patients with dementia.72 Positive associations between WM changes and psychiatric symptoms have been reported in subjects without dementia.73,74 Although previous studies have failed to find a relationship between WM changes and neurobehavioral signs in patients with dementia,14,21,28,30 the present study clearly demonstrated that WM changes were involved in the development of aberrant motor behaviors. Aberrant motor behaviors, including wandering, pacing, and rummaging, belong to repetitive and excessive behaviors, which are likely to be caused by frontal lobe dysfunction. Our findings indicate that WM changes would Hirono et al at least add frontal lobe–related neurological and neurobehavioral features as manifestations of dementia. In conclusion, WM changes in AD patients without any obvious cerebrovascular diseases are related to hypertensive microangiopathy and are independent of brain atrophy that would be attributable to a degenerative process. WM changes contribute to the development of some frontal lobe–related neurological and neurobehavioral signs but not to the development of a global cognitive impairment, which is more closely associated with brain atrophy. Further studies are needed to generalize our findings to include AD patients with more severe vascular disease. Acknowledgments Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 2, 2016 We thank Satoshi Tanimukai, MD, Tokiji Hanihara, MD, Toru Imamura, MD (Division of Clinical Neurosciences), Mieko Matsui, MD, Setsu Sakamoto, MD, and Kazunari Ishii, MD (Division of Neuroimaging Research) for their help with various parts of the study. References 1. Roman GC. Senile dementia of the Binswanger type: a vascular form of dementia in the elderly. JAMA. 1987;258:1782–1788. 2. Caplan LR. Binswanger’s disease–revisited. Neurology. 1995;45:626–633. 3. Rezek DL, Morris JC, Fulling KH, Gado MH. Periventricular white matter lucencies in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type and in normal aging. Neurology. 1987;37:1365–1368. 4. Mirsen TR, Lee DH, Wong CJ, Diaz JF, Fox AJ, Hachinski VC, Merskey H. Clinical correlates of white-matter changes on magnetic resonance imaging scans of the brain. Arch Neurol. 1991;48:1015–1021. 5. McDonald WM, Krishnan KR, Doraiswamy PM, Figiel GS, Husain MM, Boyko OB, Heyman A. Magnetic resonance findings in patients with early-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry. 1991;29:799–810. 6. Scheltens P, Barkhof F, Valk J, Algra PR, van der Hoop RG, Nauta J, Wolters EC. White matter lesions on magnetic resonance imaging in clinically diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease: evidence for heterogeneity. Brain. 1992;115:735–748. 7. Waldemar G, Christiansen P, Larsson HB, Hogh P, Laursen H, Lassen NA, Paulson OB. White matter magnetic resonance hyperintensities in dementia of the Alzheimer type: morphological and regional cerebral blood flow correlates. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57:1458–1465. 8. Scheltens P, Barkhof F, Leys D, Wolters EC, Ravid R, Kamphorst W. Histopathologic correlates of white matter changes on MRI in Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging. Neurology. 1995;45:883–888. 9. Fazekas F, Kapeller P, Schmidt R, Offenbacher H, Payer F, Fazekas G. The relation of cerebral magnetic resonance signal hyperintensities to Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Sci. 1996;142:121–125. 10. Barber R, Scheltens P, Gholkar A, Ballard C, McKeith I, Ince P, Perry R, O’Brien J. White matter lesions on magnetic resonance imaging in dementia with Lewy bodies, Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia, and normal aging. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1999;67:66–72. 11. Kertesz A, Polk M, Carr T. Cognition and white matter changes on magnetic resonance imaging in dementia. Arch Neurol. 1990;47:387–391. 12. Bondareff W, Raval J, Colletti PM, Hauser DL. Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and the severity of dementia in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145:853–856. 13. Bondareff W, Raval J, Woo B, Hauser DL, Colletti PM. Magnetic resonance imaging and the severity of dementia in older adults. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47:47–51. 14. Harrell LE, Duvall E, Folks DG, Duke L, Bartolucci A, Conboy T, Callaway R, Kerns D. The relationship of high-intensity signals on magnetic resonance images to cognitive and psychiatric state in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol. 1991;48:1136–1140. 