Recall Performance in Air Traffic Controllers Across the 24

advertisement
Chapter 3
Recall Performance in Air Traffic Controllers
Across the 24-hr Day: Influence of Alertness
and Task Demands on Recall Strategies
Claudine Mélan and Edith Galy
Additional information is available at the end of the chapter
http://dx.doi.org/10.5772/50342
1. Introduction
Air traffic controllers’ (ATCs') work evolves constantly, concerning in particular route
complexity and traffic density, but also development of supporting technology. Introducing
more automation to allow more efficient ATC control and increased safety and security also
requires enhanced supervisory activity, situation awareness, processing of larger amounts of
data. These cognitive processes place a heavy load on ATCs’ memory functions as they
require item processing and recall, which are also involved in control operations such as
monitoring traffic, controlling aircraft movements, managing air traffic sequences, resolving
conflicts. Better understanding of memory processes and of their limitations in expert ATCs
may thus be crucial for the development of future automation tools, but also for training
and selection of controllers. The aim of the present contribution is to give a comprehensive
overview of memorisation performance in air traffic controllers, in light of the most recent
memory models. More especially, a series of experiments reveal that ATCs’ memorisation
performance varies in a complex manner according to both task-related factors (presentation
modality, number of items, recall protocol), and task-independent factors. The latter are
related more especially to shift-scheduling (time-of-day, on-shift time) and physiological
capacities (alertness, automatic item processing).
2. ATCs’ performance variations according to task-related factors
2.1. Information processing during control operations
En route ATC involves the processing of information relative to a variable number of
aircrafts coming from different directions, at diverse speed and altitudes, and heading to
© 2012 Mélan and Galy, licensee InTech. This is an open access chapter distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0), which permits
unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
36 Advances in Air Navigation Services
various destinations or that are, on contrary, grouped in a more restricted space, thus
requiring more in-depth processing in order to anticipate air-plane conflicts. Presentation
modality is of interest as information about an aircraft is presented visually on a strip or
script 10 to 15 min before its real-time presentation in the visual modality (radar
information) or in the auditory modality (radio information). Radar information includes
instantaneous level, attitude (stable, climbing, descending) and speed group, and strips
present many information as aircraft call sign, aircraft type and associated speed (or power),
provenance and destination, route case (estimated hours of flying), and three cases
completed by ATCs with information concerning coordination information with other
ATCs, or changes requested in flight.
A number of simulation studies and in situ ATC observations explored the way such factors
may impact on memory performance. Thus, expert ATCs show high recall performance of
aircrafts and their position on a sector map, and poorer recollection of details regarding an
aircraft (Means et al., 1988). Higher recognition accuracy for aircrafts involved in an
impending conflict compared to those that would not cross or cross in some near future
confirmed the impact of aircraft status on memory performance (Gronlund et al., 1998,
2005). Simulation studies revealed higher recall when navigational messages were presented
in the auditory rather than in the visual modality. In addition, performance dropped
considerably when message length increased beyond three commands, while command
wordiness (2 or 4 words) had only limited effects on recall (Wickens & Hollands, 2000;
Barshi & Healy, 2002; Schneider et al., 2004).
2.2. Information processing according to presentation modality: The auditory
superiority effect
Controlled laboratory studies have systematically explored task-related factors that may
affect mnemonic performance and have led to the proposal of integrated theoretical models.
More especially, encoding and processing of auditory compared to visual verbal material
has systematically revealed superior recall of heard material in short-term memory (for a
review, Penney, 1989). A common test in this research field contrasts silent reading and
reading aloud of unrelated lists of words, nonwords, letters, or digits (see, e.g., Conrad &
Hull, 1968; Frankish, 1985). It is assumed that both silent reading and reading aloud provide
phonological information and that only reading aloud provides acoustic–sensory
information. This effect was found for the recency part of the serial position curve in
immediate free recall and in serial recall. Overt vocalization of a visually presented list by
the subject produced much the same effect as did auditory presentation on the recency part
of the serial position curve, but subject vocalization tended to reduce recall in the non
recency part of the serial position curve. The modality effect has been observed with both
written and oral recall, but seems to be slightly more marked in the former case. This
auditory superiority effect, known as the modality effect, was accounted for by modalityrelated processing differences at different stages of the memorisation process, rather than by
strategic differences.
Recall Performance in Air Traffic Controllers Across the 24-hr Day:
Influence of Alertness and Task Demands on Recall Strategies 37
2.2.1. Differences during processing of auditory and visually presented material
Several authors suggested that a long-lasting sensory acoustic trace would be generated for
auditory presented words but not for visually presented words (Cowan, 1984; Crowder &
Morton, 1969; Crowder et al., 2004; Penney, 1989). The term acoustic–sensory information
refers to sensory representations of sounds. Though there is some evidence of a sensory visual
trace for seen words, it would be very short-lived compared to the long-lasting sensory
acoustic trace that would favour more efficient encoding of heard material. In addition, orally
presented list-items would be associated with temporal cues that would not be generated by
successively presented visual words. These temporal cues would then result in stronger
memory traces and ensure higher recall for heard items (Frankish, 1985, 2008).
Other authors argued that the differences between the visual and the auditory information
processing streams would occur when the sensory trace is processed into a short term
memory trace. The working memory model (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974) includes a dedicated
phonological subsystem, in which the code used to represent verbal items broadly
corresponds to the phonetic level. Hence, auditory information would benefit from
automatic phonological coding while visual information would require effortful
phonological recoding (Baddeley, 1986, 2000; Penney, 1989). Phonological coding would
favour mental rehearsal and maintenance of information in a short-term store (Baddeley,
1986), what would result in a larger number of phonologically coded memory traces in a
short-term store following hearing than following seeing item-lists. According to Penney
(1989) the auditory superiority would be based on the combination of a longer-lasting
sensory acoustic trace and automatic phonological coding of orally presented materiel.
