Supporting Preschoolers` Vocabulary Learning

advertisement
Tanya Christ and X. Christine Wang
Supporting
Preschoolers’ Vocabulary Learning
Using a Decision-Making Model to
Select Appropriate Words and Methods
Young children
© Ellen B. Senisi
learn new vocabulary
with great agility and
speed, but their learning is dependent on
the range of words
they are exposed to.
Teachers can naturally
facilitate children’s
vocabulary learning using a variety of
strategies, including
making conversation
and posing thoughtful
questions. But there is
also an important role
for direct instruction,
which is what we focus
on in this article.
Ms. Allen is an
experienced Head Start teacher who
welcomed us into her classroom to
observe and mentor her as she facilitated children’s vocabulary learning.
As part of a research project, we
spent 12 weeks helping her implement
effective vocabulary practices in her
classroom. When we first sat down to
discuss how we could work on this
goal together, she told us that she
especially wanted to learn how to better select words and teaching meth-
Tanya Christ, PhD, is an assistant professor of reading and language arts at Oakland
University in Rochester, Michigan. Her research focuses on early childhood vocabulary
acquisition, issues of educational access, and teacher education. christ@oakland.edu
X. Christine Wang, PhD, is an associate professor of early childhood education at State
University of New York at Buffalo. Her research focuses on technology-supported collaborative learning, early science inquiry, and vocabulary development, as well as early
childhood education in international contexts. wangxc@buffalo.edu
®
74
2, 3
ods for helping children
in her class directly learn
vocabulary. To support
Ms. Allen and other teachers, we developed a simple
three-step model to inform
the selection of appropriate vocabulary words from
curriculum materials and
activities. Such words
might come up during
shared book readings or
be words children need to
understand a classroom
science project. The model
also addresses how teachers can plan ways to teach
those words during classroom activities (Christ &
Wang 2010a) (see “The
Vocabulary Decision-Making Model”).
At the heart of good instruction
is thoughtful and informed teacher
decision making (NAEYC 2009). These
steps are research-based and were
field-tested in Ms. Allen’s classroom,
as well as university practicum experiences spanning three years:
1. Identify all the words that most
children in the class are unlikely to
know;
2. Select a small set of vocabulary
words to focus on; and
3. Determine what methods will best
support children’s acquisition of
the selected vocabulary.
Young Children • March 2012
Step 1.
Identify all difficult and potentially unknown words in a text.

Step 2.
Select words for instruction.
Select a small set of words that are:
Read all other
words without
teaching their
meaning.


a. necessary for comprehension,
b. usable in children’s lives,
c. able to be taught multiple times
across the curriculum, and
d. related to other vocabulary being
taught.
Step 3.

The word represents
a new concept.
No meaning
clues in text.



Directly teach the
word’s meaning.

Select methods for
teaching words.
Young Children • March 2012
To narrow the list of words identified in step 1, Ms. Allen first considered how many words would be
appropriate to teach the children
during the storybook reading: “I try
The Vocabulary Decision-Making Model
Identify all the words that most
children in the class are unlikely
to know.
When Ms. Allen first read the book
Where the Forest Meets the Sea, before
bringing it to her classroom, she identified all the words in the text for which
the children were unlikely to know
meanings: reef, cockatoos, squawking,
creek, creepers, bank, ancient, hollow,
aboriginal, and twisted. She also identified two words that she thought a
few of the younger children might not
know: pretend and toward. Then she
noted words that the children already
knew from previous instruction, which
could be reviewed while reading this
book: vines (which occurred in both
Select a small set of vocabulary
words to focus on.
The word represents
a familiar concept.

Step 1.
Step 2.
the text and illustrations); and camouflage, canopy, and understory (which
did not occur in the text, but were
depicted in the illustrations). Because
it would be inappropriate (and ineffective) for Ms. Allen to teach all twelve
of these potentially new words to children during a single reading of the storybook, she needed to winnow her list.

In the following sections, we explain
and illustrate each step in the model
using examples from a shared reading
lesson. Teachers can apply the decisionmaking model to plan ways to support
vocabulary learning during other classroom activities as well. For example,
teachers can identify curriculumbased vocabulary that will occur during a social studies project, and then
use the model to select specific words
and teaching methods. Similarly,
teachers can consider new vocabulary
to use during morning meetings (for
example, new words to follow up on a
nature walk), and then use the model
to decide which of these words to
teach and how to do so.