15. Diaz JF, Merskey H, Hachinski VC, Lee DH, Boniferro M, Wong CJ, Mirsen TR, Fox H. Improved recognition of leukoaraiosis and cognitive impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol. 1991;48:1022–1025. 16. Stout JC, Jernigan TL, Archibald SL, Salmon DP. Association of dementia severity with cortical gray matter and abnormal white matter volumes in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Arch Neurol. 1996;53:742–749. Quantitative Study of WM Hyperintensities 2187 17. Ott BR, Faberman RS, Noto RB, Rogg JM, Hough TJ, Tung GA, Spencer PK. A SPECT imaging study of MRI white matter hyperintensity in patients with degenerative dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 1997;8:348–354. 18. Fazekas F, Chawluk JB, Alavi A, Hurtig HI, Zimmerman RA. MR signal abnormalities at 1.5 T in Alzheimer’s dementia and normal aging. Am J Neuroradiol. 1987;8:421–426. 19. Kozachuk WE, DeCarli C, Schapiro MB, Wagner EE, Rapoport SI, Horwitz B. White matter hyperintensities in dementia of Alzheimer’s type and in healthy subjects without cerebrovascular risk factors: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Neurol. 1990;47:1306–1310. 20. Leys D, Soetaert G, Petit H, Fauquette A, Pruvo JP, Steinling M. Periventricular and white matter magnetic resonance imaging hyperintensities do not differ between Alzheimer’s disease and normal aging. Arch Neurol. 1990; 47:524–527. 21. Lopez OL, Becker JT, Rezek D, Wess J, Boller F, Reynolds CF III, Panisset M. Neuropsychiatric correlates of cerebral white-matter radiolucencies in probable Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol. 1992;49:828–834. 22. Schmidt R. Comparison of magnetic resonance imaging in Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia and normal aging. Eur Neurol. 1992;32:164–169. 23. Bennett DA, Gilley DW, Wilson RS, Huckman MS, Fox JH. Clinical correlates of high signal lesions on magnetic resonance imaging in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol. 1992;239:186–190. 24. Bennett DA, Gilley DW, Lee S, Cochran EJ. White matter changes: neurobehavioral manifestations of Binswanger’s disease and clinical correlates in Alzheimer disease. Dementia. 1994;5:148–152. 25. Wahlund LO, Basun H, Almkvist O, Andersson-Lundman G, Julin P, Saaf J. White matter hyperintensities in dementia: does it matter? Magn Reson Imaging. 1994;387–394. 26. Brilliant M, Hughes L, Anderson D, Ghobrial M, Elble R. Rarefied white matter in patients with Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1995;9:39–46. 27. Marder K, Richards M, Bello J, Bell K, Sano M, Miller L, Folstein M, Albert M, Stern Y Clinical correlates of Alzheimer’s disease with and without silent radiographic abnormalities. Arch Neurol. 1995;52:146–151. 28. Lopez OL, Becker JT, Jungreis CA, Rezek D, Estol C, Boller F, DeKosky ST. Computed tomography–but not magnetic resonance imaging–identified periventricular white-matter lesions predict symptomatic cerebrovascular disease in probable Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol. 1995;52:659–664. 29. DeCarli C, Grady CL, Clark CM, Katz DA, Brady DR, Murphy DG, Haxby JV, Salerno JA, Gillette JA, Gonzalez-Aviles A, et al. Comparison of positron emission tomography, cognition, and brain volume in Alzheimer’s disease with and without severe abnormalities of white matter. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1996;60:158–167. 30. Starkstein SE, Sabe L, Vazquez S, Di Lorenzo G, Martinez A, Petracca G, Teson A, Chemerinski E, Leiguarda R. Neuropsychological, psychiatric, and cerebral perfusion correlates of leukoaraiosis in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1997;63:66–73. 31. Doody RS, Massman PJ, Mawad M, Nance M. Cognitive consequences of subcortical magnetic resonance imaging changes in Alzheimer’s disease: comparison to small vessel ischemic vascular dementia. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 1998;11:191–199. 32. Teipel SJ, Hampel H, Alexander GE, Schapiro MB, Horwitz B, Teichberg D, Daley E, Hippius H, Moller HJ, Rapoport SI. Dissociation between corpus callosum atrophy and white matter pathology in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1998;51:1381–1385. 33. Leifer D, Buonanno FS, Richardson EP Jr. Clinicopathologic correlations of cranial magnetic resonance imaging of periventricular white matter. Neurology. 1990;40:911–918. 34. Fazekas F, Kleinert R, Offenbacher H, Schmidt R, Kleinert G, Payer F, Radner H, Lechner H. Pathologic correlates of incidental MRI white matter signal hyperintensities. Neurology. 1993;43:1683–1689. 