2.2.2. Differences during restitution of heard and seen item lists
Differences during item recall were also proposed to account for better immediate recall of
heard than of seen materiel. This research is based in particular on the robust finding that
when participants recall a sequence of spoken digits the last one is almost always correctly
recalled, but if the same sequence is presented visually, recall of the final item is relatively
poor. According to Crowder and Morton (1969), this auditory recency effect indicates that
an acoustically coded representation of the final list item is maintained in a sensory store,
while representations of earlier items are overwritten by successively incoming speech
sounds.
Alternatively, Cowan and co-workers accounted for the recency effect by suggesting greater
resistance to output interference for heard compared to seen verbal material (Cowan et al.,
2002; Harveay & Beaman, 2007; Madigan, 1971). Output interference is defined as the
degradation of memory representations as recall proceeds across output positions. Evidence
in favour of a greater resistance to interference for heard items came from studies reporting
a marked auditory superiority when restitution was based on a free recall procedure
(without providing any cues) and a reduced effect when a recognition procedure was used
(list items are presented together with new items). Item recognition may be explained by an
impression of familiarity for the list-items and would thus not require in-depth processing.
38 Advances in Air Navigation Services
In contrast, free recall requires subjects to explicitly recall each item, thereby degrading the
remaining memory traces (Brébion et al., 2005).
Lower output interference has also been proposed to account for the auditory advantage of
sentence recall (Rummer & Schweppe, 2005). In line with this hypothesis, Beaman and
Morton (2000) showed that following presentation of 16-item lists, subjects preferentially
recalled 2-, 3- and 4-item sub-sequences (of the form 15-16, 14-15-16, 13-14-15-16) from the
end of the lists. However, while end sub-sequences were recalled with a similar frequency in
both modalities on the opening run of a trial, they were recalled significantly more often for
heard than for seen items during the course of a trial.
2.3. Auditory superiority for heard versus seen item-lists in ATCs
In light of these findings we explored whether output interference while recalling seen items
may be reduced in participants well-practiced in processing visual information, i.e. ATCs
habitually processing visual information on a radar. In addition we tested to what extent the
effect may be explained by memory load, typically explored by presenting a list of items of
variable length. Previous research with this kind of procedure has shown that the amount of
proactive interference is much less for smaller set sizes (Oberauer & Vockenberg, 2009)
although there can be some proactive interference even at small set sizes (Carroll et al., 2010).
We used the procedure described by Beaman and Morton (2000) to explore free recall of heard
versus seen 6- and 9-item lists in 15 volunteer ATCs of an en-route centre in southern France.
Participants were aged 31.3 years (range: 27 to 42 years old) and had been working for 7 years
and 4 months (range: 3 to18 years) in the control centre. The sub-sequences recalled by ATCs
at initial and non-initial output positions are summarized in table 1. Analyses revealed
significant higher mean numbers of items recalled following hearing (4.9 and 3.6 respectively
for 6- and 9- item lists) than seeing the lists (4.2 and 3.2 respectively).
Visual
Terminal item sequences
6
5, 6
4, 5, 6
3, 4, 5, 6
2, 3, 4, 5, 6
1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6
Terminal item sequences
9
8, 9
7, 8, 9
6, 7, 8, 9
5, 6, 7, 8, 9
4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9
Auditory
6-word lists
13
13
12
3
7
10
7
17
12
2
2
27
9-word lists
24
22
7
3
0
1
20
18
14
4
3
1
Recall Performance in Air Traffic Controllers Across the 24-hr Day:
Influence of Alertness and Task Demands on Recall Strategies 39
Visual
Terminal item sequences
5, 6
4, 5, 6
3, 4, 5, 6
Terminal item sequences
8, 9
7, 8, 9
6, 7, 8, 9
Auditory
6-word lists
16
3
1
34
13
2
9-word lists
14
1
1
30
4
2
Table 1. Number of each of the terminal item sequences as the opening run (upper panel) and in noninitial response positions (lower panel) during free recall of 6- and 9-word lists with auditory and visual
presentation (Galy et al., 2010).
As shown in table 1, ATCs recalled complete 6-item lists three times more frequently
following auditory (27) than following visual list (10) presentation, while recall of five-item
sub-sequences (2-3-4-5-6) was rare in both modalities (2 vs. 7 respectively). These results
further indicate that occurrences for heard lists were tenfold more frequent for complete sixitem lists than for five-item sub-sequences, while no such difference occurred for seen lists.
Occurrences of equivalent end sub-sequences of 9-item lists (5-6-7-8-9; 4-5-6-7-8-9) ranged
between zero and three in both modalities. The auditory recall advantage of these lists
appeared to result from higher occurrences of ordered 2- and 3-item end sub-sequences in
other than initial recall positions.
Taken together, the findings show that ATCs would spontaneously adopt the output
strategy consisting of uttering end sub-sequences more frequently for heard than for seen
items, leading to, or contributing to significant higher overall performance. They thus
further stress the proposal that “auditory presentation seems to protect the end of the list
from output interference” (Cowan et al., 2002, p.168). In favour of this hypothesis, these
authors showed that the auditory advantage is even more pronounced when output
interference is high. Alternatively it has been proposed that the auditory advantage that
extends over the last few serial positions is retrieved independently for each item from an
echoic trace (Frankish, 2008). According to this author, pronounced recency in immediate
serial recall is limited to stimuli that engage the perceptual mechanism involved in linguistic
decoding of speech. Further research intended to disentangle different sources of sensory
information at input. Thus, Rummer and Schweppe (2005) observed a modality effect for
spoken sentences compared to conditions without acoustic–sensory information, i.e., both
silent reading and mouthing. As the latter two conditions did not differ from each other, the
results would rule out articulatory information at input as a source of the modality effect.
Differences due to output modality were also investigated. For written recall the auditory
advantage was larger with high than with low output interference, while this difference was
not maintained for spoken recall (Harvey & Beaman, 2007). Taken together, the data suggest
that both superior auditory encoding and reduced output interference would contribute to
the auditory modality effect.