To illustrate the steps succinctly, we
will present the decision-making steps
using an activity that occurs in most
early childhood classrooms—reading
a story aloud. The planning strategies
presented below come from an interview with Ms. Allen near the end of our
time working with her. In the excerpts
that follow, she discusses her use of
the model to select several words and
methods to support children’s word
learning. She applied the model to
reading aloud the book Where the Forest Meets the Sea, by Jeannie Baker.

Meaning clues
in text.

Teach a wordlearning strategy.
75
Using the same guiding questions may lead different
teachers to select different words to focus on, due to
differences in the children’s experiences, interests,
and needs.
a. Is this word important for story
comprehension? Decide whether
understanding this word is important to comprehension—if it is,
76
then teach the children the word
meaning (Sternberg 1987). A good
test for this is deciding whether
a listener would have difficulty
understanding the story if the word
were excluded from the text.
b. Will children use this word independently? Consider whether children will be able to use this word in
their everyday interactions (Beck,
McKeown, & Kucan 2002). For
example, ask yourself, Can children
use this word in conversations with
friends, on the playground, during
classroom activities, or at home?
c. Can this word be discussed multiple times across multiple contexts
in the classroom? When children
have opportunities to reuse and
review a word during numerous
other activities, they are more
likely to retain its meaning (Beck,
McKeown, & Kucan 2002; Wang &
Christ 2010).
d. Is this word related to other
words the children are learning?
When children learn words that
are related to one another, they are
likely to organize new word meanings in their minds (Hyerle 2004).
For example, if children are learning about the animals that live in
the rain forest, the word predator
provides one way to categorize the
animals; when children learn predator, a next step is to teach related
words, such as herbivore, carnivore,
and omnivore. Then all of these
words can be used to discuss more
complex concepts, such as food
chains and food webs.
Ms. Allen explained her word selections with respect to the four questions:
© Julia Luckenbill
to pick only a few vocabulary words,
because 4- and 5-year-olds have a
hard time sitting still for that long and
really staying focused on the activity.”
Currently, there is no research to
indicate the exact number of new
words that specific age groups can
learn at once. However, our experience suggests that learning two or
three words at a time is appropriate
for most preschoolers, whereas kindergartners may handle three to five
new words at a time. The number of
words children can learn at one time
varies from classroom to classroom
and year to year, depending on the
skills and knowledge of the children
in any given group. The only way to
determine the exact number of words
that most of the children in the classroom can learn is through trial and
error: Focus on two words at first,
and if the children learn these easily,
add another word the next time. Or,
if it seems more appropriate for the
group, begin with three words, and if
they have difficulty with three words,
try two the next time. All good teaching emerges from reflection on what
is working and flexible adaptation of
methods in the classroom.
Ms. Allen asked herself four questions about each of the 12 words
on her list, to guide her selection of
which words to teach during the first
reading of Where the Forest Meets the
Sea. (Note that she read the book
several times to the class and focused
on helping the children learn different
words each time.)
There are three really strong
words that I will use because I can
keep talking about them throughout the entire story—with vocabulary, the more they hear it, the
more they’ll remember it and think
about it. And that’ll be the words
creek, creepers, and twisted. I feel
that those words help explain the
story better together. I probably
would do creepers because it is an
action word, and they can actually have a movement associated
with it. Children could potentially
use the word creepers. They can
creep in the gym, they can creep
outside—same with twisted—next
time their shoelaces get all tangled
up, I can use the word twisted. So
the more exposure children have
to the words from the book, and
the more they can use those words
outside of the book, the stronger
their understanding of the vocabulary will become.
Keep in mind that using the same
guiding questions may lead different
teachers to select different words to
focus on, due to differences in the
children’s experiences, interests, and
Young Children • March 2012
Use story context clues to promote vocabulary learning. Children
need to learn how to actively, purposefully learn vocabulary (Blachowicz, Beyersdorfer, & Fisher 2006).
Teaching them to use context clues
to figure out word meanings can help
increase their vocabulary learning
(Sternberg 1987). The more supportive
the clues are, the better the opportunity to model how to use clues to figure out word meanings and/or to guide
children in learning how to figure out
word meanings on their own. After
lots of modeling and guided practice,
teachers can prompt children to try to
use the strategy independently.