35. Johnson KA, Davis KR, Buonanno FS, Brady TJ, Rosen TJ, Growdon JH. Comparison of magnetic resonance and roentgen ray computed tomography in dementia. Arch Neurol. 1987;44:1075–1080. 36. Erkinjuntti T, Ketonen L, Sulkava R, Sipponen J, Vuorialho M, Iivanainen M. Do white matter changes on MRI and CT differentiate vascular dementia from Alzheimer’s disease? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1987;50:37–42. 37. Kobari M, Meyer JS, Ichijo M, Oravez WT. Leukoaraiosis: correlation of MR and CT findings with blood flow, atrophy, and cognition. Am J Neuroradiol. 1990;11:273–281. 38. Lechner H, Schmidt R, Bertha G, Justich E, Offenbacher H, Schneider G. Nuclear magnetic resonance image white matter lesions and risk factors for stroke in normal individuals. Stroke. 1988;19:263–265. 39. Mäntylä R, Erkinjuntti T, Salonen O, Aronen HJ, Peltonen T, Pohjasvaara T, Standertskjöld-Nordenstam CG. Variable agreement between visual rating 2188 40. 41. 42. 43. 44. 45. Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 2, 2016 46. 47. 48. 49. 50. 51. 52. 53. 54. 55. 56. Stroke September 2000 scales for white matter hyperintensities on MRI: comparison of 13 rating scales in a poststroke cohort. Stroke. 1997;28:1614–1623. DeCarli C, Murphy DG, Tranh M, Grady CL, Haxby JV, Gillette JA, Salerno JA, Gonzales-Aviles A, Horwitz B, Rapoport SI, et al. The effect of white matter hyperintensity volume on brain structure, cognitive performance, and cerebral metabolism of glucose in 51 healthy adults. Neurology. 1995;45: 2077–2084. Swan GE, DeCarli C, Miller BL, Reed T, Wolf PA, Jack LM, Carmelli D. Association of midlife blood pressure to late-life cognitive decline and brain morphology. Neurology. 1998;51:986–993. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition-Revised (DSM-III-R). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1987. Fazekas F, Niederkorn K, Schmidt R, Offenbacher H, Horner S, Bertha G, Lechner H. White matter signal abnormalities in normal individuals: correlation with carotid ultrasonography, cerebral blood flow measurements, and cerebrovascular risk factors. Stroke. 1988;19:1285–1288. Hirono N, Yasuda M, Tanimukai S, Kitagaki H, Mori E. Effect of the apolipoprotein E e4 allele on white matter hyperintensities in dementia. Stroke. 2000;31:1263–1268. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 1984;34:939–944. Sano M, Devanand DP, Richards M, Miller LW, Marder K, Bell K, Dooneief G, Bylsma FW, Lafleche G, Albert M, et al. A standardized technique for establishing onset and duration of symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease. Arch Neurol. 1995;52:961–966. Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, Coben LA, Martin RL. A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry. 1982;140:566–572. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. ‘Mini-Mental State’: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. Wechsler DA. WAIS-R Manual. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation; 1981. Mohs RC, Rosen WG, Davis KL. The Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale: an instrument for assessing treatment efficacy. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1983;19:448–450. Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, Rosenberg-Thompson S, Carusi DA, Gornbein J. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology. 1994;44:2308–2314. Takahashi M, Korogi Y. Problems and solutions in mass screening for asymptomatic brain disease. Jpn J Diagn Imaging. 1998;18:1094–1103. Hajnal JV, Bryant DJ, Kasuboski L, Pattany PM, De Coene B, Lewis PD, Pennock JM, Oatridge A, Young IR, Bydder GM. Use of fluid attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) pulse sequences in MRI of the brain. J Comput Assist Tomogr. 1992;16:841–844. Rydberg JN, Hammond CA, Grimm RC, Erickson BJ, Jack CR Jr, Huston J III, Riederer SJ. Initial clinical experience in MR imaging of the brain with a fast fluid-attenuated inversion-recovery pulse sequence. Radiology. 1994; 193:173–180. Yasuda M, Mori E, Kitagaki H, Yamashita H, Hirono N, Shimada K, Maeda K, Tanaka C. Apolipoprotein E e4 allele and whole brain atrophy in late-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155:779–784. Yamato K, Hata Y, Kamiura N, Kobashi S, Mori E. Automatic extraction of cerebral and intracranial regions from MRI images for functional cerebral diagnosis. Vis Computing. 1995;6:96–97. 