40 Advances in Air Navigation Services
As ATCs are well-practiced in processing successively presented visual information, on
contrary to participants in the above-cited laboratory experiments, the present findings
favour the idea that differential physiological features may characterize the visual and
auditory information processing streams (Penney, 1989). The modality effect may then be
implemented in the current models of spoken and of written words, which are divided on
the relation between the different levels of processing, which will not be discussed in this
issue. Briefly, in some models the transmission of information through successive levels of
representation is represented as a unidirectional flow within a feed forward network.
Perceptual analysis begins with encoding of acoustic features, which are then translated into
phonetic and then lexical representations. In contrast, interactive activation models propose
that communication between these levels is bidirectional. Whenever a lexical unit becomes
active, feedback connections boost activation of the units that represent its constituent
phonemes (for a review, Frankish, 2008).
3. Shift-scheduling and ATCs’ functional state
Like in other safety-related job situations, ATC requires operators to work successive shifts;
i.e. different teams work in succession to cover the whole 24h-day. As a consequence,
controllers are subjected to the negative impact of shift work on biological rhythms, sleep,
job performance, and psychological measures (Costa, 1999; Della Rocco & Nesthus, 2005;
Dinges et al., 1997; Folkard & Tucker, 2003).
3.1. Regulation mechanisms of circadian variations
It is now largely accepted that the negative effects of shift work result from a disruption of
the habitual circadian regulation of physiological and psychological measures (Costa, 2003;
Siegrist, 2010). Circadian variations across the 24h-day are under control of two endogenous
systems, the homeostatic system expressed by the fatigue accumulating since awakening,
and the circadian system evidenced by a sinusoidal variation across the 24h period. Hence,
shift work is systematically associated with a cumulative sleep deficit (homeostatic system),
and a decreased amplitude of sinusoidal variations (circadian system), thereby interfering
with the two powerful factors limiting human ability and, as a consequence, safety and
security (Akerstedt, 1991; Akerstedt et al., 2004; Dinges et al., 1997; Folkard & Akerstedt,
1992; Tucker et al., 2006). As stated by Akerstedt (2007, p. 209) “Being exposed to the
circadian low (during work/activity), extended time awake or reduced duration of sleep will
impair performance”.
3.2. Circadian and non circadian variations of subjective and physiological
measures in shift-workers
In order to investigate a person’s functional state, behavioural, physiological or subjective
measures have been used. It is generally accepted that these measures are subjected to a
circadian rhythmicity. Shift work effects have mostly been documented by reporting
decreased self-rated alertness (Akerstedt & Gillberg, 1990; Galy et al., 2008) and increased
Recall Performance in Air Traffic Controllers Across the 24-hr Day:
Influence of Alertness and Task Demands on Recall Strategies 41
occurrences of incidents on the night shift when the circadian decline in human capabilities
is further aggravated by a chronic sleep deficit and fatigue (Costa, 2003; Folkard & Akersted,
2003). Laboratory studies have established that alertness is low in the morning, increases
during the day until the late afternoon, before decreasing in the evening and reach a
minimal level on early morning hours. The shift work literature revealed that in real-job
situations alertness also varies with time of day and that the typical diurnal trend would
only be marginally modified by shift work scheduling features. However, early morning
shifts, extended shift duration and repeated night shifts have been shown to be associated
with increased sleepiness, more especially during the last half of extended shifts,
particularly on night shifts (Kecklund et al., 1997; Rosa, 1995), but also on day shifts (Tucker
et al., 1998).
Circadian variations appear to be less consistent for other psychological measures recorded
in several shift work studies, and in particular for self-reported tension (Folkard, 1990;
Kecklund et al., 1997; Monk et al., 1985; Owens et al., 2000 ; Prizmic et al., 1995). While some
studies reported a circadian trend for perceived tension, others did not, and still others
reported an atypical trend. More especially, operators supervising a satellite across 24h-day
displayed significant increased self-rated tension and heart rate on the first hour of each
shift, even on the night-shift, despite a lower baseline level for heart rate during the night
(Cariou et al., 2008). In contrast, when the same satellite controllers rated Thayer’s (1989)
Activation-Deactivation checklist, their alertness level was highly correlated with their body
temperature (Fig. 1), largely considered as an index of subjects’ functional state. Both
measures followed a typical circadian trend, indicating a strong dependency of these
measures on the endogenous regulation systems.
Taken together with the shift-work literature, these results indicate that some subjective
and objective measures (here alertness and body temperature) show a strong dependency
on the endogenous regulation systems, as they display a circadian trend in different shiftwork conditions, like in controlled laboratory conditions, despite minor variations in this
trend by external factors. This then implies that working during the circadian trough
requires an additional effort as operators’ functional state is at its lowest level. Most
interestingly, other measures (here subjective tension and heart rate), which are known to
display a circadian trend in controlled laboratory conditions, are much more influenced
by external factors, so that the trends may vary considerably between situations (or jobsituations). Andorre and Queinnec (1998) reported a significant increase on the first shifthour for real-job performance (pages checked on a computer-screen) in operators
controlling a chemical production process. In both studies the atypical trend was
interpreted as indicating enhanced cognitive demands following shift take-over in jobsituations concerned with supervisory control of a dynamic process. Thus, in shift-work
conditions, some psychological and physiological measures, and in particular those that
are and other stress-sensitive, would be largely influenced by environmental factors,
including meal-timing, task demands, time-pressure and so on, which may mask the
otherwise circadian trend of these measures (Averty et al., 2004; Brookings et al., 1996;
Khaleque, 1984; Rose et al., 1982).
42 Advances in Air Navigation Services
3,5
2,5
1,5
6h
23
h
2h
30
21
h
18
h
15
h
13
h
8h
10
h3
0
0,5
Figure 1. Upper panel: Mean (+/-S.E.) alertness level on 3 occasions within each of three shifts (1h
following shift-start, middle, and 1h before shift-end). Lower panel: Mean (+/-S.E.) sublingual
temperature on the same recordings (Cariou et al., 2008).