The storybook illustration included
clues to the meaning of the word creek,
so Ms. Allen decided to guide the children through the process of figuring
out the word meaning as follows:
needs. This is why teacher decision
making is critical—no curriculum
guide can tell you which words the
children will need to know or will be
able to use in other contexts; only
you can make these decisions based
on your knowledge of the children in
your class.
(Ms. Allen reads a sentence from
the book.) “I followed a creek into
the rainforest.” I would definitely
talk about creek, and use this
(an illustration of the boy wading knee-high in the creek toward
the rainforest) as a picture clue.
I would say, “All right, they followed the creek. I wonder what
the creek is?” And I would have
them look at the picture. Then
once the children understood that
it was a body of water, I would
say, “Why do you think it is not
called ‘the river’?” Or, “Why do you
think it is not called ‘the lake’?”
Asking them about those words
would help them realize that it’s
a smaller body of water, that it
isn’t an ocean. So comparing and
contrasting what they know bodies
of water look like might help them
realize that a creek is smaller. And
hopefully they would see that in
the picture, the boy is standing
in it—it’s not deep, and they can
probably walk in it.
When Ms. Allen addressed this word
while reading the story to the class,
Basir explained that he figured out
where the creek was in the illustration because he knew it was water
and “saw the fishies in it.” After reading the story, Ms. Allen extended the
children’s understanding of the meaning of creek by using the picture clues
and asking questions such as, “Which
do you think is larger—an ocean or a
creek?” Since the children had studied
oceans previously, they deduced that
the creek was smaller based on the
A RECENT RELEASE FROM NAEYC
Step 3.
Determine what methods will best
support children’s acquisition of
the selected vocabulary.
Once Ms. Allen decided on an initial
set of words to teach, she thought
about the most meaningful way to
convey their meanings to the children
in her class. First, she considered
whether the children already were
familiar with related concepts; then,
whether there were context clues
available to help them figure out the
word meanings independently; and
last, whether directly teaching any of
the words would be useful.
Young Children • March 2012
Spotlight on Infants and Toddlers
Derry Koralek, NAEYC, and
Linda Groves Gillespie, ZERO TO THREE, editors
Quality child development programs for infants and toddlers tailor their care to meet the particular characteristics and developmental needs of these age groups.
This book offers engaging articles from Young Children
for those who work with or in support of infants and
toddlers and their families. It includes study guides for
many of the articles plus a comprehensive resource list.
ISBN: 9781928896760 • Item #296
$14.00 • Members: $11.20 20% savings
®
Order online at www.naeyc.org /store
or call 800-424-2460 option 5 (9:00 a.m. – 5:00 p.m. EST, Mon.–Fri.)
77
illustration. Ms. Allen asked
children the illustration of the
the children what clues helped
twisted roots and explain that
When a word is not clear from
them figure out that the creek
twisted means all stuck together
context clues, or represents a new
was smaller. Daren suggested,
and around one another.
“There are fish.” Ms. Allen
Later, after reading the story,
concept, plan to teach children the
countered, “There are fish
the teacher can provide mulword through a direct approach.
in the ocean, too.” Another
tiple examples of the word in
teacher prompted, “How do
context (Beck, McKeown, &
you know that it’s not deep
Kucan 2002). Ms. Allen planned
like an ocean?” Daren replied,
to share examples of twisted
exposures across varying contexts
“Because the boy’s standing in it!”
with the children in her class: when
(Vygotsky 1962; Christ 2007), it’s best
With careful prompting, the children
your shoelaces get twisted together
to take several approaches to teachwere able to use clues in the text to
and are hard to untie, or when strands
ing a particular word, depending upon
learn more about a creek.
of spaghetti get wound around one
what children already know about the
another on your plate and make it
Directly teach words that repreword’s meaning.
hard to get a small bite because the
sent new concepts. When a word is
When a new word is first introduced,
noodles are all twisted together.