57. Mori E, Hirono N, Yamashita H, Imamura T, Ikejiri Y, Ikeda M, Kitagaki H, Shimomura T, Yoneda Y. Premorbid brain size as a determinant of reserve capacity against intellectual decline in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:18–24. 58. Erkinjuntti T, Gao F, Lee DH, Eliasziw M, Merskey H, Hachinski VC. Lack of difference in brain hyperintensities between patients with early Alzheimer’s disease and control subjects. Arch Neurol. 1994;51:260–268. 59. Smith CD, Snowdon DA, Wang H, Markesbery WR. White matter volumes and periventricular white matter hyperintensities in aging and dementia. Neurology 2000;54:838–842. 60. Lee PN. Smoking and Alzheimer’s disease: a review of the epidemiological evidence. Neuroepidemiology. 1994;13:131–144. 61. Yamaji S, Ishii K, Sasaki M, Imamura T, Kitagaki H, Sakamoto S, Mori E. Changes in cerebral blood flow and oxygen metabolism related to magnetic resonance imaging white matter hyperintensities in Alzheimer’s disease. J Nucl Med. 1997;38:1471–1474. 62. Snowdon DA, Greiner LH, Mortimer JA, Riley KP, Greiner PA, Markesbery WR. Brain infarction and the clinical expression of Alzheimer disease: the Nun Study. JAMA. 1997;277:813–817. 63. Hershey LA, Modic MT, Greenough PG, Jaffe DF. Magnetic resonance imaging in vascular dementia. Neurology. 1987;37:29–36. 64. Almkvist O, Wahlund LO, Andersson-Lundman G, Basun H, Backman L. White-matter hyperintensity and neuropsychological functions in dementia and healthy aging. Arch Neurol. 1992;49:626–632. 65. Bracco L, Campani D, Baratti E, Lippi A, Inzitari D, Pracucci G, Amaducci L. Relation between MRI features and dementia in cerebrovascular disease patients with leukoaraiosis: a longitudinal study. J Neurol Sci. 1993;120: 131–136. 66. van Swieten JC, Staal S, Kappelle LJ, Derix MM, van Gijn J. Are white matter lesions directly associated with cognitive impairment in patients with lacunar infarcts? J Neurol. 1996;243:196–200. 67. Mori E, Ishii K, Hashimoto M, Imamura T, Hirono N, Kitagaki H. Role of functional brain imaging in the evaluation of vascular dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1999;13(suppl 3):S91–S101. 68. Inzitari D, Diaz F, Fox A, Hachinski VC, Steingart A, Lau C, Donald A, Wade J, Mulic H, Merskey H. Vascular risk factors and leuko-araiosis. Arch Neurol. 1987;44:42–47. 69. Loeb C, Gandolfo C, Croce R, Conti M. Dementia associated with lacunar infarction. Stroke. 1992;23:1225–1229. 70. Steingart A, Hachinski VC, Lau C, Fox AJ, Fox H, Lee D, Inzitari D, Merskey H. Cognitive and neurologic findings in subjects with diffuse white matter lucencies on computed tomographic scan (leuko-araiosis). Arch Neurol. 1987;44:32–35. 71. Cadelo M, Inzitari D, Pracucci G, Mascalchi M. Predictors of leukoaraiosis in elderly neurologic patients. Cerebrovasc Dis. 1991;1:345–351. 72. Ishii N, Nishihara Y, Imamura T. Why do frontal lobe symptoms predominate in vascular dementia with lacunes? Neurology. 1986;36: 340 –345. 73. Breitner JC, Husain MM, Figiel GS, Krishnan KR, Boyko OB. Cerebral white matter disease in late-onset paranoid psychosis. Biol Psychiatry. 1990; 28:266–274. 74. Brown FW, Lewine RJ, Hudgins PA, Risch SC. White matter hyperintensity signals in psychiatric and nonpsychiatric subjects. Am J Psychiatry. 1992; 149:620–625. Impact of White Matter Changes on Clinical Manifestation of Alzheimer's Disease: A Quantitative Study Nobutsugu Hirono, Hajime Kitagaki, Hiroaki Kazui, Mamoru Hashimoto and Etsuro Mori Downloaded from http://stroke.ahajournals.org/ by guest on October 2, 2016 Stroke. 2000;31:2182-2188 doi: 10.1161/01.STR.31.9.2182 Stroke is published by the American Heart Association, 7272 Greenville Avenue, Dallas, TX 75231 Copyright © 2000 American Heart Association, Inc. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0039-2499. Online ISSN: 1524-4628 The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on the World Wide Web at: http://stroke.ahajournals.org/content/31/9/2182 Permissions: Requests for permissions to reproduce figures, tables, or portions of articles originally published in Stroke can be obtained via RightsLink, a service of the Copyright Clearance Center, not the Editorial Office. Once the online version of the published article for which permission is being requested is located, click Request Permissions in the middle column of the Web page under Services. Further information about this process is available in the Permissions and Rights Question and Answer document. Reprints: Information about reprints can be found online at: http://www.lww.com/reprints Subscriptions: Information about subscribing to Stroke is online at: http://stroke.ahajournals.org//subscriptions/