3.3. Circadian variations of subjective measures in ATCs
In light of the inconsistencies of the findings in the literature concerning other subjective
measures than alertness, we investigated whether ATCs displayed typical circadian trends
for subjective measures and whether on-shift time would modulate these measures. In this
job-situation, traffic density variations across the 24h-day determine, partly at least, shift
schedules that include in particular overlapping shifts and variable shift-duration.
Shift
06:30 -14:00
07:00 - 17:30
09:00 - 20:00
11:00 - 20:00
15:30 - 23:00
20:00 - 07:00
01:00
Time of recording
07:00
13:00
0h30
6h30
0h00
6h00
4h00
2h00
19:00
10h00
8h00
3h30
5h00
Table 2. Time on duty of controllers on each of six shifts at the time each recording was performed
(Mélan et al., 2007).
Recall Performance in Air Traffic Controllers Across the 24-hr Day:
Influence of Alertness and Task Demands on Recall Strategies 43
Therefore, 15 volunteer ATCs were asked to rate Thayer’s (1989) checklist on 01:00, 07:00,
13:00, 19:00 by indicating whether they were on shift for four hours at most or for six hours
at least (table 2). Statistical analyses revealed significant time-of-day and time-on-duty
effects for both measures. ATCs rated alertness at a lower level at 01:00 and 07:00 than at
13:00 and 19:00 and tension at 07:00 compared to 01:00, 13:00 and 19:00. However, while selfrated tension was higher following long on-shift time, the opposite pattern was observed for
self-rated alertness. In other words, when controllers started day-duty they experienced
high alertness and low tension, whereas they reported decreased alertness and higher
subjective tension after several hours on duty.
Index of Subjective Alertness
3
2,8
2,6
2,4
2,2
< 4 hours
2
> 6 hours
1,8
1,6
1,4
1,2
1
13:00
19:00
Index of Subjective Tension
8
7,5
7
< 4 hours
6,5
> 6 hours
6
5,5
5
13:00
19:00
Figure 2. Mean (+/-SE) subjective alertness (upper pannel) and tension (lower pannel) in ATCs as a
function of recording time (Mélan et al., 2007).
These data favour the interpretation that lower alertness reported by 12-h workers
compared to 8-h workers on the early afternoon resulted from the fact that the former were
on the second half of duty while the latter started their afternoon-duty (Tucker et al., 1998).
Reduced day time alertness was observed on shifts starting late in the morning, though
controllers had probably sufficient sleep on the night prior the shift. This raises the
44 Advances in Air Navigation Services
possibility that the chronic sleep deficit observed in shift-workers may, partly, account for
decreased day-time alertness.
Further, both measures were negatively correlated, indicating that the lower ATCs quoted
alertness, the higher they quoted tension. The data thus extend the findings of a circadian
variation of subjective measures in ATCs and they favour the interpretation that in stressrelated job-situations enhanced tension may compensate for decreased alertness (Thayer,
1989), thereby enabling the maintenance of safety. Further investigations will however be
necessary to establish firmly whether ATCs’ tension-ratings indicate a direct influence by
environmental factors (i.e. hours at work, heavy traffic), or whether the observed
variations are merely the consequence of their functional state, as indicated by the
negative correlation between tension and alertness. Thus, air traffic control activities
differ indeed between day- and night-work, as high traffic on day-time involves sustained
periods of high task requirements and attentional demands, whereas low traffic during
the night would favor boredom proneness and sleepiness (Costa, 1999; Lille & Cheliout,
1982; Luna et al., 1997).
In recent years, most studies exploring the impact of shift work on health and well-being
have reported troubles in psychological and social well-being, performance efficiency and
increased stress levels (Costa, 2003). Some of these issues will be highlighted in the next
section by exploring the relationship between ATCs’ physiological state and their
performance efficiency during short-term recall of verbal material.
4. Complex interactions between task-related factors and ATCs’
functional state
The shift-work literature shows that psychological measures differ not only according to
time of day but also according to task characteristics, with high performance for tasks
requiring rather automatic processing such as short-term memory task when alertness is
low (in the morning), and high performance in more demanding tasks, relying for instance
on long-term memory processing when alertness is high (in the evening). Laboratory studies
reported indeed higher immediate recall in short-term memory tasks in the morning and
enhanced delayed recall from long-term memory in the evening (Folkard, 1979; Folkard et
al., 1976; Folkard & Monk, 1980; Monk & Embrey, 1981). Further, recall performance was
reported to be higher in the morning when a recognition procedure was used, and in the
afternoon when participants had to recall the text with a more demanding free recall
procedure (Lorenzetti & Natale, 1996; Oakhill & Davies, 1989). Free recall would be
cognitively more demanding as it involves an active search of memory traces in the absence
of any cues, while item recognition would rely on less demanding item familiarity (Brébion
et al., 2005; Mandler, 1980; Prince et al., 2005).
In light of the findings of the literature, it was important to investigate whether and to what
extent the above-reported modality effect may be sensitive to alertness variations, but also to
the cognitive effort required to remember the verbal material (Mélan, et al., 2007). ATCs’
recall performance was recorded on different times of the day (01:00, 07:00, 13:00 and 19:00)
Recall Performance in Air Traffic Controllers Across the 24-hr Day:
Influence of Alertness and Task Demands on Recall Strategies 45
while varying item modality (auditory or visual) during encoding and restitution, list-length
(6- and 9-item lists) and restitution processes (recognition and free recall).