not clear from context clues, or repa teacher might provide a brief defini Once children have some familiarresents a new concept, plan to teach
tion while reading the story, to help
ity with the word, teachers might ask
children the word through a direct
children maintain story comprehenthem to point to a representation of
approach (Kibby 1995). Since children
sion. For example, Ms. Allen planned
the word in an illustration in the book
learn word meanings incrementally,
to explain the word twisted while
they are reading, or ask brief quesover time and through multiple
reading the story. She would show the
tions that elicit the use of the word or
Approaches to Supporting Children’s Word Learning*
Approach
Description
Objective
References
Pointing and
labeling
Teacher uses labeling or pointing to
direct a child’s attention to an illustration of the word, usually while
saying the word, or the teacher asks
children to point to a word that they
have been exposed to previously
To help a child connect
the word with its related
concept visually or to help
solidify a child’s previous
connection between the
word and the image
Senechal 1997
Eliciting
questions
Teacher asks a comprehension
question that elicits a child’s use of
the word
To elicit use of the word and
to create a memory of the
word’s sound
Walsh & Blewitt 2006
Non-eliciting
questions
Teacher asks a comprehension question that requires a child to provide
information related to the word’s
meaning, but not the word itself
To get a child to notice and
provide information related
to the concept the word
represents
Justice 2002; Walsh & Blewitt 2006
Brief definitions
Teacher gives brief, child-friendly
explanation of the word’s meaning
To maintain comprehension
Coyne et al. 2004; Biemiller & Boote
2006
Extended
approaches
Teacher provides opportunities for
a child to more deeply process the
word’s meaning by having a child
respond using the target word correctly in a meaningful context
To elicit use of the word in
meaningful contexts
Beck, McKeown, & Kucan 2002; Coyne,
McCoach, & Kapp 2007;
Juel et al. 2003; Silverman 2007
*For a review of the research on vocabulary practices for pre-K and kindergarten, see Christ & Wang 2010b and Christ & Wang 2011.
78
Young Children • March 2012
NEW FROM NAEYC!
information about
the word’s meaning
Advancing the Early Childhood
during the story
Profession: NAEYC Standards and
(Senechal 1997;
Guidelines for Professional Development
Walsh & Blewitt
Alison Lutton, editor
2006). For example,
since the children
This new title offers NAEYC’s latest standards for early
had already learned
childhood professionals. It includes the newest versions of
the word vine from
our Standards for Early Childhood Professional Preparation
a previous story,
Programs; NAEYC’s Code of Ethics; Workforce Designs,
Ms. Allen asked
A Policy Blueprint for State Early Childhood Professional
Moses to point
Development Systems; glossaries for Training and Education
terms; and guidance on how to use these materials.
to the vine in the
ISBN: 9781928896814 • Item #364
illustration while
$20 • Member: $16 20% savings
she read Where
the Forest Meets
the Sea. Moses
Order online at confidently pointed
to a leaf on the
or call option 5 (9:00 a.m. – 5:00 p.m. EST, Mon.–Fri.)
vine, so Ms. Allen
clarified, “That’s
the leaf—here’s the
Selection of an appropriate method
learning. Her methods for supporting
vine” (while running her finger along
for teaching vocabulary depends on
the children’s word learning helped
the vine in the illustration). Then she
children’s (1) lack of or prior exposure
them gain new understanding of the
showed another illustration of a vine
to the word, (2) lack of or partial underfocus words. After using the decisionon the back cover of the book. To
standing of the word’s meaning, and
making model to guide vocabulary
check children’s comprehension, she
(3) background knowledge. Deciding
teaching over eight weeks, the chilasked all the children to point to the
which method to use is also based on
dren in Ms. Allen’s class scored signifivine they had planted in their class(4) how many or what kinds of other
cantly higher on a curriculum-based
room. Most of the children pointed
opportunities to revisit this word’s
assessment of vocabulary knowledge
to the terrarium that housed the
meaning are likely to arise in future
than similar children in a control class.
vine in response to her request. As is
classroom activities. Teachers’ deci As in any strategic thinking approach,
evident in this example, children typision making is critical for successful
this step-by-step planning is only neccally need multiple opportunities to
vocabulary instruction, because only
essary until a teacher has internalized
encounter a word to learn its meaning.
a teacher who knows the children well
the decision-making process. Over
Since it takes longer to provide
will provide appropriate challenges to
time, this process will become more
extended responses and discussions
advance their development (NAEYC
automatic and efficient. Ms. Allen
about word meanings that help chil2009).
summarized for us her experience
dren deeply process the meanings,
The table “Approaches to Supusing the model: “When you first came
they should occur after reading, so as
porting Children’s Word Learning”
in, I thought, ‘Oh my goodness, what
not to impair the flow of the story and
includes the descriptions of several
am I doing?’ Then I read about the
thus children’s story comprehension
methods for supporting children’s
model; I saw how it worked. Now I feel
(Beck, McKeown, & Kucan 2002). For
vocabulary learning. It details what
confident about being able to use this
example, after reading the story Ms.
kinds of learning objectives might be
model in my classroom. I really feel
Allen asked the children what they
met through each method, and prothat it is workable for any teacher.”
might find in a creek. John recalled
vides references for further study.