6 items auditory
3,8
9 items auditory
Correct responses
3,6
6 items visual
9 items visual
3,4
3,2
3
2,8
2,6
2,4
01:00
07:00
13:00
19:00
6 items auditory
0,9
6 items visual
Correct probability
0,85
9 items auditory
0,8
9 items visual
0,75
0,7
0,65
0,6
0,55
0,5
0,45
0,4
01:00
07:00
13:00
19:00
Figure 3. Mean number of correct responses in a probe recognition task (upper panel) and in a free
recall task (lower panel) on each of four recordings according to the number of items presented (6- vs. 9item lists) and presentation modality (auditory vs. visual; Mélan, et al., 2007).
Overall, ATCs’ performance was lowest on 07:00 compared to 13:00 and 19:00, in particular
for visual items, for the longer lists and in the free recall task (Fig.3). In consequence, when
the tasks involved more demanding processing performance was decreased particularly
when alertness was low. Interestingly, significant main effects occurred for time-of-day, listlength and modality with the free recall procedure, while the three variables interacted
when restitution was based on a recognition procedure. In that case, performance dropped
in the early morning only for visual 9-item lists, i.e. in the more demanding task conditions.
In the literature, differential time-of-day effects on participants' recall performance in
recognition and free recall tasks were accounted for by similar processing differences
(Folkard, 1979; Folkard, et al., 1976; Folkard & Monk, 1980; Lorenzetti & Natale, 1996;
Oakhill & Davies, 1989). Accordingly, the finding of an overall effect on free recall but not
on recognition performance may indicate that external factors (time of day) more readily
46 Advances in Air Navigation Services
impacted on ATCs’ task performance when deeper processing was required to solve a task.
As indicated above, item recognition requires less in-depth processing of the to-beremembered material than free recall (Brébion et al., 2005; Mandler, 1980; Prince et al., 2005).
Thus, task-dependent factors (modality, list-length and recall procedure in the present case)
even further impact on ATCs’ performance when their functional state is low.
5. Discussion
The main finding of the present contribution is that ATCs’ performance depends in a
complex manner from the task to be performed, but also from job organisation (shift
schedules, shift-duration) and from physiological aspects (alertness, sensory and cognitive
processing). Even though it is difficult to generalize between different job-situations, given
in particular differences between activities, to-be-performed tasks and shift-scheduling, the
data confirm findings reported in other safety-related job-situations for security agents in a
nuclear power plant and for operators controlling satellites (Costa, 1999; Cariou et al., 2008;
Galy et al., 2008). Working during the night causes a mismatch between the endogenous
circadian timing system and the environmental synchronizers (the light/dark cycle in
particular), with consequent disturbances of the normal circadian rhythms of psychophysiological functions, beginning with the sleep/wake rhythm, and thus operators’
alertness. In addition to the disruptive effects of shift work on performance efficiency, its
impact on health and well-being are now well-documented (Costa, 2003; Siegrist, 2010). In
this respect, some international directives have recently stressed the need for the careful
organization of shift and night work and the protection of shift workers’ health.
Elsewhere, the present findings favour the idea of a more general model to account for the
complex interactions reported so far. In this regard, we recently extended Sweller’s
cognitive load theory elaborated in the educational field (Sweller, 1988; 1994), to a real-job
situation (Galy et al., 2012).
5.1. Towards an integrated model of mental load during ATC?
Sweller defined three categories of cognitive load in order to embrace the complexities of a
given task, the conditions in which it is performed, and subject-related variables. “Intrinsic
cognitive load” refers to the load induced by the material to be processed, such as task
difficulty that is defined in particular by the number of items to be processed, and by item
interactivity (Ayres, 2006; Kalyuga et al., 2003; Sweller & Chandler, 1994). “Extraneous
mental workload” refers to the load induced by external factors, including work situation,
work organization, time pressure, and noise (Sweller, 1994). Finally, “germane mental
workload” corresponds to the load induced by conscious application of strategies to solve
tasks more easily (Schnotz & Kürschner, 2007). We showed an additive effect of intrinsic
load (task difficulty) and extrinsic load (time pressure) factors, and that this effect was only
observed in the morning. In other words, when participants performed a difficult task under
high time pressure in the morning, when alertness and thus available mental resources were
low, they probably had to use specific strategies generating germane load. This sequence of
Recall Performance in Air Traffic Controllers Across the 24-hr Day:
Influence of Alertness and Task Demands on Recall Strategies 47
events would have led to decreased performance in the morning, while no such effect was
observed in the afternoon, when alertness and thus available mental resources were high.
Figure 4. Graphic representation of putative relationships between cognitive load factors and cognitive
load categories (Galy et al., 2012).
This model, summarized in figure 4, further indicates that the different cognitive load
measures used in the study, i.e. subjective measures (self-rated effort), behavioural measures
(correct responses) and psychophysiological measures (heart rate variability) display a
differential sensitivity to the three kinds of load factors investigated. More especially, heart
rate variability increased with germane load, operationalized by high self-reported
48 Advances in Air Navigation Services
alertness, whereas participants’ self-rated mental effort was sensitive to both extrinsic and
intrinsic load and task performance was determined both by alertness and by an interaction
between task difficulty and time pressure. The latter results stress the difficulties
encountered in order to identify a reliable measure of cognitive load on one hand (Backs &
Seljos, 1994; Brookings et al., 1996; Carroll et al., 1986), and suggest that it may be hazardous
to generalize the proposed model to other situations, on the other hand.
With these limitations in mind, it may nevertheless be challenging to test the mental load
model in ATCs, given that the findings reported in the previous sections indicate how work
organization (shift schedules, shift-duration) affected alertness (germane mental load), and
that when ATCs’ alertness was low, their mnemonic performance was lower in the
demanding task conditions (intrinsic mental load). Further, as conflicts between aircrafts
require ATCs to make the right decisions under high time pressure and to give ground-toair instructions in a limited time, it seems indeed worthwhile to include this extrinsic load
factor to a more integrative approach of mental load generated during ATC activities.
5.2. Experimental designs to explore ATC activities
Experimental paradigms like the ones used in the studies reported in this contribution may
be regarded as simplified models of real-life activities. As such, the results reported may be
of interest for ATC activities involving similar cognitive processes than those explored in the
experimental paradigms. This is in particular the case for task-related factors, as the
influence of such factors may only be demonstrated in controlled study conditions. In this
regard, experimental approaches are complementary to in situ observations, which point to
the relevant aspects to be explored more systematically by using experimental designs.
Further, the observations reported here were performed while ATCs were in their habitual
work environment and work conditions. In contrast, in a number of field studies subjective
data across the 24h-day are collected retrospectively, i.e. participants are asked to rate these
measures on a single session by remembering what they perceived for instance during the
night or during the morning shift. In contrast, subjective and performance measures
reported in the present contribution were collected in real-time fashion, in order to gain
some insight into ATCs’ cognitive abilities in real-job conditions. This is particularly
important when investigating job-activities performed on a continuous 24h-day, like ATC.
The data reported here clearly show that shift-work features (time-of-day and time-on-shift)
are indeed critical factors that impact not only on operators’ functional state, but also on a
number of psychological measures. ATCs’ information processing ability is crucial for the
safety of the air traffic management system as well as for the sector capacity of a given
complexity in a particular time driving the overall system performances.
The present findings may be relevant to ensure productivity and/or safety in job-situations
involving supervisory control, and in particular ATC, all relying on processing visual and
auditory information from various sources on control panels and interfaces. The present
findings suggest that short sequences of auditory information would less readily tax
controller’s processing capacities than longer sequences and/or visual material. As stated by
Recall Performance in Air Traffic Controllers Across the 24-hr Day:
Influence of Alertness and Task Demands on Recall Strategies 49
Frankish (2008), all models of speech perception incorporate some form of auditory short
term memory, because speech comprehension requires the integration of information from
successive elements. The findings reported here may thus be useful for various control
operations and in particular during conflict resolution, when controllers’ memory span may
be more readily taxed. As shown by several studies, short-term recall of navigational
messages decreases when message length increases beyond three commands (Wickens &
Hollands, 2000; Barshi & Healy, 2002; Schneider et al., 2004). Taken together with several
other studies, the present results may also be useful for ATC selection and training, as they
stress the importance of using tests that manipulate, in addition to the more traditional
quantitative aspects of memory, more qualitative parameters, such as presentation modality
or information type and aircraft status (Gronlund et al., 2005; Means et al., 1988).
ATC, like several other safety-related job-activities rely preferentially on visual interfaces,
most probably because graphical representations enhance the understanding of complex
principles or spatial relations, for instance flight direction, speed and other information that
an operator needs to synthetize in order to solve conflicting situations for instance. Within
the auditory stream, successive items are strongly associated; in contrast, in the visual
modality, it is simultaneously presented items that are strongly associated (Penney, 1989).
Speech, with its linear progression through time does not bear these properties. In addition,
sound and speech generate considerable noise when compared to silent reading of messages
or integration of graphical representations.
However, for precisely the same reason, i.e. the disturbance induced by auditory
information, this modality is typically used for alarm devices, as one cannot avoid an
auditory signal. Indeed, the physical nature of sounds, i.e. waves that cross space in all
directions, ensures that this kind of information automatically reaches and stimulates the
auditory receptors and is transferred thereafter to the auditory cortex. There is thus a pretty
good chance for heard messages or signals to be processed, even without paying attention to
one’s environment or when a subject’s motivation or alertness are decreased for some
reason. Furthermore, in addition to a specific activation of the auditory system, sounds also
induce a non-specific activation of the nervous system, both the autonomous nervous
system as evidenced for instance by increased heart-rate, and the central nervous system, as
indicated by enhanced arousal. On contrary, written messages may be processed by the
visual system only once a subject has oriented his/her gaze on the specific location where the
message has occurred. Thus, unless the operator turns his/her attention to a particular
location on a radar screen, he/she will not be able to appreciate whether for instance some
airplane may be in a difficult position. Research concerning participant’s focus of attention
that would reflect conscious awareness and its relation to visual working memory tasks
have been reviewed recently (Cowan, 2011).
In light of these considerations it appears that supervisory control greatly benefits from a
well-weighted combination of visual representations of complex multi-dimensional data
and auditory presentation of essential information which have to be maintained in a shortterm memory for problem resolution.
50 Advances in Air Navigation Services
Author details
Claudine Mélan
Shift-work and Cognition Laboratory, Toulouse University, France
Edith Galy
Research Centre in the Psychology of Cognition, Language, and Emotion, Aix-Marseille University,
France
6. References
Akerstedt, T. (1991). Sleepiness at work: Effect of irregular work hours. In: Sleep, sleepiness
and performance, T. Monk, (Ed.), 129-152, John Wiley & Sons ltd,
Akerstedt, T. (2007). Altered sleep/wake patterns and mental performance. Physiology and
Behavior, Vol.90, No.2-3, pp. 209-18
Akerstedt T, Folkard S. & Fortin C. (2004). Predictions from the three-process model of
alertness. Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine, Vol.75, pp.75-83
Akerstedt, T. & Gillberg, M. (1990). Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active
individual. International Journal of Neuroscience, Vol.52, pp. 29-37
Andorre, V. & Quéinnec, Y. (1998). Changes in supervisory activity of a continuous process
during night and day shifts. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics, Vol.21, pp. 179186
Averty, P., Collet, C., Dittmar, A., Athenes, S. & Vernet-Maury E. (2004). Mental workload in
air traffic control: an index constructed from field tests. Aviation Space and
Environmental Medicine, Vol.75, pp. 333-341
Ayres, P. (2006). Using subjective measures to detect variations of intrinsic cognitive load
within problems. Learning and Instruction, Vol.16, pp. 389–400
Backs, R.W. & Seljos, K.A. (1994). Metabolic and cardio-respiratory of mental effort: the
effects of level of difficulty in a working memory task. International Journal of
Psychophysiology, Vol.16, pp. 57-69
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press, UK
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends
in Cognitive Science, Vol.4, No.11, pp. 417–423
Baddeley, A. D. & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In: The Psychology of Learning and
Motivation, G.A. Bower, (Ed.), 48–79, Academic Press
Brookings, J.B, Wilson, G.F. & Swain, C.R. (1996). Psychophysiological responses to changes
in workload during simulated air traffic control. Biological Psychology, Vol.42, pp. 361377
Beaman, C.P. & Morton, J. (2000). The separate but related origins of the recency effect and
the modality effect in free recall. Cognition, Vol.77, 59-65
Barshi, I. & Healy, A. F. (2002). The effects of mental representation on performance in a
navigation task. Memory and Cognition, Vol.30, pp. 1189-1203
Recall Performance in Air Traffic Controllers Across the 24-hr Day:
Influence of Alertness and Task Demands on Recall Strategies 51
Brébion, G., David, A.S., Bressan, R.A. & Pilowski, L.S. (2005). Word frequency effects on
free recall and recognition in patients with schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatry Research,
Vol.39, pp. 215-222
Conrad, R. & Hull, A.J. (1968) Input modality and the serial position curve in short-term
memory. Psychonomic Science, Vol.10, no.4), pp.135-136.