Teachers’ decision making and
“fishies.” Gina, who is a dual language
interactions with children impact
learner, suggested “a blue-morpho
what children learn (NAEYC 2009). We
butterfly.” Ms. Allen clarified, “You
know that when teachers use planned
might find a blue-morpho butterfly
Planning for success
and purposeful methods to teach
flying over a creek.” Gina’s suggestion
While reading Where the Forest Meets
children vocabulary, the children
exemplifies the importance of offering
the Sea, Ms. Allen effectively used the
learn more words (Biemiller & Boote
multiple opportunities to revisit word
model to guide her decision making
2006; Beck & McKeown 2007). Planmeanings to ensure all children’s sucfor supporting children’s vocabulary
ning for vocabulary learning in early
cessful word learning.
www.naeyc.org /store
800-424-2460
Young Children • March 2012
®
References
When teachers use
planned and purposeful methods to teach
children vocabulary,
the children learn
more words.
childhood is critical, because children
who have not developed a vast repertoire of words by the time they reach
the upper elementary grades may
experience reading and academic difficulty (Biemiller 2003; Chall & Jacobs
2003). Early intervention is especially
important for children from families
with low incomes (Hart & Risley
1995), children experiencing speech
and language difficulties (Nathan et
al. 2004), and dual language learners
(Shatz & Wilkinson 2010), who may
come to school knowing or being able
to articulate far fewer words than
other children (Hart & Risley 1995),
and for whom the vocabulary knowledge gap typically widens as they get
older (Biemiller & Slonim 2001). Given
the importance of understanding
word meanings to children’s reading
success, we hope that our model will
support more teachers as they make
decisions about implementing effective vocabulary teaching strategies in
early childhood classrooms.
Beck, I.L., & M.G. McKeown. 2007. “Increasing
Young Low-Income Children’s Oral Vocabulary Repertoires through Rich and Focused
Instruction.” The Elementary School Journal
107 (3): 251–73.
Beck, I.L., M.G. McKeown, & L. Kucan. 2002.
Bringing Words to Life: Robust Vocabulary
Instruction. New York: Guilford.
Biemiller, A. 2003. “Vocabulary: Needed If
More Children Are to Read Well.” Reading
Psychology 24 (3–4): 323–35.
Biemiller, A., & C. Boote. 2006. “An Effective
Method for Building Meaning Vocabulary
in Primary Grades.” Journal of Educational
Psychology 98 (1): 44–62.
Biemiller, A., & N. Slonim. 2001. “Estimating
Root Word Vocabulary Growth in Normative
and Advantaged Populations: Evidence for
a Common Sequence of Vocabulary Acquisition.” Journal of Educational Psychology 93
(3): 498–520.
Blachowicz, C.L.Z., J. Beyersdorfer, & P. Fisher.
2006. “Vocabulary Development and Technology: Teaching and Transformation.” In
International Handbook of Literacy and Technology: Volume II, eds. M.C. McKenna, L.D.
Labbo, R.D. Kieffer, & D. Reinking, 341–48.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Chall, J.S., & V.A. Jacobs. 2003. “The Classic
Study on Poor Children’s Fourth-Grade
Slump.” American Educator 27 (1): 14–15.
Christ, T. 2007. “Oral Language Exposure and
Incidental Vocabulary Acquisition: An Exploration across Kindergarten Classrooms.”
PhD diss., University at Buffalo, SUNY.
Christ, T., & X.C. Wang. 2010a. “A Spiral of
Words: Providing Multiple Exposures to
Vocabulary in Early Childhood through
Classroom Center Activities.” Workshop presented at International Reading Association
55th Annual Convention, Chicago, IL.
Christ, T., & X.C. Wang. 2010b. “Bridging the
Vocabulary Gap: What the Research Tells Us
about Vocabulary Instruction in Early Childhood.” Young Children 65 (4): 84–91.
Christ, T., & X.C. Wang. 2011. “Closing the
Vocabulary Gap? A Review of Research on
Early Childhood Vocabulary Practices.”
Reading Psychology 32 (5): 426–58.
Coyne, M.D., D.B. McCoach, & S. Kapp. 2007.