Cariou, M.; Galy, E. & Mélan, C. (2008). Differential 24-h variations of alertness and
subjective tension in process controllers: investigation of a relationship with body
temperature and heart rate. Chronobiology International, Vol.25, No.4, pp. 97-609
Carroll, D., Turner J.R. & Hellawell, J.C. (1986). Heart rate and oxygen consumption during
active psychological challenge: the effects of level of difficulty. Psychophysiology.
23:174-181
Carroll, L.M.; Jalbert, A., Penney, A.M., Neath, I., Surprenant, A.M. & Tehan, G. (2010).
Evidence for proactive interference in the focus of attention of working memory.
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol.64, pp. 208–214
Costa, G. (1996). The impact of shift and nightwork on health. Applied Ergonomics, Vol.27,
no.1, pp. 9-16
Costa, G. (1999). Fatigue and biological rhythms. In: Handbook of aviation human factors. D.J.
Garland, J.A. Wise & V.D. Hopkin, (Eds.), 235-255, London: Erlbaum
Costa, G. (2003). Shift work and occupational medicine: An overview. Occupational Medicine,
Vol.53, pp. 83-88
Cowan, N. (1984). On short and long auditory stores. Psychological Bulletin, Vol.96, pp. 341370
Cowan, N. (2011).The focus of attention as observed in visual working memory tasks:
Making sense of competing claims. Neuropsychologia, Vol.49, pp. 1401–1406
Cowan, N.; Saults, J.S., Elliott, E.M. & Moreno, M.V. (2002). Deconfounding serial recall.
Journal of Memory and Language, Vol.46, pp. 153-177
Cowan, N.; Saults, J.S. & Brown, G. D. (2004). On the auditory modality superiority effect in
serial recall: separating input and output factors. Journal of Memory and Language, Vol.46,
pp. 153-177
Crowder, R.G. & Morton, J. (1969). Precategorical acoustic storage (PAS). Perception and
Psychophysics, Vol.5, pp. 365-373
Frankish, C. (1985). Modality-specific grouping effects in short-term memory. Journal of
Memory and Language, Vol.24, pp. 200-209
Frankish, C. (2008). Precategorical acoustic storage and the perception of speech. Journal of
Memory and Language, Vol.58, pp. 815–836
Della Rocco, P.S. & Nesthus, T.E. (2005). Shift-work and air traffic control: Transitioning
research results to the workforce. In: Human factors impacts in air traffic management. B.
Kirwan, M. Rogers, D. Schäfer, (Eds), 243-278, Aldershot, UK: Ashgate
Dinges, L.S.; Pack, F., Williams, W., Gillen, K.A., Powell, J.W., Ott, G.E., Aptowicz, C. &
Pack, A.I. (1997). Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance
performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. Sleep,
Vol.20, No.4, pp. 267-277
52 Advances in Air Navigation Services
Folkard, S. (1979). Times of day and level of processing. Memory and Cognition, Vol.7, PP.
247-252.
Folkard, S.; Knauth, P., Monk, T.H. & Rutenfranz, J. (1976). The effect of memory load on the
circadian variation in performance efficiency under a rapidly rotating shift system.
Ergonomics, Vol.19, pp. 479-488.
Folkard, S. & Monk, T. H. (1980). Circadian rhythms in human memory. British Journal of
Psychology, Vol.71, pp. 295-307.
Folkard, S. & Akerstedt, T. (1992). A three-process model of the regulation of alertnesssleepiness. In: Sleep, arousal, and performance, R.J. Broughton & R.D. Ogilvie, (Eds.), 1126, Boston: Birkhaüser
Folkard, S. & Tucker, P. (2003). Shift work, safety and productivity. Occupational Medicine
(London), Vol.53, PP. 95-101.
Galy, E.; Cariou, M. & Mélan, C. (2012). What is the relationship between mental workload
factors and cognitive load types?, International Journal of Psychophysiology, Vol.83, No.3,
pp. 269-275
Galy, E.; Mélan, C. & Cariou, M. (2008). Investigation of task performance variations
according to task requirements and alertness across the 24-h day in shift-workers.
Ergonomics, Vol.51, No.9, pp. 1338-51
Galy, E.; Mélan, C. & Cariou, M. (2010). Investigation of ATCs' response strategies in a free
recall task: what makes auditory recall superior to visual recall? International Journal of
Aviation Psychology, Vol.20, No.3, pp. 295-307.
Glenberg, A. M., & Swanson, N. G. (1986). A temporal distinctiveness theory of recency and
modality effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
Vol.12, No1, pp. 3-15
Gronlund, S.D.; Dougherty, M.R.P., Durso, F.T., Canning, J.M. & Mills, S. H. (2005).
Planning in air traffic control: Impact of problem type. International Journal of Aviation
Psychology, Vol.15, pp. 269-293
Gronlund, S.D.; Ohrt, D.D., Dougherty, M.R.P., Perry, J.L. & Manning, C.A. (1998). Role of
memory in air traffic control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, &
Cognition, Vol.15, pp. 846-858
Harvey, A.J. & Beaman, C.P. Input and output modality effects in immediate serial recall.
Memory, Vol.15, No.7, pp. 693-700
Kecklund, G., Akerstedt, T. & Lowden, A. (1997). Morning work: effects of early rising on
sleep and alertness. Sleep, Vol. 20, pp. 215-223
Kalyuga, S., Ayres, P., Chandler, P. & Sweller, J. (2003). The expertise reversal effect.
Educational Psychologist, Vol.38, pp. 23–31
Khaleque, A. (1984). Circadian rhythms in heart rate of shift and day workers. Journal of
Human Ergology, Vol.13, pp. 23-29
Lille, F. & Cheliout, F. (1982). Variations in diurnal and nocturnal waking state in air traffic
controllers. European Journal of Applied Physiology, Vol.49, pp. 319-328
Lorenzetti, R. & Natale, V. (1996). Time of day and processing strategies in narrative
comprehension. British Journal of Psychology, Vol.87, pp. 209-221
Recall Performance in Air Traffic Controllers Across the 24-hr Day:
Influence of Alertness and Task Demands on Recall Strategies 53
Luna, T.D., French. J. & Mitcha, J.L. (1997). A study of USAF air traffic controller shiftwork:
sleep, fatigue, activity, and mood analyses. Aviation Space and Environmental Medicine,
Vol.68, pp. 18-23
Madigan, S. A. (1971). Modality and recall order interactions in short-term memory for serial
order. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol.87, pp. 294-296
Means, B.; Mumaw, R.J., Roth, C., Schlager, M.S., McWilliams, E., Gagne, E. et al. (1988).
ATC training analysis study: Design of the next-generation ATC training system. report N°.
FAA/OPM 342-036, Washington, DC: Department of Transportation/federal Aviation
Administration, USA
Mélan, C.; Galy, E. & Cariou, M. (2007). Mnemonic Processing in Air Traffic Controllers
(ATCs): Effects of Task Parameters and Work Organization. International Journal of
Aviation Psychology, Vol.17, No.4, pp. 391-409
Monk, T.H.; Fookson, J.E., Moline, M.L. & Pollak, C.P. (1985). Diurnal variation in mood and
performance in a time isolated environment. Chronobiology International, Vol.2, pp. 185193
Oakhill, J. & Davies, A.M. (1989). The effects of time of day and subjects' test expectations on
recall and recognition of prose materials. Acta Psychologica, Vol.72, pp. 145-157
Oberauer, K. & Vockenberg, K. (2009). Updating of working memory: Lingering bindings.
The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol.62, pp. 967-987
Owens, D. S.; Macdonald, I., Tucker, P., Sytnik, N., Totterdell, P., Minors, D., et al. (2000).
Diurnal variations in the mood and performance of highly practiced young women
living under strictly controlled conditions. British Journal of Psychology, Vol.91, pp. 41-60
Penney, C. G. (1989). Modality effects and structure of short-term verbal memory. Memory
and Cognition, Vol.17, No.4, pp. 398-422
Prizmic Z.; Vidadek S., Radosevic-Vidadek B. & Kaliterna L. (1995). Shiftwork tolerance and
24-h variations in moods. Work Stress, Vol. 9, pp. 327-334
Rose, R.M.; Jenkins, C.D., Hurst, M., Herd, J.A. & Hall RP. (1982). Endocrine activity in air
traffic controllers at work. II. Biological, psychological and work correlates.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, Vol.7, pp. 113-123
Rosa R. (1995). Extended workshifts and excessive fatigue. Journal of Sleep Research, Vol.4,
pp. 51-56
Rummer, R. & Schweppe, J. (2005). Evidence for a modality effect in sentence retention.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, Vol.12, No.6, pp.1094-1099
Prince, S.E., Daselaar, S.M. & Cabeza, R. (2005). Neural of relational memory: successful
encoding and retrieval of semantic and perceptual associations. Journal of Neuroscience,
Vol.25, pp. 1203-1210
Schneider, V.I.; Healy, A.F. & Barshi, I. (2004). Effects of instruction modality and read-back
on accuracy in flowing navigation commands. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Applied, Vol.10, pp. 245-257
Schnotz, W. & Kürschner, C. (2007). A reconsideration of cognitive load theory. Educational
Psychology Review, Vol.19, pp. 469–508
Siegrist, J. (2010). Effort-reward imbalance at work and cardiovascular diseases. International
Journal of Occupational Medicine and Environmental Health, Vol.23, pp. 279-285
54 Advances in Air Navigation Services
Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: effects on learning. Cognitive
Science, Vol.12, pp. 257–285
Sweller, J. (1994). Cognitive load theory, learning difficulty and instructional design.
Learning and Instruction, Vol.4, pp. 295–312
Sweller, J. & Chandler, P. (1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognition and
Instruction, Vol.12, pp. 185–233
Thayer, R.E. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and arousal. New-York: Oxford University Press
Tucker, P., Lombardi, D., Smith L. & Folkard, S. (2006). The Impact of Rest Breaks on
Temporal Trends in Injury Risk. Chronobiology International, Vol. 23, pp. 1423–1434
Tucker P, Smith L, Macdonald I, Folkard S. (1998). Shift length as a determinant of
retrospective on-shift alertness. Scandinavian Journal of Work and Environmental Health,
Vol.24, pp. 49-54
Wright, K.P., Hull, J.T. & Czeisler, C.A. (2002). Relationship between alertness, performance,
and body temperature in humans. American Journal of Physiology, Vol. 253, pp. 1370-1377
Wickens, C.D. & Hollands, J.G. (2000). Engineering psychology and human performance (3rd
ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall, USA
Download