“Vocabulary Intervention for Kindergarten
The NAEYC Spring/Summer 2012
Resources Catalog is now online!
re!
ight futu !
a br
rces
re
ren12foresr ou
mer 20
ight tufuretu! !
g child
ring/sum
spun
g yo
fu future
n foraabrbr
ight ht
Preparin young childre
r a br
ig
fo
n
re n for
g young child
re
g ung child
PrepPrarepinarinyo
g
Preparin
Inside the spring/summer catalog are
powerful resources tailored to meet
the needs of early childhood educators at all levels of their service.
Look for new and best-selling titles
covering best practices, standards,
research, and applicable guidance to
assist early education professionals in
making a difference. Choose from a
wide selection of professional books,
multiple media, brochures, posters,
and other resources.
Members save 20% on all resources
in this catalog.
er today
a memb
Become
gs and
ial savin
for spec
Visit www.naeyc.org/store and receive the latest
resources to help you reach your goals!
s
privilege
2/8/12
8:55 AM
Students: Comparing Extended Instruction
to Embedded Instruction and Incidental
Exposure.” Learning Disability Quarterly 30
(2): 74–88.
Coyne, M.D., D.C. Simmons, E.J. Kame’enui, &
M. Stoolmiller. 2004. “Teaching Vocabulary
during Shared Storybook Readings: An
Examination of Differential Effects.” Exceptionality 12 (3): 145–62.
Hart, B., & T.R. Risley. 1995. Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young
American Children. Baltimore: Brookes.
Hyerle, D.N. 2004. “Thinking Maps as a
Transformational Language for Learning.”
In Student Successes with Thinking Maps:
School-Based Research, Results, and Models
for Achievement Using Visual Tools, eds. D.N.
Hyerle, L. Alper, & S. Curtis, 1–16. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
Juel, C., G. Biancarosa, D. Coker, & R. Deffes.
2003. “Walking with Rosie: A Cautionary Tale
of Early Reading Instruction.” Educational
Leadership 60 (7): 12–18.
Justice, L.M. 2002. “Word Exposure Conditions
and Preschoolers’ Novel Word Learning
during Shared Storybook Reading.” Reading
Psychology 23 (2): 87–106.
Kibby, M.W. 1995. “The Organization and
Teaching of Things and the Words That Signify Them.” Journal of Adolescent and Adult
Literacy 39 (3): 208–23.
NAEYC. 2009. “Developmentally Appropriate
Practice in Early Childhood Programs Serving Children from Birth to Age 8.” Position
statement. Washington, DC: Author. www.
naeyc.org/positionstatements/dap.
Nathan, L., J. Stackhouse, N. Goulandris, &
M.J. Snowling. 2004. “The Development of
Early Literacy Skills Among Children with
Speech Difficulties: A Test of the ‘Critical Age
Hypothesis.’ Journal of Speech, Language,
and Hearing Research 47: 377–91.
Senechal, M. 1997. “The Differential Effect of
Storybook Reading on Preschoolers’ Acquisition of Expressive and Receptive Vocabulary.”
Journal of Child Language 24 (1): 123–38.
Shatz, M., & L.C. Wilkinson, eds. 2010. The
Education of English Language Learners:
Research to Practice. New York: Guilford.
Silverman, R. 2007. “A Comparison of Three
Methods of Vocabulary Instruction during
Read-Alouds in Kindergarten.” The Elementary School Journal 108 (2): 97–113.
Sternberg, R.J. 1987. “Most Vocabulary Is
Learned from Context.” In The Nature of
Vocabulary Acquisition, eds. M.G. McKeown
& M.E. Curtis, 89–105. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Vygotsky, L. 1962. Thought and Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT.
Walsh, B.A., & P. Blewitt. 2006. “The Effect of
Questioning Style during Storybook Reading on Novel Vocabulary Acquisition of
Preschoolers.” Early Childhood Education
Journal 33 (4): 273–78.
Wang, X.C., & T. Christ. 2010. “Bridging the
Vocabulary Gap: A Model of Meaning Vocabulary Instruction in Early Childhood Classrooms.” Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Pacific Early Childhood Education
Research Association, Hangzhou, China.
Copyright © 2012 by the National Association for the
Education of Young Children. See Permissions and Reprints
online at www.naeyc.org/yc/permissions.
dd 1
ver.in
_CatalogCo
AEYC
2012N
Young Children • March 2012
Download